ontogeny of behavior in larvae of marine demersal fishes

Upload: jonas-correa

Post on 05-Apr-2018

225 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 7/31/2019 Ontogeny of Behavior in Larvae of Marine Demersal Fishes

    1/19

    RE V I E W

    Ontogeny of behaviour in larvae of marine demersal fishes

    Jeffrey M. Leis

    Received: 13 May 2010 / Revised: 22 July 2010/ Accepted: 23 July 2010 / Published online: 3 September 2010

    The Ichthyological Society of Japan 2010

    Abstract The development of behaviours that are rele-

    vant to larval dispersal of marine, demersal fishes is poorlyunderstood. This review focuses on recent work that

    attempts to quantify the development of swimming, ori-

    entation, vertical distribution and sensory abilities. These

    behaviours are developed enough to influence dispersal

    outcomes during most of the pelagic larval stage. Larvae

    swim in the ocean at speeds similar to the currents found in

    many locations and at 315 body lengths per second

    (BL s-1), although, based on laboratory measurements,

    species from cold environments swim slower than those

    from warm environments. At least in warm-water species,

    larvae swim in an inertial hydrodynamic environment for

    most of their pelagic period. Unfed swimming endurance is

    [10 km from about 810 mm, and reaches more than

    50 km before settlement in several species. Larval fishes

    are efficient swimmers. In most species, a large majority of

    larvae have orientated swimming in the ocean, but the

    precision of orientation does not improve with growth.

    Swimming direction of the larvae frequently changes

    ontogenetically. Vertical distribution changes ontogeneti-

    cally in most species, and both ontogenetic ascents and

    descents are found. Development of schooling is poorly

    understood, but it may influence speed, orientation and

    vertical distribution. Sensory abilities (hearing, olfaction,

    vision) form early, are well developed and are able to

    detect cues relevant to orientation for most of the pelagic

    larval stage. All this indicates that the passive portion of

    the pelagic larval duration will be short, at least in most

    warm-water species, and that behaviour must be taken into

    account when considering dispersal, and in particular in

    dispersal models. Although quantitative information on theontogeny of some behaviours is available for a relatively

    small number of species, more research in this field is

    required, especially on species from colder waters.

    Keywords Connectivity Dispersal Orientation

    Swimming Vertical distribution

    Introduction

    A large majority of marine teleost fish species, regardless

    of whether they occupy pelagic or demersal habitats as

    adults, have a pelagic larval stage. During this pelagic

    larval stage, which may last days to months, several

    important processes take place. First, the larvae grow,

    increasing greatly in size and weight (Houde 1989), but

    perhaps more importantly, most somatic and behavioural

    development takes place during the larval period (Moser

    1981). New structures and behaviours appear, both may be

    modified, and some disappear. Finally, at least in demersal

    species, most dispersal takes place during the pelagic larval

    stage, thus setting the spatial scale for population structure

    and connectivity.

    Larval fishes begin the pelagic portion of their life his-

    tory with limited behavioural abilities, but by the time they

    settle, their ability to swim, orientate and detect sensory

    cues is well developed (Leis 2006; Montgomery et al.

    2006). The ontogeny of these behavioural abilities has

    important implications for both dispersal and survival

    during the larval stage, yet we know little about the

    development of behaviour in marine larval fishes. Research

    on behavioural development in larval marine fishes has

    traditionally focused on feeding and vertical distribution

    J. M. Leis (&)

    Ichthyology, Australian Museum, 6 College St, Sydney,

    NSW 2010, Australia

    e-mail: [email protected]

    123

    Ichthyol Res (2010) 57:325342

    DOI 10.1007/s10228-010-0177-z

  • 7/31/2019 Ontogeny of Behavior in Larvae of Marine Demersal Fishes

    2/19

    (Miller et al. 1988; Pearre 2003). More recently, behav-

    iours relevant to dispersal, particularly swimming and

    orientation, have received attention (Leis 2006). Sensory

    abilities have been less studied, yet a knowledge of the

    capacity of larvae to detect and respond to sensory cues,

    including those produced by predators, is critical to

    understanding how larvae survive and disperse in the sea

    (Kingsford et al. 2002; Arvedlund and Kavanagh 2009).Thus far, the majority of research has been on structural

    rather than functional development of sense organs, and

    little has been quantitative (Arvedlund and Kavanagh

    2009).

    This review concentrates on the ontogeny of behaviours

    and sensory abilities that are relevant to the dispersal of

    demersal fish species: swimming, orientation, vertical dis-

    tribution, vision, hearing and olfaction. It emphasises

    recent research results, attempts to put these into context,

    and points out directions for future research. The traditional

    view was that the behavioural abilities of marine fish larvae

    are so feeble as to be irrelevant to dispersal, but theresearch reviewed here shows this view to be wrong, and

    wrong for a large portion of the pelagic larval stage, not

    just when larvae are ready to settle. The emphasis in the

    present paper is on larvae of marine demersal fishes,

    especially those of rocky and coral reefs. Masuda (2009)

    provides an excellent recent review of the ontogeny of

    behaviour in larvae of pelagic fishes. Earlier reviews dealt

    with some aspects of the ontogeny of larval behaviour,

    often feeding, but few marine demersal species were

    included (Blaxter 1986, 1991; Boehlert and Mundy 1988;

    Miller et al. 1988; Noakes and Godin 1988), and much

    research has taken place in the last 20 years.

    With the exception of studies of vertical distribution

    based on samples taken with plankton nets or midwater

    trawls, nearly all research on the behavioural ontogeny of

    demersal fishes has been done with reared larvae. Ideally,

    results based on reared larvae should be checked using wild

    larvae (e.g. Smith and Fuiman 2004; Faria et al. 2009).

    This is seldom possible due to the difficulty of obtaining

    wild larvae over a range of developmental stages, but it

    may be possible to obtain wild settlement-stage larvae with

    light traps, passive nets or seines, and use them in behav-

    ioural comparisons of part of the pelagic larval phase (e.g.

    Clark et al. 2005; Leis et al. 2007). Yet, in only a few

    studies were comparisons made between the behaviour of

    reared larvae and wild settlement-stage larvae of the same

    or a related species, and comparisons involving younger

    larvae are very rare.

    Generally, size has been found to be a better predictor of

    swimming ability than age (Leis 2006), so in this paper,

    size will be used as a proxy for developmental stage in

    preference to age. However, nearly all studies of swimming

    ontogeny have utilised reared larvae, and reared larvae may

    frequently have a wider range of growth rates than wild

    larvae. Therefore, in wild larvae, size may or may not be a

    better proxy than age: this simply has not been tested.

    Some published works report size as standard length (SL),

    whereas others report total length (TL). No attempt has

    been made here to convert TL to SL, and this will introduce

    a small amount of additional variation into the figures. In

    this paper, speed is reported as cm s-1, except wherelabelled as body lengths per second (BL s-1). Regression

    statistics reported in Table 2 are from the publications cited

    in Table 1.

    The nomenclature of early life-history stages of fishes is

    complex, with many different systems of terminology and

    no consensus on the most appropriate. I do not attempt to

    distinguish between larvae and juveniles, but for this

    review adopt an ecological perspective of ontogeny of

    behaviour in marine, demersal fishes that includes all post-

    hatch stages prior to settlement that are subject to pelagic

    dispersal. To avoid awkward phrasing, and for simplicity, I

    refer to the young fish considered here as larvae, butacknowledge that some terminologies might refer to them

    by other labels.

    Swimming

    Various methods of measuring swimming ability provide

    vastly different measures, ranging from laboratory raceway

    measures of time (or distance) swum until exhaustion and

    maximum swimming speed potential to the speed at which

    larvae actually swim in the ocean (Leis 2006; Fisher and

    Leis 2009). Therefore, it is important to be clear about what

    is being measured and to avoid mixing different measures.

    All studies have found a wide variation in swimming

    performance at any size. In the present paper, the plotted

    values are mean swimming performance for each 1 mm

    increment in size.

    Most authors have ignored possible differences between

    day and night in swimming behaviour. Further, when

    attempting to apply the results of their studies that typically

    measured swimming speed over only short periods, they

    have implicitly assumed that the larvae actively swim

    constantly in the ocean, or at least that the proportion of

    time spent swimming does not change temporally. Labo-

    ratory studies (reviewed in Leis 2006) show that many

    factors can influence either the proportion of time spent

    swimming or the swimming speed itself, including food

    density, time since feeding, and laboratory tank size. These

    laboratory studies were based on a measure called routine

    speed; that is, speed measured in a laboratory container

    without any intentional intervention by the investigator.

    Routine speed returns swimming speeds that are much

    lower than those found by other methods (see Fisher and

    326 J. M. Leis

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Ontogeny of Behavior in Larvae of Marine Demersal Fishes

    3/19

    Leis 2009), and for that reason, studies of routine speed

    will not be included here except for the important work of

    Fisher and Bellwood (2003). They found that larvae of five

    species (families Apogonidae and Pomacentridae) swam

    constantly during the day, but at night younger larvae

    swam only about 15% of the time, increasing to about

    6080% at the end of the pelagic period, with a mean of34% across the full larval period. At night, individual lar-

    vae were either active and swam constantly as during the

    day, or were inactive and hung in the water without

    swimming. Late in the larval phase of an anemonefish,

    active larvae swam nearly twice as fast at night as during

    the day (inactive individuals were not included in this

    calculation). These ontogenetic and daynight differences

    in speed and proportion of time spent swimming in the

    laboratory indicate that in the ocean, day and night

    swimming behaviours are unlikely to be equivalent, and

    that the proportion of time spent swimming may vary

    ontogenetically. These behaviours will be difficult to studyin the ocean, but more research is undoubtedly required.

