original article comprehensive psycholexical ... · cludes 1,641 adjectives and participles, 612...

13
current issues in personality psychology · 2019 doi: https://doi.org/10.5114/cipp.2019.82792 background Lexical research based on the assumption that all the main human characteristics are encoded in the natural language constitutes one of the alternative ways of developing a tax- onomy of individual differences in psychology. The major- ity of studies to date, including Polish ones, have been restricted to the analysis of the adjective lexicon, which means their results are at risk of reductionism. The aim of the presented research was to develop a classification of the complete Polish lexicon of person-descriptive terms (adjectives, participles, adverbs, nouns, and verbs). participants and procedure We analyzed 100,000 entries found in a universal diction- ary of Polish and identified 27,813 terms used to describe human characteristics. The identified person-descriptive terms were classified by a team of 13 trained judges into 16 subcategories. The judges’ taxonomic decisions were tested for validity and reliability. results Personality (dispositional) descriptors (5,598) constitute 20.1% of the Polish lexicon of person-descriptors; this in- cludes 1,641 adjectives and participles, 612 adverbs, 1,442 at- tribute-nouns, 1,029 type-nouns, and 916 verbs. The analysis of the semantic redundancy of terms representing different parts of speech but having the same common morpheme among dispositional descriptors revealed 1,979 morphemes with distinct meanings. Only 64% of these morphemes are represented by adjectives. conclusions Adjectives constitute the largest group of personality (dis- positional) descriptors but do not account for the entire Polish personality lexicon. The results of the study are a point of departure for research into the specificity of the structure of personality descriptions in the Polish lan- guage using various linguistic categories and for a com- prehensive study on the entire Polish personality lexicon. key words individual differences; psycholexical taxonomy; personal- ity lexicon; Polish language Comprehensive psycholexical classification of Polish person-descriptive terms corresponding author – Oleg Gorbaniuk, Ph.D., Institute of Psychology, The John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin, Al. Racławickie 14, 20-950 Lublin, Poland, e-mail: [email protected] authors’ contribution – A: Study design · B: Data collection · C: Statistical analysis · D: Data interpretation · E: Manuscript preparation · F: Literature search · G: Funds collection to cite this article – Gorbaniuk, O., Korczak, A., Toruj, N., Czarnejko, A., Macheta, K., Jałoszewska, M., Charęzińska- Nowocień, M., Karpiński, R., Garwoliński, Ł., Misiuro, T., Zygnerska, M., Bojan, I., Rykowska, K., Wawrzaszek, P., Jeliński, J., & Hawryluk, M. (2019). Comprehensive psycholexical classification of Polish person-descriptive terms. Current Issues in Personality Psychology. received 13.11.2018 · reviewed 11.01.2019 · accepted 14.01.2019 · published 07.02.2019 original article Oleg Gorbaniuk 1,2 · A,B,C,D,E,F,G , Anna Korczak 1 · C,D,F , Nasturcja Toruj 1 · B,D , Anna Czarnejko 1 · B,D , Krystian Macheta 1,3 · D,F , Magdalena Jałoszewska 1 · B,D , Małgorzata Charęzińska-Nowocień 1 · B,D , Rafał Karpiński 1 · B,D , Łukasz Garwoliński 1 · B,D , Tomasz Misiuro 2 · B,D,G , Milena Zygnerska 1 · B,D , Iwona Bojan 1 · D , Karolina Rykowska 1 · D , Paula Wawrzaszek 1 · D , Juliusz Jeliński 1 · D , Marta Hawryluk 1 · D 1: Institute of Psychology, The John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin, Poland 2: Institute of Psychology, University of Zielona Góra, Poland 3: Department of Psychology, Pedagogical University of Cracow, Poland

Upload: others

Post on 09-Jul-2020

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: original article Comprehensive psycholexical ... · cludes 1,641 adjectives and participles, 612 adverbs, 1,442 at-tribute-nouns, 1,029 type-nouns, and 916 verbs. The analysis of

current issues in personality psychology · 2019doi: https://doi.org/10.5114/cipp.2019.82792

backgroundLexical research based on the assumption that all the main human characteristics are encoded in the natural language constitutes one of the alternative ways of developing a tax-onomy of individual differences in psychology. The major-ity of studies to date, including Polish ones, have been restricted to the analysis of the adjective lexicon, which means their results are at risk of reductionism. The aim of the presented research was to develop a classification of the complete Polish lexicon of person-descriptive terms (adjectives, participles, adverbs, nouns, and verbs).

participants and procedureWe analyzed 100,000 entries found in a universal diction-ary of Polish and identified 27,813 terms used to describe human characteristics. The identified person-descriptive terms were classified by a team of 13 trained judges into 16 subcategories. The judges’ taxonomic decisions were tested for validity and reliability.

resultsPersonality (dispositional) descriptors (5,598) constitute 20.1% of the Polish lexicon of person-descriptors; this in-

cludes 1,641 adjectives and participles, 612 adverbs, 1,442 at-tribute-nouns, 1,029 type-nouns, and 916 verbs. The analysis of the semantic redundancy of terms representing different parts of speech but having the same common morpheme among dispositional descriptors revealed 1,979 morphemes with distinct meanings. Only 64% of these morphemes are represented by adjectives.

conclusionsAdjectives constitute the largest group of personality (dis-positional) descriptors but do not account for the entire Polish personality lexicon. The results of the study are a  point of departure for research into the specificity of the structure of personality descriptions in the Polish lan-guage using various linguistic categories and for a  com-prehensive study on the entire Polish personality lexicon.

key wordsindividual differences; psycholexical taxonomy; personal-ity lexicon; Polish language

Comprehensive psycholexical classification of Polish person-descriptive terms

corresponding author – Oleg Gorbaniuk, Ph.D., Institute of Psychology, The John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin, Al. Racławickie 14, 20-950 Lublin, Poland, e-mail: [email protected]

authors’ contribution – A: Study design · B: Data collection · C: Statistical analysis · D: Data interpretation · E: Manuscript preparation · F: Literature search · G: Funds collection

to cite this article – Gorbaniuk, O., Korczak, A., Toruj, N., Czarnejko, A., Macheta, K., Jałoszewska, M., Charęzińska-Nowocień, M., Karpiński, R., Garwoliński, Ł., Misiuro, T., Zygnerska, M., Bojan, I., Rykowska, K., Wawrzaszek, P., Jeliński, J., & Hawryluk, M. (2019). Comprehensive psycholexical classification of Polish person-descriptive terms. Current Issues in Personality Psychology.

received 13.11.2018 · reviewed 11.01.2019 · accepted 14.01.2019 · published 07.02.2019

original article

Oleg Gorbaniuk1,2 · A,B,C,D,E,F,G, Anna Korczak1 · C,D,F, Nasturcja Toruj1 · B,D, Anna Czarnejko1 · B,D, Krystian Macheta1,3 · D,F, Magdalena Jałoszewska1 · B,D, Małgorzata Charęzińska-Nowocień1 · B,D, Rafał Karpiński1 · B,D, Łukasz Garwoliński1 · B,D, Tomasz Misiuro2 · B,D,G, Milena Zygnerska1 · B,D, Iwona Bojan1 · D, Karolina Rykowska1 · D, Paula Wawrzaszek1 · D, Juliusz Jeliński1 · D, Marta Hawryluk1 · D

1: Institute of Psychology, The John Paul II Catholic University of Lublin, Poland

2: Institute of Psychology, University of Zielona Góra, Poland

3: Department of Psychology, Pedagogical University of Cracow, Poland

Page 2: original article Comprehensive psycholexical ... · cludes 1,641 adjectives and participles, 612 adverbs, 1,442 at-tribute-nouns, 1,029 type-nouns, and 916 verbs. The analysis of

