participation of long-day inhibition in flowering

6
Plant Physiol. (1971) 47, 784-789 Participation of Long-Day Inhibition in Flowering of Xanthium strumarium L.1 Received for publication July 13, 1970 DAVID D. GIBBY AND FRANK B. SALISBURY Plant Science Department, Utah State University, Logan, Utah 84321 ABSTRACT Two basic experiments defined a long-day inhibitory effect on Xanthium flowering: the basal half of a single leaf on long day inhibits response of the tip half to a short day; and a long-day leaf inhibits response of a short-day leaf, providing it is between the short-day leaf and a receptive bud (whether above or below the short-day leaf). Five hypotheses were explored with the conclusions that the tip half can synthesize florigen, and inhibition is not due to prevention of florigen synthesis, translocational effects, or a translocatable long-day inhibitor. Inhibition is localized and may be a conditon of the leaf or a relatively immobile substance. Studies of critical dark period, light intensity, and interruption of a dark period show that, when the leaf is not producing florigen, it is actively inhibitory. Immature leaves are more inhibitory than older leaves. The effect was found not to pass dead tissue, and iron- deficient tissue will cause inhibition, though it will not cause promotion. Xanthium strumarium (cocklebur) has been widely used in flowering research, but seldom have inhibitor studies used this plant. Perhaps this is because evidence for a promotive florigen is so strong in this plant (23), and inhibitor studies have traditionally used plants requiring several inductive cycles (18). Hamner and Bonner in 1938 (14) indicated that fully expanded leaves under long-day conditions on receptor branches of two- branched plants were inhibitory to floral evocation! of a donor branch subjected to an inductive photoperiod. In 1956, Lin- coln, Raven, and Hamner (19) used the same donor-receptor system, finding that inhibition could be quantitatively related to the amount of mature leaf area exposed to long-day con- ditions, carbohydrate movement toward the receptor branch was necessary for its evocation, and immature leaves were capable of inhibition. Days incorporating 7¼4 hr of darkness or less were inhibitory to their receptor branches, and only a 1This project was supported by Grant GB-15291 from the Na- tional Science Foundation. The work was submitted by the senior author in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science. Journal Paper 1057, Utah State Agricultural Ex- periment Station. 'Evocation is a term suggested by L. T. Evans (8). The term is meant to describe the processes at the stem apex that cause the morphological change, committing the bud to a state of flowering. The term was suggested to distinguish these events from those that take place in the leaf during the act of induction. part of their data could be explained on the basis of assimilate translocation. The processes of induction and evocation constitute a system of many interrelated factors that must be considered as a whole. Could the lack of success in extraction and identifi- cation of flower-promoting factors be caused by ignoring flower-inhibiting factors? If we apply an extract from a flower- ing plant to one under noninductive conditions, the promotive factor in the extract could be nullified by inhibitors in the vegetative plant acting upon it or the site of its action. There are several reports of floral-inhibiting factors besides those summarized above relating to cocklebur. Schwabe (24) studied the inhibitory effect of long days interspersed among short days. In flowering of Biloxi soybean, 1 long day nullified the following 2.2 short days. Similar experiments have been done on Kalanchoe by Harder and Bimnsow (15) and Schwabe (24). Fratianne (10) demonstrated the likelihood that an in- hibitory substance or substances, produced in the leaves of various short-day plants, inhibits flowering in dodder (Cuscuta campestris). Flowering experiments with Fragaria (1 1-13, 27) strongly demonstrate the participation of inhibitor(s). Pro- motive factors may not even be necessary to explain induction of this plant. Evans (5-7, 9) has demonstrated a photoperiodic floral inhibitor in the long-day plant Lolium temulentum. Ap- parently the inhibitor is transported to the bud, where it acts. Evidence with "4C-labeled assimilates disputes the hypothesis that translocational and dilutional effects cause inhibition (9). He suggested that abscisic acid may be the short-day inhibitor in Lolium (7). Chailakhyan in 1945 (3) induced various parts of a single leaf of Perilla, while other parts remained under noninductive conditions. The basal leaf half could strongly induce flowering under short days, even when the tip half was under long days, but the reverse was not true. Harder, Westphal, and Behrens (16) obtained similar results with Kalanchoe. In addition, they showed that flowering resulted when the apical half was in- duced, providing basal leaf tissue was trimmed off. In related experiments Lona (20) demonstrated that in- duction of the apical half of the Perilla leaf did not render the basal part effective as a donor in grafting experiments. The basal and apical halves could exist side by side in the leaf without affecting one another. Inhibition may also be the result of interference with trans- location of florigen to the bud. Chilakhyan and Butenko (4) correlated movement of 14C-labeled assimilates and flowering of Perilla. Conditions promoting transport of assimilates from induced leaves to the buds also promoted flowering; conditions that blocked this translocation were inhibitory. In the present study, two experiments clearly indicate a long-day inhibitory effect in flowering of Xanthium. Five hypotheses were devised to account for this effect. Experiments were then designed to test these hypotheses, essentially elimi- 784