    In this section, an attempt is made to compare swim-

    ming performance among taxa and among the environ-

    ments occupied by the study species (e.g. tropical vs.

    temperate). However, because not only the species but also

    the families and in some cases the orders of fishes differ

    among environments, the comparisons are confounded

    taxonomically. That is, it is not possible to determine if any

    differences revealed in these comparisons are due to

    taxonomic differences or to environmental differences, or

    to some combination of the two. In reality, this may not

    matter, because when considering the influence of behav-

    iour on larval dispersal and how this may differ among

    locations or environments, it is only relevant to examine

    the taxa that naturally occur in the location or environment

    in question. In this context, the only meaningful compari-son is not just between different environments, but between

    the combination of environments and the species that occur

    in them; for example, the comparison of temperate species

    in the temperate environment versus tropical species in the

    tropical environment.

    The present paper focuses on research that addresses

    ontogeny of performance rather than ontogeny of mor-

    phology. It would be very useful to predict swimming

    performance from morphology, but few studies have cor-

    related the two. Kohno and co-workers (Kohno et al. 1983;

    Taki et al. 1987; Narisawa et al. 1997; Doi et al. 1998;

    Kohno and Sota 1998) described the development of finsand body shape in reared larvae of a variety of marine

    demersal fishes, and related morphology to qualitative

    descriptions of swimming ability in the laboratory (e.g.

    less active swimming, rush and manoeuvrability,

    complete swimming ability). But, without quantitative

    information on speed or endurance, it is difficult to apply

    these descriptive measures to questions of dispersal, or to

    predict swimming performance from larval morphology.

    Fisher and Hogan (2007) were able to predict the critical

    Table 1 Demersal marine fish taxa for which the ontogeny of swimming speed has been studied in larvae

    Order Family Habitat Critical

    speed

    In situ

    speed

    Endurance Reference

    Gonorynchiformes Chanidae Tropical 1 Leis et al. (2007)

    Gadiformes Gadidae Cool-temperate 1 Guan et al. (2008)

    Perciformes Apogonidae Tropical 1 1 Fisher et al. (2000)

    Perciformes Carangidae Tropical 2 1 1 Leis et al. (2006b, 2007)Perciformes Ephippidae Tropical 1 1 Leis et al. (2007, 2009a)

    Perciformes Leiognathidae Tropical 1 1 1 Leis et al. (2007, 2009b)

    Perciformes Lutjanidae Tropical 1 1 Leis et al. (2007, 2009a)

    Perciformes Percichthyidaea

    Warm-temperate 1 1 Clark et al. (2005)

    Perciformes Polynemidae Tropical 1 1 1 Leis et al. (2007, 2009b)

    Perciformes Pomacentridae Tropical 2 2 Fisher et al. (2000)

    Perciformes Sciaenidae Warm-temperate 2 1 1 Clark et al. (2005),

    Leis et al. (2006a),

    Faria et al. (2009)

    Perciformes Serranidae Tropical 3 2 Leis et al. (2007, 2009a)

    Perciformes Sparidae Warm-temperate 2 2 1 Clark et al. (2005),

    Leis et al. (2006a)

    Scorpaeniformes Cottidae Cool-temperate 1 Guan et al. (2008)

    Pleuronectiformes Pleuronectidae Cool-temperate 1 Ryland (1963)

    Values are the number of species studieda

    Adults of the study species live in rivers and upper estuaries, but larvae are found in coastal and estuarine environments of SE Australia

    Ontogeny of larval fish behaviour 327

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Ontogeny of Behavior in Larvae of Marine Demersal Fishes

    4/19

    speed (Ucrit, see below) of wild, settlement-stage larvae

    from a few morphological measures, including body size

    (they had measures of both morphology and speed for 100

    mostly tropical species). Settlement-stage larvae have very

    well developed fins, musculature and other swimming

    structures that are absent or incompletely developed in

    smaller larvae, and Fisher and Hogan (2007) rightly caution

    that predictions based on their model are unlikely to applyto smaller, less developed larval stages. That is, the Fisher

    Hogan model is likely to be applicable only to swimming at

    the end of the larval stage and not to the ontogeny of

    swimming. Clearly, however, this approach has potential,

    even if a different relationship between morphology and

    speed might be found in species from colder environments.

    An attempt to predict Ucrit from size alone for larvae of four

    cold-temperate marine species (a gadiform, a salmoniform,

    a scorpaeniform, and a pleuronectiform; Guan et al. 2008)

    found a significant relationship between total length

    (mm) and speed [Ucrit = 0.79 TL - 2.03, R2 = 0.90,

    P\ 0.0001. Note that Guan et al. (2008) wrote dah (daysafter hatch) instead of TL, but this is an error; P. Snelgrove,

    personal communication]. The slow speeds of these cold-

    water species compared to tropical species (see Fig. 1) serve

    as reminder that any such relationship is almost certain to be

    temperature and taxon dependent.

    Putting larval-fish swimming speeds into an ecological

    context is not always easy, and different approaches have

    been attempted. The concept of effective speed (mean-

    ing a swimming speed on average at least as fast as the

    average current in a particular location; Leis and Stobutzki

    1999) can leave the mistaken impression that larvae that

    are slower than the effective speed will have little influence

    on dispersal. In reality, sustained swimming at almost any

    speed has the potential to influence dispersal outcomes, in

    part depending on the direction that is swum. For example,

    swimming at speeds that are slower than local currents but

    normal to the current direction, which is frequently parallel

    to depth contours in continental shelf waters, can oftenenable larvae to reach a coastal settlement habitat when

    passive larvae would not (e.g. Porch 1998). Several larval-

    fish modelling exercises (reviewed in Leis 2006) have

    concluded that speeds of 23 cm s-1 are able to influence

    dispersal outcomes, leading to the concept of influential

    speed, which is much lower than effective speed. The

    important point in the context of dispersal is whether larvae

    are able to swim ecologically meaningful distances or

    speeds relative to their larval duration. But, once again,

    what is ecologically meaningful is context dependent.

    Critical speed

    Critical speed (Ucrit) is a laboratory raceway measure of

    potential swimming ability: speed is increased incremen-

    tally in short (25 min) steps until the larva can no longer

    swim against the current (Brett 1964; Fisher et al. 2000).

    Critical speed is a very useful standard comparative mea-

    sure of the prolonged swimming speed of fishes, but it is

    not the speed at which larval fishes swim in the ocean

    (Fisher and Leis 2009). Critical speed is relatively easy to

    Fig. 1 Ontogeny of critical

    speed (Ucrit) in larvae of marine

    demersal fishes. Plotted values

    are family means of up to three

    species for 1 mm size

    increments. Sources of data are

    shown in Table 1. Solid dark

    symbols are tropical taxa.

    Hollow symbols are warm-

    temperate taxa. Solid medium,

    larger symbols with thick

    borders are cool-temperate taxa.

    Straight broken lines

    correspond to relative speeds in

    body lengths per second

    (BL s-1)

    328 J. M. Leis

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Ontogeny of Behavior in Larvae of Marine Demersal Fishes

    5/19

    measure, hence there is more information on the ontogeny

    of Ucrit than for other measures of swimming performance.

    The ontogeny of critical speed has been reported for 22

    marine species in 15 families (Table 1). Most of the species

    are tropical (n = 13), but five are warm-temperate in dis-

    tribution, and four are from cool-temperate environments

    (Table 1). Ontogenetic data on critical speed are summa-

    rised in Fig. 1. The plotted points are family values (meansof 23 species in six families, and single species values for

    nine families).

    It can be seen from Fig. 1 that critical speed increases

    with size, although it appears that at least two families

    (Lutjanidae, Polynemidae) and perhaps three more (Ser-

    ranidae, Leiognathidae, Pomacentridae) have a decrease

    or at least a levelling offin critical speed at the largest

    sizes; i.e. at about the size at which the larvae settle. A

    post-settlement decrease in critical speed was documented

    in pomacentrids (Stobutzki and Bellwood 1994), so per-

    haps this should be expected as part of the transition

    between pelagic and demersal environments. Therefore, adeparture from a positive relationship between size and

    speed is possible at larger sizes, and attempts to predict

    swimming speeds of pelagic larvae from those of recently

    settled individuals should be done with great caution [for

    example, see Nilsson et al. (2007) regarding physiological

    changes associated with settlement].

    Aside from the decrease in speed at larger sizes noted

    above, most species show a relatively linear increase of

    speed with size over the pelagic stage (although few studies

    have measured Ucrit for the youngest larvae), and further,

    the most common pattern is for the relative speed (i.e.

    speed in body lengths per second, BL s-1) to remain

    approximately constant (Fig. 1). Notable exceptions are the

    tropical families Lutjanidae and Serranidae, where small

    larvae have exceptionally large spines in the dorsal and

    pelvic fins (Leis and Carson-Ewart 2004). Smaller larvae of

    these families are much slower than similar-sized larvae of

    other families, and their critical speed does not exceed

    5 cm s-1 until about 79 mm (Fig. 1), after the excep-

    tionally large fin spines have reached maximum relative

    size and are becoming relatively smaller. From about 7 to

    9 mm, serranid and lutjanid larvae increase rapidly in

    speed with size (23 cm s-1 per 1 mm increase in size),

    and as settlement approaches, they are among the fastest

    larvae tested.