Comprehensive psycholexical classification

2 current issues in personality psychology

Background

The taxonomy of the abundance of individual differ-ences between people is one of the important areas of interest in psychology. One of the approaches, which has made it possible to reach relatively high taxonomic agreement among researchers, is the lexi-cal approach. The use of a  similar methodology in research devoted to various natural languages has contributed to a  considerable degree of consensus about the structure of characteristics universal across cultures, meeting most of the requirements for struc-tural models (Saucier & Srivastava, 2015). The point of departure in the lexical approach is the lexical as-sumption, originally proposed by Galton (1884) and reformulated nearly 100 years later by Goldberg (1981). According to him, the most important indi-vidual differences have been observed by people and named in the course of evolution in order for indi-viduals to be able to communicate them to others. If these observations were regarded as useful by other users of a given language and if they are still valid, the terms referring to the observed differences have survived until the present day and function as part of the lexicon of that language. The more important an individual difference is, the more terms have been created to refer to this difference, taking the form of synonyms and antonyms, for instance. Therefore, in order to identify the individual differences that are the most important to the users of a given language, one should analyze the structure of the language used to describe them. Currently, studies devoted to the structure of personality traits constitute the largest number of studies on the lexicon of individ-ual differences. Their outcomes include the Big Five (Goldberg, 1990; Peabody & De Raad, 2002), the Big Six (Ashton et al., 2004), or the Big Seven (Almagor, Tellegen, & Waller, 1995). The Big Five and the Big Six became the basis for developing, respectively, the five-factor model (OCEAN/FFM; McCrae &  Costa, 1985, 1987) and the six-factor model (HEXACO; Lee &  Ashton, 2004, 2006) of personality, which have been measured by means of numerous question-naires often used to measure personality traits (e.g., NEO-FFI: Costa, McCrae, &  Dye, 1991; BFI: John & Srivastava, 1999).

A personality study can be called lexical and its results can be compared with the results of studies devoted to other natural languages if it is based on a  representative sample of the personality lexicon of that language and if the factor structure of this list of personality terms has been established on the basis of a  representative sample of personality de-scriptions generated by the users of that language (De Raad, 1998; De Raad & Ostendorf, 1996; Saucier, 1997). Due to the time-consuming nature of lexical research, the structure of the personality lexicon has been explored to date in about 30 natural languages

out of the several thousand that exist in the world, which still limits the possibilities of formulating conclusions about personality traits universal across different cultures. Moreover, the method applied in research on the languages investigated so far is not free from limitations; the most important one among them is the fact that research on the lexicon of individual differences was restricted to the adjec-tive lexicon. This involves the risk of reductionism: a personality trait could be exclusively a character-istic that it is possible to describe by means of an adjective, and what cannot be described by means of adjectives would not be considered a personality de-scriptor, which goes against the lexical assumption. Both in the case of the languages examined so far and in the case of those not examined yet, there is a need to verify whether the non-adjectival person-ality lexicon describes the same personality traits, or whether some of them have been ignored so far due to the error of reductionism.

Psychologists tend to give personality a  rather wide definition while measuring it with instruments that capture only a part of this grand, inclusive range (Saucier, 2009a). Also in psycholexical studies, we can distinguish two approach based on a narrow vs. a wide operational definition of personality, which is expressed in how one selects variables when study-ing the phenomena of personality (Saucier, 2009b). According to the restrictive/narrow approach, per-sonality dispositions are indicated only by terms that primarily describe an enduring pattern of typi-cal behavioral tendency in action, thought, and emo-tions (Ashton & Lee, 2005). This approach excludes terms which are considered as highly evaluative (e.g., dumb, sleazy, backward) or referring to states (e.g., anxious, pensive, frustrated) or describing ef-fects an individual has on others (e.g., frustrating, astonishing, boring) – either at the stage of compil-ing the personality lexicon or at the stage of research aimed at identifying the factor structure of person-ality descriptions. According to the non-restrictive/inclusive approach, most above-mentioned terms are included in the study of structure of personality lexicon (e.g., Almagor et  al., 1995; Benet-Martinez & Waller, 1997; Tellegen & Waller, 1987). Inclusive variable selection enables the researcher to find ad-ditional sources of variance of personality descrip-tions (Saucier, 2009a).

Comprehensive and unrestrictive research, based on the complete lexicon of individual differences potentially useful in personality description, has so far been conducted only in the Dutch language (De Raad & Barelds, 2008). Studies of specific categories of individual differences such as pure evaluations, social effects or worldviews have been conducted only in a few languages (De Raad, Van Oudenhoven, & Hofstede, 2005; Krauss, 2006; Mlačić, 2016; Sauci-er, 2000, 2010).

Page 3: original article Comprehensive psycholexical ... · cludes 1,641 adjectives and participles, 612 adverbs, 1,442 at-tribute-nouns, 1,029 type-nouns, and 916 verbs. The analysis of

O. Gorbaniuk, A. Korczak, N. Toruj, A. Czarnejko, K. Macheta, M. Jałoszewska, M. Charęzińska-Nowocień, R. Karpiński, Ł. Garwoliński, T. Misiuro, M. Zygnerska, I. Bojan, K. Rykowska, P. Wawrzaszek, J. Jeliński, M. Hawryluk

39

research oBjective

Polish is a natural language belonging to the West Slavic group of languages and to the Indo-European family of languages. It is estimated that Polish is the mother tongue for approximately 40 million peo-ple (Simons &  Fennig, 2018). Although the Polish language has been twice subjected to independent psycholexical analyses (Szarota, 1995; Gorbaniuk, Czarnecka, & Chmurzyńska, 2011) and is included in nearly all comparisons aimed at identifying cul-turally universal personality traits (e.g., Ashton et al., 2004; De Raad et al., 2010; De Raad, Perugini, Hrebícková, &  Szarota, 1998; Peabody &  De Raad, 2002), both lexical studies were limited to adjectives only. The aim of the presented research was to sys-tematize and classify the complete domain of Polish person-descriptive terms based on homogeneous and up-to-date lexical data in order to provide the basis for future quantitative studies on the factor structure of individual differences in the Polish lan-guage. This main objective can be made more spe-cific in the form of the following research questions:Q1: How many person-descriptive terms are there in Polish? What proportion of Polish person-descriptive terms does each word class (nouns, adjectives, ad-verbs, verbs, and participles) account for?Q2: What is the proportion of dispositional descrip-tors out of all categories of person-descriptive terms in Polish?Q3: Does the psycholexical structure of Polish per-son-descriptive terms differ across word classes?Q4: Do adjectives morphemically and semantically account for the entire Polish personality lexicon?Q5: Do Polish dispositional adjectives account for all the markers of the cross-language Big Factors?

Due to the exploratory character of the study, we did not formulate research hypotheses.

Method

The selecTion of person-descripTive Terms

As the basis for the selection of words describing individual differences in Polish, we chose the elec-tronic version of The Universal Dictionary of the Polish Language (Uniwersalny Słownik Języka Pol-skiego; Dubisz, 2008), containing 100,000 entries. According to the independent opinions of six top Polish linguists whom we consulted about the choice, this dictionary was the most complete and up-to-date source of the Polish lexicon at the be-ginning of the selection process (in 2014). The se-lection of person-descriptive terms was carried out by eight graduate (fourth- and fifth-year) and doc-toral (third- and fourth-year) psychology students

writing their MA theses and doctoral dissertations in the field of psycholexical research. Before com-mencing the selection, all the judges went through three-month theoretical and practical training, con-cluded by writing a chapter of their diploma thesis devoted to a review of the previous selections and classifications of person-descriptive terms. During practical training, the judges twice performed trial selection based on 200 words; the results of the trial selection were discussed in the light of the adopted selection criteria.

