mirror neuron function: an examination of …

37
MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF DIFFERENCES RELEVANT TO EMPATHY AND AUTISM HONORS THESIS Presented to the Honors Committee of Texas State University in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for Graduation in the Honors College by Erin Celeste Timperlake San Marcos, Texas May 2014

Upload: others

Post on 28-Jan-2022

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF DIFFERENCES

RELEVANT TO EMPATHY AND AUTISM

HONORS THESIS

Presented to the Honors Committee of Texas State University in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements

for Graduation in the Honors College

by

Erin Celeste Timperlake

San Marcos, Texas May 2014

Page 2: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF DIFFERENCES

RELEVANT TO EMPATHY AND AUTISM Thesis Supervisor: ________________________________ Crystal D. Oberle, Ph.D. Department of Psychology

Approved: ____________________________________ Heather C. Galloway, Ph.D. Dean, Honors College

Page 3: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

COPYRIGHT

by

Erin Celeste Timperlake

2014

Page 4: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

FAIR USE AND AUTHOR’S PERMISSION STATEMENT

Fair Use

This work is protected by the Copyright Laws of the United States (Public Law 94-553, section 107). Consistent with fair use as defined in the Copyright Laws, brief quotations from this material are allowed with proper acknowledgment. Use of this material for financial gain without the author’s express written permission is not allowed.

Duplication Permission As the copyright holder of this work I, Erin Celeste Timperlake, authorize duplication of this work, in whole or in part, for educational or scholarly purposes only.

Page 5: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

v

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

I would like to express my very great appreciation to my thesis supervisor, Dr.

Crystal Oberle, for her guidance and encouragement in completing this project. I would

also like to thank Professors John Hood and Diann McCabe for their help and support in

the development of this topic. My grateful thanks are also extended to Dr. Heather

Galloway and the Texas State University Honors College. Special thanks should be given

to Genelle Sanders for her valuable and constructive suggestions and keen proofreading

skills. I would also like to take this moment to thank my college sponsors, Dr. and Mrs.

Roger Timperlake, who have provided me with endless support and wisdom throughout

my lifetime. Lastly, I would like to thank my roommates and friends, Amanda Dorta and

Sarah Miller, for tolerating my obscure sleep schedule and interest in this subject. I

promise we can find something else to talk about now.

Page 6: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

vi

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Page ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS .................................................................................................v ABSTRACT ...................................................................................................................... vii CHAPTER

I. INTRODUCTION ................................................................................................1

Background on Mirror Neurons ...................................................................1 Previously Proposed Relationships to Empathy and Autism .......................3 Purpose and Significance of the Study ........................................................4

II. LITERATURE REVIEW ....................................................................................6 Mirror Neurons and Behavior ......................................................................6 Mirror Neurons and Empathy ......................................................................7 Sex Differences in Empathy ......................................................................12 Autism and Mirror Neuron Dysfunction ....................................................14 Sex Differences in Autism .........................................................................16

III. DISCUSSION ..................................................................................................19 Discussion of Past Research ......................................................................19 Implications and Applications ...................................................................20 Future Research .........................................................................................21 Conclusion .................................................................................................22

REFERENCES ..................................................................................................................23

Page 7: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

vii

ABSTRACT

This thesis presents a comprehensive review of past research to investigate

whether evidence supports that the mirror neuron system of the brain is the physiological

mechanism behind our ability to empathize with others. Regarding sex differences in

empathy, females are regarded to be more empathetic than male counterparts. Research

indicates females frequently score higher on empathy quotients, social sensitivity, and

emotion recognition than males, and these behavioral differences correspond to changes

in the mirror neuron system. Regarding autism and empathy, research indicates

individuals diagnosed with autism have markedly diminished levels of empathy, and

possible dysfunction of the mirror neuron system. Sex differences in mirror neuron

function may further be related to the greater prevalence of these disorders in males than

females.

Key words: Mirror neurons, empathy, autism, sex differences

Page 8: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

1

I. INTRODUCTION

Background on Mirror Neurons

Mirror neurons were discovered in the early 1990s at the University of Parma

while researchers were investigating macaque monkeys’ action performance (di

Pellegrino, Fadiga, Fogassi, Gallese, & Rizzolatti, 1992; Rizzolatti, Fadiga, Gallese, &

Fogassi, 1996). Electrodes implanted in the F5 pre-motor cortex of the monkeys’ brains

revealed an interesting reaction from the neurons. It was observed that the same neuron

fired both when performing an action as well as when that same action was observed. The

neurons were believed to be a part of the brain's motor system and correlated with

specific goals rather than specific actions. The discovery of these mirror neurons has

altered the way psychologists believe humans interact with others.

Mirror neuron activation in response to observed action is more than just motor

preparation, but also understanding. The activation of the mirror neurons in observation is

representative of an individual’s mental processing as a formation of an image of the

action (Rizzolatti et al., 1996). This mental image formed in observation will later be

used when planning or preparing for the action (Rizzolatti et al., 1996). In the observer,

the meaning of the observed action does not result from the emotion it evokes, but by

matching of the observed action with the motor activity that occurs when the observer

performs the same action (Rizzolatti et al., 1996).

Page 9: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

2

The evidence that distinguishes mirror neurons from pre-motor neurons that fire

in advance of movement execution comes from further study by Rizzolatti and collegues

(1996). Using electroencephalography (EEG) measures of macaques’ motor processes,

activation of mirror neurons was observed in response to observation of an action.

However, this activation of the neurons was not followed by the action for which the

monkey had mentally prepared. The monkey observing another monkey grabbing for

food does not move his fingers. Another important aspect is that activation ceases when

the food is made available to him. Whereas, if these neurons were related to pre-motor

neurons then activation would have increased in response to preparation of movement

execution, not decreased. This supports the idea that mirror neurons are involved in more

than just motor preparation, but also awareness of another’s perception in performing an

action.

Although humans were presumed to have a mirror neuron system (MNS), for

many years, it was difficult to find conclusive evidence. The invasive measure of single-

cell recordings, used to directly study neurons, has been avoided. Since mirror neurons

could not be directly recorded, mu rhythm suppression has been utilized in studies. Mu

rhythm is the EEG measurement of the pattern of oscillating electrical frequencies

(Braadbaart, Williams, & Waiter, 2013; Cuevas, Cannon, Yoo, & Fox, 2014). Motor

neurons are normally suppressed by input through action observation or movement

execution, which is why it is used indirectly to measure mirror neuron function

(Braadbaart et al., 2013).

Studies using mu rhythm suppression in relation to MNS function were met with

skepticism since evidence of mirror neurons in humans was lacking. Multiple studies

Page 10: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

3

determined that suppression of mu rhythm did indicate an activation of the MNS

(Braadbaart et al., 2013; Oberman et al., 2013). However, concrete proof of the existence

of the MNS in humans could not be provided until single cell recordings could be

conducted.

With the development of new techniques to placate symptoms of epilepsy,

opportunities to conduct single-cell recordings of mirror neurons in humans have become

possible. Research conducted at the UCLA Medical Center on patients with intractable

epilepsy attempted to identify seizure foci in the brain using intracranial depth electrodes

implanted in the brain (Wheeler, 2013). Electrodes were implanted with the intent to

develop a surgical treatment; however, their use provided neuroscientists with the

opportunity to directly observe mirror neurons in humans (Wheeler, 2013). This study

provided a unique chance to demonstrate the existence of the MNS.

UCLA’s single-cell recording study provided direct electrophysiological evidence

for the existence of mirror neurons in the human brain (Keysers & Gazzola, 2010).

Results confirmed these neurons are located in areas of the brain associated with the

MNS, the ventral pre-motor cortex and inferior parietal lobe, as well as extending beyond

into other areas of the brain (Keysers & Gazzola, 2010). Establishing evidence of mirror

neurons in the human brain further spurred research towards understanding the function

of these neurons. However, additional research still needs to be conducted to fully

understand the presence of the MNS in the human brain.

Previously Proposed Relationships to Empathy and Autism

When Giacomo Rizzolatti and his colleagues first discovered mirror neurons at

the University of Parma, they believed these neurons had the potential to explain how and

Page 11: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

4

why we empathize with others (di Pellegrino et al., 1992; Rizzolatti et al., 1996;

Winerman, 2005). According to these researchers, observing a particular action and

performing the same action invokes the same activation in these neurons. This may

explain why we flinch when we see another individual trip or cry when watching a sad

movie. Mirror neurons are implicated in the study of empathy for this reason.

Other researchers proposed that a defect in the MNS of individuals with autism or

autism spectrum disorder (ASD) could explain their difficulty in translating action

observation to action understanding (i.e., interpreting the actions and intentions of others)

and their diminished empathizing ability (Gallese, Rochat, & Berchio, 2013; Hamilton,

2013). Individuals diagnosed with ASD, more commonly found in males than females

with a ratio of 5:1, share three similar impairments: social interaction, verbal

communication, and non-verbal communication (Baio, 2013; Gallese et al., 2013). These

communicative and social deficits encompass a lack of a "theory of mind" whereby

individuals with ASD frequently misunderstand others’ perspective.

Relevant to the MNS, research reveals that mirror neuron therapy (introducing

people to various stimuli to activate the MNS) has resulted in improvement of

comprehension of the intentions and actions of others (McGarry & Russo, 2011).

Examination of the successes and failures of these therapies could identify the function of

these neurons and further our understanding of the role they play.

Purpose and Significance of the Study

This thesis presents a comprehensive review of previous research to investigate

whether the MNS is responsible for the process of empathizing with others. Much of

research concerns sex differences in MNS function and the corresponding sex differences

Page 12: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

5

in empathetic ability. Prevalence of ASD, which is associated with diminished

empathetic ability, is also studied.

Although mirror neurons have recently been discovered, there is an abundance of

research examining the function and process of these neurons. What we know about them

is rapidly increasing and the sooner we identify their role, the more we understand motor

learning and empathy, as well as organic causes and symptoms of ASD. Understanding

the relationship between mirror neuron function and sex will further current knowledge

of ASD, as well as sex differences in empathizing with others.

Page 13: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

6

II. LITERATURE REVIEW

Mirror Neurons and Behavior

The relevance of mirror neurons is far reaching, with numerous studies examining

their role in motor learning, communication, and empathy. The most palpable role that

the neurons play is their role in motor learning. If mirror neurons comprise the

neurophysiological center transforming visual information into motor commands, then

they would indeed play a significant role in observational learning (Lago-Rodriguez,

Lopez-Alonso, & Fernández-del-Olmo; 2013). Observational learning has proven to

improve an observer’s efficiency in execution strategy and yields better performance than

physical practice alone (Al-Abood, Davids, & Bennett, 2001; Buchanan & Dean, 2010).

Observational learning incorporates imitation, which is an important development

technique in children, and mirror neurons are proposed as the mechanism behind this

method of motor learning (Braadbaart et al., 2013).

Related to motor learning, there is existing research supporting the idea that

mirror neurons are responsible for action identification and social learning (Burgess,

Arnold, Fitzgibbon, Fitzgerald, & Enticott, 2013). Studies have shown a positive

correlation between empathy self-report and MNS activation, as well as observations of

impaired imitation and empathy in ASD (Baird, Scheffer, & Wilson, 2011; Hunter,

Hurley, & Taber, 2013).

Page 14: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

7

Mirror Neurons and Empathy

Empathy allows us to relate to others and our environment. If mirror neurons are

the mechanics behind this function, then they are essentially the key to what makes us

who we are by defining our interactions with others. While some psychologists support

this idea, it is still highly debated. Since the discovery of mirror neurons in humans,

research in the field of cognitive empathy has exploded. However, it is difficult to draw

supported conclusions with the little amount of understanding we have. Several theories

have been presented to explain the function and role of the MNS in humans.

The first individual to bring the idea of empathy to light was Theodore Lipps in

1903 (Montag, Gallinat, & Heinz, 2008). Empathy was perceived as how we recognize

the mental states of other people (Montag et al., 2008). Although the idea has evolved

over time, empathy is still defined as “the experience of understanding another person’s

condition from their perspective” (“Psychology Today,” 2013). In today’s field, there are

three different forms of empathy: cognitive empathy, motor (sensory) empathy, and

affective (emotional) empathy.

Cognitive empathy is known as the ability to recognize and attribute mental states

to others (Edele, Dziobek, & Keller, 2013). It can be defined as the ability to recognize

others intentions during their actions. Experiencing emotional states of others can lead to

an understanding of reasons for action, leading to identification of the behavior intentions

(Corradini & Antonietti, 2013). This form of empathy is based on our ability to (a)

process the actions of others through observation of their behavior and facial expressions

and (b) assign an emotional state in order to determine their intention. Cognitive empathy

Page 15: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

8

is used in conversing with others. By observing facial expressions and posture, it is

possible to interpret another’s interest in the topic.

Motor, or sensory, empathy is the phenomenon of experiencing similar sensory

input as the individual experiencing the stimulus firsthand (Loggia, Mogil, & Bushnell,

2008). This is the form of empathy behind experiencing sensory pain when observing an

individual who is experiencing a type of pain. A matching sensory experience is

suggested to be the response of activation of the MNS during observation, as well as

during the immediate experience of the pain (Avenanti, Bueti, Galati, & Aglioti, 2005;

Hutchison, Davis, Lozano, Tasker, & Dostrovsky, 1999). This explains why we may

cringe when observing an individual trip or stub their toe.

Affective, or emotional, empathy is known as “the experience of feelings

congruent with another’s emotional situation” (Edele et al., 2013). This experience is also

referred to as emotional contagion, when you feel physically along with the other person

as if their emotions are contagious. An example of this would be crying in response to a

scene of a movie in which the character is sad or crying. Emotional empathy is a

connection between individuals resulting in a genuine concern for the other’s well being.

Understanding how these different forms of empathy function could potentially

illuminate the role of the MNS in the process of empathizing with others. The MNS is the

proposed biological center for humans’ use of empathy during interactions with others.

Evidence for this idea comes from (a) behavioral studies that examined the link between

imitation and empathy, (b) function neuroimaging studies that reveal a positive

correlation between mirror neuron activation and self-reported empathy scores, and (c)

Page 16: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

9

observations of impaired imitation and empathy in individuals with ASD. These studies

provide insight into how activation of the MNS is expressed in human behavior.

The MNS seems to respond to observation of others' actions and processes the

visual information into identifiable goals by establishing a relationship between the

action and corresponding intentions (Corradini & Antonietti, 2013). This function can be

applied to both cognitive and affective empathy. Facial expression is a commonly used

communication technique allowing us to nonverbally express our thoughts and emotions.

An individual with a functioning MNS would be capable of identifying facial cues and

ascribing a specific intention or emotion to each expression.

Emotionally expressive facial expressions demand subtle shifts in expression,

which is why they are often used as a stimulus of imaging studies of mirror neurons to

identify the role of these neurons in social cognition (Corradini & Antonietti, 2013). A

study conducted using preadolescent participants supported the idea that the MNS is

linked to both the subjective experience of emotion as well as the identification of the

same emotion in other individuals (Pfeiffer, Iacoboni, Mazziotta, & Dapretto, 2008).

Functional MRI results revealed that activation of the MNS in subjects during

observation and imitation of facial expressions was positively correlated with the level of

empathetic skill (Pfeiffer et al., 2008). This supports the idea that the MNS is involved in

everyday social functions.

Empathy is part of a group of related processes, including imitation. Imitation is

an observational learning technique that has been linked to function of the MNS. A study

examining imitation of facial expressions revealed a correlation between imitation

accuracy and empathetic ability (Braadbaart, de Grauw, Perrett, Waiter, & Williams,

Page 17: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

10

2014). Effective imitation of emotion in another's facial expression requires the observer

to have a previous understanding of the relationship between the emotional state behind

the expression as well as the motor plan for producing the expression (Braadbaart et al.,

2014; Carpenter, 2006).

Instances in which individuals lose the ability to imitate others provide further

support for a link between observation and perception in emotional recognition. Emotion

recognition was examined in individuals prevented from imitating muscle contraction in

facial expressions (Niedenthal, Barsalou, Winkielman, Krauth-Gruber, & Ric, 2005).

Participants’ accuracy in identifying the expressed emotion was significantly diminished.

Further support comes from individuals with Moebius syndrome in which the ability to

move facial muscles is hindered (Cole, 2001). Individuals with this syndrome were

unable to recognize the emotions expressed by others.

Considering the different types of empathy, mirror neurons may serve as the

unifying framework. Neuroimaging studies support this concept, revealing that in

different neural regions of the brain, pathways activate independently depending on the

type of empathy being observed (Bien, Roebroeck, Goebel, & Sack, 2009; Shamay-

Tsoory, Aharon- Peretz, & Perry, 2008). Correlations have also been found between grey

matter in these mirror neuron areas and empathetic behavior (Cheng, Tzeng, Decety,

lmada, & Hsieh, 2006). A positive correlation was discovered between grey matter

volume and empathetic traits, which suggests that the MNS is involved in development of

empathetic traits (Cheng et al., 2009).

Chartrand and Bargh (1999) conducted a study observing unconscious motor

mimicry action, which they called the “chameleon effect.” Chartrand and Bargh observed

Page 18: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

11

individuals’ tendency to automatically become engaged in behavior observed previously

in another. They considered this reaction to be a “perception-behavior” link. These

mimicked behaviors included actions as shaking one's foot and facial expressions such as

smiling. They considered this effect to be a passive and cognitive process requiring very

little association with an affective or an emotional state. They discovered a higher

frequency of motor imitation in individuals with higher cognitive empathy scores. Their

findings provide evidence for a link between observation and perception in regard to

empathy.

Further evidence for the MNS’s role in the different types of empathy comes from

imaging studies of motor empathy’s involvement with pain and disgust. Functional MRI

results revealed that the processing of one’s own emotional experience and the

observation of another individual undergoing a similar experience may engage

overlapping neural matter (Singer et al., 2004; Wicker et al., 2003) The results also

showed the relative activation of these neural networks correlated with participants'

individual differences in empathic abilities (Singer et al., 2004). These studies provide

evidence that the relationship between observation and perception involves empathy.

Discovering the neural networks for empathy is important to the field of mirror

neuron research and has supported the idea that the MNS is involved in the empathy

process. There is still limited knowledge of mirror neurons ascribing an emotional or

mental state to another individual. A fMRI study attempted to identify the differential

neural networks involved in “self” and “other” related attribution of emotional states

(Schulte-Rüther, Markowitsch, Fink, & Piefke, 2007). The researchers of the study

discovered attribution of emotion either to a facial expression or to the self activates brain

Page 19: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

12

areas involved in emotional processing, as well as the MNS. Their study indicated that

mirror neuron mechanisms attribution of emotions in the self in the process of empathetic

intrapersonal cognition. These results provide support of the MNS’s involvement in

social cognition, including detection and identification of intent, emotional states, or

empathy. The MNS’s function in emotional social interactions substantiates the neural

origin of the observer’s resonance of the emotional state of another individual.

The current understanding of the MNS suggests that it is broadly involved in the

empathy process. The future for MNS research is to identify its role in the forms of

empathy, including motor, emotional, and cognitive empathy. Further research needs to

be conducted in order to identify the function of the processes involved with MNS and

empathy.

Sex Differences in Empathy

Culturally, it is a common belief that women are the more empathetic and

emotional of the sexes, and behavioral research supports this belief. Women’s

interactions tend to be more intimate than men’s, despite equal levels of motivation for

empathy in both sexes (Clark & Reis, 1988). Secondly, considerable research on

cognition has revealed that women tend to be superior in verbal and social abilities

(Halpern, 2000; Wakabayashi, Sasaki, & Ogawa; 2012). Women consistently

demonstrate greater accuracy in identifying and interpreting non-verbal cues than the

opposite sex (Hall, 1978). Finally, support for the belief that women are the more

empathetic sex was largely based on self-report data on empathy quotients, which may

have resulted in a bias due to the culturally supported idea that women were more

Page 20: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

13

sensitive to emotions (Groen, Wijers, Tucha, & Althaus, 2013). Imaging techniques a

nonbiased support to research sex differences in empathetic abilities.

Recent neuroimaging studies, which remove the aforementioned bias, have only

begun to illuminate sex differences in empathetic ability and their underlying neural

mechanisms. Currently, the research focuses on sex differences in cognitive empathy,

including identification, inference, and imagination of the observer's feeling while

observing expressions of another individual (Schulte-Rüther, Markowitsch, Shah, Fink,

& Piefke, 2008). An imaging study required male and female participants to indicate the

extent of their emotional response to images exhibiting pain and suffering, in different

contexts, compared to emotionally neutral images of scenes and landscapes (Mercadillo,

Diaz, Pasaye, & Barrios, 2011). The results of this study revealed that females had more

activation in the brain regions related to cognitive empathy than their male counterparts.

As information of mirror neurons has increased, researchers have become better at

designing studies that identify and isolate mirror neuron activation providing better

insight into sex differences. Research examining sex differences in mirror neurons

reveals significant differences between the sexes; females showed significantly more

suppression of mu rhythm than male participants (Cheng, Tzeng, Decety, Imada, &

Hsieh, 2006). A similar study revealed both males and females exhibited activation

related to pain empathy, but females showed greater mu suppression for pain empathy, as

well as in non-pain simulations (Yang, Decety, Lee, Chen, & Cheng, 2008). This finding

supports the existence of sex difference in MNS activation.

A recent event-related potential (ERP) study examined sex differences in brain

activation to congruent and incongruent actions and attempted to isolate attention bias by

Page 21: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

14

adding a second task. The secondary task separated selective attention from the action-

processing task, which was automatic rather than guided by subjective evaluation

(Proverbio, Riva, & Zani, 2010). Females showed a significantly greater response in the

neural areas of the cortex associated with mirror neurons when viewing congruent

actions. The results of this study suggest that action-processing differs between males and

females. This data provides evidence that there exists a sex difference within the action-

processing center of the brain, the MNS.

In order to determine the function of the mirror neurons in the human brain, it is

important to recognize how they may differ between the sexes. Clearly defining how

these neurons differ could unlock the potential to understand how and why they work to

connect us with others. Further evidence comes from individuals with a diminished MNS

function. Individuals with autism have a dysfunction in the MNS as well as an inhibited

ability to empathize with others.

Autism and Mirror Neuron Dysfunction

Autism or ASD is characterized by deficits in three areas: social interaction,

verbal communication, and non-verbal communication (Gallese et al., 2013). Among the

symptoms concerning these deficits is the diminished ability to empathize with others,

and impaired ability to interpret the actions and intentions of others. However, certain

characteristics remain prevalent. Exploring the connection between autism and MNS

function furthers the understanding of how symptoms of ASD arise.

Currently, the neurobiology behind ASD is little understood, but recent studies

suggest dysfunction of the MNS may be responsible for social-relating characteristics of

the disorder (Enticott et al., 2012). The broken mirror theory of autism has been proposed

Page 22: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

15

as an explanatory model for these symptoms. The broken mirror theory suggests that a

dysfunction of the MNS may result in the chief diagnostic signs of ASD, which are social

isolation, lack of eye contact, poor language capacity, and absence of empathy

(Ramachandran, & Oberman, 2006). Mirror neurons have been implicated in perception

of other’s intention, interpretation of emotion, and empathy (Ramachandran & Oberman,

2006).

Evidence for the broken mirror theory comes from using imaging techniques to

compare activation in individuals with and without ASD. An electromyography (EMG)

recording study used healthy children and children with ASD as participants to examine

MNS activation during observation (Cattaneo et al., 2007). Observation of an action led

to mirrored activity in healthy participants, but this response was absent in those with

ASD. This study suggests a deficit in motor chains to understand intention.

EEG recordings have revealed disruption of mu rhythm suppression in individuals

with ASD. An early study conducted comparing individuals with ASD to a healthy

control group revealed intact mu suppression during execution (Oberman et al., 2005).

This response was absent in ASD participants during observation of action execution

(Oberman et al., 2005). Replication of this study revealed similar results and additionally

revealed a correlation between mu attenuation and imitative abilities (Bernier, Dawson,

Webb, & Murias, 2007).

Investigation into the MNS’s role in the symptoms of autism may offer an

explanation for the development of the disorder. Research suggests there are sex-

differences in the activation of mirror neurons and ASD has been linked to a dysfunction

of the MNS. Examining sex differences in ASD could further current understanding of

Page 23: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

16

the MNS’s role in how symptoms of the disorder may arise. This could provide

information necessary for improving the course of diagnosis and potential treatments.

Sex Differences in Autism

The majority of individuals diagnosed with ASD are male. The current statistics

regarding male to female ratio of ASD is estimated to range from 5.5:1.4 to 16.8:4.0

(Center for Disease Control and Prevention, 2007). Examining the current evidence for

sex differences in mirror neuron function could lead us to evidence of why this disorder

tends to affect males more than females. Moreover, the broken mirror theory of ASD

proposes that MNS dysfunction is responsible for the core social and cognitive deficits in

individuals with ASD.

Sex differences in ASD symptoms are variable, but general patterns surface.

Females with ASD tend to have greater communication deficits than males, who tend to

experience more restricted, repetitive, and stereotyped behaviors than girls (Hartley &

Sikora, 2009). Young males with ASD were found to show an increased amount of

unusual motor movements and abnormal use of their body (Hartley & Sikora, 2009; Lord

et al., 1982). Sex differences were most prominent in young children (Hartley & Sikora,

2009). Using IQ as a control, a study of sex differences in ASD, conducted by Lord et al.

(1982), observed higher rates of restricted or repetitive behaviors, and inappropriate play

in males. A similar study conducted by McLennen, Lord, and Schopler (1993) yielded the

same results, but it was also observed that early adolescent males tended to display

greater deficits in reciprocal social interaction and communication.

Despite the research supporting the idea that there are sex differences in ASD,

there are few studies that focus on females with the disorder. The existing data suggests

Page 24: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

17

that there are subtle differences between the sexes that could play a significant role in the

differential way that ASD manifests itself in males and females (Hartley & Sikora, 2009).

Further research with males, however, has revealed results that support the broken

mirror theory of ASD. A study of the effect of MNS activation during a perceived task

revealed a dysfunction in individuals with ASD (Cattaneo et al., 2007). The study

examined the normal activation of the MNS during task observation and task

performance and compared it to activation in individuals with ASD. In healthy

individuals, the observation of the action led to mirrored activation within the MNS, but

this mirrored activation was absent in individuals with ASD. The absence of activation in

individuals with ASD suggests a deficit in linking or representing motor chains for

understanding intention.

Mindblindness is defined as a lack of a “theory of mind” when one understand the

perspective (thoughts and feelings) of another. Having mindblindness is essentially the

inability to empathize with others (Baron-Cohen, 2011). Mindblindness is frequently

labeled as a symptom of ASD. Some individuals with the disorder severely lack the

ability to recognize and interpret the actions of others (Baron-Cohen, 2011). Current

studies involving theory of mind and ASD are attempting to provide concrete proof that

mindblindness is linked to the function of the MNS. A severe dysfunction in the MNS

has been observed in individuals with complete mindblindness, as well as a slight

dysfunction of the MNS in individuals who have difficulty interpreting explicit actions

(Baron-Cohen, 2011). This evidence supports the idea that mirror neurons play a role in

how we interpret and attribute others' emotions and actions.

Page 25: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

18

The role of mirror neurons in ASD is still little understood. Researchers have only

managed to scrape the surface of both the MNS and ASD. However, there is a

considerable amount of evidence that supports the broken mirror theory of ASD.

Continued research will hopefully lead to a better comprehension of the disorder, as well

as potential for better methods of treatment and diagnosis.

Page 26: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

19

III. DISCUSSION

Discussion of Past Research

Research has confirmed the presence of the MNS in the brain and has explored

its role in various forms of empathy. Whether or not the MNS is the neurological basis

behind the process of empathy has yet to be confirmed. Location of the MNS has found

to be extensive throughout the brain and activation tends to vary according to the form of

empathy being observed.

Although the link between the MNS and empathy has yet to be supported,

differences between the sexes suggest its role. Females are considered to be the more

empathetic sex. Correlation studies reveal a link between high self-reported empathy and

stronger MNS activation. While research continues to clarify the role of the MNS and

empathy, it does suggest that females tend to be the more emotionally intelligent sex.

Research supports the idea that females are more efficient at interpreting and

comprehending actions of others than their male counterparts.

Activation of the MNS in a wide range of action and observation of performance,

such as facial expression, interpretation, and motor mimicry, strongly suggests that the

MNS serves a neurological basis for action understanding. The activation of the MNS to

variable degrees for a variety of actions indicates the location of the neurons may serve a

Page 27: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

20

specific purpose in identification. Location of mirror neurons in the brain may account

for the different types of empathy.

Studies conducted using individuals with ASD correlate the dysfunction of the

MNS in individuals with the disorder. A key symptom of ASD is the diminished ability

to comprehend or interpret another’s actions or intent. The correlation between the

dysfunction of the MNS and diminished empathetic ability suggests that the MNS plays a

significant role in empathy. Considering the significant difference in the prevalence of

ASD between the sexes, as well as increased activation of MNS in females compared

with males, sex differences in the MNS could potentially play a role in the development

of the disorder.

Implications and Applications

Identifying the sex differences of the MNS could possible provide insight into the

prevalence of ASD in males and its expression of symptoms between the sexes. ASD

tends to express itself differently among individuals, including between the sexes. The

current method of diagnosis relies on chief diagnostic signs, such as social isolation, lack

of eye contact, poor language capacity, and absence of empathy. Defining how these

symptoms express themselves between the sexes would lead to earlier, more efficient

forms of diagnosis.

Determining the role of the MNS in empathy could possibly provide new

information on how autism and its symptoms may arise. This knowledge could also

provide the information necessary to improve current treatment methods of the symptoms

of ASD. A current form of treatment, known as mirror neuron therapy, has had success in

improving the social functioning of an individual with ASD. This form of therapy

Page 28: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

21

attempts to stimulate the regions of the brain overlapping with the MNS to strengthen

neural connections (Wan, Demaine, Zipse, Norton, & Schlaug, 2010). A study conducted

using music as the method for activating MNS found success in this form of therapy.

Individuals undergoing mirror neuron therapy were shown to have increased

communication, even in non-verbal individuals with ASD, and improved motor function.

The broken mirror theory provides a possible explanation for symptoms of ASD.

Evidence of the MNS’s role in action-understanding and empathy as well as evidence of

a dysfunction in individuals with ASD provide the main support for this theory. This

theory provides much insight into the symptoms of ASD, as well as a potential

explanation for how they may arise in individuals. However, it is unlikely that MNS

dysfunction alone can provide the answer for why this disorder may occur. It is necessary

to continue to explore the validity of the broken mirror theory, as well as the role of the

MNS in empathy and behavior.

Future Research

Science is far from determining the cause of ASD, but there is a growing need for

more information. The estimates for children affected with autism have been increasing

each survey year. It is currently estimated that one child in 68 has been identified with

autism (Baio, 2014). ASD and its relationship to the function of the MNS should continue

to be studied. The limitations of existing studies of MNS function often are small sample

sizes. A comprehensive study should be conducted in order to better identify the sex

differences in MNS function, furthering the knowledge of sex differences in empathy.

There is a lack of recent information on sex differences relating with symptoms of

individuals with ASD. There are very few studies that explore the relationship between

Page 29: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

22

sex differences and ASD symptoms in depth. There is a significant difference in the ratio

of males and females diagnosed with the disorder, so few studies of autism represent both

sexes equally. Future studies should attempt to represent both sexes. Considering females

are diagnosed later than males, it is important to determine how symptoms of ASD

express themselves between the genders in order to improve current standards of

diagnosis.

Studies regarding the current methods of diagnosis are necessary in order to

improve their efficacy. The cost of behavioral treatment can also vary between $40,000

to $60,000 per child annually (Lavelle et al., 2014). These costs could potentially be

reduced if diagnosis and implementation of treatment occur earlier in children with the

disorder. The total social cost for care of children with ASD was an estimated 9 billion

dollars for 2011 (Lavelle et al., 2014). Considering this cost, it is necessary to increase

our understanding of autism in order to improve treatment and decrease costs of care.

Conclusion

The knowledge we have of the relationship between mirror neuron function,

empathy, and autism is far from complete. As research continues to explore the

relationship between mirror neuron function and empathy, the role of the MNS in

symptoms of autism will become more defined. Examination of previous research

suggests sex differences in MNS function, which could be potentially be related to

prevalence of autism in males. Future research regarding symptoms of autism in females

needs to be conducted before the existence of this relationship can be assumed.

Page 30: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

23

REFERENCES

Al-Abood, S. A., Davids, K., & Bennett, S. J. (2001). Specificity of task constraints and

effects of visual demonstrations and verbal instructions in directing learners'

search during skill acquisition. Journal of Motor Behavior, 33(3), 295-305.

doi:10.1080/00222890109601915.

Avenanti, A., Bueti, D., Galati, G., & Aglioti, S. M. (2005). Transcranial magnetic

stimulation highlights the sensorimotor side of empathy for pain. Nature

Neuroscience, 8(7), 955-960. doi:10.1038/nn1481.

Baio, J. (2014). “Prevalence of Autism Spectrum Disorder Among Children Aged 8

Years.” Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 63, 1-21.

Baio, J. (2012). “Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders--Autism and developmental

disabilities monitoring network.” Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 61, 1-

19.

Baird, A. D., Scheffer, I. E., & Wilson, S. J. (2011). Mirror neuron system involvement

in empathy: A critical look at the evidence. Social Neuroscience, 6(4), 327-335.

doi:10.1080/17470919.2010.547085.

Baron-Cohen, S. (2011). What is theory of mind, and is it impaired in ASC? In S. Bölte

& J. Hallmayer (Eds.), Autism spectrum conditions: FAQs on autism, Asperger

syndrome, and atypical autism answered by international experts (pp. 136-138).

Cambridge, MA: Hogrefe Publishing.

Bernier, R. R., Dawson, G. G., Webb, S. S., & Murias, M. M. (2007). EEG mu rhythm

and imitation impairments in individuals with autism spectrum disorder. Brain

and Cognition, 64(3), 228-237. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2007.03.004.

Page 31: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

24

Bien, N., Roebroeck, A., Goebel, R., & Sack, A. T. (2009). The brain’s intention to

imitate: The neurobiology of intentional versus automatic imitation. Cerebral

Cortex, 19, 2338–2351.

Braadbaart, L., Williams, J. G., & Waiter, G. D. (2013). Do mirror neuron areas mediate

mu rhythm suppression during imitation and action observation?. International

Journal of Psychophysiology, 89(1), 99-105. doi:10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.05.019.

Braadbaart, L. L., de Grauw, H. H., Perrett, D. I., Waiter, G. D., & Williams, J. G.

(2014). The shared neural basis of empathy and facial imitation accuracy.

Neuroimage, 84, 367-375. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.061.

Buchanan, J. J., & Dean, N. J. (2010). Specificity in practice benefits learning in novice

models and variability in demonstration benefits observational practice.

Psychological Research, 74(3), 313-326. doi:10.1007/s00426-009-0254-y.

Burgess, J. D., Arnold, S. L., Fitzgibbon, B. M., Fitzgerald, P. B., & Enticott, P. G.

(2013). A transcranial magnetic stimulation study of the effect of visual

orientation on the putative human mirror neuron system. Frontiers in Human

Neuroscience, 7, 679.

Carpenter, M. (2006). Instrumental, social, and shared goals and intentions in imitation.

In S. J. Rogers & J. G. Willams (Eds.), Imitation and the social mind: Autism and

typical development (pp. 48-70). New York: Guilford Press.

Carr, L., lacoboni, M., Dubeau, M., Mazziotta, J. C., & Lenzi, G. (2005). Neural

mechanisms of empathy in humans: A relay from neural systems for imitation to

limbic areas. In J. T. Cacioppo & G. G. Berntson (Eds.), Social neuroscience: Key

readings (pp. 143-152). New York: Psychology Press.

Page 32: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

25

Cattaneo, L., Fabbri-Destro, M., Boria., S, Pieraccini, C., Monti, A., Cossu, G., &

Rizzolatti, G. (2007). Impairment of actions chains in autism and its possible role

in intention understanding. Proceedings from the National Academy of Science,

104, 17825- 17830.

Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. (2007). Prevalence of autism spectrum

disorder—Autism and developmental disability monitoring network, 14 sites,

United States, 2002. In Surveillance Summary (pp. 12–27). 9, Feb, 2007.

Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 56.

Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception-behavior

link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76,

893-910.

Cheng, Y., Tzeng, O. L., Decety, J., lmada, T., & Hsieh, J. (2006). Gender differences in

the human mirror system: A magnetoencephalography study. Neuroreport: For

Rapid Communication of Neuroscience Research, 17(11), 1115-1119.

doi:10.1097/01.wnr.0000223393.59328.21.

Cheng, Y., Chou, K. H., Decety, J., Chen, I. Y., Hung, D., Tzeng, O. J., & Lin, C. P.

(2009). Sex differences in the neuroanatomy of human mirror-neuron system: a

voxel-based morphometric investigation. Neuroscience, 158(2), 713–720.

Clark, M. S., & Reis, H. T. (1988). Interpersonal processes in close relationships. Annual

Review Of Psychology, 39609-672. doi:10.1146/annurev.ps.39.020188.003141.

Cole, J. (2001). Empathy needs a face. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 8(5-7), 51-68.

Page 33: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

26

Corradini, A., & Antonietti, A. (2013). Mirror neurons and their function in cognitively

understood empathy. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal,

22(3), 1152-1161. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2013.03.003.

Cuevas, K., Cannon, E. N., Yoo, K., & Fox, N. A. (2014). The infant EEG mu rhythm:

Methodological considerations and best practices. Developmental Review, 34(1),

26-43. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2013.12.001.

di Pellegrino, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (1992).

Understanding motor events: A neurophysiological study. Experimental Brain

Research, 91, 176-180.

Edele, A., Dziobek, I., & Keller, M. (2013). Explaining altruistic sharing in the dictator

game: The role of affective empathy, cognitive empathy, and justice sensitivity.

Learning and Individual Differences, 24, 96-102.

doi:10.1016/j.lindif.2012.12.020.

Enticott, P. G., Kennedy, H. A., Rinehart, N. J., Tonge, B. J., Bradshaw, J. L., Taffe, J.

R., & ... Fitzgerald, P. B. (2012). Mirror neuron activity associated with social

impairments but not age in autism spectrum disorder. Biological Psychiatry,

71(5), 427-433. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.09.001.

Gallese, V., Rochat, M. J., & Berchio, C. (2013). The mirror mechanism and its potential

role in autism spectrum disorder. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology,

55(1), 15-22. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8749.2012.04398.x.

Groen, Y. Y., Wijers, A. A., Tucha, O. O., & Althaus, M. M. (2013). Are there sex

differences in ERPs related to processing empathy-evoking pictures?

Neuropsychologia, 51(1), 142-155. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.11.012.

Page 34: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

27

Hall, J. A. (1978). Gender effects in decoding nonverbal cues. Psychological Bulletin,

85(4), 845-857. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.85.4.845.

Halpern, D. F. (2000). Sex differences in cognitive abilities (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ:

Erlbaum.

Hamilton, A. C. (2013). Reflecting on the mirror neuron system in autism: A systematic

review of current theories. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 3(1), 91-105.

doi:10.1016/j.dcn.2012.09.008.

Hartley, S. L., & Sikora, D. M. (2009). Sex differences in autism spectrum disorder: An

examination of developmental functioning, autistic symptoms, and coexisting

behavior problems in toddlers. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders,

39(12), 1715-1722. doi:10.1007/s10803-009-0810-8.

Hunter, S., Hurley, R. A., & Taber, K. H. (2013). A look inside the mirror neuron system.

The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 25(3), 170-175.