    The ontogeny of critical speed has strong phylogenetic

    and environmental influences. Larvae of taxa that are

    distributed in cool-temperate waters (Pleuronectidae,

    Gadidae, Cottidae) have some of the slowest reported

    critical speeds, and consistently swim at about 5 (BL s-1)

    over the size ranges tested (Fig. 1). In addition, Guan et al.

    (2008) plot (their fig. 5) but do not formally analyze Ucrit

    data from two other species of cool temperate fishesan

    osmerid and a pleuronectidwhich have size versus speed

    relationships similar to the other cool-temperate taxa in

    Fig. 1. Larvae of taxa distributed in warm-temperate

    waters (Sciaenidae, Sparidae, Percichthyidae) are more

    variable in the development of critical speed. Larvae of

    such taxa are initially relatively slow (510 cm s-1), but at

    sizes larger than 5 mm, sciaenids remain at 10 BL s-1,

    whereas sparids and percichthyids larger than 78 mm canswim at 1520 BL s-1. Aside from the serranids and lut-

    janids mentioned above, larvae of tropical taxa are fast

    throughout development, with critical speeds faster than

    10 cm s-1 and with most species swimming at

    1520 BL s-1 for much of their larval phase (pomacentids

    reach almost 30 BL s-1). On a per-size basis, larvae of

    pomacentrids are among the fastest swimmers, although

    percichthyids and carangids are faster than pomacentrids at

    certain sizes (Fig. 1).

    In addition to the data summarised above that cover a

    substantial portion (but not all) of the pelagic larval phase,

    information on Ucrit at hatching is available for a few coral-reef fishes (six pomacentrids, two apogonids, one blenniid

    and one acanthurid, only the last of which has pelagic eggs;

    Fisher 2005). These just hatched larvae (1.54.5 mm TL) of

    ten species had Ucrit values as high as 4 cm s-1 (up to

    14 BL s-1) and 2 cm s-1 on average. Among these ten

    species, speed increased at a rate of 1.24 cm s-1 for each

    1 mm increase in hatching size. Larvae smaller than

    2.25 mm at hatching (two pomacentrids and the acanthurid)

    had speeds that were virtually zero, whereas the species

    larger than 2.25 mm at hatching swam at speeds of

    24 cm s-1. Based on these limited data, it appears that

    most species that spawn nonpelagic eggs have swimming

    speeds that are shown by modellers to influence dispersal

    outcomes (Leis 2006) from the time they hatch. By

    assuming that the among-species increase in speed with size

    applied throughout the larval phase, Fisher (2005) predicted

    that the larvae of most of the 11 families considered would

    have critical speeds of at least 13 cm s-1 (the mean current

    speed at Lizard Island, Great Barrier Reef) for more than

    50% of their larval phase. Fishers prediction was not very

    different from the empirical data in Fig. 1.

    Comparisons of critical speed to ambient currents are

    problematical because larvae seldom swim at their critical

    speed in the ocean, and, of course, current speeds vary

    widely among locations and times. However, average

    current speeds of 1015 cm s-1 are common in many

    coastal environments (Fisher 2005), which means that

    larvae of many tropical and warm temperate species are

    capable of effectively opposing currents from sizes of 5 to

    10 mm, and influencing their dispersal when considerably

    smaller. Cool-temperate species, in contrast, did not reach

    an average critical speed of 10 cm s-1 over the measured

    size range (Fig. 1).

    Ontogeny of larval fish behaviour 329

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Ontogeny of Behavior in Larvae of Marine Demersal Fishes

    6/19

    Much interest has been expressed about when fish larvae

    move from swimming in a viscous hydrodynamic envi-

    ronment to an inertial one, because it is thought that it is

    too energetically costly to swim any significant distance in

    a viscous environment. Early work considered that the

    transition from viscous to inertial swimming began at a

    Reynolds number (Re) of 30 and was largely complete

    when the Reynolds number reached 200 (Webb and Weihs

    1986), but it is now considered that these values should be

    closer to 300 and 1,000, respectively (see Leis 2006). The

    value of Re is inversely dependent on the viscosity of seawater, and is therefore temperature dependent, because

    viscosity is higher at lower temperatures. For temperatures

    above 20C, fish larvae swimming at the critical speed

    (Fig. 1) will reach Re 300 by 58 mm, depending on the

    species. Therefore, for most of the pelagic larval phase,

    larvae in warmer water will be capable of swimming in a

    largely inertial hydrodynamic environment. In contrast,

    because of their slower speed, and the increased viscosity

    of cold water, larvae of cool-temperate species will not

    reach Re 300 until 1011 mm.

    In situ speed

    Much less information is available on the ontogeny of

    swimming of larvae in the ocean, or in situ speed (Leis and

    Carson-Ewart 1997), and because of the difficulties

    involved in working with very small larvae (\56 mm), in

    situ speed data are available for a narrower range of larval

    sizes than for Ucrit. Measurements of the development of

    swimming speed in the ocean are available for ten species

    of eight families (Table 1): three species (two families) are

    warm-temperate and seven species (six families) are

    tropical.

    The relationship between size and speed is more com-

    plex and variable for in situ speed than it is for critical

    speed (Fig. 2). For nine of the ten species above, there is a

    positive linear relationship between size and in situ speed,

    the exception being the lutjanid Lutjanus malabaricus, for

    which there was not a significant relationship between size

    and in situ speed. In situ speeds are lower than critical

    speeds, with values generally between 3 and 15 BL s-1. Aswith the critical speed, some species decreased in speed at

    larger sizes: Epinephelus coioides, Eleutheronema tetra-

    dactylum and Caranx ignobilis. The three warm-temperate

    species had relative speeds of 38 BL s-1, and values for

    the tropical species ranged widely from about 2 to

    18 BL s-1, encompassing the speeds of the warm-tem-

    perate species, but generally being higher. At any size,

    warm-temperate species were slower by 410 cm s-1 than

    tropical species. Unfortunately, no in situ studies of larvae

    of cool-temperate species are available.

    In situ speed was nearly always slower than critical

    speed (Fisher and Leis 2009), but the ratio of the twovaried with species (Table 2). It is not generally possible to

    measure both critical and in situ speed in the same indi-

    vidual, so comparisons between the two measures are

    based on mean values within 1 mm size increments. In half

    of the ten species above, there was a significant positive

    correlation between size-specific measures of critical and

    in situ speed, but the slope of that relationship varied

    widely among species, from 0.19 to 2.28 (Table 2). The

    overall mean of the ten species-specific ratios of in situ

    Fig. 2 Ontogeny of in situ

    speed in larvae of marine,

    demersal fishes. Plotted values

    are species means for 1 mm size

    increments. Sources of data are

    shown in Table 1. Solid dark

    symbols are tropical taxa.

    Hollow symbols are warm-

    temperate taxa. Straight broken

    lines correspond to relative

    speeds in body lengths per

    second (BL s-1

    )

    330 J. M. Leis

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Ontogeny of Behavior in Larvae of Marine Demersal Fishes

    7/19

    speed to critical speed (based on 1 mm size increments)

    was 0.57 (SE = 0.065, and 95% CI = 0.410.72), but thevalues for individual species ranged from 0.29 to 0.91. This

    mean value is similar to the 0.5 ratio determined from

    studying settlement-stage larvae (Leis and Fisher 2006).

    Clearly, larvae in the ocean do not swim as fast as they are

    able, and a value of about 50% of the critical speed seems a

    reasonable central value for in situ speed. However, the

    wide range of values among individual species and within

    families suggests that caution should be applied when

    using the central value to predict in situ speed in taxa for

    which no data on in situ speed are available.

    The ratios of in situ speed to critical speed seem to differ

    among taxa from different environments. In warm-tem-perate species, the ratios are relatively low, with a narrow

    range of values: 0.29, 0.31 and 0.43, median 0.31 (n = 3).

    The tropical species have a wider range of values

    (0.390.91, n = 7), and a much higher median (0.65). The

    medians are significantly different (Wilcoxon rank-sum

    test, P\0.05). This suggests that the relationship between

    critical speed and in situ speed may differ between envi-

    ronments, temperatures, or taxa, and that tropical species

    may swim closer to their potential speed (i.e. Ucrit) in the

    ocean than do temperate species. If true, then comparisons

    of critical speed between different environments may not

    be informative about possible differences in swimming

    speeds in the ocean. An additional dimension of cross-

    environment (or temperature) comparisons is the observa-

    tion that in gadids and cottids, temperature influences the

    trajectory of larval critical swimming speed development,

    but that the relationship is species-specific (Guan et al.

    2008). Until now, relatively little attention has been paid to

    the influence of temperature on the development of

    swimming abilities in larvae. Further research in this area

    is needed (see, for example, Munday et al. 2009).

    Larvae swim in the ocean at 515 BL s-1 throughout

    most of their pelagic period (Fig. 2), and they swim atspeeds considered by dispersal modellers to be influen-

    tial for dispersal outcomes (i.e. more than 3 cm s-1) for

    most of their pelagic period (Leis 2006). Mean in situ

    speed reaches 10 cm s-1 at 818 mm, depending on spe-

    cies. At 10 cm s-1, larvae are swimming as fast as average

    currents in many coastal areas. As noted in the original

    publications (Table 1), most larvae for which there are data

    on in situ swimming abilities were swimming at Reynolds

    numbers larger than 300, and thus out of a hydrodynamic

    environment dominated by viscous forces, but it is

    important to keep in mind that all such species are from

    warm-temperate or tropical environments.A recent study of the influence of schooling on orien-

    tation in settlement-stage larval fishes showed that larvae

    in groups swam about 10% faster than individual larvae

    (J.-O. Irisson, personal communication). So, it is possible

    that the ontogeny of schooling may interact with the

    ontogeny of swimming behaviour to produce faster than

    expected speeds when larvae begin to school.

