pik chard fluency

12
 C u r r e n t R e s e a r c h IN READING  /L ANGUAGE ARTS FLUENCY: THE BRIDGE FROM DECODING TO READING COMPREHENSION  JOHN J. PIKULSKI DAVIDJ. CHARD INTRODUC TI ON Fluency, which has been referred to as a neglectedand ignoredaspect of reading (National Reading Panel , 2000), is receiving substan- tial attention at this time from both researchers and practitioners. This attention may stem, at least in part, from the fact that the highly influential Report of the National Reading Panel discusses fluency as one of only five critical components of the reading process. Definitions of Reading Fluency The National Reading Panel report defines reading fluency as …the ability to read text quickly , accurately, and with proper expre ssion(p. 3–5). All three dimensions appear critical to a full definition of reading fluency (Dowhower , 1991). The fact that two of the three dimensions of fluency, accuracy and expressiveness , can be observed onl  y through oral reading may have contributed to the limited amount of attention that fluency received until re cently . Fluency was seen essentially as a word re cognition and oral reading phenomenon, and the importance of oral reading pales dramatically in comparison to that of silent reading comprehension. Except , per- haps, as beginning readers in school , we spend a miniscule amount of time doing expressive oral reading as compared to silent reading compre hension. The Literacy Dictionary: The Vocabulary of Reading and Writin  g, on the other hand, defines fluency as freedom from word identification problems that might hinder compre hension(Harris and Hodges , 1995, p. 85). Samuels, a pioneer in research and theo- ry in reading fluency , cites the alteration and enlargement of the construct of fluency to include reading comprehension as a major force in elevating the importance of the construct in the field of read- ing. He notes, To experience good reading compre- hension, the reader must be able to identify word s quickly and easily (Samuels , 2002, p. 167). The correlation between fluency and reading comprehension was clearly established by a

Upload: daniel-coffin

Post on 04-Nov-2015

246 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

DESCRIPTION

xczx

TRANSCRIPT

  • C u r r e n t R e s e a r c hI N RE A D I NG/LA N G UAG E ARTS

    FLU E N CY: THE BRIDGE FROM DECODING TO

    R EADING CO M P R E H E NS I O N

    JOHN J. PIKULSKI DAVID J. CHARD

    IN T RODUC TI O NFluency, which has been referred to as a

    neglected and i g n o re d aspect of re a d i n g(National Reading Panel, 2 0 0 0 ), is receiving substan-tial attention at this time from both re s e a rchers andpractitioners. This attention may stem, at least inp a r t, f rom the fact that the highly influential Reportof the National Reading Panel discusses fluency asone of only five critical components of the re a d i n gp rocess.

    Definitions of Reading Fluency

    The National Reading Panel report defines reading fluency as the ability to read text quickly,a c c u r a t e l y, and with proper expre s s i o n (p. 35). A l lt h ree dimensions appear critical to a full definition ofreading fluency (Dowhower, 1991). The fact that twoof the three dimensions of fluency, accuracy ande x p re s s i v e n e s s, can be observed o n l y t h rough oralreading may have contributed to the limited amount

    of attention that fluency received until re c e n t l y.Fluency was seen essentially as a word re c o g n i t i o nand oral reading phenomenon, and the importanceof oral reading pales dramatically in comparison tothat of silent reading comprehension. Except, p e r-h a p s, as beginning readers in school, we spend aminiscule amount of time doing expressive oralreading as compared to silent reading compre h e n s i o n .

    The Literacy Dictionary: The Vocabulary of Readingand Wr i t i n g, on the other hand, defines fluency asf reedom from word identification problems thatmight hinder compre h e n s i o n (Harris and Hodges,1 9 9 5, p. 85). Samuels, a pioneer in re s e a rch and theo-ry in reading fluency, cites the alteration ande n l a rgement of the construct of fluency to includereading comprehension as a major force in elevatingthe importance of the construct in the field of re a d-ing. He notes, To experience good reading compre-h e n s i o n, the reader must be able to identify word squickly and easily ( S a m u e l s, 2 0 0 2, p. 167).

    The correlation between fluency and re a d i n gc o m p rehension was clearly established by a

  • l a rge-scale analysis of data from the NationalAssessment of Educational Pro g ress in Reading(Pinnell et al., 1995). In that study, 44 percent of thesubjects were found to be disfluent when re a d i n ggrade-level appropriate materials that they had p re v -iously read silently; the study also showed a sign i f i-c a n t, positive relationship between oral reading flu-ency and reading comprehension performance.

    A c o m p rehensive definition then would seem to relate the centrality of fluency to reading compre-hension and the established dimensions of the con-s t ruct. We would propose the following definition:Reading fluency refers to rapid, e ff i c i e n t, accurateword recognition skills that permit a reader to con-struct the meaning of text. Fluency is also manifest-ed in accurate, rapid, expressive oral reading and isapplied during, and makes possible, silent readingcomprehension.

    Constructs of Reading Fluency

    While discussion of the construct of reading fluency is found as early as in the classic 1908 publi-cation by Edmund Huey (Chard, Va u g h n, and Ty l e r,2 0 0 2 ), most discussions of fluency trace its modernt h e o retical foundations to the 1974 seminal article byL e B e rge and Samuels. These re s e a rchers argued that,based on information-processing theory and re s e a rc h,human beings are single-channel pro c e s s o r s; that is,we can attend to only one thing at a time. We areable to do more than one thing at a time if we alter-nate our attention between two or more activities orif one of the activities is so well learned that it can beperformed automatically. They pointed out that re a d-ing re q u i res at least two activities1) word identifi-cation or decoding and 2) compre h e ns i o n or the con-s t ruction of the meaning of text. In order for re a d i n gto proceed efficiently and eff e c t i v e l y, the reader can-not focus attention on both of the processes. Thenon-fluent reader can, as do many beginning re a d e r swho have not yet developed automatic decodings k i l l s, alternate attention between the two pro c e s s e s .

