nt4 - self and social regulation in learning contexts an integrative

Upload: francisco-ramirez-varela

Post on 05-Jul-2018

212 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/16/2019 NT4 - Self and Social Regulation in Learning Contexts an Integrative.

    1/13

     

    PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE

    This article was downloaded by: [Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile] 

    On: 1 September 2010 

    Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 906706830] 

    Publisher Routledge 

    Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37-

    41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK

    Educational Psychologist

    Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information:http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/title~content=t775653642

    Self and Social Regulation in Learning Contexts: An Integrative

    Perspective

    SIMONE VOLETa; MARJA VAURASb; PEKKA SALONENba School of Education, Murdoch University, Australia b Department of Teacher Education and Centrefor Learning Research, University of Turku, Finland

    To cite this Article VOLET, SIMONE , VAURAS, MARJA and SALONEN, PEKKA(2009) 'Self- and Social Regulation inLearning Contexts: An Integrative Perspective', Educational Psychologist, 44: 4, 215 — 226

    To link to this Article: DOI: 10.1080/00461520903213584

    URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00461520903213584

    Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdf

    This article may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial orsystematic reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply ordistribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden.

    The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contentswill be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug dosesshould be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss,actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directlyor indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.

    http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/title~content=t775653642http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00461520903213584http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdfhttp://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdfhttp://dx.doi.org/10.1080/00461520903213584http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/title~content=t775653642

  • 8/16/2019 NT4 - Self and Social Regulation in Learning Contexts an Integrative.

    2/13

    EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGIST, 44(4), 215–226, 2009

    Copyright   C Division 15, American Psychological Association

    ISSN: 0046-1520 print / 1532-6985 online

    DOI: 10.1080/00461520903213584

    Self- and Social Regulation in LearningContexts: An Integrative Perspective

    Simone Volet

    School of Education

     Murdoch University, Australia

    Marja Vauras and Pekka Salonen

     Department of Teacher Education and Centre for Learning Research

    University of Turku, Finland 

    This article outlines the rationale for an integrative perspective of self- and social regulation

    in learning contexts. The role of regulatory mechanisms in self- and social regulation models

    is examined, leading to the view that in real time collaborative learning, individuals and social

    entities should be conceptualized as self-regulating and coregulated systems at the same time.

    Living systems theory provides support for the claim that although all forms of regulation have

    an adaptive function, the distinct, regulatory processes occurring at different systemic levels

    (e.g. individual, social) are concurrent and interdependent. Challenges for future research from

    an integrative perspective are discussed.

    In this article we argue that current concepts and models

    of regulation, both individual (self-regulation) and social

    (coregulation), need added conceptual and empirical scrutiny

    to allow us to better understand learning in real-life, time-

    framed collaborative activities among peers. Our claim is that

    although the necessity to account for the inherently social na-

    ture of learning and motivation is widely accepted in the so-

    ciocultural, sociocognitive, and situative literature, it remains

    unclear how self- and social regulatory mechanisms interre-

    late and might cocontribute to explain individual and group

    engagement in collaborative activities as these unfold in real

    time. We outline our rationale for an integrative perspec-

    tive and more holistic analyses of the dynamic psychological

    and social nature of self- and coregulation, and examine the

    critical question of reductionism in the pursuit of buildingmodels of regulation that have strong exploratory and pre-

    dictive power. Our argument for an integrative perspective is

     based on the view that different regulatory constructs share

    a common adaptive function, yet each with a distinct applied 

    focus. This justifies the proposal for a concurrency princi-

     ple that stresses the interdependent nature of all regulatory

    mechanisms.

    The article is divided into four sections. The first exam-

    ines the role of regulatory mechanisms in self-regulation and 

    Correspondence should be addressed to Simone Volet, School of Educa-

    tion, Murdoch University, South Street, Murdoch 6150, Australia. E-mail:

    [email protected]

    social regulation models. Accordingly, we take a systemic,

    cross-level approach to review, in turn, the role of social

    regulatory mechanisms in self-regulation models and recip-

    rocally, the place of self-regulation in social regulation mod-

    els. The second section reconsiders the notion of regulation

    in learning contexts, pointing out the danger of reduction-

    ism and presenting our case for an integrative perspective.

    After referring to living systems theory, which treats self-

    and coregulatory mechanisms as interdependent, fundamen-

    tal adaptive mechanisms of any self-organizing system, we

    discuss the common function and distinct foci of regulatory

    constructs, concluding with an examination of the critical

    role of agency. The third section briefly comments on the

    conceptual value of an integrative perspective in regard to

    regulation of motivation and emotion in learning. We con-clude by discussing some challenges for future research from

    an integrative perspective.

    THE ROLE OF REGULATORY MECHANISMSIN SELF-REGULATION AND SOCIAL

    REGULATION MODELS

    The significance of regulation (self-, other- and coregula-

    tion) for effective cognitive performance, identified in the

    developmental literature and stressed both in sociocognitive

  • 8/16/2019 NT4 - Self and Social Regulation in Learning Contexts an Integrative.

    3/13

    216   VOLET, VAURAS, SALONEN

     psychology and sociocultural perspectives, has had a major 

    impact on recent research related to learning and instruction.

    The seminal work of John Flavell and Ann Brown in the

    1970s and 1980s (Brown, 1978; Flavell, 1976) on metacog-

    nition and self-regulation stimulated a rethinking of the

    components of effective learning and how it is best pro-moted through instruction. This led to an explosion of in-

    tervention studies theoretically grounded in the Vygotskian

    (1930/1978) notions of Zone of Proximal Development and 

    scaffolded guidance from other-regulation to self-regulation.

    These interventions, which contributed to the development

    of the individual-in-context perspective for understanding

    learning in real-life situations within sociocognitive theory

    (Pintrich, 2000), occurred concurrently with the emergence

    of the situated and distributed cognition movements (Collins,

    Brown, & Newman, 1989; Lave & Wenger, 1991; Salomon,

    1993), as well as sociocultural and situative perspectives

    on learning and development (Greeno, 1998; 2006; Hickey,

    2003; McCaslin & Hickey, 2001; McCaslin, 2009; Nolen &

    Ward, 2008; Rogoff, 2003).

    Across perspectives, regulatory constructs have been used 

    to explain individual and social processes of adaptation, en-

    gagement, participation, learning, and development. Self-

    regulation focuses on the cognitive and metacognitive reg-

    ulatory processes used by individuals to plan, enact, and 

    sustain their desired courses of action, whereas social reg-

    ulation captures how individuals reciprocally regulate each

    other’s cognitive and metacognitive processes and sometimes

    engage in genuinely shared modes of cognitive and metacog-

    nitive regulation. The power of regulatory constructs lie in

    their capacity to highlight two often overlooked aspects of human adaptive behavior, dynamics (i.e., the continuous sit-

    uational and developmental adjustments of an individual’s

     behavior to environmental changes) and relationality (i.e.,

    the functional relatedness of an individual’s behavior to the

     behavior of others and to the characteristics of environmen-

    tal objects; Fogel, 1993; Hinde & Stevenson-Hinde, 1987).

    On the one hand, scholars interested in the development of 

    self-regulation have stressed the significance of  individual’s

    metacognitive and scaffolded experiences in naturalistic or 

    instructional learning environments, occasionally using the

    term coregulation to represent the transitional process in the

    development of self-regulation. On the other hand, schol-

    ars interested in the importance of coregulatory mechanisms

    have stressed the significance of the social systems that indi-

    viduals are an integral part of, and which stretch beyond 

    teacher–learner interactions. This latter perspective, thus,

    considers individual adaptation as the outcome of enabling

    coregulatory processes.

    Overall, while both perspectives recognize the other en-

    tity, self or social, their relation to the “unstressed” en-

    tity is not always adequately defined. According to Bailey

    (2006, p. 293), cross-level analyses (in this case, analyses

    that combine individual and social levels) are very impor-

    tant but not often undertaken since “intimidating” and re-

    quiring “intellectual courage.” In our view, the challenge

    may be because of the difficulty of theoretically integrating

    the self-regulatory mechanisms, assumed to explain indi-

    vidual adaptation from the person’s subjective perspective,

    with the coregulatory mechanisms, assumed to explain in-

    dividual adaptation from a social system perspective. As aconsequence, the concurrent and interdependent nature of 

    self-regulatory and coregulatory processes has been over-

    looked. We argue that this presents major limitations for un-

    derstanding the complex regulatory processes operating in

    real-time collaborative learning activities, such as inquiry-

     based projects or problem-based learning.