    Endurance

    Critical speed and in situ speed are typically measured over

    minutes, yet the larval stage lasts for days to months, so it

    is important to know when during ontogeny larvae are

    capable of swimming over periods and distances that are

    ecologically meaningful. The time or distance over which

    larvae can swim is called endurance, and is typically

    reported as kilometres swum rather than as hours swum.

    Swimming endurance is measured by forcing unfed larvae

    to swim to exhaustion in a laboratory raceway at a constant

    speed (Stobutzki and Bellwood 1997). In most published

    Table 2 Comparison of in situ speed (IS) to critical speed (U) in studies that included a range of developmental sizes of fish larvae

    Family Species IS Slope 95% CI P Overall

    mean IS/U

    Habitat

    Carangidae Caranx ignobilis 0.45 U ? 1.45 -0.2 to 1.1 0.13 NS 0.52 Tropical reef

    Ephippidae Platax teira 1.66 U - 8.18 0.7 to 8.8 0.016 0.87 Tropical reef

    Leiognathidae Leiognathus equulus 2.28 U - 2.17 1.4 to 5.0 0.002 0.65 Tropical non-reef

    Lutjanidae Lutjanus malabaricus 0.24 U ? 9.03 -0.1 to 0.7 0.080 NS 0.63 Tropical reef Polynemidae Eleutheronema tetradactylum 0.34 U ? 5.00 -0.2 to 1.1 0.098 NS 0.91 Tropical non-reef

    Sciaenidae Argyrosomus japonicus 0.26 U ? 0.38 0.1 to 0.4 0.009 0.31 Warm-temperate

    Serranidae Epinephelus coioides 0.55 U ? 2.26 0.2 to 1.1 0.012 0.67 Tropical reef

    Serranidae Epinephelus fuscoguttatus 0.69 U - 8.03 -0.3 to 7.9 0.068 NS 0.39 Tropical reef

    Sparidae Acanthopagrus australis 0.19 U ? 3.80 -0.0 to 0.4 0.081 NS 0.29 Warm-temperate

    Sparidae Pagrus auratus 0.92 U - 10.80 0.6 to 1.3 0.003 0.43 Warm-temperate

    The P value is for the null hypothesis that the slope of the regression line is not different from zero.NS not significant

    Sources of data and statistical analyses are given in Table 1

    Ontogeny of larval fish behaviour 331

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Ontogeny of Behavior in Larvae of Marine Demersal Fishes

    8/19

    studies, the speed was the same for all developmental

    stages, but in some (e.g. Fisher et al. 2000), speed was

    scaled to body size. Although this provides a standard

    value that can be compared across taxa or developmental

    stages, measuring endurance swimming in this way is

    problematical for several reasons. First, it is very unlikely

    that larvae in the ocean actually swim to exhaustion.

    Second, larvae that are able to feed have more-or-less

    open-ended endurance, and can grow and develop while

    swimming (Fisher and Bellwood 2001; Leis and Clark

    2005). Third, any fixed raceway speed is arbitrary, and

    endurance (distance swum) is inversely proportional to

    swimming speed (Fisher and Bellwood 2002). Finally, like

    critical speed, laboratory endurance measures are not

    directly applicable to the ocean, and there are no in situ

    measures of endurance with which to calibrate labora-

    tory measures of endurance. Endurance values do, how-

    ever, provide some indication of when larvae are able to

    swim meaningful distances in the ocean. Because of the

    relatively long time it takes to measure endurance in fish

    larvae (larvae of some reef fishes can swim for a week or

    more before exhaustion), there are only a few studies of the

    ontogeny of swimming endurance. Much more data are

    available on the endurance of settlement-stage larvae (see

    Leis 2006; Fisher and Leis 2009).

    The ontogeny of endurance swimming has been studied

    in only nine species of eight families (Table 1, Fig. 3). In

    all cases, endurance in larvae smaller than 7 mm was small

    (\4 km). Between 7 and 10 mm, endurance started to

    increase, with values of 515 km, but it was often variable

    among individuals of a species (Fig. 3). By the time larvae

    reached the size of settlement (assuming it is more than

    10 mm), endurance values in excess of 20 km were com-

    mon, and may reach 50 km or more. There is no obvious

    difference in endurance between warm-temperate and

    tropical species, except that tropical species attain greater

    endurance prior to settlement, primarily because many

    settle at larger size. There are no endurance data for cool-

    temperate species. Clearly, at sizes larger than 710 mm,

    the endurance swimming abilities of larvae can be

    remarkably large.

    All studies of ontogeny of endurance were done with

    reared larvae. It is likely that endurance measured in this

    standard way is strongly influenced by larval condition or

    body reserves. It is also likely that reared larvae will often

    have greater reserves than wild larvae due to optimal

    feeding conditions in rearing containers. If so, this could

    lead to higher endurance estimates for reared larvae than

    wild larvae. Unfortunately, there are few comparisons of

    reared and wild larvae for endurance, and all involve set-

    tlement-stage larvae, but there is at least one example

    where reared larvae of a pomacentrid had greater endur-

    ance than wild larvae (Leis and Clark 2005).

    Orientation

    Without orientation, swimming by larvae will act primarily

    to increase diffusion, and influence dispersal outcomes by

    increasing the area that larvae pass over or through, thus

    increasing the possibility of finding a suitable habitat

    by chance. The two types of orientation should not be

    Fig. 3 Ontogeny of endurance

    swimming in larvae of marine

    demersal fishes. Plotted values

    are species means for 1 mm size

    increments. Sources of data are

    shown in Table 1. Solid dark

    symbols are tropical taxa.

    Hollow symbols are warm-

    temperate taxa

    332 J. M. Leis

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Ontogeny of Behavior in Larvae of Marine Demersal Fishes

    9/19

    confused: (1) within-individual trajectories (that is, the

    orientation of individual larvae), and (2) among manyindividual trajectories (that is, the orientation of many

    larvae, based on the mean bearing of individuals). Orien-

    tation has been studied only by in situ techniques. Until

    recently, the only way of doing this was by diver obser-

    vations of larvae in the ocean (Leis et al. 1996), but a new

    technique using a drifting in situ chamber (or DISC; Paris

    et al. 2008; Irisson et al. 2009) offers great potential to

    complement and extend diver observations.

    Significant within-trajectory orientation is found in the

    large majority of individual larvae of most species that have

    been studied (Table 3). This means that individual larvae of

    most species do not swim randomly in the ocean. The pre-

    cision of orientation (i.e. the straightness of a trajectory), can

    be expressed by a statistic called r (the length of the mean

    vector; see Batschelet 1981), which ranges from 0 (fully

    random) to 1 (entirely linear). When values of r are plotted

    against size of larvae, there is no obvious ontogenetic trend

    in the precision of orientation (Fig. 4). Although individual

    trajectories are usually significantly different from random

    swimming, the within-trajectory precision is frequently low.

    This means that net velocity (the combination of speed and

    direction that takes into account the variation in swimming

    direction) is usually noticeably slower than the nominal insitu swimming speed. For example, for the four species

    studied by Leis et al. (2009a), net speed was 6283% of the

    nominal in situ speed, and as expected, this ratio was cor-

    related with r (ratio of net speed to nominal in situ

    speed = 1.11 9 r- 0.08, R2 = 0.625).

    Larvae of species that live in non-reef habitats as adults

    apparently have lower orientation precision than do larvae

    of reef species (Leis et al. 2009b; Fig. 4), but this has been

    tested in only a few species. The four tropical reef species

    in Fig. 4 have a mean r of 0.593 (SE = 0.024, n = 84),

    which is significantly greater (P = 0.003, t test on arcsine-

    transformed data) than the mean r of the two tropical

    species that live on muddy or sandy bottom as adults

    (mean = 0.477, SE = 0.032, n = 42).

    Ontogeny of orientation in situ has been studied in the

    larvae of only ten species: three species of warm-temperate

    fish and seven species of tropical fish (four of which live on

    reefs as adults and one carangid that is reef-associated;

    Table 3). Larvae as small as 5 mm have been studied in

    situ. The percentage of larvae with (within trajectory)

    directional swimming was 6790% in nine of the species,

    Table 3 Studies of ontogeny of orientation of marine fish larvae

    Family: species Size range

    (mm SL)

    Proportion (%)

    of individuals

    significantly

    orientated

    Among-individual

    orientation

    Ontogenetic changes in

    orientation

    Adult habitat

    Carangidae:

    Caranx ignobilis

    8.518.0 67 Yes; location

    dependent

    No Tropical reef-

    associated

    Ephippidae:

    Platax tiera

    6.010.0 82 Yes No Tropical reef

    Leiognathidae:

    Leiognathus equulus

    7.513.6 29 No No Tropical soft bottom

    Lutjanidae:

    Lutjanus malabaricus

    12.023.0 9 0 Yes, only small

    larvae

    Bimodal directionality in small

    larvae, none in large larvae

    Tropical reef

    Polynemidae:

    Eleutheronema tetradactylum

    7.521.0 75 Yes, only small

    larvae

    Smallest larvae to NE (to shore),

    others not directional

    Tropical soft bottom

    Sciaenidae:

    Argyrosomus japonicus

    5.014.0 72 No Higher among-individual

    precision with size

    Warm-temperate

    estuary and reef

    Serranidae:

    Epinephelus coioides

    9.020.5 74 Yes, some size

    groups

    Small larvae to N; medium to S;

    large to N

    Tropical inshore reef

    Serranidae:

    Epinephelus fuscoguttatus

    13.020.5 71 No overall Medium larvae to shore (NW);

    large parallel to shore (SE)

    Tropical reef

    Sparidae:

    Pagrus auratus

    7.09.5 74 Yes, only large

    larvae

    Only large larvae had significant

    among-individual orientation

    Warm-temperate

    estuary and reef

    Sparidae:

    Acanthopagrus australis

    7.012.0 84 Yes Small larvae to shore (NW),

    large parallel to shore (NE)

    Warm-temperate

    estuary and reef

    Tropical species were studied off southern Taiwan (Leis et al. 2006b, 2009a, b)

    Warm-temperate species were studied off southeastern Australia (Leis et al. 2006a)

    For E. fuscoguttatus, significant orientation was not found in either medium or large larvae, possibly due to low sample size, but the swimming

    directions of the two size groups were significantly different

    Vertical distribution of the same species was also studied

    Ontogeny of larval fish behaviour 333

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Ontogeny of Behavior in Larvae of Marine Demersal Fishes

    10/19

    and only 29% in Leiognathus equulus. The last species

    lives as an adult in soft-bottom habitats, and the authors of

    the study (Leis et al. 2009b) suggested that the larvae of

    such species may have less of a need for orientated

    swimming than do species that live on coral reefs. In none

    of these ten species was there an ontogenetic trend in

    within-trajectory orientation precision either in the r sta-

    tistic (Fig. 4) or in the proportion of individual larvae that

    had within-trajectory orientation. So, it seems that orien-

    tation abilities are formed at a relatively early stage in

    ontogeny (by 5 mm), and do not improve with growth orage thereafter. Very small larvae cannot be studied in situ

    with diver observation methods, so we know nothing about

    the orientation abilities of larvae smaller than about 5 mm,

    and for some species, the smallest individuals studied are

    considerably larger. It is reasonable to assume that orien-

    tation abilities are poor at hatching and improve between

    hatching and 5 mm, but they do not seem to improve

    thereafter. There are, however, differences in orientation

    precision among species.

    In seven of the ten species, among-trajectory orientation

    was found; i.e. the frequency distribution of the mean

    orientations of individual larvae was significantly different

    from random. Ontogenetic changes in among-trajectory

    orientation were also found in seven of the ten species

    (Table 3). Ontogenetic changes ranged from a simple

    increase in the precision of among-individual orientation

    (one species) to clear changes in the direction in which the

    larvae were swimming (three species, e.g. Fig. 5). In two

    species, the smallest larvae studied showed significant

    orientation while larger larvae did not, and in one species

    only the largest larvae had directional (among trajectory)

    swimming. One might expect ontogenetic improvement in

    orientation; e.g. that orientated swimming would be more

    common or more precise in larger larvae. However, the

    opposite (apparent ontogenetic deterioration in orientation

    ability) was just as common. With only ten species studied,

    the variety of ontogenetic changes in among-individual

    orientation is somewhat surprising. So, it seems that each

    species must be considered individually, and it is premature

    to generalise about the ontogeny of among-individual

    orientation.

    Ontogenetic changes in orientation imply several thingsabout the sensory cues used for orientation. If orientation

    changes with development, then as larvae grow, either the

    cues used for orientation change, sensory abilities change,

    the motivation to respond to cues changes, or perhaps all

    three changes occur.

    The available studies of orientation ontogeny (Table 3)

    have some limitations. The ontogeny of orientation has

    been studied in more than one location for very few spe-

    cies, and because location-dependent orientation has been

    found in settlement-stage larvae of some species (e.g. Leis

    and Carson-Ewart 2003), an assumption that the orientation

    will be spatially consistent may not be justified. In some

    cases, the number of larvae studied in some of the size

    classes was low, which can be problematical given the

    generally low overall precision of the among-trajectory

    orientations of larvae of many species. All four of the

    studies used reared larvae, and none were able to compare

    results to anything other than wild settlement-stage larvae

    of related species. All of these studies were based on

    observing larvae for at most 10 min at a time, which is a

    small proportion of the pelagic larval duration. It is unclear

    Fig. 4 Ontogeny of precision

    of directional swimming (length

    of the mean vector) in larvae of

    marine demersal fishes. This

    shows a lack of any ontogenetic

    trend in within-individual

    orientation. Plotted values are

    species means for 1 mm size

    increments. Solid dark symbols

    are tropical reef taxa. Solid

    medium symbols with thick

    black borders are tropical non-

    reef taxa. Hollow symbols are

    warm-temperate taxa. Symbols

    above the broken horizontal line

    represent an orientation that is

    significantly different from

    uniform for points based on

    (typically) 10 min of

    observation (n = 21). Sources

    of data are shown in Table 3

    334 J. M. Leis

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Ontogeny of Behavior in Larvae of Marine Demersal Fishes

    11/19

    whether the results from such short-term studies can be

    scaled up to longer periods. Clearly, there is room for

    further study of the ontogeny of orientation in larvae in

    marine fishes. Especially needed are studies that examine

    the consistency of orientation patterns over time and at

    different locations, directly test if the behaviours of reared

    and wild larvae differ, and expand taxonomic coverage.

    Based on the available studies, several broad statements

    can be made that seem to apply across most of the pelagic

    larval period of larvae of demersal fishes from warmer

    waters. Most individual larvae swim directionally, and

    within-individual orientation precision does not improve

    with growth. Among-individual orientation is common, but

    may not be very precise. Ontogenetic changes in among-

    individual orientation are common, and orientation variesamong species.

    A species with an overall among-trajectory orientation is

    able to have the greatest influence on dispersal. Yet a

    species in which only within-trajectory orientation is

    present can have an increased probability of finding set-

    tlement habitat if the larvae maintain their within-trajectory

    orientation over time (Huebert and Sponaugle 2009), and

    larvae of such a species will have dispersal outcomes very

    different from passive drift with the currents.

    Schooling

    There are reasons to expect that larvae in groups or schools

    may have better orientation than individual larvae (Larkin

    and Walton 1969; Simons 2004). A study that examined this

    issue with settlement-stage pomacentrid larvae showed that

    groups of about ten larvae had more precise orientation both

    within-trajectories and among trajectories (J.-O. Irisson,

    personal communication), thus supporting this expectation.

    However, little is known about the ontogeny of schooling

    in larvae of demersal fishes, or if schooling by larvae of

    demersal species is even common. Two exceptions are the

    mugilid Aldrichetta forsteri, which forms aggregations

    when as small as 46 mm (Kingsford and Tricklebank

    1991), and the gobiid Gobiosoma bosci, which begins to

    shoal at about 6 mm (Breitburg 1991); in both species,

    larvae of this size do not have complete fins. Some species

    are known to school shortly before settlement, but most of

    them also school following settlement (Leis and Carson-

    Ewart 1998), so schooling at this stage may have more to do

    with preparation for post-settlement existence than it does

    with the pelagic stage.

    In larvae of pelagic fishes, schooling begins after fin

    formation is complete, and may not commence until well

    into the juvenile stage: pelagic species began to school at

    sizes ranging from 10 to 40 mm (Masuda 2009; Sabate

    et al. 2010). Carangids school from 12 to 16 mm, and

    although their fins are fully formed, this size did not cor-

    respond to any change in sensory organs (Masuda 2009).

    So, there seem to be a variety of patterns in the develop-

    ment of schooling among pelagic fishes, and it is not

    possible to generalise. Nor is it safe to assume that larvae

    of demersal fishes are any different. More work is needed

    on the ontogeny of schooling in fish larvae. This is

    Fig. 5 Ontogenetic change in among-individual orientation in larvae

    of a sparid, Acanthopagrus australis, in coastal waters 1 km from

    shore. Above: small larvae (710 mm SL) swam on average in a

    north-west direction toward shore (n = 18). Bottom: large larvae

    (1012 mm SL) swam on average in a north-east direction parallel to

    shore (n = 19). The bars represent the frequency distribution of the

    mean swimming directions of individual larvae, and the thin radius

    that penetrates the outer circle is the overall mean direction (after Leis

    et al. 2006a). The two frequency distributions are significantlydifferent

    Ontogeny of larval fish behaviour 335

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Ontogeny of Behavior in Larvae of Marine Demersal Fishes

    12/19

    especially important, as schooling may strongly influence

    other orientation and swimming abilities.

    Sensory abilities

    Orientation, whether vertically or horizontally, requires

    the ability to detect and respond to cues. Therefore, thedevelopment of sensory abilities is intimately related to the

    ability of larval fish to orientate. Reviews of sensory

    abilities that are relevant to the orientation of fish and

    invertebrate larvae provide a good overview (Kingsford

    et al. 2002; Montgomery et al. 2006; Arvedlund and

    Kavanagh 2009). In this review, the emphasis is on vision,

    olfaction and hearing, because these are the senses that are

    thought to have the most potential to be involved in the

    orientation and dispersal of the larvae of marine demersal

    fishes. Research on the ontogeny of these senses has

    included only a limited range of species and families,

    concentrating on pomacentrids. Therefore, the generality ofthe statements in this section remains to be tested. This

    section focuses on work demonstrating actual sensory

    function, rather than studies of structural development of

    sense organs, because most studies of sensory organ

    structure are not clearly informative about function (for a

    more complete consideration of structural sense organ

    development; see Arvedlund and Kavanagh 2009).

    Vision is relevant to orientation via direct observation,

    but is limited by underwater visibility to a few tens of

    metres. Vision may be relevant to orientation over much

    larger scales if a solar compass or some other celestial cue

    is used. There is no direct evidence from marine larval

    fishes of the latter, but circumstantial evidence exists for

    the use of a solar compass by pomacentrid larvae (e.g. Leis

    and Carson-Ewart 2003), and its use by juveniles and adult

    salmon is well established (e.g. Quinn 1980).

