music as time's measure

36
Music as Time's Measure Author(s): Manfred Clynes and Janice Walker Source: Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, Vol. 4, No. 1 (Fall, 1986), pp. 85-119 Published by: University of California Press Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/40285353 . Accessed: 09/07/2014 23:21 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. . University of California Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Upload: m-clynes

Post on 27-Jan-2017

213 views

Category:

Documents


2 download

TRANSCRIPT

Music as Time's MeasureAuthor(s): Manfred Clynes and Janice WalkerSource: Music Perception: An Interdisciplinary Journal, Vol. 4, No. 1 (Fall, 1986), pp. 85-119Published by: University of California PressStable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/40285353 .

Accessed: 09/07/2014 23:21

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range ofcontent in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new formsof scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

.

University of California Press is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to MusicPerception: An Interdisciplinary Journal.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Music Perception Fall 1986, Vol. 4, No. 1, 85-120

©1986 BY THE REGENTS OF THE UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA

Music As Time's Measure

MANFRED CLYNES & TANICE WALKER New South Wales State Conservatorium of Music

Repeated public performances including Beethoven Opus 130, 133, Haydn Opus 76, No. 5, Ravel Quartet, Janacek Quartet 2 by a well-known string quartet recorded live over a number of years in various cities and concert halls (a total of 170 entire movements) when analyzed show a striking long term ultrastability of timing; of the order of 0,2-0.6% for a number of fast and moderately fast movements. Stability was as good as that previously observed for single performers including Toscanini (Clynes & Walker, 1982; Clynes 1969). This indicates that shared musical concepts can be highly stable. Slower movements tend to be relatively less stable. Tempo compensation is often present so that if parts of a piece are played somewhat faster, other parts are correspondingly played slower to tend to maintain overall timing (as had been earlier found for single performances). Seven performances of Janacek's second quartet revealed what appeared as quantized small total duration changes. When examined for quantal tempo steps, 84 other performances of movements revealed apparent preferred tempo steps of 0.5% (0.48 ± 0.02). Performances of the first five movements of Beethoven's Opus 130 were all significantly slower when the Grosse Fuge was substituted for the finale. The high degree of stability otherwise observed indicates that environmental factors such as temperature, humidity, size of the hall, and variation of acoustics had little influence on the timing. The findings are significant in relation to the properties of the psychobiologic clocks involved; they show how musical concepts can precisely govern their long-term temporal realization, involving time spans of the order of 30 min., even when shared by four individuals; and further, that such shared concepts can remain stable over periods of several years.

Introduction

We are concerned here with temporal stabilities observed in perform- ances of large works involving four individuals working together musically over many years, the Sydney String Quartet. It is shown how musical con- cepts shared by several individuals can interact with psychobiologic clocks that may be involved in thinking and producing music. Temporal stabilities are revealed in this study also, as with single musical performances (Clynes

Requests for reprints may be sent to Manfred Clynes, Music Research Center, New South Wales State Conservatorium of Music, Sydney 2000, Australia.

85

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

86 Manfred Clynes & Janice Walker

&C Walker, 1982), greater by an order of magnitude or more than those typ- ically observed with nonmusical time intervals (Eisler, 1976).

The representation of time in the central nervous system is one of the im- portant unsolved problems in neuroscience. The problem has two main as- pects: 1 The experience of time. 2. The temporal organization of behavior and of central nervous system

functions. Whether one takes the view that time is experience only as events, as Pöp-

pel (1978) suggests or that time is biologically reckoned or measured in the absence of conscious experience as suggested by other observations involv- ing dreamless sleep, it is clear that we need a far greater knowledge of the temporal organization of nervous system interactions, in order to under- stand the control of temporal patterns of behavior, including even the sim- plest voluntary movements. This was emphasized by Lashley as long ago as 1951. Since then questions have been raised concerning psychobiologic clocks; what their anatomical characteristics may be within the structure of the brain, and their properties. Some observers have postulated the exist- ence of several clocks active in rhythmic and motor functions (Latour, 1967; Eisler, 1981; Gibbon, Church & Meek, 1984; Hopkins, 1984). Oth- ers have found evidence for quantal time perception phenomena (e.g., Kris- tofferson, 1980; 1984), some of which appear to be artifacts (Vroon, 1974).

Recently it has been possible to isolate and map the DNA sequence of the gene responsible for the diurnal clock in Drosophila {per) (Konopka & Benzer, 1971; Bargiello & Young, 1984). It turned out surprisingly that modifications in the gene that changes the diurnal clock rate also change courtship "song" rhythm behavior in the same direction, on a much shorter time scale (Kyriacou & Hall, 1980). The progress of these findings is excit- ing and foreshadows a better understanding of clock processes in man.

In human research, the investigators in the main concentrated on the per- ception of time intervals shorter than 2 sec using a variety of nonmusical techniques (Eisler, 1976; Hornstein & Rotter, 1969; Polzella, Dapolito, & Hinsman, 1977). Work by Allan (1979, 1983, 1984) suggests that earlier apparently contradictory findings concerning the question of whether or how the time sense obeys Weber and Fechner laws or Stevens functions ap- pear to be resolved in finding the exponent generally to be 1, that is, a linear proportionality. Indeed, as Mach (1865, 1885) and Pöppel (1978) have pointed out, the perception of time is not the same kind of phenomenon as the sensory perception of touch, sight, or hearing. Moreover it is clear that the experience of time is qualitatively different on different time scales (as well as under various conditions). Different processes are likely to be in- volved in the perception of time at various time scales.

Relatively little work has been reported in the literature on the percep-

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Music As Time's Measure 87

tion of time on the scale of minutes and hours. Our work with musicians involves time stabilities encompassing that range as well as shorter time spans concerned with musical rhythms. How musical rhythms are orga- nized hierarchically (in the range of up to about 15 sec) has been well de- scribed qualitatively by Martin (1972), who draws a parallel between speech accentuation and musical articulation. In our own work (Clynes, 1983; 1984; 1985; Clynes & Walker, 1982), we have been able to quantify in detail the phenomena concerning the musical pulse - how the micro- structure of individual tones of music is governed precisely by the charac- teristic musical pulse of the piece, in terms of (a) amplitude relations and (b) time deviations from arithmetic notated values.

Pulse microstructure can comprise several hierarchical levels, including as many as 48 to 96 tones in the time of a pulse unit.

Sensing of time and music are intimately interwoven. In the functioning of our brain we may even consider music as a measure of time: musical thought and musical memory are an indication of mental time rate and di- rection. (Physical law does not prescribe the direction of time, but con- scious memory does; rational thought would not be possible without the inherent knowledge of which of two events was "later" - shared indeed with animals.) Clearly, music engages and programs a psychobiologic clock, or clocks, which function subconsciously but give conscious read- outs, and what in computer function are "interrupts." These "interrupt" signals can be considered to guide the realization of the proportions of mu- sical rhythm and duration. Much remains to be known about the mecha- nisms of the psychobiologic clock or clocks involved (Church, 1984; Kris- tofferson, 1984; Gibbon et al., 1984).

The resolution of the conventional metronome is 5% between successive

steps. Most composers have limited themselves to this resolution - with some notable exceptions such as Bartok who decreased the step by a factor of two or three. However it was found that actual stabilities and resolutions of performances by Toscanini proved to be an order of magnitude greater than his own metronome indications for these performances, which he him- self marked on his scores (Clynes, 1969). Moreover the total time of per- formances as much as 12 years apart showed an extraordinary stability greater even than that of individual subunits. Such studies, not previously practicable, had become possible through modern tape-recording methods.

Later it became clear (Clynes &c Walker, 1982) that this extraordinary stability was not the particular province of the great and unique musical mind of Toscanini, as was first thought, but that other performers can un-

consciously exhibit the same remarkable temporal stability with respect to the overall length of the piece. Thus, a thoroughly known musical piece of duration of the order of 1000 sec appears to exist in the mind of a musician as an entity of precise temporal size although he is not aware of this.

Changes in duration of subsections from one performance to another often

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

8 8 Manfred Clynes & Janice Walker

tend to be compensated by changes in other subsections so as to maintain the same overall duration.

Other studies of temporal precision in musical thought and in perform- ance, involving both long and short periods (Clynes & Walker, 1982) had found further that

1. the idea of a musical beat has a considerably greater stability than the execution of individual beats, that is, the time stability of groups of beats increases the larger the number of beat groups, up to the order of 1000.