    To provide a basis to our argument for an integrative per-

    spective, we take a systemic, cross-level approach to consider 

    the ways in which self-regulation models and social coreg-

    ulation models have conceptualized the interdependency of 

    self- and coregulatory processes. This is done first through

    reviewing the role of social regulatory mechanisms in self-

    regulation models and second through examining the place

    of self-regulation in social models of regulation. The pur-

     pose of taking a systemic, cross-level approach is intended 

    to avoid dichotomizing the regulatory constructs and instead 

    highlighting the diverse ways in which they have been related 

    to each other in various bodies of literature.

    The Role of Social Regulatory Mechanismsin Self-Regulation Models

    Self-regulation models can be found in developmental psy-

    chology, cognitive psychology, and research on learning and 

    instruction. The self-regulation models found in  develop-mental psychology   have been used to explain how young

    children gradually develop autonomy and control of their ac-

    tions (i.e., self-regulation) in interaction with others. From

    a developmental perspective, the roots of self-regulation can

     be found in the intentional actions of infants (Bandura, 1986;

    Borkowski, 1996; Schunk & Zimmerman, 1997) as they in-

    teract with significant others. This position contrasts some-

    what with the Piagetian view (DeVries, 2000; Inhelder &

    Piaget, 1958) of loosely age-related cognitive progressions

    toward greater independence from immediate sensory stimuli

    and concrete situational objects to abstract formal reasoning.

    The significance of reciprocal interactions between individ-uals aiming at self- and social regulation provided by the

    environment is common in developmental research, where,

    if not coregulation then other-regulation is seen to play an

    essential role in the growth of self-regulation capacities and 

    skills.

    A significant body of research focusing on infant– 

     parent transactions has suggested that describing person– 

    environment relationships in infancy as more “other-

    regulated” or “bound to immediate stimuli” does not

    adequately reflect early development (cf., e.g., Beebe &

    Lachmann, 1998). This research has revealed that the so-

    cial is already present in the early stage of the development

  • 8/16/2019 NT4 - Self and Social Regulation in Learning Contexts an Integrative.

    4/13

    SELF- AND SOCIAL REGULATION   217

    of self-regulation. At around 6 to 12 months of age, infants

     begin to increasingly show participation in coregulation with

     both caregivers and shared external objects (Bakeman &

    Adamson, 1984; Fogel & Thelen, 1987). Through such social

    mediation, the infant becomes distanced from the immedi-

    acy of sensorystimuli, firstthrough dyadiccoordinations, and then through “triadic” coordinations, as the infant embeds his

    or her object-focused attention into social contexts (Bakeman

    & Adamson, 1984). Along with the growing understanding

    of others as intentional agents like themselves, infants not

    only begin to accommodate to other-regulations but also par-

    ticipate and actively initiate cocoordinated actions related 

    to caregivers and to shared objects (Fogel & Thelen, 1987;

    Tomasello & Haberl, 2003). In these self-regulation models,

    therefore, elementary forms of other-, self-, and coregulatory

    functions seem to work in concert even in such early coordi-

    nations, and as such, developmental “causal primacy” cannot

     be assigned to any of them (Fogel & Thelen, 1987).

    In contrast, the self-regulation models that emerged 

    from  cognitive psychology   in the 1970s have tended—until

    recently—to focus on individuals’ unique capacity to reg-

    ulate their own endeavors. Since Flavell’s (1976) pioneer-

    ing work, metacognition, that is, “cognition about cognition”

    and “regulation of cognition,” has been recognized as a cru-

    cial concept in understanding the human mind. In general,

    metacognition has referred to the awareness that individ-

    ual learners have about their general cognitive strengths and 

    weaknesses, and of the cognitive resources they apply to meet

    the demands of particular tasks, as well as their knowledge

    and skills about how to regulate engagement in tasks, to adapt

    to situational demands, or to optimize learning processes and outcomes (cf., e.g., Winne & Perry, 2000).

    Once applied to the field of   learning and instruc-

    tion, the concept of self-regulation has been transformed 

    into self-regulated learning (SRL; Boekaerts, Pintrich, &

    Zeidner, 2000; Schunk & Zimmerman, 1997, 2008; Zimmer-

    man, 2000), with a suggestion that SRL may be a special case

    of self-regulation applied to academic endeavors (Dinsmore,

    Alexander, & Loughlin, 2008). SRL refers to the skill or 

    executive aspect of metacognition, understood as individual

    dynamic awareness, monitoring and regulation of cognitive

     processes during engagement in academic tasks, that is, any

    cognitive activities endorsed in learning environments (e.g.,

    writing, reading comprehension, problem solving; Brown,

    1987; Flavell, Miller, & Miller, 1993). Self-regulated learn-

    ers are viewed as capable of generating internal feedback 

    from their cognitive processing (Butler & Winne, 1995) as

    well as being adept at modifying their learning behaviors

    in response to shifting situational demands or conditions

    (Zimmerman, 1989), thus possessing   agency   of their own

    actions. An important aspect of the self-regulation models

    grounded in cognitive psychology is, therefore, the adjust-

    ment of one’s ongoing action to perceived (current) or an-

    ticipated (future) changes in the affordances and constraints

    of the environment. This, according to Fogel (1992), may

    involve counteracting inertia, the natural tendency to con-

    tinue the existing direction and intensity of behavior. Rec-

    ognizing that the environment creates affordances and con-

    straints for individual self-regulation, however, explicitly

     points to external coregulatory elements in the self-regulation

     process. The significance of others in the development of self-regulation is explicit in the seminal work of both Piaget and 

    Vygotsky (Fox & Risconscente, 2008).

    Research on SRL in the classroom has highlighted 

    these phenomena, with findings that young children’s ef-

    forts to self-regulate during task performance is not merely

    determined by personal processes but rather influenced 

     by environmental and behavioral variables in a reciprocal

    way (Meyer & Turner, 2002; Patrick & Middleton, 2002;

    Zimmerman, 2000). Over the years, many interventions

    aimed at fostering the development of self-regulation have

    therefore incorporated reciprocal regulatory processes in

    their instructional design (Bearison, Dorval, & LeBlanc,

    2001; Palincsar & Brown, 1984; Pressley, 1995). Most of 

    this research is theoretically grounded in the Vygotskyan

    (1930/1978) view of a gradual shift from other- to self-

    regulation of the mediational processes. Individual appropri-

    ation of the regulatory control processes is viewed as taking

     place through structured interactions with a more able other 

    (e.g., Palincsar & Brown, 1984) or through joint problem

    solving with peers (e.g., Artzt & Armour-Thomas, 1997).

    Hadwin, Wozney, and Pontin’s (2005) recent work labeled 

    the gradual shift in regulatory control from teacher regulation

    to learner self-regulation as a temporary process of coregu-

    lation, which was operationalized in their study as evidence

    of teacher indirect and learner indirect regulation. Hadwinet al.’s approach can be treated as hybrid in nature because

    it recognizes explicitly the contribution of social regulatory

     processes in individual’s development of self-regulation.

    Overall, the cognitive models of self-regulation have

    tended to privilege the self as a rational, thinking agent with

    goals and strategies at the expense of reciprocal regulatory

     processes in social activities. Matusov (1998) criticized this

    approach, however, arguing that such models are ethnocen-

    tric because of their limited power “for analyzing develop-

    ment in diverse socio-cultural practices where participants’

    solo activities are not necessarily privileged and emphasized”

    (p. 326). Others, like the developmental and the learning and 

    instruction models of self-regulation, have incorporated so-

    cial regulatory elements as instrumental to the development

    of self-regulation. Many of the criticisms expressed by so-

    ciocultural theorists, like Valsiner (1991, 2002), Matusov

    (1998), Wenger (1998), Rogoff (2003), and Hickey (2003),

    have therefore been directed at models of self-regulation in

    the strict cognitive tradition, which are perceived as overem-

     phasizing individual agency and treating individuals and col-

    lectivities as separate entities instead of mutually constituted.

     In sum, and consistent with their grounding in devel-

    opmental and cognitive psychology, self-regulation mod-

    els have been conceptualized mainly with a focus on

  • 8/16/2019 NT4 - Self and Social Regulation in Learning Contexts an Integrative.

    5/13

    218   VOLET, VAURAS, SALONEN

    individuals’ (conscious or unconscious) processes of adapta-

    tion to personal and contextual aspects. None of these mod-

    els, however, have totally ignored the cocontribution of social

    regulatory processes in enabling the development of self-

    regulation. Although the idea that self-regulation takes place

    in social systems where self-regulating individuals inevitablyinfluence each other would not be contested, an integration

    of self- and social regulatory processes as interdependent

    and concurrent to understand real-life time-framed collab-

    orative learning activities has not yet emerged within the

    self-regulation literature. Furthermore, the idea that individ-

    uals may be conceived as self-regulating systems like any

    other living systems, but with added metacognitive capac-

    ity, has not yet been put forward. The idea, based on living

    systems theory (Miller, 1978), that individuals represent one

    type of living systems among others is elaborated further as

     part of our theoretical argument for an integrative perspective

    of self and social regulation. We now turn to social regulation

    models, which have taken as their focus social entities and 

    stressed coregulatory processes.

