maria adamuti-trache (university of british columbia – canada)

13
Draft copy only: Do not quote without permission ([email protected]) Language acquisition among adult immigrants in Canada Maria Adamuti-Trache University of British Columbia, Vancouver, CANADA ABSTRACT Gaining proficiency in one of Canada’s official languages is perhaps the most important task of immigrants during their first years in the new country. In this paper, I put forward the idea that ability to communicate in one of Canada’s official languages is not only a condition for full social and economic integration of immigrants, but it also facilitates immigrant understanding of culture and customs, and finally contributes to shaping a sense of belonging to the host country. First, this study demonstrates the variability in pre-migration language capital among recent adult immigrants to Canada and the increase in language capital within four years of arrival. Second, it examines various opportunities for language acquisition and it identifies vulnerable groups such as women, older immigrants and less educated immigrants who have limited access to such opportunities. Third, the analysis points to the relationship between language capital and immigrants’ socio-economic integration. The study emphasizes the centrality of practice (i.e., formal and informal education and training, labour market participation, social networking) to language acquisition and immigrant integration within the host society. BACKGROUND Language proficiency is acknowledged as one of the most important factors contributing to the successful economic integration of migrant populations; more generally, it is a critical ingredient underlying social integration and civic participation. In Canada, only the Federal Skilled Worker applications are evaluated through an immigration point system that includes some procedures to determine language proficiency in English or French (i.e., up to 24 of the total of 100 points are awarded for language proficiency in one or both of English and French). However, the Canadian immigration policy did not make the official language competence compulsory and a skilled immigrant could qualify without meeting this criterion. The efficacy of immigrant selection and settlement policies has been highly contested. Since April 2010, amendments to the Canadian immigration policy require that language proficiency is no more self-reported by applicants, but proofs of English and French language ability will be provided at the time of application. The change applies to the Federal Skilled Worker and Canadian Experience immigration classes, which actually constitute the main source of skilled immigrants to Canada. The current study is based on the Longitudinal Survey of Immigrants to Canada that covers the period 2000-2005 when proof of language skills was not required. Brief literature review. Language skills are a form of human capital: “they are embodied in the person; they are productive in the labor market and/or in consumption; and they are created at a sacrifice of time and out-of-pocket resources” (Chiswick & Miller, 1995, p.248). In an era of increasing global migration, foreign language competencies are essential skills that immigrant workers and their families need in order to build successful lives in the destination countries. 1

Upload: certifica-il-tuo-italiano

Post on 17-Jan-2015

1.167 views

Category:

Documents


4 download

DESCRIPTION

Maria Adamuti-Trache (University of British Columbia – CANADA)

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Maria Adamuti-Trache (University of British Columbia – CANADA)

Draft copy only: Do not quote without permission ([email protected])

Language acquisition among adult immigrants in Canada

Maria Adamuti-Trache

University of British Columbia, Vancouver, CANADA ABSTRACT Gaining proficiency in one of Canada’s official languages is perhaps the most important task of immigrants during their first years in the new country. In this paper, I put forward the idea that ability to communicate in one of Canada’s official languages is not only a condition for full social and economic integration of immigrants, but it also facilitates immigrant understanding of culture and customs, and finally contributes to shaping a sense of belonging to the host country. First, this study demonstrates the variability in pre-migration language capital among recent adult immigrants to Canada and the increase in language capital within four years of arrival. Second, it examines various opportunities for language acquisition and it identifies vulnerable groups such as women, older immigrants and less educated immigrants who have limited access to such opportunities. Third, the analysis points to the relationship between language capital and immigrants’ socio-economic integration. The study emphasizes the centrality of practice (i.e., formal and informal education and training, labour market participation, social networking) to language acquisition and immigrant integration within the host society.

BACKGROUND Language proficiency is acknowledged as one of the most important factors contributing to the successful economic integration of migrant populations; more generally, it is a critical ingredient underlying social integration and civic participation. In Canada, only the Federal Skilled Worker applications are evaluated through an immigration point system that includes some procedures to determine language proficiency in English or French (i.e., up to 24 of the total of 100 points are awarded for language proficiency in one or both of English and French). However, the Canadian immigration policy did not make the official language competence compulsory and a skilled immigrant could qualify without meeting this criterion. The efficacy of immigrant selection and settlement policies has been highly contested. Since April 2010, amendments to the Canadian immigration policy require that language proficiency is no more self-reported by applicants, but proofs of English and French language ability will be provided at the time of application. The change applies to the Federal Skilled Worker and Canadian Experience immigration classes, which actually constitute the main source of skilled immigrants to Canada. The current study is based on the Longitudinal Survey of Immigrants to Canada that covers the period 2000-2005 when proof of language skills was not required.