2. Music thought by musicians appears to be systematically slightly slower than when it is performed by the same musicians.

These findings concern time processes on a scale of a fraction of a second to several thousand seconds. For the shorter time periods of this range, it is found that the precision and stability of musical thought exceeds the possi- ble precision of execution. Short-term deviations from this thought preci- sion seems to be like random noise which tends to cancel out over longer periods, as the musical rhythm or beat is repeated.

In these previous studies, the temporal stability observed was that of a single musical mind, as conductor or as performer. Other studies of per- formance (Shaffer, 1984; Sternberg, Knoll, & Zukofsky, 1982) did not ad- dress the question of long-term stability.

The present study reports finding similar stability in the performance of a string quartet - four individual musical minds who work together over a number of years. Performances in different cities and concert halls of the Sydney String Quartet, an internationally recognized quartet of high stand- ard,1 were analysed covering a period from 1975 to 1979 and comprising the following works:

6 performances of Haydn Quartet Op 76, No. 5 7 performances of Beethoven Op 130

12 performances of the Ravel Quartet 11 performances of Janacek Quartet No. 2, and 3 performances of the Bartok No. 6 Quartet.

The Beethoven performances are also of particular interest in that the first three include the shorter finale composed by Beethoven for this quar- tet, while in the four later performances this finale was replaced with the Grosse Fuge, Op. 135, Beethoven's first choice, a much longer finale. This provided experimental data on the effect of the substitution of a different, longer sixth movement on the temporal sizes of the other five movements, a matter of considerable theoretic interest. The substitution of the Grosse Fuge had in fact a systematic effect on the durations of all the previous five movements.

1. Members and ages in 1979: Harry Curby, 1st violin (46), Dorai Tincu, 2nd violin (43), Alex Todicescu, violin (40), Nathan Waks, cello (28).

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Music As Time 's Measure 8 9

Of further interest is that of the 1 1 Janacek performances, the first 4 may be regarded as learning performances that document the development of the duration stability that the last 7 performances display. Thus, we have evidence here concerning the process of how the stability develops.

In quartet performance, individual parts predominate at various por- tions of the music - in general each performer has some responsibility for setting the tempo at various places in the course of the music. (In general the voice with the most notes per time unit tends to lead the tempo.) Signi- ficantly and surprisingly, the stability observed with four performers was as great as that of single performers previously studied, including Toscanini. This appears to show that the overall time concept of a piece can exist in the context of social music making (ensemble playing) as a collective mental concept.

Methods

All the performances were public performances at various concert halls, seating from 500 to 3000 people, held at various seasons in different cities and were either evening or afternoon performances. All performances had been recorded on the same Nakamishi 500 tape recorder. Each recorded performance was timed three times with a digital timing clock and mean times taken. Each day the playing tape recorder (a Nakamishi 680) was time calibrated both before and after measurement. Calibrations were done by timing three times a 10-min tape, having ten timing clicks spaced 1 min apart. All tapes were also checked against tuning - the tuning process of the quartet had also been recorded. (The latter, however, can be taken only as a crude timing guide.) Timing stability of the equipment proved to have a standard deviation of ± 0.06 sec in 10 min or ± 0.01%.

All subsections were timed to end at the beginning of the last tone of the music. This was in order to avoid distortions in timing because of the difficult timing determination of the end of a decaying tone. Each piece was timed cumulatively. All timing was done by playing on a Nakamishi 680 tape recorder, a machine of greater precision than the Nakamishi 500. Standard deviation between repeated timings of the same performance was 0.01%, or 0.1 sec per 16 min.

The stability of the Nakamishi 500 during the years of recording is not known. At the time of this study it was ± 0.08 sec std. in 10 min, or + 0.013%, compared with 0.01% of the Nakamishi 680. We can be reasonably assured however that it did not get better with age.

The stability of the performances times might possibly have been even somewhat greater than was observed, since of course all sources of error would tend to decrease the measured stability.

Results

Performances of Haydn Op. 76, No. S

Table 1 and Figure 1 show the performance times of the four movements of this quartet, at the dates shown. The greatest variability is shown in the slow Largo movement; a finding that is typical - the tempo stability of

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

90 Manfred Clynes & Janice Walker

movements with a moderate or fast beat seems to be greatest and that of slow, lyrical pieces least.

It can be seen that the four stablest performances of the six performances of the Minuet have only a 0.4 second standard deviation in more than 3 min, or 0.2%. Because of this high stability, these four performances were

Fig. 1. Six performances of Haydn Quartet, Op. 76, No. 5 showing deviations from the first performance on a percentage scale. Times for the first performance (9/20/77) for each move- ment are stated on the left. The horizontal axis shows the sequence of performances and is not a linear time axis.

Table 1 Four Performances Showing Especially High Stability in the Minuet

(Haydn, Op. 76, No. 5)a

9/20/77 10/6/77 11/24/77 10/28/78 St.Dev. %

Allegretto 4:55.6 4:59.8 4:51.8 4:52.7 1.22 Largo 7:44.0 8:02.2 7:50.4 8:03.2 1.96 Menuetto 3:14.0 3:13.6 3:13.6 3:14.4 0.20 Finale 3:33.2 3:27.0 3:31.1 3:30.7 1.38

Total playing time 19:26.8 19:42.6 19:26.9 19:41.0 0.737

aNote also that performances group themselves into two pairs (1,3 and 2, 4) in regard to total playing time.

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Music As Time's Measure 9 1

chosen to investigate the detailed course of the subsections in relation to the overall stability.

The subsections here are clearly demarcated as the halves of the Minuet and Trio, respectively and are repeated except in the reprise of the Minuet. Figure 2 and Table 2 show the stability of subsections of the Minuet. It is seen that the subsections are also stable, but the stability is less than that of the overall time duration. Where portions of the performance are slightly faster, as in the performance of 9/20/77, a compensation occurs in a later section, in this instance the Trio. The total duration of the Minuet in all four performances is practically identical. The proportions of the larger sections are generally preserved, except for a slight tradeoff between the duration of the trio and the first playing of the minuet.

Fig. 2. Haydn Quartet Op. 76, No. 5, Minuet and Trio played by Sydney String Quartet. This figure illustrates the convergence of the time tracks for four performances illustrating the tendency to conserve the total performance time in spite of small fluctuations of the tim-

ing subsections. The vertical axis shows the cumulative deviation from the mean of the four

performances at any point of the performance. The slope of a line segment thus indicates how much faster or slower than the mean rate that particular section was played. One per- cent faster and slower slopes are given as calibration marks at the right bottom portion of the

figure. The figure shows that most segments were played at tempos well within 1% devia- tion from the mean tempo for that segment, and that the maximum deviation from the cu- mulative mean of any performance was less than 0.9 sec (0.4% of the total time) and was

mostly much less than that.

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

92 Manfred Clynes & Janice Walker

« on on oô on ^r *t -* «o on oo' •§

c öööö öööö öö 2 .5 'S H* ^ ^ °°. ^ ^ S 0> ÒÒ ON K OO Ö OO £ > rq Tï *<i ^ *3 *V. S

*<D © öö öö ö ^ •S " s 2 oo o\ ro q o E vd oo oo ^ ««

U t^ r^ ö fö .52 T3 _^_ g cd ososvot^ ooq»ooo q^I * ^ ONOnoÓON CO ̂ " ^" ^f ÖOO g «S ^^^^ ^^^^ T!^ ö Jj öööö öööö öö §< O^ o 0>tv ooro ooro ^ U

On^ osoô Kon oo ^ ^î^ ^«O (STf rn c

S2 öö öö ö .g O u ü

§ ss ^ ? ? II Z |f ^ vq r^ -^ 1-H cnrnoNvq °^^ 'S"?

•S Os ON OÔ ON ^ ^" "*" «O ON OÔ '> O ^ ^^^^ *?. ^ ^ ^ ^^ Oh

cslO öööö öööö öö 'Sg

£Os ÖÖ ÖÖ Ö ug

VîT °°. h »o i; g g 1^ \d on oo ^- .2 w

rH rH CO rH £ £

§ " * *

fr! G

" Oh-c

>* ONONOO'ON <*i:Tt:^:tO ÖOO* Sjj

PC öööö öööö öö *^v2 ° £ t °o *"3 ^ ^ ^ S^ onK oôon oô >^^ öS ^^ ^^ ^ ^^ g^ öö öö ö 51 2 2 S

5 ^ rH O ro > g a- 2 8 Ih nj ai

I PCS? pigS-s | If

J 1«2«2^^ o<2£$$ SAS -g ^«a ><hhN(N Hhh(SM <h(S H +3

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Music As Time's Measure 93

The first movement and finale of the Quartet, which have a relatively fast beat, have next higher degree of stability and the slow movement is least stable. (jF 3,3 test comparing these movements to the slow movement shows p < .01.) Note that the standard deviation of the whole work in seconds is actually less than that of the slow movement alone - a trend also illustrat- ing compensation.

