pdf (2.0mb)

18
319 612.534:612.82 THE CENTRAL AND THE REFLEX MECHANISM OF PANTING. BY M. HAMMOUDA. (From the Physiological Laboratory, University of Cairo.) RICHET [1898], when describing the effect of exposure to heat on respiration in dogs, pointed out the striking difference between the behaviour of anwsthetized and non-ansesthetized animals. Non-anesthe- tized dogs pant without a rise of body temperature, by simply being exposed to the sun for 2-10 min. To induce panting after administration of an anaesthetic (Richet used chloralose), however, the body tem- perature of the animal must be raised appreciably above normal. Richet assumes that in the first case the panting is based on a reflex, initiated by warming the terminations of the cutaneous nerves and of the fifth cranial nerve, while in the second panting is the direct result of raising the temperature of the brain centres. A previous paper, published jointly with Prof. G. V. Anrep [Anrep and Hammouda, 1932], dealt with the respiratory changes which take place in the animal during panting. In the present counication it is proposed to study the nervous mechanism underlying the phenomenon of panting. Richet thought that this peculiar form of respiratory activity was governed by the medulla. But subsequent researches on the Sherrington decerebrate preparation show that the medulla cannot be responsible for panting, since, in decerebrate dogs and cats, even at very high body temperatures, the highest respiratory rate does not exceed 60-70 per min.; decerebrate animals do not pant. Nikolaides and Dontas [1911] found that heat polypncea cannot occur if the medulla is separated from the. brain. They describe a heat polypnoea centre in the corpus striatum. Bazett and Penfield [1922] found in acute decerebrate cats and dogs a. maximum rate of 60 at a temperature of 42.80 C., and Sherrington. [1924] found in acute decerebrate cats and dogs a maximum rate of 75 with a rectal temperature of 41.10 0. On comparing the maximum rate of 60-75 per min. obtained in these decerebrate preparations with the PH. LXXVII. 21

Upload: vudang

Post on 02-Jan-2017

214 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: PDF (2.0MB)

319

612.534:612.82

THE CENTRAL AND THE REFLEX MECHANISMOF PANTING.

BY M. HAMMOUDA.

(From the Physiological Laboratory, University of Cairo.)

RICHET [1898], when describing the effect of exposure to heat onrespiration in dogs, pointed out the striking difference between thebehaviour of anwsthetized and non-ansesthetized animals. Non-anesthe-tized dogs pant without a rise of body temperature, by simply beingexposed to the sun for 2-10 min. To induce panting after administrationof an anaesthetic (Richet used chloralose), however, the body tem-perature of the animal must be raised appreciably above normal. Richetassumes that in the first case the panting is based on a reflex, initiatedby warming the terminations of the cutaneous nerves and of the fifthcranial nerve, while in the second panting is the direct result of raisingthe temperature of the brain centres.

A previous paper, published jointly with Prof. G. V. Anrep [Anrepand Hammouda, 1932], dealt with the respiratory changes which takeplace in the animal during panting. In the present counication it isproposed to study the nervous mechanism underlying the phenomenon ofpanting.

Richet thought that this peculiar form of respiratory activity wasgoverned by the medulla. But subsequent researches on the Sherringtondecerebrate preparation show that the medulla cannot be responsible forpanting, since, in decerebrate dogs and cats, even at very high bodytemperatures, the highest respiratory rate does not exceed 60-70 per min.;decerebrate animals do not pant. Nikolaides and Dontas [1911] foundthat heat polypncea cannot occur if the medulla is separated from the.brain. They describe a heat polypnoea centre in the corpus striatum.Bazett and Penfield [1922] found in acute decerebrate cats and dogs a.maximum rate of 60 at a temperature of 42.80 C., and Sherrington.[1924] found in acute decerebrate cats and dogs a maximum rate of 75with a rectal temperature of 41.10 0. On comparing the maximum rateof 60-75 per min. obtained in these decerebrate preparations with the

PH. LXXVII. 21

Page 2: PDF (2.0MB)

M. HAMMOUDA.

maximum of 300-400 observed in anaesthetized panting animals, one isled to the conclusion that the parts of the central nervous system re-sponsible for the very high rates of respiration lie above the mid-brain.

THE "PANTING CENTRE.

The following experiments were carried out in order to try to localizemore precisely the part of the central nervous system which is responsiblefor panting. It was hoped that by removal of various parts of thecerebral hemispheres it would be possible to locate, at least with someprobability, that part of the brain which, when destroyed, interferespermanently with panting. Two different procedures were adopted. Inthe first, the section of the brain was performed beforehand (i.e. beforepanting); the dog was left to recover from the aneesthetic and from theimmediate effect of the operation, and then warmed up. In the second,the brain was freely exposed on one or both sides, depending on thesection to be performed; the animal was then warmed up until pantingbecame definitely established, and the section of the brain was madeduring the progress of panting. When the first procedure was adopted,the dogs were operated upon under chloroform and ether anaesthesia;in the second case they were given chloralose intravenously (0 05-0 075 g.per kg.). All these animals were supplied with a tracheal cannula andhad both carotid arteries tied. They were warmed up either in a hot bathat 40-42' C., or in an air thermostat by a method described in a previouscommunication [Anrep and Hammouda, 1932]. The respiratory ratewas recorded by a stethograph. After the end of an experiment, the brainwas hardened in situ with formalin; it was then removed from the skulland carefully examined to determine the extent of the injury'. Theoperations on the brain were all bilateral. The experiments with thesection of the brain at different levels can be classified under the followingfour headings:

(1) Removal of the cortex with preservation of the basal ganglia.(2) Removal of the two hemispheres leaving the mid-brain intact.(3) Removalof the cortex and of the corpora striata without damaging

the optic thalami.(4) Local injuries to the optic thalami.Fig. 1 shows the levels at which these sections were made.In the first group, the animals were decorticated on both sides by

scooping out the brain substance, care being taken not to injure the basal1 I am greatly indebted to Dr B. Boulgakow, of the Anatomy Department, for the

help he gave me by making the post-mortem examinations of all the brains.

320

Page 3: PDF (2.0MB)

CENTRAL AND REFLEX MECHANISM OF PANTING. 321

ganglia; the cavity of the skull was then lightly packed with cotton-woolsoaked in warm saline. The post-mortem examinations of these brainsshowed that the basal ganglia were completely intact, and that practicallythe whole cortex had been removed, i.e. only occasionally had smallscraps of the cortex been left. All these animals reacted to the rise ofbody temperature by panting as vigorously as normal animals. This wasobserved in dogs in which the cortex was removed beforehand, as wellas in dogs which were decorticated during the progress of panting. Inthe latter case, the panting respiration was slightly disturbed during the

C.C.

Th. P. M.Fig. 1. Sagittal section of dog's brain, demonstrating the operations (1, 2, 3, 4) performed.

C.C. corpus callosum; Th. optic thalamus; N.C. nucleus caudatus; A.C. anterior colli-culus; P.C. posterior colliculus; P. pons; M. medulla. Further description is given inthe text.

actual surgical interference, but afterwards it continued without inter-ruption. Panting as a response to raising the body temperature is thusshown to be independent of the cortex.

In the second group of dogs, the section was made at the level of theanterior colliculi. The post-mortem examinations showed that the brainstem was completely severed in all cases; no parts of the optic thalamihad been left behind. In a few experiments it was also found that theanterior colliculi had been destroyed, but the posterior colliculi remaineduninjured in every experiment. During the operation on non-pantingdogs, the respiration stopped for a moment and then resumed its originalrhythm. On warming up these animals, the respiration accelerated onlyto a maximum of 40-60 per mm. with a rise of rectal temperature to

21-2

Page 4: PDF (2.0MB)

M. HAMMOUDA.

42.00 C. The higher respiratory rates were observed in those dogs inwhich the plane of section was more anterior, thus causing less dis-turbance to the mid-brain. When the decerebration was made in apanting animal, the respiration stopped during the section and thenresumed at a slow rate, which gradually rose to a maximum of 40-60 permi. In two of these experiments, the temperature was raised until thedogs succumbed to heat; panting, however, never took place. Beforedeath, the respiration became slow and was occasionally interrupted bygasps. In the other experiments, the animals were allowed to cool. Itwas noticed that in these cases the temperature dropped considerablymore rapidly than in normal animals; it fell in a comparatively shorttime to 33-31° C. The results obtained on decerebrate animals confirmthe observations of Bazett and Penfield [1922] and of Sherring-ton [1924] that the maximum rate obtained in such preparations doesnot exceed 75 per min., a rate which is far below even the slowest rateobtained in panting. These experiments also make it clear that the partof the central nervous system which is responsible for panting lies some-where between the cortex and the mid-brain, i.e. in the basal ganglia.

In order to find out which of these ganglia is concerned, experimentswere made in which the corpus striatum was destroyed with the leastpossible damage to the optic thalamus. A slightly curved spatula, 1 3 cm.in breadth, was employed for this purpose. After freely opening the skullon both sides, having previously measured the exact distances by com-.parison with a preserved brain of the same size, the spatulawas introducedthrough the occipital lobe. It was held so that its lateral edge wassomewhat lower than the other edge. The spatula was pushed into thebrain substance in this slanting position until the anterior limits of thethalami were passed, and then it was pressed downwards to the base of theskull. The operation was repeated on the other side of the brain. As aresult of this operation it was hoped to destroy the greater part of thecorpora striata without injuring the optic thalami. Three experimentswere successfully performed. The post-mortem examinations of theseanimals, made by serial frontal sections, showed that the lenticular andthe caudate nuclei as well as the internal and the external capsulae werein every case destroyed almost completely; the colliculi and the mid-brain were intact. The optic thalami were intact in two cases, whilein the third their anterior parts were slightly bruised. Of the two dogswith the intact thalami, one was operated upon during panting; itsrespiratory rate was 200 per min. and its rectal temperature 41-4' C.The respiration was momentarily interrupted during the sections, but

322

Page 5: PDF (2.0MB)