    C o n s t ructing meaningwhich involves puttingw o rds into meaningful thought units, making infer-e n c e s, relating information being derived from thetext with background knowledge, and re s p o n d i n gcritically to the meaning that is constru c t e d a l w a y sre q u i res attention. For readers who must alternatebetween attending to the decoding of words and thec o n s t ruction of meaning, reading is a slow, l a b o r i o u s,i n e ff i c i e n t, i n e ff e c t i v e, and often punishing pro c e s s .If the limited attention and cognitive capacity isdrained by the processing of decoding word s, l i t t l eor no capacity is available for the attention-demand-ing process of constructing and responding to the meaning of a text. There f o re, automaticity ofdecoding fluency is essential for high levels of reading achievement.

    Keith Stanovich (1986) also contributed significantly to elevating the importance of re a d i n gfluency in a classic article in which he indicated are c i p rocal relationship between fluency and theamount of reading in which a reader engages.Readers who have achieved some fluency are morelikely to engage in more extensive amounts of re a d-ing than readers who lack fluency. The latter wouldfind reading difficult and laborious. However,Stanovich goes on to point out that as a result ofengaging in extensive amounts of re a d i n g, re a d e r sg row in all those skills that contribute to fluency andin fluency itself. Non-fluent readers who avoid re a d-ing fall further and further behind.

    Fluency has also been related to theoretical c o n s t ructs of how reading proceeds through develop-m e n t a l stages. Kuhn and Stahl (2000) summarizehow the development of fluency is related to thestages of development described by Chall (1996) andby Ehri (1995). Challs is a broad theoretical formula-tion that describes several stages of reading compre-hension development in addition to decoding; t h e re-f o re we will focus on Ehris theory, which focuses ondecoding through a stage of fluency development.

    2

  • E h r i s Stages of Reading Development as They Relate to Fluency

    In line with the theory of automaticity and thedefinition of fluency we have pro p o s e d, Ehri (1998)has noted, Being able to read words by sight auto-matically is the key to skilled reading of text. Thisallows readers to process words in text quickly,without attention directed to the word itself (p. 11 ) .Ehri has developed a carefully re s e a rc h e d, e l e g a n ttheory of how readers systematically pro g ress instages from being non-readers to the point wherethey can recognize words eff o r t l e s s l y.

    Readers at the Pre-alphabetic Stage ofD e v e l o p m e n t have no appreciation of the alphabeticprinciplethat in languages like English, t h e re is asystematic relationship between the limited numberof sounds of a language (approximately 40 in thecase of English) and the graphic forms, or letters, o fthe language. At this stage children attempt to trans-late the unfamiliar visual forms of print into familiaroral language through some visual clue that is partof the print. For example, c h i l d ren might re m e m b e rthe printed word m o n k e y by associating the descend-ing shape of the last letter of the word with a mon-k e ys tail. Obviously this is not a productive appro a c hand quickly leads to confusion since m y, p o n y, h o n e y,and m a n y other words would also be read as m o n k e ybased on the selected visual clue.

    At the Partial Alphabetic Stage of Development,readers have latched onto the notion that there is a relationship between the letters and sounds andbegin to use that insight. However, their ability todeal with the complexity of the sounds of word sresults in an incomplete use of that re l a t i o n s h i p .T h e re f o re, they tend to focus on the most salient,easiest parts of a word to deal with and, c o n s e q u e n t l y,use initial and, l a t e r, final letters as the clues to aprinted words pronunciation. For example, if re a d-ers at this stage of development are taught that theletter sequence g-o is the word g o, they may focusjust on the g and the sound it re p resents to identifythe word. However, using this strategy of focusingon the first letter, the letter sequences g i v e, g e t, g o n e,

    Partial Alphabetic Stage of Development

    While recognizing a relationship between let-ters and sounds, the reader may only focus onspecific easily identifiable parts of the word .

    Fully Alphabetic Stage of Development

    Recognizing that sounds correspond to letters,readers are able to blend sounds to arrive at a pronunciation. Eventually these words arem e m o r i zed as a unit and known by sight.

    Consolidated Alphabetic Stage of Development

    Repeated encounters with words allowthe reader to store letter patterns across different words.

    Ehris Four Stages of Reading Development

    Pre-Alphabetic Stage of Development

    The reader has no appreciation of the alphabetic principle and attempts to usevisual clues in the printed word to identifythe word.

    3

  • and g o r i l l a might also, i n c o r re c t l y, be identified as go.While children at this stage of development willmake errors in identifying word s, they are in a posi-tion to make pro g ress since they have developed theinsight that the letters of a printed w o rd are clues tothe sounds of the word .