Brief literature review. Language skills are a form of human capital: “they are embodied in the person; they are productive in the labor market and/or in consumption; and they are created at a sacrifice of time and out-of-pocket resources” (Chiswick & Miller, 1995, p.248). In an era of increasing global migration, foreign language competencies are essential skills that immigrant workers and their families need in order to build successful lives in the destination countries.

1

Page 2: Maria Adamuti-Trache (University of British Columbia – CANADA)

Fluency in the host country language is a function of three fundamental variables: exposure to the language (e.g., “linguistic distance”, school instruction in the destination language, duration in the host country, intensity of exposure), efficiency in language acquisition (i.e., depends on factors like age at immigration, linguistic distance and level of education) and expected returns to language investment economic incentives from language fluency (e.g., higher wages). Standard human capital models would suggest that acquisition of host country language capital requires time and investment (Dustman, 1999); and one’s decision to invest depends on potential future benefits, on the cost of skill acquisition, and on the individual’s efficiency acquiring language capital which in turn depends on the stock of human capital gained in the past (e.g., level of education, pre-migration language skills).

Neither the immigrant nor the host society can control all the above factors (e.g., linguistic distance). However, both parts should strive to create ways to improve language fluency through practice in the destination language, which would ‘naturally’ occur through participation in the labour market, bridging to the host society, pursuing education and training. From Bourdieu’s sociological perspective (1990), the accumulation and conversion of forms of capital is only possible through practice in a social field – in this case, both acquisition or enhancement of host country language capital and its subsequent conversion into human capital are conditional upon immigrant active engagement with host country institutions.

Recent immigrants to Canada have to overcome language, cultural and social barriers when entering new workplaces and communities. For instance, data from the Longitudinal Survey of Immigrants to Canada (LSIC) administered in 2000-2001 show that a majority (82%) of new immigrants reported they were able to speak one of the two official languages (Statistics Canada, 2005). Language skills were more likely to be reported among the working-age immigrants (25-44 years old) with university education (92%). With respect to language skills acquisition, almost all immigrants (95%) thought it was important or very important to improve their language skills. Within 6 months of arrival 67% of immigrants had plans to improve their English and many enrolled in training. Among the most common methods of learning English (all provinces except Quebec) were language classes or training (60%), media (39%) and self-learning (30%), as well as through interactions with family and friends, or at school and work. In addition, there is a clear trend toward pursuing Canadian post-secondary credentials which offers an indirect way to improve host country language skills. LSIC data show that 89% of recent immigrants acknowledged the worth of taking further education; about two-thirds of them had educational plans at arrival, and 40% of immigrants were interested in university studies (Statistics Canada, 2005). Obtaining further education after arrival in Canada is a strategy embraced in particular by immigrants who have completed university education in their countries of origin (Adamuti-Trache, 2011; Adamuti-Trache & Sweet, 2010; Green & Green, 1999). Anisef, Sweet and Adamuti-Trache (2009) also found that high self-perception of language proficiency in one of the two official languages is associated with immigrants’ engagement in post-secondary education after arrival to Canada and impacts significantly on the employment outcomes of recent immigrants.

There is evidence that inadequacy of language skills leads to the unsuccessful economic integration and also controls the type of social interaction and civic participation of immigrants. As Lancee (2008) demonstrates, there is a significant relationship between language proficiency and structural bridging social capital which consists in the collection of ties established among people (i.e., host society) that are embedded in institutions where more likely resources can be

2

Page 3: Maria Adamuti-Trache (University of British Columbia – CANADA)

exchanged. As a result, bridging networks are likely to affect employment and income. Bonding social capital that emphasizes ties within family and ethnic community may not lead to direct economic outcomes but helps the growth of bridging capital. Boyd (2009) employs data from the 2002 Canadian Ethnic Diversity Survey to examine whether lack of language proficiency affects membership in a variety of organizations as well as immigrants’ feeling of belonging. While participation in non-ethnic associations is indeed lower for immigrants who are less proficient in the host language (i.e., both mother tongue and language used mostly at home other than English/French), these immigrants are active in ethnic or immigrant associations, and do not have negative feeling about belonging to Canada.