Upload: dinhtram

Post on 31-Dec-2016

217 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Participation of Long-Day Inhibition in Flowering

Plant Physiol. (1971) 47, 784-789

Participation of Long-Day Inhibition in Floweringof Xanthium strumarium L.1

Received for publication July 13, 1970

DAVID D. GIBBY AND FRANK B. SALISBURYPlant Science Department, Utah State University, Logan, Utah 84321

ABSTRACT

Two basic experiments defined a long-day inhibitory effecton Xanthium flowering: the basal half of a single leaf on longday inhibits response of the tip half to a short day; and along-day leaf inhibits response of a short-day leaf, providingit is between the short-day leaf and a receptive bud (whetherabove or below the short-day leaf). Five hypotheses wereexplored with the conclusions that the tip half can synthesizeflorigen, and inhibition is not due to prevention of florigensynthesis, translocational effects, or a translocatable long-dayinhibitor. Inhibition is localized and may be a conditon of theleaf or a relatively immobile substance. Studies of critical darkperiod, light intensity, and interruption of a dark period showthat, when the leaf is not producing florigen, it is activelyinhibitory. Immature leaves are more inhibitory than olderleaves. The effect was found not to pass dead tissue, and iron-deficient tissue will cause inhibition, though it will not causepromotion.

Xanthium strumarium (cocklebur) has been widely used inflowering research, but seldom have inhibitor studies used thisplant. Perhaps this is because evidence for a promotive florigenis so strong in this plant (23), and inhibitor studies havetraditionally used plants requiring several inductive cycles (18).Hamner and Bonner in 1938 (14) indicated that fully expandedleaves under long-day conditions on receptor branches of two-branched plants were inhibitory to floral evocation! of a donorbranch subjected to an inductive photoperiod. In 1956, Lin-coln, Raven, and Hamner (19) used the same donor-receptorsystem, finding that inhibition could be quantitatively relatedto the amount of mature leaf area exposed to long-day con-ditions, carbohydrate movement toward the receptor branchwas necessary for its evocation, and immature leaves werecapable of inhibition. Days incorporating 7¼4 hr of darknessor less were inhibitory to their receptor branches, and only a

1This project was supported by Grant GB-15291 from the Na-tional Science Foundation. The work was submitted by the seniorauthor in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree ofMaster of Science. Journal Paper 1057, Utah State Agricultural Ex-periment Station.

'Evocation is a term suggested by L. T. Evans (8). The term ismeant to describe the processes at the stem apex that cause themorphological change, committing the bud to a state of flowering.The term was suggested to distinguish these events from those thattake place in the leaf during the act of induction.

part of their data could be explained on the basis of assimilatetranslocation.The processes of induction and evocation constitute a

system of many interrelated factors that must be considered asa whole. Could the lack of success in extraction and identifi-cation of flower-promoting factors be caused by ignoringflower-inhibiting factors? If we apply an extract from a flower-ing plant to one under noninductive conditions, the promotivefactor in the extract could be nullified by inhibitors in thevegetative plant acting upon it or the site of its action.

There are several reports of floral-inhibiting factors besidesthose summarized above relating to cocklebur. Schwabe (24)studied the inhibitory effect of long days interspersed amongshort days. In flowering of Biloxi soybean, 1 long day nullifiedthe following 2.2 short days. Similar experiments have beendone on Kalanchoe by Harder and Bimnsow (15) and Schwabe(24). Fratianne (10) demonstrated the likelihood that an in-hibitory substance or substances, produced in the leaves ofvarious short-day plants, inhibits flowering in dodder (Cuscutacampestris). Flowering experiments with Fragaria (1 1-13, 27)strongly demonstrate the participation of inhibitor(s). Pro-motive factors may not even be necessary to explain inductionof this plant. Evans (5-7, 9) has demonstrated a photoperiodicfloral inhibitor in the long-day plant Lolium temulentum. Ap-parently the inhibitor is transported to the bud, where it acts.Evidence with "4C-labeled assimilates disputes the hypothesisthat translocational and dilutional effects cause inhibition (9).He suggested that abscisic acid may be the short-day inhibitorin Lolium (7).