Person-descriptive terms are defined in this study as all of the attributes differentiating people. When deciding on whether a given word was a person-de-scriptive term, the judges applied the selection crite-ria and test sentences (see Table 1) proposed by An-gleitner, Ostendorf and John (1990). The following categories of words were used as exclusion criteria: (1) words that do not differentiate people from one another and that describe the characteristics of all people; (2) words that refer to geographical origin, nationality, citizenship, and religious or professional affiliation; (3) terms that are rarely understood as de-scribing human characteristics; (4) words describing colors or their shades; (5) numerals.

The selection of person-descriptive terms was performed separately for each of the four groups of lexical items: (1) adjectives, participles, adverbs; (2) attribute-nouns; (3) type-nouns; (4) verbs. This was done by a different pair of judges, each of them working independently. In the case of doubts re-garding whether a given word met the criteria for a  person-descriptive term, the judge put it on the list because in subsequent stages the list was rated by a larger number of judges. In their decisions, the judges first relied on their own understanding of the word and then on its meaning as defined in the dic-tionary; they copied the definition that suggested the usefulness of the word in describing human char-acteristics into the database and supplemented the definition with their own when they decided that the dictionary definition was insufficient in the light of the word’s practical usage. This made it possible to minimize both the influence of particular judges’ linguistic competence on selection results and the effect of additional meanings of the analyzed word, significant in describing individual differences, not being included in the dictionary.

We tentatively classified all the words selected by at least one judge as person-descriptive terms, so as to minimize the likelihood of excluding any words during selection that can be used to describe human characteristics. The selection of person-descriptive terms representing different word classes mini-mized the likelihood of missing a morpheme based on which it is possible to form useful descriptors of individual differences. Differences in the selection of words were then analyzed by one PhD holder in

Page 4: original article Comprehensive psycholexical ... · cludes 1,641 adjectives and participles, 612 adverbs, 1,442 at-tribute-nouns, 1,029 type-nouns, and 916 verbs. The analysis of

Comprehensive psycholexical classification

4 current issues in personality psychology

psychology and two doctoral students in psychology. The final list representing all word classes contained 27,819 person-descriptive terms.

psycholexical classificaTion procedure

The person-descriptive terms selected from the dic-tionary were classified by 13 judges preparing their MA theses and doctoral dissertations in psycholexical research, whose mother tongue was Polish. For the purpose of the study, we adapted the taxonomy pro-cedure from a German lexical study (Angleitner et al., 1990), which is one of the most often used procedures in this kind of research and which offers the widest

possibilities of subsequently applying the results of classification both in the inclusive/non-restrictive approach and in the narrow/restrictive approach. First, the judges decided whether or not a given word was comprehensible to them. Next, based on his or her own understanding of the word as well as on the dictionary definition, each judge decided whether or not a  given word qualified as a  person-descriptive term. In the third step, the judges rated the valence of the word on a 5-point scale (1 – strongly negative, 2  –  negative, 3  –  neutral, 4  –  positive, 5  –  strongly positive). After the judges’ ratings were averaged, the words for which the mean rating ranged from 1.00 to 2.74 were classified as negative, those with mean ratings between 2.75 and 3.25 as neutral, and those with mean ratings between 3.26 and 5.00 as positive.

Table 1

Test sentences used in the selection of person-descriptive terms

Word class Heuristic criterion sentence Example

Adjectives and participles

Jan jest [przymiotnik].John is [adjective].

łagodny [gentle or mild], sprawiedliwy [fair, just], przystojny [handsome], pobożny [pious]

Jak bardzo [przymiotnik] jestem?How [adjective] am I?

wysoki [tall], uczuciowy [emotional, sentimental], biedny [poor], pobudzony [excited]

Adverbs Jak (bardzo) [przysłówek] Jan się zachowuje?How [adverb] did John behave?

uczciwie [honestly], spokojnie [calmly]

Attribute nouns

[Rzeczownik] Jana jest zadziwiające.John’s [noun] is remarkable.

zrównoważenie [balanced], skupienie [focusing], prawdomówność [truthfulness]

Czy on/ona ma/posiada [rzeczownik]?Does he/she have or possess [noun]?

cierpliwość [patience], logikę [logic], wynalazczość [inventiveness]

W jakim stopniu cechuje go [rzeczownik]?To what extent [noun] characterizes him?

kreatywność [creativity], kłamliwość [falseness], słowność [reliability], roztargnienie [distraction]

Type-nouns Czy Jan jest [rzeczownik]?Is John a [noun]?

konserwatystą [conservative], ojcem [father], cholerykiem [choleric]

Czy można go/ją nazwać [rzeczownik]?Can you call him/her a [noun]?

tchórzem [coward], zdrajcą [traitor], zbawicielem [savior]

Verbs Jan jest osobą, która często/rzadko/nigdy [czasownik].John is a person who often/rarely/never [verb].

płacze [cries], spóźnia się [is late], denerwuje [annoys], medytuje [meditates]

Jan jest osobą, która umie/potrafi lepiej/gorzej od Pawła [czasownik].John is a person who can [verb] better/worse than Paul.

kłamać [lie], tańczyć [dance], pracować [work]

Jan lubi [czasownik]. John likes [verb].

ponarzekać [complain], napsioczyć [grumble]

Jan często/nigdy/rzadko [czasownik] innych. John often/rarely/never [verbs] other people.

frustruje [frustrates], godzi [reconciles]

Page 5: original article Comprehensive psycholexical ... · cludes 1,641 adjectives and participles, 612 adverbs, 1,442 at-tribute-nouns, 1,029 type-nouns, and 916 verbs. The analysis of

O. Gorbaniuk, A. Korczak, N. Toruj, A. Czarnejko, K. Macheta, M. Jałoszewska, M. Charęzińska-Nowocień, R. Karpiński, Ł. Garwoliński, T. Misiuro, M. Zygnerska, I. Bojan, K. Rykowska, P. Wawrzaszek, J. Jeliński, M. Hawryluk

59

Every word classified as a person-descriptive term was further classified into one of 16 subcategories, which in turn made up seven higher-order catego-ries: (1) dispositions; (1a) temperament and charac-ter; (1b) mental abilities and talents or their absence; (2)  temporary conditions; (2a) emotions, internal states, and readiness to react; (2b) bodily states; (2c) ac-tivities and behaviors; (3) social aspects; (3a) roles and relationships; (3b) hobbies and interests; (3c) social effects: reactions of others; (3d) pure evaluations; (3e) attitudes and worldviews; (4) overt characteris-tics and appearance; (4a) anatomy, constitution, and morphology; (4b) external attributes: appearance and posture; (5) specialist and technical terms referring to (5a) mental/psychological or (5b) physical chronic diseases or disorders; (6) physical abilities and talents or their absence; (7) metaphorical terms. The last cat-egory was non-classifiable terms.

The original classification proposal by Angleitner et al. (1990) was modified in order to streamline the process of judges making classification decisions as well as based on the experience acquired when per-forming the taxonomy of the lexicon of individual differences in Belarusian (Gorbaniuk et  al., 2014), Ukrainian (Gorbaniuk et al., 2018), and Russian (Iva-nova et al., 2017) and in the course of earlier research on the Polish lexicon (Gorbaniuk et al., 2011; Gorba-niuk, Budzińska, Owczarek, Bożek, &  Juros, 2013). The “specialist, rare, and technical terms” category was too general and vaguely defined, which made the judges’ decisions arbitrary and marked by low inter-judge agreement in this category. For this reason, we narrowed it down to a more precisely defined cate-gory: “specialist and technical terms referring to (5a) mental/psychological or (5b) physical chronic diseas-es or disorders,” and at the same time distinguished “metaphorical terms” as a  separate category. We distinguished “physical abilities and talents or their absence” as an additional category, modeled on the taxonomy of the Filipino language (Church, Katigbak, & Reyes, 1996) and absent from the original classifi-cation system: abilities that influence strength, flex-ibility, endurance, balance and coordination. Also, for the same reasons, we introduced the “hobbies and in-terests” subcategory, thus reducing the number of un-classified person-descriptive terms which described this kind of individual differences, and at the same time making the classification process faster.