CENTRAL AND REFLEX MECHANISM OF PANTING. 323

soon after the operation the dog was breathing at 60 per mi., andwithin 1 min. it was again panting at the rate of 200. The animal wasthen cooled; at a rectal temperature of 40.70 0. panting disappeared, therespiratory rate being 40 per m. The dog was cooled down to 370 C.and, on rewarming, it began to pant at the rate of 200-250 per min.when the rectal temperature reached 400 C. The second dog, which wasoperated upon 3 hours before the exposure to heat, panted at 120 per min.with a rectal temperature of 40.50 C., and at 250 per min. with a tenM..'perature of 410 C. In the third dog also the operation was performed3 hours before warming. On exposing this dog to heat, its rectal tem-perature gradually rose to 42.50 C., and the respiration accelerated withthe rise of temperature to 58 per min.; the breathing then becameirregular and was interrupted by deep gasps. No panting was observed.The dog was cooled and then warmed up to 430C. with exactly the sameresult. This was the dog in which the thalami were found to be injured.

These experiments with acute spino-thalamic animals show that thedevelopment and the extent of panting is of the same character as thatobserved in normal animals, and that the least injury to the thalamiabolishes panting and makes the animal behave as if it were -decere-brated. As in all the three experiments the corpora striata were almostcompletely destroyed one is led to the conclusion that these ganglia arenot concerned in panting. The experiments strongly suggest that thepart of the central nervous system on which panting depends, or to giveit a name the "panting-centre," -is located in the optic thalamus. Thisconclusion is supported by experiments in which four or -five serialfrontal sections of the brain were made during the progress of panting,and by experiments in which localized injuries to the thalami wereinflicted. The roof of the skull was completely removed, the longitudinalsinus was removed between ligatures, and the brain was exposed. Thedog was then kept immersed up to its neck in a hot bath, until pantingbecame well established. With a sharp flat spatula, frontal sections weremade across the hemispheres down to the base of the skull. The firstsection was made through the anterior parts of the frontal lobes; thepanting was undisturbed. After an interval of a few minutes, a newsection was made about 1 cm. behind the preceding section; this alsohad no effect. Sections were continued at short distances behind eachother until panting disappeared, after which no more sections were made.In the three experiments in which this was carried out, panting remainedunaffected until a section passed through the optic thalami. It should benoted that a section which passes just in front of the thalami definitely

Page 6: PDF (2.0MB)

M. HAMMOUDA.

cuts through the heads of the caudate nuclei. Such a section, however,has no permanent effect on panting.

Localized injuries to the thalami were made by means of a smallmetal tube. During the progress of panting, the tube was introducedbetween the hemispheres, through the corpus callosum and the fornixinto the third ventricle. This in itself had no effect on panting. Thethalami were then injured on both sides, and the extent of the injury wasdeterminedinthepost-mortem examina-tion of the brain. Four such experimentswere made. During the actual injury, therespiration either stopped orshowed con-siderable irregularities. After removal ofthe metal tube these irregularities per-sisted for a short time, but soon therespiration assumed a regular rate whichvaried in different experiments from 60to 100 per min. When the warming ofthese animalswas continued, the respira-tion rate did not increase any further.In a few experiments, the damage of thethalami was made by cauterization witha fine electro-cautery, which was passedinto the third ventricle in the samemanner as the metal tube. It was usuallyfound that the greater the damage tothe optic thalami, the slower was therespiration; but even comparativelysmall amounts of damage may con-siderably reduce the rate of panting.Fig. 2 shows the extent of the damage

Fig.2. Diagramtoillustratethelocalizedinjuries to the thalamus, as found inthe post-mortem performed after thetwo experiments referred to in thetext. The extent of the injuries inthe two experiments is represented bythe boundaries of the two differentlyshaded areas. In each case the injurywas almost identical on both sides ofthe brain. Th. optic thalamus; N.C.caudate nucleus; A.C. anterior colli-culus.

to the optic thalami in two cases. The results of serial sections of thebrain and of local injuries to the thalami thus definitely indicate thatpanting is dependent on the integrity of the optic thalami.

Abundant evidence is given in the literature that the temperaturecontrol of the animal is situated above the level of the mid-brain.Barbour [1912] locates the thermotaxic centre in the corpus striatum,but Cloetta and Waser [1914] do not support this view. Isenschmidand Krehl [1912], Isenschmid and Schnitzler [1914], Leschke[1913], de Barenne [1920], and Bazett and Penfield [1922] are of theopinion that the actual position of the centre which controls body tem-

324

Page 7: PDF (2.0MB)

CENTRAL AND REFLEX MECHANISM OF PANTING. 325

perature is probably in the thalamus, hypothalamus or tuber cinereum.Panting is a specialized mechanism of regulation of the body temperature.It is therefore not suprising that the centre for panting should belocalized in the same region as the other temperature controlling centres.

THE CENTRAL MECHANISM OF PANTING.