    Many species that spawn nonpelagic eggs (i.e. demersal

    or brooded) hatch with functional eyes, but in most cases,

    larvae that hatch from pelagic eggs do not have functional

    eyes for one to a few days. Therefore, vision can be used

    for orientation for most or all of the pelagic larval period.

    Research on feeding behaviour in the laboratory suggests

    the distance over which larvae can see and respond to food-

    size objects is limited, on the order of mm to cm (Job and

    Bellwood 1996, 2000), and therefore of limited use to

    orientation by direct observation. However, calculations

    based on retinal structure (Job and Bellwood 1996; Shand

    1997; Lara 2001) and anecdotal field observations of

    behaviour suggest that settlement-stage larvae can see

    underwater as well as human divers can (Leis and Carson-

    Ewart 2001). Little work has been done on the visual

    abilities of larvae in the context of orientation, and even

    less has addressed the ontogeny of such abilities.

    By settlement, fish larvae have well-developed olfactory

    abilities [see reviews by Kingsford et al. (2002) and

    Arvedlund and Kavanagh (2009) and a recent paper by

    Dixson et al. (2008)], but there is little information on the

    ontogeny of olfaction in marine fish larvae. Embryonic

    anemonefishes (Pomacentridae: Amphiprion spp.) imprint

    on the smell of the anemone taxa upon which the larvae

    subsequently settle, implying an olfactory sense while stillin the demersal egg (Arvedlund and Kavanagh 2009), and

    larval anemonefish (Amphiprion percula) can detect by

    smell alone both predatory and nonpredatory reef-fish

    species within 24 h of hatching (Dixson et al. 2009). These

    newly hatched larvae avoided water with the scent of adult

    fishes, which, in the sea, would help them move from their

    hatching location on the reef and into open water, where

    predator threats are assumed to be lower (Johannes 1978).

    This result indicates that larvae from nonpelagic eggs have

    functional olfactory organs for their entire pelagic period,

    and well before the nasal pit is roofed over, forming separate

    nares (Kavanagh and Alford 2003). Whether anemonefishesare representative of other species with nonpelagic eggs

    let alone those with pelagic eggsremains unknown, but

    structurally, the olfactory organs of pomacentrids and a

    lethrinid appear to be functional in newly hatched larvae

    (Arvedlund and Kavanagh 2009). Arvedlund and Kavanagh

    (2009) conclude that larvae of coral reef fishes develop

    their olfactory organs rapidly (including olfactory receptor

    neurons). Therefore, we can anticipate that olfaction could

    be used in orientation for most or all of the pelagic larval

    period.

    Hearing abilities of fish larvae are well developed by

    settlement (reviewed in Arvedlund and Kavanagh 2009).

    The only work on the ontogeny of hearing abilities in

    larvae of marine fishes shows that larvae as small as could

    be studied (i.e. 810 mm) were able to hear, and that

    hearing ability increased with size of larvae until settlement

    (Wright 2006). However, a study of embryos of anemo-

    nefishes (Amphiprion rubrocinctus and A. ephippium,

    Pomacentridae) demonstrated a response to sound with

    increased sensitivity as the eggs developed (Simpson et al.

    2005), implying that, at least for species with demersal

    eggs, larvae may have the ability to hear throughout their

    pelagic period, and even before.

    All three senses considered here are functional relatively

    early in the pelagic larval phase, and are therefore poten-

    tially useful in orientation over most of the larval phase.

    Research on the ontogeny of sense organ function in

    marine demersal fishes is limited, but shows that sense

    organ performance increases with growth. However, the

    distance over which each sense organ is effective for ori-

    entation and how this changes with growth is generally

    unknown, due to uncertainties about the actual cues that

    larvae utilise (e.g. what frequencies of sound or what

    336 J. M. Leis

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Ontogeny of Behavior in Larvae of Marine Demersal Fishes

    13/19

    scent), how cue strength varies spatially and temporally,

    and in spite of significant recent advances, how sense organ

    performance varies ontogenetically. The field of the sen-

    sory abilities of larval fishes is important, if difficult, to

    advance.

    Vertical distribution

    The behaviours involved with horizontal swimming con-

    sidered above can directly influence dispersal outcomes,

    whereas vertical distribution behaviour can influence

    dispersal only indirectly, through interaction with currents

    that are vertically stratified with respect to velocity (e.g.

    Lagardere et al. 1999; Forward and Tankersley 2001; Paris

    and Cowen 2004). Vertical distribution and migrations

    constitute the most studied aspect of larval fish behaviour,

    and a huge literature exists. It has long been known that

    larval fish distribution is seldom uniform vertically, and

    ontogenetic changes in vertical distribution are well doc-umented in a range of species (Neilson and Perry 1990;

    Leis 2006). Therefore, a broad review of vertical distri-

    bution behaviour is not attempted here. Rather, some recent

    examples of research on ontogenetic change in vertical

    distribution using different methods will be emphasised. If

    larvae do undertake ontogenetic vertical migrations, they

    will be exposed differentially and in a time-dependent

    manner to a range of physical and biological factors that

    vary with depth. Many of these are relevant to dispersal.

    The traditional means of studying vertical distribution is

    by towed plankton nets or midwater trawls, and these

    means have provided much valuable information. Many

    net-based studies have revealed ontogenetic changes in

    vertical distribution (e.g. Barnett et al. 1984), but some

    have not (e.g. Boehlert et al. 1985). In a recent example, off

    an island in the tropical oceanic Pacific, and using a

    MOCNESS net within the upper 100 m, Irisson et al.

    (2010) found that wild larvae of all five families (Apo-

    gonidae, Acanthuridae, Holocentridae, Labridae, Serrani-

    dae) with a significant ontogenetic change in vertical

    distribution moved deeper with development (Fig. 6).

    Postflexion larvae of these families were on average 25 m

    deeper than preflexion larvae. In contrast, three other

    abundant families (Lethrinidae, Lutjanidae, Pomacentri-

    dae) lacked ontogenetic changes, although pomacentrids

    had a statistically nonsignificant upward movement. Irisson

    et al. (2010) point out, however, that in most families,

    many postflexion larvae were present in the surface layer in

    spite of the apparent downward ontogenetic migration

    (which they described as a spread), and that in contrast to

    preflexion larvae, postflexion larvae were abundant at

    depth as well as the surface. Irisson et al. (2010) noted that

    studies using towed nets cannot provide information on the

    movement of individual larvae, only on mass transfers of

    populations that integrate the movement of many individ-

    uals (Pearre 2003). They further note that what is inter-

    preted from net tow data as ontogenetic migration might be

    the result of the differential distribution of mortality, and

    that in such a case, larvae may not actually move vertically.

    This study clearly shows some of the advantages and

    limitations of studying vertical distribution in the most

    traditional way.

    Another recent study of vertical distribution using nets

    found ontogenetic changes in the effects of wind on the

    vertical distribution of larval hake, Urophycis regia

    (Gadidae; Hernandez et al. 2009). In this case, preflexion

    larval hake actively avoided turbulent surface waters cre-

    ated by strong winds. In contrast, postflexion hake larvae

    had a higher dispersion when strong winds increased tur-

    bulence in the upper water column, presumably because the

    ability of larvae to maintain a preferred vertical distribution

    was compromised as wind stress increased, and this

    resulted in increased variance. Interestingly, no effect of

    wind stress was found when the data for preflexion and

    postflexion larvae were combined. Hernandez et al. (2009)

    point out the complex effect of wind on larval distribution

    and dispersal: wind-driven currents can affect the hori-

    zontal distribution of fish larvae, whereas wind-induced

    turbulence affects the vertical distribution of larvae, which

    in turn influences horizontal movement of larval fish via

    depth-stratified flow.

    Observations of larvae by divers provide information on

    vertical movement of individual larvae in the ocean, but

    over only short time periods (minutes) and in the upper

    portion of the water column (20 m), both of which are

    Fig. 6 Ontogenetic descent in a community of larval fishes, as

    revealed from a towed-net study in the tropical open ocean (from

    Irisson et al. 2010, fig. 6, copyright Limnology and Oceanography,

    used with permission). Each shape represents the probability density

    function of the vertical centre of mass of the larval patch sampled

    over the range 0100 m. The distribution of larvae has a deeper centre

    of mass as larvae develop from the preflexion to the postflexion stage

    Ontogeny of larval fish behaviour 337

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Ontogeny of Behavior in Larvae of Marine Demersal Fishes

    14/19

    limitations of the method. Diver observation has docu-

    mented both ontogenetic ascents and ontogenetic descents

    over the upper 20 m by observing reared larvae of different

    sizes and ages, although an individual larva was only

    observed at one size or age (Fig. 7). Ontogenetic ascents

    (n = 4) were as common as ontogenetic descents (n = 3)

    among the nine species (of seven families; see Table 3 for

    the taxa) studied, two of which exhibited no ontogenetic

    change in vertical distribution (Leis et al. 2006a, 2009a, b).

    Ontogenetic vertical migrations were about equal in mag-

    nitude: ascents were 310 m, descents were 37 m (values

    refer to the modal depth). Vertical movement by individual

    larvae can be quantified by a measure such as amplitude

    (the difference between the shallowest and greatest depth

    achieved by an individual larva over the period of obser-

    vation, usually 10 min). In these nine species, mean

    amplitude ranged from 3 to 9 m. An ontogenetic increase

    in amplitude was observed in only two species (a sciaenid

    and a serranid), and values were 0.50.9 m for each 1 mm

    increase in size of the larvae.