Due to the ambiguity of some of the words, the judges were allowed to classify a person-descriptive term into more than one lexical category/subcat-egory. A term was eventually assigned to a particular category if it was classified into it by at least seven of the thirteen judges. The rating process took 16 to 18 weeks. The judges were supposed to send a classi-fied 1/16 of the list of person-descriptive terms at the end of each week – a requirement that was meant to motivate them to work systematically.

caTegorizaTion validiTy and reliabiliTy

Before the judges commenced their classification work, they twice went through group-based lexical training and performed a  trial classification of two lists of words, containing 115 and 200 terms, respec-tively. After every trial classification the judges re-ceived individual feedback concerning the correctness of the classification; typical errors were discussed in a group, and errors specific to particular judges were discussed with them on an individual basis.

To assess the validity of the classification, we se-lected 240 adjectives that had been classified into subcategories in the course of the earlier lexical taxonomy of the Polish language (Gorbaniuk et al., 2011) by at least eight out of nine judges, recognizing them as prototypal of their respective subcategories and treating them as the external criterion when as-sessing the validity of judges’ decisions in the cur-rent study of the Polish language lexicon. After thor-ough training, each of the 13 judges independently decided whether or not the adjectives met the criteria for person-descriptive terms and classified them into 16 categories/subcategories. Next, we computed the proportion of decisions consistent with the external criterion in the total number of prototypal adjectives from a given category/subcategory (the first column in Table 2). As the second validity index, we comput-ed Cohen’s κ coefficient of agreement between the judge’s decision and the external criterion based on all the 240 adjectives (the second column in Table 2), taking into account the false alarms that decrease the validity level. The average validity of classifications into categories ranged from 69.2% to 95.7% (Cohen’s κ: from .63 to .92) for higher-order categories and from 73.1% to 95.9% (Cohen’s κ: from .74 to .98) for subcategories. These validity figures can be regarded as satisfactory (Fleiss, 1981).

The last two columns of Table 2 contain infor-mation concerning inter-rater composite reliability based on the classifications of the list of 27,813 terms by 13 judges. Alpha coefficients ranged from .82 to .97 for higher-order categories and from .64 to .98 for subcategories. In general, the inter-rater reliability coefficients should be regarded as satisfactory. Com-pared to the levels of composite reliability (cf.  An-gleitner et  al., 1990; Di Blas &  Forzi, 1998; Mlačić & Ostendorf, 2005; Szarota, Ashton, & Lee, 2007) and validity (cf. Angleitner et al., 1990) in other similar taxonomies, they also turn out to be very good.

results

The results of the analyses of the contents of the universal dictionary of the Polish language (Dubisz, 2008) allowed research question Q1 to be answered:

Page 6: original article Comprehensive psycholexical ... · cludes 1,641 adjectives and participles, 612 adverbs, 1,442 at-tribute-nouns, 1,029 type-nouns, and 916 verbs. The analysis of

Comprehensive psycholexical classification

6 current issues in personality psychology

the percentage of words in the dictionary describ-ing differences between people is 27.8% (see Table 3). Verbs form the largest group of person-descriptive terms, accounting for 40.3%. The second largest group consists of nouns (34.2%), including attribute-nouns (19.0%) and type-nouns (15.2%). They are followed by adjectives, whose proportion among person-de-scriptive terms is 19.5%. The smallest group is that of adverbs (6.0%). Because the authors of the dictionary did not include participles other than those classified as adjectival participles and used as person-descrip-tive adjectives (e.g., zrównoważony [even-tempered], otwarty [frank]), we did not distinguish participles as a category distinct from adjectives (cf. Saloni, 2012).

In the study, we adopted a narrow operational def-inition of personality traits by narrowing its range to

descriptors of traits that are psychological in nature and relatively stable over time, i.e. (1a) to descrip-tors of temperament and character, for which almost everyone would agree are attributes of personality (cf.  Saucier, 1997), and (1b) to descriptors of mental abilities and talents or their absence. Answering re-search question Q2, it was established that in the total lexicon of person-descriptive terms, 20.1% are words describing personality traits (see Table 3). The larg-est proportion of dispositional descriptors among dif-ferent word classes in the analyzed Polish language dictionary is found in the case of adverbs (36.5%, e.g., powściągliwie [guardedly], rozważnie [prudently], samokrytycznie [self-critically], impulsywnie [impul-sively]), type-nouns (33.9%, e.g., gaduła [chatterbox], pracuś [eager beaver, workaholic], perfekcjonista

Table 2

Validity and reliability of judges’ decisions

Category/subcategory Criterion validity Inter-judge agreement

P κ α

Person-descriptive term 80.0 .79 –

1. Dispositions 93.6 .83 .90

1a. temperament and character 89.2 .74 .91

1b. mental/psychological abilities, talents, or their absence 82.6 .81 .86

2. Temporary conditions 95.7 .91 .82

2a. experiential states 95.4 .85 .77

2b. physical and bodily states 92.3 .89 .88

2c. observable activities 88.7 .92 .64

3. Social and reputational aspects 94.5 .92 .89

3a. roles and relationships 88.2 .93 .94

3b. hobbies and interests 92.3 .96 .92

3c. social effects: reactions of others 88.7 .92 .82

3d. pure evaluations 83.1 .75 .86

3e. attitudes and worldviews 96.4 .98 .98

4. Overt characteristics and appearance 95.6 .89 .95

4a. anatomy, constitution, and morphology 85.1 .86 .94

4b. appearance and looks 95.9 .86 .89

5. Diseases and disorders 81.5 .90 .97

5a. mental/psychological 73.1 .89 .95

5b. physical 81.5 .84 .96

6. Physical abilities, talents, or their absence 83.8 .85 .84

7. Metaphorical terms 69.2 .63 .87Note. P – mean percentage of decisions consistent with the external criterion; κ – average Cohen’s kappa coefficient of agreement with the external criterion, α – inter-rater composite reliability coefficient based on the classifications of 27,813 person-descriptive terms

Page 7: original article Comprehensive psycholexical ... · cludes 1,641 adjectives and participles, 612 adverbs, 1,442 at-tribute-nouns, 1,029 type-nouns, and 916 verbs. The analysis of

O. Gorbaniuk, A. Korczak, N. Toruj, A. Czarnejko, K. Macheta, M. Jałoszewska, M. Charęzińska-Nowocień, R. Karpiński, Ł. Garwoliński, T. Misiuro, M. Zygnerska, I. Bojan, K. Rykowska, P. Wawrzaszek, J. Jeliński, M. Hawryluk

79

Table 3

The psycholexical classification of Polish person-descriptive terms

Category/subcategory All word classes

Adjectives & parti-ciples

Adverbs Attribute-nouns

Type-nouns

Verbs

f % f % f % f % f % f %

1. Dispositions 5598 20.1 1641 30.0 612 36.5 1442 33.9 1029 19.3 916 8.1

1a. temperament and character

4083 14.7 1362 24.9 519 30.9 1090 25.6 839 15.7 302 2.7

1b. mental/psychological abilities, talents, or their absence

1020 3.7 225 4.1 60 3.6 287 6.8 154 2.9 305 2.7

2. Temporary conditions 7502 27.0 677 12.4 127 7.6 609 14.3 68 1.3 6050 53.6

2a. experiential states 2360 8.5 404 7.4 115 6.9 466 11.0 22 0.4 1367 12.1

2b. physical and bodily states

807 2.9 226 4.1 9 0.5 129 3.0 37 0.7 418 3.7

2c. observable activities 3959 14.2 53 1.0 6 0.4 41 1.0 11 0.2 3855 34.1

3. Social and reputational aspects 13311 47.9 3267 59.7 1136 67.7 1884 44.3 4505 84.4 2664 23.6

3a. roles and relationships 3843 13.8 520 9.5 46 2.7 230 5.4 2650 49.6 469 4.2

3b. hobbies and interests 327 1.2 26 0.5 1 0.1 42 1.0 219 4.1 39 0.3

3c. social effects: reactions of others

1084 3.9 233 4.3 147 8.8 63 1.5 19 0.4 626 5.5

3d. pure evaluations 5232 18.8 1919 35.1 861 51.3 917 21.6 1083 20.3 515 4.6

3e. attitudes and worldviews

1633 5.9 446 8.2 60 3.6 475 11.2 530 9.9 129 1.1

4. Overt characteristics and appearance 1250 4.5 610 11.2 12 0.7 103 2.4 303 5.7 238 2.1

4a. anatomy, constitution, and morphology

635 2.3 363 6.6 6 0.4 58 1.4 186 3.5 33 0.3

4b. appearance and looks 502 1.8 215 3.9 3 0.2 20 0.5 90 1.7 179 1.6

5. Diseases and disorders 966 3.5 135 2.5 15 0.9 599 14.1 224 4.2 10 0.1

5a. mental/psychological 360 1.3 60 1.1 12 0.7 171 4.0 114 2.1 8 0.1

5b. physical 601 2.2 71 1.3 2 0.1 437 10.3 103 1.9 1 0.0

6. Physical abilities, talents, or their absence

250 0.9 62 1.1 17 1.0 54 1.3 76 1.4 42 0.4

7. Metaphorical terms 1215 4.4 223 4.1 31 1.8 132 3.1 260 4.9 573 5.1

Terms without majority classifications

2169 7.8 80 1.5 45 2.7 103 2.4 6 0.1 1936 17.1

Terms in the initial pool 27813 100.0 5469 100.0 1678 100.0 4250 100.0 5338 100.0 11294 100.0

Page 8: original article Comprehensive psycholexical ... · cludes 1,641 adjectives and participles, 612 adverbs, 1,442 at-tribute-nouns, 1,029 type-nouns, and 916 verbs. The analysis of

Comprehensive psycholexical classification

8 current issues in personality psychology

[perfectionist]), and adjectives (30.0%, e.g., leniwy [lazy], energiczny [energetic], kreatywny [creative]). In the case of verbs, this proportion is the lowest (8.1%, e.g., lękać się [to fear], izolować się [to seclude oneself], chytrzyć [to scheme]).

In answer to research question Q3 we compared the psycholexical structure of different word classes. One will observe that a characteristic feature of verbs is a very high proportion of words describing states (53.6%), particularly behavioral states (34.1%), due to the considerable proportion of action verbs (e.g., dzwonić [to call], jąkać się [to stutter], lecieć [to fly]), which should be considered an element of the spec-ificity of this part of speech. The situation is quite the contrary in this respect in the case of attribute-nouns, among which we identified only 1.3% of state descriptors (e.g., pokrzykiwanie [shouting], drżenie [trembling]).

The predominant group among attribute-nouns comprises descriptors of individuals’ social function-ing (84.4%); words describing social roles and rela-tions are particularly numerous (49.6%; e.g., brater-stwo [brotherhood], podrzędność [subordination], niepełnoletność [minority]). The proportion of attri-bute-nouns in the total number of person-descriptive terms classified into this subcategory is 69.9%.

In the pure evaluation subcategory, adjectives and adverbs account for the largest proportion. Ev-ery third adjective (35.1%, e.g., brzydki [ugly], cenny [precious], dziadowski [shabby]) and every second adverb (51.3%, e.g., fenomenalnie [phenomenally], fajnie [great], wadliwie [defectively]) was classified into the pure evaluation subcategory.

In worldview description, nouns are much more important (accounting for 61.5% of worldview de-scriptors): 11.2% of type-nouns (e.g., rusofob [Rus-sophobe], scjentysta [scientist], lewicowiec [left-winger]) and 9.9% of attribute-nouns (e.g., faszyzm [fascism], stoicyzm [stoicism], liberalizm [liberal-ism]) were classified into this subcategory.

Among the words found in the dictionary and classified as external appearance descriptors, ad-

jectives make up the largest group (48.8%) – even though descriptors of external attributes constitute only 11.2% of all adjectives (e.g., chudy [thin], wysoki [tall], umięśniony [muscular]).

Among the words in the dictionary describing mental and physical disorders, type-nouns account for the largest proportion (62.0%, e.g., dyslektyk [dys-lexic], schizofrenik [schizophrenic], lunatyk [som-nambulist]).

The frequency of words whose metaphorical meanings refer to human characteristics is similar for various word classes and ranges from 1.8% (for ad-verbs, e.g., paraliżująco [paralyzingly], gorączkowo [feverishly], szatańsko [devilishly]) to 5.1% (for verbs, e.g., puszyć się [to put on airs], wykoleić się [to become demoralized], skamienieć [to petrify]).

Terms describing individual differences can be an-alyzed not only in terms of content but also in terms of valence (see Table 4). The results of analyses show that the proportion of negative descriptors (47.2%) is the highest in the lexicon of person-descriptive terms, while positive characteristics are in a minority (only 17.9%). These proportions strongly differ across the types of word classes. A characteristic feature of verbs and attribute nouns is the much higher propor-tion of neutral descriptors (43.1% and 42.7%, respec-tively) than in the case of the remaining word classes. Negative descriptors are predominant in the case of type-nouns (56.6%) and adjectives (51.2%).

In the psycholexical studies conducted to date, dispositional descriptors (restrictive/narrow ap-proach) enjoyed the greatest interest. Table 5 pres-ents a comparison of different types of word classes describing personality traits in terms of their va-lence. The proportion of neutral terms among per-sonality descriptors is approximately two times lower in each type of word classes than their propor-tion among all person-descriptors. The proportion of neutral dispositional descriptors is the highest in the case of verbs (27.6%). Negative descriptors are predominant in the remaining types of word classes, accounting for more than half of all terms in their

Table 4

Valence of person-descriptive terms

Valence All word classes

Word class

Adjectives & participles

Adverbs Attribute-nouns

Type-nouns Verbs

f % f % f % f % f % f %

Negative 13170 47.4 2798 51.2 817 48.7 2407 56.6 2389 44.8 4889 43.3

Neutral 9652 34.7 1372 25.1 321 19.1 850 20.0 2278 42.7 4863 43.1

Positive 4991 17.9 1299 23.8 540 32.2 993 23.4 671 12.6 1542 13.7

Total 27813 100.0 5469 100.0 1678 100.0 4250 100.0 5338 100.0 11294 100.0

Page 9: original article Comprehensive psycholexical ... · cludes 1,641 adjectives and participles, 612 adverbs, 1,442 at-tribute-nouns, 1,029 type-nouns, and 916 verbs. The analysis of

O. Gorbaniuk, A. Korczak, N. Toruj, A. Czarnejko, K. Macheta, M. Jałoszewska, M. Charęzińska-Nowocień, R. Karpiński, Ł. Garwoliński, T. Misiuro, M. Zygnerska, I. Bojan, K. Rykowska, P. Wawrzaszek, J. Jeliński, M. Hawryluk

99

categories, and their percentage is the highest in the case of attribute-nouns (67.5%).