Raising the body temperature of an aneesthetized dog leads topanting. Rich et ascribed this to a direct stimulation of the respiratorycentre by heat, but he advanced no conclusive evidence in favour of thisview. Rich et refers to the experiments of some older observers whofound that panting can be produced by warming the carotid blood supply.These observations were confirmed by Kahn [1904] and by Heymansand Heymans [1926]. Bazett [1927], however, rightly remarks thatthe experiments in which the carotid blood stream is warmed cannot beregarded as conclusive evidence of the direct stimulation of the centresby heat, unless precautions are taken to exclude the influence of sensoryimpulses through the cranial nerves. This criticism assumes a specialimportance since Richet points out that the application of heat to theterminations of the fifth cranial nerve plays an important part in theproduction of panting in non-anaesthetized animals.

The following experiments were performed in order to determinewhether the panting produced by warming the carotid blood is reallydue to a direct heat effect on the centre. The vertebral arteries were tiedand the animal was heparinized. The central ends of both commoncarotid arteries were connected with a glass spiral which was immersedin a water bath. The blood flowing through the spiral (where it could bewarmed or cooled) was led into the peripheral ends of the common or ofthe internal carotid arteries. The temperature of the blood was measuredclose to the peripheral ends of the arteries. The venous blood was allowedto return to the heart of the animal. The temperature of the body, whichwas measured in the rectum, was kept as constant as possible. Onwarming the carotid blood, panting could be obtained with great ease.No difference was observed between the panting of animals which wereperfused through the internal or the common carotid arteries, showingthat the presence of the carotid sinus plays no role in panting. This wasconfirmed by two experiments in which the whole animal was warmedafter extirpation of the carotid sinus on both sides. The panting presentedno difference from that in animals with intact carotid sinus. These resultsare in agreement with the observations of Heymans and Heymans[1926] and Tournade and Malmejac [1930].

Page 8: PDF (2.0MB)

M. HAMMOUDA.

In the experiments in which the head is perfused through the internalcarotid arteries (the vertebrals are tied) besides the exclusion of thecarotid sinus, the blood supply to the peripheral structures of the headand therefore to the terminations of the fifth cranial nerve must beconsiderably reduced. In order to test whether panting may be caused bywarming these or other sensory terminations in the head, the followingadditional observations were made. The head of an animal, while beingperfused through the internal carotid arteries by a blood supply suffi-ciently warmed to cause panting, was surrounded by ice, and the nasaland the buccal cavities were continually irrigated with cold saline. Thepanting, however, remained unchanged. Further, it may be added thatthe removal of the whole skin of the head also fails to affect panting.These experiments show that at least the external sensory terminationsof the head play no role in the causation of panting in response to raisingthe temperature of the blood. It has already been mentioned thatextensively opening the skull and the meninges, which necessarilyprevents their warming, does not modify panting. Unless there arespecialized heat receptors in the meninges at the base of the brain or inthe cerebral blood vessels, all the evidence points to panting as beingproduced by an entirely central mechanism, i.e. by the direct effect ofheat on the "panting centre."

In several experiments an attempt was made to produce panting bydirect application of heat to the optic thalami. A narrow Kronecker'scannula was introduced into the third ventricle by the technique de-scribed above. Great care was taken to fix the cannula in such a positionthat it should not exert pressure upon the thalami. One branch of thecannula was connected with a reservoir containing warm saline, and theother was connected with a rubber tube which served as an outlet. Thethird ventricle was perfused at a low pressure with warm saline. Thetemperature was measured in the reservoir and in the inlet branch ofthe cannula. During the irrigation of the ventricle with saline at 380 C.,the respiration was regular throughout. On raising the temperature ofthe perfusing fluid in the inlet cannula to 420 C., it was observed in somecases that the animal started to pant. The panting could easily bestopped by dropping the temperature of the perfusing fluid. The pantingrate was in no case very high, but, as can be shown by a method de-scribed below, it was a genuine panting and not a simple acceleration ofthe ordinary respiration. Warming of the thalami by this method wouldobviously raise the temperature chiefly at the surface of the thalami.without much affecting the rest of the thalamus; moreover, manipu-

326

Page 9: PDF (2.0MB)

CENTRAL AND REFLEX MECHANISM OF PANTING. 327

lations in this region would invariably cause some disturbance ordamage. This is probably the reason why local warming of the thalaminever leads to such high rates of panting as are observed in the case ofwarming the carotid blood or the whole animal. In fact, in experimentsin which the introduction of the cannula caused definite damage to thethalami, perfusion with hot saline failed to induce panting. A macroscopicexamination of the thalami was made after the end of each experiment.It must be specially emphasized that experiments with the direct warmingof the thalamus are not invariably successful, but those which weresuccessful leave no doubt about their result. It is important to choose asuitable anmesthetic. In chloralosed animals, the effect was not obtained.Urethane or alcohol anesthesia was found to be the best. It is of interestto mention that in some experiments a gentle mechanical stimulation ofthe mesial surfaces of the thalami frequently produced panting, but therate did not exceed 120-130 per min. The following is an experimentwith direct warming of the thalami.