Chailakhyan in 1945 (3) induced various parts of a singleleaf of Perilla, while other parts remained under noninductiveconditions. The basal leaf half could strongly induce floweringunder short days, even when the tip half was under long days,but the reverse was not true. Harder, Westphal, and Behrens(16) obtained similar results with Kalanchoe. In addition, theyshowed that flowering resulted when the apical half was in-duced, providing basal leaf tissue was trimmed off.

In related experiments Lona (20) demonstrated that in-duction of the apical half of the Perilla leaf did not render thebasal part effective as a donor in grafting experiments. Thebasal and apical halves could exist side by side in the leafwithout affecting one another.

Inhibition may also be the result of interference with trans-location of florigen to the bud. Chilakhyan and Butenko (4)correlated movement of 14C-labeled assimilates and flowering ofPerilla. Conditions promoting transport of assimilates frominduced leaves to the buds also promoted flowering; conditionsthat blocked this translocation were inhibitory.

In the present study, two experiments clearly indicate along-day inhibitory effect in flowering of Xanthium. Fivehypotheses were devised to account for this effect. Experimentswere then designed to test these hypotheses, essentially elimi-

784

Page 2: Participation of Long-Day Inhibition in Flowering

LONG-DAY INHIBITION IN XANTHIUM FLOWERING

nating all but one. Further experiments were performed tostudy the properties of this long-day inhibition.

MATERIAlS AND METHODS

Culture methods have been described (22). To summarize,plants of Xanthium strumarium L. (cocklebur) were germinatedin sand, transplanted into 4-inch square plastic pots after about2 weeks, and kept vegetative by extending day length to 20 hrwith fluorescent light. The length of the night was kept con-stant regardless of natural day length by illuminating from2:00 AM to 10:00 AM and again from 4:00 PM to 10:00 PM.

Eight-week-old plants were usually used. The smallest leaflonger than 1 cm (midrib) was called leaf 1, the next largest,leaf 2, and so on. Plants were used with the no. 3 leaves(maximally sensitive to induction [22]) between 6.9 and 8.5 cmlong. Typically, all leaves except leaf 3 were removed the daybefore induction, but other leaves were sometimes used, asnoted. Leaf 3 was used as the source of promotion in all ex-periments. The tip half of the leaf was usually covered withan envelope of black construction paper for 16 hr, while thebasal half was exposed to various light and dark treatments.When two leaves were used, the entire no. 3 leaf was covered,while the entire no. 2 (or other) leaf was treated in variousways. Flowering was scored after 9 days according to asystem of floral stages (22). The temperature during treatmentwas 23 C, unless stated otherwise.

Plants used in the iron deficiency experiment (no. 11) weretransplanted to pots containing washed perlite, and the potswere placed in plastic-coated steel trays containing Hoagland'ssolution minus iron to a depth of 1/2 inches. After marked irondeficiency symptoms appeared, iron chelates corrected thechlorosis within 24 hr. Because iron moves very slowly exceptin the xylem, it was possible to correct the deficiency in one ormore leaves by foliar application of iron chelate withoutcorrecting the deficiency in other parts of the plant. The leaveswere dipped in a solution of 4 g/liter iron chelate [GeigySequestrene 138 Fe, active ingredient: technical sodium ferricethylenediamine di-(o-hydroxyphenylacetate)], and the excesswas blotted off.

RESULTS AND DISCUSSION

In experiment 1, basal or tip halves of leaf 3 were coveredfor 16 hr, normally an optimal inductive dark period. Un-shaded areas were kept in continuous incandescent light ofabout 15 ft-c intensity. Plants with the basal halves coveredflowered only slightly less than controls with entire leavescovered (Fig. 1), but plants with the tip half covered were com-pletely vegetative.

Experiment 2 tested the ability of leaf 2 to inhibit floweringwhen leaf 3 was induced. Leaf 2 was removed from controls.Leaf 3 was induced, but leaf 2 received 10 ft-c of continuousincandescent light. Leaf 2 completely inhibited the action ofthe induced leaf 3. Hence we confirm that immature leavesare capable of strongly inhibiting the photoperiodic stimulus(19).We were able to propose the following five hypotheses to

account for the results of experiments 1 and 2.1. The tip half of the leaf is not capable of induction (ex-

periment 1).2. An inhibitor is produced on long days that inhibits pro-

duction of promoter by short days (as in Schwabe's experi-ments [24]).