The list of descriptors of individual differences contained words representing different word classes, some of which referred to the same meaning and had a common morpheme (e.g., pracowity [diligent], pra-cowitość [diligence], pracuś [eager beaver, workahol-ic], pracowicie [diligently]); this resulted in redundan-cy in the personality lexicon. Particularly in the case of dispositional descriptors we analyzed 5,598 words for identical morphemes and similar meanings, iden-tifying 1,979 irredundant morphemes with unique dispositional meaning; we treated antonyms having the same morpheme as referring to the same char-acteristic but to the opposite end of the continuum (e.g., przyjazny [friendly], nieprzyjazny [unfriendly]). Figure 1 illustrates the results of the analyses. In an-swer to research question Q4 we established that ad-jectives describe the highest percentage of the non-redundant personality lexicon (64.0% of morphemes), 6.4% of adjectives being irreplaceable by other word classes (e.g., defensywny [defensive], koncyliacyjny [conciliatory], łatwy [easy], merytoryczny [knowl-edgeable, to-the-point]). To express the meaning of the remaining 36.0% of morphemes, it is necessary to use other categories of words: verbs, type-nouns, or attribute-nouns. On the other hand, limiting research of the personality lexicon only to adjectives would mean ignoring these morphemes. As in the case of adjectives, each of these word categories makes its unique contribution to the representation of the Pol-ish personality lexicon that cannot be replaced by any other category of words. This contribution is 6.2% in the case of attribute-nouns (e.g., chciejstwo [wishful thinking], kuraż [courage], rezerwa [reserve], werwa [verve]), 7.9% in the case of type-nouns (e.g., kataryn-ka [chatterbox], bałaguła [roisterer], cykor [chicken], dusigrosz [skinflint], jęczydusza [grumbler], niedoraj-da [loser]), and 6.8% in the case of verbs (e.g., kołować [to wangle], kraść [to steal], załatwiać [to fix]).

In answer to research question Q3 we can state that the vast majority of markers distinguished by

De Raad et  al. (2014) are represented in the Polish personality lexicon. A total of 92% of the 241 cross-language Big Six markers identified in cross-language analyses were listed among Polish person-descrip-tive terms. For three dimensions – Agreeableness, Intellect, and Honesty–Humility – we found 95% correspondence of the list of markers with the Pol-ish personality lexicon. For the Conscientiousness dimension the correspondence level was 93%, and for the remaining two dimensions (Extraversion and Emotional Stability) it was 88%. Some of the differ-ences between the Polish personality lexicon and the broadest list of markers of the cross-language Big Six stems from the fact that in order to repro-duce the meaning of some English words it would be necessary in Polish to use longer expressions (e.g., goal-oriented – zorientowany na cel), and these were not included in the list, in accordance with the meth-odology we adopted. The next issue is the fact that some markers, translated into Polish, are words de-scribing mainly emotional states or visible activities (e.g., szczęśliwy [happy], smutny [sad], uśmiechnięty [smiling]) and were not rated as dispositional by the majority of judges.

Table 5

Valence of personality-descriptive terms

Valence All word classes

Word class

Adjectives & participles

Adverbs Attribute-nouns

Type-nouns Verbs

f % f % f % f % f % f %

Negative 3016 53.9 903 55.0 322 52.6 754 52.3 695 67.5 371 40.5

Neutral 864 15.4 216 13.2 67 10.9 169 11.7 156 15.2 253 27.6

Positive 1718 30.7 522 31.8 223 36.4 519 36.0 178 17.3 292 31.9

Total 5598 100.0 1641 100.0 612 100.0 1442 100.0 1029 100.0 916 100.0

Figure 1. Frequency of use of word classes in perso-nality description and their unique contribution.

personality morphemes [%]

unic terms

verbs

type- nouns

atribute- nouns

adverbs

adjectives

0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100

6.8%

7.9%

6.2%

0.1%

6.4%

Page 10: original article Comprehensive psycholexical ... · cludes 1,641 adjectives and participles, 612 adverbs, 1,442 at-tribute-nouns, 1,029 type-nouns, and 916 verbs. The analysis of

Comprehensive psycholexical classification

10 current issues in personality psychology

discussion

The presented research results are the third attempt at conducting qualitative and quantitative analyses of the Polish lexicon of person-descriptive terms. As opposed to the previous studies, limited only to the adjective lexicon (Szarota, 1995; Szarota et al., 2007; Gorbaniuk et al., 2011), the current analyses system-atize the complete lexicon used to describe human characteristics, including adjectives, adjectival par-ticiples, adverbs, attribute-nouns, type-nouns, and verbs. The source of the lexicon that we used was the most recent and up-to-date universal diction-ary of Polish, which is in the top quarter in terms of size among the studies conducted so far (median: 70,000 entries). From the psychometric point of view, the distinctive feature of the present study is the verification of judges’ decisions not only in terms of reliability but also in terms of validity, which is rare in psycholexical research (cf. Angleitner et al., 1990; Gorbaniuk et  al., 2014; Gorbaniuk et  al., 2018; Iva-nova et al., 2017).

The percentage of words describing differences between people in the Polish lexicon represented by the dictionary we used is relatively high (27.8%) com-pared, for instance, to the studies devoted to the Ger-man (Angleitner et al., 1990), Italian (Di Blas & Forzi, 1999), Croatian (Mlačić & Ostendorf, 2005) and Czech (Hřebíčková, 2007) lexicons, similar in terms of vo-cabulary size, the investigated word classes and the procedure, where the proportion of person-descrip-tive terms was lower than 20%. Also the proportion of personality descriptors is very high compared to the taxonomy of other languages based on a similar procedure: it accounts for 5.6% of the entire lexicon included in the dictionary and for 20.1% of person-descriptive terms, whereas in other languages these percentages usually are much lower (cf. Angleitner et  al., 1990; Hřebíčková, 2007; Mlačić &  Ostendorf, 2005). One of the causes is the research procedure enabling an exhaustive analysis of the meanings of terms, since the judges took into account both col-loquial and dictionary meanings. On the one hand, this made it possible to avoid the limitations stem-ming from the structure of the dictionary, failing to fully reflect the colloquial meanings of words; on the other hand, taking dictionary meanings into account made it possible to avoid the linguistic limitations of each individual judge. The richness of the Polish per-sonality lexicon is also confirmed by the fact that it contains 92% of the 241 markers of the Big Six factors used in cross-language comparisons by De Raad et al. (2014), which makes it possible at least to replicate the Big Six and, potentially, to supplement it with other factors if they are not represented to a  suffi-cient degree by adjectives.

The results of the classification of person-descrip-tive terms representing different word classes con-

firm De Raad and Ostendorf’s (1996) assertion about the frequency of personality adjectives being higher than that of type-nouns. What is more, we found a predominance of adjectives over all the remaining word classes in personality description. This, how-ever, does not mean that adjectives exhaust the Pol-ish personality lexicon. The comparative analysis of words representing different linguistic categories but having the same morpheme and meaning revealed that in more than 36% of cases the Polish person-ality lexicon requires the use of word classes other than adjectives. Therefore, limiting the research on the structure of personality description to adjectives involves the risk of reductionism, as pointed out by Saucier (2003) as well as De Raad and Barelds (2008). It is necessary to study the lexicon of various lin-guistic categories (attribute-nouns, type-nouns, and verbs) in order to confirm the adequacy of the six dimensions identified in research narrowed to the adjective lexicon (Szarota et  al., 2007; Gorbaniuk et  al., 2013) for the description of the entire Polish personality lexicon. What is particularly needed is research based on a non-redundant and exhaustive list of personality descriptors representing various parts of speech, which would fully follow the idea of the lexical assumption (Goldberg, 1981).

To date, verbs belong to the most often neglect-ed word classes in psycholexical studies, although many researchers at the same time emphasize their uniqueness in comparison with other descriptors of individual differences (De Raad &  Barelds, 2008; Quevedo‐Aguado, Iraegui, Anivarro, &  Ross, 1996). So far, attempts to learn the structure of the lexicon of personality-verbs have been made only in Dutch (De Raad, Mulder, Kloosterman, &  Hofstee, 1988) and Czech (Hřebíčková, Ostendorf, Osecká, &  Cer-mák, 1999). The Polish lexicon of person-descriptive verbs can be a good starting point for future research in both restrictive and non-restrictive approaches, including cross-language comparisons (e.g. Polish, Dutch and Czech).

The identified Polish lexicon of person-descriptive terms is not limited to the lexicon of dispositions – terms referring to relatively stable characteristics – which is usually examined in the narrow/restrictive approach, dominant in psycholexical research. There-fore, the complete lexicon of individual differences can be a  point of departure for inclusive/unrestric-tive studies on the Polish lexicon, analogous to those devoted to the Dutch language (De Raad & Barelds, 2008). Moreover, thanks to the use of the German tax-onomy (Angleitner et al., 1990), we identified lexicons for 13 subcategories of individual differences unre-lated to personality, which can be objects of separate lexical studies and may contribute to the achievement of taxonomic agreement on the classification of dif-ferences between people, for instance, in terms of worldview (cf. Krauss, 2006; Saucier, 2000), cognitive,

Page 11: original article Comprehensive psycholexical ... · cludes 1,641 adjectives and participles, 612 adverbs, 1,442 at-tribute-nouns, 1,029 type-nouns, and 916 verbs. The analysis of

O. Gorbaniuk, A. Korczak, N. Toruj, A. Czarnejko, K. Macheta, M. Jałoszewska, M. Charęzińska-Nowocień, R. Karpiński, Ł. Garwoliński, T. Misiuro, M. Zygnerska, I. Bojan, K. Rykowska, P. Wawrzaszek, J. Jeliński, M. Hawryluk

119

emotional, and motivational reactions to other people (cf. Mlačić, 2016; Saucier, 2010), social effects (cf. De Raad et al., 2005), as well as emotional, motivational, or cognitive states. Due to the small number of lan-guages investigated so far in non-dispositional terms, it is still too early for cross-cultural generalizations.

Although the authors’ intention was to perform an exhaustive classification of the Polish lexicon of person-descriptive terms, the ideal achievement of this aim will encounter two kinds of barriers, which are present in all psycholexical studies (De Raad, 1996, 1998; Saucier, Hampson, &  Goldberg, 2000). The first barrier is the dictionary representing the Polish lexicon: even though we chose the most com-plete and up-to-date Polish dictionary, considered by linguists to be one of the least normative dictionar-ies, for reasons of space it does not contain all the parts of speech and all the related specific meanings that may be significant from the point of view of psychology. In particular, what should be checked is the completeness of the lexicon of active and passive participles, which can be formed based on the select-ed verbs. Because in Polish they are used similarly to adjectives to describe differences between people, participles may be a valuable supplement to the ad-jective lexicon of personality descriptors. Second, the necessity of receiving the majority of judges’ votes in the process of classification results in part of the lexicon of individual differences not being unam-biguously classified into specific categories and sub-categories, which means these terms cannot be fur-ther used in quantitative studies. Third, De Raad and Barelds (2008) point out the role of idioms.

The analyses performed as part of the present study stopped at classifying terms into the cat-egories and subcategories borrowed from the Ger-man psycholexical study. The next stage should be quantitative research aimed at identifying the factor structure of the lexicon of individual differences, fol-lowing either the non-restrictive approach or an ap-proach restricted to a particular category or subcat-egory of person-descriptive terms (e.g. social affects, worldviews or temporary states). This in turn makes it necessary to refine the list of descriptors for the purposes of specific studies, taking into account their possible redundancy, usage frequency, familiarity to an average respondent, unambiguity, etc. Conduct-ing quantitative research will make it possible to con-clude the entire series of studies on the Polish lexicon of individual differences.

conclusions

Qualitative studies on the lexicon of individual dif-ferences are crucial to the outcome of psycholexical studies. Like any other study in psychology, they should be subject to multifaceted psychometric

validation, including assessment of the validity and reliability of judges’ decisions. If language is to be a  source of knowledge about individual differences and a chance to reach taxonomic agreement in aca-demic debate, it is necessary to conduct comprehen-sive studies into the lexicon of individual differences of many languages, not limited to one part of speech, whose outcome will make it possible to analyze vari-ous categories of these differences without limiting them to narrowly defined personality traits.

acknowledgMents

This paper was partly supported by the National Science Centre grant (Poland) [registration number 2017/25/N/HS6/02618] to Tomasz Misiuro.

References

Almagor, M., Tellegen, A., &  Waller, N. (1995). The Big Seven model: A cross-cultural replication and further exploration of the basic dimensions of natural language of trait descriptions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 300–307.

Angleitner, A., Ostendorf, F., & John, O. P. (1990). To-wards a  taxonomy of personality descriptors in German. A psycho-lexical study. European Journal of Personality, 4, 89–118.

Ashton, M. C., & Lee, K. (2005). A defence of the lexi-cal approach to the study of personality structure. European Journal of Personality, 19, 5–24.

Ashton, M. C., Lee, K., Perugini, M., Szarota, P., de Vries, R. E., Di Blas, L., & De Raad, B. (2004). A six-factor structure of personality-descriptive ad-jectives: Solutions from psycholexical studies in seven languages. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 356–366.

Benet-Martínez, V., & Waller, N. G. (1997). Further evidence for the cross-cultural generality of the Big Seven Factor model: Indigenous and imported Spanish personality constructs. Journal of Person-ality, 65, 567–598.

Church, A. T., Katigbak, M. S., & Reyes, J. A. S. (1996). Toward a taxonomy of trait adjectives in Filipino: Comparing personality lexicons across cultures. European Journal of Personality, 10, 3–24.

Costa, P. T. Jr., McCrae, R. R., & Dye, D. A. (1991). Facet scales for agreeableness and conscientious-ness: A revision of the NEO Personality Inventory. Personality and Individual Differences, 12, 887–898.

De Raad, B. (1998). Five Big, Big Five issues: Ratio-nale, content, structure, status, and cross-cultural assessment. European Psychologist, 3, 113–124.

De Raad, B., & Barelds, D. P. H. (2008). A new taxon-omy of Dutch personality traits based on a com-prehensive and unrestricted list of descriptors.

Page 12: original article Comprehensive psycholexical ... · cludes 1,641 adjectives and participles, 612 adverbs, 1,442 at-tribute-nouns, 1,029 type-nouns, and 916 verbs. The analysis of

Comprehensive psycholexical classification

12 current issues in personality psychology

Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 347–364.

De Raad, B., Barelds, D. P., Levert, E., Ostendorf, F., Mlačić, B., Blas, L. D., Hřebíčková, M., Szirmák, Z., Szarota, P., Perugini, M., & Church, A. T. (2010). Only three factors of personality description are fully replicable across languages: A comparison of 14 trait taxonomies. Journal of Personality and So-cial Psychology, 98, 160–173.

De Raad, B., Barelds, D. P. H., Timmerman, M. E., De Roover, K., Mlačić, B., & Church, A. T. (2014). To-wards a pancultural personality structure: Input from 11 psycho-lexical studies. European Journal of Personality, 28, 497–510.

De Raad, B., Mulder, E., Kloosterman, K., &  Hof-stee,  W.  K. (1988). Personality‐descriptive verbs. European Journal of Personality, 2, 81–96.

De Raad, B., &  Ostendorf, F. (1996). Quantity and quality of trait-descriptive type nouns. European Journal of Personality, 10, 45–56.