Dog 6 kg., morphine 25 mg., 5 c.c. of 42 p.c. alcohol intravenously. The injection ofalcohol was repeated in 1 c.c. doses during the experiment. The roof of the skull wasremoved, the meninges opened and the longitudinal sinus cut out between ligatures. Allbleeding points were stopped. The respiratory rate was 22 per min. 30 min. after openingthe skull, a thin Kronecker's cannula was introduced into the third ventricle. Therespiration at first increased to 34, and then returned to 22 per min. Saline at 380 C. wasnow pased through the cannula into the ventricle; the outflowing fluid at first containedtraces of blood, but in a few minutes it became clear. The respiratory rate fluctuatedbetween 20 and 22. The perfusion was now changed to saline at 420 C. (registered in theinlet of the cannula). The following were the respiratory rates during successive 20 sec.,starting from the moment of the irrigation with hot saline; 20, 60, 80, 80, 100, 100, 120,120, 120, 130, 130, 130 per min. The ventricle was now perfused with cold saline at 25° C.The rates were 100, 80, 60, 60, 50, 48, 36, 22, 18 per min.

This experiment was repeated several times on the same animal withthe same result. The cannula was then introduced more deeply into thethird ventricle, and moved in a frontal plane so as to produce localizeddamage to the thalami. The irrigation was continued. The respiration,after some irregularities, returned to 18 per min. and remained at thisrate. Irrigation of the ventricle with hot and cold saline now producedno change in the respiratory rate. The rectal temperature of the animalwas kept between 37*0 and 37.50 C. throughout the experiment. Thepost-mortem examination showed extensive damage of the mesial partsof both thalami; the corpora striata and the anterior colliculi wereintact.

Page 10: PDF (2.0MB)

M. HAMMOUDA.

THE REFLEX MECHANISM OF PANTING.

The experiments with perfusion of the carotid arteries, and with thedirect application of heat to the optic thalamus, are strong evidence infavour of the existence of a central mechanism of panting. Richetassumed that this was the only mechanism on which panting depends,in animals which have been subjected to anaesthesia. He did not, how-ever, perform any experiments in order to verify this assumption. Thequestion whether there is also a reflex mechanism, which co-operateswith the central one, still therefore remains unanswered. In other words,we do not know whether warming of some sensory terminations of thebody in an an8esthetized animal may evoke reflex panting. It wouldbe legitimate to suppose that the skin of the animal would be the mostlikely organ to act as a receptor in such a reflex. This is, however, notthe case, as a dog will pant when its body is warmed even after theremoval of the whole skin. The body temperature at which pantingdevelops does not differ from the panting temperature of a dog whoseskin is intact. The rate of panting is also of the same order. The followingexperiment is an example.

Dog 3*5 kg., morphine 20 mg., chloralose 0-25 g. On exposure to heat in a water bathat 420 C., the dog began to pant at a rectal temperature of 40.00 C. When its temperaturerose to 40 5° C., the dog panted at a regular rate of 200. The dog was cooled and theanaesthesia was deepened by administration of chloroform and ether mixture. The wholeskin of the animal from the tip of the nose to the end of the tail was then removed, carebeing taken to stop all bleeding points. The volatile ansesthetic was discontinued, and theanimal was at once placed in a saline bath at 380 C. and allowed to rest for 45 min., afterwhich the temperature of the bath was raised to 41° C. The dog, as before, started pantingat a rectal temperature of 40.00 C. and soon reached a rate of 230 per min. with a rectaltemperature of 40 2° C.

This experiment definitely shows that the skin plays no role inpanting produced in response to the warming of the whole body. Thefollowing experiments, in which the carotid blood stream was warmedor cooled independently of the changes of the body temperature,demonstrate further that the warming of the internal organs of the dogalso does not cause panting.

The blood stream in the common carotid arteries was passed through a water bath, inthe same manner as in the experiments referred to before, while the body of the dog wasplaced in an air thermostat. The temperature of the head was measured by a thermometerplaced deeply under the tongue, and the temperature of the body by a thermometer inthe rectum. In the first part of the experiment, the body and the carotid blood werewarmed at the same rate. The animal, which breathed at the rate of 18-22 per min.,gradually increased the respiration to 33 per min. At a temperature of 410 C. in the mouth

328

Page 11: PDF (2.0MB)

CENTRAL AND REFLEX MECHANISM OF PANTING. 329and rectum the animal abruptly began to pant, increasing its panting within 4 min. to240 per min. The head was now gradually cooled to 38.50 C., but the body was warmedstill further until the rectal temperature reached 42.50 C. In spite of this, the respirationdropped to 26 per min. On rewarming the carotid blood to 410 C., the panting returned to250 per min. The head was then kept at 41° C., while the whole body was gradually cooledto 37.50 C. The panting rate did not change; it fluctuated between 230 and 250 per min.

A second experiment was started by warming the body alone, the mouth temperaturebeing kept at 37.50 C. The rectal temperature was gradually raised to 41*5' C. but therespiration remained unchanged; it varied between 26 and 30 per min. The body was thencooled to 370 C. while the head was gradually warmed; at a mouth temperature of 390 C.,the respiratory rate became 200 and, at 39.50 C., 290 per min.

These experiments give strong support to Richet's statement thatin chloralosed dogs panting is entirely of central origin. Reflex pantingcould not be observed in anesthetized dogs.

RESPIRATORY REFLEXES DURING PANTING.

Richet [1898] refers to Goldstein and Sihler as having been thefirst to show that section of the vagi does not affect panting. This obser-vation was verified and extended in a previous communication [Anrepand Hammouda, 1932]. It can now be added that in panting, besidesplaying no role, the vagus nerve also loses its normal controlling actionof the respiratory movements. The reflex arrest of the respiration inresponse to inflation of the lung [Hering and Breuer, 1868] is absentduring panting (see Figs. 3 and 4). This could be seen in every experiment.It is so constant a phenomenon that it is convenient to take the dis-appearance of the reflex to inflation as an arbitrary criterion for dis-tinguishing between panting and fast rates of normal respiration. Usuallythe conspicuous difference in the rate between ordinary respiration andpanting leaves no doubt as to which type of respiration one is dealingwith. But in some cases, especially in deeply anaesthetized animals inwhich the panting rate may not exceed about 120 per min., the dis-appearance of the reflex is of considerable help. The disappearance of thepulmonary vagal reflex is not abrupt. During the period of transitionto panting, inflation of the lungs can still produce a brief arrest or slowingdown of the respiration. This arrest, however, differs from that observedin the case of ordinary respiration by being considerably delayed in itsappearance. The normal respiration is stopped at once as soon as thelungs are inflated, while in panting there are always a few respiratorymovements before the arrest; these respiratory movements are frequentlyunchanged in rate (see Fig. 4a and b). As panting develops, progressivelylarger degrees of inflation are required in order to produce this delayed

Page 12: PDF (2.0MB)

M. HAMMOUDA.

pause. Finally, as panting becomes established, inflation of the lungsceases to have any inhibitory effect on respiration. In several experi-ments, an acceleration of the respiration was noticed during the inflation,instead of an inhibition. Although gradual, the disappearance of thisHering-Breuer reflex occurs rapidly. In most experiments, the tran-sition period lasts approximately the same length of time as it takes thedog to develop the fast rate of respiration which is definitely recognizedas panting. This may require from 10-15 sec. to 1-2 min.

A

+11 lllnl~11m.Hg

.,~ ~~~~~~ t ~

Fig. 3. Effect of deflation and inflation of the lungs during the transition from normalrespiration to panting. The lungs were deflated and inflated by positive and negativepressures applied to the animal which was enclosed in a box. A. Effect of deflationby +11 mm. Hg at a respiratory rate of 80 per min. The respiration accelerated to100 per min. B. Effect of inflation by - 9 mm. Hg at a respiratory rate of 95 per mm.Read from left to right. Time in 10 sec.

In the great majority of the experiments (over 200 dogs), the re--spiratory rate of 120 per min. could be recognized as an unquestionablyestablished panting. Rates between 75 and 120 showed a progressivediminution, but not a complete absence, of the respiratory response toinflation of the lungs.

The usual acceleration of the respiration in response to a deflation ofthe lungs [Hammouda and Wilson, 1932] could not be observed duringpanting. For instance, in one experiment, just before the developmentof panting, the animal's respiration, which was 54 per min., accelerated

330

Page 13: PDF (2.0MB)

CENTRAL AND REFLEX MECHANISM OF PANTING. 331

to 108 when its chest was exposed to an outside positive pressure of13 mm. Hg. A few minutes later the animal started panting at the rateof 120. The compression of the chest by 16 mm. Hg air pressure did not

;~~~~

B

-8 inm. Hg

-R l l l~~~~~Ill. Hg

D

_+ ~~~~~9nmm.Hg

10 sec.

Fig. 4. Effect of inflation and deflation of the lungs during panting, from the same dog asFig. 3. A, B and C show the effect of inflation ( - 8 mm. Hg); the respiratory rateswere 114, 120 and 150 respectively. In D, deflation ( +9 mm. Hg) did not change therate of 162 per min. Time in 10 sec. Read from left to right. The line under eachrespiratory record shows the pressure change in the box enclosing the animal. Therespiration was recorded by means of a bellows recorder which was attached to aclosed respiratory system of 20 litres capacity, connected with the trachea of theanimal. In between the records, the trachea of the dog was opened to the air.

change the rate. It is obvious that these experiments are not as con-clusive as those with the inflation of the lungs, since it is always possibleto assume that the respiration was already too fast to be further ac-celerated by the deflation of the lungs.

Page 14: PDF (2.0MB)

M. HAMMOUDA.

The absence of the Hering-Breuer reflex is not due to a possibleeffect of heat upon the terminations of the vagus nerve in the lungs.This is seen from experiments in which panting was produced by warmingthe carotid blood while the body temperature was not raised; in this caseinflation and deflation of the lungs also failed to change the rate ofpanting.

PANTING IN THE NORMAL DOG.