Purpose. The study extends the existing research literature in several ways. First, I will examine the role of self-reported language ability in the immigrant settlement process – touching on social, economic and emotional domains of individual adaptation. Second, the strategies employed by immigrants to improve their language competence in English or French will provide information to better understand funded language training programs and how these are augmented by the initiatives and personal situations of individual immigrants who pursue language in various settings. The study has the following key research objectives:

• To examine differences in language ability (e.g., speaking skills) at arrival by various individual and immigrant-specific factors (e.g., gender, age, pre-migration level of education, immigration class, country of origin).

• To examine immigrants’ engagement in various forms of formal and informal language training or other forms of practice (e.g., formal post-secondary education) that contribute to the development of language capital within 4 years of arrival.

• To assess changes in language capital (i.e., self-reported language proficiency) over four years of arrival while controlling for individual factors.

• To examine the effect of language proficiency on immigrants’ participation in society (e.g., employment, social network) and sense of belonging after 4 years in Canada.

Data. The analysis is based on data from the Longitudinal Survey of Immigrants to Canada (LSIC) following a representative sample of new immigrants and refugees who landed between October 2000 and September 2001. The surveys were conducted 6 months, 2 years and 4 years after their arrival. The study includes only respondents who had never lived in Canada before applying for immigration and were 20 to 59 years old at arrival. The age selection ensures that youth 19 years old or less who are still eligible to enroll in high school as regular students were excluded; I also excluded the older immigrants who were roughly out of the working age population by 2005, the time of the last LSIC interview, and possibly had fewer incentives for language training. A total of 6090 immigrants were selected which represents about 87% of respondents who participated in all 3 interviews. The study follows Statistics Canada recommendations for the analysis and presentation of LSIC data. Analysis is conducted with normalized weights obtained from LSIC survey weights.

Variables. The outcome variable is the self-reported speaking proficiency in one of Canada’s official languages – it is based on the English scores for all provinces except Quebec, and the best of the English or French scores for Quebec respondents (Adamuti-Trache, 2011). For each wave, a four-category variable indicates whether the immigrant reported: could not speak (0), spoke the language poorly (1), spoke fairly well (2), spoke well (3), spoke very well or

3

Page 4: Maria Adamuti-Trache (University of British Columbia – CANADA)

English/French was his/her mother tongue (4). Although immigrants whose mother tongue was one of Canada/s official languages did not have to answer the question, they were assigned to the highest level (Grondin, 2007). Language speaking proficiency in wave 1 (i.e., low/high by aggregating first two and last two groups) is also used as an explanatory variable of different language acquisition patterns. The analysis includes individual variables such as gender, age (i.e., 6 groups) and highest educational attained (i.e., high school or below, college/trades, university), immigrant-specific factors (Adamuti-Trache & Sweet, 2010) such as region of last permanent residence and immigration class (i.e., economic-principal applicants, economic-spouse/dependants, family immigrants, refugees). The ‘source country’ variable carries a cultural dimension (Reitz, 2007); it also informs on the ‘linguistic distance’ between the country of origin and the host country. The immigration class contains information on immigrant human capital because most economic immigrants have been assessed under the point system.

An important set of variables describes the steps taken by immigrants to improve their language skills. The information is cumulated over all waves and variables describe whether immigrants took language classes, learned from family/friends or at work, improved through self-study or media. While language training represents non-formal learning, all other forms of language acquisition are typical informal learning ways in which learners engage purposefully. It is also expected that language skills improve through practice in educational institutions even if the learner engages in such activities to pursue a post-secondary credential. The analysis will be based on variables to describe involvement with any of these activities within four years of arrival. Finally, improved language skills are expected to lead to immigrants’ better economic integration (i.e., employment status), the formation of host country social capital (i.e., diverse friends network outside ethnic community) and a growing feeling of belonging to the destination country. These outcomes will be discussed based on the Wave 3 data (4 years of arrival).