    The Place of Self-Regulation in SocialRegulation Models

    The identification of social regulation models is not easy

     because the field is still emerging, messy, and in constant

    flux. Most models tend to be underdeveloped with limited 

    empirical validation, which is reflected in the diverse ways

    in which social regulation has been conceptualized. The pic-

    ture is further complicated by various appropriations of the

    term sociocultural  in the literature (cf. Nolen & Ward, 2008),a perspective commonly claimed as theoretical grounding

    for social regulation. In this section, we nevertheless attempt

    to present several models of social regulation, as they have

    emerged from three theoretical perspectives—sociocultural,

    sociocognitive, and situative—remaining fully aware that re-

    searchers’ interpretations of these perspectives do not al-

    ways converge. Following Nolen and Ward, we distinguish

    them by the ways in which each of them has construed the

    relationship of individual and context, namely, individuals’

    internalization process of social and cultural influences (so-

    ciocultural), individuals’ cognitions and interpretations of 

    contexts that afford or constrain engagement and participa-tion (sociocognitive), or whole activity systems that allow

    coconstruction and negotiation of meaning (situative). Com-

    mon across most social regulation models is the focus on

    social entities as the object of analysis, the key role given to

    the notion of coregulation, and the limited articulation with

    the concept of self-regulation. Consistent with the distinct

    theoretical groundings, the conceptualization and use of the

    term coregulation varies, which provided a useful focus for 

    our analysis of the various models of social regulation.

    Within the   sociocultural perspective   represented by

    Hickey and McCaslin (Hickey, 2003; McCaslin, 2004, 2009;

    McCaslin & Hickey, 2001), coregulation refers to the overall

    dynamic regulatory process by which the social environment

    supports individuals’ internalization of social and cultural

    influences. Within this perspective, coregulation is not di-

    rected at the achievement of explicit individual or collective

    goals (which would provide a direct link to self-regulation)

     but aimed at productive coparticipation in a social activity,with impact on individual development in the broad sense

    (e.g., identity development in McCaslin, 2009).

    The place given to the individual within these socio-

    cultural models of regulation varies, the emphasis being

    on coregulated social activities that provide opportunities

    and constraints for individuals to participate and develop

    (which includes side references to the development of self-

    regulation; e.g. McCaslin & Good, 1996). Hickey’s (2003)

    most socially focused model of coregulation stresses the

    “mutual constitution” (cf. Wenger, 1998) of individuals and 

    collectivities and pays little attention to individual psycho-

    logical processes, and in particular the personal agency un-

    derlying self-regulatory processes. According to McCaslin

    (2009), however, it is through engagement in activities and 

    emergent interactions that individuals come to internalize or 

    alternatively resist social and cultural influences, and thus de-

    velop as individuals. Her model gives personal dispositions

    and readiness a mediating role in the process but the notion

    of personal choice, a key feature of self-regulation, is also

     played down. For McCaslin, opportunity supports personal

    strivings or, alternatively, leads individuals to reconsider their 

    goals, struggle through conflict and eventually settle for an

    outcome that accommodates the multiple coregulatory pres-

    sures.

    Furthermore, and in contrast to Hickey (2003), the ideathat participation ultimately leaves some “cognitive residue”

    (Salomon, 1993, p. 133) for subsequent improved individ-

    ual participation is given some recognition in McCaslin’s (in

     press) model. Consistent with the sociocultural grounding,

    however, this learning is not conceptualized as metacogni-

    tive knowledge that could be accessed in abstraction for self-

    regulatory purposes. Instead, it represents a form of knowing

    which gets embedded in the individual’s personal dispositions

    that may mediate subsequent opportunity. How learning envi-

    ronments as social systems create opportunity and coregulate

     participants is no doubt a powerful explanatory perspective

    at the macro level of development. But the extent to which

    sociocultural models of social regulation can contribute to

    explaining development and learning at the more microlevel

    remains to be established. This might depend on how mod-

    els, such as McCaslin’s (2009), will be able to address and 

    integrate the dynamics of real-time regulatory processes that

    take place in actual social learning activities. It may be at

    that level that an explicit articulation with individual self-

    regulatory processes becomes critical.

    Other references to coregulation from a sociocultural per-

    spective can be found in Hadwin et al.’s (2005) research.

    Their hybrid approach was, however, reviewed in the previ-

    ous section as a self-regulation model due to the authors’ own

  • 8/16/2019 NT4 - Self and Social Regulation in Learning Contexts an Integrative.

    6/13

    SELF- AND SOCIAL REGULATION   219

    framing of their work within a “sociocultural perspective of 

    self-regulated learning” (p. 413). Hadwin et al.’s work repre-

    sents an interesting example of overlapping perspectives, as it

    considers self-regulation along a gradual process of appropri-

    ation where social regulation (coregulation) plays a key role

    while it maintains the ultimate focus on the self-regulatingindividual.

    In contrast to sociocultural views, coregulation from a so-

    ciocognitive perspective (Beebe & Lachmann, 1998; Iiskala,

    Vauras, & Lehtinen, 2004; Järvelä & Järvenoja, in press;

    Vauras, Iiskala, Kajamies, Kinnunen & Lehtinen, 2003) has

     been used to understand how individuals interact with oth-

    ers in a joint activity. From this perspective, coregulation

    refers to the dynamic processes of coconstructed knowledge,

    shared problem solving or other forms of collaborative learn-

    ing. The emphasis on individual learning and development

    as the outcome of participation in coregulated learning pro-

    cesses highlights that this perspective has emerged out of the

    cognitive, person-in-context perspective (Pintrich, 2000) and 

    repeated calls for a conceptual shift toward understanding

    learning, motivation, and development within social activi-

    ties and real time.

    A number of empirical studies of coregulation from a so-

    ciocognitive perspective have been carried out at microlevels

    (e.g., Iiskala et al., 2004; Järvenoja & Järvelä, 2005; Salonen,

    Vauras, & Efklides, 2005; Vauras et al., 2003) and mesolevels

    (e.g., Nolen et al., 2009) levels of learning and development,

    and with a focus on social activities that take place in real

    time. These studies have shown how peers play a mediat-

    ing role in the participation and learning of others, through

     bidirectional, reciprocal, or mutual modes of coregulation.These concepts are used to distinguish between strength

    of reciprocity, where “mutual” characterizes the strongest

    degree of sharing and symmetry between participants. The

    term  shared regulation  has also emerged from the analysis

    of highly able peers’ consensual monitoring and regulating

     joint cognitive processes in demanding joint problem-solving

    situations (Iiskala et al., 2004; Vauras et al., 2003). This phe-

    nomenon is considered the most profoundly social mode of 

    regulation, because it refers to individuals’ metacognitive

     processes that operate as a genuine social entity, aimed at a

    single direction, that is, the fully shared goal for the activity.

    In this mode, regulation is not reducible to individual activity

     but is explained by the activity of the social entity. This is re-

    flective of Salomon andGloberson’s (1987) notion that a team

    is a social system, a qualitatively different entity than a group

    of individuals working side-by-side. The metacognitive na-

    ture of such productive coregulation processes (in contrast

    to the pragmatic nature of other coregulatory processes, cf.

    Salonen et al., 2005) brings metacognitive coregulation of 

    learning close to the notion of effective self-regulated learn-

    ing, but at the level of the social entity (cf. thus the notion of 

    coordinated self-regulations) ratherthan the individual entity.

    Consistent with the cognitive theoretical underpinning,

     participants in the aforementioned studies are implicitly con-

    ceptualized as multiple self-regulating agents who coregulate

    each other’s engagement and learning, and at times oper-

    ate as a social entity or system that is not reducible to ag-

    gregated individual metacognitions. Sociocognitive perspec-

    tives of coregulation thus acknowledge the mediating role

    of individuals’ metacognitive experiences and subsequentactions in time-framed coregulated activities, an aspect un-

    derdeveloped in sociocultural perspectives of coregulation

    (e.g., Hickey, 2003; McCaslin, 2004, 2009). But the extent to

    which aggregate measures of group members’ self- or other-

    regulated learning processes lead and contribute to effective

    metacognitive coregulation of the group as a whole remains

    speculative, as it has not yet been scrutinized analytically on

    the basis of empirical evidence.