3. The apparent inhibitory effects depend upon florigenmoving only with the assimilate stream. For example, long-day tissue may be acting as an assimilate source (photosyn-

thesizing) in such a manner that assimilate from the short-daytissue cannot reach the bud. Altematively, under some con-ditions (low light intensity), the long-day tissue may be actingas a sink for assimilate produced by the short-day tissue.

4. There is a long-day-produced, translocatable inhibitor.5. A substance or condition inhibitory to flowering is

produced on long days, and its effect is localized near thetissues in which it originates.

Experiment 3 was designed to test the first two hypotheses.In all cases the tip half of leaf 3 was covered for 16 hr. Intreatment 3, tissue was removed from the basal half (leavingno leaf tissue on the veins) before covering the tip half; intreatment 4, basal tissue was exposed to 2-ft-c incandescentlight during induction of the tip and then removed. The firsttwo treatments (Fig. 3) demonstrate the same promotion andinhibition as in experiment 1, but flowering occurred in treat-ments 3 and 4, where the leaf tissue of the basal half of theleaves was removed. The tip half of the cocklebur leaf is there-fore fully capable of induction, eliminating the first hypothesis.If inhibition were due to some action of an inhibitor on thesubsequent production of a promotive factor, plants in treat-ment 4 would not have flowered because the long-day tissuewas removed after the tip half of the leaf had been induced.This eliminates the second hypothesis. (Note that controls inthis experiment flowered considerably less than in some otherexperiments. This is correlated with time of year-lowest inwinter-and is familiar from previous work.)The question of assimilate translocation was studied in

several ways. Experiment 3 has bearing on the hypothesis. Ifthe long-day tissue (the base of the leaf) were acting as an as-similate source and the bud and the short-day tissue as sinks,then the long-day tissue would have to be actively photo-synthesizing. Yet the long-day inhibtion is almost completewith only 2 ft-c of incandescent light, certainly far below thecompensation point.What is the threshold light intensity of the inhibitory re-

sponse compared to the threshold intensity for promotion?

Treatment

Controk # 3 leaf16 hr. dork

,t Basal 1/2 # 3 leaf16 hr. dork, tip 1/2in continuous light

Basal V2 # 3 leaf incontinuous light, tip 1/216 hr. dark

Av. Floral Stage

6.00±0.45

5.20+0.83

0.00

FIG. 1. Effect on flowering of dark periods given to the tip orbasal half of the leaf. Confidence interval was computed at the a= 0.05 level.

Treatment

Contrl,dark

Av. Floral Stage

# 3 leaf 16 hr.

#3 leaf 16hr. dark, #2leaf 0 f t-c continuouslight

5.10t0.51

0.00

FIG. 2. Inhibitory effects on flowering of leaf 2 on long day.

785-P-Ia-nt Physiol. Vol. 47, 1971

i

Page 3: Participation of Long-Day Inhibition in Flowering

GIBBY AND SALISBURY

A preliminary experiment (no. 4) was conducted to de-termine if the act of holding the leaves stationary would affectflowering. Leaves were held with cardboard and wire braces inthree positions: horizontal, vertical, and 450 off vertical. These

No. Treatment

2 t

3

_,4

Av. Floral Stage

Control, # 3 leoaf16 hr. dark

Basal 1/2 # 3 leaf2 ft-c continuous light,tipl/2 16 hr. dark

Basal 1/2 tissue removedbefore dark period,tip 1/2 16 hr. dark

Basal 1/2 2ft-c light then itstissue removed ofter dark period,tip 1/2 16 hr. dark

3.60+1.18

0.10±0.23

2.30±1.55

2.20t1.00

FIG. 3. Effect of removal of the basal long-day leaf tissue on re-sponse of tip tissue to short day.

No. TreatmentLeaf position

N Unfi

I

Fixe

2 4 .

3 '1

, Fixe

4 vert

4

Av. Floral Stage

xed contol3.90' 1.43

ed at horizontal

ed at vertical

ed ot 450 ofticol

3.60±1.62

3.80+1.06

3.70+1.22

FIG. 4. The effect on flowering of holding the leaves stationary.

5-

3-

2

30 60 90 120 150 180 210cm From the Light Source

FIG. 5. The intensity of light needed for long-day inhibitioncompared to that required for inhibition of short-day promotion.The upper curve resulted from exposing the basal half of the leafto the light source (tip half shaded). The lower curve resulted fromexposing the entire leaf.

wereall given an inductive 16-hr dark period along with con-trols in which the leaves remained free. The results (Fig. 4) in-dicate no significant difference between treatments. Biinningand Moser (1) reported that the act of holding the leavesstationary and interfering with leaf movements had an adverseeffect on the flowering of Perilla and Chenopodium, but wefailed to observe this with Xanthium, which also has a distinctdiurnal leaf movement.