Outline. This is an empirical exploratory analysis that consists in four parts. First, descriptive statistics will contrast language ability profiles in Wave 1 by social structural factors and prior level of education, as well as immigrant specific factors. Second, I will provide data to compare immigrant engagement with various modes of language acquisition during the first four years in Canada by social structural factors, prior level of education and language skills at arrival. Third, I will make use of the longitudinal design of LSIC and conduct a multivariate analysis of language proficiency growth by examining the effect of social structural factors (gender and age), prior level of education and language skills at arrival (Wave 1). Finally, I will briefly examine how language ability in Wave 3 relates to employment status, social capital and feeling of belonging.

FINDINGS 1. Language capital. Wave 1 data allow to assess immigrants’ language capital close to arrival. Percentage of immigrants across four levels of proficiency in one of Canada’s official languages are shown in detail (i.e., cannot speak and speaks poor are aggregated to avoid small sample size for some groups); the contrast will be mostly made between poor speakers (i.e., low language skills) and good speakers (i.e., high language skills). Table 1 shows that about two thirds of adult immigrants speak well or very well one of Canada official languages. Gender differences in language capital indicate that men self-report higher proficiency: 71% of men as compared to 55% of women are good speakers. Almost one quarter of women cannot speak or have a poor command of the language. Age at arrival is associated with language capital. Except the age

4

Page 5: Maria Adamuti-Trache (University of British Columbia – CANADA)

group 20 to 24 that is about half distributed across the low and high skill levels, there is a quite clear increase in the proportion of immigrants with low language skills by age. About two thirds of the immigrants 50 years old and above reported having poor mastery of one of Canada official languages. As expected language proficiency is increased with level of education. While only one-third of the immigrants with high school education or less reported good language skills, this proportion is 59% and 73% among those with college and university experience, respectively.

Table 1. Distribution of immigrants by speaking language abilities in Wave 1 (row %)

Low skill level High skill level

N Cannot speak & Poor a

Fairly well Well Very well & Mother tongue

ALL 6090 17 19 26 38 Gender Male Female

3000 3090

11 23

18 21

29 22

42 33

Age 20-24 25-29 30-34 35-39 40-44 45-49 50-59

570 1170 1610 1130 750 460 410

20 12 11 15 20 22 50

26 19 20 21 18 17 14

24 28 30 28 22 19 11

30 41 40 37 40 41 25

Pre-migration education High school or below College University

1150 1010 3960

45 20 9

23 22 18

14 20 30

18 39 43

Region of last permanent residence US/UK/NZ/AUS WSN Europe E Europe Central/South America Middle East E/SE Asia South Asia Africa

320 390 460 340 600 2000 1450 520

4 15 10 14 16 20 24 6

7

14 26 16 21 25 17 10

14 23 39 17 23 34 21 14

76 48 25 53 41 21 38 70

Immigration class Economic- Principal applicant Economic- Spouse/Dependant Family Refugee

2450 1770 1460 410

4 20 31 36

14 24 20 30

32 23 19 20

50 33 30 14

a The 2 categories are aggregated because some groups have small sample size.

Region/country of last permanent residence is associated with immigrant language capital at arrival. Obviously, those coming from Anglophone countries or from African countries that have either British or French heritages, have high proficiency in one of Canada’s official languages (i.e., up to 90% and 84%, respectively). Good language skills are also noticeable from immigrants from Western/South/Northern Europe and Central/South America who may have had access to training or more contacts with the North American culture. Eastern Europeans show the

5

Page 6: Maria Adamuti-Trache (University of British Columbia – CANADA)

most centered distribution: many immigrants had exposure to one of Canada’s official languages, but a relatively low proportion report very good language skills. While language skills are quite polarized among immigrants from South Asia (e.g., 38% have very food skills, but also 24% cannot speak or speak the language poorly), immigrants for East and South East Asia show the highest percentage of low skill level (a total of 45%) and the lowest percentage of immigrants who speak the language very well (21%). Finally, there is a clear relation between language skills at arrival and immigration class. Over 80% of the economic immigrants principal applicants report high language proficiency as compared to about half of the spouse and dependant economic immigrants or family immigrants, and about two-third of the refugees. This is not surprising since selection criteria for skilled workers include a language proficiency component. Most of these individuals will likely carry the burden of social and economic integration for their families. However, other immigrant groups are vulnerable in terms of pre-migration language capital. Women, older immigrants, those with lower levels of education, immigrants from Asia, family immigrants and refugees will likely need significant support to acquire language skills.