    Finally, the concept of coregulation is also found in re-

    search from a  situative perspective. This perspective, artic-

    ulated by Greeno (1998, 2006) and Nolen and colleagues

    (e.g., Nolen & Ward, 2008) calls for whole activity systems

    as the prime units of analysis on the ground that systems

    need to be understood before claims can be made about the

    individuals who constitute them (Greeno, 2006). Accord-

    ing to Nolen and Ward (2008), studying “the joint activity

    of individuals, materials, and anything else that is imbued 

    with meaning in a social and physical space” using multiple

    methodologies (e.g., cognitive tools, ethnographic analyses)

    has the potential to provide insight into the “on-going devel-

    opment of individuals, classroom communities and schools”

    as they engage in meaningful socially organized work 

    (p. 16). Although the term   coregulation   is not commonly

    used in empirical work from a situative perspective (one

    exception is Volet, Summers, & Thurman, 2009), the no-tion of social regulation is omnipresent through references

    to “discourse practices co-constructed by the community’s

     participants” (Nolen & Ward, 2008, p. 450) or ways of inter-

    acting being “co-constructed by the individual and others in

    the group” (Greeno, 2006, p. 88).

    Like the sociocultural perspective, the situative perspec-

    tive appears to pay limited attention to the development of 

    mental representations of knowledge and instead stresses

    the processes of negotiation and coconstruction of mean-

    ing that individuals cocontribute to developing in interaction

    (Greeno, 1998). Similarly, the relation between coregulatory

    and self-regulatory processes has not been elaborated upon

    within the situative perspective.

    In sum, and across sociocultural, sociocognitive, and situ-

    ative perspectives, the notion of coregulation is conceptually

    attractive, because it stresses the mutuality and sharing em-

     bedded in social regulation within group and collaborative

    learning settings. Coregulation highlights the significance

    of affordances and constraints for individual participation

    that are created in social learning environments, and where

    individuals’ sociohistorical and current processes, artifacts

    and other environmental aspects cocontribute to engagement

    and participation. Yet, despite recognition of the mediating

    role of individuals in the overall process of coregulation

  • 8/16/2019 NT4 - Self and Social Regulation in Learning Contexts an Integrative.

    7/13

    220   VOLET, VAURAS, SALONEN

    (sociocultural perspective), of reciprocal and mutual pro-

    cesses of regulation (sociocognitive perspective), and of in-

    dividual regulation within the group (situative perspective),

    there still appears to be limited integration of self- and so-

    cial regulatory processes as interdependent and concurrent

    within the social regulation literature.

    REGULATION IN LEARNING CONTEXTSRECONSIDERED: THE CASE FOR AN

    INTEGRATIVE PERSPECTIVE

    Our understanding of how self-regulation and coregulation

    cross-fertilize each other in real-life collaborative learning

    situations to create adaptive learning and development is

    still in its infancy. There is, however, growing conceptual

    agreement in the literature from both sociocultural and so-

    ciocognitive perspectives that both self and social forms

    of regulation are needed to understand regulation in actual

    learning activities. For example, sociocultural theorists, like

    Wertsch (1994), Matusov (1998), and Valsiner (2001) have

    argued that both solo and joint activities are inherently social.

    McCaslin’s (2009) model of coregulation also acknowledges

    the potentially mediating role of personal dispositions, in in-

    teraction with social and cultural sources of influences, on

    emerging identity and adaptive learning. From a sociocogni-

    tive perspective, effective coregulation of learning is assumed 

    to be goal directed and aimed at participants’ coconstruction

    of knowledge and cognitive growth for all partners. How-

    ever, the role of the self and the social is given different

    weight and lucidity depending on the perspective. Movingfrom the macrolevel and cultural and social angle, sociocul-

    tural theorists have not yet elaborated the self  in relation tothe

     social . One exception is Martin’s (2007) compelling case that

    selfhood had to be conceptualized as “social and intersubjec-

    tive” in nature” but also displaying a “socially engendered 

    reactivity that displays an always present agentic quality”

    (p. 87). Vice versa, moving from the individual, psychologi-

    cal angle, sociocognitive theorists have not yet well concep-

    tualized   the social   in relation   to the self   . We have argued 

    (Vauras, Salonen, & Kinnunen, 2008) that a group is a social

    system, a qualitatively different entity from individuals work-

    ing side by side (Hinde & Stevenson-Hinde, 1987; Salomon& Globerson, 1987), and therefore conceptualizations, which

    avoid reducing the group’s motivational and interrelational

    characteristics and functioning to its individuals’ character-

    istics, are needed to understand group dynamics as a com-

     plex situational interplay across different systemic levels.

    The latest developments of sociocognitive and situative per-

    spectives, where the focus is on interpersonal systems with

    flexible shifting of agency (e.g., Vauras, Salonen, Lehtinen &

    Kinnunen, 2009), cognitive-interactional synthesis (Greeno,

    2006; Volet et al., 2009) or continual coconstruction and ne-

    gotiation of meaning (Nolen & Ward, 2008), may lead in this

    direction.

    An integrative model, therefore, which could accommo-

    date the unique capacity of individuals to reflect upon, feel

    about, and act on their experience as they actually partici-

     pate in coregulated social activities would have significant

     potential to further advance our understanding of learning

    and development. We argue that by treating both individualsand social entities as self-regulating and coregulated living

    systems at the same time, one can avoid the danger of reduc-

    tionism of regulatory functions to either the individual or the

    social level of analysis. Conceptualizations of regulation that

    ignore the interdependence of self and social levels of analy-

    sis are faced with a number of neglected aspects. These can

     be addressed through avoiding reductionism of regulatory

    functions and adopting a concurrency principle.

    Danger of Reductionism and Need

    for a Concurrency Principle

    One of the main grounds obscuring current conceptualiza-

    tions of regulatory constructs has been the tendency toward 

    reducing the complex interplay of regulations at different

    systemic levels to the impact of one single systemic level.

    The danger of reductionism can be in two opposite direc-

    tions: reduction to the individual or reduction to the social.

    Reduction to the individual, on one hand, can be seen in most

    cognitive self-regulation models that have been formulated 

    merely in individualistic terms, with little consideration of 

    the “vertical” infiltrations from higher systemic levels (i.e.,

    interpersonal interactions, relationships, social structure, so-

    ciocultural structure), a point made by Hinde and Stevenson-Hinde (1987). Reduction to the social, on the other hand, is

    exemplified in some models grounded in a sociocultural per-

    spective. Although conceptualizing participantsand theirdis-

     positions through sociocultural participation structures and 

    social mediation, these models underestimate the simultane-

    ous effects arising from the interactions and regulations dis-

     played at other systemic levels (e.g., genetic, physiological,

    intrapsychological, interpersonal; cf. Hinde & Stevenson-

    Hinde, 1987, p. 17).

    Systemic approaches, as exemplified by living systems

    (Bailey, 2006; Miller, 1978; Weiss, 1969) and dynamic sys-

    tems (Lewis, 2000) that are widely applying regulation con-cepts, avoid the danger of reductionism through considering

    the unique contribution and relative independence of each

    systemic level but also the interdependence of systemic lev-

    els (Hinde & Stevenson-Hinde, 1987). The systemic view is

    elaborated, for example, in infant research. Beebe and Lach-

    mann (1998) discussed systems thinking in this research area

    (Fogel, 1992; Sander, 1995; Tronick, 1989), and bring forth

    the argument of self- and interactive regulation as concurrent

    and reciprocal processes, which are viewed as simultaneous,

    complementary, and optimally in dynamic balance. The self-

    regulation process and the relational process therefore simul-

    taneously affect the success of the other, with the possibility

  • 8/16/2019 NT4 - Self and Social Regulation in Learning Contexts an Integrative.

    8/13

    SELF- AND SOCIAL REGULATION   221

    of imbalanceand lack of symmetry in participants’ respective

    engagement in bidirectional coregulatory processes.

    To date, the advantage of systemic theories and their im-

     plications have not yet been taken up in educational research.

    Most empirical work on regulatory processes in educational

    contexts tends to focus on level-specific (“horizontal”) regu-lations and ignores the simultaneous, cross-level (“vertical”)

    interactions that may contribute to the developmental and 

    contextual-processual (dis)continuities (Hinde & Bateson,

    1984; Miller & Miller, 1995). In our view, the tendency to

    reduce to either the individual or the social leads to level-

    specific systemic conceptualizations that are characterized 

     by the neglect of the following four important aspects of 

    actual learning settings:

    1. The real time multimodal and multilevel processes (e.g.,

    the situational interplay of concurrent physiological, psy-

    chological and interpersonal processes, i.e. concurrency

     principle).

    2. The context (e.g., situational cues triggering differential

    appraisals and regulatory patterns in persons with differ-

    ent response tendencies).

    3. The developmental history and individual psychological

    organization (e.g., differential cognitive-developmental

    structures, cognitive-metacognitive skills, motivational

     belief-systems).

    4. The developmental history of interpersonal organization

    (e.g., participant’s differential sociocognitive response

    tendencies in different dyadic or group settings).

    These points, we believe, contribute to making a strongcase for the adoption of an integrative perspective on self- and 

    social regulation that recognizes that distinct regulatory pro-

    cesses occur concurrently at different systemic levels (e.g.,

    individual, social) and that these levels are interdependent

    and continually effecting each other.

    Argument for an Integrative Perspective

    In this section, we outline our argument for an integrative

    and concurrent conceptualization of regulatory constructs to

    explain adaptive learning in actual, time-framed collabora-tive learning contexts. First, we examine the interdependent

    role of regulatory mechanisms in living systems more gen-

    erally. We consider that this is necessary to provide broader 

    theoretical support for the need to integrate self- and coregu-

    latory constructs. Second, we argue that in regard to learning

    anddevelopment, both self- andcoregulation processes play a

    commonadaptive function,each with a distinct applied focus,

    which highlights their complementary explanatory capacity.

    Third, we briefly discuss the relationship between self- and 

    coregulatory functions to understand learning and develop-

    ment in real-life learning contexts and tease out the critical,

    dynamic role of agency.

    Interdependent role of regulatory mechanisms in living systems.   Our theoretical case for an integrative

     perspective of self- and coregulation constructs is supported 

     by living systems theory. Living systems are defined as self-

    organizing systems that have the characteristics of life and 

    that experience interactions and exchange with their environ-ment to adapt and self-maintain (Bailey, 2006; Miller, 1978).

    Living systems can be individual systems, but also biological

    or social systems, thus representing a range of disciplines,

    for example, genetics, biology, medicine, psychology, man-

    agement, sociology, social policy, and political science, to

    name just a few.

    Although Miller’s (1978) living systems theory does not

    explicitly use the terms self- and coregulation to describe how

    living systems maintain themselves and adapt in interaction

    with the environment, these terms are widely used across

    disciplines dealing with living systems. Self-regulatory pro-

    cesses are mentioned consistently as fundamental adaptive

    mechanisms of living systems, regardless of whether the sys-

    tem is a simple cell or organ like in biology or a large and 

    complex social organization like in social policy or political

    science. Through interactions with their environment, self-

    organizing systems are described as experiencing stress and 

     pressures that threaten their operations. In response, sponta-

    neous regulatory mechanisms are activated to self-adjust and 

    maintain functioning.

    In turn, coregulatory processes are used to describe the

     pull–push mechanisms that force living systems to adapt as

    they interact and reciprocally influence each other. Coreg-

    ulatory processes refer to the distributed, uncoordinated,

    and reciprocal influences of interacting systems that are lo-cated at similar or different hierarchical levels in relation

    to each other. Although references to both self-regulatory

    and coregulatory mechanisms are common across disciplines

    to explain adaptation of living systems, we were unable to

    locate any theoretical integration of these two regulatory con-

    structs. Across disciplines, self-regulatory and coregulatory

     processes tend to be called upon separately to explain a par-

    ticular regulatory phenomenon, even though the overarching

    function of both forms of regulation is adaptation.

    This brief examination of regulatory mechanisms in living

    systems more generally, provides a broader theoretical per-

    spective for an integrative and concurrent conceptualizationof regulatory constructs. Most important, it puts a stronger 

    emphasis on the “regulatory” component of self-regulation,

    at the expense of the “self,” which loses some of its individ-

    ual emphasis once it is treated as any living system, aiming

    at self-maintenance and adaptation.

    Common function and distinct foci of regulatory con- structs.   To further strengthen our case for an integrative

     perspective of self- and coregulation processes, we argue

    that in regard to learning and development, both regulatory

     processes play an adaptive function but each with a dis-

    tinct applied focus, which highlights their complementary

  • 8/16/2019 NT4 - Self and Social Regulation in Learning Contexts an Integrative.

    9/13

    222   VOLET, VAURAS, SALONEN

    explanatory capacity. Our analysis of the concurrent psycho-

    logical and social nature of self- and coregulation points to

    the view that the essence of regulatory activity is a (indi-

    vidual, group, or community) system’s  adaptive adjustment .

    Regardless of whether that system represents an individual

    or a collectivity, adaptive adjustment includes corrective ma-neuvers, with the goal of maintaining or re-establishing the

    dynamic system-environment fit. Taking as point of refer-

    ence the person(s) in actual contexts of learning, the value

    of theoretical constructs, such as self- and coregulation, is

     based in part on their capacity to tap into the essential phe-

    nomena, relationships and transactions displayed in actual

    self-governed, guided and joint learning.

    Derived from this general definition, a distinction is made

     between self-, other- and coregulation, based on the different

    foci of the regulatory activity. In the case of self-regulation,

    the actor’s corrective perception-action loops are based on

    internal reference values or norms and tend to change the

    state or direction taken by the actor’s own self-system (i.e.,

    self-focused metacognitive awareness – metacognitive con-

    trol; see Fogel, 1992). In other-regulation, a participant’s

     perceptions and corrective efforts are also based on inter-

    nal reference values or norms, but focus on another partici-

     pant’s activity (i.e., other-focused metacognitive awareness – 

    metacognitive control). In coregulation, conceptualized from

    a sociocognitive perspective, several participants, relating to

    a joint task and goal of an activity, use shared referencevalues

    and norms to maintain a joint space of activity and mutually

    correct deviations at the dyadic or group level (i.e., “we”-

    focused metacognitive awareness – metacognitive control).

    Although coregulation from a sociocultural perspective (Mc-Caslin, 2009) is taking a more macrolevel perspective, it also

    stressed the coregulating influences of sociocultural struc-

    tures, activities, and expectations on individual participation

    and adaptive learning. Although all forms of regulation have

    the common function of maintaining or restoring adaptive

    learning, each form is directed at a distinct social learning

    system (self, other, group) and therefore has its unique role

    and characteristics. As argued in our section on the danger of 

    reductionism and need for a concurrency principle, research

    that limits itself to “horizontal” or level-specific regulations

    (i.e., individual or social level) would be neglecting the sig-

    nificance of multimodal and multilevel processes, the impor-

    tance of context, and the criticality of individual as well as

    interpersonal developmental history.

    Dynamic role of agency.   Finally, our integrative per-spective also posits that self-, other-, and coregulatory pro-

    cesses need to be considered concurrently, on the ground 

    that these regulatory processes can take place simultaneously,

    may fluctuate during an activity, or can even be part of an

    activity that explicitly induces shifts in regulation based on

    agency. Although weagree that self-governed solo learning is

    socially mediated in part through internalized reference val-

    ues and norms, its perception-action loops are purely internal

    to the self-system and represent self-regulatory activity. On

    the other hand, however, there is no reason why naturally

    occurring episodes of dyadic and polyadic learning (teacher-

    /parent-guided learning, joint learning, collaborative small-

    group learning) could not display different configurations of,

    and fluctuations between, self-, other-, and coregulation. Weargue that, for example, participants’ changing relative level

    of  agency may relate to situationally triggered task- or non-

    task-focused coping tendencies. This would affect fluctua-

    tions between self-, other-, and coregulatory activities within

    dyads and small-groups.

    Summary of Our Case for an IntegrativePerspective

    Our case for an integrative perspective is based on the pro-

     posal to combine self-regulation as a fundamental concept

    to understanding human adaptation (Labouvie-Vief, 1980;

    Piaget, 1977) with coregulation as a fundamental concept

    to understanding social instructional environments (Hickey,

    2003). We have pointed out how, from a developmental per-

    spective, other-, self-, and coregulation seem to work in con-

    cert from very early in infancy, and that causal primacy can-

    not be attributed to any of them (Fogel & Thelen, 1987).

    This orchestration of simultaneous regulations at different

    systemic levels is characteristic of any living organism. Al-

    though current theories of regulation recognize both entities,

    self and social, the other entity often remains insufficiently

    addressed and analyzed either at the conceptual or empiricallevel. This has lead to an overlook of the concurrent and in-

    terdependent nature of the self and the social in the regulatory

     processes that take place in real-time collaborative learning

    activities. We argue that an integrative model, which could 

    accommodate the unique capacity of individuals to reflect

    upon, feel about, and act on their experiences, as they partic-

    ipate in learning activities, which are also socially regulated 

     by all the participants, would significantly advance our un-

    derstanding of development and learning as it takes place in

    real time.

    The core idea is that in all real-time learning activities,

    from solo to collaborative, the opportunities and constraintscreated in social interactions have to be recognized alongside

    the role of agency, andreductionism to eitherthe individual or 

    the social avoided. This has led to our conclusion that more

     powerful explanatory and predictive models of regulation

    in real-life learning activities could be developed through a

     balance and integration of self- and coregulatory processes.

    Before examining the challenges for future research on

    regulation in learning contexts, we briefly touch on one im-

     portant aspect related to the proposed integrative perspective,

    namely, regulation of motivation and emotions in learning.

    Regulation of motivation and emotions is briefly discussed 

    here, with a view to opening our perspective to the pragmatic,

  • 8/16/2019 NT4 - Self and Social Regulation in Learning Contexts an Integrative.

    10/13

    SELF- AND SOCIAL REGULATION   223

    off-task types of regulation that are frequently observed in

    interpersonal interactions during real-life learning.

    THE INTEGRATIVE PERSPECTIVE IN

    REGARD TO REGULATION OF MOTIVATIONAND EMOTIONS IN LEARNING

    Conceptualizing regulation in learning contexts as interac-

    tive and dynamic, and involving concurrent self and so-

    cial processes naturally brings the construct and its empir-

    ical validation into the field of motivation and emotions.

    Task-processing regulation carried out as a collaborative

    or group learning activity can be challenging because sev-

    eral self-regulatory systems must cooperate to generate and 

    achieve shared learning objectives. The dynamic interplay

    of multiple sociocultural worlds can create socioemotional

    challenges that interfere with task-processing regulation.

     Negative affectivity can emerge from conflicting personal

    goals, significant discrepancies in understanding of the task,

    dysfunctional relational control among participants, per-

    ceived unfairness due to imbalance in participation, or in-

    compatible interpersonal differences in styles of communi-

    cation and external constraints. The pragmatic process of 

    regulation of emotions and motivation in collaborative and 

    group learning activities has received increased attention in

    recent years. This research has built upon earlier work on

    volitional control (Corno, 1989; for a review see Pintrich,

    1999) and more recent attempts to examine the significance

    of the regulation of motivation and emotions within a self-

    regulation perspective (e.g., Boekaerts & Niemivirta, 2000;McCann & Garcia, 1999; Wolters, 1999).

    Once applied to collaborative learning activities and so-

    cially shared learning environments, conceptual and method-

    ological challenges have emerged. By nature, the process of 

    regulation of emotions and motivation within a group cannot

     be examined as dissociated from the actual social activity (as

    in inventories of regulation process), as it forms an integral

     part of the overall situated and dynamic regulation process.

    Within an individualistic perspective focusing on the expe-

    riential and psychological processes, the social context can

     be perceived as the source of emotions and fluctuations in

    individual motivated engagement. One person and the groupare expected to cope via self-regulation, and via attempts

    to regulate or even scaffold other members’ motivation and 

     behaviors. In contrast, an integrated perspective of regula-

    tion makes it possible to conceptualize the social context

    from a process-oriented approach in which the social rep-

    resents an (inter)active element that contributes to affording

    or constraining members’ participation (Järvelä, Volet, &

    Järvenoja, in press). From this perspective, both self- and 

    social regulation play a unique, concurrent role.

    Consequently, regulation of emotions, like regulation of 

    cognitions, can be conceptualized and investigated as a sin-

    gle process that also integrates self-, other- and coregula-

    tion (Järvenoja & Järvelä, 2005; Järvenoja, Volet, & Järvelä,

    2009). There is growing empirical evidence that in real-life,

    collaborative and group learning activity, regulation of emo-

    tions and motivation takes a pragmatic, instrumental role in

    trying to sustain shared regulation of task-related processing

    (Järvenoja & Järvelä, 2005; Salonen et al., 2005; Vauras etal., 2003; Volet & Mansfield, 2006).

    The extended conceptual value of an integrative perspec-

    tive on the regulation of motivation and emotion in learning

    contexts may lead to an overarching, integrative and multi-

    dimensional view of the regulation of learning. This would 

     be consistent with McCaslin’s (2004) general idea that reg-

    ulation of affect and cognitive processes cannot be sepa-

    rated. Bringing together all regulatory processes, including

    metacognitive regulation and regulation of motivation and 

    emotions, as well as self- and social regulation, would pro-

    vide a more comprehensive framework for understanding

    adaptive learning in real-life, socially challenging learning

    environments.

    CONCLUSIONS AND DIRECTIONSFOR FUTURE RESEARCH

    Our case for an integrative perspective of self- and social

    regulation in learning contexts suggests a number of direc-

    tions for future research. First, we briefly discuss two areas

    that will require research attention but have not been elab-

    orated in our argumentation, namely, the unique challenges

    created by virtual learning environments for social regula-

    tion and the significance of culture, conceptualized in multi- ple ways, on regulatory processes. Then we examine in turn

    some of the most important challenges emerging from our 

    integrative proposal, in the context of research on learning

    in real-life, time-framed collaborative activities, namely, the

    interdependence and independence of regulations at differ-

    ent systemic levels, the dynamic role of (multi)agency in

    collaborative learning settings, and the phenomena and con-

    ditions of shared regulation, from both the situational and 

    developmental aspects.

    The challenges for regulatory activities generated in vir-

    tual collaborative learning environments will need special re-

    search attention. Virtual learning environments create uniquechallenges for individual and social regulation. These can

     be triggered through the limited social and emotional cues

    available or through insufficient human scaffolding, some-

    times partly delegated to peers or technology. In any case,

    collaboration between participants is expected, and it is not

    well understood how learners actually regulate each other’s

    contribution in the absence of visible emotional cues and a

    limited sense of social presence. Furthermore, little is known

    about the extent to which metacognitive regulation is facili-

    tated, maintained, or alternatively inhibited in such contexts.

    We also need to extend our understanding of the   sig-

    nificance of culture, conceptualized in multiple ways, on

  • 8/16/2019 NT4 - Self and Social Regulation in Learning Contexts an Integrative.

    11/13

    224   VOLET, VAURAS, SALONEN

    regulatoryprocesses in adaptive learning. It would be particu-

    larly useful to investigate the regulatory patterns of teachers

    and students, as well as students and students, in cultural

    contexts that give more or less importance to autonomy and 

    collaboration in particular activities. One would also expect

    that metacognitive scaffolding and guiding processes might be conceptualized differently depending on beliefs about de-

    velopment, and the respective role of teachers and learners.

    Multicultural environments generate their own challenges

    and this may lead to fruitful questions for research on adap-

    tive learning and regulatory processes that relate to the di-

    versity of student populations.

    To deepen our understanding of processes of individual

    and social regulation in real-life, time-framed collaborative

    activities, one of the most urgent tasks will be to clarify

    the unique contribution and   relative independence of each

     systemic level  (individual, social) in relation to their  simul-

    taneous interdependence, and in particular, how to concep-

    tualize top-down and bottom-up interactions and regulations

    at each level. There is a need to pay attention to the fluc-

    tuation patterns between forms of regulation that can be

    observed in actual real-life learning patterns, with a view

    to identifying adaptive pathways that could form the basis

    of design experiments. This calls for a search for research

    designs, methodologies, data sources, and methods of anal-

    yses that would be best suited to examining the complex

    relationships between social elements, such as relationships

    and sociocultural structures, and individual elements, such as

     physiological and intrapsychological processes. Illustrations

    of empirical work combining different systemic levels can

     be found in developmental research grounded in dynamicsystems theories (e.g., Fogel, 1992; Thelen & Smith, 2006).

    For example, Fogel’s work on the coordination of perception

    and action during the early formation of infant movement

    stresses the coregulations across physiological, psychologi-

    cal, and social levels, even with regard to these most elemen-

    tary movements. Similarly Thelen and Smith’s research on

    understanding learning to reach and grasp objects in infants

     provides supportive evidence of the need to combine multiple

    levels of the developing system in research designs and anal-

    yses. How overall conceptual coherence can be maintained,

    if the underlying theoretical assumptions at each level differ,

     presents particular challenges in such research.

    Another important and promising area for future research

    will be to scrutinizefurther the dynamic role of agency,inpar-

    ticular the dynamic and coregulatory role of  multiple agents

    in time-framed collaborative learning settings. Individuals

    have a unique capacity to reflect on their experience as it

    relates to their engagement in coregulated activities, but to

    date, the actual nature of this reflection is not well under-

    stood. How perceptions of self-efficacy and collective effi-

    cacy interrelate in collaborative learning activities and lead 

    to specific patterns of regulation needs to be examined. These

    issues are consistent with Martin’s (2007) call for consider-

    ing more communal conceptualizations of self and agency in

    self-regulation research, and educational psychology more

    generally.

    The phenomena and conditions of shared regulation  will

    also need further investigation. There is a dearth of empirical

    research on how metacognitive and other forms of regulation

    interface in constantly evolving socially challenging real-lifesituations. Such understanding is required to investigate how

    adaptive learning could be enhanced through metacognitive

    instruction that simultaneously addresses regulation of mo-

    tivation and emotions. Rare instances of genuine forms of 

    shared regulation have been observed when individuals en-

    gage, as a single mind, in mutual, productive coconstruction

    of knowledge. What brings about such episodes of shared 

    regulation, how do these emerge, and what seems to inhibit

    their occurrence are all unanswered questions. If situational,

    instructional, or developmental patterns could be identified,

    these might guide the design of activities that promote these

     productive forms of learning. The emotional and motiva-

    tional processes inevitably play a significant role, and these

    will need to be included in research designs and analyses.

    To conclude, we wish to stress the importance of empiri-

    cally scrutinizing the fluctuation patterns between individual

    and social forms of regulation, the dynamic role of multiple

    agents in the coregulation of collaborative learning, and the

    critical events at the microlevel of time-framed learning that

    trigger particular cognitive, affective, and motivational pro-

    cesses. This calls for multimethod research designs, which

     provide a theoretically driven approach for a more coherent

    study of the complex, situational, and culturally embedded 

     processes at both individual and social levels.

    ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

    This research was supported by grants No. DP0666993

    and No. DP0986867 from the Australian Research Council,

    awarded to the first author, and grants No. 201782 and No.

    114048 from the Council for Cultural and Social Science

    Research, the Academy of Finland, awarded to the second 

    author.

    REFERENCES

    Artzt, A. F., & Armour-Thomas, E. (1997). Mathematical problem solv-

    ing in small groups: Exploring the interplay of students’ metacognitive

     behaviors, perceptions, and ability levels. The Journal of Mathematical 

     Behavior, 16 (1), 63–74.

    Bailey, K. D. (2006). Living systems theory and social entropy theory.  Sys-

    tems Research and Behavioral Science, 23, 291–300.

    Bakeman, R., & Adamson, L. B. (1984). Coordinating attention to people

    and objects in mother–infant and peer–infant interaction. Child Develop-

    ment, 55, 1278–1289.

    Bandura, A. (1986).   Social foundations of thought and action: A social-

    cognitive theory. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Bearison, D., Dorval, B., & LeBlanc, G. (2001).  Collaborative cognition:

    Children negotiating ways of knowing. Westport, CT: Greenwood Press.

  • 8/16/2019 NT4 - Self and Social Regulation in Learning Contexts an Integrative.

    12/13

    SELF- AND SOCIAL REGULATION   225

    Beebe, B., & Lachmann, F. M. (1998). Co-constructing inner and relational

     processes: Self and mutual regulation in infant research and adult devel-

    opment. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 15, 480–516.

    Boekaerts, M., & Niemivirta, M. (2000). Self-regulated learning: Finding

     balance betweenlearning goals and ego-protective goals. In M. Boekaerts,

    P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.)  Handbook of self-regulation (pp. 417– 

    450). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Boekaerts, M., Pintrich, P. R., & Zeidner, M. (Eds.) (2000).  Handbook of  

     self-regulation. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Borkowski, J. G. (1996). Metacognition: Theory or chapter heading? Learn-

    ing and Individual Differences, 8(4), 391–403.

    Brown, A. L. (1978). Knowing when, where and how to remember: A

     problem of metacognition. In R. Glaser (Ed.),  Advances in instructional 

     psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 77–165). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Brown, A.L. (1987). Metacognition, executive control, self-regulation, and 

    other more mysterious mechanisms. In F. Weinert & R. Kluwe (Eds.),

     Metacognition, motivation and understanding   (pp. 65–116). Hillsdale,

     NJ: Erlbaum.

    Butler, D. L., & Winne, P. H. (1995). Feedback and self-regulated learning:

    A theoretical synthesis. Review of Educational Research, 65, 245–281.

    Collins,A., Brown, J.S., & Newman, S. E. (1989). Cognitive apprenticeship:

    Teaching thecraftsof reading, writing, andmathematics. In L. B.Resnick (Ed.),  Knowing, learning, and instruction: Essays in honor of Robert 

    Glaser  (pp. 453–494). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Corno, L. (1989). Self-regulated learning: A volitional analysis. In B.

    Zimmerman & D. Schunk (Eds.)  Self-regulated learning and academic

    achievement  (pp. 111–142). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    DeVries, R. (2000). Vygotsky, Piaget, and education: A reciprocal assimi-

    lation of theories and educational practices. New Ideas in Psychology, 18,

    187–213.

    Dinsmore, D. L., Alexander, P. A., & Loughlin, S. M. (2008). Focusing

    the conceptual lens on metacognition, self-regulation and self-regulated 

    learning. Educational Psychology Review, 20, 291–409.

    Flavell, J. H. (1976). Metacognitive aspects of problem solving. In L. B.

    Resnick (Ed.),  The nature of intelligence   (pp. 231–236). Hillsdale, NJ:

    Erlbaum.

    Flavell, J. H., Miller, P. H., & Miller, S. A. (1993).  Cognitive development (3rd ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Fogel, A. (1992). Co-regulation, perception and action: Reply to reactions.

     Human Movement Science, 11, 505–523.

    Fogel, A. (1993). Developing through relationships: origins of communica-

    tion, self, and culture.  Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Fogel, A., & Thelen, E. (1987). Development of early expressive and com-

    municative action: Reinterpreting the evidence from dynamic systems

     perspective. Developmental Psychology, 23, 747–761.

    Fox, E., & Riconscente, M. (2008). Metacognition and self-regulation in

    James, Piaget and Vygotsky.  Educational Psychology Review, 20, 373– 

    389.

    Greeno, J. G. (1998). The situativity of knowing, learning and research.

     American Psychologist, 53(1), 5–26.

    Greeno, J. G. (2006). Learning in activity. In R. K. Sawyer (Ed.),   The

    Cambridge handbook of the learning sciences  (pp. 79–96). New York:Cambridge University Press.

    Hadwin, A. F., Wozney, L., & Pontin, O. (2005). Scaffolding the appro-

     priation of self-regulatory activity: A socio-cultural analysis of changes

    in teacher-student discourse about a graduate research portfolio.  Instruc-

    tional Science, 33, 413–450.

    Hickey, D. T. (2003). Engaged participation versus marginal nonparticipa-

    tion: A stridently sociocultural approach to achievement motivation. The

     Elementary School Journal, 103, 401–429.

    Hinde, R. A., & Bateson, P. (1984). Discontinuities versus continuities in

     behavioural development and the neglect of process. International Journal 

    of Behavioral Development, 7 , 129–143.

    Hinde, R. A., & Stevenson-Hinde, J. (1987). Interpersonal relationships and 

    child development. Developmental Review, 7, 1–21.

    Iiskala, T., Vauras, M., & Lehtinen, E. (2004). Sociallyshared metacognition

    in peer learning? Hellenic Journal of Psychology, 1, 147–178.

    Inhelder, B., & Piaget, J. (1958). The growth of logical thinking from child-

    hood to adolescence. New York: Basic Books.

    Järvelä, S., & Järvenoja, H. (in press). Socially constructed self-regulated 

    learning in collaborative learning groups. Teacher’s College Record.

    Järvelä,S., Volet, S.E.,& Järvenoja, H. (in press). Research on motivation incollaborative learning: Moving beyond the cognitive-situative divide and 

    combining individual and social processes. Educational Psychologist.

    Järvenoja, H., & Järvelä, S. (2005). How students describe the sources of 

    their emotional and motivational experiences during the learning process:

    A qualitative approach. Learning and Instruction, 15, 465–480.

    Järvenoja, H., Volet, S. E., & Järvelä, S. (2009).  Investigating students’

    regulation of emotion: An instrument to assess dynamics of regulation

     processes in socially challenging learning situations. Manuscript submit-

    ted for publication.

    Labouvie-Vief, G. (1980). Beyond formal operations: Uses and limits of 

     pure logic in life-span development. Human Development, 23, 141–161.

    Lave, J., & Wenger, E. (1991).   Situated learning: Legitimate peripheral 

     participation. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Lewis, M. D. (2000). The promise of dynamic systems approaches for an

    integrated account of human development.  Child Development ,  71, 36– 43.

