it scars': meaning making and psychological impacts of

140
University of Calgary PRISM: University of Calgary's Digital Repository Graduate Studies The Vault: Electronic Theses and Dissertations 2017 "It Scars": Meaning Making and Psychological Impacts of Parental Feeding Control Brun, Isabel Brun, I. (2017). "It Scars": Meaning Making and Psychological Impacts of Parental Feeding Control (Unpublished master's thesis). University of Calgary, Calgary, AB. doi:10.11575/PRISM/26687 http://hdl.handle.net/11023/3937 master thesis University of Calgary graduate students retain copyright ownership and moral rights for their thesis. You may use this material in any way that is permitted by the Copyright Act or through licensing that has been assigned to the document. For uses that are not allowable under copyright legislation or licensing, you are required to seek permission. Downloaded from PRISM: https://prism.ucalgary.ca

Upload: others

Post on 05-Oct-2021

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

University of Calgary

PRISM: University of Calgary's Digital Repository

Graduate Studies The Vault: Electronic Theses and Dissertations

2017

"It Scars": Meaning Making and Psychological

Impacts of Parental Feeding Control

Brun, Isabel

Brun, I. (2017). "It Scars": Meaning Making and Psychological Impacts of Parental Feeding Control

(Unpublished master's thesis). University of Calgary, Calgary, AB. doi:10.11575/PRISM/26687

http://hdl.handle.net/11023/3937

master thesis

University of Calgary graduate students retain copyright ownership and moral rights for their

thesis. You may use this material in any way that is permitted by the Copyright Act or through

licensing that has been assigned to the document. For uses that are not allowable under

copyright legislation or licensing, you are required to seek permission.

Downloaded from PRISM: https://prism.ucalgary.ca

UNIVERSITY OF CALGARY

“It Scars”: Meaning Making and Psychological Impacts of Parental Feeding Control

by

Isabel Brun

A THESIS

SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES

IN PARTIAL FULFILMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE

DEGREE OF MASTER OF SCIENCE

GRADUATE PROGRAM IN EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY

CALGARY, ALBERTA

JULY, 2017

© Isabel Brun 2017

ii

Abstract

Given the negative physiological and psychosocial outcomes associated with childhood obesity,

it is no wonder that parents may want to utilize parental feeding control practices in order to

promote weight loss, or prevent weight gain, in their children. However, regardless of parents’

best intentions, parental feeding control practices have been found to have counterproductive

effects on children’s eating behaviours and weight status. Despite these findings, minimal

research has been conducted to examine how these parental feeding control practices are

subjectively experienced by children. As such, the current study explored perceived meaning

making and psychological impacts of parental feeding control practices through constructivist

grounded theory methods. Results from the preliminary constructivist grounded theory indicate

that individuals face lasting negative meaning making (e.g., viewing self-worth as contingent on

weight), psychological (e.g., fearing weight gain), and behavioural (e.g., engaging in

maladaptive eating behaviours) impacts as a result of experiencing parental feeding control

practices during their childhoods. Findings have implications for counselling and future

research.

Keywords: parental feeding control practices, childhood overweight and obesity,

constructivist grounded theory

iii

Acknowledgements

This thesis would not have been possible without the help, guidance, and support of

several individuals. First and foremost, I would like to thank my advisor, Dr. Shelly Russell-

Mayhew, for her tremendously helpful support and guidance throughout this project. I am

incredibly grateful to have you as my advisor. To my student mentor, Kirsten, thank you for

being my cheerleader. Your enthusiasm for this project, as well as your helpful feedback, has

been invaluable in shaping this project. I would also like to thank my partner, Stephane, for his

unwavering love and support. You have kept me sane throughout this completely insane process

and I could not have done this without you. To my baby dog, Piper, thank you for keeping me

company during the writing process, and for giving me puppy cuddles and kisses when I was

feeling stressed out. Thank you to my fabulous cohort for their encouragement. The supportive

environment we shared made this experience a truly enjoyable one. To my exam committee

members, Dr. Tom Strong and Dr. Janet Groen, thank you for all your helpful feedback. I would

also like to thank the rest of the Counselling Psychology faculty for making my time at

University of Calgary incredibly valuable – thank you. To my long-distance friends and family,

thank you for always supporting and believing in me. I would also like to extend a big thank you

to all of the employees at Sorso for keeping me fed and caffeinated throughout the writing

process. Finally, thank you to the wonderful women who participated in my study. This project

would not have been possible without your candour and vulnerability.

iv

Table of Contents

Abstract ........................................................................................................................................ ii

Acknowledgements ..................................................................................................................... iii

Table of Contents ........................................................................................................................ iv

List of Tables ............................................................................................................................ viii

List of Figures and Illustrations .................................................................................................. ix

Chapter One: Introduction ........................................................................................................1

Parental Feeding Control Practices ...................................................................................1

Personal Relevance ...........................................................................................................2

The Current Study.............................................................................................................4

Overview of Thesis Chapters ............................................................................................4

Chapter Two: Literature Review ..............................................................................................6

Childhood Overweight and Obesity Rates and Consequences .........................................6

Prevalence Rates ...................................................................................................6

Consequences of Obesity ......................................................................................7

Physiological Consequences ........................................................................8

Psychosocial Consequences .........................................................................9

Factors Contributing to Childhood Obesity .......................................................10

The Current Food Environment .................................................................11

Multidimensional Framework....................................................................11

Parenting Factors .......................................................................................12

Parenting and Feeding .....................................................................................................13

Parenting .............................................................................................................13

Parenting Styles .........................................................................................13

Parenting Dimensions ................................................................................14

Parenting Practices .....................................................................................15

Parental Feeding ..................................................................................................15

Feeding Styles ............................................................................................15

Parental Feeding Practices and Control .....................................................16

Covert and Overt Feeding Control Practices ....................................17

Relationships between Feeding Styles and Feeding Practices ...................17

v

Impacts of Parental Feeding Control Practices ...............................................................18

Children’s Self-Regulation of Food Consumption ..............................................18

Children’s Food Preferences and Weight Status .................................................19

Impacts of Covert versus Overt Feeding Control Practices ................................20

Factors that Predict the Use of Parental Feeding Control Practices ...............................21

Parental Factors ...................................................................................................21

Child Factors .......................................................................................................22

Multidirectional Model .......................................................................................23

Summary .........................................................................................................................24

Current Study ..................................................................................................................25

Conclusion ......................................................................................................................26

Chapter Three: Methodology ..................................................................................................27

Grounded Theory ............................................................................................................27

Traditional Grounded Theory Methodology .......................................................28

The Evolution of Grounded Theory ....................................................................29

Constructivist Grounded Theory Methodology ..................................................30

Rationale for Constructivist Grounded Theory Methodology ............................31

Procedures .......................................................................................................................33

Participant Recruitment .......................................................................................33

Data Collection and Analysis ..............................................................................34

Interviews ...................................................................................................35

Grounded Theory Methods and Data Analysis ...................................................36

Questioning ................................................................................................36

Theoretical Sampling .................................................................................37

Constant Comparison Method ...................................................................37

Coding ..............................................................................................38

Memo Writing and Reflexivity ..................................................................39

Theoretical Sensitivity ...............................................................................40

Ethical Considerations .........................................................................................40

Evaluating Grounded Theory ..........................................................................................41

Strategies for Evaluation .....................................................................................42

vi

Thick Descriptions .....................................................................................42

Triangulation ..............................................................................................42

Chapter Summary............................................................................................................43

Chapter Four: Results ..............................................................................................................44

Participants ......................................................................................................................44

Development of Themes and Categories ........................................................................47

Constructivist Grounded Theory Data Analysis Procedures Revisited ..............47

The Constructivist Grounded Theory .............................................................................48

Identified Core Concepts, Themes, and Sub-Themes .........................................48

Identified Dynamic Processes – The Theoretical Model ....................................50

Making Meaning of Parental Feeding Control ....................................................52

Perceiving Parental Motivation for Feeding Control .................................52

Appearance and Child’s Future Success ..........................................53

Weight Stigma and Bullying ............................................................54

Health ...............................................................................................54

Internalizing Parental Messages about Weight ..........................................55

Viewing Self-Worth as Contingent on Weight ..........................................57

Experiencing Psychological Impacts ..................................................................59

Becoming Self-Conscious of Weight ........................................................59

Feeling Guilt about Eating .........................................................................61

Fearing Weight Gain ..................................................................................62

Experiencing Behavioural Impacts .....................................................................64

Engaging in Maladaptive Eating Behaviours ............................................64

Restricting ........................................................................................64

Bingeing and Engaging in Compensatory Eating Behaviours .........65

Developing a Long-Lasting Preoccupation with Weight ....................................68

Seeing Parental Feeding Control Practices as Unhelpful ....................................70

Offering advice ..........................................................................................70

Summary of the Constructivist Grounded Theory ..........................................................72

Chapter Five: Discussion ..........................................................................................................74

Contributions to the Literature ........................................................................................75

vii

Contributions to the Feeding Literature ..............................................................76

Contributions to the Weight Stigma and Weight Bias Literature .......................77

Weight Stigma Literature ...........................................................................77

Internalized Weight Bias Literature ...........................................................79

Consequences of Weight Stigma and Internalized Weight Bias ......80

Consequences of Weight Stigma ............................................80

Consequences of Internalized Weight Bias ............................80

Intergenerational Internalized Weight Bias ............................81

Contributions to the Childhood Obesity Treatment Literature ...........................82

Implications for Counselling Psychology .......................................................................84

Prevention and Treatment Implications ..............................................................85

Strengths and Limitations ...............................................................................................86

Future Research ..............................................................................................................88

Conclusion ......................................................................................................................90

References ..................................................................................................................................93

Appendices ...............................................................................................................................118

Appendix A: Recruitment Poster ..................................................................................118

Appendix B: Recruitment Screening Questions ...........................................................119

Appendix C: Consent Form ..........................................................................................120

Appendix D: Participant Demographic Information Form ...........................................125

Appendix E: Interview Questions .................................................................................128

Appendix F: Sample Memos .......................................................................................130

viii

List of Tables

Table 1: Participant Demographic Information ..........................................................................45

Table 2: Participants’ Parental Demographic Information .........................................................46

Table 3: Meaning Making and Psychological Impact of Parental Feeding Control ...................49

ix

List of Figures and Illustrations

Figure 1: Birch and Ventura’s (2008) Multidirectional Model ..................................................23

Figure 2: Meaning Making and Psychological Impacts of Parental Feeding Control ................51

1

CHAPTER ONE

Introduction

In North America, childhood overweight and obesity continues to be a serious public

health concern (Roberts, Shields, de Groh, Aziz, & Gilbert, 2012). Canadian statistics indicate

that 31.5 % of children (5 to 17 years of age) are classified as overweight or obese (Roberts et

al., 2012). This statistic is concerning as researchers have found that children who are

categorized as obese are at a greater risk for developing a variety of health issues, are more

susceptible to weight-related bullying, stigmatization, and marginalization, and are more likely to

suffer from co-morbid psychological disorders (e.g., depression) than their peers who are

classified as normal weight (Gray, Janicke, Wistedt, & Dumont-Driscoll, 2010; Hayden-Wade et

al., 2005; Roberts et al., 2012). Moreover, it has been found that childhood obesity often persists

into adulthood (Whitaker, Wright, Pepe, Seidel, & Dietz, 1997). Given the potential

implications of childhood obesity, it is no surprise that parents want to protect their children from

becoming, or being classified as, overweight or obese.

Parental Feeding Control Practices

Given the various negative physiological and psychosocial consequences associated with

childhood obesity, it is not shocking that there has been a push towards developing and

implementing strategies that aid in the management of children’s weight (i.e., to help children

lose weight or to prevent children from gaining weight). Furthermore, as the current societal

standard of beauty glorifies thinness and marginalizes fatness, some parents may want to

implement weight management strategies in order to ensure that their children meet societal

beauty standards.

2

While professionally administered child weight management programs aim to maximize

benefits and minimize negative repercussions, not all parents seek professional support while

implementing weight loss, or weight gain prevention, strategies for their children. As a result,

some parents may choose to employ their own tactics to encourage weight loss, or prevent

weight gain, in their children. In fact, some parents may utilize parental feeding control practices

(i.e., restricting and monitoring a child’s diet, as well as pressuring them to eat specific foods like

fruits and vegetables) to limit their children’s consumption of palatable and energy-dense foods

(i.e., foods high in sugar, fat, and calories). Yet, despite their best efforts and intentions, parental

feeding control practices aimed at promoting weight loss or preventing weight gain have been

found to hinder children’s ability to regulate their food consumption which increases their risk

for becoming, or remaining, overweight or obese (Carper, Fisher, & Birch, 2000; Costanzo &

Woody, 1985; Cutting, Fisher, Grimm-Thomas, & Birch, 1999; Shunk & Birch, 2004).

Despite these findings, which indicate that parental feeding control practices aimed at

promoting weight loss or preventing weight gain can negatively impact children’s eating

behaviours and weight status, little research has been conducted to examine how parental feeding

control practices are subjectively experienced by children, and how they impact children’s

psychological well-being. As such, the current study examined remembered childhood

experiences of parental feeding control practices aimed at promoting weight loss or preventing

weight gain. Specifically, the meaning making and psychological impacts of these practices

were investigated.

Personal Relevance

“Nothing tastes as good as skinny feels.” It may not be surprising, in this current thin

obsessed society, that these words were spoken to me by one of my parents during my childhood.

3

As I had dealt with weight-related struggles during my teenage years, I was intensely curious

about the factors that could have contributed to my immense discomfort with living in a body

that did not perfectly fit societal standards. During my undergraduate studies in Psychology, I

began to wonder how my past food-, body-, and weight-related parent-child interactions had

affected my eating behaviours and overall well-being. Consequently, this curiosity sparked my

passion for researching the influence of parenting practices on children’s body image and eating

behaviours.

It was not until I read Dara-Lynn Weiss’ (2013) memoir entitled The Heavy: A Mother, A

Daughter, A Diet that I became interested in examining the effects of parental feeding control

practices on children’s psychological well-being. In her tell-all book, Weiss (2013) chronicled

her experience of putting her seven-year-old daughter on a diet. She described how she put her

daughter on a strict eating regimen that was based on dietary restriction (e.g., calorie counting,

portion control, and withholding the occasional meal). Overall, the book describes Weiss’

experience of getting her daughter’s weight into the “medically-acceptable”, and “socially

desirable”, range.

After reading this book, I felt as though there was a missing voice in the narrative. In

fact, there was little to no mention of Weiss’ daughter’s experience of parental feeding control

practices. How did she feel about being put on a restrictive diet? How did these feeding

methods impact her psychological well-being? How did she make sense of her mother’s

decision to control her eating in that way? I had many questions for Weiss’ daughter, but no

answers. Given these queries, I conducted a preliminary literature review to see if there had been

any research conducted on the subjective experience of parental feeding control practices from

the child’s perspective. To my surprise, most of the research focused on parental experiences of,

4

or motivations for, using these types of feeding methods (e.g., Carnell, Cooke, Cheng, Robbins,

& Wardle, 2011; Gray et al., 2010). Furthermore, when the research was focused on the child, it

explored the behavioural impacts of these practices (e.g., Birch & Fisher, 1995; Johnson &

Birch, 1994; Thompson, 2010; Vos & Welsh, 2010) and did not investigate the effect of these

methods on children’s psychological well-being. Therefore, I made it my mission to give a voice

to individuals who had experienced parental feeding control practices during their childhoods, in

order to gain a deeper understanding of the impacts of, and meanings associated with, these

feeding methods.

The Current Study

The objective of the current study was to examine remembered childhood experiences of

parental feeding control practices from a qualitative perspective. The study investigated the

following two research questions: How do young adults make meaning of their childhood

experiences of parental feeding control practices? and What are the perceived psychological

impacts of parental feeding control practices? A constructivist grounded theory analysis was

used to systematically construct a preliminary conceptualization that was grounded within the

data (i.e., interview transcripts; Charmaz, 2006). Constructivist grounded theory methods were

fitting for the aim of the current study as they enabled me to not only describe experiences of

parental feeding control, but also co-create theoretical hypotheses about the impacts of this

phenomenon (Charmaz, 2006).

Overview of Thesis Chapters

So far, I have provided background information on parental feeding control practices,

explained the personal relevance of this research, and briefly described the current study. In

chapter two, I review the research literature on childhood overweight and obesity, parental

5

feeding control practices, and their associated concepts. Following this, in chapter three, I

describe the study’s methodology, and discuss the ways in which trustworthiness was addressed

according to this method. In chapter four, I describe the results of the data collection and

analysis. Lastly, in chapter five, I discuss the study’s findings in relation to the research

literature. Furthermore, I address the strengths and limitations of the study, implications for

counselling psychology, and recommendations for future research.

6

CHAPTER TWO

Literature Review

This chapter provides a review of the literature on the concepts associated with childhood

obesity and parental feeding control practices. To begin, Canadian statistics on childhood

overweight and obesity will be addressed. This is followed by an overview of the potential

negative physiological and psychosocial consequences of childhood obesity. Factors that may be

contributing to the development of childhood obesity will then be discussed. Next, concepts

associated with parenting and feeding will be described. Parental feeding control practices will

then be defined, and possible consequences of these practices will be outlined. Subsequently,

factors contributing to the use of parental feeding control practices will be provided. Lastly, a

summary of this chapter, as well as an overview of the current study, will be presented.

Childhood Overweight and Obesity Rates & Consequences

In order to understand the context within which parents decide to utilize parental feeding

control practices to promote weight loss or to prevent weight gain in their children, I have

decided to frame my study within the childhood obesity literature. It is important to note that I

am using the language and discourses that are prevalent within this literature (e.g., “childhood

obesity epidemic”) in order to exemplify the messages that parents receive about childhood

overweight and obesity. Altogether, it is necessary to consider the impact of these discourses on

parents’ decision making process related to the use of feeding control practices.

Prevalence Rates

As was previously mentioned, childhood obesity continues to be a serious public health

concern across the globe. According to the World Health Organization (WHO, 2016), the

number of children (0 to 5 years of age) who are classified as overweight or obese worldwide has

7

increased from 32 million in 1990 to 42 million in 2013. The WHO (2014) estimates that, if

current trends persist, the global number of children who are deemed overweight or obese will

climb to 70 million by 2025.

While global statistics on childhood overweight and obesity are unnerving, Canadian

statistics on this topic do not offer much solace. Canadian childhood overweight and obesity

prevalence rates indicate that approximately one third of 5 to 17 year old children (i.e., 1.6

million children) were classified as overweight (19.8%) or obese (11.7%) from 2009 to 2011

(Roberts et al., 2012). Among children ages 5 to 11, approximately 19.7% were categorized as

overweight and 13.1% were classified as obese (Roberts et al., 2012). Prevalence rates among

adolescents (i.e., ages 12 to 17) reveal that approximately 19.9% were deemed overweight, while

10.2% were considered obese (Roberts et al., 2012).

As can be recognized based on the previously mentioned statistics, the obesity epidemic

is no longer an adult issue; children and adolescents are being impacted by obesity in

increasingly large numbers. This is reason for concern as obesity has been associated with a

wide variety of negative physiological and psychosocial consequences.

Consequences of Obesity

In light of these childhood obesity rates, it is crucial to consider its potential negative

impact on children’s physiology and psychosocial well-being. Researchers have found that

children with obesity are more likely to develop a variety of physiological issues (e.g., type II

diabetes; Daniels, 2006), are more vulnerable to social judgement (e.g., weight related

marginalization; Strauss & Pollack, 2003), and are more likely to develop co-morbid

psychological disorders (e.g., depression; Daniels, 2006) than children who are classified as

normal weight (Gray et al., 2010; Hayden-Wade et al., 2005; Roberts et al., 2012). Moreover,

8

researchers suggest that children and adolescents who are classified as overweight or obese are

more likely than their peers who are categorized as normal weight to struggle with adult obesity

(Whitaker et al., 1997).

Physiological consequences. Researchers have established a strong relationship between

childhood obesity and negative physiological conditions, which include: endocrine issues (e.g.,

early onset of puberty in girls), neurological problems (e.g., pseudotumor cerebri),

gastroenterological issues (e.g., gallstones and steatohepatitis), and musculoskeletal conditions

(Dietz, 1998, Must & Strauss, 1999, Philippas & Lo, 2005). Additionally, researchers have

found a relationship between childhood obesity and cardiovascular risk factors such as

dyslipidemia, hypertension, coagulopathy, chronic inflammation, and endothelial dysfunction

(Burke, 2006; Dietz, 1998; Freedman, Dietz, Srinivasan, & Berenson, 1999; Kiess et al., 2001;

Must & Strauss, 1999; Philippas & Lo, 2005; Raitakari, Juonala, & Viikari, 2005; Reilly et al.,

2003; Wabitsch, 2000). In fact, a study conducted by the American Medical Association found

that 70% of the obese children studied had at least one cardiovascular disease risk factor, while

more than 25% of those children possessed two or more risk factors (Bell et al., 2011).

Moreover, childhood obesity has also been associated with certain pulmonary risks which

include: sleep apnea, asthma, and exercise intolerance (Dietz, 1998; Must & Strauss, 1999;

Philippas & Lo, 2005; Reilly et al., 2003).

While early onset cardiovascular and pulmonary risks can significantly impact an

individual’s health, perhaps the most concerning physiological consequence of childhood obesity

is the rising prevalence of insulin resistance and type II diabetes. Type II diabetes is being

diagnosed in children and adolescents much more frequently than in previous years (Vivier &

Tompkins, 2008). While a definitive cause is presently unknown, it is believed that obesity,

9

along with other risk factors (e.g., lack of exercise), can lead to insulin resistance and type II

diabetes (Daniels, 2006; Ludwig & Ebbeling, 2001). The rising prevalence rates of insulin

resistance and type II diabetes in children and adolescents is worrisome as long-term

consequences of these issues include vascular disease leading to heart attacks, strokes, kidney

disease and failure, blindness, and other health problems (Vivier & Tompkins, 2008).

Overall, obesity can significantly affect an individual’s life and health; however,

consequences appear to be more severe when obesity is diagnosed during childhood and

adolescence. Due to the rising prevalence rates of obesity among children, susceptibility to the

development of many chronic diseases has increased and the age of possible onset has decreased

(Flegal, Tabak, & Ogden, 2006; Park, Falconer, Viner, & Kinra, 2012). What is more, childhood

obesity has been linked to negative psychosocial outcomes. As such, the following section will

address the ways in which childhood obesity can affect children’s mental health and social well-

being.

Psychosocial consequences. While greatly understudied in comparison to the potential

physiological consequences, researchers have also investigated the impacts of childhood obesity

on children’s psychological and social well-being. To begin, children who are classified as

obese have been shown to be more susceptible to weight-based stigmatization and

marginalization (Puhl & Latner, 2007; Strauss & Pollack, 2003). In a study conducted by

Strauss and Pollack (2003), the social relationships of adolescents who were classified as

overweight were compared to those of adolescents categorized as normal weight; findings

indicated that adolescents who were deemed overweight were more likely to be socially isolated,

as well as to be peripheral to social groups than their peers who were classified as normal weight.

Furthermore, researchers have found that children who are categorized as overweight or obese

10

are up to two times more likely to fall victim to overt (e.g., hitting) and relational (e.g., teasing

and rumours) bullying than children who are classified as normal weight (Eisenberg, Neumark-

Sztainer, & Story, 2003; Hayden-Wade et al., 2005). Additionally, in a study conducted by

Hayden-Wade and colleagues (2005), it was found that children who were deemed overweight

experienced more prevalent, frequent, and upsetting appearance-related teasing (e.g., derogatory

nicknames) than children who were categorized as normal weight.

Researchers also indicate that children who are classified as overweight and obese are

victims of stigmatization and marginalization not only by their peers, as outlined above, but also

by their teachers (Bauer, Yang, & Austin, 2004; Neumark-Sztainer, Story, & Harris, 1999) and

parents (Crandall, 1995; Davison & Birch, 2004b). This type of treatment is especially

distressing during childhood and adolescence when the development of social relationships is of

utmost importance. As such, it has been found that children who are deemed overweight or

obese may be more susceptible to developing depression, increased suicidal ideation and

attempts, as well as decreased body satisfaction and lower self-esteem as a consequence of

experiencing weight related bias and marginalization (Eisenberg et al., 2003, Hayden-Wade et

al., 2005; Puhl & Latner, 2007; Storch et al., 2007). Lastly, childhood experiences of weight

related stigmatization could encumber children’s psychological, social, and emotional

development, while also exacerbating the negative physiological complications associated with

childhood obesity (Daniels, 2006; Weiss & Caprio, 2005). Given the potential consequences of

childhood obesity stigmatization, it is imperative that researchers and practitioners consider

factors that may be contributing to the development childhood obesity.