On comparing panting in ansesthetized and non-anaesthetized animals,one is at once struck with the fact that, while in the former panting isundoubtedly of a central nature, in the latter it exhibits all the proper-ties of a reflex. This contrast is still further accentuated by the factthat, under ordinary experimental conditions, it seems impossible toobtain reflex panting in the anaesthetized dog, while in the normal dogit takes place with the greatest ease. A dog which is exposed to the sunbegins to pant in a few minutes. Careful measurements of the rectaltemperature in most cases show an initial rise of about 0.10 C. This riseof temperature cannot, however, be the cause of panting since, as soonas panting starts, the temperature drops below the prepanting level,usually not less than 0.3-0.5° C. and sometimes as much as a wholedegree. This was observed by Richet and used by him as a proof of thepurely reflex character of panting in non-anaesthetized animals.

It can easily be shown that not only exposure of the whole animalto the sun but also exposure of parts of its surface leads to panting.For instance on one occasion, when the head of a dog was exposed tothe sun while its body was kept in the shade, panting started in 51 min.On exposing the hind quarters it started in 6 min., while exposure ofthe whole animal evoked panting in 3 min. The temperature of the sunin this case was only 460 C.' On another occasion on exposing the headalone, when the temperature in the sun was 520 C., panting started in51 min., while on exposing the body with the head kept in the shade itstarted in 30 sec. At a still higher temperature (650 C.), the dog maystart panting almost at once. No precise determinations were made ofthe minimal surface which had to be exposed to the sun in order toobtain panting. It became evident, however, from the observations madethat the exposure of the head does not play any special role in com-

1 The monthly mean temperature in Cairo, taken with the black bulb thermometer,at 2 p.m. in the sun, varies between 510 C. in December and 680 C. from June to October.The relative humidity is 62-66 p.c. during winter and 45-55 p.c. from February to Sep-tember.

332

Page 15: PDF (2.0MB)

CENTRAL AND REFLEX MECHANISM OF PANTING. 333

parison with the rest of the body. The rectal temperature of theseanimals either remained unchanged or rose not more than 0.10 C.

Richet states that, in the conscious animal, panting does not takeplace at all if some obstruction to respiration is introduced. For instance,he quotes as an example that a muzzled dog does not pant. As a con-sequence of this, its body temperature rises in a short time. This in-hibitory action (of closing the mouth) on panting is, however, far frombeing so simple. Muzzling a dog for the first time may delay the appear-ance of panting for half an hour or more, and the animal's temperaturemay rise quite appreciably; but in the end panting invariably starts.Placing the muzzle on an already panting animal may stop pantingcompletely for many minutes. It is of importance that if the muzzle isused for the second or third time it loses its inhibitory effect. An animalwhich is accustomed to the muzzle begins to pant almost as soon aswithout the muzzle; muzzling such an animal while panting is in progressdoes not stop panting any more. There is, however, one effect of closureof the mouth which remains. With the muzzle, the panting is slower anddeeper than without. For example, placing the muzzle on an animal,which was already comparatively accustomed to it, reduced the pantingrate from 300 to 220 per min.; on removing the muzzle, it returned to300. It has not yet been found whether this effect is constant or showssigns of diminution on repetition of the experiments. The abolition ofpanting observed during the first application of the muzzle is not due toclosure of the mouth. In fact, any other discomfort may also producearrest of panting. A slap, rapid lifting of the animal, or the applicationof any other stimulus, if sufficiently strong, abolishes panting for sometime. Auditory and strong visual stimuli may do the same. All thesestimuli are alike in that their repetition rapidly ceases to have an effect.In this respect these stimuli show a striking similarity to the well-knownphenomenon of external inhibition of conditioned reflexes [P a vlo v, 1927].

It is not only external stimuli which may stop panting. If the animalis thirsty or hungry, if its bladder or bowels are full, exposure to sundoes not cause panting until its rectal temperature rises appreciablyabove normal, which usually takes 3045 min. at an external temperaturein the sun of 37450 C. When the cause of the discomfort is removed,exposure to the sun again produces panting within a very short time,and without a rise of body temperature. In many cases the disturbingcause could not be traced.

The similarity between panting in the non-ansesthetized animal withconditioned reflexes was so striking that it seemed probable that what is

Page 16: PDF (2.0MB)

M. HAMMOUDA.

known as reflex panting is really a conditioned reflex. It is as yetimpossible to state whether every case of reflex panting, occurring withoutor with a minimal rise of body temperature, is conditioned panting. Butthe failure to obtain panting in ansesthetized animals lends support tothis supposition.

Conditioned panting can be established with great ease. In severalanimals this was done in the following manner. A dog, which was quitenew to this type of experiment but accustomed to the laboratory, wasplaced in a specially constructed large box, where it was subjected to ahigh temperature. The box was well ventilated. Ten shielded carbon-filament lamps were used as a source of heat, and the temperature of thebox was kept at 450 C. In every case the respiration was recordedgraphically by means of a stethograph. The development of conditionedpanting will be described in relation to one dog only. On several daysbefore the experiment the dog was placed in the unheated box (20-22° C.).In no case did it start panting. The dog was then placed in the heatedbox once or twice a day. The latent period during the first six exposuresvaried between 8 and 4 min., gradually becoming shorter. On the thirdweek of working with this animal, the latent period shortened to between30 sec. and 2 min. On several occasions the dog panted as soon as itwas placed in the box. The experiment would usually be terminated afew minutes after the onset of panting. The maximum panting rates werebetween 150 and 200 per min. After forty-one exposures, the dog wasplaced in the box which was not heated (200 C.). The respiration of theanimal was 22 per min. Thirty seconds after the introduction into thecold box the respiration increased to 60-80, and in the following minuteto 120. The second test was made the next week, after a few morereinforcements of the reflex. The latent period was again 30 sec. Duringseveral trials, in between which the reflex was steadily reinforced everyday, the latent period was found to vary between 15 and 30 sec. Themaximum rate of this conditioned panting was between 120 and 150per mi.