2. Steps taken to acquire language proficiency. Typical ways to improve language skills are language classes, self-study, media, family/friend interactions, work interactions, as well as participation in formal education (e.g., post-secondary education). Although questions about language practices were asked in each wave, this analysis is based on overall responses across all waves. For instance, participation in language training classes means that the immigrant reported taking language classes at least once within 4 years of arrival. Table 2 contrasts the percentages of immigrants who engaged in these language practices by demographic factors and prior level of education, as well as language proficiency at arrival. In general, immigrants with lower skill level at arrival are more likely to engage in various forms of language acquisition, except for pursuing post-secondary education which is controlled by admission requirements, so mostly good speakers can access this opportunity. Media seems to be a popular way to acquire language skills among a majority of immigrants.

Table 2: Percentages of immigrants who took specific steps toward improving language skills by individual factors and Wave 1 language skill level a

Language classes Self-Study Media W1 Language skill level Low b High b Low High Low High ALL 70 36 45 37 77 60 Gender Male Female

64 74

33 41

46 44

38 37

77 78

60 60

Age 20-24 25-29 30-34 35-39 40-44 45-49 50-59

70 74 76 79 77 58 41

46 40 39 36 32 25 16

39 48 51 56 53 29 23

35 39 42 38 33 29 22

81 82 82 83 82 68 51

58 62 64 62 52 53 48

Pre-migration education High school or below College University

59 75 76

33 29 38

29 55 54

28 28 40

66 83 83

52 52 63

6

Page 7: Maria Adamuti-Trache (University of British Columbia – CANADA)

(Table 2, con’t)

Family/Friends At Work PSE W1 Language skill level Low High Low Low High Low ALL 47 36 46 47 36 46 Gender Male Female

42 50

33 39

60 38

42 50

33 39

60 38

Age 20-24 25-29 30-34 35-39 40-44 45-49 50-59

55 52 47 45 43 44 42

40 35 36 40 31 33 31

55 55 46 49 49 38 26

55 52 47 45 43 44 42

40 35 36 40 31 33 31

55 55 46 49 49 38 26

Pre-migration education High school or below College University

50 53 42

38 35 36

39 51 51

50 53 42

38 35 36

39 51 51

a Percentages are calculated as the ratio between the number of immigrants answering Yes and the total number of immigrants in each group. b Low= Cannot speak & Poor & Fairly well; High= Well & Very well & Mother tongue. Language classes. Formal language training is considered by 70% of the poor speakers as compared to 36% of the good speakers. Gender and age differences are significant: women and young immigrants are more likely to take language classes. Participation in language training by prior level of education reveals that exactly the groups that need training the most (i.e., those with high school or below) are less likely to consider this step. Apparently, older immigrants and those with lower level of education who were identified as having insufficient language capital at arrival are not sufficiently engaged with language training within four years of arrival.

Self-study. This is an important form of informal language training that seems to be considered by 45% of the poor speakers and 37% of the good speakers. Gender differences are not noticeable, but interesting patterns are visible by age and education level. For instance, among the poor speakers, active self-learners are the working age immigrants (25 to 44) and those with prior post-secondary education. To note that self-study is identified as a step to language improvement only by 29% and 22-23% of the immigrants aged 45-49 and 50-59, respectively.

Media. This is the most popular informal learning tool recognized by 77% of the poor speakers and 60% of the good speakers. However, it is acknowledged only by half of the immigrants aged 50 to 59. The younger and the more educated immigrants, regardless of language skill level at arrival, are likely to benefit from this informal leaning tool.

Family/friends. Overall, poor speakers seem to rely more on family/friends interaction to learn the language as compared to good speakers (47% vs. 36%). This is particularly true for women regardless of language skill level at arrival. It is also true for immigrants with lower levels of education, especially poor speakers. The younger immigrants also feel they learn from the interactions with family and friends more than the older immigrants, who might have a friends network that is less ethnically diverse.