Factors Contributing to Childhood Obesity

11

The current food environment. The increasing trend towards higher childhood

overweight and obesity rates has largely been attributed to the ‘obesogenic’ food environment

that is common in developed countries (e.g., the United States of America and Canada), whereby

sedentary lifestyles and the high consumption of palatable, energy dense foods creates an

imbalance in energy intake and energy expenditure; this phenomenon tends to result in excessive

weight gain (Golan & Crow, 2004). In today’s society, parents are held responsible for

promoting the development of healthy dietary habits in their children, and protecting them from

the current obesogenic food environment in which “unhealthy” foods are readily accessible.

Given the current ‘obesogenic’ food environment, it is not shocking that children’s diets

tend to be too high in fat, sugar, and calories, and too low in the intake of fruits and vegetables

(Reedy & Krebs-Smith, 2010). Human predispositions and the current food environment come

together to explain this phenomenon (Savage, Fisher, & Birch, 2007). Children are born with a

preference towards sugar and fat, which are present in energy-dense foods (i.e., high in calories),

and often dislike sour and bitter tastes found in plant-based foods such as fruits and vegetables

(i.e., nutrient-rich foods; Savage et al., 2007). As such, children tend to prefer the “unhealthy”

foods, over “healthier” options. Moreover, marketing strategies that encourage children to

consume energy-dense, nutrient poor foods (e.g., children’s menus) greatly influence children’s

preferences for such foods (Grier, Mensinger, Huang, Kumanyika, & Stettler, 2007; O'Donnell,

Hoerr, Mendoza, & Goh, 2008). Given all of these factors, it is not surprising that some parents

struggle with managing their children’s weight.

Multidimensional framework. As was previously mentioned, the ‘obesogenic’ food

environment has been largely credited for the increasing prevalence rates of childhood

overweight and obesity; however, a growing number of researchers suggest that multiple factors

12

come together to result in childhood obesity. Gable and Lutz (2000) propose that childhood

obesity is a consequence of a multidimensional system with interacting elements (e.g., food

environment, economy, parental factors, child factors, and level of physical activity). With this

being said, they suggest that parental influence is one of the most significant factors contributing

to the development of unhealthy eating habits in children, and their resulting weight status

(Gable & Lutz, 2000). In fact, researchers have shown that a child’s health is more significantly

impacted by their familial microsystems than any other sphere of influence (e.g., media,

community, and school). As such, it is important to take into account parenting factors that may

influence the development of eating habits in children, and their subsequent weight

categorization.

Parenting factors. Researchers suggest that parents can either promote or discourage the

development of particular eating habits through specific feeding practices (Ventura & Birch,

2008). In fact, it has been shown that parental feeding practices can impact children’s food

preferences and consumption which, in turn, may affect children’s weight status (Campbell,

Crawford, & Ball, 2006; Thompson, 2010). Additionally, researchers indicate that eating habits

develop during early childhood and tend to persist into later childhood, adolescence, and

adulthood (Birch & Fisher, 1998). Altogether, the previously mentioned research findings

highlight the importance of considering the influence of parental feeding practices on the

development of children’s eating habits, and their resulting weight status. However, before we

consider the role of parental feeding practices on children’s eating behaviours and weight status,

it is necessary to first understand the concepts surrounding parenting and feeding.

13

Parenting and Feeding

Eating patterns are developed during early childhood, persist into later childhood,

adolescence and beyond, and are significant determinants of weight status (Birch & Davison,

2001a; Robinson et al., 2007). From early childhood, what, when, and how much children

consume is influenced by a multitude of factors including: children’s behaviours and internal

appetitive cues, parents’ recognition of, and responsiveness to, these cues, and the parents’

parenting beliefs and attitudes (Satter, 1986). Researchers suggest that early parent-child

interactions related to food and eating influence children’s development of food preferences,

eating habits, and dietary self-regulation, which subsequently impacts children’s weight status

(Birch, 2006; Hetherington, Cecil, Jackson, & Schwartz, 2011). As such, it is necessary to

consider the role of parenting and feeding in the development of children’s eating behaviours and

habits, as well as their weight status.

Parenting

Parenting refers to child-rearing activities that aim to promote and support development

across a range of domains (Davies, 2000). In the research literature, parenting has been

described in terms of styles, dimensions, and practices.

Parenting styles. Parenting styles refer to the overall emotional environment produced

by parents during interactions with their children (Jansen, Daniels, & Nicholson, 2012).

Traditionally, parenting styles have been characterized using a taxonomical approach based on

combining the two dimensions: parental demandingness (i.e., the extent to which parents have

expectations for, and control over, their children) and parental responsiveness (i.e., the extent to

which parents exhibit warmth and support towards their children; Darling & Steinberg, 1993).

These dimensions come together to form four different parenting styles, these are: (a)

14

authoritative (i.e., high demandingness and high responsiveness); (b) authoritarian (i.e., high

demandingness and low responsiveness); (c) indulgent/permissive (i.e., low demandingness and

high responsiveness); and (d) uninvolved/neglectful parenting (i.e., low demandingness and low

responsiveness; Maccoby & Martin, 1983). Researchers have found that, within a western

context, authoritative parenting has been related to more positive developmental outcomes (e.g.,

children demonstrate better socio-emotional skills and cognitive ability, as well as engage in less

health risk behaviours) than those of authoritarian, indulgent/permissive, and

uninvolved/neglectful parenting styles (Baumrind, 1991; Bornstein & Zlotnik, 2009; Jackson,

Henriksen, & Foshee, 1998; Lytle et al., 2003; Sleddens et al., 2011; Smith, 2011).

Parenting dimensions. Parenting dimensions refer to ways of describing parenting

practices (Lucas, Maguire, & Nicholson, 2010). In addition to the previously mentioned

parenting dimensions, demandingness and responsiveness, other dimensions of parenting

practices have been recognized, including: self-efficacy (i.e., parental beliefs about parenting

ability), irritability or hostility (i.e., displaying feelings of anger or frustration towards their child

or emotional reactivity), consistency (i.e., consistently setting and applying age-appropriate rules

and expectations for their child), autonomy-encouragement (i.e., encouraging self-reliance

through helping the child to make their own decisions, learn rules, and achieve tasks in a

manageable, stepwise manner), and over-protection (i.e., giving too much guidance, protection,

and assistance in relation to the child’s capabilities; Lucas et al., 2010). Commonly, children

demonstrate better developmental outcomes when parents are high in self-efficacy, consistency,

and autonomy-encouragement, as well as low in irritable or hostile affect and over-

protectiveness (Bayer et al., 2011; Wake, Nicholson, Hardy, & Smith, 2007).

15

Parenting practices. Parenting practices refer to situation-specific strategies that parents

use to shape their children’s development; these strategies may change over time, across

situations, and with different children (Bornstein & Zlotnik, 2009; Walker & Kirby, 2010).

Parenting practices are used to operationalize parenting dimensions and styles. In brief,

parenting practices are what parents do (i.e., behaviours they display), while parenting styles

describe how parents do it (i.e., the overall emotional environment characterized by parenting

dimensions).

Parental Feeding

Parenting and feeding are intimately related; in fact, one of the most important

responsibilities associated with early parenting is feeding (Satter, 1986). Given the rising

interest in researching the potential relationships between parenting and the development of

eating habits in children, there has been an attempt to relate conventional parenting concepts to

feeding (e.g., feeding styles and feeding practices).

Feeding styles. The previously mentioned parenting framework (i.e., parenting

taxonomy related to parental responsiveness and demandingness) has also been applied to the

area of feeding (Faith, Scanlon, Birch, Francis, & Sherry, 2004; Hughes, Power, Fisher, Mueller,

& Nicklas, 2005). In feeding, demandingness refers to the extent to which parents have control

over their children’s food intake, while responsiveness refers to how parents encourage their

children to eat (Hughes et al., 2005). For instance, parents who exhibit an authoritative feeding

style (i.e., high responsiveness and high demandingness) may allow their children to choose

among a variety of presented foods, while parents who display an authoritarian feeding style

(i.e., low responsiveness and high demandingness) may disregard the child’s food preferences

and make the dietary decision for the child.

16

Parental feeding practices and control. Parental feeding practices refer to the

situation-specific behavioural strategies that parents use in order to manage when, what, and how

much their children eat (Birch, Fisher, & Davison, 2003; Ventura & Birch, 2008; Vereecken,

Legiest, De Bourdeaudhuij, & Maes, 2009). If parents are concerned about their child’s weight

(i.e., they want their child to lose weight, or are concerned about their child gaining weight), they

may choose to employ an assortment of feeding practices to monitor and control their child’s

eating (Birch et al., 2003). These types of feeding strategies are categorized in the literature as

feeding control practices, and are used to limit children’s access to palatable and energy dense

foods (i.e., foods that are high in fat, sugar, and calories; Fisher & Birch, 1999). For instance,

parents who utilize parental feeding control practices may try to restrict their children’s access

to, and consumption of “unhealthy” foods (Birch et al., 2003). It should be noted that parental

feeding control practices are not always utilized to promote weight loss or to prevent weight gain

in children (e.g., pressuring a child to eat more in order to promote weight gain in a child who is

underweight). However, for the purpose of this study, I have chosen to focus on parental feeding

control practices that are employed to combat the development, or progression, of childhood

overweight and obesity.

Researchers that have investigated feeding control practices have demonstrated that

parents use a myriad of practices to control their children’s consumption of palatable and energy-

dense foods, including: a) restriction, b) pressure to eat, c) monitoring, d) emotional feeding, and

e) using food as a reward (Birch, Fisher, Grimm-Thomas, Markey, Sawyer, & Johnson, 2001;

Davison & Birch, 2001a; Rhee, 2008; Skouteris, McCabe, Swinburn, Newgreen, Sacher, &

Chadwick, 2011; Wardle, Sanderson, Guthrie, Rapoport, & Plomin, 2002). Restriction entails

limiting a child’s total amount of food consumption and/or access to unhealthy foods (Birch et

17

al., 2001). Pressure to eat refers to insisting that a child to eat certain foods (e.g., fruits and

vegetables; Birch et al., 2001). Monitoring involves supervising a child’s consumption of

energy-dense foods (Birch & Fisher, 2000). Emotional feeding has been defined as feeding a

child in an attempt to remedy emotional distress (Wardle et al., 2002). Lastly, using food as a

reward entails giving a child food rewards for good behaviours (Rhee, 2008). While all of the

previously mentioned feeding practices have been recognized as controlling, the use of

restriction, monitoring, and pressure to eat are most commonly described as the three primary

feeding practices associated with parental feeding control aimed at promoting weight loss or

preventing weight gain in children (Birch et al., 2001).

Covert and overt feeding control practices. Parental feeding control practices can also

be described as either covert or overt (Ogden, Reynolds, & Smith, 2006). Parental feeding

control practices are described as covert when they are less perceivable to the child. Such covert

feeding control practices can include: restricting the amount of times children are taken to

restaurants that serve “unhealthy” foods, and not eating unhealthy foods in front of children

(Ogden et al., 2006). Other parental feeding control practices may be categorized as overt (i.e.,

more perceivable to children) and often occur after unhealthy foods are present in the home

(Ogden et al., 2006). Examples of overt parental feeding control practices could include: putting

the “unhealthy” foods out of children’s reach, or getting children to ask permission to eat or

access these foods (Ogden et al., 2006).

Relationships between feeding styles and feeding practices. Researchers that have

investigated the relationship between parental feeding styles and parental feeding practices have

indicated that increased use of parental feeding control practices (e.g., restriction, pressure to eat,

negative monitoring, and using food as a reward) tends to be related to authoritarian feeding

18

styles, while increased levels of parental responsiveness in feeding (e.g., feeding the child in

response to his/her hunger and satiety cues) are associated with authoritative feeding styles

(Hubbs-Tait, Kennedy, Page, Topham, & Harrist, 2008; Hughes et al, 2005).

Impacts of Parental Feeding Control Practices

During early childhood, it is vital that parents help children develop an ability to

appropriately recognize their internal cues for hunger and satiety through their feeding practices

(DiSantis, Hodges, Johnson, & Fisher, 2011; Satter, 1986). Utilizing parental feeding practices

that ignore or override children’s internal satiety cues (e.g., parental feeding control practices)

can have a negative impact on children’s self-regulation of food consumption, food preferences,

and weight status (Birch & Fisher, 1995; Johnson & Birch, 1994; Thompson, 2010; Vos &

Welsh, 2010).

Children’s Self-Regulation of Food Consumption

As was previously discussed, parents are responsible for encouraging the development of

dietary self-regulation in children. In fact, researchers indicate that children’s ability to self-

regulate food consumption can be enhanced through training in which they are taught to respond

to internal sensations of hunger or fullness (Johnson, 2000). For instance, parents can teach their

children to eat more slowly, as well as to stop eating when they feel full. Conversely, if parents

are not able to recognize, or if they actively overrule, their children’s internal cues, children do

not learn to appropriately identify and interpret their hunger and satiety cues and, as a result,

develop poor self-regulation related to food and eating (Satter, 1990).

What is more, parents may utilize inappropriate feeding practices which will further

disrupt children’s ability to regulate their food consumption (Birch et al., 2001; Monasta et al.,

2010). Researchers have shown that children who have parents that employ feeding control

19

practices display diminished ability in self-regulating dietary consumption, and are more likely

to overeat than children with parents who do not exert great control over feeding (Birch &

Fisher, 1995; Johnson & Birch, 1994). For instance, pressuring a child to eat has been found to

result in decreased responsiveness to internal satiety cues for the child, and disinhibited eating

later in life (Carper et al., 2000). Other researchers have suggested that parental, particularly

maternal, use of feeding control practices may encourage eating in the absence of hunger in

young girls (Birch, Fisher, & Davison, 2003; Fisher & Birch, 2002). Furthermore, using food to

reward good behaviour, or as a means to alleviate distress, has also been associated with

children’s reduced ability to recognize internal cues of appetite (Birch, McPheee, Shoba,

Steinberg, & Krehbiel, 1987; Cooke, Chambers, Añez, & Wardle, 2011).

Children’s Food Preferences and Weight Status

Researchers have demonstrated that a variety of parental feeding practices influence a

child’s food intake and weight (Campbell, Crawford, & Ball, 2006). In particular, feeding

control practices have been found to greatly impact a child’s consumption (Campbell et al.,

2006). Researchers propose that parents may inadvertently change their child’s preference for

certain foods through feeding control practices (Thompson, 2010). For example, by restricting a

child’s access to sweets, parents may be unintentionally increasing their child’s desire for the

restricted food (Thompson, 2010). Parents who utilize restrictive feeding practices may

encourage disinhibited eating in their children, which correlates positively with weight status

(Vos & Welsh, 2010).

Moreover, feeding control practices have consistently demonstrated a positive

relationship with childhood overweight and obesity in the literature. In a study conducted by

Gable and Lutz (2000), it was found that as parental control over feeding increases, the child’s

20

ability to self-regulate food consumption decreases. As such, when parents override children’s

ability to regulate their food consumption, it can result in children ignoring their internal

satiation cues and, as result, they continue to eat when they are no longer hungry (Campbell et

al., 2006). In one study that investigated parental feeding pressure and food consumption in four

year old children, it was found that children who are rewarded for finishing their plates

consumed significantly more calories than those who stopped eating when they were full (Birch,

McPhee, Shoba, & Steinberg, 1987).

Impacts of Covert versus Overt Feeding Control Practices

As of yet, most of the research studies on parental feeding control practices have viewed

these methods on a continuum (i.e., from low control to high control). The literature on this

subject matter is unclear on which practices represent high versus low forms of control.

Furthermore, there is no consensus on whether qualitative differences among feeding control

practices impact children’s eating behaviours and weight outcomes in distinctive ways. For

instance, Ogden and colleagues (2006) used a measure of overt and covert feeding control to

evaluate feeding control practices. Findings indicated that covert control was negatively

associated with children’s consumption of unhealthy foods, while no association was found for

overt control; however, overt control was positively linked to children’s consumption of healthy

snacks (Ogden et al., 2006). Similar to Ogden and colleagues’ (2006) conceptualization of

parental feeding control, Grolnick and Pomerantz (2009) presented a model in which the

methods parents employ to structure the home environment (e.g., rules and routines that offer

children occasions to develop self-regulation) differ from the coercive methods that parents may

utilize to control children’s behaviours (e.g., pressure, and forcing children to behave in a certain

manner). Studies examining this type of dichotomy in parental control found that structure-

21

based parenting practices (e.g., setting household rules) were related to positive behavioural

outcomes (e.g., autonomy related to school work; Marbell & Grolnick, 2012) and academic

achievement (Farkas & Grolnick, 2010). While informative, Grolnick and Pomerantz’s (2006)

model does not specifically address parental feeding practices; as such, more research is needed

to qualitatively investigate how different types of parental feeding control practices (e.g., covert

versus overt parental feeding control practices) may affect children’s eating behaviours and

weight status in distinctive ways.

Factors that Predict the Use of Parental Feeding Control Practices

Parents and children bring their own preferences, objectives, and personalities to parent-

child feeding interactions; these factors influence each other to impact parental feeding practices

(Satter, 1990). The following outlines parental and child factors that predict the use of parental

feeding control practices.

Parental Factors

Multiple parental factors come together to predict parents’ use of feeding control

practices. For instance, Costanzo and Woody (1985) theorized that parents are more likely to

engage in controlling feeding practices when they a) are concerned with their own weight and

eating issues, b) observe a tendency towards weight gain in their child, c) believe their child is at

risk for developing eating problems, and d) are worried about their child becoming obese. More

recent literature has provided support for this theory. Researchers have found that mothers are

more likely to use feeding control practices when they struggle with eating and weight issues

(e.g., restrained eating or frequent dieting; Birch & Fisher, 2000), experience body

dissatisfaction (Brown & Ogden, 2004), are worried about their child’s weight status (Francis &

Birch, 2005a; Spruijt-Metz, Li, Cohen, Birch, & Goran, 2006), view their child as overweight

22

(Francis, Hofer, & Birch, 2001), and/or observe excessive weight gain in their child (Rhee et al.,

2009; Rifas-Shiman et al., 2011). Qualitative studies on parental motivations for employing

feeding control practices have corroborated these findings. For example, research investigating

Latino mothers’ feeding beliefs and practices found that mothers employed restrictive feeding

practices when they experienced significant concern over their child becoming overweight in

spite of cultural ideals which favour “chubby” children (Lindsay, Sussner, Greaney, & Peterson,

2011).

Furthermore, researchers have demonstrated that parents are more likely to use feeding

control practices if they are biased towards individuals with larger bodies (Musher-Eizenman,

Holub, Hauser, & Young, 2007). Research conducted by Musher-Eizenman and colleagues

(2007) investigated how parental anti-fat attitudes impact parental feeding practices. They found

that parents’ prejudice towards large individuals predicted the use of restrictive feeding practices

above and beyond the influence of parent and child weight status, as well as parental concern

about the child becoming overweight. These results indicate that parental anti-fat attitudes also

predict the use of parental feeding control practices.

Child Factors

Child temperament may also be associated with parents’ use of feeding control practices.

For example, lower levels of inhibitory control (i.e., ability to plan and suppress inappropriate

responses) have been found to predict weight gain in children as they move from childhood to

adolescence (Anzman & Birch, 2009), and lower self-regulation has been associated with

increased consumption and weight gain in children (Francis & Susman, 2009; Guxens et al.,

2009; Nederkoorn, Smulders, Havermans, Roefs, & Jansen, 2006; Riggs, Spruijt-Metz, Sakuma,

Chou, & Pentz, 2010). Despite a dearth of research in the area, preliminary results have shown

23

that individuals who exhibit increased levels of reward sensitivity (i.e., ability to derive pleasure

from natural reinforcers such as food) are more likely to overeat and be overweight during their

childhoods (Guerrieri, Nederkoorn, & Jansen, 2007; Nederkoorn, Braet, Van Eijs, Tanghe, &

Jansen, 2006). As such, in instances where children have predispositions towards lower levels of

inhibitory control and self-regulation, as well as higher levels of reward sensitivity, it is possible

that parents may choose to exert more control over their feeding in order to manage their

children’s weight.

Multidirectional Model

Ventura and Birch (2008) proposed a conceptual model (shown in Figure 1) for further

investigating the role of parenting feeding styles and practices on children’s eating behaviours

and weight status, suggesting a bidirectional relationship between these constructs (Ventura &

Birch, 2008).

Figure 1. A conceptual model for the influence of parenting (e.g., feeding styles and practices)

on children’s eating behaviour and dietary intake, as well as subsequent weight status, developed

by Ventura and Birch (2008, p.16).

In line with Ventura and Birch’s (2008) model, multiple studies have investigated the

three proposed pathways by using multivariate analyses to examine parental and child factors

associated with parental feeding control practices. The results of two longitudinal studies point

to a positive relationship between parental feeding control practices, children’s eating in the

24

absence of hunger, and children’s weight status (Birch et al., 2003; Francis & Birch, 2005).

Furthermore, it was found that the child’s eating in the absence of hunger mediated the

relationship between parental feeding control practices and the child’s weight status (Francis &

Birch, 2005). In addition, researchers who conducted a cross-sectional study found that the use

of parental feeding control practices predicted children’s weight status, and disinhibited eating

behaviour mediated this relationship (Joyce & Zimmer-Gembeck, 2009).

The interactional relationship between parenting, child eating behaviours, and child

weight status echoes the dynamics between parents and their children. It indicates that children’s

eating behaviours impact, and are impacted by, their parents’ use of feeding control practices

(Webber, Cooke, Hill, & Wardle, 2010). In addition, a child’s weight status and eating habits

may both separately influence their parents’ use of feeding control practices (Webber et al.,

2010).

Summary

Despite the growing amount of literature on parental feeding control practices, little is

known about the subjective experience of these feeding methods during childhood. Most of the

literature on this subject matter focuses on the behavioural impacts of, or parental motivations

for, employing feeding control practices. As a result, children’s subjective experience of these

feeding practices has remained largely ignored. By solely investigating the behavioural impacts

of parental feeding control practices, researchers are missing the feeling and thinking aspects of

experiencing parental feeding control practices. Without acknowledgement of these aspects, a

truly rich understanding of how parental feeding control practices impact children cannot be

attained. A qualitative examination of subjective experiences of parental feeding control would

25

allow for a deeper understanding of the psychological impacts of these feeding practices, and

would shed light on how children make meaning of their parents’ decision to control their diet.

Furthermore, the current research literature has not addressed the messages (e.g., beauty

ideals) that are being transmitted by parents, either implicitly or explicitly, about food, weight,

and the body through parental feeding control practices. In a study conducted by Shomaker and

Furman (2009), the impacts of parental messages about thinness on disordered eating in

adolescent girls were examined. Shomaker and Furman (2009) found that both adolescents’

reports of parental pressure to be thin and parents’ reports of pressuring daughters to be thin were

significantly associated with disordered eating. These results emphasize the influence of explicit

messages about food, weight, and the body within the parent-child relationship. Furthermore, in

addition to the communication of direct messages about food, weight, and the body, parents may

also be unintentionally communicating implicit messages regarding these concepts. These

implicit messages may be in agreement with, or completely oppose, the explicit parental

messages that are being communicated to the child. For example, a mother may explicitly

encourage her daughter to love herself the way she is, while implicitly sending her the message

that her body is not good enough through parental feeding control practices. An examination of

the messages, either direct or indirect, communicated through parental feeding control

interactions, has not been conducted as of yet.

Current Study

As most research on this topic has focused on the behavioural consequences of parental

feeding control practices, many psychological aspects of these experiences are unknown. As

such, the objective of the current study is to examine remembered childhood experiences of

parental feeding control practices from a qualitative perspective. The current study investigated

26

the following two research questions: How do young adults make meaning of their childhood

experiences of parental feeding control practices? and What are the perceived psychological

impacts of parental feeding control practices? The use of qualitative research methods was

purposefully chosen as it allowed the researcher to gain access to the intricacies of human

experiences (e.g., underlying meanings, and involved psychological processes). More

specifically, this research study was conducted from the epistemological position of social

constructivism, which suggests that reality is historically and culturally situated, and constructed

through all types of human interactions (Mills, Bonner, & Francis, 2006). Experiences of

parental feeding control practices occur in a culture that glorifies thinness and vilifies fatness;

and acknowledgment of this provided a context for understanding experiences of parental

feeding control practices.

Conclusion

In summary, there has been a considerable amount of research that has focused on the

behavioural impacts of parental feeding control practices on children. Specifically, researchers

have shown that these feeding practices may cause children to exhibit decreased ability in self-

regulating dietary intake (Birch & Fisher, 1995; Birch et al., 1987; Birch et al., 2001; Birch et al.,

2003; Carper et al., 2000; Cooke et al., 2011; Fisher & Birch, 2002; Johnson & Birch, 1994;

Monasta et al., 2010), develop a preference towards restricted foods (Campbell et al., 2006;

Thompson, 2010), as well as increase their risk for becoming overweight or obese (Birch et al.,

1987; Spruijt-Metz, 2011; Vos & Welsh, 2010). However, research examining the impacts of

parental feeding control practices on children’s psychological well-being, as well as research on

how individuals make meaning of these experiences, is particularly absent.

27

CHAPTER THREE

Methodology

This chapter offers an overview and description of the research methods employed to

answer the following two questions: How do young adults make meaning of their childhood

experiences of parental feeding control practices? and What are the perceived psychological

impacts of parental feeding control practices? This chapter begins with a description of

traditional and constructivist grounded theory methodologies, followed by a discussion of

constructivist grounded theory’s suitability for the present study. Then, procedures and ethical

considerations for the present study are described. To conclude, a discussion of the evaluation of

the constructivist grounded theory is provided.

Grounded Theory

Grounded theory (GT) is a qualitative research method used to systematically (i.e.,

through explicit strategies and techniques) develop theories that are ‘grounded’ in qualitative

data (Glaser & Strauss, 1967). GT begins with inductive qualitative data collection, and is

followed by the utilization of strategies and techniques that aid in the constant comparison and

analysis of data (Glaser & Strauss, 1967). GT involves abductive reasoning; as such,

preliminary interpretations of the qualitative data are considered, and further data is collected to

determine whether or not the hypothesized theory accurately explains the process (Charmaz,

2014). This procedure is repeated until the researcher feels that their interpretation correctly

explains the psychological processes involved in the studied experience. Ultimately, through GT

methods, the researcher is able to identify common patterns, processes, and meanings that exist

across a specific experience (e.g., childhood experiences of parental feeding control), and

construct a theory that captures the researched phenomenon (Tweed & Charmaz, 2012).

28

Traditional Grounded Theory Methodology

GT was introduced by two sociologists, Barney G. Glaser and Anselm L. Strauss, in the

mid 1960s with the publication of the book The Discovery of Grounded Theory (Tweed &

Charmaz, 2012). GT offered an innovative and rigorous inductive approach to qualitatively

studying various experiences (Tweed & Charmaz, 2012). This method was distinctively

disparate from the deductive, positivist, quantitative methods that were considered to be the

scientific research standard in the 19th

century (Charmaz, 2014). Glaser and Strauss (1967)

claimed that the quantitative research approach (i.e., testing hypotheses that are pulled from

previously established knowledge) resulted in theories that were disconnected from empirical

reality. Consequently, GT provided a research approach that could help researchers develop

theories directly from the data (Glasser & Strauss, 1967). The main features of traditional GT

are: theoretical sensitivity, theoretical sampling, the constant comparison method, coding, and

theoretical memos (McCann & Clark, 2003).

Theoretical sensitivity refers to the researcher’s ability to develop a theory as it “emerges

from the data” (Glaser & Strauss, 1967, p. 46). As such, grounded theorists do not impose their

preconceived notions or hypotheses onto the data; they discover the theory as it surfaces through

the process of data collection and analysis (Glaser & Strauss, 1967). Glaser and Strauss (1967)

argued that theoretical sensitivity develops and deepens through time and research practice (i.e.,

through gaining theoretical knowledge and personal experience with the research).

In GT, researchers employ two types of sampling, purposive and theoretical. Initially,

grounded theorists utilize purposive samples based on the study’s inclusion and exclusion

criteria. As the research process continues, researchers become more selective in their sampling

in order to advance theory development (McCann & Clark, 2003). Typically, theoretical

29

sampling takes place until saturation is met (i.e., no new data emerges; McCann & Clark, 2003;

Glaser & Strauss, 1967).

Through the constant comparison method, grounded theorists conduct data collection and

analysis simultaneously (McCann & Clark, 2000; Glaser & Strauss, 1967). Glaser and Strauss

(1967) describe four stages of the constant comparison method: comparing incidents related to

each category, exploring relationships between categories, defining the theory, and writing the

theory. Grounded theorists continue this process until a detailed theory is developed (McCann &

Clark, 2000).

Researchers who use traditional GT methods utilize two levels of coding: open and

selective (McCann & Clark, 2000a). Open coding occurs when the researcher breaks down the

data into discrete components and assigns codes (i.e., conceptual labels) to them (McCann &

Clark, 2000a). Grounded theorists use selective coding to define relationships between the

emerging categories (McCann & Clark, 2000a).

Lastly, grounded theorists employ theoretical memos to demonstrate, and make note of,

their analytical process. Throughout the research, memos are written by the researcher to

document ideas and observations, as well as to develop the theory (Glaser & Strauss, 1967;

McCann & Clark, 2000a).

The Evolution of Grounded Theory

Grounded theory has evolved tremendously since its first conceptualization (Charmaz,

2014). Traditional GT (Glaser & Strauss, 1967) adopted an objectivist and realist perspective

(Ghezelije, & Emami, 2009). Glasser and Strauss (1967) described grounded theorists as neutral

observers who witness the emergence of reality (i.e., an objective and observable truth).

However, in 1990, Strauss collaborated with Juliet Corbin to develop a more relativist or

30

subjectivist version of GT (Ghezelije, & Emami, 2009). During this time, many researchers,

including Strauss and Corbin, began embracing constructivist versions of GT. Although they

never explicitly address the constructivist epistemology, Strauss and Corbin adopted a relativist

perspective by recognizing the existence of multiple truths, acknowledging personal biases, and

understanding that theory is constructed through social interactions (Mills, Bonner, & Francis,

2006).

Constructivist Grounded Theory Methodology

Many researchers have tailored GT to suit an array of ontological and epistemological

stances. Kathy Charmaz, a former graduate student of both Glaser and Strauss, developed an

adaptation of GT called constructivist GT founded on the constructivist paradigm (Charmaz,

2006). In Charmaz’s (2006) constructivist version of GT, the researcher is viewed as a filter

through which data is collected and analyzed (Lichtman, 2013). As a result, the researcher’s past

experiences and biases inevitably influence the research process (Charmaz, 2014).

Constructivist grounded theorists recognize that data collection and analysis are impacted by the

researchers’ and their participants’ social locations and perspectives (Ghezelije, & Emami, 2009;

Tweed & Charmaz, 2012). Thus, constructivist grounded theorists argue that theory is not

discovered, but co-created in an interactive process between the researcher, participants, and the

data (Henwood & Pidgeon, 2003).

Constructivist grounded theorists assume a relativist ontological position and a

subjectivist epistemological stance, in which it is believed that multiple realities exist as reality is

understood to be constructed through social interactions between individuals with differing

perspectives (Mills et al., 2006). Contrary to traditional GT’s focus on a discoverable objective

reality, constructivist GT asserts that knowledge is co-created through social interactions

31

between researchers and their participants (Ghezelije & Emami, 2009). Therefore, constructivist

grounded theorists believe that theories are subjectively co-constructed versus objectively

discovered (Charmaz, 2014). As such, theories are personal (i.e., from a particular perspective)

interpretations of data (Mills et al., 2006), rather than exact replications of reality (Andrews,

2012).

Traditional grounded theorists have critiqued other qualitative approaches (including

constructivist GT) by arguing that qualitative researchers frequently mold their data to fit their

preconceived ideas (Glaser, 1992). However, constructivist grounded theorists believe in the co-

construction of knowledge between researchers and their participants, and emphasize that theory

needs to be rooted in the data (Mills et al., 2006). Moreover, constructivist grounded theorists

take a reflexive position towards their analysis of data, experiences, decisions, and

conceptualizations (Charmaz, 2006). In this way, they are reflexive in evaluating how their

interests, social positions, biases, and assumptions impact their research.

Finally, Charmaz (2006) called her approach constructivist GT to state her belief that

theory is developed through social interactions between researchers and their participants.

Charmaz (2014) argues that constructivism is in line with contemporary definitions of social

constructionism. Social constructionism can be described as the belief that reality is constructed

within a historical, social, and cultural context (Charmaz, 2006; Mischler, 1979; Raskin, 2002;

Young & Collin, 2004). As Charmaz (2014) understood constructivism and social

constructionism as encompassing similar notions and meanings, the term constructivism will be

utilized throughout the rest of this paper.

Rationale for Constructivist GT Methodology

In this study, I was interested in exploring the meaning making and psychological

32

impacts of experiencing parental feeding control practices during one’s childhood. Such a

research question seeks to understand subjective meaning, rather than explain or predict

objective phenomena and, therefore, is more aligned with qualitative research methods

(Charmaz, 2006). Qualitative research endeavours to a) reveal how individuals experience a

specific phenomenon, b) gain a better understanding of a phenomenon about which little is

known, and c) provide researchers with the opportunity to gain information about a phenomenon

that would be challenging to obtain via quantitative methods (Strauss & Corbin, 1990). These

three goals are consistent with the aims of the current study and, thus, qualitative methods were

employed.

Of the many qualitative research approaches, constructivist GT was chosen as the most

appropriate methodology for this study as this method allows the researcher to investigate new

areas and over time, develop original theory (Glaser & Strauss, 1967). Little is known about the

child’s subjective experience of parental feeding control practices. As such, GT aligns with this

study as it offers the researcher tools to develop a preliminary theoretical conceptualization of

the processes involved in these experiences (Glaser & Strauss, 1967). Furthermore, GT methods

enable the researcher to not only describe specific experiences, but also create abstract

theoretical hypotheses about the processes involved in the studied phenomenon (Glaser &

Strauss, 1967). Overall, GT methods offer strategies and techniques to identify patterns and

processes that are common within a studied phenomenon (e.g., childhood experiences of parental

feeding control practices; Glaser & Strauss, 1967).

Furthermore, as this study aimed to examine the meaning making and psychological

processes found in descriptions of childhood experiences of parental feeding control, the

constructivist version of GT was found to be most aligned with the current study’s objectives.

33

Past studies on parental feeding control have not adequately considered the numerous social

factors involved in these practices. Social relationships (e.g., family) are central to childhood

experiences of parental feeding control and, according to Charmaz (2006), individuals make

sense of their world through social relationships and interactions. As such, it is important to

consider the social influences involved in childhood experiences of parental feeding control.

With this being said, constructivist GT offers strategies and techniques that help the researcher

explore participants’ social experiences of a specific phenomenon, as well as co-construct the

meanings they attach to it (Tweed & Charmaz, 2012). Altogether, constructivist GT methods

lend themselves well to the aims of the current study.

Finally, it is recognized that the aim of GT is to develop theory that is grounded within

the data; however, GT methods often result in preliminary conceptualizations instead of formal

theories (Charmaz, 2008; Charmaz, 2014; Lichtman, 2013). As this study was conducted for a

Master’s thesis, the current constructivist GT study offers a preliminary conceptualization of the

processes involved in childhood experiences of parental feeding control. Based on the

previously mentioned arguments, constructivist GT was deemed a valuable and appropriate

method for this study as it allows the researcher to identify patterns and processes that are

common to a specific experience through the co-construction of meaning with their participants.

Procedures

Participant Recruitment

Recruitment for this study was conducted through poster advertisements. Posters

promoting the research study (see Appendix A) were displayed around the University of Calgary

campus. Posters included a brief description of the study, participation criteria, and my contact

information in case of interest.

34

Upon contacting me, interested individuals underwent a screening interview (see

Appendix B) to ensure that they met participation criteria for the current study. Inclusion and

exclusion criteria were as follows:

Participants must be between 18-29 years of age. This age group was chosen based on

their ability to reflect upon their childhood experiences of parental feeding control.

Participants must self-identify as individuals who have experienced parental feeding

control practices during their childhood.

Participants must have experienced parental feeding control practices for the purpose of

weight loss promotion or weight gain prevention.

Participants must not have memory impairment as this could interfere with their ability

to recall their childhood experiences.

Participants must not have impaired perceptions (e.g., psychosis), as impaired perceptions

could affect understanding of childhood events.

Participants must not have experienced feeding control practices as a result of abuse or

neglect. The purpose of this study is to examine parental feeding practices that were

purposeful and implemented with good intention (i.e., even though food was available it

was restricted).

Six individuals met the participation criteria, and shared their childhood experiences of parental

feeding control practices during an individual face-to-face interview with me. The interviews

took place in private rooms located in the Taylor Family Digital Library on the University of

Calgary campus. The interviews took 45 minutes on average to conduct.

Data Collection and Analysis

35

I followed constructivist GT data collection and analysis procedures (Charmaz, 2014).

As such, data collection and analysis took place concurrently which allowed me to develop a

deeper understanding of the studied experience (Charmaz, 2006). In order to be transparent

about the research process, strategies and procedures for data collection and analysis are outlined

below.

Data was collected over a period of 3 months (from January to March of 2017).

Informed consent forms (see Appendix C) were sent to participants via e-mail prior to the

interview process for their review. Additionally, I reviewed informed consent with participants

at the beginning of the face-to-face interview, and answered questions, as necessary. Once

informed consent was obtained, participants filled out personal demographic information (see

Appendix D) including gender, age, highest level of education attained, income status, and ethnic

background, as well as participants’ perception of parental demographic information which

included the parental household income, and the mother and father’s ethnic background. Lastly,

participants were asked to report which parent(s) was responsible for controlling their food

intake.

Interviews. This study aimed to explore how young adults describe their childhood

experiences of parental feeding control practices. Thus, it was necessary to implement an open

and exploratory approach to interviewing that allowed for psychological processes to be explored

(Strauss & Corbin, 1990). Consequently, a semi-structured interview approach, outlined by

Charmaz (2014), was used. In line with GT, interview questions were open-ended (see

Appendix E) which allowed participants to comment on the experiences and topics that were

most significant to them.

I transcribed all of the interviews verbatim (this included utterances and non-verbal

36

language) to ensure accuracy of analysis, and to enable increased contact with the data. This

increased contact allowed me to become immersed in the language and meanings of the

participants (Charmaz, 2006), as well as generate further research ideas (Charmaz, 2006). I

utilized these ideas to later develop more direct interview questions to obtain a more

comprehensive understanding of childhood experiences of parental feeding control.

GT Methods and Data Analysis

There are various essential strategies and procedures that constructivist GT studies

employ. These techniques include questioning, theoretical sampling, the constant comparison

method, memo writing and reflexivity, and theoretical sensitivity.

Questioning. In constructivist GT, semi-structured interviews are often utilized to

collect qualitative data for analysis (Charmaz, 2014). During these semi-structured interviews,

constructivist grounded theorists must strike a balance between the use of non-directive

interview strategies (e.g., asking a small number of open-ended questions in order to grasp the

participant’s experience), with directive interview methods (e.g., asking many focused questions

to solidify or dispute conceptualizations; Charmaz, 2014). In order to attain this balance,

researchers can begin the research process by asking general open-ended questions, and adding

more focused questions later on (Charmaz, 2014).

For this study, an interview guide was developed. This guide included broad open-ended

questions, as well as possible follow-up queries. Later in the data collection process, more

purposeful open-ended interview questions were added to the guide in order to help me explore

particular theoretical hunches. Moreover, in order to mitigate the potential risk of influencing

the data to confirm preconceived ideas, I kept questions open-ended, used the interviewees’

language, followed the interviewees’ lead, and asked the interviewees to define their taken-for-

37

granted meanings (Charmaz, 2014). With this being said, the interview guide was not always

followed, and the language used during interviews changed depending on the participant’s

preferred terms.

Theoretical sampling. Theoretical sampling is used to obtain new data in order to

further develop the emerging GT (Charmaz, 2014). This technique is employed after

preliminary categories have been conceptualized (Charmaz, 2008, 2014). In order to carry out

theoretical sampling, the researchers must ask: “Who do I need to interview to further develop

this category?” In general, theoretical sampling takes place until saturation is reached (i.e.,

where no new developments arise; Charmaz, 2008). In this study, theoretical sampling began

after many categories had been developed. During the theoretical sampling portion of data

collection, interview questions were more purposeful and related to the categories that had

previously emerged through data analysis.

Constant comparison method. Through the constant comparison method, GT data

analysis occurs at the same time as data collection (Charmaz, 2008; Tweed & Charmaz, 2012).

By using the constant comparison method, constructivist grounded theorists become more apt to

notice similarities, differences, and relationships that are grounded within the data (Boeije, 2002;

Charmaz, 2006; Hutchinson & Wilson, 2001). Ultimately, this method aids in the development

of conceptual and theoretical categories (Boeije, 2002; Charmaz, 2008).

In this study, I collected most of the data through semi-structured interviews. In addition

to interview transcripts, interesting thoughts and observations that took place during the data

collection process were also noted (i.e., memos) and analyzed (Charmaz, 2014). Interview

transcript and memo data were coded and organized into categories through the use of GT’s

constant comparison method (Charmaz, 2006).

38

Coding. Coding refers to describing or summarizing meaningful segments of the data

(Charmaz, 2006; Miles & Huberman, 1994). Through coding, constructivist grounded theorists

focus on the actions participants describe, and on the meanings they assign to those actions

(Charmaz, 2008). In this study, various coding phases were utilized. The initial coding

techniques used in this study were line-by-line and in-vivo coding (Charmaz, 2006). In the

second coding phase, focused coding took place (Charmaz, 2006). Finally, the third coding

phase consisted of theoretical coding (Charmaz, 2006).

During the initial coding process, I created “active codes” (i.e., reflect the action

described) for each sentence (i.e., line-by-line coding) of the interview transcripts and memos.

Line-by-line coding enabled me to remain immersed in the data, while also taking an analytic

position towards it (Charmaz, 2008). Using gerunds (i.e., the noun form of a verb) to code the

data preserved action by indicating what individuals were doing in the studied data segment

(Tweed & Charmaz, 2012). This technique helped me to make implicit processes and meanings

more explicit (Tweed & Charmaz, 2012). Therefore, line-by-line coding with gerunds allowed

me to remain close to the data (Charmaz, 2014; Tweed & Charmaz, 2012), while staying focused

on developing meaningful categories (Charmaz, 2006).

Furthermore, during the initial coding phase, I engaged in in-vivo coding. In-vivo codes

are direct quotes used by participants during the interview process that convey a taken-for-

granted meaning (Charmaz, 2014). These codes help to give meaning to the data, and inhibit

researchers from imposing their personal ideas on the codes (McCann & Clark, 2000a). In this

study, I made every effort to ask questions about participants’ taken-for granted meanings, and

made sure to include in-vivo data in the report to accurately reflect participants’ experiences.

Once line-by-line and in-vivo coding were complete, I began focused coding, where the

39

codes are more conceptual (Charmaz, 2006). During this coding phase, I used the most

significant initial codes and conceptualized them into focused codes in order to organize the data

from each interview in a meaningful manner (Charmaz, 2006, 2008). Through this method,

preliminary categories were developed. Focused coding further encouraged me to define the

relationships between emerging categories, and prompted additional insight and questions that

led to the refinement and elaboration of the emerging GT (Charmaz, 2006).

Lastly, I engaged in theoretical coding where links between categories were made.

Through theoretical coding, I was able to organize the substantive categories, and make

hypotheses about how they relate to one another (Charmaz, 2014). Altogether, theoretical

coding facilitated the development of an initial conceptualization of how young adults describe

their childhood experiences of parental feeding control practices (Charmaz, 2006, 2014).

Memo writing and reflexivity. Memo writing allows researchers to make note of their

thoughts, hypotheses, conceptualizations, and decision-making during the research process

(Charmaz, 2006). Charmaz (2008) argued that category development is unavoidably impacted

by the researcher’s personal background and biases. Consequently, reflexivity becomes a

necessary practice throughout the progression of research in constructivist GT. Reflexivity

refers to the researcher’s ability to recognize and evaluate how their past experiences influence

their research related decision making process (Charmaz, 2014; Hutchinson & Wilson, 2001). In

constructivist GT, this recognition of personal biases and their impact on the research, as well as

the decision making process, is documented through memo writing (Hutchinson & Wilson,

2001).

In this study, memo writing took place throughout the entire research process (Tweed &

Charmaz, 2012). Memos were written for three main reasons: to aid in my analysis of the data,

40

to keep a research audit trail, and to demonstrate reflexivity (Charmaz, 2014; Miles and

Huberman, 1994). Thus, I developed codes and categories while also conceptualizing their

relationships through the process of memo writing (Hutchinson & Wilson, 2001). Furthermore, I

presented evidence (i.e., quotes) from the raw interview data to support category definitions and

relationships in the memos (Charmaz, 2008). Lastly, in order to demonstrate my process of self-

evaluation, personal biases were recognized and assessed through memo writing (Charmaz,

2008, 2014).

Theoretical sensitivity. Theoretical sensitivity refers to the researcher’s ability to build

a theory as it emerges from the qualitative data (Charmaz, 2014). Theoretical sensitivity

involves going to the root of the studied phenomenon and defining the processes involved

without undue influence from other established theories (Charmaz, 2014). In this study, I aimed

for theoretical sensitivity by conducting a preliminary literature review to become aware of

sensitising concepts, and by being open to unanticipated results throughout the research process

(Charmaz, 2014; Glasser & Strauss, 1967).

Ethical Considerations

The University of Calgary Conjoint Faculties Research Ethics Board approved this study.

Participants received a $20 gift card to Cadillac Fairview malls to thank them for their

participation in the study. Given that the incentive to participate was less than $50, it is believed

that the payment encouraged individuals to engage in the research, but did not cause undue

influence or manipulation. Furthermore, it was recognized that recalling childhood experiences

of parental feeding control could be distressing for some participants. As such, a distress

protocol was developed and outlined in the consent form (see Appendix C).

41

Many steps were taken to guarantee the ethical and confidential storage of data. To

begin, the project supervisor and I had access to the confidential data. The electronic data files

(e.g., audio recordings and transcripts) were stored on password protected computers in

encrypted files that only myself and the project supervisor had access to. Additionally, during

the informed consent process, each participant selected a pseudonym to which their information

(i.e., demographic information and interview transcripts) was ascribed. In this way, any

accessible data provided by this individual was only associated with the pseudonym provided.

Lastly, consent forms were stored in a locked cabinet separate from participant demographic

forms.

Evaluating Grounded Theory

Methods for evaluating qualitative research continue to be highly debated (Corbin &

Strauss, 2008). However, Charmaz (2014) developed four substantive criteria for assessing GT

studies; these include credibility, originality, resonance, and usefulness. These four criteria

require the researcher to engage in self-evaluation throughout the research process (Corbin &

Strauss, 2008). The present study’s emerging GT was evaluated through its ability to answer the

following questions provided by Charmaz (2014, p. 337-338):

1. Credibility: Has your research achieved intimate familiarity with the topic? Are the

data sufficient to merit your claims, considering range, depth, and number of

observations in the data? Do the categories cover a wide range of empirical

observations? Has your research provided enough evidence for your claims to allow

the reader to form an independent assessment and agree with your claims?

2. Originality: Do your categories offer new insights? Does your analysis provide a new

conceptual interpretation of the data? How does your GT challenge, extend, or refine

42

current ideas and concepts?

3. Resonance: Do the categories portray the fullness of the studied experience? Have

you revealed taken-for-granted meanings? Does your GT make sense to your

participants or people who share their circumstances?

4. Usefulness: Does your analysis offer interpretations that people can use in their

everyday worlds? Can the analysis spark further research in other substantive areas?

How does your work contribute to knowledge? How does it contribute to making a

better world?

Strategies for Evaluation

Charmaz’s (2014) criteria and questions guide constructivist grounded theorists to

evaluate their emerging GT during the research process. However, other strategies may also be

useful in evaluating qualitative research. For example, Creswell (2007) outlined eight validation

techniques for evaluating qualitative research, and suggested that qualitative researchers use at

least two of these strategies to assess research credibility. The following two techniques from

Creswell’s (2007) list were employed to attend to trustworthiness.

Thick descriptions. Thick descriptions of research participants allow readers to

determine the transferability of the study’s findings (Charmaz, 2014; Creswell, 2007). As such,

participant demographic information has been thoroughly described in the results section.

Furthermore, a researcher can also gain thick descriptions when they endeavour to make implicit

meanings more explicit (Charmaz, 2008). In this study, I asked participants to elaborate on their

wording and definitions in order to accurately understand their taken-for-granted meanings.

Triangulation. Triangulation involves using different sources (e.g., consultation with an

expert in the field and revisiting the research literature) to provide corroborating evidence for

43

category formation and theory development (Creswell, 2007). By substantiating results through

triangulation, the researcher adds to the trustworthiness of the data analysis (Creswell, 2007;

Hutchinson & Wilson, 2001). In order to enhance the credibility of the current study’s results, I

consulted with my project supervisor and doctoral candidate mentor regarding the process of

category and theory development, as well as conducted a second literature review in order to

explain and find further evidence for my research findings.

Chapter Summary

A constructivist GT methodology was utilized to answer the present study’s research

questions: How do young adults make meaning of their childhood experiences of parental

feeding control practices? and What are the perceived psychological impacts of parental feeding

control practices? Constructivist GT consists of strategies used to construct a theory from

qualitative data (Tweed & Charmaz, 2012). Constructivist grounded theorists assume that

multiple realities exist, and that realities are co-constructed through social interactions between

individuals who embody different perspectives (Mills et al., 2006). Consequently, researchers

who employ this method acknowledge how researchers’ and their participants’ backgrounds and

biases influence the data and its analysis (Ghezelije, & Emami, 2009; Tweed & Charmaz, 2012).

Thus, a constructivist GT emerges from the data, and is co-constructed between researchers and

their participants (Charmaz, 2014). Constructivist GT methods fit the aim of this study, and

guided the process of data collection and analysis.

44

CHAPTER FOUR

Results

This chapter offers a description of the results of data analysis and the generation of a

preliminary constructivist grounded theory (GT). The objective of this study was to describe

meaning making and psychological impacts of experiencing parental feeding control practices,

intended to promote weight loss or to prevent weight gain (i.e., dietary restriction, monitoring,

and pressure), during one’s childhood. As a formal GT is typically developed over many years,

and with numerous research participants, I recognize that the current analysis represents a

preliminary theoretical conceptualization of the meaning making and psychological processes

associated with childhood experiences of parental feeding control. Data collection was achieved

through semi-structured interviews, and the constructivist interpretation of GT methods was used

to code and analyze the transcribed interviews. Results of the data analysis process formed the

preliminary constructivist GT.

Participants

Eighteen individuals contacted me to participate in the study upon viewing the

recruitment poster. I responded to all of the potential participants with an initial e-mail. Twelve

individuals followed up with a phone call to undergo the screening interview (see Appendix B).

Nine individuals met inclusion criteria, based on the recruitment screening questions, and

interviews were scheduled. In GT, data collection and analysis occurs concurrently (Charmaz,

2006); therefore, one or two interviews were booked at a time, and I scheduled them one to two

weeks apart to allow for concurrent data analysis. One participant did not show up for the

interview, and two participants were not included in the analysis as it became apparent, during

the interview, that they did not meet participation criteria (i.e., one participant had not

45

experienced parental feeding control practices for the purpose of weight loss promotion or

weight gain prevention, and the other participant did not experience parental feeding control,

instead he was taught to eat “unhealthy” foods in moderation). As such, six participant

interviews were transcribed and analyzed. The core meaning making and psychological

processes had emerged by interview four. Saturation of these categories became apparent after

interview five, and I completed one additional interview to confirm saturation for a total of six

participants.

All of the participants were females between the ages of 19 and 29. Participants had all

attained a minimum of a high school diploma. Furthermore, participants’ ethnic backgrounds

were reported as follows; two participants indicated their ethnic origin as Asian, one participant

had a Middle-Eastern background, another reported being French-Canadian, a different

participant stated having a European background, while my last participant described herself as

Latin-Canadian. Five of the participants reported their income status at under $20,000, while

one participant stated earning between 80,000$ and 100,000$ a year. A summary of

demographic information for each participant is listed in Table 1.

Table 1. Participant Demographic Information

Pseudonym/Name Gender Age Education Income

Status

Ethnic

Background

Sarah F 28 Bachelor’s

Degree

Under

$20,000 European

Atoosa F 29 Master’s

Degree

Under

$20,000

Middle-

Eastern

May F 19 High School

Diploma

Under

$20,000 Asian

Rebecca F 24 Bachelor’s

Degree

Under

$20,000 Asian

46

Jackie F 27 Bachelor’s

Degree

$80,000 to

just under

$100,000

French-

Canadian

Emma F 28 Bachelor’s

Degree

Under

$20,000 Latin

Participants’ perceived parental demographic information was also collected.

Participants’ parents had educational backgrounds raging from high school diplomas to doctoral

degrees. Parental income statuses spanned from $40,000 to $150,000 and above. Moreover, all

of the participants indicated that their mothers were responsible for the dietary control; however,

three participants reported that their fathers also participated in controlling their dietary intake.

A summary of participants’ parental demographic information is listed in Table 2.

Table 2. Participants’ Parental Demographic Information

Pseudonym

/Name

Mother’s

Education

Father’s

Education

Mother’s

Ethnic

Background

Father’s

Ethnic

Background

Parental

Income

Status

Responsible

for Feeding

Sarah Bachelor’s

Degree

Bachelor’s

Degree European European

$100,000 to

just under

$150,000

Mother

Atoosa Diploma Master’s

Degree

Middle

Eastern

Middle

Eastern

$40,000 to

just under

$60,000

Mother

May Doctoral

Degree

Bachelor’s

Degree Asian Asian

$150,000

and above

Mother and

Father

Rebecca College

Degree

College

Degree Asian Asian

$80,000 to

just under

$100,000

Mother and

Father

Jackie Bachelor’s

Degree

High

School

Diploma

French-

Canadian European

$100,000 to

just under

$150,000

Mother and

Father

47

Emma College

Degree

Master’s

Degree Latin Latin

$150,000

and above Mother

Development of Themes and Categories

Theory development in constructivist GT is emergent and constructed (Charmaz, 2006).

It involves abductive reasoning in which the researcher studies the data, develops hypotheses

about the processes that are emerging from the data, and then verifies them empirically by re-

examining the data (Charmaz, 2006).

Constructivist GT Data Analysis Procedures Revisited

Data was collected over a three month period. The interviews were guided by pre-

established semi-structured interview questions (see Appendix E). Throughout the interviews, I

asked follow-up questions and clarifying probes to ensure that I was reasonably interpreting and

understanding participants’ answers. The interview questions were not always asked in the same

order and some probes were omitted in order to reduce redundancy. During the interviews, I

asked broad questions such as, “In what ways, if any, have you been psychologically impacted

by being put on a diet by your parents as a child?” Asking such questions enabled me to gain a

deeper understanding of participants’ experiences of parental feeding control practices and

whether those experiences were consistent with emerging core concepts, themes, and sub-

themes. Furthermore, later in the data collection process, I asked participants more focused

questions in order to develop and refine core concepts, themes, and sub-themes that had emerged

in previous interviews. For example, participants were asked if a previous theme, such as fearing

weight gain, fit within their experience of parental feeding control practices, and if so, how it fit.

Thus, later data collection was impacted by earlier data collection and analysis.

48

Altogether, five preliminary core concepts, and their encompassing themes and sub-

themes, were co-constructed through the process of data collection and analysis. I developed

several different versions of the theoretical model to attempt to conceptualize how the core

concepts, themes, and sub-themes were connected. I engaged in memoing throughout data

collection and analysis (Appendix F provides examples of memos written during the data

collection and analysis). Once it appeared that no new categories were emerging from the data, I

interviewed two more participants to assess whether saturation had been met. When the data fit

within the previously established core concepts, themes, and sub-themes it was deemed that

saturation had been achieved and data collection concluded.

The Constructivist Grounded Theory

The results of this study are presented as a preliminary theoretical model aimed at

capturing the meaning making and psychological processes associated with childhood

experiences of parental feeding control. Core concepts, themes, and sub-themes that form the

constructivist GT emerged through a process of coding, constant comparative analysis, and

theorizing. Participant quotes are used throughout the results section to exemplify the core

concept, themes, and sub-themes, as well as their properties. Participant quotes have been edited

to increase readability (e.g., removing “like”); however, the substantive meanings of the quotes

were not changed. When direct quotes from participants are used, participants’ chosen

pseudonyms are shared. In the general discussion of the constructivist GT, participants are

referred to in the third person as the GT is proposed to represent the participants as a whole.

Identified Core Concepts, Themes, and Sub-Themes

Data analysis resulted in the identification of five core concepts, these included: (a)

making meaning of parental feeding control, (b) experiencing psychological impacts, (c)

49

experiencing behavioural impacts, (d) developing a lasting preoccupation with weight, and (e)

viewing parental feeding control as unhelpful. The first core concept, making meaning of

parental feeding control, is comprised of three themes, these are: perceiving parental motivation

for feeding control, internalizing parental messages about weight, and viewing self-worth is

contingent on weight. Experiencing psychological impacts, the second core concept, also

encompasses three themes: becoming self-conscious about weight and size, feeling guilt about

eating, and fearing weight gain. The third core concept, experiencing behavioural impacts,

includes one theme: engaging in maladaptive eating behaviours. Developing a long-lasting

preoccupation with weight was recognized as the forth core concept, and is not comprised of any

themes. The last core concept, seeing parental feeding control as unhelpful, is made up of one

theme: offering advice. Table 3 offers a representation of the identified core concepts, themes,

and sub-themes associated with childhood experiences of parental feeding control.

Table 3. A Constructivist Grounded Theory of Meaning Making and Psychological Impacts of

Parental Feeding Control

Core Concepts Themes Sub-Themes

Making meaning of parental

feeding control

Perceiving parental motivation

for feeding control

Appearance and child’s future

success

Weight stigma and bullying

Health

Internalizing parental

messages about weight

Viewing self-worth as

contingent on weight

Experiencing psychological

impacts

Becoming self-conscious

about weight and size

Feeling guilt about eating

Fearing weight gain

50

Experiencing behavioural

impacts

Engaging in maladaptive

eating behaviours

Restricting

Bingeing and engaging

compensatory eating

behaviours

Developing a long-lasting

preoccupation with weight

Seeing parental feeding

control as unhelpful

Offering advice

Identified Dynamic Processes – The Theoretical Model

Important dynamics among the core concepts were also identified through the process of

data collection and analysis. In constructivist GT, the aim is not to develop an objectivist theory

(i.e., void of subjectivity), instead constructivist grounded theorists endeavour to create an

interpretive theory (Charmaz, 2014; Strauss & Corbin, 1998). Charmaz (2014) describes

interpretive theory in the following manner: “A constructivist approach theorizes the interpretive

work that research participants do, but also acknowledges that the resulting theory is an

interpretation. The theory depends on the researcher’s view; it does not and cannot stand outside

of it” (p. 239). Thus, based on this study’s co-constructed data, as well as my own and

participants’ interpretations of the relationships between core concepts, I developed a

preliminary and interpretive theory of the meaning making and psychological impacts of

experiencing parental feeding control during one’s childhood.

To begin, experiencing parental feeding control practices during one’s childhood impacts

how individuals view themselves and the world (i.e., meaning making), as well as negatively

effects their psychological well-being. Next, as a result of experiencing parental feeding control

during one’s childhood, in addition to undergoing the meaning making and psychological effects

51

of those practices, individuals experience behavioural impacts (i.e., engaging in maladaptive

eating behaviours). Overall, the meaning making, psychological, and behavioural impacts of

experiencing parental feeding control practices during one’s childhood have a lasting effect on

individuals’ psychological well-being and eating behaviours in the form of persistent weight

preoccupation. Altogether, due to its negative meaning making, psychological, and behavioural

impacts, parental feeding control practices were seen as unhelpful. The following figure further

illustrates the dynamics between the recognized core concepts.

Figure 2. The Constructivist Grounded Theory of Meaning Making and Psychological Impacts

of Parental Feeding Control

In order to better understand the core concepts, themes, and sub-themes involved in

childhood experiences of parental feeding control, as well as how they relate to one another, the

following sections describe and exemplify the differing components of the current study’s

preliminary constructivist GT.

52

Making Meaning of Parental Feeding Control

The first identified core concept was making meaning of parental feeding control.

Participants described three themes under this core concept, these were: (a) perceiving parental

motivation for feeding control, (b) internalizing parental messages about weight, and (c) viewing

self-worth as contingent on weight.

To begin, participants reported receiving parental messages, either implicitly or

explicitly, about weight based on their perception of their parents’ motivation for controlling

their dietary intake. For example, if a participant’s parents employed feeding control practices

for appearance reasons, they received the implicit message that being overweight is unattractive.

Moreover, as a result of receiving these implicit or explicit messages about weight, participants

reported internalizing and adopting these messages as their own way of conceptualizing

themselves and the world. In line with this internalization of negative parental messages about

weight, participants began viewing being overweight as less attractive, desirable, as well as less

worthy of love and attention. Subsequently, participants indicated that they started viewing their

own self-worth as being conditionally dependent on their weight. For example, if they gained

weight, they felt less worthy of love. The following sections illustrate in greater detail the

meaning making process associated with experiencing parental feeding control practices during

one’s childhood.

Perceiving parental motivation for feeding control. Participants described perceiving

a variety of parental motivations for utilizing feeding control practices, these sub-themes

included: (a) appearance and child’s future success, (b) weight stigma and bullying, and (c)

health motivations. Whether the parental feeding control practices were utilized for appearance

53

or health reasons, participants received implicit or explicit messages about weight based on their

perceptions of their parents’ motivation for controlling their dietary intake.

Appearance and child’s future success. Many participants indicated perceiving

appearance as the motivating factor for their parents’ use of feeding control practices, and that

appearance was related to future success. For example, Emma recounted her perception of her

mother’s motivation for utilizing feeding control practices:

I think every mom wants their kid to be good looking or successful, right? In Argentina,

it’s a culture thing. Girls are meant to be perfect and have the perfect body and so a lot of

it [using parental feeding control practices] I think was coming from there.

Emma continued to describe the culture in which she was raised, and how it could have impacted

her mother’s motivation for using feeding control practices:

Back there [Argentina], if you are skinny or if you are pretty, you’re going to be

successful. For example, when you apply for a job, you have to send a picture. That’s

how things go. There's a lot of pressure on that front so I think she was trying to kind of

steer me that way. It’s like if you’re skinny, you’re going to be more successful. I think

that was her reasoning behind it.

May described the explicit parental messages about weight that revealed their appearance and

future success motivations for utilizing feeding control practices:

They [my parents] were like, ‘you need to lose weight and you need to be pretty’ and

‘this is for you [and your future success].’

Rebecca recalled perceiving her father’s appearance motivations for utilizing feeding control

practices:

54

He would always emphasize the health part but I think for him it was probably about

appearance as well. He would always talk about being healthy, but he would also add:

‘when you're older and people don't find you attractive or beautiful, don’t blame me.’

By perceiving their parents’ appearance and future success motivations for using feeding control

practices, participants received the following messages: being overweight makes you (a) less

attractive, and (b) less likely to be successful in the future.

Weight stigma and bullying. Some participants viewed their parents’ motivation for

using feeding control practices as related to fears about their child being stigmatized or bullied.

Rebecca exemplified this perception in the following quote:

I think my dad was just worried about how society would perceive me [if I was

overweight] and whether or not I would be able to fit in, and that people would bully me

or pick on me.

Jackie explained her perception of her dad’s bullying related motivation for using feeding control

practices:

My dad was like, ‘maybe she’ll feel better if she doesn’t get teased’ because I was

bullied a little bit for my weight.

In these cases, participants received the message that being overweight will result in weight-

related stigmatization and/or bullying.

Health. Health was also perceived to be a motivator for the use of parental feeding

control practices by a few participants. Participants reported that their parents became concerned

about their weight due to a doctor or health professional’s feedback. Jackie recalled her parents

being motivated to change her diet based on a doctor’s recommendation:

55

Based on my understanding of it [parents’ motivation for using feeding control practices],

the Dr. was concerned about where my weight was, so they [my parents] basically took

that on as needing to do something about it.

Atoosa experienced parental feeding control practices due to feedback her parents received from

a physical education teacher. She recounted her experience in the following quote:

I remember it was part of the exercise course [at school]. We went to a gym and they

weighed all the children in front of each other and it was really embarrassing. They told

me that they needed to talk to my mom and they asked me for her phone number. They

consulted with my mom and told her to put me on a diet.

Perceiving health motivations for using parental feeding control practices indicated to

participants that being overweight is unhealthy.

Altogether, participants described that their parents were implicitly or explicitly

expressing particular messages about weight depending on their motivation for controlling their

dietary intake. These messages included: being overweight (a) is unattractive and will negatively

impact future success, (b) will lead to being stigmatized and bullied, and (c) is unhealthy. In

sum, through parental feeding control practices, parents were reinforcing the message that being

overweight is bad.

Internalizing parental messages about weight. As a consequence of receiving implicit

or explicit parental messages about weight, participants recounted internalizing and adopting

these weight-related messages as their own way of conceptualizing themselves and the world.

Therefore, participants began viewing being overweight as less attractive, desirable, and worthy

of love and attention. Collectively, these internalized parental messages about weight began to

skew how participants’ viewed themselves, others, and the world. For example, Emma

56

described how she internalized her mother’s concern for appearance through her explicit

messages about weight and eating:

I think it all started with the comments [about my weight and eating habits] but

eventually I started internalizing it and making them part of how I see myself and how

much importance I put on appearance because of my mom...I feel like now, in a way,

even my own values are affected by the way I was brought up.

May described internalizing appearance expectations from parental messages about weight:

When I was young I wanted to diet because of their [parents’] expectations, and now, I

want to diet because of my expectations.

May further reported internalizing parental messages about her body, weight, and eating due to

parental feeding control practices:

They [my parents] made me realize that there’s always a problem [with my body, weight,

and eating]. Their constant pressure, for years and years, shaped the way I see things [my

body, weight, and eating] too.”

Emma stated internalizing explicit parental messages about size:

Sometimes, I still hear my mother's voice [in my head] saying, ‘your legs are fat.’

She continued to explain that despite experiencing the negative effects of parental feeding

control practices, she would still be compelled to make comments about her child’s weight. The

following example shows how entrenched the internalization of parental messages about weight

can become:

As much as I wouldn’t like to admit it, and as much as I would like to say that if I had a

kid and they were gaining weight, I would just let them. If I have to be honest with

myself, I probably wouldn’t. I probably would say something, which is bad because I

57

went through that [experiencing parental feeding control practices] and I’m telling you

about all the trouble it caused.

These internalized parental messages about weight came together to form participants’ way of

viewing themselves and the world. Again, through the internalization of negative parental

messages about weight, participants started to adopt the view that being overweight is bad.

Consequently, participants described believing that their self-worth was dependent on their

weight.

Viewing self-worth as contingent on weight. Due to the internalization of negative

parental messages about weight, participants described seeing their worth as contingent on their

weight. They explained that they often felt, and continue to feel, less worthy, less attractive, and

less desirable when they were, or are, overweight. For example, Atoosa described how being

overweight affected, and continues to affect, her self-worth, confidence, and her ability to

socialize:

Whenever I gained weight, I became less confident, shy, and anxious. Even now. This is

my second year in university. In first year, I gained 8 kilos and now, I socialize less.

In this example, Atoosa outlined her experience of lowered self-worth and self-confidence due to

weight gain. She explained that, as a result of gaining weight, she feels less comfortable

socializing for fear of being judged by others. She went on to discuss how her overall self-

concept changes when she gains weight:

Today, I feel ugly again because I didn't exercise for a while and I gained weight. I don't

like to go to the gym and show my body to everybody. Something like that is always

going through my mind.

58

Sarah also described how her self-worth had become contingent on weight, and how she

continues to struggle with untangling the two:

It [feeling worthy when overweight] is something that I still struggle with. Because I

wasn't pretty or small, I was somehow less-than so I had to try harder [in other areas]

which is total societal bullshit and it’s really stupid and it’s not true at all but we’re lead

to believe these things [that being overweight makes you less-than] and so it's very hard

to untangle that as a conscious adult.

Sarah further explained how weight and appearance, over other qualities, had become paramount

in determining her self-worth:

It is almost like your appearance becomes everything and you ignore the fact that you

have other skills or other qualities, and that those also make you a whole person and they

make you worthwhile.

Emma echoed Sarah’s sentiment:

In a way, because I was so self-conscious [about my body] it was almost like my

confidence as a whole wasn't what it should have been. I wasn’t able to see that other

things, like the fact that I was smart and doing well in school, [made me worthy].

She further described how approaching a potential romantic partner was impacted by viewing

herself as less worthy of love because of her weight:

If I liked a guy, I wouldn’t just go for it and think, ‘maybe they like me back.’ I would

think, ‘they would never like me’ or ‘that guy would never go for me.’

Additionally, when participants reported being the only individual in the family that was made to

diet, they further internalized the idea of being less-than as a result of their weight status. For

example, Atoosa reported:

59

I felt, at that time, that she [my mother] didn’t like me because of my weight, because I

felt like she was saying, ‘because you are overweight, you are ugly. You are less likeable

than your other siblings.’ I was comparing myself to my other siblings all the time

because they were girls and I felt like she liked me less and I was ashamed.

In total, participants stated that the meaning making process associated with childhood

experiences of parental feeding control deeply impacted how they view themselves, others, and

the world. Furthermore, participants indicated that experiencing parental feeding control

practices during their childhoods affected their psychological well-being. The following section

describes the reported psychological impacts of experiencing parental feeding control.

Experiencing Psychological Impacts

The second recognized core concept was experiencing psychological impacts.

Participants described three psychological consequences of undergoing parental feeding control

practices during their childhoods, these themes were: (a) becoming more self-conscious about

their weight and size, (b) feeling guilt about eating, and (c) developing a fear of gaining weight.

The following sections illustrate the previously mentioned psychological consequences in more

detail.

Becoming self-conscious of weight. As a result of experiencing parental feeding control

practices, participants described becoming more self-conscious about their weight and size.

They reported feeling uncomfortable or embarrassed when being observed by others due to fears

that they would be judged for their weight and size. To begin, Emma described how she became

more self-conscious about the size of her legs due to explicit parental messages about weight and

size, as well as her perception of appearance motivations for her parents’ use of feeding control

practices:

60

When I was younger, I had a more muscular body type and I remember my mom

mentioning something like, ‘Emma’s legs are so big compared to her friends.’...That’s

when the whole thing with my legs started and me thinking that my legs were fat, and I

didn’t like showing my legs and I didn’t like wearing shorts and all this stuff. I feel like

that’s when I started changing the way I felt about myself. Looking back, I was not

overweight and it makes me a little bit angry now because I just had a different body

shape but it wasn’t a bad thing.

She continued to describe how being self-conscious about her legs persisted into adulthood:

I bought my first pair of skinny jeans when I was in 3rd year of university because I felt

so self-conscious about my legs and my butt and my hips...So it [feeling self-conscious

about my legs] continued on and [because of experiencing parental feeding control

practices] it did take me a very long time to accept my body.

Rebecca described becoming self-conscious about her weight at a very young age as a result of

her experience of parental feeding control practices:

I was a lot more conscious of my weight at a younger age. Like how I was already

noticing my size and my weight in grade one in comparison to my peers. I think I'm a lot

more conscious. Even now, I have to think about what food I’m eating and whether or

not I’m exercising enough.

Atoosa described not only being self-conscious of her body, but of the stretch-marks she

developed as a consequence of gaining weight during her childhood:

Even now, I am so upset about it [how my body looks], it has become an obsession for

me...all those stretch marks...that I am embarrassed of...I can still see the history [of

experiencing parental feeding control practices] on my body.

61

She further outlined how feeling self-conscious about her body made her feel uncomfortable in

social interactions:

It [being overweight] was awful because I felt like I was different from others. I

remember I couldn’t even look people in the eyes [because I was self-conscious about my

body].

In sum, as a consequence of experiencing parental feeding control behaviours, participants

became more focused on their weight and size, as well as became embarrassed of, and

uncomfortable in, their bodies.

Feeling guilt about eating. Participants indicated feeling guilty when they made food

decisions that went against their parents’ dietary wishes. They outlined that the guilt they felt,

and still feel, when eating particular foods was related to internalized parental messages they had

received as a result of experiencing parental feeding control practices. For example, Jackie’s

guilt was associated with consuming carbohydrates because she was put on a “low carb” diet

during her childhood. She described that the guilt she felt when eating carbohydrates carried on

into her adulthood:

I would feel awful, usually because it was carbs. I never threw up in middle school or

high school. In my undergrad, in first year, that was kind of when I struggled with that

[purging].

Rebecca explained how her guilt associated with eating developed. She described feeling as

though she would get in trouble with her father if she engaged in “sneak eating”:

So they [my parents] would just tell me not to snack very often. You know how kids

sneak eat? I’d do that quite a bit. That’s when my dad started being more on guard.

62

Whenever I went to the kitchen, he would watch me and so then I started being like a

little bit more scared and guilty around that aspect.

Furthermore, she indicated that she continues to feel guilty about certain dietary choices, even as

an adult:

I think I still carry guilt. Even nowadays, if I'm eating ice cream, I’ll have that in the

back of my mind, ‘I probably shouldn’t be eating this.’

Overall, through experiencing parental feeding control practices, participants reported

internalizing parental messages about “good” and “bad” foods, which lead to feelings of guilt

when participants consumed “bad” foods. Moreover, most participants outlined that this guilt

stuck with them even into adulthood.

Fearing weight gain. As an outcome of experiencing parental feeding control

behaviours, as well as internalizing associated parental messages about weight (e.g., being

overweight is bad), participants described developing a fear of gaining weight. They indicated

fearing weight gain as they did not want to be seen as less desirable, attractive, or worthy of

other peoples’ love and attention. Atoosa explained that she fears gaining weight as it affects her

confidence and elicits anxiety:

I become anxious and feel less confident and ugly whenever I see myself in the mirror

[and I have gained weight]...So I am really kind of [scared of gaining weight], it became

like a phobia for me of gaining weight.

Atoosa further described how she feels when she gains weight, as well as how she attributes

these feelings to her mother’s use of parental feeding control practices:

63

I feel scared or panic when I gain weight and I don't know why. I can't logically think

about it. I guess it is all because of my mom [and how she handled my weight “issue”]...I

blame her for that.

Rebecca reported the following when asked why she fears gaining weight:

I think that I’m not very attractive at that size and that people will probably think less of

me if I’m that large.

When asked why she feared gaining weight, Jackie offered the description below:

I think it is [due to] other people’s perception of it [being overweight]. As being

unattractive and something to make fun of someone for, or a deficit I guess.