In experiment 5, a 7Y/2-w incandescent light source wasused. This was dimmed to a 75% setting on a powerstat (chang-ing the spectral distribution of the light slightly). The plantswere placed in semicircles around the light source at increasingdistances, resulting in decreasing intensities of light. The leaf ofeach plant was held in place with its surface normal to thelight rays. Half of the plants had the tip halves of their leavescovered; others were left uncovered. Results are shown inFigure 5. The intensity of light needed for the long-day inhibi-tion (basal halves) is only slightly higher than that required forinhibition of short-day promotion. It is difficult to imagine howan assimilate hypothesis could account for this, making thethird hypothesis at least doubtful.

In experiments 6 and 7, the inhibitory effect of matureleaves on long days was investigated: In experiment 6, treat-ment 1, only leaf 3 remained; in treatment 2, leaves 3 and 4remained, and in treatment 3, leaves 3, 4, and 5 remained.Leaf 3 was always given a 16-hr dark period, and the otherleaves, when present, were kept in 10-ft-c incandescent light.

In experiment 7, all buds including the terminal bud wereremoved except one located below leaf 4 (treatments la andlb) or leaf 6 (treatments 2a and 2b). Treatments are shown inFigure 7. Treatmentsla and lb were separated in time fromtreatments 2a and 2b by a period of 2 weeks, explaining thedifference in the flowering level between controls la and 2a.The lone bud remaining on the plants was allowed to becomeactive before leaf 3 was induced by giving it a 16-hr darkperiod, and all other leaves were kept in 10-ft-c continuouslight. Compare the relative amount of inhibition in experiments6 and 7 (Figs. 6 and 7). It is clear from a comparison of thesetwo experiments, as has been shown earlier by various workers(e.g., 14, 16, 25), that to be effective, long-day tissue must besituated between the short-day tissue and the bud, a fact difficultto reconcile with the fourth hypothesis. It is also interestingthat the level of inhibition caused by the mature leaves in ex-periment 7 was low compared to that caused by the immatureleaf 2 in experiment 2.

Lincoln et al. (19) reported that the critical day for inhibi-tion was longer than that for promotion (critical night for re-moval of inhibition being 71/4 hr). Hence we studied timingeffects in the next two experiments.

Experiment 8 was designed to determine the critical nightfor removal of inhibition of the leaf base. The tip halves of theleaves were covered for 16 hr, and basal halves were coveredwith separate envelopes. The basal covers were removedperiodically throughout the 16-hr dark period, exposing thebasal halves to various dark periods (0-16 hr) followed by3000 ft-c of light from VHO fluorescent lamps and 200-w in-candescent bulbs. Figure 8 shows that the critical day forinhibition under our conditions is the same as that for pro-motion.

In experiment 9, the entire leaf was given a 16-hr darktreatment, interrupted only on the basal half with 5 min ofincandescent light at 50 ft-c. These light breaks were givento various groups of plants at 2-hr intervals throughout thedark period (Fig. 9). The shape of the resulting curve isvirtually identical with curves produced by illumination of theentire leaf, although inhibition at 8 hr would then be complete.Apparently, a light interruption of a long dark period (normallypromotive) causes the leaf to become actively inhibitory, in

C ontrol

0,O

.1T~~ __,.

a)0'

U,

- 3

11

786 Plant Physiol. Vol. 47, 1971

Page 4: Participation of Long-Day Inhibition in Flowering

LONG-DAY INHIBITION IN XANTHIUM FLOWERING

Treatment

Control, # 3 leaf 16 hr.dark

2

3

# 3 leaf 16 hr. dark,# 4 leaf in liqht

# 3 leaf 16 hr. dark,# 4 a # 5 leaves inlight

Av. Floral Stage

5.3 3±0.38

4.70t 0.48

4.80±0.30

FIG. 6. Effect of flowering of long-day leaves situated-below short-day promotive leaf.

Treatment Av. Floral Stage(Of remaining lower bud)

Control, all buds removedexcept one at node of #5leaf, # 3 leaf 16 hr. dark

Same as la control with# 4 leaf remaining andin light

Control, all buds removedexcept one at node of # 7leaf, # 3 leaf 16 hr. dark

Same as 2a control with# 6 leaf remaining andin light

FIG. 7. Inhibition of long-day leaves when situated between the promotive leaf and the flowering bud.