    Martin, J. (2007). The selves of educational psychology: Conceptions, con-

    texts and critical considerations.  Educational Psychologist, 42, 79–89.

    Matusov, E. (1998). When solo activity is not privileged: Participation and 

    internalization models of development.  Human Development, 41, 326– 

    349.

    McCann, E. J., & Garcia, T. (1999). Maintaining motivation and regulating

    emotion: Measuring individual differences in academic volitional strate-

    gies. Learning and Individual Differences, 11, 259–279.

    McCaslin, M. (2004). Coregulation of opportunity, activity, and identity

    in student motivation. In D. M. McInerney & S. Van Etten (Eds.),  Big 

    theories revisited  (Vol. 4, pp. 249–274). Greenwich, CT: Information

    Age.

    McCaslin, M. (2009). Co-regulation of student motivation and emergent

    identity. Educational Psychologist , 44, 137–146.McCaslin, M., & Good, T. (1996). The informal curriculum. In D. Berliner 

    & R. Calfee (Eds.),  Handbook of educational psychology (pp. 622–673).

     New York: Macmillan.

    McCaslin, M., & Hickey, D. T. (2001). Educational psychology, social con-

    structivism, and educational practice: A case of emergent identity.  Edu-

    cational Psychologist, 36 , 133–140.

    Meyer,D. K., & Turner, J.C. (2002). Using instructional discourseanalysis to

    study the scaffolding of student self-regulation. EducationalPsychologist,

    37 (1), 17–25.

    Miller, J. G. (1978).  Living systems. New York: McGraw Hill.

    Miller, J. G., & Miller, J. L. (1995). Applications of living systems theory.

    Systems Practice, 8(1), 19–45.

     Nolen, S. B., & Ward, C. J. (2008). Sociocultural and situative research

    on motivation. In M. Maehr, S. Karabenick, & T. Urdan (Eds.),   Social 

     psychological perspective on motivation and achievement. Advances inmotivation and achievement   (Vol. 15, pp. 428–460). London: Emerald 

    Group.

     Nolen, S. B., Ward, C. J., Horn, I. S., Childers, S., Campbell, S. S., & Mahna,

    K. (2009). Motivation development in novice teachers: The development

    of utility filters. In M. Wosnitza, S. A. Karabenick, A. Efklides, & P.

     Nenniger (Eds.), Contemporary motivation research:Fromglobalto local 

     perspectives (pp. 265–278). Ashland, OH: Hogrefe & Huber.

    Palincsar, A. S., & Brown, A. L. (1984). Reciprocal teaching of 

    comprehension-fostering and monitoring activities.  Cognition and In-

     struction, 1, 117–175.

    Patrick, H., & Middleton, M. J. (2002). Turning the kaleidoscope: What

    we see when self-regulated learning is viewed with a qualitative lens.

     Educational Psychologist , 37 (1), 27–39.

  • 8/16/2019 NT4 - Self and Social Regulation in Learning Contexts an Integrative.

    13/13

    226   VOLET, VAURAS, SALONEN

    Piaget, J. (1977).   The development of thought: Equilibrium of cognitive

     structures. New York: Viking.

    Pintrich, P. (1999). Taking control of research on volitional control: Chal-

    lenges for future theory and research.   Learning and Individual Differ-

    ences, 11, 335–354.

    Pintrich, P. (2000). Educational psychology at the millennium: A look back 

    and a look forward.  Educational Psychologist, 35, 221–226.Pressley, M. (1995). A transactional strategies instruction Christmas carol.

    In A. McKeough, J. Lupart, & A. Marini (Eds.),   Teaching for trans-

     fer: Fostering generalization in learning   (pp. 177–214). Mahwah, NJ:

    Erlbaum.

    Rogoff, B. (2003).  The cultural nature of human development . New York:

    Oxford University Press.

    Salomon, G. (1993). No distribution without individuals’ cognition: A dy-

    namic interactional view. In G. Salomon (Ed.),  Distributed cognitions:

     Psychological and educational considerations (pp. 111–138). New York:

    Cambridge University Press.

    Salomon, G., & Globerson, T. (1987). Skill is not enough: The role of 

    mindfulness in learning andtransfer. International Journalof Educational 

     Research, 11, 623–637.

    Salonen, P., Vauras, M., & Efklides, A. (2005). Social interaction: What

    can it tell us about metacognition and coregulation in learning?  European Psychologist, 10, 199–208.

    Sander, L. (1995). Identity and the experience of specificity in a process of 

    recognition.  Psychoanalytic Dialogues, 5, 579–593.

    Schunk, D. H., & Zimmerman, B. J. (1997). Social origins of self-regulated 

    competence. Educational Psychologist, 32, 195–208.

    Schunk, D. H., & Zimmerman, B. J. (2008).  Motivation and self-regulated 

    learning: Theory, research, and applications. New York: Taylor &

    Francis.

    Thelen, E., & Smith, L. B. (2006). Dynamic systems theories. In R. M.

    Lerner (Ed.),   Theoretical models of human development. Handbook of   

    child psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 258–312). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.

    Tomasello, M., & Haberl, K. (2003). Understanding attention: 12- and 18-

    month olds know what is new for other persons.  Developmental Psychol-

    ogy, 39, 906–912.

    Tronick, E. (1989). Emotions and emotional communication in infants. American Psychologist, 44, 112–119.

    Valsiner,J. (1991). Construction of the mental: Fromthe cognitive revolution

    to the study of development.  Theory & Psychology, 1, 477–494.

    Valsiner, J. (2001). The first six years: Culture’s adventures in psychology.

    Culture and Psychology, 7 (1), 5–48.

    Valsiner,J. (2002). Mutualities under scrutiny:Dissectingthe complexwhole

    of development. Social Development, 11, 296–301.

    Vauras, M., Iiskala, T., Kajamies, A., Kinnunen, R., & Lehtinen, E. (2003).

    Shared-regulation and motivation of collaborating peers: A case analysis.

     Psychologia: An InternationalJournal of Psychology in the Orient, 46 (1),

    19–37.

    Vauras, M., Salonen, P., Lehtinen, E., & Kinnunen, R. (2009). Motivation in

    school fromcontextualand longitudinalperspective. InM. Wosnitza,S. A.

    Karabenick, A. Efklides, & P. Nenniger (Eds.),  Contemporary motivationresearch: From global to local perspectives (pp. 1–24). Cambridge, MA:

    Hogrefe

    Vauras, M., Salonen, P., & Kinnunen, R. (2008). Influences of group pro-

    cesses and interpersonal regulation on motivation,affect andachievement.

    In M. L. Maehr, S. A. Karabenick, & T. C. Urdan (Eds.),  Social psycho-

    logical perspectives. Advances in motivation and achievement  (Vol. 15,

     pp. 275–314). Bingley, UK: JAI Press–Emerald.

    Volet, S. E., & Mansfield, C. (2006). Group work at university: Significance

    of personal goals in the regulation strategies of students with positive and 

    negative appraisals. Higher Education, Research and Development, 25 ,

    341–356.

    Volet, S. E., Summers, M., & Thurman, J. (2009). High-level co-regulation

    in collaborative learning: How does it emerge and how is it sustained?

     Learning and Instruction, 19, 128–143.

    Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psycho-logical processes   (M. Cole, V. John-Steiner, & E. Souberman, Trans.).

    Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. (Original work published 

    1930)

    Weiss,P. A. (1969). Thelivingsystem:Determinism stratified.In A. Koestler 

    & J. R. Smythies(Eds.), Beyond reductionism: New perspectives in the life

     sciences. The Alpbach Symposium 1968 (pp. 3–42). London: Hutchinson.

    Wenger, E. (1998). Communities of practice: Learning, meaning, and iden-

    tity. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

    Wertsch, J. V. (1994). Mediated action in sociocultural studies. Mind, Cul-

    ture, and Activity, 1, 202–208.

    Winne, P. H., & Perry, N. E. (2000). Measuring self-regulated learning. In

    M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.),   Handbook of self-

    regulation (pp. 531–566). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.

    Wolters, C. A. (1999). The relation between high school students’ mo-

    tivational regulation and their use of learning strategies, effort and classroom performance.  Learning and Individual Differences, 11, 281– 

    299.

    Zimmerman,B. J.(1989).A social cognitive viewof self-regulatedacademic

    learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 81, 329–339.

    Zimmerman, B. J. (2000). Attaining self-regulation: A social cognitive per-

    spective. In M. Boekaerts, P. R. Pintrich, & M. Zeidner (Eds.), Handbook 

    of self-regulation (pp. 13–39). San Diego, CA: Academic Press.