Emma outlined her reasoning for fearing weight gain as being related to her worries of not being

worthy of the opposite sex’s attention:

If they [male peers] talked about girls that they liked, they would never mention the

chubby girl and I didn’t want to be that girl.

Overall, participants outlined developing a fear of gaining weight as a result of experiencing

parental feeding control practices. They described not wanting to gain weight for fear of being

seen as less desirable, attractive, and worthy of other peoples’ love and attention due to their

weight.

In sum, experiencing parental feeding control practices impacted participants’ overall

psychological well-being. Moreover, participants reported that the meaning making process and

the psychological impacts of experiencing parental feeding control practices came together to

affect their eating behaviours. In the following section, I describe how participants reported

engaging in maladaptive eating behaviours in order to remedy their weight-related psychological

distress.

64

Experiencing Behavioural Impacts

The third identified core concept was experiencing behavioural impacts. As a

consequence of undergoing parental feeding control practices during their childhoods, as well as

experiencing the negative meaning making and psychological outcomes of these practices,

participants described engaging in maladaptive eating behaviours, the only theme of this core

concept, in order to lessen their weight-related psychological distress. Furthermore, I interpreted

two sub-themes from participants’ descriptions of engaging in maladaptive eating behaviours,

these were: (a) restricting, and (b) bingeing and engaging in compensatory eating behaviours.

The following sections describe the maladaptive eating behaviours reported by participants in

greater detail. It is important to note that participants did not report engaging in all of the

compensatory behaviours outlined below; however, every participant did describe engaging in at

least one of the following behaviours as a consequence of experiencing parental feeding control

practices.

Engaging in maladaptive eating behaviours. Participants reported engaging in certain

maladaptive eating behaviours in order to manage their weight concerns, reduce their self-

conscious feelings about their weight and size, diminish their guilt associated with eating

particular foods, and lessen their fear of weight gain. Additionally, they reported engaging in

maladaptive eating behaviours to ensure that their worth would not be diminished by weight

gain.

Restricting. In order to promote weight loss, or to guarantee that they would not gain

weight, some participants indicated engaging in self-imposed dietary restriction. Emma

described restricting her own eating due to parental comments about her weight and size:

65

I would ruminate on her comments [about my weight and size] and they would really get

under my skin and eventually would change my [eating] behaviours.

May indicated engaging in calorie restriction in order to lose weight, as well as being motivated

to continue restricting her calories due to weight loss:

I was kind of excited to go on a stricter diet with 1,200 calories a day. After a week, I

saw that it worked... Once you see your weight drop, you’re more motivated to do that

[continue restricting calories].

Sarah described how she internalized her parents’ use of dietary restriction:

Going from really overeating when I was young and being commented on that [my eating

and weight], to eating way less [due to parental feeding control practices], and then I

developed sort of an eating disorder after that point.

In summary, as a result of experiencing parental feeding control practices during their

childhoods, participants described engaging in self-imposed dietary restriction in order to

continue managing their weight concerns.

Bingeing and engaging in compensatory eating behaviours. As a result of prolonged

restriction, some participants reported going through cycles of bingeing and restricting, while

others indicated experiencing phases of bingeing and purging. Participants described growing

tired of dietary restriction, which resulted in bingeing in order to experience dietary freedom.

They stated that the freedom they felt, as a consequence of bingeing, was short-lived as they

were quickly consumed with feelings of guilt, which prompted further dietary restriction or

purging. The sections below describe the binge/restrict and binge/purge cycles reported by

certain participants.

66

Some participants described bingeing as a result of being on a diet for a prolonged period

of time, and trying to remedy the binge through additional dietary restriction. These behaviours

came together to form a binge and restrict cycle. Sarah described her bingeing and restricting as

follows:

It [experiencing parental feeding control practices] set me up for a lifetime of that

restricting, binge, restrict, binge, restrict behaviour. Because it's [restricted food]

forbidden, [I would think,] ‘I’m going to eat all the things [restricted foods] now because

I’m never having it again...I’ll go forward being good: tomorrow and forever.’

Emma described her inhibition and restriction cycles in the following manner:

I remember all of my friends had the thigh gap and I hated the fact that my thighs rubbed

against each other. So I would take care of my eating and then I would realize that I still

didn’t have a thigh gap and I would just be like, ‘screw it [dietary restriction]’ and I’d eat

whatever I wanted. Then, I would kind of go back and start, because my mom would

make a comment like, ‘you shouldn’t be eating so much junk food’, and then I would go

back to taking care of what I ate. So it would go in cycles.

Emma continued to explain how parental messages about her weight and size would push her to

cycle from inhibited to restricted eating:

If she [my mother] would be like, ‘I think your thighs are a little bigger’ or like ‘I think

you’re gaining weight a little. Your butt looks bigger’, then I would go sort of steering

the bus the other way to get on the restricting side of things.

In summary, participants reported being stuck in a cycle of restriction and inhibition, where they

craved freedom from their dietary restriction but once they strayed from their diets, they felt

guilty about their food intake which lead them to, once again, restrict their eating.

67

Other participants reported purging the food they had consumed during a binge in order

to neutralize their feelings of guilt. May outlined her experience of bingeing and purging in the

following quotes:

I know it's super unhealthy but I do have binges sometimes, especially [when I’m] on

diets. When I was younger, after binging, I would throw up but then I googled all of the

symptoms of doing this and they were super bad, but even though I knew it was super

bad I, the more restrictive it is, the more I feel like I need to break free of that [restriction]

and I would binge and then I would feel bad and so then I would throw up.

She went on to explain the thought-process involved in bingeing and purging as it relates to

dietary restriction:

Being on a diet for a long time, you’re just kind of sick of it and you want to eat. But

then there's a lot of pressure from other people to stay on your diet so then that makes you

want to throw up. At the same time, [I’m thinking,] ‘I worked so hard [to lose weight]’

so I just want to have a day of break [from my diet] but then afterwards I feel like, ‘I

worked so hard [to lose weight] and maybe I should not do this [take a break from my

diet and binge eat].’

Sarah explained that she found relief in purging as it meant that she could take a break from

restriction and engage in bingeing without experiencing its negative consequences (e.g., weight

gain):

Between grade 8 and grade 9 it evolved into an eating disorder. And then I was like, ‘I’m

going to super restrict, and restrict’ and then I was like ‘Oh, I can binge and then

compensate’ and like I didn’t know how to do that before. I was like, ‘I can purge! Oh

my god, [this is] amazing!’

68

Altogether, some participants reported finding solace in purging as it allowed them to

momentarily break free from their dietary restriction without experiencing weight gain and the

psychological distress that comes with it. They reported knowing the negative consequences

associated with purging; however, they believed that the benefits of purging outweighed the

costs.

Overall, participants’ reports of engaging in maladaptive eating behaviours demonstrates

how engrained their beliefs about weight (i.e., being overweight is bad) were, and still are, and

how important it was, and continues to be, for them to keep their weight in check.

Developing a Long-Lasting Preoccupation with Weight

The fourth identified theme associated with childhood experiences of parental feeding

control was developing a long-lasting preoccupation with weight. Many participants reported

that they were still struggling with weight-related psychological concerns, as well as engaging in

maladaptive eating behaviours, due to their childhood experiences of parental feeding control

practices. When asked whether or not she had experienced any psychological impacts as a result

of parental feeding control practices, Sarah reported the following:

A lifetime of disordered eating that I’m still not [over]...I’ve tried to manifest that

recovery and affirm that recovery but it’s been fucking challenging. [I’ve experienced] a

lifetime of disordered relationships with my body and food.

May described the following impact of experiencing parental feeding control during her

childhood:

I never thought of myself as not being on a diet or not being restrictive.

69

Rebecca described still feeling the need to monitor her diet and exercise as a result of her

experience of parental feeding control practices and her parents’ emphasis on health and

thinness:

Even now, I have to think about what food I’m eating and whether or not I’m exercising

enough so I guess it's one of my top [concerns]...I guess like health and weight is

something that I'm still pretty concerned about.

Emma described the long-lasting effects of parental feeding control practices:

[It effected] the way I viewed myself and the way I viewed my relationships with others,

and how pervasive their [my parents] comments were...to the point where I still hear my

mother's voice being like, ‘your legs are fat.’ I really have to fight against that [the

thoughts] and be like, ‘No, stop.’ I need to stop that maladaptive thought basically. I feel

like it [experiencing parental feeding control] does affect you and whether you fight it or

not as an adult, it’s [the impact] still there.

Atoosa reported that experiencing parental feeding control practices has had a lasting impact on

her emotional and psychological well-being:

None of those bad feelings about myself or the low confidence due to that childhood

experience, none of them actually went away...I am sure that it’s not going to go away

completely; this problem is stuck with me...It won't go away emotionally, it still exists.

Physically, all the stretch marks remind me of the experiences [of parental feeding

control practices]. So this is a childhood experience that sticks with you, and you carry

[it] for your whole life. It [experiencing parental feeding control practices] scars.

According to participants, the consequences of parental feeding control practices are intense and

long-lasting. Participants indicated that they continue to struggle with the belief that their self-

70

worth is contingent on their weight and still grapple with the psychological consequences of

experiencing parental feeding control practices. Furthermore, participants stated that they

continue to engage in maladaptive eating behaviours in order to lessen their weight-related

psychological distress. Overall, experiencing parental feeding control practices was perceived

by the participants of this research study to cause lasting weight preoccupation.

Seeing Parental Feeding Control Practices as Unhelpful

The last identified core concept was seeing parental feeding control practices as

unhelpful, which included one theme: offering advice. Overall, as a result of living with the

consequences associated with experiences of parental feeding control practices, participants

reported coming to the conclusion that these feeding practices were unhelpful. Atoosa explained

that despite experiencing parental feeding control, her struggle with weight was not resolved:

It's not that it [parental feeding control] happened just once and I became thin. No, I

always dealt with this issue [struggling with weight] after.

Jackie explained how she views parental feeding control practices in the following quote:

I don’t think putting kids on a diet is helpful at all. I think a moderation approach is okay,

but I don’t think you should tell your kid, ‘you can’t have dessert and you can’t have

treats’ regardless of their weight because I don’t think that works.

Emma described that, despite having her dietary intake controlled and restricted, she did not

experience weight loss, which resulted in feelings of frustration:

I wouldn’t eat all these things and my weight would go nowhere. So it was frustrating to

the point that I was like, ‘screw this, I’m going to eat whatever.’

Offering advice. Given that participants disagreed with the use of parental feeding

control practices and believed that they were unhelpful, I asked them to provide advice for

71

parents who are currently raising children that may be struggling with their weight. In response,

Sarah said the following:

Just teach them really explicitly and from day one...reinforce that self-esteem...that

because you are here, you are worthy, [and your worth is] not contingent on anything.

Rebecca suggested that parents not make their children feel guilty about eating certain foods as

that can impact how they view themselves and their relationships:

I think it [my advice] would be not to guilt them [children] because I think that you don’t

often realize how guilty kids can feel and how negatively they can perceive something. I

think at that age, they [children] really want that parental acceptance and so that guilt

[associated with eating certain foods] can isolate them a little bit.

Sarah proposed that parents focus on the fostering a healthy lifestyle. She suggested not

restricting certain foods but instead, adding healthy foods. Furthermore, she recommended that

promoting a healthy lifestyle should become a family bonding activity:

I think it’s really kind of about getting excited about food prepping together, doing things

together to facilitate a healthier lifestyle. So instead of [saying,] ‘we’re not going to do

this [eat particular foods]’, focus on the positives. For example, [saying,] ‘we’re going to

go pick healthy groceries that you like to eat together.’ And you know you can prepare

food as a family. So [saying,] ‘let’s find some recipes’ or ‘let’s go to the market.’

Emma emphasized focusing on health instead of weight loss. She also described working

towards health as a family unit, instead of targeting or alienating a particular family member:

I would focus more on health because I feel like body or weight [change] follows from

health. If you are more health focused, you don’t end up cutting things you shouldn’t. If

you focus more on health and you think about, ‘this as a family, a system, how are we

72

functioning? Are we all eating badly and some of us are gaining more weight than

others?’ But I think working as a family and being like, ‘we’re all going to eat healthier

and try to be more active’ that kind of stuff. So putting a more positive spin on it rather

than [saying,] ‘you’re fat.’

In general, participants agreed that experiencing parental feeding control during their childhoods

was unhelpful and damaging. They believed that parents should focus on encouraging healthy

eating behaviours and family bonding, instead of restricting certain foods and emphasizing

weight loss.

Summary of the Constructivist Grounded Theory

The preliminary constructivist GT consists of five core categories: making meaning of

parental feeding control, experiencing psychological impacts, experiencing behavioural impacts,

developing a long-lasting preoccupation with weight, and viewing parental feeding control

practices as unhelpful. All participants described making meaning of their experience of parental

feeding control practices (i.e., developing the view that being overweight is bad) and

experiencing negative psychological impacts. It was further stated that the two previously

mentioned core concepts came together with experiencing parental feeding control practices to

result in experiencing behavioural impacts (i.e., engaging in maladaptive eating behaviours). All

of the research participants reported developing a long-lasting preoccupation with weight as a

result of their experience of parental feeding control practices and its related meaning-making,

psychological, and behavioural impacts. In total, due to experiencing the negative consequences

of parental feeding control practices, participants viewed these feeding methods as unhelpful and

damaging. Altogether, the categories, themes, and sub-themes of this preliminary and

73

interpretive constructivist GT offer a comprehensive narrative of the meaning making and

psychological impacts of experiencing parental feeding control practices during one’s childhood.

74

CHAPTER FIVE

Discussion

The objective of the current study was to develop a preliminary constructivist grounded

theory (GT) that explains the meaning making and psychological processes associated with

childhood experiences of parental feeding control practices. The constructivist version of GT

methods provided the tools necessary to collect accounts of childhood experiences of parental

feeding control. The preliminary constructivist GT offers insight into how individuals make

meaning of their experience of parental feeding control practices, as well as provides an

understanding of the psychological and behavioural impacts of these feeding methods.

The preliminary constructivist GT details important dynamics between the core concepts

associated with experiencing parental feeding control practices during one’s childhood. To

begin, individuals face meaning making and psychological impacts as a result of experiencing

parental feeding control practices. Furthermore, undergoing parental feeding control practices

during one’s childhood, as well as experiencing the meaning making and psychological impacts

of these practices, combine to support the use of maladaptive eating behaviours. Lastly, the

meaning making, psychological, and behavioural impacts have a lasting effect on individuals’

overall psychological well-being and eating behaviours in the form of persistent weight

preoccupation.

The following chapter attends to the contributions and limitations of the current research

study. In line with constructivist GT methods, a secondary literature review was conducted.

According to constructivist GT methods, a preliminary literature review can be utilized by

novice researchers to clarify research aims, contextualize the research study, and help the

researcher become aware of sensitising concepts (Charmaz, 2006; Heath & Cowley, 2004;

75

Wilson & Barn, 2012). The secondary literature review, based on constructivist GT methods,

allows the researcher to compare and contrast findings with past research studies (Charmaz,

2006). Thus, given the results of the current study, the second literature review, focused on

relevant feeding, weight stigma/bias, and childhood obesity literature, is described. Furthermore,

contributions to the literature and counselling psychology implications of the current study are

also discussed. Finally, the strengths and limitations of this study are addressed, as well as future

directions for research.

Contributions to the Literature

Given the potential negative health and psychosocial consequences associated with

childhood obesity (e.g., type II diabetes, social stigmatization and marginalization, as well as

depression), it is no wonder parents may want to utilize feeding control practices (i.e., dietary

restriction and monitoring, as well as pressuring a child to eat certain foods such as fruits and

vegetables) in order to manage their children’s weight (i.e., promote weight loss or prevent

weight gain). Despite parents’ best intentions, however, researchers have found that parental

feeding control practices can have counterproductive weight-related and eating effects (Birch &

Fisher, 1995; Birch, et al., 2003; Campbell et al., 2006; Carper et al., 2000; Fisher & Birch,

2002; Gable & Lutz, 2000; Johnson & Birch, 1994; Thompson, 2010; Vos & Welsh, 2010).

Furthermore, results of the current study indicate that experiencing parental feeding control

practices during one’s childhood can also contribute to negative and lasting meaning making,

psychological, and behavioural outcomes.

While the results of this study resonate with existing ideas in the literature, they also

provide new insights on how these ideas interact with one another. Altogether, it is my hope that

this study’s preliminary theoretical model will incite discussion about, as well as prompt future

76

research on, the impacts of parental feeding control practices on children’s overall well-being.

The following sections aim to incorporate the findings of this study with the current literature,

while also offering a new way of conceptualizing the impacts of experiencing parental feeding

control practices during one’s childhood.

Contributions to the Feeding Literature

As was noted in chapter two, most of the literature on parental feeding control practices

focuses on the behavioural impacts of employing these feeding methods on children. For

example, parental feeding control practices have been found to be negatively associated with

children’s ability to self-regulate food consumption, positively associated with weight status, as

well as linked to the development of a preference for restricted foods (Birch & Fisher, 1995;

Birch, et al., 2003; Campbell et al., 2006; Carper et al., 2000; Fisher & Birch, 2002; Gable &

Lutz, 2000; Johnson & Birch, 1994; Thompson, 2010; Vos & Welsh, 2010). As the feeding

literature has mostly focused on the behavioural impacts of parental feeding control practices,

children’s subjective experiences of these feeding methods have remained largely ignored.

Moreover, the long-term impacts of parental feeding control practices have not been explored.

For these reasons, the present study aimed to investigate the short- and long-term meaning

making and psychological impacts of experiencing parental feeding control practices during

one’s childhood.

Findings from this study indicate that experiencing parental feeding control practices

during one’s childhood can have a lasting effect on how individuals view themselves and the

world (i.e., adopting the belief that being overweight is bad), as well as impacts their

psychological well-being and eating behaviours. Given the scarcity of research on the meaning

making and psychological impacts of parental feeding control practices, it is necessary to go

77

beyond the feeding literature in order to find an explanation for these results. As such, I have

related findings to the weight stigma and weight bias literature. This literature provides a

framework for comprehending the meaning making, psychological, and behavioural impacts of

experiencing parental feeding control practices during one’s childhood.

Contributions to the Weight Stigma and Weight Bias Literature

Results of this study indicate that individuals who experience parental feeding control

practices during their childhoods engage in a meaning making process which involves: (a)

perceiving parents’ motivation for utilizing feeding control practices, (b) internalizing parental

messages about weight, and (c) viewing self-worth as contingent on weight. During the

interview process, participants’ described receiving and internalizing negative parental messages

about weight through experiencing parental feeding control practices. These parental messages

about weight included that being overweight (a) is unattractive and will negatively impacts

future success, (b) leads to being stigmatized and bullied, as well as (c) is unhealthy. Overall,

through the use of parental feeding control practices, parents reinforce the message that being

overweight is bad. Additionally, the meaning making process was found to be related to lasting

psychological and behavioural impacts. These findings fit within, and add to, the weight stigma

and weight bias literature.

Weight stigma literature. Weight-related stigmatization has been researched largely in

adult populations; in fact, it has been found that adults who are classified as overweight or obese

encounter social disadvantages and stigmatization in various life arenas (e.g., employment,

healthcare, education, and interpersonal relationships) due to their weight status (Brownell, 2005;

Puhl & Brownell, 2001; Russell-Mayhew et al., 2015; Russell-Mayhew et al., 2016). In addition

to the weight stigma literature with adult populations, there is an increasing amount of research

78

showing that children who are categorized as overweight and obese are targets of weight-related

societal stigmatization and marginalization. Researchers have demonstrated that children who

are classified as overweight and/or obese experience weight-related bias and stereotyping by

peers (Brylinsky & Moore, 1994; Kraig & Keel, 2001; Latner & Stunkard, 2003; Neumark-

Sztainer et al., 2002; Neumark-Sztainer, Story, & Faibisch, 1998), teachers (Bauer et al., 2004;

Neumark-Sztainer et al., 1999), and even parents (Adams, Hicken, & Salehi, 1988; Crandall,

1991, 1995; Davison & Birch, 2004).

Despite the limited amount of research on parental weight-related bias, consistent and

disheartening results have emerged (Adams et al., 1988; Crandall, 1991, 1995; Davison & Birch,

2004). To illustrate these findings, I highlight research conducted by Davison and Birch (2004)

in which they investigated weight-related stereotyping among 9-year-old girls and their parents.

They found that parents, and their daughters, attributed significantly more negative

characteristics (e.g., laziness) to individuals who are deemed obese than to thinner individuals.

Additionally, daughters were more likely to endorse negative weight-related stereotypes if their

parents stressed the importance of being thin, as well as emphasized weight loss. Lastly, parents

who were overweight or obese were just as likely to support negative weight-related stereotypes

as thinner parents.

The current study adds to the weight stigma literature by providing evidence that parents

may be expressing weight stigma towards their children through the use of parental feeding

control practices. The current study’s findings endeavour to explain Davison and Birch’s (2004)

results by providing a theory about how negative weight-related stereotypes are transmitted to

children by their parents. Based on this study’s constructivist GT model, children recognize and

adopt negative weight-related stereotypes through perceiving their parents’ motivation for

79

utilizing parental feeding control practices. For example, if parents endorse appearance

motivations for utilizing feeding control practices, they are transmitting the message that

overweight individuals are less attractive than thinner individuals. Through perceiving their

parents’ appearance motivation for using feeding control practices, as well as through receiving

implicit and explicit parental messages about weight, children begin to adopt their parents’

negative weight-related beliefs.

Internalized weight bias literature. The results of the current study suggest that

experiencing parental feeding control practices, as well as receiving negative parental messages

about weight, can lead to the internalization of negative parental weight-related messages (i.e.,

internalized weight bias). Internalized weight bias, defined as having a negative view of oneself

due to weight or size, has been found to develop as a result of experiencing weight stigmatization

and/or marginalization (Latner, Barile, Durso, & O'Brien, 2014). In contrast to other stigmatized

groups (e.g., Jewish and Asian individuals) who do not tend to endorse internalized bias,

researchers have observed internalized weight stigma among individuals who are classified as

obese (Rudman, Feinburg, & Fairchild, 2002; Wang, Brownell, & Wadden, 2004). That is,

despite inclusion in the stigmatized group, researchers have found that individuals who are

categorized as overweight and obese endorse implicit anti-fat attitudes, and some even report an

explicit anti-fat bias (e.g., using unflattering adjectives when describing obese individuals;

Rudman et al., 2002; Wang et al., 2004).

The current study’s findings fit within the internalized weight bias literature. In line with

this study’s preliminary theoretical model, individuals were found to develop internalized weight

bias as a result of experiencing parental feeding control practices and receiving stigmatizing

parental messages about weight. Moreover, despite being part of the stigmatized group (i.e.,

80

individuals who perceive themselves as overweight), participants reported endorsing anti-fat

attitudes (e.g., being overweight makes you less worthy of other people’s love and attention),

which is believed to have impacted their overall psychological well-being and eating behaviours.

Consequences of weight stigma and internalized weight bias. Experiencing weight

stigma and internalized weight bias have been found to be associated with a variety of negative

outcomes. The following sections outline those outcomes in greater detail and relate them back

to the current study’s findings.

Consequences of weight stigma. As was previously mentioned in chapter two,

researchers have found that children who are categorized as overweight or obese may be more

likely to develop depression and suicidal ideation, as well as experience decreased body

satisfaction and lower self-esteem as a result of experiencing weight-related stigmatization and

marginalization (Eisenberg et al., 2003, Hayden-Wade et al., 2005; Puhl & Latner, 2007; Storch

et al., 2007). These findings align with the current study’s results. A number of participants

viewed their childhood experience of parental feeding control as stigmatizing, and they reported

experiencing decreased body satisfaction (i.e., becoming more self-conscious about their weight

and size) as well as lowered self-esteem due to their weight and size (i.e., viewing self-worth as

contingent on weight). However, depression and suicidal ideation were not reported by

participants in this study.

Consequences of internalized weight bias. In samples of individuals who are classified as

overweight and/or obese, internalized weight bias is consistently found to be related to adverse

psychological and behavioural consequences. Various researchers have found a negative

correlation between internalized weight bias and psychological well-being; depression, anxiety,

stress, and lowered self-esteem have all been found to be associated with experiencing

81

internalized weight bias (Carels et al., 2013; Carels et al., 2010; Durso & Latner, 2008; Schvey,

Roberto, & White, 2013; Schvey & White, 2015). Furthermore, internalized weight bias has

been shown to be linked to maladaptive eating and body image-related variables, which include

disordered eating (e.g., bingeing and purging, as well as dietary restraint), body dissatisfaction,

and weight and shape concerns (Carels et al., 2013; Carels et al., 2010; Durso & Latner, 2008;

Pearl, White, & Grilo, 2014; Schvey et al., 2013).

These findings also correspond to the results of the current study. Participants reported

internalizing negative parental messages about weight and size (i.e., internalized weight bias),

and described experiencing weight-related anxiety and stress (i.e., fearing weight gain), as well

as lowered self-esteem in relation to their beliefs about weight-contingent self-worth.