7

Page 8: Maria Adamuti-Trache (University of British Columbia – CANADA)

Work. Acquiring language skills at work certainly depends on whether the immigrant is employed and whether English or French are the languages used in the workplace. Overall, about 45-46% of immigrants believe that the work environment helped them improve their proficiency and there are no differences by language skill level at arrival. However, gender, age and level of education differences are significant, especially among poor speaker immigrants. While 60% of men who are poor speakers believe they improved their language skills at work, this is reported by only 38% of women in the same language skill group. Similarly, 55% of the 20 to 29 years old immigrants as compared to 26% of the 50 to 59 years old immigrants, and 39% of those with high school education or below as compared to 51% of those with post-secondary education, recognize that language improvements occurred at work. These patterns are clearly controlled by employment patterns as well as type of jobs held by immigrants (e.g., manual jobs may not require good language skills and offer few opportunities for improvement).

PSE participation. Although formal education aiming to degree completion is not a direct step to language improvement, it may engage the learner in language practice more than any other activity. There is no surprise that only 24% of the poor speakers as compared to 45% of the good speakers engage in PSE because participation is not only a matter of choice but also of meeting admission requirements. It is still remarkable that one quarter of the adult immigrants who do not have remarkable language skills at arrival succeed to pursue PSE within four years. Women and older immigrants, especially those with low language skills, are less likely to engage in PSE. However, PSE participation rates are quite high among university-educated immigrants, regardless of language proficiency.

3. Change in language proficiency. Time since arrival in the host country (i.e., duration of exposure) and steps taken to improve language skills or participation in education, work and social networking (i.e., intensity of exposure) allow immigrants to become gradually more comfortable with the host country language. Improvement of spoken language skills is visible in Figure 1 showing the average scores at each interview time for immigrants with low and high skill level at arrival.

Fig.1: Mean language scores by W1 skill level

0

1

2

3

4

Wave1 Wave2 Wave3

High

Low

Poor speakers become over time more confident regarding their language abilities while good speakers become more exigent. For the poor speakers, the major change occurs within 2 years of arrival when they reach on average level 2 (speaking fairly well). No significant changes are observed for the good speakers. Figure 1 shows that change in language proficiency occurs during Wave 2, and next skills remain quite steady.

8

Page 9: Maria Adamuti-Trache (University of British Columbia – CANADA)

A one-way repeated measures ANOVA was conducted to compare the effect of gender, age at arrival, prior level of education and language skill group at arrival on self-reported language ability in Wave 1, Wave 2 and Wave 3. All tests of between-subjects effects (i.e., gender, age, prior level of education, language proficiency at arrival) are statistically significant (p<0.001). The tests of within-subject effects show statistically significant changes in language proficiency over time (p<0.001). Significant interaction terms indicate that the changing patterns in language proficiency are affected by gender, age, prior level of education and language skills at arrival (the latter effect is portrayed in Figure 1). The interaction effects between language skills at arrival and one of the individual variables are shown in Figures 2-4.

Fig.2: Mean language scores by W1 skill level and gender

0

1

2

3

4

Wave1 Wave2 Wave3

High_Male

High_Female

Low_Male

Low_Female

Fig.3: Mean language scores by W1 skill level and age

0

1

2

3

4

Wave1 Wave2 Wave3

High 20-2425-2930-3435-3940-4445-4950-59Low 20-2425-2930-3435-3940-4445-4950-59

Fig.4: Mean language scores by W1 skill level and prior level of education

0

1

2

3

4

Wave1 Wave2 Wave3

High_UniversityHigh_CollegeHigh_HS/BelowLow_UniversityLow_CollegeLow_HS/Below

9

Page 10: Maria Adamuti-Trache (University of British Columbia – CANADA)

More specifically, Figure 2 confirms no significant gender although male immigrants improve slightly more their skills (i.e., the slope of change between Wave 1 and Wave 2 is greater for men). Figure 3 describes the effect of age on language proficiency growth. For the high skill level, the most pronounced drop is noticeable for the 45 to 49 years old. For the low skill group, the slope of change is greater for all age groups except immigrants aged 45 to 49 and 50 to 59. The older group also experiences a slight drop in perception of language speaking skills in Wave 3. Finally, Figure 4 shows the effect of prior level of education on language proficiency growth. While there is little variability in the change patterns for the good speakers, there is a clear effect of prior level of education on the slope lines with university-educated immigrants experiencing the greatest language proficiency growth in Wave 2.