7

6 Tip Half of all Plants~~Given 16 hr. Dark

5 Period4

-30

i22

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16Length of Dark Period on Basal Half

FIG. 8. The critical night for removal of inhibition. The curveresulted from giving various dark periods to the basal half of theleaf while giving the tip 16 hr of dark.

this case to the products of the leaf tip tissue held in continuousdarkness. Indeed, in all of our kinetic experiments, it appearsthat when conditions do not allow promotion, they lead to an

active inhibition. Van Senden (25) in related work conducted

experiments in which leaves situated between induced leavesand the bud were made inhibitory by light interruptions in themiddle of their respective dark periods.

Experiment 10 was designed to test the ability of the inhibi-tor to move through dead tissue. Controls consisted of plantswith a single induced leaf (no. 3) and plants having both the

I I I i I I I6

5

02e 4t

2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16Time of Interruption on Basal Half

FIG. 9. Inhibition of a 5-min, 50-ft-c light break given at varioustimes during a 16-hr dark period to the basal half of the leaf. The16-hr dark period on the tip half was uninterrupted.

No.

N o.

la

lb t

2a

2bt

5.70+0.82

2.75±0.98

6.50t 1.35

3.55t1.39

I I I I

787PI-ant Physiol. Vol. 47, 1971

Page 5: Participation of Long-Day Inhibition in Flowering

GIBBY AND SALISBURY

Treatment

Control, # 3 leof 16 hr.dork

Av. Floral Stage

2.85±0.8E

# 3 loof 16 hr. dark, # 2 leafin light but with a section ofpetiole flame killed

# 3 leaf 16 hr. dark,V 2 leoaf in light

FiG. 10. Effect of killing the petiole with a small flame on the ability of leaf 2 to inhibit.

induced leaf 3 and the long-day leaf 2 intact. In test plants,leaf 3 was induced, but a section of the petiole of leaf 2 waspreviously killed with a small jet of flame. In Figure 10, theexpected long-day inhibition is clear cut (compare treatments1 and 3), but inhibition brought about by the leaf with theburned petiole is only slight (probably insignificant). It isimportant to note that that most of the no. 2 leaves with burnedpetioles appeared healthy and turgid for several days (75%were turgid after 10 days).

Experiment 11 was designed to study the effect of irondeficiency on inhibition. It was known that iron-deficientplants could not be induced to flower, or that they floweredabnormally even though they were placed in normal nutrientsolutions immediately following the photoinductive period(26). This result was apparently not due to low levels of carbo-hydrate, since applied sucrose failed to overcome the inhibi-tory effects of iron deficiency. Figure 11 shows the pattern ofapplication and the results. Controls were soil-grown plantsof the same age. Iron-deficient plants did not flower, butfoliar applications of iron only to the induced leaf reversed

No. Treatment Av. Floral Stage

iii.

Soil grown control#3 leof 16 hr. dark

Wi

e

Minus iron, corrected

2 z on # 3 leaf, # 3 leaf

16 hr. dark

Minus iron, # 3 leaf

3e|' 16 hr. dark

Minus iron, corrected on

#3 leaf, # 3 leaf 16 hr.

dark, # leaf

A Fe-% *;Fe Minus iron, corrected on #3&# 2 leaves, #3 leaf 16 hr.

dark, # 2 leaf in light

+Fe-%Minus iron, corrected on# 2 leaf, # 3 leaf 16 hr.

9

3.70

2.50

0.30

0.10

0.10

0.00

FIG. 11. Effect of iron deficiency on short-day promotion andlong-day inhibition.

this deficiency inhibition. Apparently iron deficiency affectsonly induction and not evocation. Furthermore, the no. 2leaves were capable of long-day inhibition whether or not theywere deficient in iron.

Experiment 12 was an attempt to learn whether the inhibitormoves or is stationary in the leaf. In all plants, the leaf tissuewas cut except for the large veins to separate the basal tissuefrom the tip tissue. This was done as a matter of course in theexperiments of Harder et al. (16). The tip half was then givena 16-hr inductive treatment, and the basal half was exposed to15-ft-c incandescent light. All plants remained vegetative.Two possible explanations occurred to us. First, the promotermoved through the veins where it partitioned back into thebasal tissue, leading to inhibition. Second, inhibitory sub-stances did move at least as far as the veins to destroy orinactivate the promoter.