Additionally, participants endorsed engaging in self-imposed dietary restriction, and described

engaging in bingeing and purging. Participants also reported experiencing body dissatisfaction,

as well as overall and lasting weight and shape concerns (i.e., feeling self-conscious about

weight and size, fearing weight gain, viewing self-worth as contingent on weight, and developing

lasting weight preoccupation). These findings suggest that experiencing parental feeding control

practices during one’s childhood can lead to internalized weight bias, and that internalized

weight bias can significantly impact one’s psychological well-being and eating behaviours.

Intergenerational internalized weight bias. Past research conducted by Musher-

Eizenman and colleagues (2007) has demonstrated that parents are more likely to utilize parental

feeding control practices if they endorse anti-fat attitudes. Moreover, findings from the current

study indicate that parents transmit anti-fat attitudes to their children through parental feeding

control practices which results in the internalization of negative parental messages about weight

(i.e., internalized weight bias). In combination, these findings suggest that anti-fat attitudes and

82

internalized weight bias may be passed down intergenerationally through the use of parental

feeding control practices. This intergenerational passing down of stigmatizing weight-related

attitudes creates a damaging cycle in which parents perpetuate anti-fat messages through

utilizing parental feeding control practices, which then leads to internalized weight bias for the

child. This cycle of body hatred is likely to repeat itself if the internalized weight bias, incurred

by the child through experiencing parental feeding control practices, is not addressed (Russell-

Mayhew, in press).

In sum, the weight stigma/bias literature offers insight into why individuals who

experience parental feeding control practices also report psychological and eating disturbances.

Altogether, the current study’s preliminary conceptualization of the meaning making and

psychological impacts of parental feeding control practices provides an explanation of the

mechanism through which individuals internalize anti-fat attitudes, as well as offers a narrative

for its associated psychological and behavioural impacts.

Contributions to the Childhood Obesity Treatment Literature

Overall, the results of the current study indicate that parental feeding control practices are

unhelpful in the treatment of childhood overweight and/or obesity. Furthermore, findings

indicate that utilizing parental feeding control practices in order to remedy childhood overweight

and/or obesity can have a lasting negative impact on children’s overall well-being. These results

fit within the childhood obesity treatment literature. Generally, the literature does not endorse

utilizing parental feeding control practices in order to address childhood overweight or obesity as

these practices have been associated with counterproductive outcomes (e.g., weight gain,

disinhibited eating, and eating in the absence of hunger; Birch & Fisher, 1995; Birch, et al.,

2003; Campbell et al., 2006; Carper et al., 2000; Fisher & Birch, 2002; Gable & Lutz, 2000;

83

Johnson & Birch, 1994; Thompson, 2010; Vos & Welsh, 2010). Additionally, the results of the

current study provide further evidence against the use of parental feeding control practices to

treat childhood overweight and obesity (e.g., negative meaning making, psychological, and

behavioural impacts).

Due to their beliefs about the unhelpful and damaging effects of parental feeding control

practices, participants provided advice for parents of children who may be dealing with weight-

related struggles. Participants’ suggestions included: parental encouragement of healthy eating

behaviours, turning the weight struggle into an opportunity for family bonding (e.g., cooking

healthy meals together), as well as refraining from restricting particular foods and emphasizing

weight loss. These participant recommendations are consistent with the childhood obesity

treatment literature. Current guidelines for childhood obesity treatment recommend

multidisciplinary interventions that highlight change in diet, activity, and behaviour (Barlow,

2007). More specifically, parental/familial involvement is highly recommended, with family-

based behavioural weight loss treatment being the most widely supported method in the literature

for addressing childhood obesity (Altman & Wilfley, 2015; American Dietetic Association,

2006; Ball et al., 2012; Ho et al., 2012; Holt et al., 2008; Snethen, Broome, & Cashin, 2006;

Young, Northern, Lister, Drummond, & O’Brien, 2007).

Family-based behavioural weight loss treatment (FBT) is an evidence-based intervention

which treats the whole family system through comprehensive methods intended to diminish

energy intake, increase physical activity, modify family behavioural patterns, and adjust the

home environment (Holland et al., 2014). FBT endeavours to alter the family environment by

encouraging parents to remove energy-dense foods from the home and make nutrient-dense

foods readily available to all family members (Holland et al., 2014). These alterations to the

84

family home provide an environment that encourages children to make healthy choices, and

removes the need for excessive parental dietary restriction (Holland et al., 2014). As such,

children feel less deprived within their familial environment (Holland et al., 2014). Furthermore,

parents are encouraged to model healthy behaviours (e.g., healthy eating and exercise), and are

taught to create a supportive familial environment in which the child does not feel isolated or

targeted due to their weight struggles (Holland et al., 2014).

Overall, research has demonstrated that FBT is related to a reduction in the use of

parental feeding control practices, which is also related to a reduction in child weight status

(Epstein, Paluch, Beecher, & Roemmich, 2008; Holland et al., 2014). Furthermore, FBT was

found to be more effective than child-only treatments (Brownell, Kelman, & Stunkard, 1983;

Epstein, Valoski, Wing, & McCurley, 1990), as well as control conditions (Epstein et al., 1990;

Graves, Meyers, & Clark, 1988; Israel, Stolmaker, & Andrian, 1985; Kirschenbaum, Harris, &

Tomarken, 1984; Sacher et al., 2010; Savoye et al., 2007) in addressing childhood overweight

and obesity. Generally, FBT methods are line with participants’ previously mentioned

recommendations for dealing with childhood weight concerns, and research supports the use of

these methods as a means for remedying childhood obesity. Although FBT has been associated

with positive weight-related outcomes, the psychological impacts of these weight loss methods

have not been explored. As such, further research is needed to ensure that FBT does not result in

unintended negative consequences.

Implications for Counselling Psychology

The deeper understanding of the impacts of parental feeding control practices, gained as a

result of the current constructivist GT study, offers prevention and treatment implications for

counselling psychologists.

85

Prevention and Treatment Implications

Given the current study’s findings on the potential negative meaning making,

psychological, and behavioural outcomes of parental feeding control practices, as well as the

previously established findings on the counterproductive weight-related and eating impacts of

these feeding tactics, it is important for counselling psychologists to fervently advise against the

use of parental feeding control practices for child weight management. For example, if a parent

presents to counselling with concerns about their child’s weight, it would be imperative to

educate the parent on feeding control practices and their potential negative impacts (i.e.,

explaining how parental dietary restriction, monitoring, or pressure can lead to lasting

maladaptive meaning making, psychological, behavioural outcomes in children), as well as

strongly advise against the use of feeding control practices for addressing their child’s weight. In

fact, Russell-Mayhew (in press) suggests that counselling psychologists should refrain from

discussing weight loss efforts with their clients. She proposes that helping clients in their weight

loss endeavours, or their efforts to promote weight loss in their children, can be described as a

micro-aggression as it perpetuates dominant oppressive discourses about weight, which can be

damaging to all involved parties (Russell-Mayhew, in press). Counselling psychologists can

prevent the potential damage that a child could incur as a result of experiencing parental feeding

control practices by enthusiastically advising against the use of these feeding methods, helping

parents work through their thoughts and emotions related to having a child who is struggling

with their weight, and employing a family-based approach towards health and wellness.

If the parent is still adamant about addressing their child’s weight, it may be best to refer

them to another professional, who has an awareness of the negative impacts of parental feeding

control practices, in order to provide them with the necessary guidance in their endeavour. For

86

example, as it has been found to be the most effective treatment in the childhood obesity

literature, it would be advisable for the counselling psychologist to suggest that the parent, child,

and the family as a whole, seek FBT in order to address the child’s weight concerns through

means that do not involve parental feeding control.

Strengths and Limitations

The current study had many strengths and limitations. To begin, given the scarcity of

research on the meaning making and psychological impacts of experiencing parental feeding

control practices during one’s childhood, the first strength of the current study was filling these

gaps in the literature. Through demonstrating that parental feeding control practices contribute

to negative and lasting meaning making, psychological, as well as behavioural outcomes, the

current study added to the feeding, weight stigma/bias, and childhood obesity treatment

literature. Furthermore, prominent weight bias researchers, Puhl and Latner (2007), outlined a

need for future research studies to determine the ways in which parents communicate

stigmatizing weight-related messages to their children, as well as investigate the impacts of these

messages on children’s well-being. The current research study fulfilled this need, and

contributed to the weight stigma and weight bias literature, by indicating that experiencing the

stigmatizing effects of parental feeding control practices during one’s childhood can lead to

internalized weight bias.

Moreover, Charmaz (2008) argues that a researcher’s active involvement in the process

of data collection and analysis, rather than passively analyzing previously collected and

transcribed data, provides a significant strength or advantage in the process of developing a

constructivist GT. As such, by collecting, transcribing, as well as coding participant interviews

myself, I was able to maintain closeness with the data, and recognize themes and patterns that I

87

may have overlooked if I had not been so involved with data collection and analysis (Charmaz,

2014).

In addition to the strengths, there were several limitations associated with this research

study. Firstly, although Charmaz (2006) argues that semi-structured interviews offer

inexperienced researchers with a format for organized data collection, it is possible that this

interviewing method could have limited the range of collected information (Tweed & Charmaz,

2012). Therefore, future studies should consider adopting a more open interviewing style in

order to collect a broader array of information from participants. Moreover, limiting data

collection to one interview per participant could have resulted in less complete information about

participants’ experience of parental feeding control practices. It is possible that participants

could have felt more comfortable sharing their experiences of the impacts of parental feeding

control, or could have had more time to reflect upon their experiences, if a second or third

interview had been administered.

Secondly, Charmaz (2014) explains that researcher subjectivity and the influence of

previously established research and theories are inevitable in qualitative research. As such,

despite my efforts to remain open to the data, as well as my intent to develop a preliminary

theory that was grounded in participants’ descriptions of the impacts of parental feeding control

practices, it is possible that personal biases and expectations influenced my research findings.

However, memo writing, discussions with my thesis advisor and student mentor, as well as

reflexive interviewing (i.e., asking participants clarifying questions in order to ensure that I am

accurately understanding their experience of parental feeding control instead of imposing my

research agenda onto the data) aided in enhancing the credibility of this study’s research

findings.

88

Thirdly, even though data collection terminated when it was deemed that no new themes

where emerging from the data, it is possible that further interviews could have resulted in the

emergence of additional themes. Therefore, true saturation may not have been achieved.

Hutchinson and Wilson (2001) argue that the premature termination of research can result in an

incomplete theory; thus, I recognize that the results of this study provide a preliminary

theoretical model of the meaning making and psychological impacts of experiencing parental

feeding control practices during one’s childhood.

Lastly, due to the low number of participants, as well as an entirely female sample, it is

difficult to assert whether or not the findings of the current study are applicable to all children

who have experienced parental feeding control practices during their childhoods. With this

being said, the current study’s preliminary constructivist GT was assessed based on its ability to

answer Charmaz’ (2014) evaluation questions regarding the GT’s credibility, originality,

resonance, and usefulness. Altogether, through answering these questions, the current study’s

constructivist GT was deemed to have met Charmaz’ (2014) substantive criteria and, therefore, is

viewed as valuable. Altogether, future research should endeavour to address the previously

mentioned limitations in order to develop a model that has increased theoretical applicability.

Potential future research directions are outlined below.

Future Research

There are numerous directions for future research based on the current study’s findings

and limitations. To begin, although the current study’s participants ranged in ethnicities and

educational backgrounds, the low number of participants, as well as the wholly female sample,

was a limitation of the current study. As such, it would be important to conduct a similar study,

on a grander scale (i.e., with more research participants of all genders), to discern whether or not

89

the findings of the current study apply to all genders. Alternatively, it could also be interesting

to investigate gender differences in childhood experiences of parental feeding control practices.

Another direction for future research could include a quantitative analysis of the meaning

making, psychological, and behavioural impacts of parental feeding control practices. It would

be interesting to discern whether or not the findings of this study could be replicated through

quantitative means. For example, future research could explore the internalization of weight bias

in individuals who have experienced parental feeding control during their childhoods by utilizing

internalized weight bias measures.

Moreover, future research could endeavour to evaluate the temporal aspect of the current

study’s findings. For instance, does the meaning making process occur before the psychological

and behavioural impacts appear? In line with this idea, it would be interesting to research this

phenomenon longitudinally to establish the order of events associated with childhood

experiences of parental feeding control practices.

Finally, it would be important that future research studies attempt to identify ways to

reduce weight stigma among parents of children who are classified as overweight or obese (e.g.,

evaluating the effectiveness of empathy inducing methods related to childhood overweight and

obesity; Puhl & Latner, 2007). As was previously discussed, results from a study conducted by

Musher-Eizenman and colleagues (2007) indicate that the more parents endorse anti-fat attitudes,

the more likely they are to employ parental feeding control practices to manage their children’s

weight. As such, if weight stigma among parents is reduced, the likelihood of using parental

feeding control practices, as well as the likelihood that children will experience the negative

meaning making and psychological impacts associated with these feeding methods, also

diminishes.

90

Conclusion

The aim of the current research study was to understand the meaning making and

psychological impacts of experiencing parental feeding control practices, aimed at promoting

weight loss or preventing weight gain, during one’s childhood. This aim was achieved by

conducting a constructivist GT study in which transcribed semi-structured interviews, with

individuals who experienced parental feeding control during their childhoods, were analyzed.

This study’s preliminary constructivist GT represents the interconnection between the identified

core concepts associated with childhood experiences of parental feeding control. Specifically,

the theory proposes a meaning making process, consisting of perceiving parental motivation for

using parental feeding control practices, internalizing parental messages about weight, and

viewing self-worth as contingent on weight. The preliminary constructivist GT suggests that

individuals experience a process of meaning making which leads to the internalization of the

belief that being overweight is bad. Furthermore, the theory outlines a psychological process

which involves becoming self-conscious about their weight and size, feeling guilt about eating,

and fearing weight gain. Next, it is theorized that undergoing parental feeding control practices

during one’s childhood, as well as experiencing the meaning making and psychological impacts

of these practices, come together to encourage the use of maladaptive eating behaviours.

Furthermore, the meaning making, psychological, and behavioural impacts are theorized to have

a lasting impact on participants’ psychological well-being, which takes the form of persistent

weight preoccupation. Overall, parental feeding control practices were seen as unhelpful and

damaging. Altogether, this preliminary constructivist GT fits within, and adds to, the existing

literature. Notably, the theory draws a link between the feeding literature and the weight

stigma/weight bias literature by creating a new understanding of how individuals come to

91

internalize weight bias, as well as how individuals are impacted by the internalization of anti-fat

attitudes. Moreover, this preliminary constructivist GT offers further evidence against the use of

parental feeding control practice to promote weight loss or prevent weight gain in child

populations.

To conclude, in today’s society, parents feel an incredible amount of pressure, from a

variety of sources (e.g., media, physicians, other parents, etc.), to manage their children’s weight

(i.e., ensure that their children fall within the “medically-acceptable” and “socially desirable”

weight range; Ristovski-Slijepcevic, Chapman, & Beagan, 2010). Furthermore, as it is common

practice for adults to address their personal weight concerns through self-imposed dietary

control, it is not surprising that parents may choose to address their children’s weight struggles

by exerting control over their eating (i.e., utilizing parental feeding control practices). Given the

small amount of research on the impacts of parental feeding control practices, as well as the

resulting lack of parental knowledge on the negative consequences of these feeding methods, I

do not blame parents for choosing to address their children’s weight concerns through parental

feeding control practices. In fact, it is my belief that these feeding methods are most often

implemented by parents who have the best of intentions (i.e., they want to protect their children

from experiencing the negative consequences associated with childhood overweight and

obesity); however, despite these good intentions, parental feeding control practices have been

found to be associated with unintended negative psychological, meaning making, and

behavioural consequences. Therefore, I believe it is imperative that parents become aware of the

potential negative outcomes of parental feeding control practices so that they can make informed

decisions related to addressing their children’s weight concerns. Overall, it is my hope that

parents become informed of the unintended negative meaning making and psychological impacts

92

of parental feeding control practices through the dissemination of the current study’s research

findings, and that fewer children experience the lasting eating- and weight-related struggles

associated with experiencing these controlling feeding methods.

93

References

Adams, G. R., Hicken, M., & Salehi, M. (1988). Socialization of the physical attractiveness

stereotype: Parental expectations and verbal behaviors. International Journal of

Psychology, 23, 137-149. doi:10.1080/00207598808247757

Altman, M., & Wilfley, D. E. (2015). Evidence update on the treatment of overweight and

obesity in children and adolescents. Journal of Clinical Child & Adolescent Psychology,

44, 521-537. doi:10.1080/15374416.2014.963854

American Dietetic Association. (2006). Position of the American Dietetic Association:

Individual-, family-, school-, and community-based interventions for pediatric

overweight. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 106, 925-945.

doi:10.1016/j.jada.2006.03.001

Andrews, T. (2012). What is social constructionism? The Grounded Theory Review, 11, 39-46.

Retrieved from http://groundedtheoryreview.com/2012/06/01/what-is-social-

constructionism/

Anzman, S. L., & Birch, L. L. (2009). Low inhibitory control and restrictive feeding practices

predict weight outcomes. The Journal of Pediatrics, 155, 651-656.

doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2009.04.052

Ball, G. D., Garcia, A. P., Chanoine, J. P., Morrison, K. M., Legault, L., Sharma, A. M., ... &

Holt, N. L. (2012). Should I stay or should I go? Understanding families’ decisions

regarding initiating, continuing, and terminating health services for managing pediatric

obesity: The protocol for a multi-center, qualitative study. BMC Health Services

Research, 12, 486-486. doi:10.1186/1472-6963-12-486

94

Bauer, K. W., Yang, Y. W., & Austin, S. B. (2004). “How can we stay healthy when you’re

throwing all of this in front of us?” Findings from focus groups and interviews in middle

schools on environmental influences on nutrition and physical activity. Health Education

& Behavior, 31, 34-46. doi:10.1177/1090198103255372

Baumrind, D. (1991). The influence of parenting style on adolescent competence and substance

use. The Journal of Early Adolescence, 11, 56-95. doi:10.1177/0272431691111004

Bayer, J. K., Ukoumunne, O. C., Lucas, N., Wake, M., Scalzo, K., & Nicholson, J. M. (2011).

Risk factors for childhood mental health symptoms: National longitudinal study of

Australian children. Pediatrics, 128, e865-e879. doi:10.1542/peds.2011-0491

Bell, L. M., Curran, J. A., Byrne, S., Roby, H., Suriano, K., Jones, T. W., & Davis, E. A. (2011).

High incidence of obesity co-morbidities in young children: A cross-sectional study.

Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health, 47, 911-917. doi:10.1111/j.1440-

1754.2011.02102.x

Birch, L. L. (2006). Child feeding practices and the etiology of obesity. Obesity, 14, 343-344.

doi:10.1038/oby.2006.45

Birch, L. L., & Davison, K. K. (2001). Family environmental factors influencing the developing

behavioral controls of food intake and childhood overweight. Pediatric Clinics of North

America, 48, 893-907. doi:10.1016/S0031-3955(05)70347-3

Birch, L. L., & Fisher, J. A. (1995). Appetite and eating behavior in children. Pediatric Clinics of

North America, 42, 931-953. doi:10.1016/S0031-3955(16)40023-4

Birch, L. L., & Fisher, J. O. (1998). Development of eating behaviors among children and

adolescents. Pediatrics, 101, 539-549. Retrieved from

http://www.adinapearson.com/kvpaHB2WmRY5/wp-

95

content/uploads/2014/09/Development-of-eatign-behaviors-among-children-and-

adolescents.pdf

Birch, L. L., & Fisher, J. O. (2000). Mothers' child-feeding practices influence daughters' eating

and weight. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 71, 1054-1061. Retrieved from

https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2530928/

Birch, L. L., Fisher, J. O., & Davison, K. K. (2003). Learning to overeat: Maternal use of

restrictive feeding practices promotes girls’ eating in the absence of hunger. The

American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 78, 215-220. Retrieved from

https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2530927/

Birch, L. L., Fisher, J. O., Grimm-Thomas, K., Markey, C. N., Sawyer, R., & Johnson, S. L.

(2001). Confirmatory factor analysis of the Child Feeding Questionnaire: A measure of

parental attitudes, beliefs and practices about child feeding and obesity proneness.

Appetite, 36, 201-210. doi:10.1006/appe.2001.0398

Birch, L. L., McPhee, L., Shoba, B. C., Pirok, E., & Steinberg, L. (1987). What kind of exposure

reduces children's food neophobia?: Looking vs. tasting. Appetite, 9, 171-178.

doi:10.1016/S0195-6663(87)80011-9

Birch, L. L., McPheee, L., Shoba, B. C., Steinberg, L., & Krehbiel, R. (1987). “Clean up your

plate”: Effects of child feeding practices on the conditioning of meal size. Learning and

Motivation, 18, 301-317. doi:10.1016/0023-9690(87)90017-8

Birch, L. L., & Ventura, A. K. (2009). Preventing childhood obesity: What works? International

Journal of Obesity, 33, S74-S81. doi:10.1038/ijo.2009.22

96

Boeije, H. (2002). A purposeful approach to the constant comparative method in the analysis of

qualitative interviews. Quality and Quantity, 36, 391-409.

doi:10.1023/A:1020909529486

Bornstein, M. H., & Zlotnik, D. (2009). Parenting styles and their effects. In J. Benson & M.

Haith (Eds.), Social and emotional development in infancy and early childhood (pp. 496-

509). Oxford, UK: Academic Press.

Brown, R., & Ogden, J. (2004). Children’s eating attitudes and behaviour: A study of the

modelling and control theories of parental influence. Health Education Research, 19,

261-271. doi:10.1093/her/cyg040

Brownell, K. D., Kelman, J. H., & Stunkard, A. J. (1983). Treatment of obese children with and

without their mothers: Changes in weight and blood pressure. Pediatrics, 71, 515-523.

Retrieved from http://pediatrics.aappublications.org.ezproxy.lib.ucalgary.ca/content/

pediatrics/71/4/515.full.pdf

Brownell, K. D. (2005). Weight bias: Nature, consequences and remedies. New York, NY:

Guilford Press.

Brylinsky, J. A., & Moore, J. C. (1994). The identification of body build stereotypes in young

children. Journal of Research in Personality, 28, 170-181. doi:10.1006/jrpe.1994.1014

Burke, V. (2006). Obesity in childhood and cardiovascular risk. Clinical and Experimental

Pharmacology and Physiology, 33, 831-837. doi:10.1111/j.1440-1681.2006.04449.x

Campbell, K. J., Crawford, D. A., & Ball, K. (2006). Family food environment and dietary

behaviors likely to promote fatness in 5–6 year-old children. International Journal of

Obesity, 30, 1272-1280. doi:10.1038/sj.ijo.0803266

97

Carels, R. A., Burmeister, J., Oehlhof, M. W., Hinman, N., LeRoy, M., Bannon, E., ... &

Ashrafloun, L. (2013). Internalized weight bias: Ratings of the self, normal weight, and

obese individuals and psychological maladjustment. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 36,

86-94. doi:10.1007/s10865-012-9402-8

Carels, R. A., Wott, C. B., Young, K. M., Gumble, A., Koball, A., & Oehlhof, M. W. (2010).

Implicit, explicit, and internalized weight bias and psychosocial maladjustment among

treatment-seeking adults. Eating Behaviors, 11, 180-185.

doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2010.03.002

Carnell, S., Cooke, L., Cheng, R., Robbins, A., & Wardle, J. (2011). Parental feeding behaviours

and motivations. A qualitative study in mothers of UK pre-schoolers. Appetite, 57, 665-

673. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2011.08.009

Carper, J. L., Fisher, J. O., & Birch, L. L. (2000). Young girls' emerging dietary restraint and

disinhibition are related to parental control in child feeding. Appetite, 35, 121-129.

doi:10.1006/appe.2000.0343

Charmaz, K. (2006). Constructing grounded theory: A practical guide through qualitative

analysis. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Charmaz, K. (2008). Grounded theory. In J. A. Smith (Ed.), Qualitative psychology: A practical

guide to research methods, (pp. 81-110). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Charmaz, K. (2014). Constructing grounded theory (2nd

ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Cooke, L. J., Chambers, L. C., Añez, E. V., & Wardle, J. (2011). Facilitating or undermining?

The effect of reward on food acceptance. A narrative review. Appetite, 57, 493-497.

doi:10.1016/j.appet.2011.06.016

98

Corbin, J., & Strauss, A. (2008). Criteria for evaluation. In J. Corbin, & A. Strauss (Eds.), Basics

of qualitative research (3rd

ed., pp. 297-312). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Costanzo, P. R., & Woody, E. Z. (1985). Domain-specific parenting styles and their impact on

the child's development of particular deviance: The example of obesity proneness.

Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 3, 425-445. doi:10.1521/jscp.1985.3.4.425

Crandall, C. S. (1995). Do parents discriminate against their heavyweight daughters? Personality

and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 724-724. doi:10.1177/0146167295217007

Crawford, P. B., Story, M., Wang, M. C., Ritchie, L. D., & Sabry, Z. I. (2001). Ethnic issues in

the epidemiology of childhood obesity. Pediatric Clinics of North America, 48, 855-878.

doi:10.1016/S0031-3955(05)70345-X

Creswell, J. W. (2007). Qualitative inquiry and research design: Choosing among five

approaches (2nd

ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Cutting, T. M., Fisher, J. O., Grimm-Thomas, K., & Birch, L. L. (1999). Like mother, like

daughter: Familial patterns of overweight are mediated by mothers' dietary disinhibition.

Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 38, 1323-1323.

doi:10.1097/00004583-199910000-00028

Daniels, S. R. (2006). The consequences of childhood overweight and obesity. The Future of

Children, 16, 47-67. doi:10.1353/foc.2006.0004

Darling, N., & Steinberg, L. (1993). Parenting style as context: An integrative model.

Psychological Bulletin, 113, 487-496. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.113.3.487

Davies, M. (2000). The Blackwell encyclopedia of social work. Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell.

99

Davison, K. K., & Birch, L. L. (2001a). Childhood overweight: A contextual model and

recommendations for future research. Obesity Reviews, 2, 159-171. doi:10.1046/j.1467-

789x.2001.00036.x

Davison, K. K., & Birch, L. L. (2001b). Weight status, parent reaction, and self-concept in five-

year-old girls. Pediatrics, 107, 46-53. doi:10.1542/peds.107.1.46

Davison, K. K., & Birch, L. L. (2004). Predictors of fat stereotypes among 9-year-old girls and

their parents. Obesity Research, 12, 86-94. doi:10.1038/oby.2004.12

Dietz, W. H. (1998). Health consequences of obesity in youth: Childhood predictors of adult

disease. Pediatrics, 101, 518-525. Retrieved from

http://go.galegroup.com.ezproxy.lib.ucalgary.ca/ps/i.do?p=AONE&sw=w&u=ucalgary&

v=2.1&it=r&id=GALE%7CA20546878&sid=summon&asid=48e5006c6e7426a6f9adc80

01efda71c

DiSantis, K. I., Hodges, E. A., Johnson, S. L., & Fisher, J. O. (2011). The role of responsive

feeding in overweight during infancy and toddlerhood: A systematic review.

International Journal of Obesity, 35, 480-492. doi:10.1038/ijo.2011.3

Durso, L. E., & Latner, J. D. (2008). Understanding self‐directed stigma: Development of the

Weight Bias Internalization Scale. Obesity, 16, S80-S86. doi:10.1038/oby.2008.448

Eisenberg, M. E., Neumark-Sztainer, D., & Story, M. (2003). Associations of weight-based

teasing and emotional well-being among adolescents. Archives of Pediatrics &

Adolescent Medicine, 157, 733-738. doi:10.1001/archpedi.157.8.733

Epstein, L. H., Paluch, R. A., Beecher, M. D., & Roemmich, J. N. (2008). Increasing healthy

eating vs. reducing high energy-dense foods to treat pediatric obesity. Obesity, 16, 318-

326. doi:10.1038/oby.2007.61

100

Epstein, L. H., Valoski, A., Wing, R. R., & McCurley, J. (1990). Ten-year follow-up of

behavioral, family-based treatment for obese children. JAMA, 264, 2519-2523.

doi:10.1001/jama.1990.03450190051027

Faith, M. S., Scanlon, K. S., Birch, L. L., Francis, L. A., & Sherry, B. (2004). Parent-child

feeding strategies and their relationships to child eating and weight status. Obesity

Research, 12, 1711-1722. doi:10.1038/oby.2004.212

Farkas, M. S., & Grolnick, W. S. (2010). Examining the components and concomitants of

parental structure in the academic domain. Motivation and Emotion, 34, 266-279.

doi:10.1007/s11031-010-9176-7

Fernandez, J. R., Klimentidis, Y. C., Dulin-Keita, A., & Casazza, K. (2012). Genetic influences

in childhood obesity: Recent progress and recommendations for experimental designs.

International Journal of Obesity, 36, 479-484. doi:10.1038/ijo.2011.236

Fisher, J. O., & Birch, L. L. (1999). Restricting access to palatable foods affects children's

behavioral response, food selection, and intake. The American Journal of Clinical

Nutrition, 69, 1264-1272. Retrieved from http://ajcn.nutrition.org/content/69/6/1264.short

Fisher, J. O., & Birch, L. L. (2002). Eating in the absence of hunger and overweight in girls from

5 to 7 y of age. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 76, 226-231. Retrieved from

https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC2604807/

Flegal, K. M., Tabak, C. J., & Ogden, C. L. (2006). Overweight in children: Definitions and

interpretation. Health Education Research, 21, 755-760. doi:10.1093/her/cyl128

Francis, L. A., & Birch, L. L. (2005). Maternal influences on daughters' restrained eating

behavior. Health Psychology, 24, 548-554. doi:10.1037/0278-6133.24.6.548

101

Francis, L. A., Hofer, S. M., & Birch, L. L. (2001). Predictors of maternal child-feeding style:

Maternal and child characteristics. Appetite, 37, 231-243. doi:10.1006/appe.2001.0427

Francis, L. A., & Susman, E. J. (2009). Self-regulation and rapid weight gain in children from

age 3 to 12 years. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 163, 297-302.

doi:10.1001/archpediatrics.2008.579

Freedman, D. S., Dietz, W. H., Srinivasan, S. R., & Berenson, G. S. (1999). The relation of

overweight to cardiovascular risk factors among children and adolescents: The Bogalusa

Heart Study. Pediatrics, 103, 1175-1182. doi:10.1542/peds.103.6.1175

Gable, S., & Lutz, S. (2000). Household, parent, and child contributions to childhood obesity.

Family Relations, 49, 293-300. doi:10.1111/j.1741-3729.2000.00293.x

Ghezelije, T. N. & Emami, A. (2009). Grounded theory: Methodology and philosophical

perspective. Nurse Researcher, 17, 15-23. Retrieved from

http://nurseresearcher.rcnpublishing.co.uk

Glaser, B. G. (1992). Emergence vs. forcing: Basics of grounded theory analysis. Mill Valley,

CA: Sociological Press.

Glaser, B. G., & Strauss, A. L. (1967). The discovery of grounded theory. Chicago, IL: Aldine.

Golan, M., & Crow, S. (2004). Parents are key players in the prevention and treatment of weight-

related problems. Nutrition Reviews, 62, 39-50. Retrieved from

https://www.researchgate.net/profile/Moria_Golan/publication/8680901_Parents_Are_Ke

y_Players_in_the_Prevention_and_Treatment_of_Weight-

Related_Problems/links/00b4952a5efc770e17000000.pdf

102

Graves, T., Meyers, A. W., & Clark, L. (1988). An evaluation of parental problem-solving

training in the behavioral treatment of childhood obesity. Journal of Consulting and

Clinical Psychology, 56, 246-250. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.56.2.246

Gray, W. N., Janicke, D. M., Wistedt, K. M., & Dumont-Driscoll, M. C. (2010). Factors

associated with parental use of restrictive feeding practices to control their children's food

intake. Appetite, 55, 332-337. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2010.07.005.

Grier, S. A., Mensinger, J., Huang, S. H., Kumanyika, S. K., & Stettler, N. (2007). Fast-food

marketing and children's fast-food consumption: Exploring parents' influences in an

ethnically diverse sample. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 26, 221-235.

doi:10.1509/jppm.26.2.221

Grolnick, W. S., & Pomerantz, E. M. (2009). Issues and challenges in studying parental control:

Toward a new conceptualization. Child Development Perspectives, 3, 165-170.

doi:10.1111/j.1750-8606.2009.00099.x

Guerrieri, R., Nederkoorn, C., & Jansen, A. (2007). How impulsiveness and variety influence

food intake in a sample of healthy women. Appetite, 48, 119-122.

doi:10.1016/j.appet.2006.06.004

Guxens, M., Mendez, M. A., Julvez, J., Plana, E., Forns, J., Basagaña, X., ... & Sunyer, J. (2009).

Cognitive function and overweight in preschool children. American Journal of

Epidemiology, 170, 438-446. doi:10.1093/aje/kwp140

Harper, L. V., & Sanders, K. M. (1975). The effect of adults' eating on young children's

acceptance of unfamiliar foods. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 20, 206-214.

doi:10.1016/0022-0965(75)90098-3

103

Hayden-Wade, H. A., Stein, R. I., Ghaderi, A., Saelens, B. E., Zabinski, M. F., & Wilfley, D. E.

(2005). Prevalence, characteristics, and correlates of teasing experiences among

overweight children vs. non-overweight peers. Obesity Research, 13, 1381-1392.

doi:10.1038/oby.2005.167

Heath, H., & Cowley, S. (2004). Developing a grounded theory approach: A comparison of

Glaser and Strauss. International Journal of Nursing Studies, 41, 141-150.

doi:10.1016/S0020-7489(03)00113-5

Henwood, K. L., & Pidgeon, N. F. (2003). Grounded theory in psychological research. In P.

Camic, J. Rhodes, & L. Yardley (Eds.), Qualitative research in psychology: Expanding

perspectives in methodology and design (pp. 131-155). Washington, DC: APA

Publications.

Hetherington, M. M., Cecil, J. E., Jackson, D. M., & Schwartz, C. (2011). Feeding infants and

young children. From guidelines to practice. Appetite, 57, 791-795.

doi:10.1016/j.appet.2011.07.005

Ho, M., Garnett, S. P., Baur, L., Burrows, T., Stewart, L., Neve, M., & Collins, C. (2012).

Effectiveness of lifestyle interventions in child obesity: Systematic review with meta-

analysis. Pediatrics, 130, e1647-e1671. doi:10.1542/peds.2012-1176

Holland, J. C., Kolko, R. P., Stein, R. I., Welch, R. R., Perri, M. G., Schechtman, K. B., ... &

Wilfley, D. E. (2014). Modifications in parent feeding practices and child diet during

family‐based behavioral treatment improve child zBMI. Obesity, 22, E119-E126.

doi:10.1002/oby.20708

104

Holt, N. L., Moylan, B. A., Spence, J. C., Lenk, J. M., Sehn, Z. L., & Ball, G. D. (2008).

Treatment preferences of overweight youth and their parents in Western Canada.

Qualitative Health Research, 18, 1206-1219. doi:10.1177/1049732308321740

Hubbs-Tait, L., Kennedy, T. S., Page, M. C., Topham, G. L., & Harrist, A. W. (2008). Parental

feeding practices predict authoritative, authoritarian, and permissive parenting styles.

Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 108, 1154-1161.

doi:10.1016/j.jada.2008.04.008

Hughes, S. O., Power, T. G., Fisher, J. O., Mueller, S., & Nicklas, T. A. (2005). Revisiting a

neglected construct: Parenting styles in a child-feeding context. Appetite, 44, 83-92.

doi:10.1016/j.appet.2004.08.007

Hutchinson, S. A., & Wilson, H. S. (2001). Grounded theory: The method. In P. L. Munhall

(Ed.), Nursing research, a qualitative perspective (3rd

ed., pp. 209-244). Mississauga,

ON: Jones and Barlett.

Israel, A. C., Stolmaker, L., & Andrian, C. A. (1985). The effects of training parents in general

child management skills on a behavioral weight loss program for children. Behavior

Therapy, 16, 169-180. doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(85)80043-5

Jackson, C., Henriksen, L., & Foshee, V. A. (1998). The Authoritative Parenting Index:

Predicting health risk behaviors among children and adolescents. Health Education &

Behavior, 25, 319-337. doi:10.1177/109019819802500307

Jansen, E., Daniels, L. A., & Nicholson, J. M. (2012). The dynamics of parenting and early

feeding–constructs and controversies: A viewpoint. Early Child Development and Care,

182, 967-981. doi:10.1080/03004430.2012.678593

105

Johnson, S. L. (2000). Improving preschoolers' self-regulation of energy intake. Pediatrics, 106,

1429-1435. doi:10.1542/peds.106.6.1429

Johnson, S. L., & Birch, L. L. (1994). Parents' and children's adiposity and eating style.

Pediatrics, 94, 653-661. Retrieved from

http://pediatrics.aappublications.org.ezproxy.lib.ucalgary.ca/content/pediatrics/94/5/653.f

ull.pdf

Joyce, J. L., & Zimmer-Gembeck, M. J. (2009). Parent feeding restriction and child weight. The

mediating role of child disinhibited eating and the moderating role of the parenting

context. Appetite, 52, 726-734. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2009.03.015

Kiess, W., Reich, A., Muller, G., Meyer, K., Galler, A., Bennek, J., & Kratzsch, J. (2001).

Clinical aspects of obesity in childhood and adolescence-diagnosis, treatment and

prevention. International Journal of Obesity & Related Metabolic Disorders, 25, S75-

S79. doi:10.1038/sj.ijo.0801704

Kirschenbaum, D. S., Harris, E. S., & Tomarken, A. J. (1984). Effects of parental involvement in

behavioral weight loss therapy for preadolescents. Behavior Therapy, 15, 485-500.

doi:10.1016/S0005-7894(84)80051-9

Kraig, K. A., & Keel, P. K. (2001). Weight-based stigmatization in children. International

Journal of Obesity, 25, 1661-1666. doi:10.1038/sj.ijo.0801813

Kral, T. V., & Rauh, E. M. (2010). Eating behaviors of children in the context of their family

environment. Physiology & Behavior, 100, 567-573. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.04.031

Latner, J. D., Barile, J. P., Durso, L. E., & O'Brien, K. S. (2014). Weight and health-related

quality of life: The moderating role of weight discrimination and internalized weight bias.

Eating Behaviors, 15, 586-590. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2014.08.014

106

Latner, J. D., & Stunkard, A. J. (2003). Getting worse: The stigmatization of obese children.

Obesity Research, 11, 452-456. doi:10.1038/oby.2003.61

Lindsay, A. C., Sussner, K. M., Greaney, M. L., & Peterson, K. E. (2011). Latina mothers'

beliefs and practices related to weight status, feeding, and the development of child

overweight. Public Health Nursing, 28, 107-118. doi:10.1111/j.1525-1446.2010.00906.x

Lichtman, M. (2013). Qualitative research in education: A user’s guide (3rd

ed.). Los Angeles,

CA: Sage.

Lucas, N., Maguire, B., & Nicholson, J. M. (2010). Parenting practices and behaviours.

Australian Institute of Family Studies LSAC Annual Statistical Report, 2010, 43-56.

Retrieved from http://growingupinaustralia.gov.au/pubs/asr/2010/asr2010.pdf#page=59

Ludwig, D. S., & Ebbeling, C. B. (2001). Type 2 diabetes mellitus in children: Primary care and

public health considerations. JAMA, 286, 1427-1430. doi:10.1001/jama.286.12.1427

Lytle, L. A., Varnell, S., Murray, D. M., Story, M., Perry, C., Birnbaum, A. S., et al. (2003).

Predicting adolescents' intake of fruits and vegetables. Journal of Nutrition Education

and Behavior, 35, 170-178. doi:10.1016/S1499-4046(06)60331-X

Maccoby, E., & Martin, J. (1983). Socialization in the context of the family: Parent-child

interaction. In P. H. Mussen & E. M. Hetherington (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology:

Socialization, personality, and social development (Vol. 4, pp. 1-101). New York, NY:

Wiley.

Marbell, K. N., & Grolnick, W. S. (2013). Correlates of parental control and autonomy support

in an interdependent culture: A look at Ghana. Motivation and Emotion, 37, 79-92.

doi:10.1007/s11031-012-9289-2

107

McCann, T. V., & Clark, E. (2003). Grounded theory in nursing research: Part 1 -

Methodology. Nurse Researcher, 11, 7-18. Retrieved from

http://go.galegroup.com.ezproxy.lib.ucalgary.ca/ps/i.do?id=GALE%7CA111897099&sid

=summon&v=2.1&u=ucalgary&it=r&p=AONE&sw=w&asid=641e6032514a2afe19be25

0c484f0b20

McCann, T. V., & Clark, E. (2003a). Grounded theory in nursing research: Part 2 -

Critique. Nurse Researcher, 11, 19-28. Retrieved from

http://go.galegroup.com.ezproxy.lib.ucalgary.ca/ps/i.do?id=GALE%7CA111897100&sid

=summon&v=2.1&u=ucalgary&it=r&p=AONE&sw=w&asid=74a5f02cc69752fd1a3abb

2b3389d698

Miles, M. B., Huberman, A. M., & Saldaña, J. (2014). Qualitative data analysis: A methods

sourcebook. (3rd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Mills, J., Bonner, A., & Francis, K. (2006). The development of constructivist grounded theory.

International Journal of Qualitative Methods, 5, 1-10. Retrieved from

http://ejournals.library.ualberta.ca/index.php/IJQM/index

Mischler E. G. (1979). Meaning in context: Is there any other kind? Harvard Educational

Review, 49, 1-19. Retrieved from

http://www.metapress.com.ezproxy.lib.ucalgary.ca/content/b748n4133677245p/

Monasta, L., Batty, G. D., Cattaneo, A., Lutje, V., Ronfani, L., Van Lenthe, F. J., & Brug, J.

(2010). Early-life determinants of overweight and obesity: A review of systematic

reviews. Obesity Reviews, 11, 695-708. doi:10.1111/j.1467-789X.2010.00735.x

108

Musher-Eizenman, D. R., Holub, S. C., Hauser, J. C., & Young, K. M. (2007). The relationship

between parents’ anti-fat attitudes and restrictive feeding. Obesity, 15, 2095-2102.

doi:0.1038/oby.2007.249.

Must, A., & Strauss, R. S. (1999). Risks and consequences of childhood and adolescent obesity.

International Journal of Obesity & Related Metabolic Disorders, 23, S2-S11.

doi:10.1038/sj/ijo/0800852

Nederkoorn, C., Braet, C., Van Eijs, Y., Tanghe, A., & Jansen, A. (2006). Why obese children

cannot resist food: The role of impulsivity. Eating Behaviors, 7, 315-322.

doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2005.11.005

Nederkoorn, C., Smulders, F. T., Havermans, R. C., Roefs, A., & Jansen, A. (2006). Impulsivity

in obese women. Appetite, 47, 253-256. doi:10.1016/j.appet.2006.05.008

Neumark-Sztainer, D., Falkner, N., Story, M., Perry, C., Hannan, P. J., & Mulert, S. (2002).

Weight-teasing among adolescents: Correlations with weight status and disordered eating

behaviors. International Journal of Obesity, 26, 123-131. doi:10.1038/sj.ijo.0801853

Neumark-Sztainer, D., Story, M., & Faibisch, L. (1998). Perceived stigmatization among

overweight African-American and Caucasian adolescent girls. Journal of Adolescent

Health, 23, 264-270.

Neumark-Sztainer, D., Story, M., & Harris, T. (1999). Beliefs and attitudes about obesity among

teachers and school health care providers working with adolescents. Journal of Nutrition

Education, 31, 3-9. doi:10.1016/S0022-3182(99)70378-X

O'Donnell, S. I., Hoerr, S. L., Mendoza, J. A., & Goh, E. T. (2008). Nutrient quality of fast food

kids meals. The American Journal of Clinical Nutrition, 88, 1388-1395. Retrieved from

http://s3.amazonaws.com/academia.edu.documents/39858638/1388.pdf?AWSAccessKey

109

Id=AKIAIWOWYYGZ2Y53UL3A&Expires=1492639393&Signature=SJq5zIA09snX3

YHmzznmyICsLAg%3D&response-content-

disposition=inline%3B%20filename%3DNutrient_quality_of_fast_food_kids_meals.pdf

Ogden, J., Reynolds, R., & Smith, A. (2006). Expanding the concept of parental control: A role

for overt and covert control in children's snacking behaviour? Appetite, 47, 100-106.

doi:10.1016/j.appet.2006.03.330

Park, M. H., Falconer, C., Viner, R. M., & Kinra, S. (2012). The impact of childhood obesity on

morbidity and mortality in adulthood: A systematic review. Obesity Reviews, 13, 985-

1000. doi:10.1111/j.1467-789X.2012.01015.x

Pearl, R. L., White, M. A., & Grilo, C. M. (2014). Overvaluation of shape and weight as a

mediator between self-esteem and weight bias internalization among patients with binge

eating disorder. Eating Behaviors, 15, 259-261. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2014.03.005

Philippas, N. G., & Lo, C. W. (2004). Childhood obesity: Etiology, prevention, and treatment.

Nutrition in Clinical Care: An Official Publication of Tufts University, 8, 77-88.

Retrieved from http://dc8qa4cy3n.search.serialssolutions.com/?ctx_ver=Z39.88-

2004&ctx_enc=info%3Aofi%2Fenc%3AUTF-

8&rfr_id=info%3Asid%2Fsummon.serialssolutions.com&rft_val_fmt=info%3Aofi%2Ff

mt%3Akev%3Amtx%3Ajournal&rft.genre=article&rft.atitle=Childhood+obesity%3A+et

iology%2C+prevention%2C+and+treatment&rft.jtitle=Nutrition+in+clinical+care+%3A

+an+official+publication+of+Tufts+University&rft.au=Philippas%2C+Niki+G&rft.au=L

o%2C+Clifford+W&rft.date=2005-04-01&rft.issn=1096-6781&rft.eissn=1523-

5408&rft.volume=8&rft.issue=2&rft.spage=77&rft_id=info%3Apmid%2F16013226&rft

.externalDocID=16013226&paramdict=en-US

110

Puhl, R., & Brownell, K. D. (2001). Bias, discrimination, and obesity. Obesity, 9, 788-805.

doi:10.1038/oby.2001.108

Puhl, R. M., & Latner, J. D. (2007). Stigma, obesity, and the health of the nation's children.

Psychological Bulletin, 133, 557-580. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.4.557

Raitakari, O. T., Juonala, M., & Viikari, J. S. A. (2005). Obesity in childhood and vascular

changes in adulthood: Insights into the cardiovascular risk in young Finns study.

International Journal of Obesity, 29, S101-S104. doi:10.1038/sj.ijo.0803085

Raskin, J. D. (2002). Constructivism in psychology: Personal construct psychology, radical

constructivism, and social constructionism. In J. D. Raskin & S. K. Bridges (Eds.),

Studies in meaning: Exploring constructivist psychology (pp. 1-25). New York, NY: Pace

University Press.

Reedy, J., & Krebs-Smith, S. M. (2010). Dietary sources of energy, solid fats, and added sugars

among children and adolescents in the United States. Journal of the American Dietetic

Association, 110, 1477-1484. doi:10.1016/j.jada.2010.07.010

Rifas-Shiman, S. L., Sherry, B., Scanlon, K., Birch, L. L., Gillman, M. W., & Taveras, E. M.

(2011). Does maternal feeding restriction lead to childhood obesity in a prospective

cohort study? Archives of Disease in Childhood, 96, 265-269.

doi:10.1136/adc.2009.175240

Riggs, N. R., Spruijt-Metz, D., Sakuma, K. L., Chou, C. P., & Pentz, M. A. (2010). Executive

cognitive function and food intake in children. Journal of Nutrition Education and

Behavior, 42, 398-403. doi:10.1016/j.jneb.2009.11.003

111

Ristovski-Slijepcevic, S., Chapman, G. E., & Beagan, B. L. (2010). Being a ‘good mother’:

Dietary governmentality in the family food practices of three ethnocultural groups in

Canada. Health, 14, 467-483. doi:10.1177/1363459309357267

Rhee, K. (2008). Childhood overweight and the relationship between parent behaviors, parenting

style, and family functioning. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and

Social Science, 615, 12-37. doi:10.1177/0002716207308400

Rhee, K. E., Coleman, S. M., Appugliese, D. P., Kaciroti, N. A., Corwyn, R. F., Davidson, N. S.,

... & Lumeng, J. C. (2009). Maternal feeding practices become more controlling after and

not before excessive rates of weight gain. Obesity, 17, 1724-1729.

doi:10.1038/oby.2009.54

Roberts, K. C., Shields, M., de Groh, M., Aziz, A., & Gilbert, J. A. (2012). Overweight and

obesity in children and adolescents: Results from the 2009 to 2011 Canadian Health

Measures Survey. Health Reports, 23, 37-41. Retrieved from

http://s3.amazonaws.com/academia.edu.documents/44335748/Overweight_and_obesity_i

n_children_and_a20160402-18206-

7rm2uu.pdf?AWSAccessKeyId=AKIAIWOWYYGZ2Y53UL3A&Expires=1492640517

&Signature=%2FUKfV%2FSA42Ltt2xu2QbWrsNFKjk%3D&response-content-

disposition=inline%3B%20filename%3DOverweight_and_obesity_in_children_and_a.pd

f#page=39

Robinson, S., Marriott, L., Poole, J., Crozier, S., Borland, S., Lawrence, W., et al. (2007).

Dietary patterns in infancy: The importance of maternal and family influences on feeding

practice. British Journal of Nutrition, 98, 1029-1037. doi:10.1017/s0007114507750936

112

Rudman, L. A., Feinberg, J., & Fairchild, K. (2002). Minority members' implicit attitudes:

Automatic ingroup bias as a function of group status. Social Cognition, 20, 294-320.

doi:10.1521/soco.20.4.294.19908

Russell-Mayhew, S. (in press). The weight of the world in her hands. Invited chapter. In N.

Arthur & C. Collins (Eds.), Culture-Infused Counselling (3rd

ed.). Counselling Concepts.

Russell-Mayhew, S., Nutter, S., Alberga, A., Jelinski, S., Ball, G. D., Edwards, A., ... & Forhan,

M. (2016). Environmental scan of weight bias exposure in primary health care training

programs. The Canadian Journal for the Scholarship of Teaching and Learning, 7, 1-14.

doi:10.5206/cjsotl-rcacea.2016.2.5

Russell-Mayhew, S., Nutter, S., Ireland, A., Gabriele, T., Bardick, A., Crooks, J., & Peat, G.

(2015). Pilot testing a professional development model for preservice teachers in the area

of health and weight: Feasibility, utility, and efficacy. Advances in School Mental Health

Promotion, 8, 176-186. doi:10.1080/1754730X.2015.1040040

Sacher, P. M., Kolotourou, M., Chadwick, P. M., Cole, T. J., Lawson, M. S., Lucas, A., &

Singhal, A. (2010). Randomized controlled trial of the MEND program: A family‐based

community intervention for childhood obesity. Obesity, 18, S62-S68.

doi:10.1038/oby.2009.433

Satter, E. M. (1986). The feeding relationship. Journal of American Dietetic Association, 86,

352-356. Retrieved from http://www.adinapearson.com/kvpaHB2WmRY5/wp-

content/uploads/2014/09/The-feeding-relationship.pdf

Satter, E. (1990). The feeding relationship: Problems and interventions. The Journal of

Pediatrics, 117, 181-189. doi:10.1016/S0022-3476(05)80017-4

113

Savage, J. S., Fisher, J. O., & Birch, L. L. (2007). Parental influence on eating behavior:

Conception to adolescence. The Journal of Law, Medicine & Ethics, 35, 22-34.

doi:10.1111/j.1748-720X.2007.00111.x

Savoye, M., Shaw, M., Dziura, J., Tamborlane, W. V., Rose, P., Guandalini, C., ... & Caprio, S.

(2007). Effects of a weight management program on body composition and metabolic

parameters in overweight children: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA, 297, 2697-

2704. doi:10.1001/jama.297.24.2697

Schvey, N. A., & White, M. A. (2015). The internalization of weight bias is associated with

severe eating pathology among lean individuals. Eating Behaviors, 17, 1-5.

doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2014.11.001

Schvey, N. A., Roberto, C. A., & White, M. A. (2013). Clinical correlates of the Weight Bias

Internalization Scale in overweight adults with binge and purge behaviours. Advances in

Eating Disorders: Theory, Research and Practice, 1, 213-223.

doi:10.1080/21662630.2013.794523

Shomaker, L. B., & Furman, W. (2009). Interpersonal influences on late adolescent girls' and

boys' disordered eating. Eating Behaviors, 10, 97-106. doi:10.1016/j.eatbeh.2009.02.003

Shunk, J. A., & Birch, L. L. (2004). Girls at risk for overweight at age 5 are at risk for dietary

restraint, disinhibited overeating, weight concerns, and greater weight gain from 5 to 9

years. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 104, 1120-1126.

doi:10.1016/j.jada.2004.04.031

Skouteris, H., McCabe, M., Swinburn, B., Newgreen, V., Sacher, P., & Chadwick, P. (2011).

Parental influence and obesity prevention in pre-schoolers: A systematic review of

interventions. Obesity Reviews, 12, 315-328. doi:10.1111/j.1467-789X.2010.00751.x

114

Sleddens, E. F. C., Gerards, S. M. P. L., Thijs, C., de Vries, N. K., & Kremers, S. P. J. (2011).

General parenting, childhood overweight and obesity-inducing behaviors: A review.

International Journal of Pediatric Obesity, 6, e12-e27.

doi:10.3109/17477166.2011.566339

Smith, M. (2011). Measures for assessing parenting in research and practice. Child and

Adolescent Mental Health, 16, 158-166. doi:10.1111/j.1475-3588.2010.00585.x

Snethen, J. A., Broome, M. E., & Cashin, S. E. (2006). Effective weight loss for overweight

children: A meta-analysis of intervention studies. Journal of Pediatric Nursing, 21, 45-

56. doi:10.1016/j.pedn.2005.06.006

Spruijt-Metz, D. (2011). Etiology, treatment, and prevention of obesity in childhood and

adolescence: A decade in review. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21, 129-152.

doi:10.1111/j.1532-7795.2010.00719.x

Spruijt-Metz, D., Li, C., Cohen, E., Birch, L., & Goran, M. (2006). Longitudinal influence of

mother’s child-feeding practices on adiposity in children. The Journal of Pediatrics, 148,

314-320. doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2005.10.035

Strauss A. L. & Corbin J. M. (1990). Basics of qualitative research. Newbury Park, CA: Sage

Publications.

Strauss, R. S., & Pollack, H. A. (2003). Social marginalization of overweight children. Archives

of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 157, 746-752. doi:10.1001/archpedi.157.8.746

Storch, E. A., Milsom, V. A., DeBraganza, N., Lewin, A. B., Geffken, G. R., & Silverstein, J. H.

(2007). Peer victimization, psychosocial adjustment, and physical activity in overweight

and at-risk-for-overweight youth. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32, 80-89.

doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsj113

115

Thompson, M. E. (2010). Parental feeding and childhood obesity in preschool-age children:

Recent findings from the literature. Issues in Comprehensive Pediatric Nursing, 33, 205-

267. doi:10.3109/01460862.2010.530057

Tweed, A., & Charmaz, K. (2012). Grounded theory methods for mental health practitioners. In

D. Harper & A. R. Thompson (Eds.), Qualitative research methods in mental health and

Psychotherapy: A guide for students and practitioners (pp.131-146). Oxford, UK: Wiley-

Blackwell.

Ventura, A. K., & Birch, L. L. (2008). Does parenting affect children's eating and weight status?

International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 5, 15-27.

doi:10.1186/1479-5868-5-15

Vereecken, C., Legiest, E., De Bourdeaudhuij, I., & Maes, L. (2009). Associations between

general parenting styles and specific food-related parenting practices and children's food

consumption. American Journal of Health Promotion, 23, 233-240.

doi:10.4278/ajhp.07061355

Vivier, P., & Tompkins, C. (2008). Health consequences of obesity in children and adolescents.

In E. Jelalian & R. G. Steele (Eds.), Handbook of childhood and adolescent obesity (pp.

11-24). New York, NY: Springer US.

Vos, M. B., & Welsh, J. (2010). Childhood obesity: Update on predisposing factors and

prevention strategies. Current Gastroenterology Reports, 12, 280-287.

doi:10.1007/s11894-010-0116-1

Wabitsch, M. (2000). Overweight and obesity in European children: Definition and diagnostic

procedures, risk factors and consequences for later health outcome. European Journal of

Pediatrics, 159, S8-S13. doi:10.1007/PL00014368

116

Wake, M., Nicholson, J. M., Hardy, P., & Smith, K. (2007). Preschooler obesity and parenting

styles of mothers and fathers: Australian national population study. Pediatrics, 120,

E1520-E1527. doi:10.1542/peds.2006-3707

Walker, L., & Kirby, R. (2010). Conceptual and measurement issues in early parenting practices

research: An epidemiologic perspective. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 14, 958-

970. doi:10.1007/s10995-009-0532-8

Wang, S. S., Brownell, K. D., & Wadden, T. A. (2004). The influence of the stigma of obesity on

overweight individuals. International Journal of Obesity, 28, 1333-1337.

doi:10.1038/sj.ijo.0802730

Wardle, J., Sanderson, S., Guthrie, C. A., Rapoport, L., & Plomin, R. (2002). Parental feeding

style and the inter-generational transmission of obesity risk. Obesity Research, 10, 453-

462. doi:10.1038/oby.2002.63

Webber, L., Cooke, L., Hill, C., & Wardle, J. (2010). Associations between children's appetitive

traits and maternal feeding practices. Journal of the American Dietetic Association, 110,

1718-1722. doi:10.1016/j.jada.2010.08.007

Weiss, D. L. (2013). The heavy: A mother, a daughter, a diet. New York, NY: Ballantine Books.

Weiss, R., & Caprio, S. (2005). The metabolic consequences of childhood obesity. Best Practice

& Research Clinical Endocrinology & Metabolism, 19, 405-419.

doi:10.1016/j.beem.2005.04.009

Whitaker, R. C., Wright, J. A., Pepe, M. S., Seidel, K. D., & Dietz, W. H. (1997). Predicting

obesity in young adulthood from childhood and parental obesity. New England Journal of

Medicine, 337, 869-873. doi:10.1056/NEJM199709253371301

117

Wilson, W., & Barn, H. T. (2012). Constructive grounded theory in the search for meaning on

global mindedness: A novice researcher’s guide. International Journal of Evidence Based

Coaching and Mentoring, 6, 1-14. Retrieved from

http://ijebcm.brookes.ac.uk/documents/special06-paper-01.pdf

World Health Organization. (2014). Commission on Ending Childhood Obesity: Facts and

Figures on Childhood Obesity. World Health Organization. Retrieved from

http://www.who.int/end-childhood-obesity/facts/en/

World Health Organization. (2016). Media Centre: Obesity and Overweight Fact Sheet. World

Health Organization. Retrieved from

http://www.who.int/mediacentre/factsheets/fs311/en/

Young, R. A., & Collin, A. (2004). Introduction: Constructivism and social constructionism in

the career field. Journal of Vocational Behaviour, 64, 373-388.

doi:10.1016/j.jvb.2003.12.005

Young, K. M., Northern, J. J., Lister, K. M., Drummond, J. A., & O'Brien, W. H. (2007). A

meta-analysis of family-behavioral weight-loss treatments for children. Clinical

Psychology Review, 27, 240-249. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2006.08.003

118

Appendix A

Recruitment Poster

Werklund School of Education

Did your parents put you on a

diet as a child?

Are you between the ages of 18-29?

Participate in an interview study!

If you were put on a diet by your parents as a child, and are between the ages

of 18 and 29, you may be eligible to participate.

You will be asked to answer questions such as “What was your childhood

experience of being put on a diet by your parents?” and “How do you think

parental dietary control has affected you?”

If you are interested, please contact the researcher, Isabel Brun at

[email protected]

This study has been approved by the University of Calgary Conjoint Faculties Research Ethics

Board.

Isab

el Bru

n

ibru

n@

uca

lgary

.ca

Isab

el Bru

n

ibru

n@

uca

lgary

.ca

Isab

el Bru

n

ibru

n@

uca

lgary

.ca

Isab

el Bru

n

ibru

n@

uca

lgary

.ca

Isab

el Bru

n

ibru

n@

uca

lgary

.ca

Isab

el Bru

n

ibru

n@

uca

lgary

.ca

Isab

el Bru

n

ibru

n@

uca

lgary

.ca

Isab

el Bru

n

ibru

n@

uca

lgary

.ca

119

Appendix B

Recruitment Screening Questions

Thank you for your interest in my study. My primary research inquiry is to understand how

young adults describe their childhood experiences of being put on a diet by their parents.

I’d like to ask you a few questions to ensure that you fit participation criteria for my research

study on childhood experiences of parental feeding control.

1. Are you a young adult (ages 18-29)?

2. Have you ever been put on a diet by your parents during your childhood?

3. Did your parents put you on a diet the purpose of weight loss promotion or weight gain

prevention?

4. Have you ever experienced impaired memory (in ways that might get in the way of you

remembering or talking about these childhood experiences)?

5. Was the parental feeding control you experienced a consequence of a pattern of neglect

or abuse? (versus parents just trying to ‘do the right thing’)

6. Do you have any condition that causes impaired perceptions?

120

Appendix C

Consent Form

Name of Researcher, Faculty, Department, Telephone & Email:

Isabel Brun, Werklund School of Education, Counselling Psychology, (506) 999-2524, [email protected]

Supervisor:

Dr. Shelly Russell-Mayhew, Werklund School of Education, Counselling Psychology, (403) 220-8375,

[email protected]

Title of Project:

How do young adults describe their childhood experiences of being put on diets by their parents?

Sponsor:

None

This consent form, a copy of which has been given to you, is only part of the process of informed consent. If you

want more details about something mentioned here, or information not included here, you should feel free to ask.

Please take the time to read this carefully and to understand any accompanying information.

The University of Calgary Conjoint Faculties Research Ethics Board has approved this research study.

Purpose of the Study

To examine remembered childhood experiences of parental feeding control.

Study Rationale

In Canada, childhood obesity continues to be a serious public health concern. Research demonstrates that

children with obesity are at a greater risk for developing a variety of health issues, and are more susceptible to

weight-related bullying, stigmatization, and marginalization than their non-obese peers. Parents of children who

struggle with their weight might want to protect their children from the previously mentioned negative

consequences. One way of protecting their children might be to control their food intake (i.e., put them on a diet)

in order to try to manage their weight and size.

Parental feeding control practices are often implemented with the best of intentions. Yet, research suggests that

these practices can have a negative impact on children’s eating behaviours. Despite these concerns, minimal

121

research has been conducted to thoroughly examine how children experience these practices. In order to attend to

this gap in research, the current study will examine remembered childhood experiences of parental feeding control

by asking the following question: How do young adults describe their childhood experiences of being put on diets

by their parents?

What Will I Be Asked To Do?

Should you agree to participate, you will be asked to share your childhood experiences of being put on a diet by

your parents in an individual face-to-face interview. During the interview, we will discuss your experiences, and I

will ask you about how these practices have impacted you socially and psychologically. I will also ask you about

any further thoughts and views on this topic, and will make sure I understand your experiences well. Interview

questions will be sent to you via e-mail prior to the interview for your consideration. I will use an audio recorder

to record all interviews, and will transcribe the interviews verbatim, to ensure accuracy of data analysis.

Additionally, I will conduct the interview in a private room at the University of Calgary, or another quiet and

private place of your choosing. I will be emailing you initial results so that you can confirm or adjust the

information to ensure accuracy. Your participation in the interview portion of the study will require

approximately 60 – 90 minutes of your time, and is completely voluntary - you may withdraw from the study at

any time before or during the interview process. You may also decline to answer any of interview questions. If

you choose to withdraw, you may verbally indicate this to the researcher. Should you choose to withdraw before

the interview is complete, you will be asked if data up to that point may be retained for data analysis and your

decision will be honoured. Data cannot be withdrawn after the interview process is complete.

What Type of Personal Information Will Be Collected

Should you agree to participate, you will be asked to provide your, as well as your parent’s, demographic

information including: gender, age, level of education, household income, and ethnic background. This

information, along with the audio-recorded interview, will be kept separate from your name and will be identified

only by a pseudonym of your choosing. Only the primary researcher and her supervisor will have access to the

audio-recordings and demographic information.

Are there Risks or Benefits if I Participate?

There are no foreseeable risks, harms, or inconveniences to you as the participant. The interview questions will

ask about childhood experiences related to parental feeding control. The focus will be on the impacts of such

practices. If the interview topics cause negative thoughts or emotional distress, you are encouraged to let the

researcher know, and a counselling appointment may be set up through the University of Calgary Wellness Centre

(if you are a student), or through the Calgary Counselling Centre.

The University of Calgary Counselling Center is located in the Wellness Centre

(Room 370, MacEwan Student Centre)

Open Monday to Friday from 9:00-4:30

Ph: 403 210-9355 (210-WELL)

The Calgary Counselling Centre is located downtown Calgary

#200, 940 6th Ave, SW, Calgary

Ph: 403 265-4980

122

You may find it interesting to talk about your childhood experiences of parental feeding control practices. Your

participation in this study may also help to identify potential impacts of parental feeding control practices which

could inform parenting strategies related to weight issues. You will receive a $20.00 gift certificate upon arriving

at the interview site.

What Happens to the Information I Provide?

Participation is completely voluntary, anonymous and confidential. You are free to discontinue participation at

any time before or during the interview process. If you choose to withdraw from the study, you will be asked if

the information provided up to that point can be kept for analysis, and your decision will be honoured.

No one except the researcher and her supervisor will be allowed to hear any of the answers on the interview tape.

There are no names on the questionnaire. Group information will be summarized for any presentation or

publication of results. The demographic questionnaires, which include participant and parental demographic

information, will be kept in a locked cabinet only accessible by the researcher and her supervisor. The anonymous

data will be stored for five years on a computer disk, at which time, it will be permanently erased. The data you

provide will be used towards completion of Master’s thesis.

Signatures

Your signature on this form indicates that 1) you understand to your satisfaction the information provided to you

about your participation in this research project, and 2) you agree to participate in the research project.

In no way does this waive your legal rights nor release the investigators, sponsors, or involved institutions from

their legal and professional responsibilities. You are free to withdraw from this research project at any time before

or during the interview process. You should feel free to ask for clarification or new information throughout your

participation.

Participant’s Pseudonym: ______________________________________________________________________

Participant’s Name: (please print) ________________________________________________________

Participant’s Signature: __________________________________________ Date: ________________

Researcher’s Name: (please print) _______________________________________________________

Researcher’s Signature: ________________________________________ Date: _________________

123

Questions/Concerns

If you have any further questions or want clarification regarding this research and/or your participation,

please contact:

Ms. Isabel Brun

Werklund School of Education, Department of Counselling Psychology

(506) 999-2524, [email protected]

and Dr. Shelly Russell-Mayhew, Werklund School of Education, Department of Counselling Psychology

(403) 220-8375, [email protected]

If you have any concerns about the way you have been treated as a participant, please contact an Ethic

Resource Officer, Research Services Office, University of Calgary at (403) 210-9863; email

[email protected]

A copy of this consent form has been given to you to keep for your records and reference. The

investigator has kept a copy of the consent form.

Additional Community Mental Health Resources

Clinical Services Contact Information

Family physicians Yellow pages

Psychiatrists Yellow pages

Psychologists Yellow pages

Calgary Health Region

Calgary Eating Disorder Program*

May need referral from G.P.

403-955-8700

Access Mental Health 403-943-1500

Calgary Counselling Centre** 403-265-4980

Calgary Association of Self-Help 403-266-8711

Calgary Family Services 403-269-9888

Catholic Family Services 403-233-2360

Jewish Family Services 403-287-3510

Calgary Catholic Immigration Society 403-298-4111

*Consultants are available to provide information about eating disorders and treatment options.

**Provides individual, couple, family, & group counselling for individuals with an eating disorder. Offers

sliding scale fees according to family size and income.

Distress and Crisis Resources Contact Information

Emergency Services Dial 911

Distress Centre 266-1605

124

Men’s Line 266-HELP (4357)

Teen Line 264-TEEN (8336)

Calgary Mental Health Crisis and Emergency Services Mobile

Response Team

266-1605

Canadian Mental Health Association Suicide Services (available 8:30

am to 4:30 pm)

297-1744

Calgary Health Region Mental Health Line 943-1500

Organizations Contact Information

Psychologists Association of Alberta (referrals) 1-888-424-0297

Canadian Mental Health Association (Calgary Office) 297-1700

Canadian Psychological Association www.cpa.ca

National Eating Disorder Information Centre www.nedic.ca

Calgary Eating Disorder Program 943-7700

www.crha-health.ab.ca/eatingdis

Eating Disorder Education Organization (Edmonton) www.edeo.org

National Eating Disorders Association (U.S.) www.edap.org

Other eating disorder-related web sites www.anred.com

www.anad.org

http://eatingdisorders.mentalhelp.net/

125

Appendix D

Participant Demographic Information Form

Pseudonym: __________________________________________________________

Gender: ______________________________________________________________

Age: _________________________________________________________________

Highest Level of Education Attained: _____________________________________

Income Status (please check one):

□ Under $20,000 □ $100,000 to just under $150,000

□ $20,000 to just under $40,000 □ $150,000 and above

□ $40,000 to just under $60,000 □ Prefer not to answer

□ $60,000 to just under $80,000

□ $80,000 to just under $100,000

Ethnic Background (please check one or specify as “other” and explain):

□ European /European-Canadian □ French-Canadian

□ Aboriginal □ Métis

□ Asian / Asian-Canadian □ South Asian / South Asian Canadian

□ African / African Canadian □ Caribbean / Caribbean Canadian

□ Middle Eastern / Middle Eastern Canadian □ Latin American/Latin American Canadian

Other (please specify) ____________________________________________

126

Participant’s Parents’ Demographic Information

Mother’s Highest Level of Education Attained: _____________________________________

Father’s Highest Level of Education Attained: ______________________________________

Mother’s Ethnic Background (please check one or specify as “other” and explain):

□ European /European-Canadian □ French-Canadian

□ Aboriginal □ Métis

□ Asian / Asian-Canadian □ South Asian / South Asian Canadian

□ African / African Canadian □ Caribbean / Caribbean Canadian

□ Middle Eastern / Middle Eastern Canadian □ Latin American/Latin American Canadian

Other (please specify) ____________________________________________

Father’s Ethnic Background (please check one or specify as “other” and explain):

□ European /European-Canadian □ French-Canadian

□ Aboriginal □ Métis

□ Asian / Asian-Canadian □ South Asian / South Asian Canadian

□ African / African Canadian □ Caribbean / Caribbean Canadian

□ Middle Eastern / Middle Eastern Canadian □ Latin American/Latin American Canadian

Other (please specify) ____________________________________________

127

Parental Household Income (please check one)

□ Under $20,000 □ $100,000 to just under $150,000

□ $20,000 to just under $40,000 □ $150,000 and above

□ $40,000 to just under $60,000 □ Prefer not to answer

□ $60,000 to just under $80,000

□ $80,000 to just under $100,000

Who was responsible for controlling your food intake (selecting both is an option)?

Mother _______ Father _________

128

Appendix E

Interview Questions

Primary Research Question: How do young adults describe their childhood experiences of being

put on diets by their parents?

Today, I’d like to talk with you about your childhood experience of parental feeding control.

Parental feeding control can include not permitting a child to eat specific foods (e.g., junk food),

only allowing a child to eat small portions of particular foods (i.e., putting a child on a diet),

monitoring a child’s consumption of certain foods (e.g., snack foods), and/or pressuring a child

to eat particular foods (e.g., vegetables). So, putting a child on a diet in order to reduce their

weight or size would be considered parental feeding control. Today, I would like to talk with

you about your childhood experiences of these practices, as well as its effects on various aspects

of your life.

1. Start by telling me the story of the first time your parents controlled your food

intake.

What do you think lead your parents to control your food intake in this

way?

Which parent was more involved in the food control?

What was your experience with the other parent?

What is your explanation of why this parent was more involved?

How do you make sense of this? How do you explain their

decision/motivation to control your food?

2. What other experiences of this kind of controlling practices are important for me

to know about?

What was it like for you?

3. In what ways have you been impacted by being put on a diet by your parents as a

child?

4. If you could offer advice to parents who are currently raising children, what

would that be?

What would you want parents to know about your experience of being put

on a diet by your parents as a child?

5. What have you learned from this experience (three main insights)?

129

6. Other questions (if not already answered):

How did your parents control your diet?

How long did your parents control your diet?

How often did your parents control your diet?

Did your parents agree on controlling your food intake?

What other non-family members were involved in controlling your diet?

What other family members participated in dietary control?

7. What have I missed?

What else is important for me to know about your childhood experience of

being put on a diet by your parents?

130

Appendix F

Sample Memos

March 14, 2017

- Participants report that guilting a child into losing weight doesn’t work.

o *Individuals aren’t shamed into losing weight*

o Participants describe that “guilt tripping” creates disconnection within the parent-

child relationship and causes them psychological distress (e.g., intense feelings of

guilt) when they eat foods that were restricted or are perceived as “bad”.

- Internalizing dietary control

o Participants have taken on the responsibility of controlling their own eating. They

describe internalizing parental dietary restriction and control.

They often feel guilt when they act in contradiction to their parents’

dietary wishes.

They also report engaging in calorie counting, restriction, and

purging to lessen their guilt associated with eating bad or restricted

foods.

- Long-lasting impacts of parental feeding control practices

o Participants report still struggling with weight preoccupation

Feeling badly about their weight and size (e.g., self-conscious or

dissatisfied with their bodies)

Engaging in compensatory eating behaviours (e.g., going on 1200 calorie

diets to lose weight, or purging to lessen their guilt when they eat “bad”

foods or eat too much).

Related to a fear of gaining weight?

o Further related to internalized parental messages about

weight?

- Is this fear of gaining weight, that my participants have been describing, related to

internalized weight bias or internalized parental messages about weight and size?

o Through their perception of parental motivations for using parental feeding

control practices, they are receiving certain messages about weight:

Being overweight is bad

Being overweight is less-than

Being overweight is unattractive

Being overweight is unhealthy

Etc.