De Raad, B., Perugini, M., Hřebíčková, M., & Szaro-ta, P. (1998). Lingua Franca of personality: Taxono-mies and structures based on the psycholexical approach. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 29, 212–232.

De Raad, B., Van Oudenhoven, J. P., & Hofstede, M. (2005). Personality terms of abuse in three cul-tures: Type nouns between description and insult. European Journal of Personality, 19, 153–165.

Di Blas, L., & Forzi, M. (1998). An alternative taxo-nomic study of personality-descriptive adjectives in the Italian language. European Journal of Per-sonality, 12, 75–101.

Di Blas, L., &  Forzi, M. (1999). Refining a  descrip-tive structure of personality attributes in the Ital-ian language: The abridged Big Three circumplex structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psy-chology, 76, 451–481.

Dubisz, S. (Ed.) (2008). Uniwersalny słownik języka polskiego [The Universal Dictionary of the Polish Language]. Warszawa: Wydawnictwo Naukowe PWN.

Fleiss, J. L. (1981). Statistical methods for rates and proportions. New York: John Wiley.

Galton, F. (1884). Measurement of character. Fort-nightly Review, 36, 179–185.

Goldberg, L. R. (1981). Language and individual dif-ferences. The search for universals in personality lexicons. In L. Wheeler (Ed.), Review of personality and social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 141–165). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative “description of personality”. The Big Five factor structure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 59, 1216–1229.

Gorbaniuk, O., Czarnecka, E., &  Chmurzyńska, M. (2011). Taxonomy of person-descriptive terms in Polish: A psycho-lexical study. Current Problems of Psychiatry, 12, 100–106.

Gorbaniuk, O., Budzińska, A., Owczarek, M., Bożek, E., &  Juros, K. (2013). The factor structure of Polish personality-descriptive adjectives: An alternative psycho-lexical study. European Journal of Personal-ity, 27, 304–318.

Gorbaniuk, O., Kulewicz, P., Gorbaniuk, J., Kordon, A., Leoszko, W., Ivanova, A., & Suchomska, M. (2014). Taksonomia psycholeksykalna języka białoruskie-go [A psycho-lexical taxonomy of Belarusian lan-guage]. Current Problems of Psychiatry, 15, 89–95.

Gorbaniuk, O., Mirowich, A., Leoszko, W., Gorba-niuk, J., Kordon, A., Świderska, M., Kuts, O., & Kor-czak, A. (2018). A psycholexical classification of Ukrainian descriptors of individual differences. Current Problems of Psychiatry, 19, 1–8.

Hřebíčková, M. (2007). The lexical approach to per-sonality description in the Czech context. Ces-koslovenská Psychologie, 51, 50–61.

Hřebíčková, M., Ostendorf, F., Osecká, L., &  Cer-mák,  I. (1999). Taxonomy and structure of Czech personality-relevant verbs. Personality Psychology in Europe, 7, 51–65

Ivanova, A., Gorbaniuk, O., Kordon., A., Gorba-niuk, J., Leoszko, W., &  Kulewicz, P. (2017). The language lexicon as the source of knowledge of individual differences: a  psycholexical study of Russian. Paper presented on XVIII International Slavonic Conference Literatures, cultures and East Slavonic languages with regard to their historical time. Zielona Góra, Poland, 22-23.05.2017.

John, O. P., & Srivastava, S. (1999). The Big Five trait taxonomy: History, measurement, and theo-retical perspectives. In O. P. John, R. W. Robins, &  L.  A.  Pervin (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research (pp. 114–158). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Krauss, S. (2006). Does ideology transcend culture? A preliminary examination in Romania. Journal of Personality, 74, 1219–1256.

Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2004). Psychometric proper-ties of the HEXACO Personality Inventory. Multi-variate Behavioral Research, 39, 329–358.

Lee, K., & Ashton, M. C. (2006). Further assessment of the HEXACO Personality Inventory: Two new facet scales and an observer report form. Psycho-logical Assessment, 18, 182–191.

McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. X. (1985). Updating Nor-man’s “adequate taxonomy”: Intelligence and personality dimensions in natural language and in questionnaires. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 710–721.

McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. X. (1987). Validation of the Five-Factor Model of personality across instru-ments and observers. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 81–90.

Mlačić, B., & Ostendorf, F. (2005). Taxonomy and struc-ture of Croatian personality-descriptive adjectives. European Journal of Personality, 19, 117–152.

Page 13: original article Comprehensive psycholexical ... · cludes 1,641 adjectives and participles, 612 adverbs, 1,442 at-tribute-nouns, 1,029 type-nouns, and 916 verbs. The analysis of

O. Gorbaniuk, A. Korczak, N. Toruj, A. Czarnejko, K. Macheta, M. Jałoszewska, M. Charęzińska-Nowocień, R. Karpiński, Ł. Garwoliński, T. Misiuro, M. Zygnerska, I. Bojan, K. Rykowska, P. Wawrzaszek, J. Jeliński, M. Hawryluk

139

Mlačić, B. (2016). Social and reputational aspects of personality. International Journal of Personality Psychology, 2, 15–36.

Peabody, D., &  De Raad, B. (2002). The substantive nature of psycholexical personality factors: A com-parison across languages. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 983–997.

Quevedo‐Aguado, M. P., Iraegui, A., Anivarro, E. M., & Ross, P. (1996). Linguistic descriptors of person-ality in the Spanish language: A first taxonomic study. European Journal of Personality, 10, 25–34.

Saloni, Z. (2012). Podstawy teoretyczne „Słownika gra-matycznego języka polskiego” [Theoretical basics of “Polish Grammar Dictionary”]. Warszawa: Sowa.

Saucier, G. (1997). Effects of variable selection on the factor structure of person descriptors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1296–1312.

Saucier, G. (2000). Isms and the structure of social at-titudes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychol-ogy, 78, 366–385.

Saucier, G. (2003). Factor structure of English-lan-guage personality type nouns. Journal of Person-ality and Social Psychology, 85, 695–708.

Saucier, G. (2009a). Semantic and linguistic aspects of personality. In P. Corr &  G. Matthews (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of personality psychol-ogy (pp. 379–399). New York: Cambridge Univer-sity Press.

Saucier, G. (2009b). Recurrent personality dimen-sions in inclusive lexical studies: Indications for a  Big Six structure. Journal of Personality, 77, 1577–1614.

Saucier, G. (2010). The structure of social effects: Per-sonality as impact on others. European Journal of Personality, 24, 222–240.

Saucier, G., Hampson, S. E., & Goldberg, L. R. (2000). Cross-language studies of lexical personality fac-tors. In S. E. Hampson (Ed.), Advances in person-ality psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 1–36). London: Rout-ledge.

Saucier, G., &  Srivastava, S. (2015). What makes a good structural model of personality? Evaluat-ing the Big Five and alternatives. In M. Mikulincer & P. R. Shaver (Eds.), APA Handbook of Personality and Social Psychology, Vol. 4: Personality Process-es and Individual Differences (pp. 283–305). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Simons, G. F., &  Fennig, C. D. (2018). Ethnologue: Languages of the World. Dallas, Texas: SIL Inter-national.

Szarota, P. (1995). Polska Lista Przymiotnikowa (PLP): Narzędzie do diagnozy pięciu wielkich czynników osobowości [Polish Adjective List: In-strument to assess the five-factor model of per-sonality]. Studia Psychologiczne, 33, 227–256.

Szarota, P., Ashton, M., & Lee, K. (2007). Taxonomy and structure of the Polish personality lexicon. European Journal of Personality, 21, 823–852.

Tellegen, A., & Waller, N. G. (1987). Reexamining ba-sic dimensions of natural language trait descrip-tors. Paper presented at the 95th annual meeting of the American Psychological Association, New York.