    As children become more familiar with the formsof printed letters, a re able to analyze the sounds thatcompose word s, and become increasingly familiarwith the sounds that letters are likely to re p re s e n t,they move into the Fully Alphabetic Stage ofD e v e l o p m e n t. Now, even though they may neverhave seen it in print before, if they know the soundscommonly associated with the letters b-u-g, they canthink about the sounds for each of the letters andblend them together to arrive at the pronunciation ofthe word b u g. Ehris theory then indicates that as aresult of encountering the printed word b u g s e v e r a lt i m e s, as few as four times according to a widelycited study (Reitsma, 1 9 8 3 ), they come to accuratelyand instantly identify the word b u g without attend-ing to the individual letters, s o u n d s, or letter- s o u n dassociations. Ehri (1998) describes skilled reading inthe following way: Most of the words are known bysight. Sight reading is a fast acting process. The termsight indicates that sight of the word activates thatw o rd in memory including information about itss p e l l i n g, p ro n u n c i a t i o n, typical role in sentences, a n dm e a n i n g (p. 1112). This instant, a c c u r a t e, and auto-matic access to all these dimensions of a printedw o rd is the needed fluency that will allow readers tofocus their attention on comprehension rather thanon decoding. It is important to note that Ehris theo-ry and re s e a rch indicate that it is the careful pro c e s s-ing of print in the fully alphabetic stage that leads tothis rapid, instant recognition. Partial alphabeticreaders store incomplete re p resentations of word sa n d, t h e re f o re, confuse similar words such as w e re,w h e re, w i re, w o re, etc. However, once the word formis fully pro c e s s e d, with repeated encounters of thew o rd, it is recognized instantly.

    As readers gain skill in processing print, t h e ymove into the Consolidated Alphabetic Stage ofD e v e l o p m e n t and also develop another valuable,

    attention-saving decoding skill. In addition to stor-ing words as units, repeated encounters with word sallow a reader to store letter patterns across diff e re n tw o rds. Using Ehris example, the multiletter unit estwill be stored as a consolidated unit as a result ofreading the words n e s t, p e s t, re s t, t e s t, v e s t, and w e s t.Upon encountering the word c h e s t for the first time,a consolidated alphabetic reader would need to connect only two units: ch and est, rather than thefive units that the fully alphabetic reader wouldneed to combine. As noted, while this approach toreading a w o rd is faster than blending the individualp h o n e m e s, it is not as fast and efficient as sightrecognition of the word .

    B e f o re closing this discussion of Ehris theoryand re s e a rch it seems important to briefly indicatehow she addresses one other approach to decodingw o rdsthe use of context. Ehris theory is clearthebest way to recognize words is through instantrecognition that drains no attention, and there f o recontributes most to fluency. All other approaches o ruse of context re q u i re attention. Use of context hasa n o t h e r, m o re serious limitationit rarely leads tothe correct identification of the word. Ehri re v i e w sre s e a rch that indicates that the words in a text thatcarry the most meaning could be correctly identi-fied by context only about ten percent of the time.H o w e v e r, context and the other approaches todecoding words do play an important role in re a d-i n g, that of confirming the identification of word s .As she puts it: As each sight word is fixated, i t smeaning and pronunciation are triggered in memoryquickly and automatically. However, the other wordreading processes do not lie dormant; their contribu-tion is not to identify words in text, but to c o n f i r mthe identity already determined. Knowledge of thegraphophonic system confirms that the words pro-nunciation fits the spelling on the page. Knowledgeof syntax confirms that the word fits into the stru c-t u re of the sentence. Wo rd knowledge and text mem-ory confirms that the words meaning is consistentwith the texts meaning up to that point ( E h r i, 1 9 9 8,p. 11 ) .

    4

  • Foundations for Fluency

    In her discussion of how students build sightrecognition for words during their first few years ofre a d i n g, Ehri lists three pre requisite g r a p h o p h o n i ccapabilities: 1) letter familiarity; 2) phonemic aware-n e s s; and 3) knowledge of how graphemes typicallyre p resent phonemes in words. Ehri then notes thatfurther pro g ress depends on learning multiletterunits or spelling patterns. In addition, E h r is theoryand re s e a rch re q u i re that students are familiar withthe syntax or grammatical function of the word sthey are reading and with the meaning of thosew o rds. E h r i also shows that pro g ress in re a d i n gbeyond the beginning stages is dependent on orallanguage development.

    The importance of the three graphophonic factors listed above is fully documented in numero u sre s e a rch reports (e.g. A d a m s, 1 9 9 0; National ReadingP a n e l, 2 0 0 0; Snow et al., 1998). In order to move fro mthe pre-alphabetic stage to partial and fully alpha-betic stages, students need to grasp the alphabeticprinciple and to efficiently apply information aboutthe relationship between the letters and sounds ofEnglish (more commonly re f e r red to as phonics) torecognize words. This clearly re q u i res a high level of familiarity with letter forms and the ability to segment and blend the smallest units of spoken language (phonemes).

    P ro g ress in reading beyond the beginning stagesof reading depends upon the ability to re c o g n i z ew o rds instantly and to deal with spelling patterns ormultisyllabic units that can take the forms such asp re f i x e s, s u ff i x e s, s y l l a b l e s, and rimes. Recognition of these larger units comes from having read severalw o rds containing them, but also from learning tospell words.

    F i n a l l y, E h r is theory also points out the fact thatreading words is also dependent on familiarity withthem in their oral form. Recall the previously citedquote: The term sight indicates that sight of the wordactivates that word in memory including informationabout its spelling, p ro n u n c i a t i o n, typical role in sen-

    t e n c e s, and meaning (p. 1112). If the syntactic andmeaning aspects of the word are to be activated, t h e ymust be part of what the reader knows through orallanguage development. For the word re c o g n i t i o np rocess as proposed in Ehris theory to be complete,it must connect with meaning that has been devel-oped as another aspect of language development.