For the first movement (Allegretto), we note that two performances clus- ter around 4 min 58 sec and the other two around 4 min 53 sec. In the Largo also two performances cluster around 8 min 3 sec while two others are around 7 min 47 sec. The total playing time for the four movements of the two performances with the slower Largo was 19 min 41.8 sec ± 0.8 sec while the two performances with the faster largo show a playing time of 19 min 26.8 sec ±0.1 sec. The difference between the two sets of total times of 15 ± 0.9 sec is essentially accounted for by the difference in the Largo (15.5 ± 3.7 sec) in these four performances.

Beethoven Op. 130

Seven performances of Beethoven's Op. 130 are presented in Table 3, Figs. 3-5. This quartet consists of six movements. The last four perform- ances substituted the Grosse Fuge (Op. 133) in place of the Allegro move- ment as a finale (sixth movt.). All the other five movements show a consid- erable slowing in every performance where the Grosse Fuge was played as the last movement. (A t test for equality of means for each movement without the Grosse Fuge vs. with the Grosse Fuge shows this to be significant at the p < .01 level.) The lowest degree of stability occurs in the fifth movement (Cavatina), a very slow movement. But even for this movement every single performance was slower when the Grosse Fuge was played than any one of the three performances with the shorter finale.

Remarkable stability is shown by the first movement in the three per- formances with the Allegro finale. This movement is quite complex in struc- ture. The timing of subsections of these performances are analyzed in Table 4. The movement is composed of both slow (Adagio) and fast (Allegro) sec- tions. Grouping the Adagio sections together we can note also here that compensation occurs so that, for the performances where the Adagio sec- tions are slightly slower, the Allegro sections are correspondingly slightly faster resulting in an overall duration of 9 min 11.6 sec ± 0.7 sec.

The time courses of these Beethoven performances are graphed in Fig- ures 3 and 4. High degrees of stability are also shown by the second and third movements. The total times of the three performances are also con- sistent. The first two performances differ by only 1 sec in 34 min. The third performance is longer, largely as a result of an extended time for the Cava- tina. Between the first two performances one can note a compensation:

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

94 Manfred Clynes & Janice Walker

.>^T" "^" OS SO <T> H t^O

S3 r^ «o ^ *o *ö cS ^ g m so r>i «o ^ vh ö«

J* G <nK oc rn h so«o^_ë-S O U T. ^ *3 ri ^ *"* fO <U •£ £ M-. S

U * ^' ^ ^ ^ SSì'S'o2

§ oo vo -a i 1 h ^ ^§^ C \ *Cή0^TÏ®<Tici9îo^^ui O g

W ^ ^h <N <N Tf *- 0> -S

i_i W J5 3 ÖD 3 i_i «a-ososocnoN'-oySbgg

O 2° ONSÔ d -*' OS <N T3^5§'2 •^ os à{ rHsd r^sd^ ̂ röd r^*ö sb'ro JS-C4-*

T^ S2 r. S 2 ~2 Is ^" I" Uh tìkì Ö T^ Q. C^ m -s- «^ r^# 2i "t ; on <« <n oo * *-* ̂ t ; ̂ ^ so Uh g **J Ö

Hja r ^< ^r ^ö ^ö ^ c5§^^ ^.S c-^-gso wo g«o grn gos j: «p rt

tì Ji g ^ >osß^HooOsosoßfnoo>sooo§soTj- T3 > O 5

^ § | * | 1 s -a s| ä| s| 5 1-s-SÌ Hcyfi I ü g I | ,» üsi

ca tils 'ES 4? a • s • s s 5 ss ijiïl ^ü^afc • • - * ~© g&g^

S- ^ «^ *. i °°. **2 Ö««3 _H * ^HrH OO OS T-l ^VO Q«"^ "S S _H 'S.«

*

S - ^HrH T. Tî

OO OS => ^2 T-l

^h ^VO

«g:? Q«"^ "S S

« S .Sjg-flrS ° ^e ££ 8 ^ doKKKoo -gas a S S «î i\ hth \ën n\o yen Noi h u fi u S \ o ^j t «o trttjS>

h u u

C oo \

T^r^ösö^isuoo

I C oo ~ T^r^ösö^isuoo - * * " g^J h ososl^sooorsj S3^«c3

Qj S© r^O O\ SO (N 00 ^*SW K ^ «o oo rn «o o^h io os ^ ** 2à*cvo

*P ^ OSt^<NsÖ<NrnOS«ÖrH't\d Eß-Sel

42 °° T^'r^ösö^lScSc CS ^ -S P C 2 'S

-S S

P S "O

ç5? ^ t t ^. ^ ^ aj « St J ä so <N<N oo <r> io IN P

aj y O J W hv

so TT^lo.so. *?. o. ^ **".*?. °°. ^^ O g g ^

2i ïo Ôs th^ sô'ro roso «Öh h-Os ^ ÖT3 ?

C °° r^t^ösö^ OjSîî

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Music As Time's Measure 95

Thus, movements 1-3 taken together correspond closely but between themselves show some degree of divergence. Further light on the nature of such divergencies is shown by the fact that the timing for the second, fourth, and sixth movements of the second performance are then almost identically preserved in the third performance; the second and third performances dif- fer mainly in the slower performance of the Cavatina in the third perform- ance. Note that the standard deviation in seconds of the total performance time (7.3 sec) is much less than the standard deviation of the Cavatina (16.0 sec) and the standard deviation of the movements taken together consid- ered independently (17.3 sec), indicating compensation, p < .005.

All performances are generally slower when the Grosse Fuge is intro- duced. Moreover, in the last four performances we can also note a decrease in stability of the other movements. (An F 3,2 test for quality of variance shows this to be significant at the p < .05 level.) The string quartet had been

Fig. 3. Cumulative timing of the three entire performances of Beethoven Quartet (each of 6 movements) showing tendency to converge, maintaining total performance time. Cumula- tive deviation from mean does not exceed 10 sec in 36 min (0.5%) anywhere and is mostly less. Largest deviations occur in the fifth movement (Cavatina), a slow movement. Calibra- tion as in Figure 2.

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

96 Manfred Clynes & Janice Walker

Fig. 4. The same performances as in Figure 3, showing an analysis of the first movement only and its subsections. This illustrates a similar convergence and conservation of total time as does the entire performance. The cumulative deviation from the mean is less than 1 sec in 9 min (0.2%) at the end of the first movement, and maximally 2 sec (0.4% of total time). Calibration as in Figure 2.

Table 4 Times for Three Performances of Beethoven Op. 130, 1st Movement3

8/5/76 8/6/76 8/13/76

Adagio O-A 1:04.6 1:05.4 1:02.6 Allegro A-B 09.6 09.5 09.4

1:14.2 1:14.9 1:12.0 Adagio etc. B-C 20.4 20.8 20.3

1:34.6 1:35.7 1:32.3 Allegro C-F 2:21.3 2:18.9 2:19.9

3:55.9 3:54.6 3:52.2 Adagio etc. F-G 40.0 41.1 41.1

4:35.9 4:357 4:33.3 Allegro G-L 3:37.0 3:37.9 3:40.6

8:12.9 8:13.6 8:13.9 Adagio etc. L-M 34.5 34.0 32.6

8:47.7 8:47.6 8:46.5 Allegro M-N 24.8 24.3 24.4

Total playing time 9:12.2 9:11.9 9:10.9

All adagio sections 2:39.5 2:41 .3 2:36.6 All allegro sections 6:32.7 6:30.6 6:34.3

aNote that there also appears to be a balance so that when allegro sections tend to be faster adagio sections tend to be slower in apparent compensation maintaining a greater overall stability.