Keysers, C., & Gazzola, V. (2010). Social neuroscience: Mirror neurons recorded in

humans. Current Biology, 20, R353-R354, doi:10.1016/j.cub.2010.03.013.

Lago-Rodriguez, A., Lopez-Alonso, V., & Fernández-del-Olmo, M. (2013). Mirror

neuron system and observational learning: Behavioral and neurophysiological

evidence. Behavioural Brain Research, 248, 104-113.

doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2013.03.033.

Lavelle, T.A., Weinstein, M.C., Newhouse, J.P., Munir, K., Kuhlthau, K.A., Prosser,

L.A. (2014). Economic burden of childhood autism spectrum disorders.

Pediatrics, 133, 520-529. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-0763.

Page 35: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

28

Loggia, M. L., Mogil, J. S., & Bushnell, M. (2008). Empathy hurts: Compassion for

another increases both sensory and affective components of pain perception. Pain,

136(1-2), 168-176. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2007.07.017.

Lord, C., Schopler, E., & Revicki, D. (1982). Sex differences in autism. Journal of

Autism and Developmental Disorders, 12(4), 317-330. doi:10.1007/BF01538320.

McGarry, L. M., & Russo, F. A. (2011). Mirroring in dance/movement therapy: Potential

mechanisms behind empathy enhancement. The Arts in Psychotherapy, 38, 178-

184.

McLennan, J. D., Lord, C., & Schopler, E. (1993). Sex differences in higher functioning

people with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disabilities, 23, 217-

227.

Mercadillo, R. E., Diaz, J. L., Pasaye, E. H., & Barrios, F. A. (2001). Perception of

suffering and compassion experience: Brain gender disparities. Brain and

Cognition, 76(1), 5-14.

Montag, C., Gallinat, J., & Heinz, A. (2008). Theodor Lipps and the concept of empathy:

1851-1914. American Journal of Psychiatry, 165, 1261.

doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07081283.

Niedenthal, P. M., Barsalou, L. W., Winkielman, P., Krauth-Gruber, S., & Ric, F. (2005).

Embodiment in attitudes, social perception, and emotion. Personality and Social

Psychology Review, 9(3), 184-211. doi:10.1207/s15327957pspr0903_1.

Oberman, L. M., McCleery, J. P., Hubbard, E. M., Bernier, R., Wiersema, J. R.,

Raymaekers, R., & Pineda, J. A. (2013). Developmental changes in mu

Page 36: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

29

suppression to observed and executed actions in autism spectrum disorders. Social

Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(3), 300-304. doi:10.1093/scan/nsr097.

Oberman, L. M., Hubbard, E. M., McCleery, J. P., Altschuler, E. L., Ramachandran, V.,

& Pineda, J. A. (2005). EEG evidence for mirror neuron dysfunction in autism

spectrum disorders. Cognitive Brain Research, 24(2), 190-198.

doi:10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.01.014.

Pfeiffer, J. H., Iacoboni, M., Mazziotta, J. C., & Dapretto, M. (2008). Mirroring other’s

emotions relates to empathy and interpersonal competence in children.

NeuroImage, 39, 2076-2085.

Proverbio, A. M., Riva, F., & Zani, A. (2010). When neurons do not mirror the agent's

intentions: Sex differences in neural coding of goal-directed actions.

Neuropsychologia, 48, 1454-1463. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.01.015.

Ramachandran, V. S., & Oberman, L. M. (2006). Broken mirrors: A theory of autism.

Scientific American, 295, 62–69.

Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Gallese, V., & Fogassi, L. (1996). Premotor cortex and the

recognition of motor actions. Cognitive Brain Research, 3, 131-141.

Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., Aharon-Peretz, J., & Perry, D. (2008). Two systems for empathy:

A double dissociation between emotional and cognitive empathy in inferior

frontal gyrus versus ventromedial prefrontal lesions. Brain, 132, 617-627.

Schulte-Rüther, M., Markowitsch, H., Fink, G. R., & Piefke, M. (2007). Mirror neuron

and theory of mind mechanisms involved in face-to-face interactions: A

functional magnetic resonance imaging approach to empathy. Journal of

Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(8), 1354-1372. doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.8.1354.

Page 37: MIRROR NEURON FUNCTION: AN EXAMINATION OF …

30

Schulte-Rü̈ther, M., Markowitsch, H. J.,Shah, N. J., Fink, G. R., & Piefke, M. (2008).

Gender differences in brain networks supporting empathy. NeuroImage, 42(1),

393-403, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.04.180.

Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J., Kaube, H., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2004).

Empathy for pain involves the affective but not sensory components of pain.

Science, 303, 1157-1162.

Wakabayashi, A., Sasaki, J., & Ogawa, Y. (2012). Sex differences in two fundamental

cognitive domains: Empathizing and systemizing in children and adults. Journal

of Individual Differences, 33(1), 24-34. doi:10.1027/1614-0001/a000058.

Wan, C. Y., Demaine, K., Zipse, L., Norton, A., & Schlaug, G. (2010). From music

making to speaking: Engaging the mirror neuron system in autism. Brain

Research Bulletin, 82(3-4), 161-168. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2010.04.010.

Wheeler, M. (2010, April 12). UCLA researchers make first direct recording of mirror

neurons in human brain. UCLA Newsroom. Retrieved from

http://newsroom.ucla.edu/portal/ucla/ucla-researchers-make-first-direct-

156503.aspx.

Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J. P., Gallese, V., & Rizzolatti, G. (2003).

Both of us disgusted in my insula: The common neural basis of seeing and feeling

disgust. Neuron, 40, 655-664.

Winerman, L. (2005, October). The mind's mirror.” Monitor in Psychology, 36, 48.

Yang, C., Decety, J., Lee, S., Chen, C., & Cheng, Y. (2009). Gender differences in the

mu rhythm during empathy for pain: An electroencephalographic study. Brain

Research, 1251, 176-184. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2008.11.062.