    BUIL D ING FLU EN CY IND E V ELO PING REA D ERS

    Our perception is that until very recently manyeducators took a rather simplistic approach to devel-oping fluency which is summed up in the deceptive-ly simple admonition: R e a d, re a d, and read somem o re . The expectation was that if students re a dm o re, they would achieve fluency. However, E h r isre s e a rch and theories suggest that at least some stu-dents will need expert teacher guidance in order top ro g ress efficiently through stages of reading devel-opment to fluency. Students who lack the necessaryfoundations for developing decoding skills are in noposition to re a d, re a d, and read some more. Studentswho engage in re a d i n g, but who employ the guess-ing strategies of the Partial Alphabetic re a d e r, a re not likely to make optimal pro g ress in re a d i n g .F o r t u n a t e l y, several re s e a rch studies have focused on the details of instruction that seem most pro m i s-ing for improving reading fluency. These instru c t i o n a lpractices include: modeled re a d i n g, repeated re a d i n gof familiar text, wide independent re a d i n g, c o a c h e dreading of appropriately selected materials, c h u n k-ing of text, and word reading practice.

    I m p rove reading fluency thro u g h

    Modeled re a d i n g

    Repeated re a d i n gof familiar text

    Wide independent re a d i n g

    Coached reading of a p p ropriately selectedm a t e r i a l s

    Chunking of text

    Wo rd reading pra c t i c e .

    5

  • Modeled Re a d i n g

    One way to enhance fluency is for teachers toread aloud to students (Dowhower, 1 9 8 7; H o ff m a n,1 9 8 7; S m i t h, 1979). The process of reading aloud tostudents needs to be supplemented with pro c e d u re swhich actually engage students in interaction witht e x t, but reading aloud does provide them with amodel of how to pace reading in connected text andhow to infuse expression (attend to dialogue marksand punctuation). Taped or computer modeled re a d-ing is also a viable way to provide fluency support.H o w e v e r, for younger and less able readers taped orcomputer modeled reading seems more eff e c t i v ethan no model, but not as effective as a teachermodel (Daly and Martens, 1994). For lower perform-ing re a d e r s, an additional benefit of having text re a dinitially by a model improved comprehension. Itseems that the reading model allowed students tofocus on the content of the passage initially beforethey read it independently (Monda, 1989). While itvaries from study to study whether students fol-lowed along in copies of the texts, we re c o m m e n dthis as a way to engage children in the text prior totheir reading it independently.

    Repeated Reading of Familiar Te x t

    R e reading text or repeated oral reading is p e rhaps the most frequently documented appro a c hto improving fluency (National Reading Panel, 2 0 0 0;Rashotte and To rg e s e n, 1985) and has been associat-ed with improved outcomes for young students( OS h e a, S i n d e l a r, and OS h e a, 1987) as well as col-lege students (Carver and Hoff m a n, 1981). Generally,intervention re s e a rch on fluency development hasbeen dominated by re s e a rch on repeated re a d i n g .This likely reflects the application of the theory thatfluent reading is promoted by frequent opportunitiesto practice in familiar text and to increased exposureto words.

    Wide Independent Re a d i n g

    R e s e a rch does not yet clearly indicate whetherrepeated reading is superior to wide, sustained re a d-ing of diff e rent texts. Curre n t l y, it seems that for moreable re a d e r s, repeated reading of the same texts isnot as necessary as it is for struggling readers andthat increasing the amount of reading that is done iss u ff i c i e n t l y, and perhaps more, beneficial (Homan,K l e s i u s, and Hite, 1 9 9 3; Mathes and Fuchs, 1 9 9 3;Rashotte and To rg e s e n, 1985).

    The beneficial effects of wide reading weresomewhat called into question by the fairly re c e n tReport of the National Reading Panel (2000) which concluded: Based on the existing evidence, the NRPcan only indicate that while encouraging students to read might be beneficial, re s e a rch has not yetdemonstrated this in a clear and convincing manner(p. 3). It is important to keep in mind that the NRPused very restrictive criteria for re s e a rc h a n d, a l s o,that it clearly held out the possibility of beneficiale ffects for wide reading.

    P revious highly respected re s e a rch syntheseshave been far less restrained about the salutarye ffects of wide reading. For example, Becoming aNation of Readers (Anderson et al, 1985) concludes:R e s e a rch suggests that the amount of independent,silent reading that children do in school is significant-ly related to gains in reading achievement (p. 76).This same re s e a rch review concludes: R e s e a rch alsoshows that the amount of reading students do out of school is consistently related to gains in re a d i n ga c h i e v e m e n t (p. 77). In her critical review of begin-ning reading re s e a rch Adams (1990) concluded: If we want children to read well, we must find a way toinduce them to read lots (p. 5). Adams also con-cludes: C h i l d ren should be given as much opportuni-ty and encouragement as possible to practice theirreading. Beyond the basics, c h i l d re ns reading facility,as well as their vocabulary and conceptual gro w t h,depends strongly on the amount of text they re a d ( p .1 2 7 ) .

    Keith Stanovich and his colleagues(Cunningham and Stanovich, 1 9 9 8; Nathan andS t a n o v i c h, 1 9 9 1; S t a n o v i c h, 1 9 8 6; Stanovich and

    6

  • C u n n i n g h a m, 1 9 9 2; S t a n o v i c h, C u n n i n g h a m, a n dF re e m a n, 1 9 8 4; Stanovich and We s t, 1989) have pre-sented impressive re s e a rch results and theore t i c a la rgument for the value of wide reading. The evidenceand rationale that they pre s e n t, h o w e v e r, is that thepositive relationship between reading achievementand wide reading may not be affected exclusivelyt h rough the development of fluency, but through the development of language and cognitive abilitiesas well.

    While the experimental evidence may not be asclear as it should be, t h e re does appear, at least forachieving re a d e r s, s t rong evidence and support forthe conclusion of Nathan and Stanovich (1991) that:If children are to become fluent re a d e r s, they needto read a lot. Our job as educators is to see to it thatc h i l d ren want to readthat they seek new knowl-edge via the written word and derive satisfactionand joy from the reading pro c e s s ( p . 1 7 9 ) .