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Music As Time's Measure 97

Fig. 5. Seven performances, the last 4 with the Grosse Fuge in place of the Allegro Vivace finale of Opus 130. Each movement is plotted at the same vertical percentage scale. Perform- ance times of the first performance of each movement are stated on the left of the figure. On the right side of the figure, the range of performance time is shown as a bounded vertical line in percentage deviation from the first performance for each movement with (solid) and with- out (broken line) the fugue. There is no overlap in the two ranges for any movement. Per- formances with the Grosse Fuge of movements 1 -5 are all considerably lower. (Dotted lines on left side of figure connect the first three performances to the last four which include the Grosse Fuge.)

used to perform the piece without the Grosse Fuge. Introducing this had a somewhat destabilizing effect, as well as increasing the duration of all other five movements. The stability remains high however, by conventional standards (standard deviation 0.4 of a step of the metronome).

Two performances of the Grosse Fuge itself are analyzed in detail in Ta- ble 5. Noteworthy is the exquisite balance between the various sections of the Grosse Fuge, a balance brought about entirely subconsciously, as it would have been beyond human capacity to select the tempo deliberately in order to bring it about.

Thus the main allegro part of the fugue, bars 30 to 158, has its trans- formed counterpart in bars 233 to 492. The first section is in quadruple time, but the later one is in |. The first section has 128 bars (512 beats) while the later has 260 bars (520 beats). Yet the performers timed the sections of

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

98 Manfred Clynes & Janice Walker

Table 5 Two Performances of Beethoven Grosse Fuge Opus 133a

Section Time Cumulative Time Cumulative Bar Nos. (sec) Time (sec) Time

17 19.6 19.6 20.3 20.3 30 33.8 53.4 _ 35.3 55.6 58 49.3 102.7 49.5 105.1 79 38.3 141.0 Zôy'z 12Q1 38.3 143.4 ZJ/<Ö ?r7R 109 56.4 197.4 Zôy'z 12Q1

56.0 199.4 ZJ/<Ö ?r7R

139 57.2 254.6 56J 256.1 158 38.0 292.6 J 37.3 293.4 -» 209 123.3 415.9 119.3 412.7 233 56.3 472.2 n 54.5 467.2 n 272 33.9 506.1 34.4 501.6 324 45.8 551.9 46.3 547.9 358 29.5 581.4 „-. ó 30.0 577.9 „- q 404 41.1 622.5

„-. ó ' 41.4 619.3 453 46.5 669.0 46.8 666.1 477 22.0 691.0 22.3 688.4 493 16.6 707.6 J 16.7 705.1 -I 533 70.6 778.2 64.5 769.6 565 27.0 805.2 27.2 796.8 609 38.6 843.8 38.3 835.1 663 64.8 908.6 63.2 898.3 701 35.7 944.3 34.6 932.9 741 32.9 977.2 33.0 965.9

Performances were 9/22/77 (Brisbane) and 9/12/77 (Towoomba). Bracketed timings show total time for the statement of the fugue (128 bars long) and its later transformation to 6/8 time (260 bars long).

237.8 and 237.9 sec, respectively in one performance and at 239.2 sec and 235.4 sec in another.2 This corresponds to a metronome mark of 129.2 and 1 3 1 . 1 for the beat of quarter notes and of dotted quarter notes, respectively for the first performance. For the other performance the corresponding metronome marks are 128.5 and 132.5. Together the two sections ac- counted for 475.7 sec in one performance and 474.6 sec in the other (mean metronome mark 130.1 and 130.5 beats per minute, respectively).

Such a remarkable outcome in proportion is a significant clue to the sub- conscious processes involved.

Furthermore, the cumulative performance time shows 293.4 vs. 292.6 sec to the end of section one, 705.1 vs. 707.6 sec to the end of the trans- formed section. The total performance time was 965.7 vs. 977.2 sec, but much of that difference is attributable to only two connecting passages or

2. We show the analysis of the first two performances of the Fuge here in detail. Corres- ponding values for the third and fourth performance are 24 1.1 and 236.1 sec, and 243.8 and 243.0 sec respectively, displaying similar behavior.

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Music As Time's Measure 99

sections played at a considerably different tempo, namely bars 511 to 533, 64.5 sec vs. 70.6 sec - probably a changed interpretation - and bars 159 to 233, 119.3 sec vs. 123.3 sec, respectively.

We see this subconscious sense of temporal proportion exemplified here, in addition to the remarkable stability of the musical ideas and concepts. It is even more significant then that these proportions and stabilities are sub- ject to influence and show flexibility in relation to the larger musical con- text within which they are placed. That the substitution of the much longer and weightier Grosse Fuge should cause all other movements to increase in temporal dimensions shows a surprising dependence of otherwise stable concepts on the larger temporal frame within which they are placed musi- cally.

Ravel Quartet

Twelve performances were recorded between June 1975 and June 1978 (Tables 6-9, Figs. 6-8). These fall into two groups (the first group of five was recorded on a different tape recorder no longer available and thus can- not be directly compared with the last group of seven). The first five from June 1975 to June 1977 appear to start from stable versions and gradually tend to become faster in the first and third movements. The second and fourth movements are stable throughout the first four performances and become faster in the fifth. This is reflected by a gradual decrease of the total time, from performances two to five.

After the fifth performance, a rethinking of the interpretation took place and performances No. 6-12, between August 1977 and June 1978, show a considerable slower tempo for all of the movements except for the fourth movement, for which the slowing down is not as marked. As work on these new conceptions of the piece progresses we note that the fourth movement, a fast movement, becomes highly stabilized in the last four performances. But since other movements fluctuate, switched between two solutions, the total time is not stable for these last performances but also appears to have switched between two values.

Thus, in the case of the Ravel Quartet, there does not appear to be an overall length of the piece fixed in the mind of the musician, in spite of re- markable stabilities of certain of the movements. It may appear that the more sensuous orientation of this music frees it from constraint of an over- all temporal dimension. Those parts of the music that have strong rhythmic elements however achieve just as high stability as can be found in Beethoven and Haydn. (Analysis of subsections of the fourth movement of the Ravel Quartet of the last four performances shows the same stability of detail as in the stable performance of Haydn and Beethoven analyzed earlier.) Thus Figure 6 shows a very different, divergent course of drift for the first five

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

1 00 Manfred Clynes & Janice Walker

^ ^^» <N CS ON «O <N VO .,1,

^Û- AVO H ^H<N 1-1 ^ g^ oo tfoo\1; O Ë ì> vß On «o Ö <N r9 OS «O *H «O »O O U

g £*>£* g JjJ ci

00 Vß ON ON CO t*^ «-I

S »««* £ § oo ^-j fo vo h^ t^ "^3 £; <N os Tf d vd o ìo «o ^ o »o o 2J g £££<? : g ^

Ö oo oo vß O vo O *^j ^ vô K rî ^ i-î b ^ O T-H rH ^- O4 g ^ ob* vö ob* ̂ t^ <u «o (N **

^ *o K h vo rj

S ^ ob'vüooiöoo «

tt> K roh1^^ °°. ^3

< g ^Û^fi^ O4 m <N (N <N O r* & *^ £ ^^i rf V£> ON ^- ON ^" ^ *^ J UrffiT

£ ^^i rf ONIOOOON 1^ U •

<g -ot-Ä- • •- • • «-g

cm h N oo i; q ^^3 Öß hs ON ON «O »O Ö OhT« G on TÏ^TÎ^ 9

OhT« 3 8

•g * r^r^ a || C h >

î^ oô »o t^ on vß öcL "^ rOOOfO N CW Cî r^* vö oo ^* vö os ^

V0 NONOO (S v2o ^ (N >O oô O\ vô ajbû O i;OOro ro

On_r: * <n 53 ^

1^ *r-HO0<Nrn TJ-S on ?99t ^ g -g

-1 sali SJB Sill Is «g-s-e |

" I

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Music As Time's Measure 101

Fig. 6. Cumulative time tracks of all movements of the Ravel Quartet showing diverging tendency and no overall compensation and conservation of total time for these initial five performances, distinctly different behavior patterns than for the Beethoven and Haydn quartets.