DISCUSSION AND CONCLUSIONS

The first two experiments defined an inhibitory effect inflowering of Xanthium and led to five hypotheses. These,presented above, may be summarized briefly as follows:

1. The tip half of the leaf cannot synthesize florigen.2. A long-day inhibitor prevents subsequent florigen syn-

thesis.3. Long-day inhibition results from translocation effects.4. There is a translocatable long-day inhibitor.5. Long-day inhibition is localized and hence may be a con-

dition or a relatively immobile substance.Experiment 3 (removal of basal leaf tissue) clearly eliminates

the first hypothesis. It is also apparent from this and otherexperiments that the inhibitor is not acting upon subsequentflorigen synthesis, eliminating the second hypothesis. Severalexperiments have bearing on the translocation hypothesis. Verylow light intensities (well below the photosynthetic compen-

sation point) are sufficient for the long-day inhibition, makingthe translocation hypothesis suspect. Nevertheless, we planto study translocation effects more directly through the use of4C-labeled assimilates. If both an inhibitor and a promoterwere translocated to the bud where they competed for controlover development, it is certainly not apparent why long-daytissue has to be located between the induced leaf and the bud,making hypothesis 4 unlikely. Thus, of the explanations ap-

parent to us, only hypothesis 5, at best a rather nebulous andunsatisfying concept, remains.

Further experiments were carried out to characterize thelong-day inhibitory effect. Although the inhibition moves onlyslightly (tissue must be between induced leaf and bud-experi-ments 1, 2, 6, 7, and 12), it apparently moves some distanceout of the leaf (probably from leaf 2 to the stem). The effect

No.

3

2.33+1.33

0.95±0.40

788 Plant Physiol. Vol. 47, 1971

2

Page 6: Participation of Long-Day Inhibition in Flowering

LONG-DAY INHIBITION IN

will not pass through dead tissue, although the transpirationstream will. Inhibition is produced in iron-deficient tissue, al-though florigen is not.

In every respect studied by us, the kinetics of long-day in-hibition proved to be exactly opposite to those of florigenproduction. The light intensities just capable of inhibitingflorigen production are also just capable of promoting inhibi-tion. The critical night for promotion is essentially the sameas the critical night limiting inhibition. The kinetics of a lightbreak are essentially the same for inhibition of florigenproduction and for promotion of inhibition. It seems quiteclear that, when the leaf is not producing florigen, it is activelyproducing some inhibitory effect.A number of questions and consequences become apparent

from these experiments. Some of these suggest further experi-mentation.

First, how can the close interrelationships between thekinetics of florigen synthesis and long-day inhibition be re-lated to time measurement in the flowering process? Evidenceindicates that Xanthium plants oscillate in their sensitivity tolight through two phases (21, 23). When plants are in onephase, light is promotive for flowering; when in the otherphase, light is inhibitory. Future study might determine therelationship between this observation and those presented inthis paper.

Second, it has long been known (18) that grafting experi-ments designed to show transmission of florigen usually do notsucceed unless leaves are removed from the receptor plants.It has been assumed that this is because photosynthesizingleaf tissue prevents the receptor plants from acting as sinks.Threshold light intensities of this response should be studied.Perhaps receptor plants on long-day will not receive thefloral stimulus because of the inhibitory effects produced intheir leaf tissue.

Third, our results may help to explain the difficulties en-countered in development of a bioassay for florigen. If thelong-day inhibitory effect is a destruction of florigen, thenflorigen applied to plants on long-day should never be effective.Hodson and Hamner (17) and Carr (2) have reported thatbioassays are more effective in the presence of gibberellin.Does the gibberellin inactivate the long-day inhibitory effect?We have performed preliminary experiments to test thisquestion, and results were negative.

Fourth, our results suggest improved methods for a florigenbioassay. Substances should be applied to plants between long-day leaf tissue and the bud, or all leaves should be removed,or substances might be applied to iron-deficient plants on

short-day (neither florigen nor inhibitor should be produced insuch plants).

LITERATURE CITED

1. Bt-NNING, E. AND I. MOSER. 1959. Einfluss der Blattlage auf die Bluten-bildung. Naturwissenschaften 10: 519.

XANTHIUM FLOWERING 789

2. CARR, D. J. 1967. The relationship between florigen and the flowerhormones. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 144: 305-312.

3. CHAILAKHTAN, M. C. 1945. Photoperiodism of individual parts of the leaf,its halves. Dokl. Acad. Sai. USSR 47: 220. Reported by Naylor, A. W.1953. In: W. E. Loomis, ed., Reactions of Plants to Photoperiod inGrowth and Differentiation in Plants. Iowa State College Press.

4. CHAILAKHYAN, M. C. AND R. G. BuTENKO. 1957. Translocation of assimilatesfrom leaves to shoots during different photoperiodic regimes of plants.Fiziol. Rast. 4: 450.

5. EVANs, L. T. 1960. Infloresoence initiation in Lolium temulentum. II.Evidence for inhibitory and promotive photoperiodic processe involingtransmissive products. Aust. J. Biol. Sci. 13: 4240.

6. EVANs, L. T. 1962. Inflorescence initiation in Lolium temulentum. III. Theeffect of anaerobic conditions during photoperiodic induction. Aust. J.Biol. Sci. 15: 281-303.

7. EvANs, L. T. 1966. AbscisiD II: Inhibitory effect on flower induction in along-day plant. Science 151: 107-108.

8. EvANs, L. T. (ed.). 1969. The Induction of Flowering: Some Case Histories.Cornell University Press, Ithaca, N. Y.

9. EVANs, L. T. AND I. F. WARDLAW. 1964. Inflorescence initiation in Loliumtemulentum. IV. Translocation of the floral stimulus in relation to that ofassimilates. Aust. J. Biol. Sci. 17: 1-9.

10. FRATIANNE, D. G. 1965. The interrelationship between the flowering ofdodder and some long- and short-day plants. Amer. J. Bot. 52: 556562.

11. GUmIDGE, C. G. 1956. Photoperiodic promotion of vegetative growth inthe cultivated strawberry plant. Nature 178: 50-51.

12. GUrIDGE, C. G. 1959. Evidence for a flower inhibitor and a vegetativegrowth promoter in the strawberry. Ann. Bot. 23: 351-360.

13. GuTrTrDGc, C. G. 1959. Further evidence for a growth promoting and flowerinhibiting hormone in strawberry. Ann. Bot. 23: 612-621.

14. HAMNER, K. C. AND J. BoNNzB. 1938. Photoperiodism in relation to hormonesas factors in floral initiation and development. Bot. Gas. 100: 388431.

15. HARDER, R. AND R. BuNsow. 1954. UTber die Wirkung der Tageslange vorder Kurztaginduktion auf die Blutenbildung von Kalanchoe blossfeldiana.Planta 43: 315-325.

16. HARDER, R., M. WEsTPRAT, AND G. BEHRENS. 1949. Hemmung der In-floreszenbildung durch Langtag bei der Kurztagspflanze Kalanchoeblossfeldiana. Planta 36: 424438.

17. HODSON, H. K. AND K. C. HAMNER. 1969. Floral inducing extract for Xan-thium. Science 167: 384-385.

18. LAN-G, A. 1965. Physiology of flowering initiation. In: W. Ruhland, ed.,Handbuch der Pflanzenphysiologie (Encyclopedia of Plant Physiology),Vol. XV/1. Springer-Verlag, Berlin, New York. pp. 1381-1536.

19. LINCOLN, R. G., K. A. RAvEN, mNm K. C. HAMNER. 1956. Certain factorsinfluencing expression of the flowering stimulus in Xanfhium. I. Trans-location and inhibition of the flowering stimulus. Bot. Gaz. 117: 193.

20. LONA, F. 1947. 'Chimere Funzionali' come consequenza della localizzazione eautonomia del carattere fisiologico delle foglie in rapporto coi processi disviluppo della pianta. Lavori di Botanica, 277. Vol. pubbl. in occasionedel 70° genetliaco del Prof. G. Gola Rosenberg & Selli6r, Turin.

21. PAPENFUSS, H. D. AND F. B. SALIsBuRY. 1967. Properties of clock resettingin flowering of Xanthium. Plant Physiol. 43: 1562-1570.

22. SALISBURY, F. B. 1963. The Flowering Process. Pergamon Press, Oxford.23. SALISBURY, F. B. 1969. Xanthium strumarium L. In: L. T. Evans, ed., The

Induction of Flowering: Some Case Histories. Cornell University Press,Ithaca, N. Y. pp. 14-61.

24. SCHWABE, W. W. 1959. Studies of long-day inhibition in short-day plants.J. Exp. Bot. 10: 317-329.

25. SENDEN, H. VAN. 1951. Untersuchungen uber den Einfluss von Heteroauxinund anderen Faktoren auf die Bliitenbildung bei der KurztagpflanzeKalanchoe blossfeldiana. Biol. Zentralbl. 70: 537-565.

26. SMITH, J. H., W. J. MCILRATH, AND L. BOGORAD. 1956. Some effects of irondeficiency on flowering of Xanthium. Bot. Gaz. 118: 174-179.

27. THOMPSON, P. A. AND C. G. GUTTRIDGE. 1960. The role of leaves as in-hibitors of flower induction in strawberry. Ann. Bot. 24: 482-490.

Plant Physiol. Vol. 47, 1971