    M o re o v e r, if students are making adequatep ro g ress with fluency, wide reading rather thanrepeated reading may lead to greater impro v e m e n t sin vocabulary and comprehension. However, for lessable readers experiencing particular difficulties withf l u e n c y, repeated reading remains an importantaspect of an instructional pro g r a m .

    Coached or Assisted Reading

    Most re s e a rchers agree that accuracy alone i si n s u fficient and that students need to read rapidly if they are going to understand the connections thatneed to be made between ideas in print (Nathan andS t a n o v i c h, 1991). C o n t rolling the difficulty of textsand providing feedback for words missed duringreading seem to be associated with improved rateand accuracy for those students developing fluentreading. Advancing students through pro g re s s i v e l yd i fficult text based on their performance seems toenhance their overall fluency as does correction andfeedback for words read incorre c t l y.

    P roviding students with opportunities to re a dwidely and targeting specific elements of fluencyb u i l d i n g, such as pro g ressively difficult text withc o r rective feedback, appear to contribute to

    i m p roved fluency (Kuhn and Stahl, 2000). Heibertand Fisher (2002) studied fluency development as itrelates to the features of the texts used for pro m o t i n gf l u e n c y. Specifically, they were interested in examin-ing the effects of texts in which particular text dimen -sions or features were carefully controlled. The tre a t-ment texts Heibert and Fisher designed were charac-terized as having the following key features: a smallnumber of unique word s, a high percentage of mostf requently used word s, and often repeated criticalw o rd s (those words that influence the meaning ofthe text most). Students in the comparison group re a df rom texts typically associated with commercial re a d-ing programs. Using a repeated reading (three times)i n s t ructional routine in a nine-week intervention, s t u-dents reading in the treatment texts made significantgains in fluency over their peers in the comparisoncondition. There also seemed to be an effect for com-p rehension for second language learners. These find-ings suggest that the features of the texts being usedto promote fluency should be carefully considere d .

    Chunking Te x t s

    Another approach to fluency building is to p rovide struggling readers with text in which mean-ingful groups or words or phrases are signaled forthe reader as a means of improving fluency andc o m p rehension (Cro m e r, 1 9 7 0; Young and Bowers,1995). Research reveals that diff e rent amounts of textp resented in repeated reading do not seem to changethe outcome. However, c o n t rol of the amount of textp resented may be beneficial for students who areexperiencing difficulty with reading accuracy as itmay force them to focus on the words for a longerperiod of time (Cohen, 1988).

    Carbo (1981) used a phrased or chunkeda p p roach to assisted repeated reading. She had stu-dents listen to tapes and follow along in books inwhich the text was chunked into short phrases.Carbo reported significant gains in word re c o g n i t i o nability suggesting that this approach might be help-ful for improving accuracy.

    Several re s e a rchers have studied the effects ofparsing or chunking texts into phrase units. While

    7

  • most of these studies have been with older students,Kuhn and Stahl (2000) reported that reading phraseunits rather than conventional text does seem toresult in improved fluency.

    Wo rd Reading Pra c t i c e

    Based on Ehris stage model of reading and p reviously off e red theoretical descriptions of fluency,the importance of individual word reading auto-maticity would seem to have practical implicationsfor fluency building. Studies in which teachers hadstudents practice reading lists of words that theyw e re to later encounter in connected texts consistentlyresulted in increased fluency (Fleisher, J e n k i n s, a n dP a n y, 1 9 7 9 - 8 0; L e v y, A b e l l o, and Ly s y n c h u k, 1997). It is important to note, h o w e v e r, that there was noconcomitant increase in comprehension.

    T HE ASSESSMEN TOF FLU EN CY

    As noted at the beginning of this paper, f l u e n c yhas been re f e r red to as the neglected aspect o freading. The assessment of fluency, in particular,appears to have received very limited attention.T h e re are very few re s e a rch studies that have investi-gated how fluency should be assessed or the criteriathat should be applied to determine whether or not areader has achieved fluency. For example, the majorreview of fluency done by Kuhn and Stahl (2000),while devoting considerable discussion to the con-s t ruct of fluency and to approaches to developing it,does not directly discuss the topic of the assessmentof fluency. Perhaps it is this dearth of data that ledthe National Reading Panel (2000) to conclude: Anumber of informal pro c e d u res can be used in thec l a s s room to assess fluency: informal reading inven-tories (Johnson, K re s s, and Pikulski, 1 9 8 7 ), m i s c u eanalysis (Goodman and Burke, 1 9 7 2 ), pausing indices(Pinnell et al. 1995), and reading speed calculations( H a s b rouck and Ti n d a l l, 1992). All these assessmentp ro c e d u res re q u i re oral reading of text, and all can

    p rovide an adequate index of fluency (p. 39). Thisseems almost surprising in light of the Panels insis-tence on experimental evidence in order to endorsethe efficacy of instructional pro c e d u res. Few experi-mental studies have been conducted using theseinformal pro c e d u res. For example, decades agoPikulski (1974) raised questions about evidence toestablish both the reliability and validity of informalreading inventories; the qualitative scale discussed inPinnell et. al., (1995) is based on a single corre l a t i o n a ls t u d y. While the norms reported by Hasbrouck andTindal (1992) are based on a large population of stu-d e n t s, that population is not clearly described. Therigor of the study also seems compromised by thefact that a variety of diverse texts were used to col-lect the data for the oral reading fluency norms.

    P e rhaps it was recognition that there is a verypractical need for classroom assessment that led thePanel to endorse pro c e d u res that may not have thes t rong re s e a rch they more typically re q u i red in otherparts of the re p o r t .