Table 7 Ravel String Quartet: Performance Differences in the 4th Movement3

Bar Nos. 6/9/75 4/30/76 9/25/76 5/18/78 5/25/78 5/23/78 5/29/78

1-42 33.9 34.4 35.3 35.8 36.8 37.0 37.0 43-58 49.7 49.5 50.6 51.4 52.4 52.5 53.0 59-74 1:11.6 1:10.9 1:11.7 1:13.1 1:14.2 1:14.5 1:15.0 75-89 1:33.7 1:33.1 1:33.3 1:34.7 1:36.0 1:36.8 1:37.1 90-156 2:36.9 2:36.3 2: 5J 2:40.6 2:42.8 2:43.2 2:43.5 157-170 2:47.9 2:47.7 2:47.4 2:52.7 2:54.6 2:55.1 2:55.3 171-190 3:09.8 3:09.0 3:08.7 3:15.0 3:16.3 3:16.8 3:17.5 191-209 3:31.5 3:30.1 3:29.7 3:37.1 3:38.0 3:39.1 3:38.8 210-229 3:59.4 3:57.3 3:56.7 4:05.1 4:05.5 4:06.2 4:06.4 230-276 4:42.2 4:39.8 4:39.9 4:49.2 4:50.7 4:50.3 4:50.4

aThe four last performances shown in Table 7 were analyzed with subsections showing aspects of compensation within the movement.

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

1 02 Manfred Clynes & Janice Walker

Fig. 7. Performance times of the individual movements for the same five performances c Ravel Quartet (June 75 to June 1977).

Fig. 8. Seven later performances of the Ravel Quartet (Sept. 1977 to June 1978). Not development of a higher degree of stability in movement 4.

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Music As Time* s Measure 1 03

Table 8 Ravel String Quartet:

2nd Movement: Performance Pairs That Stay within ~1 sec of Each Other

Performance Performance Performance Performance 6/9/75 2/7/77 4/30/76 9/25/76 5/18/78 5/23/78 5/25/78 6/29/78 Bar No.

15.1 15.3 15.1 15.4 15.7 15.6 15.4 15.8 1-12 35.6 35.5 35.8 35.7 36.3 36.0 35.9 36.2 13-29 47.6 47.2 47.4 47.3 48.0 48.2 48.3 48.3 30-39

1:03.6 1:03.0 1:03.6 1:03.2 1:04.6 1:05.0 1:04.6 1:04.8 40-51 1:24.3 1:23.2 1:24.0 1:23.5 1:25.2 1:25.9 1:25.3 1:25.3 52-68 1:47.8 1:46.1 1:47.3 1:47.0 1:48.7 1:49.9 1:49.1 1:48.8 69-87 2:17.0 1:16.2 2:16.6 2:18.0 2:21.9 2:22.0 2:20.8 2:18.7 88-94 2:34.5 2:34.0 2:35.4 2:36.2 2:42.6 2:42.0 2:40.2 2:37.7 95-98 2:53.9 2:52.7 2:55.7 2:55.4 3:04.4 3:03.9 2:59.9 2:57.5 99-103 3:22.3 3:20.6 3:25.4 3:23.9 3:38.7 3:38.2 3:29.6 3:27.8 104-110 3:39.7 3:59.0 4:03.1 4:01.7 4:21.4 4:21.0 4:10.3 4:10.4 111-119 4:18.6 4:16.9 4:22.4 4:21.0 4:42.7 4:42.9 4:29.9 4:31.3 120-123 4:39.1 4:38.7 4:44.5 4:43.0 5:06.4 5:07.5 4:53.6 4:55.2 124-128 5:04.0 5:03.0 5:08.2 5:07.1 5:31.3 5:32.6 5:19.1 5:19.8 129-149 5:19.0 5:18.7 5:23.2 5:22.5 5:47.5 5:48.5 5:34.9 5:35.8 150-160 5:39.2 5:38.7 5:42.4 5:42.4 6:08.0 6:08.8 5:55.5 5:55.8 161-177 6:01.3 6:02.1 6:05.0 6:05.7 6:32.1 6:32.9 6:19.3 6:19.3 178-192

Performances that appear to fall into discrete timing values analyzed with respect to sub- sections. Cumulative timing shows consistent behavior within each pair of performances. This suggests that the discrete total times would not be due to random coincidence but reflects systematic behavior throughout the movement.

performances than in the examples by Beethoven and Haydn (Figures 1, 2, and 3), whose graphs display a tendency to come together at the end of the performance despite deviations during its course.

An interesting observation is a tendency for the timings to fall into dis- tinct groups rather than random values. If we look at the last four perform- ances, for example, the times of the second movement are almost identical in performances 1-3 and also in performances 2-4. The first and third movements have a similar pattern so that performances 1-2 and perform- ances 2-4 appear to group together. This is also reflected in the total time so that performances 1-3 and performances 2-4 appear to group together.

This tendency of timings to fall into discrete values or subgroups which here would seem accidental is further explored in the analysis of 11 per- formances of the Janacek Quartet No. 2, where this pattern can be observed with systematic regularity.

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

104 Manfred Clynes & Janice Walker

Table 9 Janâcek String Quartet No. 2:11 Performances

Perform. Date lstMovt. 2ndMovt. 3rdMovt. 4thMovt.

2/9/77 5:57.6 5:46.8 5:48.1 8:02.3 11:44.4 17:32.5 25.34.8

8/9/77 6:14.3 6:06.7 5:45.6 8:15.2 12:21.0 18:06.6 26:21.8

15/9/77 6:27.3 6:08.9 6:06.4 8:34.8 12:36.2 18:42.6 27.17.4

20/9/77 6:15.8 5:56.1 5:47.6 8:11.5 12:11.9 17:59.5 26.11.0

6/10/77 6:09.5 6:02.8 5:40.3 7:57.9 12:12.3 17:52.6 25:50.5

24/11/77 6:09.8 6:01.7 5:49.3 7:49.4 12:11.5 18:00.8 25:50.2

22/3/78 6:11.1 6:02.2 5:41.5 7:43.0 12:13.3 17:54.8 25:37.8

15/5/78 6:10.1 5:56.7 5:29.8 7:42.1 12:06.8 17:36.6 25:18.7

17/5/78 6:02.6 5:52.9 5:27.1 7:43.3 11:55.5 17:22.6 25:05.9

19/5/78 6:10.2 5:54.7 5:29.5 7:43.6 12:04.9 17:34.4 25:18.0

27/5/78 6:16.0 6:01.5 5:38.8 7:56.9 12:17.5 17:56.9 25.53.2

St. Dev. (5) L06 Ili JÄÖ TÂ6 Last 7 performances Total 1.25

aThe first four may be considered as learning phases. The last 7 performances show as great a stability as may be found in the Haydn and Beethoven performances. For each per- formance the first line is the timing for each individual movement and the second line repre- sents the cumulative time from the beginning of the performance without pauses between movements. The specific patterns of stability and change are analyzed schematically in Ta- ble 10.

Janacek Quartet No. 2: Balanced and Quantized Tempo Changes

There were eleven performances between September 1977 and May 1978 (Fig. 9, Table 9). We can consider the first four performances as learn- ing stages, through which the piece gradually settled to a highly stable con- cept. The performances tend first to become slower and then faster before settling into a range maintained throughout the last seven performances. The stability here is of a very high order. Again, it is greatest in the faster movements. In performances 5-8 the first movement varies only between 6 min 9.5 sec and 6 min 1 1.1 sec with a mean of 6 min 10.1 sec and standard

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Music As Time's Measure 1 05

Fig. 9. Of the 1 1 performances of Janacek String Quartet, No. 2, the first four may be consid- ered as training phases towards the stability achieved in the last 7 performances. Note also the compensation shown in the first and third movements for the performance of 5/17/78 in comparison with the previous and following performance. These results indicate that such remarkable stability can exist also in performances of modern works.

deviation of 0.7 sec. The fourth movement varied between performances 7-10 only in the range from 7 min 42.1 sec to 7 min 43.6 sec (mean 7 min 43.0 sec, standard deviation 1.1 sec). Systematic changes in the seven per- formances are illustrated in Table 10. If we begin with the first performance of this series of seven performances and tabulate the changes from the first performance we find that the changes appear to be organized into distinct groups. We can consider performances of the first movement to fall into three groups: A, B and C, where a change from group A to B or to C is a major change, while deviations within each group are quite small in com- parison. We can tabulate the changes according to whether they increase (S) or decrease (F) the duration. Tables 10 and 1 1 show us that after seven per-

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

106 Manfred Clynes & Janice Walker

Table 10

Janäcek String Quartet No. 2: 7 Performances*

Performance 1st 2nd 3rd 4th Movements

1 A A A A comp. s f A + A = B + B

2 A A B B f f

3 A A A C 2 changes, no comp. f f

4 A B C C 2 changes, no comp. f

5 B B C C 1 change s

6 A B C C 1 change s s s s

7 C A A A 4 changes (back to essentially (1))

aThis table schematically illustrates the progression of changes throughout the seven per- formances in each movement. "A" represents the initial version, "B" and "C" discretely different subsequent versions, f and s indicate whether the change is to a faster or slower version, respectively. Note that the seventh performance is identical to the first except for a distinctly slower first movement. Two distinct changes occurring simultaneously (as be- tween performances 1 and 2) may mutually compensate or not (as between performances 2 and 3, and 3 and 4). However, with regard to movements 2, 3, and 4 the cumulative effect of the changes of the 7 performances is to revert to the timings of the first performance.

formances the result of the changes is to leave the second, third, and fourth movements essentially unchanged while the first movement has a distinct change of 1.8%. Timing deviations are plotted in Figure 10 and show the tendency for the deviations to be of discrete rather than random in nature. A statistical analysis of these performances is given in Table 12. This shows that the mean fluctuation within groups is 12 times less than between groups.