    Vertical distribution behaviour and ontogenetic changes

    in it can also be studied in the laboratory by various means.

    For example, vertical movements and the influence of light

    can be studied by placing larvae in a vertical cylindrical

    tank either fitted with sensors to detect movement of the

    larvae (Blaxter 1973) or not (Hurst et al. 2009). Or,

    changes in vertical position can be initiated and quantified

    by changes in pressure. The use of such a pressure appa-

    ratus showed that in larvae of two of the four families

    studied (Balistidae, Pomacanthidae), barokinesis behaviour

    in the laboratory was a good predictor of the depth at which

    the wild larvae were captured in the ocean (Huebert 2008).

    Larvae of size 316 mm were used, and based on data

    presented in the paper, no ontogenetic change was found in

    the predicted depth (i.e. within families, predicted depth

    was not correlated with size of larvae). Based on the cap-

    ture depth, monacanthids did, however, have an ontoge-

    netic descent between sizes of 6 and 20 mm of about

    5080 m in open ocean conditions (Huebert 2008).

    Generally speaking, fish larvae are capable of regulating

    their vertical distribution from about the time they hatch, sovertical distribution behaviour can have an influence on

    dispersal throughout the larval phase. It is clear that

    ontogenetic changes in vertical distribution are common,

    and that these occur at both an individual and a population

    level. A recent study of the influence of schooling on ori-

    entation in settlement-stage larval fish showed that larvae

    in groups had more precise vertical distribution behaviour

    (i.e. the depth-frequency distribution was more narrow)

    than did individual larvae (J.-O. Irisson, personal commu-

    nication). So, it is possible that the ontogeny of schooling

    may interact with the ontogeny of vertical distribution

    behaviour.Although it is clear that vertical distribution and onto-

    genetic changes vary among species, there is conflicting

    evidence about the consistency of vertical distribution

    patterns among related species. For example, the towed-net

    study of Irisson et al. (2010) concluded that vertical dis-

    tribution patterns and their ontogeny were consistent

    among species or genera within a family, whereas the

    diver-observation studies, albeit on a different scale, found

    within-family and within-genus differences in ontogenetic

    patterns of vertical distribution. In the latter studies, larvae

    of one sparid species had an ascent while another had a

    descent (Leis et al. 2006a), and larvae of one species of

    Epinephelus (Serranidae) had an ascent, whereas a second

    Epinephelus species had no ontogenetic changes in vertical

    distribution (Leis et al. 2009a). Vertical distribution studies

    using towed nets concern larvae that are generally smaller

    than those studied by diver observation, and this may

    partially explain the differences. In any case, further study

    of a wider variety of species is necessary before firm

    conclusions can be reached.

    Conclusion

    A key conclusion from this review is that ontogeny of

    behaviour differs among different species, and may even

    differ between congeners. Thus, caution should be applied

    in any attempt to substitute the behaviour of a different

    species, for example in a dispersal model. In addition, the

    limited data available indicate that behavioural ontogeny

    may differ among different habitats. For example, larvae of

    species from cool-temperate waters swim slower at any

    size, and their swimming speeds increase at a slower rate

    Fig. 7 Ontogenetic descent by larvae of a sciaenid, Argyrosomus

    japonicus, in a warm-temperate coastal area (depth 1520 m). Larger

    larvae occur deeper than do smaller larvae. Plotted values are the

    mean depths of individual larvae as observed by divers for about

    10 min. Data are from the diver observation study of Leis et al.

    (2006a) [Figure from Leis (2006), used with permission]

    338 J. M. Leis

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Ontogeny of Behavior in Larvae of Marine Demersal Fishes

    15/19

    per increase in size than in larvae from warmer waters.

    More work is needed, especially on larvae of species in

    cool-temperate waters.

    A recurrent theme is whether larvae of species with

    demersal (and brooded) eggs have behavioural capabilities

    that differ from species with pelagic eggs. The best evi-

    dence comes from studies of swimming, and on a per-size

    basis, there is no difference in critical speed or endurancebetween larvae from the two spawning modes. However,

    larvae from demersal (and brooded) eggs generally hatch at

    larger sizes and are more developed at hatching than larvae

    from pelagic eggs (Moser 1996; Leis and Carson-Ewart

    2004), so on a post-hatch age basis, the former can be

    expected to initially have better speed and endurance than

    larvae from pelagic eggs, and thus to have more influence

    on dispersal outcomes sooner. Whether this putative head

    start lasts through the full pelagic larval stage depends on

    growth rates. A caveat is that this spawning mode head

    start should apply only within habitats. Therefore, one

    should not necessarily expect, for example, larvae of aspecies with demersal eggs in cool-temperate waters to

    have better initial swimming ability than larvae of a species

    with pelagic eggs in tropical waters. A final consideration

    is that demersal and brooded eggs are incubated in the adult

    habitat, so the embryos are potentially exposed to cues

    from that habitat that may aid in orientation during the

    larval stage and in finding settlement habitat at its end

    (Arvedlund and Kavanagh 2009). This effect would not be

    expected for larvae that hatch from pelagic eggs, because

    they are exposed to cues from the adult habitat for only a

    short time after fertilisation, if at all.

    In all measures of performancespeed, endurance,

    orientation and depth selection, variation among individu-

    als is often relatively high. This is the case throughout

    ontogeny, and with both wild and reared larvae. The rea-

    sons for this are not entirely clear, but this variation is

    likely to have both genetic and environmental elements. It

    is well established that individuals vary in condition (i.e.

    reserves, growth rate and other measures; Leis and

    McCormick 2002). Further, it is very likely that inherited

    factors may either enable better performance, or may

    predispose individuals to certain behaviours in a way that

    varies among them (for example, swimming direction).

    Regardless of the reasons, such variation has important

    implications for dispersal. Individuals that swim faster or

    straighter will reach their ultimate destination sooner,

    which would have an obvious impact on dispersal out-

    comes. Individuals that swim in different directions will

    reach different destinations, or encounter different condi-

    tions along the way, both of which have implications for

    dispersal outcomes. Such differences may also influence

    survival. However, it is also important to keep in mind that

    even small average deviations from a random swimming

    direction or vertical distribution can have a major effect on

    the spatial distribution of larvae during their pelagic phase

    and the location where they settle at the end of it.

    Over much of the pelagic larval period, swimming

    speeds of larvae in the ocean can be, depending on loca-

    tion, of a similar magnitude to the speeds of the currents in

    which the larvae swim. Therefore, over much of the pelagic

    period, and certainly from the time the caudal fin forms,larvae will have the swimming ability to influence dispersal

    outcomes. However, as larvae from cold water apparently

    swim slower than do larvae from warm water, at any given

    size they would be likely to have less influence on dispersal

    than larvae from warm water. Further, because the growth

    rates of cold-water species are likely to be slower than

    those of warm-water species (OConnor et al. 2007), the

    former will take longer to reach any given size, thus

    delaying the beginning of influential swimming speeds, and

    keeping larvae in a viscous hydrodynamic environment for

    a longer period. The same reasoning would also apply to

    the development of orientation abilities. Therefore, one canexpect larvae of cold-water species to have even less

    influence on dispersal than warm-water larvae (Leis 2007),

    and that which does occur will apply over a smaller pro-

    portion of the pelagic period.

    A major gap in our understanding of the ontogeny of

    larval-fish behaviour concerns what happens at night.

    Information on diel changes in the vertical distribution of

    larvae is available from towed net samples, but aside from

    that, almost nothing is known about diel effects, if any, on

    larval behaviour or its ontogeny in the ocean. Innovative

    thinking is required to develop means to study these

    behaviours at night.

    The behaviours and their development reviewed here

    cannot be considered separately when trying to understand

    and predict larval dispersal. Swimming will have a greater

    influence on dispersal if it is orientated, for example, and of

    course the velocity (i.e. speed and direction) of larvae will

    have a major impact on their distribution. A good example

    is provided by the larvae of the sparid Acanthopagrus

    australis. This species has a ontogenetic change in swim-

    ming direction (from toward the shore to parallel to the

    shore; Fig. 5) that coincides with ascent from midwater to

    the neuston, while all the time swimming faster with growth

    (Leis et al. 2006a). These three behaviours and their chan-

    ges will have an important and probably synergistic impact

    on the dispersal of this species. Being able to study swim-

    ming speed, orientation and vertical distribution simulta-

    neously is one clear advantage of the diver observation

    method of studying larval-fish behaviour (Leis et al. 1996).

    An apparent paradox emerges from the work on swimming

    in larval fishesat least for species fromwarmer water. These

    larvae have the highest rates of oxygen uptake ever recorded

    in exothermic vertebrates (Nilsson et al. 2007), yet they are

    Ontogeny of larval fish behaviour 339

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Ontogeny of Behavior in Larvae of Marine Demersal Fishes

    16/19

    capable of swimmingwithout food or restfor many days

    and distances of many kilometres. In some cases this is over

    100 km, or 5 9 106 body lengths (Fisher and Leis 2009).

    Such high metabolic requirements combined with very high

    enduranceseem at first glance to be paradoxical. This can only

    be explained if the larvae of demersal reef fishes are very

    efficient swimmers. This is consistent with the estimates of

    Reynolds numbers for in situ speeds which indicate that reef-fish larvae swim in an inertial hydrodynamic environment.

    Perhapsthisefficiency should not be surprising, because small

    bats and small birds are capable of even greater endurance

    with little or no food or rest (McGuire and Guglielmo 2009).

    More work on the physiology of swimming in larval fishes

    would be illuminating.

    Study of the ontogeny of behaviour in fish larvae indi-

    cates that most behaviours develop early during the pelagic

    larval phase. Therefore, the passive portion of the pelagic

    larval duration will be short, meaning that behaviour has

    the potential to influence dispersal over most of the pelagic

    larval duration, although both the quantity and the qualityof this influence will vary ontogenetically. For this reason,

    models of larval-fish dispersal need to include the behav-

    iours considered here and their ontogeny (Leis 2007).

    Dispersal models require quantitative input on the

    behavioural abilities of fish larvae throughout their pelagic

    phase (Leis 2007). However, the study of the ontogeny of

    behaviour in marine larval fishes is itself at an early stage

    of development, so the information available to modellers

    is very limited. We know most about swimming abilities

    and vertical distribution, and least about sensory abilities,

    in particular the range over which they can guide orienta-

    tion. On the other hand, even for swimming, the number of

    taxa for which there are relevant behavioural data is very

    limited compared to the phyletic diversity of marine fishes.

    More work is required on the full range of larval behav-

    ioural abilities, and researchers should investigate a wider

    range of taxa, especially species that live in cooler waters.

    Acknowledgments Preparation of this review was supported by the

    MTSRF, the Hermon Slade Foundation and the Australian Museum.

    Most of my research cited in this paper was supported by the

    Australian Research Commission (grants A19530997, A19804335,

    DP0345876). Joe Nelson provided relevant literature. J.-O. Irisson

    and the editors ofLimnology and Oceanography provided permission

    to reproduce Fig. 6 from Irisson et al. (2010). Suzanne Bullock pro-vided editorial assistance, as did Michelle Yerman, who also helped

    with data analysis. Drs Seishi Kimura, Gento Shinohara and Kunio

    Sasaki invited me to submit this review to Ichthyological Research.

    Reviewers provided constructive criticism. My great thanks to all.

    References

    Arvedlund M, Kavanagh K (2009) The senses and environmental cues

    used by marine larvae of fish and decapod crustaceans to find

    tropical coastal ecosystems. In: Nagelkerken I (ed) Ecological

    connectivity among tropical coastal ecosystems. Springer,

    Dordrecht, pp 135184

    Barnett AM, Jahn AE, Sertic PD, Watson W (1984) Distribution of

    ichthyoplankton off San Onofre, California, and methods for

    sampling very shallow coastal waters. US Fish Bull 82:97111

    Batschelet E (1981) Circular statistics in biology. Academic, London

    Blaxter JHS (1973) Monitoring the vertical movements and light

    response of herring and plaice larvae. J Mar Biol Assoc UK

    53:635647

    Blaxter JHS (1986) Development of sense organs and behavior of

    teleost larvae with special reference to feeding and predator

    avoidance. Trans Am Fish Soc 115:98114

    Blaxter JHS (1991) Sensory systems and behaviour of larval fish. In:

    Mauchline J, Nemoto T (eds) Marine biology: its accomplish-

    ment and future prospect. Elsevier, Amsterdam, pp 1538

    Boehlert GW, Mundy BC (1988) Roles of behavioral and physical

    factors in larval and juvenile fish recruitment to estuarine nursery

    areas. Am Fish Soc Symp 3:5167

    Boehlert GW, Gadomski DM, Mundy BC (1985) Vertical distribution

    of ichthyoplankton off the Oregon coast in spring and summer

    months. Fish Bull 83:611621

    Breitburg DL (1991) Settlement patterns and presettlement behavior

    of the naked goby, Gobiosoma bosci, a temperate oyster reef

    fish. Mar Biol 109:213221

    Brett JR (1964) The respiratory metabolism and swimming perfor-

    mance of young sockeye salmon. J Fish Res Board Can

    21:11831226

    Clark DL, Leis JM, Hay AC, Trnski T (2005) Swimming ontogeny of

    larvae of four temperate marine fishes. Mar Ecol Prog Ser

    292:287300

    Dixson DL, Munday PL, Jones GP (2009) Ocean acidification

    disrupts the innate ability of fish to detect predator olfactory

    cues. Ecol Lett 13:6875

    Dixson DL, Jones GP, Munday PL, Planes S, Pratchett MS,

    Srinivasan M, Syms C, Thorrold SR (2008) Coral reef fish

    smell leaves to find island homes. Proc R Soc B 275:28312839

    Doi M, Kohno H, Taki Y, Ohno A (1998) Development of swimming

    and feeding functions in larvae and juveniles of the red snapper,

    Lutjanus argentimaculatus. J Tokyo Univ Fish 85:8195

    Faria AM, Ojanguren AF, Fuiman LA, Gonclaves E (2009) Ontogeny

    of critical swimming speed of wild-caught and laboratory-reared

    red drum Sciaenops ocellatus larvae. Mar Ecol Prog Ser

    384:221230

    Fisher R (2005) Swimming speeds of larval coral reef fishes: impacts

    on self-recruitment and dispersal. Mar Ecol Prog Ser

    285:223232

    Fisher R, Bellwood DR (2001) Effects of feeding on the sustained

    swimming abilities of late-stage larval Amphiprion melanopus.

    Coral Reefs 20:151154

    Fisher R, Bellwood DR (2002) The influence of swimming speed on

    sustained swimming performance of late-stage reef fish larvae.

    Mar Biol 140:801807

    Fisher R, Bellwood DR (2003) Undisturbed swimming behaviour and

    nocturnal activity of coral reef fish larvae. Mar Ecol Prog Ser263:177188

    Fisher R, Hogan D (2007) Morphological predictors of swimming

    speed: a case study of pre-settlement juvenile coral reef fishes.

    J Exp Biol 210:24362443

    Fisher R, Leis JM (2009) Swimming performance in larval fishes:

    from escaping predators to the potential for long distance

    migration. In: Domenici P, Kapoor BG (eds) Fish locomotion: an

    etho-ecological approach. Science, Enfield, pp 333373

    Fisher R, Bellwood DR, Job SD (2000) Development of swimming

    abilities in reef fish larvae. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 202:163173

    Forward RB, Tankersley RA (2001) Selective tidal-stream transport

    of marine animals. Oceanogr Mar Biol Ann Rev 39:305353

    340 J. M. Leis

    123

  • 7/31/2019 Ontogeny of Behavior in Larvae of Marine Demersal Fishes

    17/19

    Guan L, Snelgrove PVR, Gamperl AK (2008) Ontogenetic changes in

    the critical swimming speed ofGadus morhua (Atlantic cod) and

    Myoxocephalus scorpius (shorthorn sculpin) larvae and the role

    of temperature. J Exp Mar Biol Ecol 360:3138

    Hernandez FJ, Hare JA, Fey DP (2009) Evaluating diel, ontogenetic

    and environmental effects on larval fish vertical distribution

    using generalized additive models for location, scale and shape.

    Fish Oceanogr 18:224236

    Houde ED (1989) Comparative growth, mortality, and energetics of

    marine fish larvae: temperature and implied latitudinal effects.

    US Fish Bull 87:471495

    Huebert KB (2008) Barokinesis and depth regulation by pelagic coral

    reef fish larvae. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 367:261269

    Huebert KB, Sponaugle S (2009) Observed and simulated swimming

    trajectories of late-stage coral reef fish larvae off the Florida

    Keys. Aquat Biol 7:207216

    Hurst TP, Cooper DW, Scheingross JS, Seale EM, Laurel BJ, Spencer

    ML (2009) Effects of ontogeny, temperature, and light on

    vertical movements of larval Pacific cod (Gadus macrocepha-

    lus). Fish Oceanogr 18:301311

    Irisson J-O, Guigand C, Paris CB (2009) Detection and quantification

    of marine larvae orientation in the pelagic environment. Limnol

    Oceanogr Methods 7:664672

    Irisson J-O, Paris C, Guigand C, Planes S (2010) Vertical distribution

    and ontogenetic migration in coral reef fish larvae. Limnol

    Oceanogr 55:909919

    Job S, Bellwood DR (2000) Light sensitivity in larval fishes:

    implications for vertical zonation in the pelagic zone. Limnol

    Oceanogr 45:362371

    Job SD, Bellwood DR (1996) Visual acuity and feeding in larval

    Premnas biaculeatus. J Fish Biol 48:952963

    Johannes RE (1978) Reproductive strategies of coastal marine fishes

    in the tropics. Environ Biol Fish 3:6584

    Kavanagh KD, Alford RA (2003) Sensory and skeletal development and

    growth in relationto theduration of theembryonicand larvalstages

    in damselfishes (Pomacentridae). Biol J Linn Soc 80:187206

    Kingsford MJ, Tricklebank KA (1991) Ontogeny and behavior of

    Aldrichetta forsteri (Teleostei: Mugilidae). Copeia 1991:916

    Kingsford MJ, Leis JM, Shanks A, Lindeman K, Morgan S, Pineda J

    (2002) Sensory environments, larval abilities and local self-

    recruitment. Bull Mar Sci 70:309340

    Kohno H, Sota K (1998) Ontogenetic intervals based on the

    development of swimming and feeding-related characters in

    larvae and juveniles of the lumpfish, Inimicus japonicus.

    Suisanzoshoku (Aquacult Sci) 46:333342

    Kohno H, Taki Y, Ogasawara Y, Shirojo Y, Taketomi M, Inoue M

    (1983) Development of swimming and feeding functions in

    larval Pagrus major. Jpn J Ichthyol 30:4760

    Lagardere F, Amara R, Joassard L (1999) Vertical distribution and

    feeding activity o