Conditioned panting undergoes experimental extinction as can beseen from the following experiment. On the third month of the develop-ment of the reflex, the dog was placed in the cold box and then removedabout 30 sec. after the onset of panting. After an interval of 5 min. thedog was again introduced into the box; this time the latent period wasconsiderably longer. The procedure was repeated until panting couldnot be obtained, which happened on the fifth introduction into thecold box.

334

Page 17: PDF (2.0MB)

CENTRAL AND REFLEX MECHANISM OF PANTING. 335

The phenomenon of external inhibition could also be easily observed.This effect on conditioned panting is remarkably similar to the effect ofextraneous stimuli on a dog which pants under ordinary circumstanceswhen exposed to the sun.

SUMMARY.

1. Serial sections of the brain and localized injuries to the opticthalami made in acute experiments demonstrate that panting takes placeonly when the optic thalami are intact.

2. Removal of the cortex and almost complete destruction of thecorpora striata does not abolish panting.

3. Panting observed in anaesthetized animals on raising the bodytemperature is due to the direct effect of heat upon the optic thalami.It is not due to any reflexes in response to raising the temperature ofsensory nerve endings in the head.

4. Heat directly applied to the third ventricle was, in several experi-ments, successful in evoking panting. It failed to have this effect afterinjury to the thalami.

5. The inhibition of the respiratory movements caused by the inflationof the lungs, and the acceleration of the respiration caused by deflation,is not observed during panting.

6. The experiments failed to demonstrate the existence of any specialreceptors in the body which would reflexly evoke panting in response to arise in body temperature.

7. The normal non-aneesthetized animal pants reflexly without orwith a very small initial rise of temperature. This panting exhibits thecharacters of a conditioned reflex. Conditioned panting can easily beestablished experimentally.

8. Spontaneous panting on exposure to the sun and conditionedpanting are similar in that they are both subject to external inhibition.Conditioned panting undergoes experimental extinction.

9. It is suggested that what is known as reflex panting is a conditionedreflex which is developed, not on the bases of an unconditioned reflex,but on the bases of the purely central effect of heat. In this respect itwould be similar to the conditioned reflexes established as the result ofinjection of various centrally acting drugs.

The research described in this communication was inspired by someobservations made during my conjoint work with my teacher, Prof. W. H.Wilson. I wish to express my grateful thanks for his guidance and

PH. LXXVII. 22

Page 18: PDF (2.0MB)

336 M. HAMMOUDA.

encouragement. The actual experiments were made under the directionof Prof. G. V. Anrep, F.R.S., to whom I am grateful for his constantadvice and criticism.

REFERENCES.

Anrep, G. V. and Hammouda, M. (1932). J. Phy8iol. 77, 16Barbour, H. G. (1912). Arch. exp. Path. Pharmaak. 70, 1.Barenne, J. G. Dusser de (1920). Arch. neerland. Physiol. 4, 31.Bazett, H. C. (1927). Phy8iol. Rev. 7, 531.Bazett, H. C. and Penfield, W. G. (1922). Brain, 45, 185.Cloetta, M. and Waser, E. (1914). Arch. exp. Path. Pharmaak. 77, 16.Hammouda, M. and Wilson, W. H. (1932). J. Physiol. 74, 81.Hering, E. and Breuer, J. (1868). S.-B. Akad. Wi88. Wien, Math. Naturwiss. K1. 2,

58, 909.Heymans, J. F. and Heymans, C. (1926). Ann. Soc. Sci. mead. nat. Brux. 46, 294.Isenschmid, R. and Krehl, L. (1912). Arch. exp. Path. Pharmak. 70, 109.Isenschmid, R. and Schnitzler, W. (1914). Ibid. 76, 202.Kahn, R. H. (1904). Arch. Anat. Physiol., Lpz. Physiol. Abt. Suppl. Band. 71.Leschke, E. (1913). Z. exp. Path. 14, 167.Nikolaides, R. and Dontas, S. (1911). Zbl. Phy8iol. 25, 192.Pavlov, I. P. (1927). Conditioned Reflexe8. Translated and Edited by G. V. Anrep.

Oxford University Press.Richet, C. (1898). Dictionnaire de Physiologie, 3, 178. Paris.Sherrington, C. S. (1924). J. Physiol. 58, 405.Tournade, A. and Malm6jac, J. (1930). C. R. Soc. Biol., Paris, 105, 834.