In order to test further whether the distribution of timings has statisti- cally significant gaps, that is regions of zero probability, we used the data of the seven performances of the four movements. Taking the first perform- ance as reference point and considering the percentage timing difference be-

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Music As Time's Measure 1 07

Table 11 Janacek String Quartet No. 2:

Deviations from Performance 1 (sec)a

Performances 1st 2nd 3rd 4th

1 A A A A 2 A A B B

+ 0.3 -1.1 +9.0 -8.5 3 A A A C

+ 1.6 -0.6 +1.2 -14.9 4 A B C C

+ 0.6 -6.1 -10.5 -15.8 5 B B C C

-6.9 -9.9 -13.2 -14.6 6 A B C C

+ 0.7 -8.1 -10.8 -14.3 7 C A A A

+ 6.5 -1.3 -1.5 -1.0

aDetails of times shown schematically in Table 10. Deviations in sec- onds from first performance in each movement showing the basis for grouping. All versions belonging to Group A include performances within less than .44% deviation (1.6 sec maximum). Group B at least 1.5% devia- tion. Group C a further major step (approximately 3% decrease from per- formance one in all cases except 7C where it is +1.8%).

tween this and subsequent performance yields 6 x 4 = 24 observations that were pooled.

A null hypothesis of a distribution having no "gaps" was required, and the most reasonable choice seemed to be the normal distribution with mean and variance estimated from the data. The observations were grouped into cells of size 0.5% for the absolute value. A value of 16.3 was obtained for

chi-square, compared with the 1% value of 15.1, and 0.5% value of 16.7 indicating (with a p < .01) that the gaps are not due to chance.

Thus, the quantized nature of the deviations seems to indicate that pref- erential values of timings exist. It may be that these correspond to quantized differences in the tempo concept of the music. This seems more likely than

purely physiologic reasons for the quantized behavior since performances were carried out in greatly varying environmental circumstances: halls, temperature, and humidity.

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

1 08 Manfred Clynes & Janice Walker

öS ^ $

I C & d Ö ^ vb |

C o + i i

1 * 1 la> •5 °^ °^ "*! ^ ^- K vd K on

öS $5 v? $5 $ m oo ,_i h- K

g o rn ^- r»j vo

«T vfe 8 O + + | I

sl -

o S

^ Ä ? ^ ? ? ?

fS ^ uh urn wj^ u. . <J<N

3t» ' s? s? g s? g (ÎB

' a t S S

g q^2

g « l-frdd a t ö ö »

Ä * 3 2 + ' + '

I C M >

* a C >

öS "2 oo h- <N «o h

*S o o o o o ÇJ vö vö vö vi) vë

»- ^

< < < < \<

S5 $S öS S5 ^

g -V fS T-H ^ O H ^ Ofljod <n 6 <=> o

J3 g O | | + | +

„ >B$ ri „ S Q °

^ ri

î WS IO 00 rH T-H <N tH

^ on on T-H d d d O O T-H T- I T-H T-H

\C \D VÖ VÖ ^O ^

t« T-H c« (S <tf en a Tj- cavo ^ <ï <î <3 <J <î l<3

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Music As Time's Measure 109

§ 2 S £ 2 S S ij ° ? ? ? ? ° 7 18.

"

si ^ T. 1". 1". T. ^ ^ ^

o « "5 ̂

UUUU UIU g g

ff5!

IS s S i l'i Ó o O Ö io <U ̂ + 1 + I I H

' ^ If s 00 «O 00 î| S ^ O< K ON OÔ ^ - g.

to' io* >ö «ö -5 <u g o « e

v tr u° uiu Sii T3 g -°

od do o <n S^T + 1 II I 3 ^ §

^ *' 5 § i s -s §

tv. as rv t^ oo ^ •- Ë *o«o ^^ì *CÌ tSw*^

^ f S,^ ^ S > .B

*i 2j oo a ^ > ^ ^ oo

.g -5 «

E o .«

^ <n ^ .g .5 -5 E ^

« ^3 Ö S -S G loQ IO

«^ 2 o

^ °- s § y <n vo -g g 2 - o *h >^ wo Sb g -

VC NÖ |

g- «

PQ IU jû «

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

110 Manfred Clynes & Janice Walker

Fig. 10. Seven performances of Janacek String Quartet, No. 2: discreteness of timing devia- tions from Performance 1. Each shaded block represents one timing data point in this histo- gram with 2 second bins. Note the absence of data points in the third and fourth bin and discrete patterning.

Metronome Marks of the Mind?

Having found evidence of quantumization in the Janacek performances, we can test this now predictively on previous results. We may now examine the data of previously presented performances of the 24 Beethoven, Haydn, and Ravel performances (84 movements), to see if studying the percentage

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Music As Time's Measure 111

deviation from the first performance of each series for all 84 movements reveals a like quantization.

A surprising result is then obtained: Figure 1 1 displays the autocorrela- tion function of the histograms of the timing deviations of the 84 move- ments of these works. The periodicity of the figure shows a distinctly pre- ferred timing quantum of 0.48 ± 0.02. This would seem to indicate a pre- ferred quantum step in the tempo of musical thought - a metronome mark of the mind (one-tenth the size of the conventional metronome mark); ap- plicable in a time range of 3 to 8 min.

One is not aware of such a step, consciously. A quantized rate in the main clock governing the possibility of tempo se-

Fig. 11. Autocorrelation function of the histogram of the timing deviations from the first

performance (in percent) for each movement of the Haydn, Beethoven, and Ravel quartets given in this article (84 performances). The bin size is 0.1%. Note the prevalence of a main- tained periodicity of close to 0.5% wavelength. These data do not include the Janacek per- formances in which discontinuous timing behavior was originally observed (see Figure 10). The fact that there is a sustained periodicity corresponds to a periodic signal in the histo-

gram, viz. a quantum in the rate, of size given by the mean period. The confidence level of this observation cannot be ascertained from the autocorrelation function alone.

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

1 12 Manfred Clynes & Janice Walker

lection would manifest itself in a percentage quantumization of timing du- ration appearing in various pieces in a given duration range.

The resolution of such a biologic clock rate may indeed be expected to be discrete rather than continuous. Whatever the actual construction of the clock, both molecular process considerations and neural firing events dic- tate the likelihood of such a discrete rate resolution involved in voluntary tempo choice, on any one occasion.

In this respect it may be compared to the function of the metronome's discrete marks. Moreover we have also observed longer term stability of the rate (i.e., clock). The question becomes then, what are the sizes of the "met- ronome steps" of the clock allowing tempo to be thought. Of course such quantumization may be expected to be linear over a restricted range only.

In order to determine the degree of confidence of these observations from only 84 data points which cannot be estimated from the autocorrelation function, a quantum model was then made on a computer and fitted to the data. This model represented each timing deviation (d) from the first per- formance of each series as the nearest integral number of quanta(g) plus a remainder (r) (which could be either positive or negative), d = nq + r. The calculation was repeated for a number of quanta sizes, and various ranges, and particularly in the range of 0.3 to 0.7%, with increments of 0.01. For each quantum value the squared remainder was calculated and summed for all data points 2r2. The presence of a quantum size in the data was de- tectable by a discontinuity in the slope of the squared remainder sums Sr2 plotted against quantum value. Such a discontinuity was in fact observed maximally at a value of 0. 48.