    Near the end of its discussion of the assessmentof fluency the National Reading Panel re f e re n c e dtwo standardized measures of fluency: The GrayOral Reading Test (We i n e rholt and Bryant, 1992) anda standardized measure of the speed of reading of sin-gle words. Both of the recommended standardi z e dm e a s u res seem to have limitations. The Gray OralReading Test is based on norms that are over adecade old. Using the reading of single words as afull measure of fluency seems to violate the guide-lines off e red by the Panel in the portion of theirreport in which they noted: For example, i n f o r m a lreading inventories (IRI) re q u i re students to re a dgrade-level passages aloud and silently. The teacherdetermines a reading level by calculating the prop o r-tion of words read accurately in the passage. Toe n s u re that students do not focus solely on fluencyat the expense of compre h e n s i o n the student isexpected to summarize or answer questions aboutthe text (p. 39). We read this statement to meanthat it is important to assess a childs fluency withinthe context of reading comprehension. The oral re a d-ing of single words certainly does not include am e a s u re of comprehension.

    8

  • An exception to the general lack of attentiona ff o rded fluency assessment is the work of Deno andhis colleagues. Deno (1985) introduced the notion ofassessing oral reading fluency to the field of specialeducation as a gauge of student pro g ress in re a d i n g .Oral reading fluency is measured by timing a childsreading in connected text for one minute whilere c o rding errors that the child demonstrates duringreading. Subsequent re s e a rch on this pro c e d u re hasestablished it as a robust way for teachers to trackstudent reading gro w t h, evaluate instructional pro-gram eff e c t i v e n e s s, and predict performance onn o r m - re f e renced standardized achievement tests.While critics have argued that this type of briefm e a s u re of student reading fluency does not re f l e c tthe complex processes that re p resent proficient re a d-i n g, oral reading fluency has been thoroughly testedfor concurrent validity with other well establishedreading measures as well as for sensitivity to studentg rowth across short and long periods of time (Deno,2002). In addition to its technical adequacy, n o r m a-tive data on general outcome measures of oral re a d-ing fluency have been reported (Hasbrouck andTi n d a l, 1992) and approximate levels of performancefor typical students at each grade level have beendocumented (Fuchs, F u c h s, H a m l e t t, Wa l z, a n dG e r m a n n, 1993).

    Based on the limited re s e a rch on the assessmentof fluency, and the construct and definition of fluenc yadopted in this paper, t h e re seem to be several essen-tial dimensions for the assessment of fluency,including measures of: 1) oral reading accuracy; 2) oral reading rate; 3) quality of oral re a d i n g; a n d4) reading comprehension.

    While all four of these dimensions can be evaluated informally as pointed out by the NationalReading Panel, it would seem prudent to develop a fluency measure that addresses at least some traditional reliability and validity criteria. One c o m p rehensive instrument that attempts to addre s sall the essential dimensions of fluency and which has been subjected to extensive field-test trials is the Leveled Reading Passages (L R P) Assessment Kit(Houghton Miff l i n, 2001). This instrument pro v i d e sthe materials and descriptions of pro c e d u res thatallows for the assessment of a full construct of fluencyfor students who are at the very beginning stages ofreading through sixth grade. The L R P was field test-ed in a study of 1200 students across the UnitedStates. The field tests validated the decodability andthe level of difficulty of the reading passages andw o rd lists that are part of the instrument. Field-testdata were also used to establish benchmarks ofb e l o w - l e v e l, o n - l e v e l, and above-level performancefor oral reading accuracy, oral reading rate, q u a l i t yof oral re a d i n g, and reading comprehension. Thus,the L R P a d d resses all the essential dimensions of f l u e n c y, capitalizes on the established strengths ofinformal assessment, but then uses actual field-testdata to address the validity of the instru m e n t .

    CO N C LUSI O NSWhile the construct of fluency may have been

    neglected in the past, it is receiving much deservedattention pre s e n t l y. There is a very strong re s e a rc hand theoretical base that indicates that while fluencyin and of itself is not sufficient to insure high levelsof reading achievement and compre h e n s i o n, f l u e n c yis absolutely necessary for that achievement and forc o m p rehension. While fluency is most obviouslyreflected in oral re a d i n g, it more importantly oper-ates in silent reading as well. If a reader has notdeveloped fluency, the process of decoding word sdrains attention, and insuff icient attention is avail-able for constructing the meaning of texts. Fluencybuilds on a foundation of oral language skills,phonemic aware n e s s, familiarity with letter forms,and efficient decoding skills. Ehris description of the

    Essential dimensions for theassessment of fluency includem e a s u res of

    1. Oral reading accura c y

    2. Oral reading ra t e

    3. Quality of oral re a d i n g

    4. Reading comprehension.

    9

  • stages of word recognition explains how re a d e r scome to recognize words by sight through care f u l l yp rocessing print.

    Substantial re s e a rch has also been conducted o nhow to best develop fluency for students who do not yet have it. While there is a dearth of experimen-tal re s e a rch studies on developing fluency thro u g hi n c reasing the amount of independent reading inwhich students engage, t h e re is substantial corre l a-tional evidence showing a clear relationship betweenthe amount students re a d, their reading fluency, a n dreading comprehension. However, students who a re non-achieving in reading are not in a position toengage in wide re a d i n g, and they may need moreguidance and support in order to develop fluency.R e s e a rch shows that the repeated reading of familiart e x t s, coached or assisted re a d i n g, and the chunkingof texts are all effective techniques for helping stru g-gling readers to improve their fluency.