4. Language and immigrant integration. Acquisition of language capital is expected to affect the economic, social and cultural integration of immigrants. Data show that the percentage of adult immigrants who report speaking well and very well one of Canada official languages grows from 64% in Wave 1 to 75% in Wave 3; the percentage not speaking the host country language dropped from 17% top 10%. Table 4 presents evidence that language proficiency in Wave 3 is associated with indicators of immigrants’ integration and their sense of belonging to Canada.

Table 3. Integration outcomes and speaking language abilities in Wave 3 (row %)

N Cannot speak & Poor

Fairly well Well Very well & Mother tongue

ALL 6090 10 15 28 47 Employment status (W3) Employed Unemployed Searching jobs Unemployed-No job search

4380 770 950

7 15 19

14 15 19

28 31 24

51 38 39

Pre-migration work experience accepted Had not worked before Never tried or looked for job Have tried, did not succeed Work experience accepted

980 1310 1850 1950

24 15 6 3

21 20 16 9

26 31 29 27

29 34 50 61

Friends network (ethnicity) W3 No new friends All/Most ethnic friends About half ethnic friends Few/None ethnic friends

800 2900 810 1580

19 13 2 2

15 20 12 8

24 32 29 24

42 35 56 67

Membership in organizations (W3) No Yes, ethnic/religious/cultural Yes, not ethnic/religious/cultural

4290 570 1240

12 7 3

16 14 8

29 29 26

44 50 63

Sense of belonging to Canada (W3) 1 Not strong at all 2 3 4 5 Very strong

90

210 1040 2280 2410

11 10 12 9 10

22 14 16 18 12

22 33 30 29 26

44 43 42 44 52

10

Page 11: Maria Adamuti-Trache (University of British Columbia – CANADA)

First, about 72% of the adult immigrants were employed in Wave 3. Among them, 79% reported speaking an official language well and very well at the time of the interview. The least proficient are immigrants who are unemployed and not looking for a job, although 30% of the unemployed searching for jobs have low language skill level and likely few employment chances. One issue much discussed in the literature is related to barriers experienced by immigrants to have their prior work experience recognized (see e.g., Anisef, Sweet & Adamuti-Trache, 2010). The current analysis shows that about 37% of adult immigrants never worked prior to migration or never had a chance to either work in Canada or to negotiate their work experience within 4 years of arrival. Among these immigrants, there are noticeably high percentages with low language skills. On the contrary, those who interacted with the employers and negotiated their work experience, successfully or not, are much more proficient. In particular, 88% of the adult immigrants whose work experience was accepted in Canada have good mastery of one official language.

Building social capital in the host country is facilitated by ability to communicate outside own ethnic group. As shown in table 4, there is a tendency among adult immigrants to maintain friendships within their ethnic group (i.e., over 60% of immigrants did not make friends or have all or most ethnic friends). One third of these immigrants who rely on bonding friendship networks are poor speakers after 4 years in Canada. On the contrary, about one quarter of all immigrants have few or no ethnic friends. Only 10% of these immigrants who rely on bridging friendship networks are poor speakers. The data clearly show that type of friends network is associated with language proficiency. Membership in organizations is relatively low for adult immigrants: 20% participate in non-ethnic organizations, 9% in ethnic groups and over 70% have no organization affiliation in Wave 3. The low participation could be associated with the economic difficulties experienced by adult immigrants that delay their involvement with activities outside the economic realm. However, there is a clear relation between membership in organization, particularly non-ethnic, and language proficiency which suggests that language skills may affect immigrant social integration.

Finally, sense of belonging to Canada is likely to be enhanced when immigrants are capable to adopt and share cultural values, and this attitude is favoured by an active knowledge of language. Table 4 shows that in general, adult immigrants have a high sense of belonging to Canada (mean score is about 4.1 on a 1-5 scale). Data show that sense of belonging to Canada and language skills are related but not in a linear manner. For instance, one can notice the lowest percentage of immigrants with high language skills (67%) among those whose feeling of belonging is not strong at all. The proportion of good speakers is increased among those who feel more attached, reaching 78% of those who have very strong sense of belonging. However, 76% of those who score 2 out of 5 (quite low attachment) are good speakers too.

DISCUSSION This paper is grounded in Chiswick and Miller’s (1995) argument that acquisition of language capital is affected by various pre-migration and post-migration factors. The LSIC data show that adult immigrants to Canada have a quite significant stock of host country language capital, although there are noticeable differences by gender, age, education, country of origin and immigration class. Significant associations between these factors and language proficiency at arrival show that adult immigrants do not form a homogeneous group. In short, by acquiring proficiency in one of Canada’s official languages, newcomers gradually understand the norms of

11

Page 12: Maria Adamuti-Trache (University of British Columbia – CANADA)

the host society, become culturally, socially and economically integrated as citizens, and likely grow a sense of belonging to Canada. The broad purpose of this paper is to examine the process of language capital acquisition, steps taken by immigrants to improve their language skills, and whether or not language skills impact on the socio-economic integration and sense of belonging of adult immigrants within four years of arrival to Canada.

The second perspective of this paper is that language capital, like any other form of human capital, can be acquired only through social practice. The study brings evidence that in terms of formal and informal language training, or participation in formal post-secondary education that should also have some language acquisition effects, adult immigrants are not equal. The most disadvantaged social groups in terms of initial language capital like older or less educated immigrants are also less likely to access formal education and training opportunities, or unable to make use of other informal learning tools (e.g., self-study, media). As a result, many cannot access labour market opportunities which in turn limit their chances to practice the language and gain new skills.

There is clear evidence that adult immigrants improve their language skills over time. The gain is obvious for immigrants with initial low stock of language capital; and a slight drop in self-reported skills is evident for those with high language proficiency at arrival. In addition, acquisition of language skills is visible during the first two years in Canada, reaching a plateau over the next two years for most immigrant groups. This should be of concern, considering that the skill level attained by the least language proficient immigrants it still low (only speaking ‘fairly well’).

The ability to communicate allows for full social and economic integration of adult immigrants; through understanding of culture and customs, a gradual integration contributes to shaping a sense of belonging to the host country. The paper emphasizes the centrality of practice (i.e., formal and informal education and training, labour market participation, networking) to language acquisition and immigrant integration in the host society. It also points to vulnerable immigrant groups who have limited access to opportunities such as women, older immigrants and less educated immigrants.

12

Page 13: Maria Adamuti-Trache (University of British Columbia – CANADA)

REFERENCES Adamuti-Trache, M. (2011). First four years in Canada: Post-secondary education pathways of highly-educated immigrants. Accepted in Journal of International Migration and Integration.

Adamuti-Trache, M. & Sweet, R. (2010). Adult immigrants’ participation in Canadian education and training. Canadian Journal for the Study of Adult Education, 22(2), 1-26.

Anisef, P., Sweet, R. & Adamuti-Trache, M. (2009). Impact of Canadian PSE on recent immigrants’ labour market outcomes. Ottawa: Citizenship and Immigration Canada. Available at: http://www.cic.gc.ca/english/resources/research/impact_postsecondary.asp Bourdieu, P. (1990). The logic of practice. Cambridge: Polity Press.

Boyd, M. (2009). Official language proficiency and the civic participation of immigrants. Paper presented at Metropolis Language Matters Symposium, Ottawa, Canada. Retrieved July 15 2010 http://canada.metropolis.net/events/metropolis_presents/LanguageSeminar/Presentations/Boyd_Civic_Participation_paper_e.pdf

Chiswick, B. R. & P. W. Miller (1995). The endogeneity between language and earnings: International analyses. Journal of Labor Economics, 13(2), 246–288.

Dustmann, C. (1999). Temporary migration, human capital, and language fluency of migrants. Scandinavian Journal of Economics, 101, 297–314.

Green, A. G., & Green, D. A. (1999). The economic goals of Canada’s immigration policy, Canadian Public Policy, 25(4), 425-451.

Grondin, C. (2007). Knowledge of official languages among new immigrants: How important is it in the labour market? Catalogue no. 89-624-XIE. Ottawa: Statistics Canada.

Lancee, B. (2008). The economic returns of immigrants’ bonding and bridging social capital: The case of the Netherlands. International Migration Review, 44(1), 202-226.

Reitz, J. G. (2007). Immigrant employment success in Canada, Part I: Individual and contextual causes. Journal of International Migration and Integration, 8, 11-36.

Statistics Canada (2005). Longitudinal survey of immigrants to Canada: A portrait of early settlement experiences. Catalogue no. 89-614-XIE, Ottawa: Statistics Canada, Minister of Industry.

13