To estimate the confidence level of this observation, a test model was constructed in which the distribution of the slopes was tested by the Monte Carlo method, that is, iteration using simulated data. In the simulated data, as hypothesized in the real data, a data point consisted of an integral num- ber of quanta plus an amount of noise. Using an appropriate distribution of noise, iterations of 1000 runs were repeated for various amounts of noise.

From these results, the proportion of times the slope exceeded a given amount was calculated. This proportion, when compared to that observed with the actual data gave a significance of p < .01 to the observation that there was a quantum in the range of .48 ± 0.02. This accorded well with the visual interpretation of the autocorrelation function of the histogram of timing deviations (Fig. 11).

Nevertheless this finding should be treated with caution pending further confirmation.

Such apparent quantumization would be expected to be found in single performers, although it was found here in the performances of a group.*

*Note Added in Proof: Current experiments with single executants tend to confirm fur- ther the quantal effects reported (M. Clynes & M. Rynn, in preparation).

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Music As Time's Measure 1 13

Bartok Quartet No. 6y Three Performances

These performances, unlike others studied here, show low stability and wide fluctuations both in the time for individual movements and in the total time. However, the timings of all movements deviate in the same direction from one performance to the other. The string quartet was not as familiar with this music as with the other works reported on here and were still ex- perimenting with their interpretations. These findings appear to exemplify that stability is observed only with pieces that are thoroughly known and understood by the performers. Concepts need time to settle, to be estab- lished. Only then is stability maintained effortlessly and without conscious awareness of its existence. This need of time for musical concepts to stabi- lize touches the essence of a musical concept.

Discussion

How does our mind unconsciously calculate what music is and should be?

The findings reported here pertain to the stability of such unconscious calculations: the forming of small scale and large scale musical images.

Trying to look for an understanding of how such stability in musical thinking and performance of four different individuals can occur, we need to reflect that performances of a particular movement represent an "agreed" version - agreed to by four individuals - and that the perform- ance of the whole work represents an agreed concept of the work. The con- cept of each movement gradually achieves a more and more defined form in the minds of the players.

This very clear and definite form is then reproduced whenever a perform- ance takes place, except when there is a deliberate change in interpretation

Table 13 Times for 3 Performances of Bartok 6th String Quarteta

6/8/78 6/26/78 6/27/78

1st movement 7:51.1 7:15.4 7:19.3 2nd movement 7:46.0 7:23.1 7:30.7 3rd movement 7:12.5 6:44.3 6:53.5 4th movement 7:14.3 5:50.9 6:23.0 Total playing time 30:03.9 27:13.7 28:06.5

aShows no stability. However, timings for all movements vary in the same direction from one performance to another.

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

1 14 Manfred Clynes & Janice Walker

or a major incident (the latter appears to be quite rare in the performances analyzed here).

Ordinary environmental influences such as different halls, temperatures, humidity, food eaten, and time of the day seem to have no detectable effect. On the other hand, the substitution of a different finale in Beethoven's Op. 130 has a very strong effect on the timing of the other five movements. The new finale is much longer and weightier than the one it replaces, and all other five movements are appreciably slowed as a result. Whether such in- fluences would work in a similar direction in other cases remains to be ex- plored. This behavior also further strengthens our findings that the total duration of a piece exists in the mind of the musician as a stable entity, al- though he is not consciously aware of this.

That such inner proportions are carefully meted out is also shown by the extraordinary time plan of the performance of the Grosse Fuge. Here the duration of the first exposition of the Fuge is almost exactly replicated later with a totally different meter with different numbers of bars - again testify- ing to the perception of a major section of a piece as an entity of definite time span in the minds of the musicians. Of course when they are perform- ing the piece they have no conscious awareness of realizing that tempo so as to precisely match the total duration of the earlier section, regardless of the meter. It just happens "intuitively." But it is also "intuitively" that the mus- cles activating a finger in executing a simple voluntary movement know when to start to decelerate the movement in order to bring it to a halt at the desired point in space. The temporal organization that allows us to move a limb in a coordinated way is preprogrammed in the decision to execute the movement (Clynes, 1969; Kristofferson, 1984), as Lashley had already noted (1951; see also Stelmach, Mullins, & Teulings, 1984). As an effect of such a decision, the temporal subunits involved in the motion are harmoni- ously integrated to produce the pattern desired by that single thought. Such mind- body computation goes on subconsciously in our most ordinary ac- tivities involving voluntary movement, and perhaps it is thus not more than equally astonishing that this kind of subconscious computation should take place in the performance of large musical works.

Nevertheless, it is remarkable even beyond this that such mutually agreed concepts can remain stable in four different individuals over a long period of time. As in a group of dancers, a harmoniously planned execution of a multitude of organized movements produces a total performance, so these movements of the musicians both in detail and in their totality are governed by overriding concepts. That these concepts can be so precise and have such definite form appears to be the important aspect of these find- ings. Stabilities in concept or idea and execution could be noted before in the execution of single patterns of movements such as throwing a dart, or a ball, to hit a target. The results with large musical works are, it is believed,

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Music As Time 's Measure 115

the first time such stable mind to body interactions have been shown over such long time spans and such complex motions.

Of course the stabilities observed in our studies are far greater - by more than an order of magnitude even - than those observed in the estimation and production of time intervals in which music plays no role (Eisler, 1976 for a review of the data on such experiments). Such intervals may be said to be musically empty (but note: a rest in a musical context is not an empty interval). It appears that musical significance gives time the precise measure that these performances show - greatly beyond that of musically empty in- tervals. And only when that musical significance has stabilized in the mind is the corresponding temporal stability observable.

To be able to image and space out musical tones in a time frame implies the existence of a psychobiologic clock. A signal must be given when it is time to do the next thing (Kristofferson, 1984; Gibbon et al., 1984; Church, 1984; Wing, Keele, & Margolin, 1984). That signal is clearly programmed - something like setting an alarm clock on a miniature scale. Neither an alarm nor a computer interrupt can operate without a clock and somebody has to set the alarm or set the number of ticks to be counted for the next event. These appear to be minimum requirements for a psycho- biologic clock whose unconscious function would help to translate a musi- cal concept into a moving performance.3

Even more mysterious is the way in which the music concept is converted to the right alarm setting. Our studies of the inner pulse and essentic form offer some guidance to this question. These studies have isolated forms in time as precise entities associated with the expression of specific qualities and emotions (Clynes, 1973; 1977a; 1980; 1983; Clynes & Nettheim, 1982). Time form printing by the central nervous system (Clynes, 1977b; Clynes & Walker, 1982) requires that only one decision be made for the initial form of the inner pulse of the music - subsequent repetitions are "printed" out without substantial conscious attention. Thus, the initial pre- cision of thought in forming the musical pulse carries through subsequent repetitions. What we see in these data then is that both the thought and its repetition are highly precise for these musicians in this musical context. This does not mean that the pulse is not modulated by the musical require- ments, but that it is reliably modulated according to the musical influences from one performance to another.

We can also try to formulate the problem in terms of Marvin Minsky's notion of a Society of Mind (Minsky, 1982), a society of a number of quasi-

3. Oscillating circuits controlled for their frequency of oscillation like a metronome is set by a slider could reproduce the fine structure of the musical pulse only with elaborate nonlin- ear programming and would not have the flexibility in stop and start and changes that the real musical pulse has.

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

116 Manfred Clynes & Janice Walker

independent subconscious mental Agents. We may thus consider a hypo- thetical scenario: Clock Setter is told to set the clock by Concept Builder. Arm Mover listens to the clock signals as set by the Clock Setter and moves the arm in a way that then corresponds to the form of the concepts of Con- cept Builder. Harmoniously the unconscious Agents cooperate to produce the sound patterns - which when heard may recreate the concepts in the minds of others through an inverse process of their own Agents.

If studies of essentic form and inner pulse can tells us something about Concept Builder and his instructions to Clock Setter, the workings of Clock Setter and his clock (much of whose action appears to involve the cerebel- lum) remain very largely to be explored. Of interest here is the work on re- petitive motion in primitive organisms generated by the interaction of sim- ple neuronal networks. Such studies may eventually also make a contribution to clarify how our mind unconsciously calculates what music is and should be.

Findings of a high degree of timing stability and precision presented in this study and in our earlier reports, of interest in themselves, are significant toward further experimental work on the nature of these clocks. One may now investigate ways in which disturbing influences can be introduced bio- chemically, physiologically, and psychologically, as Hoagland (1933) first suggested, and search for the biological mechanism of these clocks from a baseline of stability and precision considerably greater than was previously held to be the case. Better understanding of their function could shed light not only on musical thinking but also on thinking, and on our ability to experience time.