    Little re s e a rch is available to guide the assess-ment of fluency. While more re s e a rch is needed onissues of adequate rates of fluency at various gradelevels and for judging the quality of oral re a d i n g,t h e re is good agreement that the compre h e n s i v eassessment of fluency must include measures of oralreading accuracy, rate of oral re a d i n g, and quality oforal reading. T h e re is also good agreement that thesedimensions of fluency must be assessed within thecontext of reading comprehension. Fluency without accompanying high levels of reading compre h e n s i o nis simply not adequate.

    BIBLI OGR A P H YA d a m s, M.J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and

    learning about print. C a m b r i d g e, MA: MIT Pre s s .Anderson R.C., E.H. Heibert, J.A. Scott, and I.A.

    Wilkerson (1985). Becoming a nation of readers: The report of the commission on re a d i n g . Wa s h i n g t o n, D.C.: The NationalInstitute on Education.

    C a r b o, M. (1981). Making books talk to childre n .The Reading Te a c h e r, 3 5, 1 8 6 - 1 8 9 .

    C a r v e r, R. P. and J.V. Hoffman (1981). The effect ofpractice through repeated reading on gain in reading abilityusing a computer-based instructional system. R e a d i n gR e s e a rch Quarterly, 16 (3), 3 7 4 3 9 0 .

    C h a l l, J.S. (1996). Stages of reading development, 2nd ed.Ft. Wo r t h, TX: Harc o u r t - B r a c e .

    C h a rd, D . J ., S. Va u g h n, and B.J. Tyler (2002). A synthesis of re s e a rch on effective interventions for building fluency with elementary students with learningd i s a b i l i t i e s . Journal of Learning Disabilities, 3 5, 3 8 6 4 0 6 .

    C o h e n, A.L. (1988). An evaluation of the eff e c t i v e n e s sof two methods for providing computer-assisted re p e a t e dreading training to reading disabled students. Doctoral d i s s e r t a t i o n, Florida State University, Ta l l a h a s s e e .

    C ro m e r, W. (1970). The diff e rence model: A new explanation for some reading diff i c u l t i e s . Journal ofEducational Psychology, 6 1, 4 7 1 4 8 3 .

    C u n n i n g h a m, A.E. and K.E. Stanovich (1998). What reading does for the mind. American Educator,S p r i n g / S u m m e r, 8 1 5 .

    D a l e y, E.J. and B.K. Martens (1994). A comparison o ft h ree interventions for increasing oral reading p e r f o r ma n c e :Application of the instructional hierarc h y. Journal of AppliedBehavior Analysis, 2 7, 4 5 9 4 6 9 .

    D e n o, S.L. (1985). Curriculum-based measure m e n t :The emerging alternative. Exceptional Childre n, 5 2, 2 1 9 2 3 2 .

    D e n o, S . L ., C.A. Espin, and L.S. Fuchs (2002).Evaluation strategies for preventing and remediating basicskill deficits. In Interventions for academic and behavior pro b-lems II: Preventive and remedial appro a c h e s, M.R. Shinn, H . M .Wa l k e r, and G. Stoner (Eds.), 213241. Bethesda, M D :National Association of School Psychologists.

    D o w h o w e r, S.L. (1987). E ffects of repeated reading on second-grade transitional re a d e r s fluency and compre-h e n s i o n . Reading Research Quarterly, 2 2, 3 8 9 4 0 6 .

    D o w h o w e r, S.L. (1991). Speaking of prosody: Fluencysunattended bedfellow. Theory Into Practice, 30 (3), 1 6 5 1 7 5 .

    10

  • E h r i, L.C. (1998). Grapheme-phoneme knowledge isessential for learning to read words in English. In Wo r drecognition in beginning literacy, J.L. Metsala and L.C. Ehri(Eds.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.

    E h r i, L.C. (1995). Stages of development in learning toread words by sight. Journal of Research in Reading, 1 8,11 6 1 2 5 .

    F l e i s h e r, L . S ., J.R. Jenkins, and D. Pany (19791980).E ffects on poor re a d e r s c o m p rehension of training in rapidd e c o d i n g . Reading Research Quarterly, 1 5, 3 0 4 8 .

    F u c h s, L . S ., D. Fuchs, C.L. Hamlett, L. Wa l z, and G.Germann (1993). Formative evaluation of academicp ro g ress: How much growth can we expect? S c h o o lPsychology Review, 2 2, 2 7 4 8 .

    G o o d m a n, Y.M. and C.L. Burke (1972). Reading miscuei n v e n t o r y. New York: Macmillan.

    H a r r i s, T.L. and R.E. Hodges (1995). The literacy diction-ary: A vocabulary of reading and writing. Newark, D E :International Reading A s s o c i a t i o n .

    H a s b ro u c k, J.E. and G. Tindal (1992). C u r r i c u l u m -based fluency norms for grades two through five. Te a c h i n gExceptional Childre n, 2 4, 4144.

    H i e b e r t, E.H. and C.W. Fisher (2002). Text matters indeveloping fluent re a d i n g . Submitted for publication.

    H o ff m a n, J . V. (1987). Rethinking the role of oral re a d i n g . Elementary School Journal, 8 7, 3 6 7 3 7 3 .

    H o m a n, S ., P. Klesius, and S. Hite (1993). E ffects ofrepeated readings and nonrepetitive strategies on studentsfluency and compre h e n s i o n . Journal of Educational Researc h,8 7, 9 4 9 9 .

    Houghton Mifflin (2001). Leveled Reading Passages.Boston: Houghton Mifflin Co.

    J o h n s o n, M . S ., R.A. Kre s s, and J.J. Pikulski (1987).Informal reading inventories. Newark, DE: InternationalReading A s s o c i a t i o n .

    K u h n, M.R. and S.A. Stahl (2000). Fluency: A review ofdevelopmental and remedial practices. Ann A r b o r, MI: Centerfor the Improvement of Early Reading A c h i e v e m e n t .

    L a B e rge D. and S.J. Samuels (1974). To w a rds a theoryof automatic information processing in re a d i n g . C o g n i t i v eP s y c h o l o g y, 6, 2 9 3 3 2 3 .

    L e v y, B . A ., B. A b e l l o, and L. Lysynchuk (1997). Tr a n s f e rf rom word training to reading in context: Gains in fluencyand compre h e n s i o n . Learning Disability Quarterly, 2 0,1 7 3 1 8 8 .

    M a t h e s, P.G. and L.S. Fuchs (1993). P e e r- m e d i a t e dreading instruction in special education re s o u rce ro o m s .Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 8, 2 3 3 2 4 3 .

    M o n d a, L.E. (1989). The effects of oral, s i l e n t, and listening repetitive reading on the fluency and compre h e n-sion of learning disabled s t u d e n t s . Doctoral dissertation,Florida State University, Ta l l a h a s s e e .

    N a t h a n, R.G. and K.E. Stanovich (1991). The causesand consequences of diff e rences in reading fluency.Theory Into Practice, 30 (3), 1 7 6 1 8 4 .

    National Reading Panel (2000). Teaching children to re a d :An evidence-based assessment of the scientific-re s e a rch literatureon reading and its implications for reading instruction.Wa s h i n g t o n, D.C.: National Institute of Child Health andHuman Development.

    OS h e a, L . J ., P. T. Sindelar, and D.J. OShea (1987). The effects of repeated readings and attentional cues on the reading fluency and comprehension of learning disabledre a d e r s . Learning Disabilities Researc h, 2, 1 0 3 1 0 9 .

    P i k u l s k i, J.J. (1974). Informal reading inventories: A critical re v i e w. The Reading Te a c h e r, 28 (2), 2 5 3 2 5 8 .

    P i n n e l l, G . S ., J.J. Pikulski, K.K. Wi x s o n, J.R. Campbell,P.B. Gough, and A.S. Beatty (1995). Listening to children re a da l o u d. Wa s h i n g t o n, D.C.: Office of Educational Research andI m p ro v e m e n t, U.S. Department of Education.

    R a s h o t t e, C.A. and J.K. To rgeson (1985). R e p e a t e dreading and reading fluency in learning disabled childre n .Reading Research Quarterly, 2 0, 1 8 0 1 8 8 .

    R e i t s m a, P. (1983). P r i n t e d - w o rd learning in beginningre a d e r s . Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 7 5, 321339.

    R o s e, T.L. and J.R. Beattie (1986). Relative e ffects oft e a c h e r- d i rected and taped previewing on oral re a d i n g .Learning Disabilities Quarterly, 9, 1 9 3 1 9 9 .

    S a m u e l s, S.J. (2002). Reading fluency: Its developmentand assessment. In What re s e a rch has to say about re a d i n gi n s t r u c t i o n, 3 rd. ed., A.E. Farstrup and S.J. Samuels (Eds.).N e w a r k, DE: International Reading Association.

    S m i t h, D.D. (1979). The improvement of childre ns oralreading through the use of teacher modeling. Journal ofLearning Disabilities, 1 2, 1 7 2 1 7 5 .

    S n o w, C . E ., M.S. Burns, and P. Griffin (1998). P re v e n t i n greading difficulties in young childre n. Wa s h i n g t o n, D . C . :National Academy Pre s s .

    S t a n o v i c h, K.E. and R.F. West (1989). E x p o s u re to printand orthographic pro c e s s i n g . Reading Research Quarterly, 2 4,4 0 2 4 3 3 .

    S t a n o v i c h, K.E. (1986). Matthew effects in re a d i n g :Some consequences in individual diff e rences in the acquisi-tion of literacy. Reading Research Quarterly, 2 1, 3 6 0 4 0 7 .

    We i n e rh o l t, J.L. and B.R. Bryant (1992). Gray oral re a d i n gt e s t s, 3 rd ed. A u s t i n, TX: Pro - E d .

    Yo u n g, A. and P.G. Bowers (1995). Individual diff e r-ences and text difficulty determinants of reading fluency ande x p re s s i v en e s s . Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 6 0,4 2 8 4 5 4 .

    11

  • DR. JOHN J. PIKUL SKID r. John J. Pikulski is Professor ofEducation at the University ofD e l a w a re, w h e re he has been Dire c t o rof the Reading Center, D e p a r t m e n tC h a i r p e r s o n and President of the

    University Faculty Senate. His current re s e a rch inter-ests focus on strategies for preventing reading pro b-lems and the teaching and developing of vocabulary.An active member in the International ReadingA s s o c i a t i o n, D r. Pikulski has served on its Board ofD i re c t o r s, c h a i red various committees, and was pre s-ident of the association in 199798. He is coauthor ofThe Diagnosis, C o r re c t i o n, and Prevention of ReadingD i s a b i l i t i e s, a n d Informal Reading Inventories. Dr.Pikulski is also a senior author of Houghton Miff l i nR e a d i n g and is a coordinating author on R e a d i n gIntervention for EARLY SUCCESS.

    DAV ID J. CHARDD r. David Chard is Assistant Pro f e s s o rand Director of Graduate Studies atthe University of Oregon. His workfocuses on early literacy and mathe-matics instruction for at-risk students

    with mild disabilities. Dr. Chard has published widely on topics related to students with learningdisabilities and is on the authorship team ofHoughton Mifflin Reading.

    Copyright 2003 by Houghton Mifflin Company. All rights reserved..

    12