Conclusions

Performances by an acclaimed string quartet of the same works, over a number of years shows a remarkable temporal stability. Stability is greatest in moderate and fast pieces (S.D. on the order of 0.4%).

The temporal stability reported here for a Quartet is as great as that ob- served and previously reported for performances of individual performers, including those of Toscanini.

Total duration of a work appears to be a stable entity; small deviations of durations of parts or of movements often tend to compensate another, maintaining overall stability.

But when the overall size of the work is changed, for example, by replac- ing the finale of Op. 130 with its original finale, the Grosse Fuge, Op. 133, the durations of the other unchanged constituent parts can be substantially altered.

Stability appears to grow over an initial period of the order of a year, after this period stability may maintain itself.

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Music As Time's Measure 117

The stability is more than an order of magnitude greater than the best results described in the research literature with time intervals without mu- sic.

This stability appears to exist in spite of environmental changes, such as different halls, temperature, humidity or time of day, encountered in the working life of concertizing musicians.

Statistical analysis of the highly stable performances of Janacek's second quartet indicates that very small changes in durations of such stable per- formances appear to occur in preferred or quantized magnitudes (p < .01).

When the previous data of this article were then examined for similar quantization, 0.5% appeared as a preferred quantum of tempo deviation for the 84 performances considered together, indicated by the autocorrela- tion function of the histogram of percentage total time deviations.

The existence of such stability is significant in terms of the problem of mind-body interaction. Musical concepts appear to precisely engage cen- tral nervous temporal processes - as their embodiments.

This stability creates opportunities for study of the effects of drugs or other environmental factors on the temporal integrating function of the psychobiologic clocks and central nervous processes involved.4

References

Allan, L. G. The perception of time. Perception & Psychophysics, 1979, 26(5), 340-354. Allan, L. G. Magnitude estimation of temporal intervals. Perception & Psychophysics,

1983, 33(1), 29-42. Allan, L. G. Contingent aftereffects in duration judgments. In Symposium on timing and

time perception. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1984, 423, 116-130. Bargiello, T. A., 8c Young, M. W. Molecular genetics of a biological clock in Drosophila.

Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U.S.A., 1984, 81, 2142-2146. Church, R. M. Properties of the Internal Clock. Annals of the New York Academy of Sci-

ences, 1984, 423, 566-582. Clynes, M. Toward a theory of man, precision of essentic form in living communication. In

K. N. Leibovic & J. C. Eccles (Eds.), Information processing in the nervous system. New York: Springer Verlag, 1969, pp. 177-206.

Clynes, M. Senties: Biocybernetics of emotion communication. Annals ot the New York Academy of Science, 1 973, 220, 55-131.

Clynes, M. Senties: The touch of emotions. New York: Doubleday Anchor, 1977a. Clynes, M. Space-time form printing by the human central nervous system. Los Angeles:

Society for Neuroscience, 1977b (abs.). Clynes, M. The communication of emotion: Theory of senties. In R. Plutchik & H. Keller-

man (Eds.), Emotion, theory, research and experience (Vol. 1). New York: Academic Press, 1980, pp. 271-300.

4. Special thanks are due to the members of the Sydney String Quartet for providing the many tapes of their performances. The help of Nigel Nettheim in the statistical evaluation of the data is also gratefully acknowledged. Thanks are also due to Jane McGlone and Dorothy Capelletto for tabulating the data and preparing the manuscript.

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

1 1 8 Manfred Clynes & Janice Walker

Clynes, M. Expressive microstructure in music, linked to living qualities. In J. Sundberg (Ed.), Studies of music performance. Publication of Royal Swedish Academy of Music, Stockholm, 1983, 39, 76-181.

Clynes, M. Music beyond the score. Presented at Symposium on Music, Reason and Emo- tion held in Ghent, Belgium, 1983. Somatics, 1984, V, No. 1, pp. 4-14, and Communi- cation & Cognition, 1985, 18(4).

Clynes, M. secrets or lire in music in analytica, studies in the description and analysis or music in honour of Ingmar Bengtsson. Publication issued by the Royal Swedish Academy of Music, Stockholm, 47, 3-15.

Clynes, M., & Nettheim, N. The living quality of music: Neurobiologie basis ot communi- cating feeling. In M. Clynes (Ed.), Music, mind and brain: The neuropsychology of mu- sic. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 47-82.

Clynes, M., ÔC Walker, J. Neurobiologie functions of rhythm, time and pulse in music. In M. Clynes (Ed.), Music, mind and brain: The neuropsychology of music. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 171-216.

Eisler, H. Experiments on subjective duration 1868-1975: A collection of power function exponents. Psychological Bulletin, 1976, 83(6), 1154-1171.

Eisler, H. Applicability of the parallel-clock model to duration discrimination. Perception & Psychophysics, 1981, 29(3), 225-233.

Gibbon, J., Church, M., & Meek, W. H. Scalar timing in memory. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1984, 423, 52-77.

Hoagland, H. The physiological control of judgments of duration: Evidence for a chemical clock. Journal of General Psychology, 1933, 9, 267-287.

Hopkins, G. W. Ultrastable stimulus-response latencies: Towards a model of response- stimulus synchronization. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1984, 423, 16- 29.

Hornstein, A. D., & Rotter, G. S. Research methodology in temporal perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1969, 79(3), 561-564.

Konopka, R. J., & Benzer, S. Clock mutants of Drosophila melanogaster. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 1971, 68(9), 2112-2116.

Kristofferson, A. B. A quantal step function in duration discrimination. Perception & Psy- chophysics, 1980, 27(4), 300-306.

Kristofferson, A. B. Quantal and deterministic timing in human duration discrimination, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1984, 423, 3-15.

Kyriacou, C. P., & Hall, J. C. Circadian rhythm mutations in Drosophila melanogaster af- fect short-term fluctuations in the male's courtship song. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences U.S.A., 1980, 77(11), 6729-6733.

Lashley, K. S. The problem of serial order in behavior. In L. A. Jeffress (Ed.), Cerebral mech- anisms in behavior. New York: Wiley, 1951, pp. 112-136.

Latour, P. L. Evidence of internal clocks in the human operator. Ada Psychologica, 1967, 27,341-348.

Mach, E. Untersuchungen über den Zeitsinn des Ohres. Sitzungsberichte der mathem.- naturwiss. Klasse d. Kaiserl. Akademie d. Wissenschaften 51, II.Abt. Heft 1-5, 133- 150.

Mach, E. Die Analyse der Empfindungen und das Verhältnis des Physischen zum Psychis- chen. Jena: G. Fischer, 1885.

Martin, J. G. Rhythmic (hierarchical) versus serial structure in speech and other behavior. Psychological Review, 1972, 79(6), pp. 487-509.

Minsky, M. Music, mind, and meaning. In M. Clynes (Ed.), Music, mind and brain: The neuropsychology of music. New York: Plenum Press, 1982, pp. 1-19.

Polzella, D. J., Dapolito, F., & Hinsman, M. C. Cerebral asymmetry in time perception. Per- ception & Psychophysics, 1977, 21(2), pp. 187-192.

Pöppel, E. Time perception. In R. Held, W. Leibowitz, & H. L. Teuber (Eds.), Handbook of sensory physiology (VIII): Perception. Heidelberg, Germany: Springer, 1978, pp. 713- 729.

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Music As Time 's Measure 119

Shaffer, H. Timing in musical performance, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1984,423,420-428.

Stelmach, G. E., Mullins, P. A., Teulings, H. Motor programming and temporal patterns in handwriting, Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1984, 423, 144-157.

Sternberg, S., Knoll, R. L., Zukofsky, P. timing by skilled musicians. In D. Deutsch (Ed.), Psychology of music. New York: Academic Press, 1982, pp. 181-239.

Treisman, M. 1 emporal rhythms and cerebral rhythms. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1984, 423, pp. 542-565.

Vroon, P. A. Is there a time quantum in duration experience? American Journal of Psychol- ogy, 1974, 57(102), 237-245.

Wing, A. M., Keele, S., Margolin, D. I. Motor disorder and the timing of repetitive move- ments. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1984, 423, pp. 183-192.

This content downloaded from 157.157.233.152 on Wed, 9 Jul 2014 23:21:21 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions