irish journal of psychological medicine

67
Irish Journal of VOL 29 No 1 April/May 2012 COVER ILLUSTRATION BY JERRY HUYSMANS Psychological Medicine ISSN 0790- 9667

Upload: peter-mas-mollinedo

Post on 21-Feb-2016

248 views

Category:

Documents


2 download

DESCRIPTION

Official journal of the Irish College of Psychiatry

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Irish Journal of

VOL 29 No 1 April/May 2012

COVER ILLUSTRATION BY JERRY HUYSMANS

Psychological Medicine

ISSN 0790- 9667

Page 2: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Irish Journal of Psychological MedicineVol. 29 No 1 April/ May 2012 ISSN 0790- 9667

Editorial3 The Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine and the College of Psychiatry of Ireland

Brendan D. Kelly

Original Papers7 Did the Great Irish Famine increase Schizophrenia?

Dermot Walsh

16 Study of Presentations for Involuntary Admission to a Cork Approved CentreElaine Dunne, Eamonn Moloney

22 Mental Illness among the Homeless: Prevalence Study in a Dublin Homeless HostelBernice Prinsloo, Catherine Parr & Joanne Fenton

27 Preferences of Dress and Address: Views of Attendees and Mental HealthProfessionals of the Psychiatric ServicesChristina Sim, Brian Hallahan & Colm McDonald

33 Psychiatrists in their eyes: Children’s Drawings of what a Psychiatrist looks like...Sami Omer, Niamh O’ Connor, Gavin Sweeney, Geraldine McCarthy

Brief Reports36 Selective Mutism: A Prevalence Study of Primary School Children in the Republic

of IrelandLouise Sharkey & Fiona McNicholas

41 Psychotherapy Training in Ireland: A Survey of College TutorsAlyson Lee & Angela Noonan

46 Suicide ideation, Psychological Adjustment and Mental Health Service Support: AScreening Study in an Irish Secondary School SampleCiara Brennan & Sinéad McGilloway

Case Report52 Graphology and Psychiatric Diagnosis: Is the Writing on the Wall?

Mary Davoren, Natalie Sherrard, Eugene Breen & Brendan D. Kelly

Opinion55 Psychiatric Power: A Personal View

Pat Bracken

Book Reviews59 Coming through Depression: A Mindful Approach to Recovery by Tony Bates

Stephen McWilliams

60 Leadership With Consciousness by Tony HumphreysBrendan D. Kelly

62 Homesickness: An American History by Susan J MattSéamus MacSuibhne

Appreciation63 Professor Hugh Lionel Freeman

Brian O’Shea

Letters to the Editor64 Interaction of Duloxetine with Warfarin; a Cautionary Report

Mugtaba Osman, Elaine Greene

65 Guidelines for Authors

2

Editor–in-Chief: Brendan Kelly, SeniorLecturer in Psychiatry, Department of AdultPsychiatry, UCD Mater MisericordiaeUniversity Hospital, Dublin 7

Deputy Editor: Niall Crumlish, ConsultantPsychiatrist, St James’s Hospital, Dublin 8

Consulting Editor: Larkin Feeney,Consultant Psychiatrist, Cluain MhuireServices, Blackrock, Co. Dublin

Founding Editor: Mark Hartman

Associate Editor: Ted Dinan (Cork)

Administrator: Sibéal Farrell

Editorial Board: Brian A Lawlor (Dublin),Patricia Casey (Dublin), Stephen Cooper(Belfast), Michael Fitzgerald (Dublin), BrianLeonard (Galway), Roy McClelland(Belfast), Brian O’Shea (Wicklow), IanPullen (Edinburgh), John Waddington(Dublin), Richard Williams (Victoria)

Submissions and Correspondence to:The EditorCollege of Psychiatry of Ireland5 Herbert StreetDublin 2, Ireland

Telephone: 00 353 1 6618450Fax: 00 353 1 6629677Email: [email protected]: www.ijpm.ie

Publisher: Irish Medical InformationFir Tree LodgeCraddockstown RoadNaasCo. Kildare

This paper meets the requirements ofANSI/NISOZ39.48-1992 (Permanence of Paper)

Page 3: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

In 2012, the Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine became theofficial research journal of the College of Psychiatry of Ireland. The

coming together of the Journal and the College marks animportant moment in the histories of both, and a significant stage

in the evolution of Irish psychiatry.

Founding editor of the Irish Journal of PsychologicalMedicine: Dr Mark Hartman

The Irish Journal of Psychological Medicinewas founded in 1982 byDr Mark Hartman (1942-1994). Mark Hartman was born in

Chicago in 1942, the son of an eminent forensic psychologist.

After high school, Mark completed a BSc in mathematics and

physics, and came to Ireland in the early 1960s.1 Mark studied

medicine in Trinity College Dublin, where he qualified in 1969,

following a brilliant undergraduate career which saw him elected

a scholar of the university. Mark studied psychiatry under Professor

Peter Beckett (1922-1974), first professor of psychiatry at Trinity,2

who described Mark as “the best student in his class”.3 Dr

Hartman received his American MD in 1969.

Dr Hartman served his internship in Sir Patrick Duns Hospital and

then worked in St. Patrick’s Hospital, St. Ita’s, St. Brendan’s and St.

Vincent’s, Fairview. He was elected a Member of the Royal College

of Psychiatrists in 1973 and worked in St. Mary’s, Castlebar until

permanently appointed at St. Brendan’s in 1978. In 1983, Dr

Hartman took a three-year career break to the United States, where

he was associate professor of psychiatry in Boston University and

medical director of Lakeshore Hospital, Manchester, New

Hampshire.1

Also in 1983, Dr Hartman attained his American Board

Examinations in Psychiatry and Neurology in Boston, and was

ranked in the top 2% of all candidates who had previously sat the

examination. Following his career break in the United States, Dr

Hartman returned to Dublin and worked at Vergemount Psychiatric

Clinic. He died in 1994, following a year-long illness. Dr Hartman

was posthumously elected to Fellowship of the Royal College of

Psychiatrists.

Throughout his career, Dr Hartman demonstrated an enormous

depth of knowledge and passion for psychiatry. He had a particular

interest in psychoanalytical psychotherapy, a field which informed

his approach to both psychiatric education and clinical care.1,4 Dr

Hartman was deeply involved in psychiatric training: he was

appointed clinical tutor in St. Brendan’s in 1973, taught at the Royal

College of Surgeons in Ireland, and was specialty tutor for the

Eastern Region Postgraduate Training Programme from 1979 to

1994.3

In addition to his interests in psychiatry and psychotherapy, Dr

Hartman had interests in myriad other fields. He had a particular

love of Middle English and held a Masters Degree in English from

Trinity College, Dublin. He was an gifted musician and

accomplished pianist, and owned one of the first mobile phones in

Ireland.2

In the midst of all of these activities and accomplishments, Dr

Hartman founded the Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine, in1982.1 At first, it was an uphill battle to attract submissions, re-

write certain papers, edit the Journal and organise publication. Dr

Hartman performed all of these tasks with insight and enthusiasm,

at all hours of day and night. He brought enormous vision and

drive to the undertaking, travelling to Northern Ireland and England

to seek out submissions, and inviting international speakers to

Ireland in order to obtain more papers.

In order to produce the Journal itself, Dr Hartman designed original

computer software and programmes, and was determined from

the outset that the Journal would embrace new technologies such

as the Internet, even though the Internet was in its early infancy in

Ireland when the Journal was founded in 1982. Following its

foundation, Dr Hartman oversaw the establishment of a circulation

base for the Journal, a remarkable growth in circulation (in terms

of both numbers and geography), broad-based scientific indexing,

and the stocking of the Journal in libraries around the world. This

series of accomplishments built the Journal into a remarkably

strong scientific and clinical voice, with impact and reach well

beyond these shores.

Dr Hartman’s dedication to the Journal was as profound as it was

pragmatic and, thanks to his unstinting work, Dr Hartman saw the

Journal’s “reputation grow steadily as a quarterly specialist

publication, essential to its Irish base and attracting international

readers and authors”.2 From the outset, the Irish Journal of

Psychological Medicine was a truly extraordinary creation, created

by a truly extraordinary man.

3

Brendan D. Kelly,Consultant Psychiatrist and Senior Lecturer in Psychiatry,

Department of Adult Psychiatry,University College Dublin,Mater Misericordiae University Hospital,62/63 Eccles Street, Dublin 7, Ireland.

E-mail [email protected]

Ir J Psych Med 2012; 29 (1): 3-6

Editorial

The Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine and theCollege of Psychiatry of IrelandBrendan D Kelly

Page 4: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

The Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine: 1994-2012

In 1994, following the untimely death of Dr Hartman, Professor Brian

Lawlor became editor-in-chief of the Irish Journal of PsychologicalMedicine. On appointment, Professor Lawlor explicitly renewed theJournal’s commitment to its central objectives: disseminating theresults of original scientific research to a national and international

readership; expressing the unique identity of Irish psychiatry; and

underscoring the importance of cross-cultural differences in psychiatry

within and beyond these islands.5

Over the following years, the Journal continued to publish national andinternational research papers, as well as editorials, review papers, case-

reports and book reviews. Format was substantially revised in

conjunction with the Journal’s long-time publisher, MedMedia. Aspsychiatry in Ireland evolved over this period,6 the Journal appointed atrainee editor in 2004,7 a deputy editor in 2008, a new editor-in-chief

in 2010, and a consulting editor in 2011.

In 2009, the Journal published the first in a series of ContinuingProfessional Development (CPD) modules, to assist with learning and

self-assessment for psychiatrists.8 A broad range of topics has been

covered since then, including management of schizophrenia,9 alcohol

use disorders,10 adherence,11 morbid jealousy,12 post-traumatic stress

disorder,13 fetal alcohol spectrum disorders,14 tobacco smoking and

mental illness,15 serotonin toxicity,16 improving monitoring for

metabolic syndrome using audit 17 and antidepressant augmentation

and combination in unipolar depression.18

For over two decades, the Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine hasawarded the John Dunne Medal, established in honour of Professor

John Dunne, president of the Royal Medico-Psychological Association in

1955 and first professor of psychiatry in Ireland.19,20 Dr Mark Hartman,

founding editor of the Journal, was a strong admirer of Professor

Dunne. Consistent with the emphasis that Professor Dunne placed on

scientific research into mental illness,21 the John Dunne Medal is

awarded annually to a trainee from Ireland or the United Kingdom. To

be eligible, a trainee much have made a significant contribution (though

not necessarily as first author) to an original paper published by the

Journal over the previous year. Over the past two decades the JohnDunne Medal has come to symbolise excellence and originality in the

field of psychiatric research.22

The College of Psychiatry of Ireland

The College of Psychiatry of Ireland is the professional body for

psychiatrists in Ireland and the sole body recognised by the Medical

Council and Health Service Executive (public health care provider in

Ireland) for competence assurance and training in psychiatry. The

College offers membership, for psychiatrists and trainees, of an Irish

institution which aims to be the voice of psychiatry in Ireland

(www.irishpsychiatry.ie).

The College emerged as an organisation in 2009 when three former

bodies merged: the Irish College of Psychiatrists (a division of the

Royal College of Psychiatrists in the United Kingdom), the Irish

Psychiatric Association, and the then training body for psychiatry, the

Irish Psychiatric Training Committee. It had been apparent for many

years that both the law and mental health services in the United

Kingdom served different needs to those in Ireland and therefore an

Irish organisation was seen by many as being long overdue.

The College of Psychiatry of Ireland aims to address not only issues

relevant to its Irish members but also issues for psychiatry in general,

for mental health services, and ultimately for services-users. It aims to

promote good mental health and high quality mental health care for all

involved in mental health services. The College fully supports Ireland’s

national mental health policy, “A Vision for Change”23 and has

consistently urged that this policy be implemented in full, in order to

give Ireland the mental health service it deserves.

In conjunction with its representative, training and advocacy roles, the

College has a growing role in promoting and encouraging research, with

a view to generating an improved evidence-base for psychiatric practice.

In this respect, the College is assuming at least some of the roles

envisioned in long-standing calls for a research-oriented Irish Institute of

Psychiatry, which has not yet materialized in Ireland.24,25

Following the emergence of the College in 2009, it was readily

apparent that the College would need a journal in order to strengthen

the College’s participation in the international research community

and assist the College in shaping and improving standards of mental

health care. Given its unique role in expressing the “identity of Irish

psychiatry”,5 long history of high-quality publishing, and growing

international profile, the Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine wasthe obvious candidate for this role.

In 2012, the Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine duly became theofficial research journal of the College of Psychiatry of Ireland.

Times of change in Irish psychiatry and medicalpublishing

These are challenging and exciting times for mental health care in

Ireland, and beyond. In Ireland, the practice and profession of

psychiatry are being gradually but definitely transformed with the

implementation of the Mental Health Act 2001 (2001-2006),

introduction of “A Vision for Change” (2006),23 establishment of the

College of Psychiatry of Ireland (2009), development of new systems

for competence assurance by the Medical Council and College of

Psychiatry of Ireland (2011), and review of the Mental Health Act

2001 (2012).

Looking more broadly, the ongoing expansion of the scientific evidence-

base for psychiatric practice is also transforming mental health care;

service user movements are helping shape better systems of care; and

public services in general are seeking to become more efficient,

responsive and user-focussed. Against the backdrop of the economic

and social challenges facing Ireland and Europe at present,26 these

developments present unique challenges and opportunities to mental

health service users and providers alike.

In this medical, scientific and social context, medical journals play a

vital role in the generation and dissemination of knowledge, and in

shaping change.27 Medical journals have particular importance in

informing evidence-based practice, developing health policy, and

underpinning training programmes and continuing education for

practitioners. Medical journals can also open up new areas of

research, prompt critical and constructive debate, and generate

interest in hitherto neglected areas of thought and practice. The IrishJournal of Psychological Medicine has a long history of filling many ofthese roles.

4

Page 5: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

These are, however, also times of transformation for medical

journals themselves. Key changes relate to the evolution of

electronic communication technologies, issues related to

independent scientific peer review, and substantial changes in the

broader publishing industry.28,29 Further issues relate to the complex

roles of journals in the developing world30 and in the protection of

human rights.31 Issues of human rights are especially important in

the context of mental health policy32 and mental health law,33-35

especially following recent changes in legislation, as is the case in

Ireland.36-39

Notwithstanding these challenges – or, more likely, because of

them – medical journals are set to retain a critical role in shaping

medical knowledge, informing evidence-based practice, developing

health policy and underpinning training in health-care disciplines.27

While the format and structure of journals may change

dramatically, the necessity for them will not.

The Journal and the College: Looking to the future

As research journal of the College of Psychiatry of Ireland, the IrishJournal of Psychological Medicine will continue to publish highquality original research from around the world. These original data

papers will continue to form the central focus of the Journal,supplemented by briefer reports of smaller research projects, audit

cycles, and various other kinds of papers which reflect different

approaches to research, practice and education in mental health care.

The Journal has a long-standing commitment to publishing case-reports, which we believe serve several important purposes,

including the description of new conditions or novel effects of

treatments; suggesting new indications for existing treatments; 40

quality assurance; and education.41 In addition, case-reports are

often especially engaging for readers, many of whom find clinical

vignettes involving, informative and pragmatic.42 Like the Lancet,the Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine still sees an importantrole for relevant, well-written case-reports.43

The Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine also remains committedto publishing papers presented in a range of different formats,

including editorials, review papers, educational papers, letters to

the editor, and book reviews. The Journal is similarly committed topublishing historical papers examining key topics in the histories

of psychiatry in Ireland and elsewhere.44-46 In this issue, we are

especially pleased to publish an historical paper looking at

admission rates for schizophrenia and other disorders during

Ireland’s Great Famine (1845-1849),47 an event which was curiously

absent from the historiography of Irish psychiatry to date.19,48

The Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine has a strong tradition ofhighlighting and debating key issues in contemporary psychiatry,

and this, too, will continue. In recent years, we have published

occasional multi-author papers examining key issues,49,50 and our

regular editorials have explored diverse topics including

deinstitutionalisation in disability services,51 dementia care,52mental

health and the asylum process,53 psychiatric training and research,54

reflective practice in psychiatric training,55 primary care mental

health 56 and perinatal psychiatry.57

In March 2011, we published a special supplement dedicated to

head shop drugs,58,59 looking in detail at the clinical effects of

mephedrone and other head shop drugs,60,61 users’ experiences of

cathinones,62 “whack” induced psychosis,63 and benzlypiperazine-

induced acute delirium.64 In future years, we will continue to

highlight key issues in psychiatry, policy, training and research

throughout the Journal.

In addition to these thematic and editorial priorities, we will

continue to seek to expand the Journal’s indexing profile. From

the outset, the Journal’s founding editor, Dr Mark Hartman,ensured broad-based indexing for the Journal. At present, the

Journal is indexed in Biological Abstracts (BIOSIS Previews); CentreNational de la Recherche Scientifique/Inist; Pascal; Excerpta

Medica/Embase; Institute for Scientific Information; Current

Contents/Social and Behavioural Sciences (Social Science Citation

Index, Research Alert); Psychological Abstracts (PsycInfo/PsycLit);

Cumulative Index to Nursing and Allied Health Literature; Current

Aids Literature (CAB Abstracts); International Pharmaceutical

Abstracts; Linguistics and Language Behaviour Abstracts; Nutrition

Abstracts and Reviews (CAB Abstracts); Referativnyi Zhurnal; Social

Planning/Policy and Development Abstracts; Social Work Research

and Abstracts; and Sociological Abstracts.

In coming years, we will seek actively to expand our indexing

profile so as to better reflect the quality and diversity of original

research published in the Journal and optimise Journal impact.

The voice of psychiatry in Ireland

From the very beginning, the founding editor of the Irish Journal ofPsychological Medicine, Dr Mark Hartman, saw the Journal becomingthe voice of a national college of psychiatry in Ireland. Over

subsequent decades, the Journal duly assumed a key role “inexpressing the unique identity of Irish psychiatry”, and the “complex

tapestry of different historical traditions and experiences that mesh

together to create a system that is unique and different from any of

its derivative parts”.5 The coming together of the Journal with theCollege of Psychiatry of Ireland in 2012 represents the realisation of

Dr Hartman’s vision, and a logical and important step in the evolution

of psychiatry in Ireland.

As research journal of the College of Psychiatry of Ireland, the IrishJournal of Psychological Medicinewill continue to fulfil its establishedroles in advancing the evidence-base for practice and “expressing the

unique identity of Irish psychiatry”,5 but will also seek to expand and

develop in new directions. As part of that process, readers who wish

to make suggestions about how the Journal can develop in the futureshould feel free to submit their ideas to the editorial team

([email protected]). All suggestions are welcome.

Like the College of Psychiatry of Ireland, the Journal’s key aims areto inform and improve mental health services in Ireland and

beyond, and, ultimately, enhance the quality of mental health care

provided to those who need it.

In the end, that’s what matters most.

Conflict of interest

The author is editor-in-chief of the Irish Journal of PsychologicalMedicine.

5

Page 6: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

References

1. McGennis A. Obituary: Mark Hartman. Psych Bull 1994; 18: 442.

2. Webb M. Trinity’s Psychiatrists: From Serenity of the Soul to Neuroscience.

Trinity College, Dublin, 2011.

3. McClelland R. An appreciation of Dr Mark Hartman, founder and editor-in-

chief of the Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine. Ir J Psychol Med 1994; 11: 4.

4. Redmond L. Suicide: act of destruction or cry of despair? Irish Times 1979;

January 29.

5. Lawlor BA. Quo vadis? Ir J Psychol Med 1994; 11: 107.

6. Kelly BD. Mental health policy in Ireland, 1984-2004: theory, overview and

future directions. Ir J Psychol Med 2004; 21: 61-68.

7. Kelly BD. Investing in the future. Ir J Psychol Med 2004; 21: 111

8. McDonald G, Hallahan B. Recent advances in the biological treatment of

mood disorders (Continuing Professional Development, Module 1). Ir J Psychol

Med 2009; 26: 93-101.

9. Behan C. Current approaches to the management of schizophrenia

(Continuing Professional Development, Module 2). Ir J Psychol Med 2009;

26: 157-163.

10. Feeney L. Management of alcohol use disorders (Continuing Professional

Development, Module 3). Ir J Psychol Med 2009; 26: 214-220.

11. Duffy R, Kelly BD. Adherence in psychiatry (Continuing Professional

Development, Module 4). Ir J Psychol Med 2010; 27: 54-60.

12. Mullins D. Morbid jealousy: the green-eyed monster

(Continuing Professional Development, Module 5). Ir J Psychol Med 2010;

27: 106-112.

13. Crumlish N. Post-traumatic stress disorder: present and future

(Continuing Professional Development, Module 6). Ir J Psychol Med

2010; 27: 162-167.

14. Orakwue N, McNicholas F, O’Malley K. Fetal alcohol spectrum disorders – an

Irish perspective (Continuing Professional Development, Module 7). Ir J

Psychol Med 2010; 27: 223-227.

15. Feeney S, Hallahan B. Tobacco smoking and mental illness: important

considerations (Continuing Professional Development, Module 8). Ir J Psychol

Med 2011; 28: 50-54.

16. Nassim S, Jabbar F, Afridi A, Kelly BD. Serotonin toxicity (Continuing

Professional Development, Module 9). Ir J Psychol Med 2011; 28: 106-109.

17. Murtagh A, Petrovici R, Wong W, Obadan C, Solanke O, Nnabuchi E, Kilbride

K. Improving monitoring for metabolic syndrome using audit (Continuing

Professional Development, Module 10). Ir J Psychol Med 2011; 28: 176-179.

18. Kolshus E, Douglas L, Dunne R. Antidepressant augmentation and

combination in unipolar depression : strong guidance, weak foundations

(Continuing Professional Development, Module 11). Ir J Psychol Med 2011;

28: 263-271

19. Reynolds J. Grangegorman: Psychiatric Care in Dublin since 1815. Institute of

Public Administration/Eastern Health Board, Dublin,1992.

20. Kelly BD. Physical sciences and psychological medicine: the legacy of Prof

John Dunne. Ir J Psychol Med 2005; 22: 67-72

21. Dunne J. The Contribution of the Physical Sciences to Psychological Medicine.

J Ment Sci 1956; 102: 209-220.

22. Doherty AM, Kelly BD. The social and psychological correlates of

happiness in seventeen European countries: analysis of data from the

European Social Survey. Ir J Psychol Med 2010; 27: 130-134.

23. Expert Group on Mental Health Policy. A Vision for Change: Report of the

Expert Group on Mental Health Policy. The Stationery Office, Dublin, 2006.

24. Clare A, Daly RJ, Dinan TG, King D, Leonard BE, O’Boyle C, O’Connor J,

Waddington J, Walsh N, Webb M. Advancement of psychiatric research in

Ireland: proposal for a national body. Ir J Psychol Med 1990; 7: 93.

25. Daly RJ. Community psychiatry and the National Institute of Mental Health.

Ir J Psychol Med 1990; 7: 5.

26. Lewis, M. Boomerang: The Meltdown Tour. Penguin/Allen Lane, London, 2011.

27. Vandenbroucke JP. Medical journals and the shaping of medical knowledge.

Lancet 1998; 352; 2001-2006.

28. Fletcher RH, Fletcher SW. The future of medical journals in the western

world. Lancet. 1998; 352: Suppl 2:SII30-3.

29. Horton R. 21st-century biomedical journals: failures and futures. Lancet

2003; 362: 1510-1512.

30. Lown B, Bukachi F, Xavier R. Health information in the developing world.

Lancet 1998; 352: Suppl 2:SII34-38.

31. Kandela P. Medical journals and human rights. Lancet 1998; 352: Suppl 2:SII7-11.

32. World Health Organization. Mental Health: New Understanding, New Hope.

World Health Organization, Geneva, 2001.

33. World Health Organization. WHO Resource Book on Mental Health, Human

Rights and Legislation. World Health Organization, Geneva, 2005.

34. United Nations. Principles for the Protection of Persons with Mental Illness

and the Improvement of Mental Health Care. United Nations, Secretariat

Centre For Human Rights, New York, 1991.

35. United Nations. Convention on the Rights of Persons with Disabilities. United

Nations, Geneva, 2006.

36. O’Donoghue B, Moran P. Consultant psychiatrists’ experiences and attitudes

following the introduction of the Mental Health Act 2001: a national survey.

Ir J Psychol Med 2009; 26: 23-26.

37. Jabbar F, Kelly BD, Casey P. National survey of psychiatrists' responses to

implementation of the Mental Health Act 2001 in Ireland. Ir J Med Sci 2010;

179: 291-294.

38. Jabbar F, Doherty AM, Aziz M, Kelly BD. Implementing the Mental Health Act

2007 in British general practice: Lessons from Ireland. Int J Law Psychiatry

2011; 34: 414-418.

39. Kelly BD. Mental health legislation and human rights in England, Wales and

the Republic of Ireland. Int J Law Psychiatry 2011; 34: 439-454.

40. Vandenbroucke JP. Case reports in an evidence-based world. J R Soc Med

1999; 92: 159-163.

41. Kelly BD. In defence of the single case report. Ir Med J 2001; 94: 37.

42. Bignall J, Horton R. Learning from stories – the Lancet’s case reports. Lancet

1995; 346: 1246.

43. Kelly BD. The Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine: looking to the future.

Ir J Psychol Med 2010; 27: 170-171.

44. Kelly BD. Learning disability and forensic mental healthcare in nineteenth-

century Ireland. Ir J Psychol Med 2008; 25: 116-118.

45. Breathnach CS. Philip Crapton (1777-1858) and his description of

nominal aphasia. Ir J Psychol Med 2009; 26: 41-42.

46. Walsh D. The Ennis District Lunatic Asylum and the Clare Workhouse Lunatic

Asylums in 1901. Ir J Psychol Med 2009; 26: 206-211.

47. Walsh D. Did the Great Irish Famine increase schizophrenia? Ir J Psychol Med

2012; 29: 7-15

48. Finnane P. Insanity and the Insane in Post-Famine Ireland. Croon Helm,

London, 1981.

49. Lawlor B, Ganter K, Daly I, Owens J. Implementing the Mental Health Act

2001: what should be done? What can be done? Ir J Psychol Med 2005; 22:

79-82.

50. Kelly BD, Bracken P, Cavendish H, Crumlish N, MacSuibhne S, Szasz T,

Thornton T. The Myth of Mental Illness 50 years after publication: what does

it mean today? Ir J Psychol Med 2010; 27: 35-43.

51. Carrigan N. Deinstitutionalisation – time to move on to legalisation? Ir J

Psychol Med 2011; 28: 182-184.

52. O’Connell H. Dementia care in Ireland: what’s the plan? Ir J Psychol Med

2011; 28: 112-115.

53. Crumlish N, Bracken P. Mental health and the asylum process. Ir J Psychol

Med 2011; 28: 57-60.

54. Nkire N, Edokpolo O. Psychiatric training and research in Ireland, the trainee’s

perspective. Ir J Psychol Med 2011; 28: 3-5.

55. Omer S. McCarthy G. Reflective practice in psychiatric training: Balint groups.

Ir J Psychol Med 2010; 27: 115-116.

56. Russell V, Kelly M. Primary care mental health – a new direction? Ir J Psychol

Med 2010; 27: 63-65.

57. O’Keane V. Planning perinatal psychiatry services for Ireland. Ir J Psychol Med

2010; 27: 3-5.

58. Kelly BD. Head shop drugs: they haven’t gone away. Ir J Psychol Med

2011; 28: S1.

59. O’Keefe C. Medical problems caused by ‘legal highs’ continue. Irish Examiner

2011; May 12.

60. O’Domhnaill S. Mephedrone and “head/hemp” shop drugs: a clinical and

biochemical “heads up”. Ir J Psychol Med 2011; 28: S2-S3.

61. Uhoegbu C, Kolshus E, Nwachukwu I, Guerandel A, Maher C. “Legal

Highs”: Report on two psychiatric presentations linked with “head shop”

products. Ir J Psychol Med 2011; 28: S8-S10.

62. O’Reilly F, McAuliffe R, Long J. Users’ experiences of cathinones sold in head

shops and online. Ir J Psychol Med 2011; 28: S4-S7.

63. El-Higaya E, Ahmed M, Hallahan B. Whack induced psychosis: a case series.

Ir J Psychol Med 2011; 28: S11-S13.

64. Tully J, Hallahan B, McDonald C. Benzlypiperazine-induced acute delirium in

a patient with Schizophrenia and an incidental temporal meningioma: A

case report and a review of the literature. Ir J Psychol Med 2011; 28: S14-S16.

6

Page 7: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Abstract

Background: Evidence from regions where there have been severedietary restriction suggests that individuals in utero during periodsof starvation may subsequently be at increased risk of

schizophrenia. Because Ireland was the location of a major

nineteenth century famine an attempt has been made to

determine whether any such evidence for famine/schizophrenia

association can be found.

Method: The data used derive mainly from the Annual Reports onthe District, Criminal and Private Lunatic Asylums supplied by the

Inspectors of Lunacy in Ireland for the relevant years. Nineteenth

century diagnostic labels have been adjusted to conform to

schizophrenia as currently understood. Evidence relating to a

possible schizophrenia increase in famine-related emigrants is

examined.

Results: There was an increase in first admission rates forschizophrenia of 85.7% from 1860 to 1875. Admissions for other

disorders, chiefly melancholia, also increased. Similar admission

increases were evident in other jurisdictions over the same period.

Data relating to the mental health of famine – migrating Irish are

sparse and of difficult interpretation.

Conclusion: The evidence from available data sources attemptingto link the Irish famines of the 1840s with a subsequent increase

in the incidence of schizophrenia is equivocal and inconclusive.

Key words: Schizophrenia, Famine, 19th Century Ireland.

Introduction

The impact of famine conditions on the human embryo has been

a matter of study. For example maternal under-nutrition may result

in cephalopelvic disproportion resulting in increased birth

complications following maternal rickets due to insufficiency of

vitamin D.1 Low fertility, decreased birth weight and the survival of

underweight and premature infants as a consequence of improved

obstetric care may lead to substantial health consequences later in

life. These include developmental retardation or abnormalities. For

example there is mounting evidence that schizophrenia (as well as

autism and intellectual disability) may be a developmental disorder

associated with a variety of environmental/gene interactions, such

as maternal pre-natal infection in genetically susceptible persons,

resulting in abnormalities of synaptic formation and maintenance

and of mechanisms of neurotransmission, manifesting themselves

in cognitive, affective and perceptual anomalies.2 Among other

suspect relevant environmental traumas is maternal under-

nutrition.3

The occurrence of two relatively recent famines has led to a natural

experiment of their effect on neurodevelopment. The first was the

Dutch Winter Hunger and the second the Great Leap Forward

Chinese famine.

The Dutch famine occurred at a specific time and geographical

region and records were able to document the timing and extent

of the resultant under-nutrition. The relevant circumstances were

that towards the closing phase of World War 2 the Dutch

population attempted to assist the Allied advance. In retaliation

the German occupation authority imposed a ban on the

importation of food supplies to occupied Holland. This began

towards the end of 1944 with the west of the country most

severely affected. The under-nutrition of the population was most

marked between February and May 1945 when average daily food

intake fell to 1,000 calories per day and eventually to as low as

500. Of the 3.3 million population of the western Netherlands at

least 20,000 died as a direct result of hunger and 200,000 suffered

adverse health consequences and the birth rate fell to half pre-

famine levels.4

Researchers have followed the offspring of Dutch mothers

pregnant during the famine with particular emphasis on

neurodevelopmental conditions such as schizophrenia.4 In the case

of schizophrenia three criteria were used to define the exposed

birth cohort in the six largest Western cities of the Netherlands.

The first criterion was low food intake during the first month of

gestation and those born between August and December 1945 in

the cities of the famine regions met this criterion. The second

criterion was conception at the height of the famine as indicated

by adverse health effects in the general population. Those born

later among the birth cohorts of August- December 1945 (born

between October 15 and December 31) met this criterion but those

born earlier (between August 1 and October 14) did not. These

persons were traced through the Dutch National Psychiatric

Registry, followed through to ages 24-48 to determine whether

they had been admitted to hospital with a diagnosis of

schizophrenia between 1970 and 1992. Control cohorts from

periods immediately after and prior to the famine period were also

investigated. The risk of schizophrenia was found to be

substantially raised in the exposed cohort as compared with the

controls with a relative risk of 2.0 for males and 2.2 for females.

The schizophrenia cohort also had increased perinatal mortality and

increased birth complications both of which are known to be

associated with schizophrenia.5

7

Dermot Walsh, Submitted February 4th 2011Consultant Psychiatrist Emeritus, Health Research Board, Accepted July 29th 2011Knockmaun House, Lower Mount Street, Dublin 2.E-mail [email protected]

Ir J Psych Med 2012; 29 (1): 7-15

Original Paper

Did the Great Irish Famine increase schizophrenia?Dermot Walsh

Page 8: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

In explanation Hoek et al have hypothesised that prenatal

micronutrient deficiencies may cause neurodevelomental

schizophrenia.4 A criticism of the study is that the numbers involved,

27, was very small. A follow up study of five males and four females

with schizophrenia born between October and December, these

being the only traceable subjects of the original 27, was undertaken.6

Their diagnosis of schizophrenia was confirmed by standard

diagnostic instruments and they participated in a magnetic resonance

imaging (MRI) study. They were compared with the same number

and similarly sexed healthy subjects who had also been exposed to

the famine conditions. Two further groups were recruited, nine

schizophrenic persons and nine non-schizophrenic individuals, both

groups without famine exposure. The exposed schizophrenia group

was found by comparison to the others to have decreased

intracranial volume. This was interpreted to have indicated stunted

early brain development. However several methodological criticisms

may be made. Most obviously the numbers involved were very small

and the method of recruitment of the controls far from perfect.

The Great Leap Forward Chinese famine of 1959-61 was the

consequence of collectivisation of agriculture, flawed agricultural

practices and reduction of cultivated land. The famine was

widespread but varied from province to province. Reports of an

investigation of the effects of this famine on schizophrenia derive

from an isolated province of China in the Wuhu region of Anhui. The

area of Wuhu and six surrounding counties were served by a single

psychiatric hospital. Hospital records from 1971 to 2001 were

examined. The authors found that the risk of developing

schizophrenia doubled from 1958 and 1959 and concluded that

prenatal exposure to famine increased the risk of schizophrenia later

in life.7 The findings from this area, much larger than that of the

Netherlands, found for the most exposed cohort an effect size similar

to the Dutch. The mortality adjusted relative risk for schizophrenia

over the two year period was 2.3 for 1960 and 1.9 for 1961.

However the study did not have month of birth for cases or controls

nor was information for food intake available month by month.

Accordingly a second more rigorous study was undertaken.8 This was

centred on Liuzhou City and surrounding counties in Guangxi region

with a population of 45 million. One hospital served the area under

investigation, and had good psychiatric records covering the years

1971- 2001. The famine here was less severe than in Anhui but of

considerable dimensions nonetheless. As a result there was evidence

of 500,000 – 800,000 deaths and fertility reductions of the same

order as in Holland. All inpatient and outpatient records of the

hospital were examined using ICD 10 criteria. A twofold increased

risk for schizophrenia among those conceived or in early gestation at

the height of the famine was found. However this result was

exclusively for rural areas. The authors speculated on the possible

deviant nutritional mechanisms involved such as the micronutrients

involved in the folate pathway which may directly affect growth of

the developing brain or indirectly by affecting DNA stability and

regulation of genes.

Thus there is evidence that famine conditions may, as one of their

consequences, increase the incidence of schizophrenia. Maternal

dietary deprivation or insufficiency may translate into foetal

undernourishment with adverse effects on neurological and brain

development during pregnancy.

The Great Irish Famines

In Europe the Irish famines of the 1840s are notorious for their

severity and adverse health consequences resulting in an estimated

one million deaths and massive emigration. There is extensive

literature on the causes, characteristics and consequences of these

famines such as by O’Grada.9 These famines were the consequence

of successive and repeated failures of the potato crop on which

much of the population, that had greatly increased during the

previous half century, had come to depend as its main source of

nutrition.10 The crop failure was the result of infestation of the

potato by phyophtera infestans causing blight for which there wasno known antidote at the time. Although there had been periodic

potato crop failures and deficiencies in Ireland from at least the

late eighteenth century, these were not due to blight, were of

lesser intensity and more restricted in geographic range.10 The

blights of 1845-50 were on a very different scale. The resultant

series of famines are collectively known as The Great Famine.

Food and nutrition in Ireland from 1500 to the early 20th century

have been extensively investigated by Clarkson and Crawford.11

They quote Arthur Young in stating that by the late 18th century

the average person consumed 6 pounds of potatoes per day and

recorded that by 1845 close to 40% of the population lived chiefly

on potatoes.12 This occurred in a setting of population increase

since 1700 and an exporting of meat, butter and grain. These

authors estimate that between 1846 and 1851 over a million or

12% of the immediate pre-famine population died. “More than

one third of Ireland’s population were potato people, competing

for scarce plots on marginal soils and hard pressed for rent by

farmers who were themselves under pressure from falling grain

prices and impoverished landlords.”13

The question as to the specificity of the great famine in time is a

relevant issue having regard to the time limits of the famine in

China and the more securely identified window of time of the

Dutch occurrence. The late Professor George O’Brien doubted the

uniqueness of the 1845 -1850 situation when he said that the

country was in a constant state of sub-famine and that the years

identified by historians as “great” were but an exacerbation of a

long-standing situation.14 However scholars such as Connell 10 and

O’Grada 9 take a different view and Clarkson and Crawford argue

that “ Ireland was not chronically a famine-stricken society and that

in normal years it was well stocked with nutritious food”.15

It is therefore reasonable to accept that the years 1845 – 1851

were years of devastation and mortality in which a pre-famine

population of 8.5 million was greatly reduced by mortality and by

emigration. The physical effects of starvation and the

accompanying exposure to the fevers and vitamin deficiencies were

well documented by Wilde and others.16 Apart from the

confusional states of persons dying from famine-associated fevers

there is little information of the longer-run consequences for

mental health. O’Grada, for example, asserted “there was little

likelihood of long-lasting cohort effects in post famine Ireland” on

the physical and intellectual performance of children who were

born at the time.17

8

Page 9: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Accordingly on the assumption that what appears to be true for

Holland and China might also apply in famine Ireland the current

exercise has been undertaken. It operates from a basis of many

deficiencies. Statistical data relating to the question are sparse and

open to a myriad of interpretations; the famine was more

prolonged and less incisive in time than in the other locations

examined and the ability to diagnose schizophrenia hampered by

rudimentary descriptions of cases, exclusively inpatients.

Method

The task for this study is to identify whether it is possible to adduce

evidence to sustain the maternal hunger/schizophrenia hypothesis

in the context of one of Europe’s severest famines of modern times.

To explore this issue it is necessary to examine whether relevant

health care data substantiate an increase in the disorder some 15-

30 years following the major famines. The relevant period is,

therefore, between 1860 and 1875.

The available data relevant to this enquiry include numbers of

admissions to lunatic asylums in Ireland over the relevant time

period. In addition the issue of whether the massive emigrations

from Ireland to North America and the United Kingdom in the late

1840s and early 1850s would have included migrant under-

nourished pregnant women resulting in an excess of schizophrenic

persons in these emigrant populations in the 1860s and 1870s is

briefly considered.

Lunacy in 19th century Ireland

The 19th century was a period of rapid growth of the recognition

and institutional provision for the mentally ill or lunatic and the

intellectually disabled (idiots and imbeciles). It has been reviewed in

detail by Finanne18 and details of the temporal establishment of

district asylums from 1830 – 1845 have been presented by Walsh.19

The main provision was in district lunatic asylums but also in the

lunatic wards of workhouses established in around 1840 where

residents were predominantly of idiot or imbecile type. As the years

progressed a policy of transferring lunatics from workhouse to

asylums was embarked upon. The following table documents the

growth in the asylum population from 1824 when there was one

district asylum in Armagh (although there were asylums in Dublin

and Cork at this time they had not been absorbed into the district

asylum system until later) to 1875 when there were 22. In addition

there were smaller numbers of lunatics in goals and in workhouses.

Table 1. Years of opening of District Lunatic Asylums withnumbers of patients on opening and in 1875.

9

Asylum Year Opened Patients on Opening Patients 1875

Armagh 1824 104 162

Ballinasloe 1833 150 459

Belfast 1829 104 380

Carlow 1832 104 250

Castlebar 1866 250 281

Clonmel 1834 60 370

Cork 1852 250 500

Down 1868 300 300

Ennis 1868 260 260

Enniscorthy 1868 285 288

Kilkenny 1852 152 216

Killarney 1852 220 220

Letterkenny 1866 300 300

Limerick 1827 150 410

Londonderry 1829 120 238

Maryborough 1833 104 266

Monaghan 1869 340 340

Mullingar 1855 300 400

Omagh 1853 300 510

Richmond 1830 257 1,040

Sligo 1855 250 330

Waterford 1835 100 210

Totals 4,460 7,730

Sources: Reports on the District, Criminal and Private Lunatic Asylums in Ireland. 21st report.20 25th report.21

Page 10: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

There was therefore an increase of 3,270 or 73% inpatients

resident between the opening of the asylums and the numbers

hospitalised in 1875.

The reasons for the increase in these asylum populations were

much debated. Broadly speaking there were two schools of

commentators, those who believed that the increase reflected a

genuine increase in the incidence and prevalence of lunacy such

that the Times of London was moved to assert in an editorial in

1877 that “If lunacy continues to increase as at present the insane

will be in the majority and, freeing themselves, will put the sane in

asylums” as quoted by Scull22 and those who believed that the

increase was apparent rather than real.

In the former camp were those who believed that before 1800

schizophrenia, the most serious form of lunacy, was rare prior to

the 19th century before increasing afterwards because of genetic

mutation as did Hare 23 whereas Jablensky assigned the increase,

on the basis of German evidence, to the 20th century.24 Another

commentator concluded that schizophrenia became more

prevalent during the 19th and early 20th century due to socio-

environmental changes associated with industrial growth and

biological mediating mechanisms such as nutritional,

immunological and infectious causes.1 The opposing point of view

was promoted by Scull25 who, in rejecting the real increase

hypothesis, invoked social conditions, such as pauperism, as

contributing to the increasing influx of persons to the 19th century

asylums. However he conceded that the most satisfactory means

of deciding between the rival hypotheses was to sample the

characteristics of 19th century admission records but conceded that

“there must be serious doubt the quality of surviving records is

adequate for this purpose” and that “individual case records are

generally too skimpy to be useful for answering this question”.25

In these debates in Ireland the Inspectors of Lunatics figured largely

as did prominent asylum doctors of the time such as MacCabe26

and Drapes 27 both of whom held the increase was real. Indeed the

matter was of such sufficient interest that the Inspectors devoted

a Special Report to the Chief Secretary in 1884, on the “alleged

increased prevalence of insanity in Ireland.28 They concluded that

“the great increase in the insane under care is mainly due to

ACCUMULATION (sic) and is, so far, an apparent and not a real

increase”. This in fact was a compilation of the views of asylum

superintendents whom the Inspectors had canvassed. Finnane,

too, supported an apparent increase as explanation and cited social

and administrative considerations as catalysts.29

Administrative and legal considerations may have been just as

important as any perceived increase in lunacy as a disease in

contributing to the increase in asylum admissions throughout the

19th century. In Ireland the Dangerous Lunatics Act of 1838 made

it relatively easy for individuals who had committed what today are

called “minor public order offences”, and sometimes even those

whose level of misbehaviour was beneath this threshold, to be

consigned to asylums, or to gaols before transfer to asylums. It was

generally believed that this mechanism was abused with the result

that many persons whose dangerousness and lunacy were

questionable ended up in the asylums. An attempt to control the

conveyance of persons to asylums in this manner resulted in the

Dangerous Lunatics Act of 1867 which did not differ substantially

from its processor other than requiring a medical certificate from

the dispensary doctor and in allowing magistrates to commit

“dangerous lunatics and idiotics” to lunatic asylums rather than to

jails by legislation, the effect of which in the case of the Richmond

Asylum, was to increase admissions to 401 in 1868 compared to

247 in 186.30 In effect the new legislation, far from remedying the

situation, resulted in an increase in direct admissions to asylums

throughout the century. This issue is examined in depth by Finnane

who attributed it in part to “the widening of insanity’s

boundaries”31 thus raising further nosocomial caution on the

interpretation of the reasons for admission increases following the

famine. And for Finnane the familial context, particularly in its

socio-economic context, was at least as important in determining

admission sought by relatives as the diagnostics labels appended by

asylum doctors; ”it is the history of familial relations which is

essential to appreciating the decision to commit”.32 As an example

of commitment because of economic necessity, Malcolm, in her

history of Swift’s Hospital, illustrates the dilemma encountered by

a certain Mary Larkin in her deciding to apply for the re-admission

of her husband Thomas.33

Incidence 1860-1875

There is no reliable information available on new cases of lunacy

arising in the Irish community in the 19th century. However

periodic census counts of “lunatics at large” were carried out by

the Royal Irish Constabulary at the request of the Inspectors of

Lunacy in the 200 constabulary areas of Ireland on a regular basis.

It might have been possible to assess an increasing incidence of

such cases by comparing one year with another and interpreting an

increase as indicating new cases on an annual basis. However the

arbitrariness of the Constabulary’s identification of lunacy, which

included intellectual disability, excludes the usefulness of this

information source. Accordingly it is necessary to rely on asylum

admission data as a measure of the incidence of schizophrenia as

returned in the Annual Reports on the District, Criminal and Private

Lunatic Asylums in Ireland as provided by the Inspectors of Lunacy,

the greatest proportion of which were to the district asylums.

Given that the incidence of schizophrenia is mostly concentrated in

the population aged between 15 and 30 years, it is assumed that

the impact of the Irish famines of 1845- 51 would be manifest

mainly in an increase in hospital admissions for lunacy, and more

specifically for conditions analogous to schizophrenia, in Ireland

between 1860 and 1875. Admissions to the district asylums, over

80% of which were recorded as first admissions, rose from 1,313

in 1860 to 2,132 in 1875 and then increased only slightly to 1880

when they numbered 2,366.

10

Page 11: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

There was, thus, an increase of 62% in the number of admissions

between 1860 and 1875. As the population fell over these years

the rate per 100,000 population rose more rapidly than the raw

numbers, from 22.6 to 40.4 or by 78.7%. However, there was no

decline in admissions in the years after 1880, when any famine

effect would be expected to have petered out. On the contrary,

admissions increased by a further 32% to 3,095 in 1890 and by

1898 had reached 3,469. First admissions constituted in and

around 80% from 1861 onwards. Admissions to private hospitals

increased from 165 in 1860 to 186 in 1875 and constituted 2,882

(9%) of 32,009 total private and public admissions over 1860-75.

No diagnostic information was furnished on these private

admissions, although it was provided on those resident each year

with mania predominating. Because the numbers are small and

there was little increase over the period surveyed they are

discounted from further consideration. Similarly the 290 admissions

to the Central Criminal Asylum over these years have been

discounted.

Diagnosis in the nineteenth century

Whereas in the 20th century medical practitioners were familiar

with the concept of schizophrenia, introduced in the early 1900s,

this was not the case in the preceding century. The term lunacy

was often employed generically and administratively to include

both those suffering from mental illness and those with intellectual

disability (idiots and imbeciles). However although this generic

usage is sometimes employed in official reports, such as the annual

inspection reports employed here, these usually use lunacy in the

more restricted sense of mental illness per se. Within lunacy a very

restricted classificatory system was used. The most common

diagnostic terms were mania, melancholia, monomania and

dementia. I have examined Irish asylum casebooks of the Sligo,

Mullingar and Richmond asylums from the 1850s to 1900 and

attempted, from the limited clinical descriptions available, to

reconcile these terms with diagnostic groupings currently in use in

the International Classification of Diseases 10th edition (ICD 10).

While in a minority of cases the symptoms and historical data

recorded are specific enough to warrant a diagnosis in modern

terms of depression with some confidence, this is not so in relation

to the broader concept of schizophrenia, an entity unknown as

such at that time. For example, delusional thinking and

hallucinatory experiences, characteristic of this disorder, were rarely

noted in the Irish nineteenth century case books quoted.

The following cases from the Sligo Asylum casebooks, typical of

the generality of admissions, illustrate the practical difficulties

encountered in diagnostics of the time.35 A 45 year old woman

was admitted under warrant to the asylum which stated that she

threatened and assaulted her mother with whom she did not get

on. Nevertheless she was given a diagnosis of chronic mania and

the supposed cause of her insanity was identified as “family

disturbances”. Confusingly the “duration of present attack” was

given as 15 years. The sparse case notes indicate only that she had

“from time to time maniacal symptoms”. She was still

undischarged seven years later. A 33 year old female was admitted

because she was alleged to have assaulted her sister and

threatened to burn her father’s house. The duration of her illness

prior to admission was said to be one month. Sixteen years

following admission she was said to be “noisy and refractory” and

she died in the asylum 41 years following admission. A 35 year old

male was admitted because he threatened to kill his wife and

family and was stated to have been ill for only three weeks prior to

admission. He was noted in his case notes to be suffering from

delusional insanity with jealous delusions concerning his wife but

was noted to have taken a lot of drink prior to admission. Three

weeks after admission he “escaped”, couldn’t be found and so

was discharged from the asylum register a fortnight later.

This contrasts markedly with the situation pertaining in late

nineteenth century Denbigh Asylum in North West Wales where

case notes were “more comprehensive than in other asylums of

the period” and were sufficiently detailed to permit consultant

psychiatrists to make an ICD diagnosis on each patient.36 A similar

exercise was possible at the Retreat in York, dealing with patients

admitted 1880-1884 where diagnostic information was sufficiently

explicit to allow of ICD conversions.37 More pertinent to the Irish

dilemma is the finding of researchers seeking to extract

schizophrenia from admissions to the Bethlem Royal Hospital 1853-

1862. Having reviewed the diagnostic criteria set out in

contemporary psychiatric textbooks they concluded that mania

comprised insanity with “confusion of ideas” and “encompassed

all syndromes where thought disorder was evident”.38 Moreover, in

identifying schizophrenia they excluded all persons aged over 40.

This is not possible in Ireland because of lack of cross-classification

by disease category and age in the Irish reports.

I now turn to estimating how many of Irish asylum admissions

should be classified as schizophrenia. There has been surprisingly

little attempt to determine what proportion of 19th century lunacy

emerges as schizophrenia as it is understood today. An outstanding

exception to this general scenario is that of Hare.23 In examining the

difficult issues involved Hare warned that “Before modern

diagnostic criteria became generally accepted, there was much

difference of opinion on what signs and outcomes were indicative

of a schizophrenic illness. And what was a difficult decision in

11

Table 2. Numbers of admissions to District Lunatic Asylums 1860-1875

1860 1,313 1864 1,242 1868 2,113 1872 2,165

1861 1,262 1865 1,291 1869 2,692 1873 2,277

1862 1,171 1866 1,782 1870 2,333 1874 2,154

1863 1,420 1867 1,527 1871 2,253 1875 2,132

Sources: 10 to 25th Reports on the Public, Criminal and Private Asylums of Ireland.34

Page 12: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

clinical practice was more likely to be contentious in a historical

setting even when a detailed history was available. Kraepelin, to

whom we owe the concept that was later to emerge as

schizophrenia, and which he had called dementia praecox, stated

that many of the cases he identified as dementia praecox had

previously been classed as mania or melancholia.39 Hare, speaking

of the 19th century, states that “we may assume that almost all

cases classified as “ordinary dementia” would have been

schizophrenia”.23 Further support for this is supplied by Tuke who

subdivided ordinary dementia into two forms. Primary dementia

was the form that occurs most especially in young persons of

feeble development and secondary was the form that “follows

acute attacks of insanity, maniacal or melancholic”.40 Of course the

clinical manifestations of schizophrenia may have changed over the

past 100 years, and while this is more likely to be true of earlier

periods, it is still a factor requiring consideration, in translating 19th

century case book descriptions, even when exhaustive as they

seldom were, in Irish 19th century case books, to the modern

concept of schizophrenia.

Mania was the predominant diagnosis of mental illness in

nineteenth century Ireland, comprising 72% of admissions 1860-

75. This clearly suggests, as pointed out above, that as well as

including mania as it is known today, other categories must have

been subsumed into this entity, most notably schizophrenia. This

assumption gains support from the contemporary evidence, that of

schizophrenia and mania first admissions combined, schizophrenia

now comprises 60%. I have assumed that the relative distribution

of these conditions was similar in the nineteenth century and have

re-classified 60% of nineteenth century admissions, classified then

as mania, to what we call schizophrenia today. I further assume

that the nineteenth century category monomania connotes

delusional disorder and belongs in its entirety to the schizophrenia

spectrum. The term dementia was employed non-specifically in the

nineteenth century admissions classification and seems often to

have included those with the negative symptoms of schizophrenia,

as well as dementia in the modern sense of cognitive failure of

organic origin, particularly in older persons. Support for this

contention is provided by the observation by Hare that the age –

incidence curve or age at first admission for dementia in the 19th

century was largely in the younger age groups and therefore not

compatible with the modern concept of this condition and he

therefore aligned much of it with schizophrenia.23 However this

cannot be tested in the Irish context as no data are available for

diagnosis cross- classified by age. I have therefore, somewhat

arbitrarily, allocated 50% of dementia admissions to schizophrenia.

As melancholia closely resembles depression as used today, this

category has been regarded as not contributing to schizophrenia

on the assumption that depressive syndromes were the

predominant characteristic of those labelled melancholia. However

as noted by Tuke40 some cases of melancholia, passing into a

chronic state, might conceivably be re-labelled dementia.

There were 1,313 admissions to district asylums in 1860, a year

when one would have expected famine–related admissions to

begin to appear. Excluding the 220 “relapsed” cases there were

1,093 first admissions, 83% of the total. Four categories of mental

illness accounted for 96% of all admissions (the remainder

comprised idiots and imbeciles). The breakdown of these four

categories was: mania 892 (68%), dementia 88 (7%), monomania

53 (4%) and melancholia 220 (17%). Here one encounters the

problem of assessing or best-guessing what proportion of these

could be validly regarded as schizophrenic. The assumption, based

on the very tenuous evidence of contemporary case- books with

their admittedly poor symptomatic descriptions, is made that the

following proportions of these categories were what today is called

schizophrenia, mania 60%, monomania 100%, dementia 50%,

and melancholia 0%. This yields an estimated total of 632

admissions for schizophrenia in 1860 (50% of all mental illness

admissions) or 524 first admissions on the basis that 83% were

first admissions. In 1875 there were 2,344 admissions of which

1,777 (83%) were first admissions. The equivalent estimate for

1875 was 1,006 schizophrenia admissions of which an estimated

834 were first admissions. The estimation therefore is that the first

admission rate for schizophrenia rose from 8.4 per 100,000 in

1860 to 15.6 in 1875. However, as the admission rate for non-

schizophrenia conditions increased to the same extent, the

proportion of all admissions classified as schizophrenia remained

stable. The following figures represent these data.

Admission rates per 100,000 population.

Figure 1. Schizophrenia admission rates per 100,000population 1860-1875 from 9th to 25th Reports on thePublic Criminal and Private Asylums of Ireland.41

Percentage of all admissions that were schizophrenia

Figure 2. Schizophrenia as a percentage of all admissions1860 – 1875.

12

Page 13: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Given that schizophrenia is predominantly a disorder of early life

with most incident cases arising in the late teens and early 20s I

have attempted to determine whether there was any increase in

the proportion of young persons admitted during the relevant years

as a proportion of all-aged admissions. The Inspectorial reports

supply age of admissions in decennial groups and not cross

classified by diagnosis. The group most relevant to this enquiry is

that of 20 – 30 years. The earliest years for which admission ages

were provided were in the combined years 1856 and 1857. In

these years 427 or 27.8% of 1,532 admissions were in this age

group. This percentage rose to 34.1 in 1868 and then subsided to

28.7 in 1875 thereby giving no support to the question of whether

schizophrenia increased insofar as it could be identified by age on

admission.42

The Irish Abroad

Robins contends that “the Irish who left home during the famine

and post-famine decades were more susceptible to insanity than

immigrants of other racial origins”.43 However he cites no

quantative evidence in support of this general contention although

he quotes instances where persons who had become mentally ill

following emigration were returned by their families or, as in the

case of the North American states, were returned by administrative

decree. Nonetheless there is abundant evidence of the high

prevalence of mental illness among the immigrant Irish compared

to indigenous populations both in the US and UK in the mid and

late 19th century. For example lunacy in Massachusetts was noted

to be high among the immigrant Irish.44 However this was in 1855

less than a decade after the major famines of the 1840s and

therefore too early to be related to them. Other reports of the high

proportion of Irish in asylum populations came from the Canadian

provinces, New Brunswick, before the famine years and Prince

Edward Island, subsequent to them, where in 1864 it was claimed

that as many as one third of admissions were of Irish immigrants

reflecting perhaps more than anything else the very high

proportion of the population that was Irish.45 Nonetheless high

rates of hospital admissions among the Irish of New York City and

State were still extant up to 1900.46 These were too long after the

major famines to be attributed to them. Moreover these rates rose

progressively throughout the latter part of the nineteenth century

and showed no exceptional peak between 1865 and 1875. The

rates of hospitalisation of Irish emigrants in North American

asylums declined over time, in keeping with the observation that as

each immigrant group becomes more assimilated and more

prosperous the rate of public psychiatric hospitalisation decreases.

Overall, though, these sparse and general observations shed little

light on the question at issue.

Irrespective of nutritional status, the trauma of migration and the

socio-cultural difficulties of assimilation may of themselves be

sufficient to increase mental disorder in migrant populations.

Classical examples of this have been identified by Odegaard among

Norwegian immigrants to Minnesota 47 and by Harrison among

Afro-Caribbean migrants to the United Kingdom.48 And the

migrant Irish in the United Kingdom, even to the first generation

UK born, appear to have poorer mental health than the indigenous

London community.49 Therefore caution must be invoked in

attributing mental illness rates to 19th century Irish immigrants

solely to maternal nutritional deficiency.

Discussion

The plausibility of famine disrupting neurodevelopment has a

sound empirical basis. Famine conditions have been shown to

result in decreased fertility, increase in miscarriage, stillbirths and

low birth weight. 4 Because schizophrenia is believed, in part, to

reflect developmental anomalies in brain structure it is relevant to

postulate that famine conditions, existing during pregnancy, may

result in neurodevelopmental damage to the foetus. Accordingly

the natural experiments of famine conditions in two jurisdictions

have been examined to determine whether there was an increase

in schizophrenia in individuals exposed to famine conditions during

pregnancy. These were respectively in Holland during World War 24

and in China during the Great Leap Forward famine.8 Because of

the severity of the Great Irish Famine and its resulting mortality it

is reasonable to suspect that the accompanying under-nutrition of

pregnant women may have similarly resulted in neurological

damage leading to an increase in schizophrenia some 15 to 30

years later.

The reason for the growth in the numbers of persons in Irish

asylums during the 19th century led to vigorous debate. Essentially

there were two points of view, those that held that it reflected a

real increase in lunacy and those who claimed that it was

artefactual and apparent rather than real. Explanatory reasons

deriving from ecological and environmental causes or influences

proliferated. However this increase was not uniquely Irish but a

feature of most European and North American jurisdictions.23

The data presented here show an increase in the incidence of

lunacy in Ireland as measured by asylum admissions some 15 to 30

years following the Irish famines of 1845-1850 historically referred

to as The Great Famine. Admissions of lunatics, that is those

suffering from mental illness, to asylums increased 1860-1875 from

1,313 to 2,132 or in rates per 100,000 population from 22.6 to

40.4, an increase of 78.7%. By assigning 60% of patients

diagnosed as mania, all those designated monomania and 50% of

those designated dementia to schizophrenia, estimates have been

made of the numbers likely to have been schizophrenia in those

first admitted in 1860 and 1875. Those first admissions identified

as schizophrenia rose numerically from 524 to 834 and in rates per

100,000 population from 8.4 to 15.6 an increase of 85.7%.

At first sight these increases in Ireland would seem to suggest that

the Irish famines did lead to an increased level of schizophrenia in

the population, but account has to be taken of the similar increases

in the other “non-schizophrenia” lunacies, mainly melancholia

whose first admissions rose from 31.7 to 45.5 per 100,000 from

1860-75 an increase of 43.5.41

In addition admission rates continued to increase up to the end of

the century long after the Great Famine could be held to be an

influence. As to the migrating Irish, other factors confound the

situation such as poverty and difficulties in assimilation of the

immigrant population, which are deemed to contribute to the high

hospitalisation rates in many immigrant communities.47

Up to 1881 the rate of first admission to population was higher in

England than it was in Ireland.50 The prevalence of persons in

asylums and workhouses in England rose from 70 per 100,000

13

Page 14: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

population in 1847 to 220 in 1877.51, 52 In Ireland the

corresponding figures were 76 and 234. It can therefore be

reasonably concluded that the rates of insanity (lunacy plus a small

number of idiots and imbeciles) were remarkably similar in both

jurisdictions and further that they increased at roughly the same

rate in the second half of the nineteenth century. In consequence

the record offers little support for the thesis that the increase in

rates was attributable to the famines of 1845-51.

As far as Ireland is concerned the evidence that the famines of the

late 1840s led to more schizophrenia is, at best, equivocal. The

apparent increase in the number of those schizophrenic was in the

context of an increase in lunacy generally and the increase

observed in Ireland was manifest throughout Europe and North

America in jurisdictions where there were no major famines.45

It is concluded that no unequivocal evidence can be adduced to

support the hypothesis that the Great Irish famine increased

schizophrenia.

Limitations

It may be objected that 1860 is too early a date for the

consequences of the famines to be revealed in asylum admissions.

Furthermore, the timing of the admissions may not have coincided

exactly with the onset of the condition. Whereas the Inspectorial

Reports of the 1860s tabulate the duration of disease prior to

admission of those discharged as “recovered”, this information is

not provided for new admissions nor is it cross-classified by type of

disease. In addition, form of disease on admission was not provided

in the reports after 1875 until 1890 and by then monomania no

longer appeared as an entity. This is noteworthy as it might be

objected that delusional disorder has a later onset than other

components of the schizophrenia spectrum.

A shortage of asylum beds could have caused delays in admissions

in the 1860s and increased availability of beds – rather than

increased incidence of mental illness – could have accounted for

some of the rise in admissions in later years. By 1875, although the

number of asylum places available had increased considerably, by

almost 3,000 from 1860, admissions fell slightly between 1870

and 1875, as did the number of lunatics reported as at large.34

The use of a single year at either end of exploration may be

questioned but there was little variation in the characteristics of

the admission or residential clienteles over the years of survey.

Finally, it is acknowledged that the sparseness of clinical data in

nineteenth century asylum case books in Ireland does not allow

translation of what little clinically descriptive material there is into

meaningful diagnostic entities as applied today, a difficulty not

confined to Ireland.23 The most restricting limitation, therefore, is

the derivation of rates of schizophrenia based on purely personal

and empirical extrapolation from 19th century diagnostics.

Acknowledgement

I wish to thank Dr Brian Donnelly of the Irish National Archive for

his help and advice and particularly for making the casebooks of

the Sligo District Lunatic Asylum available to me.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

1. Warner, R. (1995) Time trends in schizophrenia: change in obstetric risk factors with

industrialization. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 21. 3, 1-30.

2. Guilmatre A, Dubourg C, Mosca A-L, Legallie S, Goldenberg A, Drouin-Garrand V,

Lavet V, Rosier A, Briault S, Bonnet-Brithault F, Laumonnier F, Odent S, Le Vacon G,

Joly-Helas G, David D, Bendavid C, Pinooit J- M, Henry C, Impallomenmi C, Germano

E, Tortorella G, Di Rosa G, Barthelemy C, Andres C, Faivre L, Frebourg T, Saugier Veber

P, Campion D (2009). Recurrent rearrangements in synaptic and neurodevelopment

genes and shared biologic pathways in schizophrenia, autism and mental retardation.

Archives of General Psychiatry 66. 947-936.

3. Haukka J, Suvisaari J, Hakkinen L, Lonnqvist J. (2008). Growth pattern and risk of

schizophrenia Psychological Medicine 38. 63-70.

4. Hoek HW, Brown AS, Susser E. (1998). The Dutch famine and schizophrenia spectrum

disorders. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 33. 373-379.

5. Jablensky A. (1986). Epidemiology of schizophrenia: a European perspective .

Schizophrenia Bulletin. 12. 52-73.

6. Hulshoff HE, Hoek HW, Susser E, Brown AS, Dingemans A, Schnack HG, Van Haren

NE, Pereira Ramos LM, Gispen de Wied C,, Kahn RS. (2000). Prenatal exposure to

famine schizophrenia and brain morphology. American of Psychiatry. 157. 1170-1172.

7. Song S, Wang W, Hu P. (2009). Famine, death and madness. Schizophrenia in early

adulthood after prenatal exposure to the Chinese Great Leap Forward Famine.

University of California eScholarship Repository.

8. St. Clair D, Xu M, Wang P, Yu Y, Fang Y, Zhang Y, Zheng X, Gu N, Feng, G, Sham P,

He L (2005). Rates of adult schizophrenia following prenatal exposure to the Chinese

Famine of 1959-1961. Journal of the American Medical Association. 294. 557-562.

9. O’Grada C. (1989). The Great Irish Famine, p. 76. McMillan. London.

10. Connell KH. (1950). The Population of Ireland 1750-1845, p. 146. The Clarendon

Press. Oxford.

11. Clarkson LA, Crawford EM. (2001). Feast and Famine. Food and Nutrition in Ireland

1500-1920. Oxford University Press. Oxford.

12. ibid p. 59

13. ibid p. 87

14. O’Brien G. (1918). The Economic History of Ireland in the Eighteenth Century, p 102.

Maunsel & Co. Dublin.

15. Clarkson LA, Crawford EM. (2001). Feast and famine. Food and Nutrition in Ireland

1500-1920, p. 112. Oxford University Press. Oxford

16. Wilde W. The census of Ireland for the year 1851, part v. Table of deaths, vol 1 11, p

1856 (2087 11) XXX, The Census of Ireland for the year 1861, part iii. Vital statistics,

vol. ii. Report and table relating to deaths, PP 1863 (3204-2) LVIII.

17. O’Grada C. (1999). Black 47 and Beyond, pp 226-227. Princeton University Press.

Princeton New Jersey

18. Finnane M. (1981). Insanity and the Insane in Post-Famine Ireland. Croom Helm.

London

19. Walsh D. (2008). The lunatic asylums of Ireland 1825-1839. Irish Journal of

Psychological Medicine. 24. 24- 28.

20. Lunatic Asylums - Ireland (1872). The Twenty-First Report of the District, Criminal, and

Private Lunatic Asylums in Ireland with appendices. HC1872 (c 647) xxvii.323. p 60

Alex Thom. Dublin.

21. Lunatic Asylums – Ireland (1876). The Twenty-Fifth Report of the District, Criminal.

and Private Lunatic Asylums in Ireland with appendices. HC 1876 (c 1496) xxxiii.363.

p53. Alex Thom, Dublin.

22. Scull AT. (1979). Museums of Madness: the Social Organisation of Insanity in

Nineteenth Century England. Allen Lane. London.

23. Hare EH. (1988). Schizophrenia as a recent disease. British Journal of Psychiatry. 153.

521-531.

14

Page 15: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

24. Jablensky A, Morgan V, Zubrick S, Bower C, Yelleachich L-A. (2005). Pregnancy,

delivery and neonatal complications in a population cohort of women with

schizophrenia and major affective disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry. 162. 79-

81.

25. Scull AT. (1984). Was insanity increasing? A response to Edward Hare. British Journal

of Psychiatry. 144. 432-436.

26. McCabe F. (1869). On the alleged increase in insanity. Journal of Mental Science. 15.

363-366.

27. Drapes T. (1894). On the alleged increase of insanity in Ireland. Journal of Mental

Science. 171. 519- 548.

28. Special Report from the Inspectors of Lunatics to the Chief Secretary (1884). Alleged

increasing prevalence of insanity in Ireland. Alexander Thom & Co. Dublin.

29. Finnane M. (1981). Insanity and the Insane in Post-Famine Ireland, p 169. Croom

Helm. London.

30. Reynolds J. (1992). Grangegorman. Psychiatric Care in Dublin since 1815, p. 140 and

quoting Statute 7 31 Vic 118. Institute of Public Administration. Dublin.

31. Finnane M. (1981). Insanity and the Insane in Post-Famine Ireland, p 223. Croom

Helm. London.

32. Finnane M. (1985). Asylums, Families and the State, p. 137. History Workshop. No 20

(Autumn, 1985).

33. Malcolm E. (1989). Swift’s Hospital. A History of St Patrick’s Hospital 1746-1989, p.

144.Gill and Macmillan, Dublin.

34. Lunatic Asylums – Ireland (1861 – 1876). The 10th to the 25th Reports on the Public,

Criminal and Private Asylums in Ireland. Alex Thom. Dublin.

35. Male and Female Casebooks of Sligo District Lunatic Asylum 1892-1893. National

Archive of Ireland. Dublin.

36. Healy D, Savage M, Michael P, Harris M, Hirst D, Carter M, Cattell D, McMonagle T,

Sohler N, Susser E. (2001). Psychiatric bed utilisation:1896 and 1996 compared.

Psychological Medicine 31. 779-790.

37. Renvoize EB, Beveridge AW. (1989). Mental illness and the Victorians. A study of

patients admitted to three asylums in York, 1880-1884. Psychological Medicine. 19.

19-28.

38. Klaf FS, Hamilton JG. (1961). Schizophrenia – a hundred years ago and today. Journal

of Mental Science. 107. 819-827.

39. Kraepelin E. (1913). Psychiatrie (8th edn). Translated by R.M. Barclay from vol 3 part

2 as Dementia Praecox and Paraphrenia. Livingstone. Edinburgh.

40. Tuke DH. (1882). Dictionary of Psychological Medicine. Churchill. London.

41. Lunatic Asylums – Ireland (1861 – 1876). The 9th to 25th Reports on the Public,

Criminal and Private Asylums of Ireland. HC 1861 (2901) xxvii. 245 and HC 1876

(c.1496) xxxiii. 363. Alex Thom. Dublin.

42. Lunatic Asylums – Ireland. (1857, 1868-1869 and 1876) The 8th, 18th and 24th

Reports on the Public, Criminal and Private Asylums of Ireland. HC 1857 Session 2

(2253) xvii. 67, HC 1868-69 (4053) xxxi. 303 and HC 1876 (c.1496) xxxiii. 363. Alex

Thom. Dublin.

43. Robins J. (1986). Fools & Mad. A History of the Insane in Ireland, p. 121. Institute of

public Administration. Dublin.

44. Jarvis E. (1855). Report on Insanity and Idiocy in Massachusetts by the Commission on

Lunacy. William White. Boston.

45. Fuller Torrey E, Miller J. (2007). The Invisible Plague. The Rise of Mental Illness from

1750 to the Present, p.163. Rutgers University Press. New Brunswick, New Jersey, and

London.46. Malzberg B, Lee ES. (1956) Migration and Mental Disease, Social Science

Research Council. New York

47. Odegaard O. (1932). Emigration and insanity. A study of Norwegian-born

populations of Minnesota. Acta Psychiatrica et Neurologica (Supplement 4).

1-206.

48. Harrison G, Owens D, Holton A, Neilson D, Boot D. (1988). A prospective

study of severe mental illness in Afro-Caribbean patients. Psychological

Medicine. 18, 3. 643-657.

49. Ryan L, Leavey G, Golden A, Blizzard M, King M. (2006). Depression n Irish

emigrants living in London: case-control study. British Journal of Psychiatry.

188. 560-566.

50. Fuller Torrey E, Miller J. (2007). The Invisible Plague. The Rise of Mental Illness

from 1750 to the Present, p. 109. Rutgers University Press. New Brunswick,

New Jersey and London.

51. Lunatic Asylums- Ireland (1849). Report on the District, Local and Private

Asylums 1847, HC 1849 (1054) xxiii. 53.

52. Lunatic Asylums – Ireland (1878), 27th report on the District, Criminal, and

Private Asylums of Ireland 1877. HC 1878 (c. 2037) xxxix. 395.

15

Page 16: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Original Paper

Study of Presentationsfor Involuntary Admission to a Cork Approved CentreElaine Dunne, Eamonn Moloney

Abstract

Objectives: To describe the characteristics of patients who presentto an approved centre with Mental Health Act 2001 forms, and

secondly, to compare those who were subsequently detained to

those who were not detained.

Methods: Specific data on patients who presented to South LeeMental Health Unit with application and recommendation forms

for Involuntary Admission over a 22 month period was gathered

from a retrospective case note review. Information on both groups

was compared statistically using Graph Pad Prism software.

Results: 71% (n=121) of patients presenting for involuntary

admission did so outside of normal working hours. Those who

were not subsequently detained were more likely to have had their

application made by the Gardai and their recommendation made

by someone other than their own General Practitioner (GP). GPs

were more likely than Consultant Psychiatrists to cite risk to self or

others as the reason for involuntary admission.

Conclusion: Although involuntary admissions most often occur inemergency situations, every effort should be made to ensure that

those who are known to the patient are involved in the process of

application and recommendation. In addition, there is a need for

ongoing training and education of those most commonly involved,

such as the Gardai and General Practitioners, as well as feedback

to these groups when a patient presents who does not require

involuntary admission.

Key words: Mental Health Act 2001; Involuntary admission;Detention

Introduction

Part 2 of the Irish Mental Health Act (MHA) 2001 was implemented

on November 1st 2006 and introduced new procedures for the

involuntary admission of patients to approved centres. The Mental

Health Commission has produced a report on the operation of the

MHA1 and there have been numerous legal challenges to the

legislation in the form of Article 40 challenges, judicial reviews and

High Court and Supreme Court challenges.

South Lee Mental Health Unit caters for a catchment area

population of 179,000. The approved centre is located in Cork

University Hospital and has 46 inpatient beds with approximately

500 admissions per year and had an involuntary admission rate of

54.7/100,000 in 2007.

The objective of this study was to describe all those patients who

presented to the South Lee Mental Health Unit at Cork University

Hospital with an application and medical recommendation for

admission under the MHA over a 22 month period in order to

establish if these patients were similar to those described in

national data. A comparison was also made of those patients for

whom an admission order was completed and those who did not

have an admission order completed. Such patients are, by

definition, deprived of their liberty for up to twenty four hours and

it is important that their management is reviewed.

Methods

Records were kept of all patients who presented to the South Lee

Mental Health Unit with completed application and medical

recommendation forms for admission under the Mental Health Act

2001 between January 2007 and October 2008. Cases of those

who were subsequently detained and those who were not

detained were identified from these records and included in the

study. The period originally planned to be studied was a two year

period from the implementation of the MHA 2001 (i.e. from

November 2006 to October 2008). It was decided; however, to

omit cases from the initial two month period as the high number

of incorrectly completed forms due to an adjustment period during

this time was likely to affect the overall results, making them less

representative.

A retrospective case note review was carried out to examine the

demographic characteristics, the reasons for referral, the past

psychiatric history, diagnosis and presenting symptoms of these

patients. Where full medical notes were not available, all data was

retrieved from copies of initial assessments, electronically recorded

patient details and copies of MHA 2001 forms.

16

* Elaine Dunne,South Lee Mental Health Services,Cork University Hospital, Wilton, Co. Cork, Ireland.E-mail [email protected]

Eamon Moloney,Consultant Psychiatrist/Clinical Director,South Lee Mental Health Services,Cork University Hospital, Wilton, Co. Cork, Ireland.

Submitted January 13th 2011Accepted August 15th 2011

Ir J Psych Med 2012; 29 (1): 16-21

* Correspondence

Page 17: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

The application, medical recommendation and admission order

forms were reviewed and specific details recorded. Data in relation

to patients where an admission order was completed was

compared to data in relation to those patients where an admission

order was not completed and comparisons were also made with

national data on patients admitted under the MHA 2001.

Voluntary patients who were regraded and detained under the

MHA during their admission were not included.

All statistical calculations were performed using Graph Pad Prism

version 4.0 for windows (Graph Pad Software, San Diego, CA,

USA) and student t test (Mann Whitney U test for nonparametric

data) or Chi square test/Fisher’s exact test were used as appropriate

to identify significant differences between groups.

Results

During the 22 month study period, 171 patients presented to the

unit with an application and recommendation for involuntary

admission completed. 81% (n=139) of these patients were

detained under the MHA following the completion of an admission

order. Comparison of details of those who were detained to

available national statistics and secondly, to those who were not

detained, is presented in Table 1 and Table 2, respectively.

Almost three quarters of those patients who presented for

assessment for involuntary detention did so outside of regular

working hours with 29% (n=50) presenting between 9am and

5pm, Monday to Friday.

In 29% (n=50) of presentations, the patient was brought to the

approved centre by the gardai without an assisted admission team

or relatives. This occurred significantly more often in those patients

where an admission order was not subsequently completed than in

those patients who were subsequently detained under the MHA,

50% (n=16) and 24% (n=34) respectively (xx2 = 8.20 ; p=0.0042)

For the group of patients where an admission order was not

completed, there was a significantly higher number of applications

made by a garda compared to the group where an admission order

was completed (31% (n=10) v 12% (n=16); xx2 = 7.86; p=0.0051).

Of all patients presenting with MHA forms during this time, 56%

(n=95) had their medical recommendation made by their own

General Practitioner. This includes 60% (n=84) of those who were

detained and only 34% (n=11) of those who were not detained.

The difference was statistically significant (xx2 = 7.15; p=0.0075).

There was a significant difference between General Practitioners

and Consultant Psychiatrists in relation to the criteria used for

recommendation or admission (Table 3). In relation to those

patients who were detained under MHA, 40% (n=56) of general

practitioners considered the patient to be a risk to self or others

but only 20% (n=28) of consultant psychiatrists thought so (xx2 =

12.52; p=0.0004). Consultant Psychiatrists used criterion (b) alone

(risk of serious deterioration etc.) in 68% (n=95) of completed

admission orders, which was a significantly higher rate than the

50% (n=70) of cases for GPs (xx2 = 8.57; p=0.0034). There was no

difference in the number of times in which consultant psychiatrists

and GPs felt that both criteria were satisfied (12% (n=17) and 7%

(n=10) respectively). Neither box was ticked by the GP in 3% (n=4)

of cases. The Consultant and GP opinion as to the reason for

admission differed in 37% (n=41) of individual cases.

In relation to primary diagnosis, there was a significantly higher

frequency of substance misuse related illness (38% (n=12) v 4%

(n=5); Fisher’s exact test p<0.0001) and personality disorder (9%

(n=3) v 0% (n=0); Fisher’s exact test p=0.0061), and a lower

frequency of schizophreniform illness (19% (n=6) v 53% (n=74); xx2

= 12.43; p=0.0004) in those who were not detained compared to

those patients where an admission order was completed. There

was no significant difference between the groups in relation to all

other primary diagnoses.

Similar proportions of those with a primary diagnosis of substance

abuse were actually reported to have a dual diagnosis (20% (n=1)

of those detained v 25% (n=3) of those not detained). 60% (n=3)

of those admitted as involuntary patients despite a primary

diagnosis of substance abuse had a secondary psychotic illness and

20% (n=1) had a secondary depressive illness. One person (0.8%)

from the group of patients with a primary diagnosis of substance

abuse who were not detained had a secondary psychiatric illness

(alcohol dementia). Of note, the Mental Health Act 2001 does not

permit detention as an involuntary patient based on a diagnosis of

substance dependence or personality disorder.

In addition, those who were detained were less likely to be using

substances at the time of presentation than those who were not

detained (25% (n=35) v 44% (n=14); xx2 = 4.39; p=0.0362).

The final part of the study examined the outcomes of the group of

patients who were not detained (Table 4). 6% (n=2) of those who

presented with MHA forms but who were not initially detained

were admitted or regraded as involuntary patients within a week

of presentation.

Discussion

This study sought to explore how Part 2 of the MHA 2001 was

operating in a busy, acute psychiatric service. The importance of

striking a balance between ensuring that those patients who have

an acute psychiatric illness and are at risk, receive appropriate

treatment, and avoiding unnecessary infringements of a person’s

liberty is of critical importance in a modern mental health service.

Recent research examining the attitudes of psychiatrists2,3 and

service users4 with regards to involuntary admission under the MHA

2001 suggests that, though significant concerns remain among

psychiatrists regarding multiple aspects of implementation of the

Act, service users reflected positively on their involuntary admission.

The vast majority of patients presenting for admission under the

MHA are presenting outside of normal working hours; in just over

half of all cases (56%), the medical recommendation is made by

the patient’s own general practitioner and in approximately one

third of cases the admission order is completed by the patient’s

sector consultant. This may simply reflect the emergency nature of

MHA admissions but the involvement of professionals who are

17

Page 18: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

most familiar with patients potentially requiring detention under

the MHA is to be preferred. There may be scope to plan MHA

assessments so that a patient’s own GP and Consultant Psychiatrist

are involved in these decisions but this is obviously not always

possible.

Approximately four out of every five patients presenting for

admission under the MHA had an admission order completed and

seventy five per cent of those patients where an admission order

was not completed were admitted to hospital either as a voluntary

psychiatric admission or to a general hospital ward. The latter

figure highlights the importance of always considering an organic

cause to a psychiatric presentation. Overall, only 4% of those

presenting for admission under the MHA did not require inpatient

treatment, indicating that the vast majority of patients presenting

in these circumstances warranted referral to hospital.

The demographic characteristics and diagnoses of patients in this

study were mainly similar to those of patients included in the

Mental Health Commission’s review of the MHA, though the study

group had a higher proportion of patients with a diagnosis of

schizophrenia. 22% (n=332) of those in the national statistics did

not have a diagnosis recorded however and it is likely that at least

some of these would have had a diagnosis of a schizophreniform

illness. Rates for schizophreniform and affective illnesses were

similar to recent studies in other centres.5

Those patients who were not detained were more likely to have a

primary diagnosis of substance abuse related disorder or

personality disorder and of course both personality disorder and

alcohol or drug dependence are specifically excluded as conditions

where admission under MHA 2001 is allowed. This is an

interesting finding, highlighting the fact that general practitioners

continue to recommend involuntary admission for those with a

diagnosis of substance dependence despite the rules of the Act. It

raises the issue as to whether forms which report substance

dependence as the reason for admission should be automatically

considered invalid if no other diagnosis is specified and rewritten

if necessary. It appears that currently, the process tends to continue

regardless of the reported illness mentioned on the application and

recommendation forms.

One concern from the findings in relation to substance

dependence might be that patients with a dual diagnosis who

need admission may be prevented from being admitted as

involuntary patients due to the stipulations in the Act. This however

is unlikely to have been the case here as most (66%; n=8) of those

with a diagnosis of substance abuse who were not detained had

no other psychiatric illness recorded and none of this group has

required involuntary admission since that presentation.

Patients who were not subsequently detained were more likely to

have had an application made by a Garda and a medical

recommendation made by a general practitioner who was not

known to them. This group of patients were more likely to have

been conveyed to the approved centre by the gardai and to have

no past psychiatric history. These findings draw attention to the

particular group of patients presenting for involuntary admission in

emergency situations without the input of someone who is known

to them. These people tended not to require detention under the

Act, and though these situations may be difficult to avoid,

especially where the person is unknown to the service, every effort

should be made by those making the application and

recommendation to gain collateral from someone known to the

patient so that any unnecessary referrals for involuntary admission

could be avoided if possible.

This also emphasises the importance of appropriate training of

those commonly involved in the process of involuntary admission

and in particular, that it is essential to provide feedback to those

parties where an inappropriate application is made. This is often

overlooked in a busy service and it is frequently difficult to contact

those who may have been involved in the emergency situation.

There may be a role for introducing standard procedures whereby

feedback should be carried out automatically by the team, for

example, within a week to discuss the case and why the person

did not require involuntary admission, where an application and

recommendation have been made but the patient is not detained.

This would hopefully reduce the frequency of such instances and

improve communication between parties involved.

The legal criteria for making a medical recommendation for

involuntary admission by a general practitioner or an admission

order by a consultant psychiatrist are detailed on the relevant forms

5 and 6 respectively. The general practitioner or consultant

psychiatrist must be satisfied that the patient is suffering from a

mental disorder and that criterion (a), criterion (b) or both criteria

(a) and (b) are satisfied. Criterion (a) refers to “a serious likelihood

of the person concerned causing immediate and serious harm” to

self or others and criterion (b) refers to the person’s impaired

judgement and that failure to admit the person “would be likely to

lead to a serious deterioration” in their condition or “prevent the

administration of appropriate treatment that could be given only by

such admission” and that this “would be likely to benefit or

alleviate the condition of that person to a material extent”. Of

note, recommendation and admission order forms originally only

permitted a choice of either (a) or (b). The forms were revised,

however, midway through the study in December 2007 following

a court case, to add the possibility of both (a) and (b) applying.

In almost two thirds of cases there was agreement between the

Consultant Psychiatrist and General Practitioner as to the reason

for involuntary admission. However, General Practitioners seemed

to have a significantly lower threshold for believing that the patient

was a risk to themselves or others. This is not surprising given that

Consultant Psychiatrists should be the most qualified to make an

accurate assessment of risk. Also, it is much more appropriate that

there is a tendency of general practitioners to over, rather than

under estimate risk. Disagreements as to the reason for detention

may be explained by the fact that the patient may have improved

with treatment by the time they were assessed by the Consultant.

Another explanation may be that General Practitioners may have

a different understanding or interpretation of the term “serious

and immediate risk” compared to Consultants.

A limitation of the study is that the number of people in the group

who were not detained is small relative to the group who were

detained. In addition, the group examined were from one

catchment area and may not be representative of other services.

18

Page 19: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Most figures, however, are consistent with national statistics and

therefore the profile of patients in other similar services would not

be expected to differ greatly. The main strength of the study is that

it provides a comprehensive profile of those all those presenting to

an approved centre under the MHA 2001 over a substantial period

of time, identifying a number of key areas of interest.

Conclusion

This study examines every case where a patient was brought to a

busy acute unit for involuntary admission over a substantial time

period. Over 70% of those presenting did so outside of normal

working hours highlighting the emergency nature of acute

psychiatry. Application and recommendation for involuntary

admission is a serious process which impacts on a patient’s freedom

and should only occur in the best interests of the patient. It is

particularly important to examine cases where the patient is not

subsequently detained. Reassuringly, out of all of those presenting

for involuntary admission, only 4% did not require any form of

admission to hospital. Similar to recent research in this area,5 the

findings of this study suggest a need for further education for

those involved in the process of application and recommendation,

in particular for the Gardai and General Practitioners, and feedback

where an inappropriate presentation for involuntary admission

occurs. Involuntary admissions most frequently occur in the context

of emergency situations, however, it is clear that every effort should

be made to involve people known to the patient in each step of the

process where possible.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

1. Mental Health Commission. Report on the operation of part 2 of the Mental

Health Act 2001(April 2008) available at http://www.mhcirl.ie/Publications

2. O’Donoghue B., Moran B. Consultant psychiatrists’ experiences and attitudes

following the introduction of the Mental Health Act 2001: a national survey.

Ir J Psych Med 2009 Mar; 26(1): 23, 24-6

3. Jabbar F., Kelly BD., Casey P. National survey of psychiatrists’ responses to

implementation of the Mental Health Act 2001 in Ireland. Ir J Med Sci 2010;

179: 291-294

4. O’ Donoghue B., Lyne J., Hill M., Larkin C., Feeney L., O’Callaghan E.

Involuntary admission from the patients’ perspective. Soc Psychiat Epidemiol

2010; 45:631-638

5. Rafiq A., O’Hanlon M. An audit of incomplete involuntary admissions to an

approved Irish centre. Ir J Psych Med 2010 Sep; 27(3): 143-147

19

Table 1: Comparison of study patients who were detained and the national statistics.

Detained National �X2/Fisher’s ORFigures1 Exact Test 95% CI

n % n %

GenderMale 80 58 831 55 1.10;

Female 59 42 632 45 0.26;p=0.61 0.77 to 1.56

Age18-64 114 82 1251 83 0.92:

65+ 25 18 252 17 0.13;p=0.71 0.58 to 1.45

Diagnosis

Schizophreniform illness 74 53 585 39 10.85;p=0.001* 1.79; 1.26 to 2.53

Mania 37 27 313 21 2.55;p=0.11 1.38; 0.93 to 2.05

Depression 8 6 84 6 0.0007;p=0.93 1.03; 0.49 to 2.18

Organic 13 9 84 6 3.24;p=0.07 1.74; 0.95 to 3.21

Substance related 5 4 57 4 0.01;p=0.92 0.95; 0.38 to 2.42

Personality 0 0 9 1 p=1.00 0.56; 0.03 to 9.75

Neurosis 2 1 22 1 p=1.00 0.98; 0.23 to 4.23

No Mental Illness 0 0 0 0 N/A N/A

Applicant

Relative 108 78 1034 69 4.76;p=0.03* 1.58; 1.04 to 2.39

Authorised Officer 4 1 102 7 p=0.07 0.41; 0.15 to 1.12

Member of Public 11 8 132 9 0.12;p=0.73 0.89; 0.47 to 1.70

Gardai 16 12 235 15 1.67;p=0.20 0.70; 0.41 to 1.20

Note: National figures relate to patients detained using Form 6 nationally in 2007.*statistically significant difference

Page 20: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

20

Detained (n=139) Not detained (n=32) t, U or Χ2;p value* OR95% CI

n % n %GenderMale 80 58 16 50 0.60; 0.44 1.36Female 59 42 16 50 0.63 to 2.93

Age18-64 114 82 26 81 2119;65+ 25 18 6 19 0.64

Diagnosis

Schizophreniform illness 74 53 6 19 12.43; 0.0004* 4.91.91 to 12.74

Mania 37 27 5 16 1.70; 0.19 1.960.70 to 5.47

Depression 8 6 2 6 1.00 0.920.19 to 4.54

Organic 13 9 3 9 1.00 0.990.27 to 3.73

Substance related 5 4 12 38 < 0.0001* 0.0620.02 to 0.20

Personality 0 0 3 9 0.01 0.030.0015 to 0.60

Neurosis 2 1 0 0 1.00 1.180.06 to 25.23

No Mental Illness 0 0 1 3 0.19 0.080.00 to 1.89

History of mental illness 122 88 17 53 22.45; 7.06< 0.0001* 2.93 to 16.98

Known to service 112 81 14 44 18.24; 0.19< 0.0001* 0.08 to 0.42

On Call Presentation 96 69 26 81 1.89; 0.17 0.520.20 to 1.35

Applicant

Relative 108 78 20 63 3.19; 2.090.07 0.92 to 4.75

Authorised Officer 4 1 0 0 1.00 2.090.11 to 39.90

Member of Public 11 8 2 6 1.00 1.250.26 to 5.93

Gardai 16 12 10 31 7.86; 0.290.0051* 0.12 to 0.71

Arrival

Family/Nursing home 53 38 10 31 N/A 0.53; 0.47 1.360.60 to 3.09

Gardai only 34 24 16 50 N/A 8.20; 0.320.0042* 0.15 to 0.72

Assisted admission 37 27 4 13 N/A 0.11 2.540.83 to 7.73

Ambulance 8 6 2 6 N/A 1.00 0.920.19 to 4.54

Transfer from another ward 7 5 0 0 N/A 0.35 3.680.20 to 66.14

Recommendation 84 60 11 34 N/A 7.15 2.92by own GP 0.0075* 1.30 to 6.52Assessed by 52 37 11 34 N/A 0.10; 0.75 1.14sector consultant 0.51 to 2.56Collateral Available 112 81 29 91 N/A 0.21 0.43

0.12 to 1.51Current Substance Use 35 25 14 44 N/A 4.39; 0.43

0.0362* 0.20 to 0.96English speaking 137 99 30 94 N/A 0.16 4.57

0.62 to 33.74

* statistically significant difference

Table 2: Comparison of study patients who were detained to those not detained

Page 21: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

21

Table 3. Criteria used for recommendation and admission

Criteria for admission General Practitioner Consultant Psychiatrist Statistic

n % n %

(a) 56 40 28 20 Χ2 =12.52;p=0.0004*(b) 70 50 95 68 Χ2 = 8.57;p=0.0034*(a) and (b) 10 7 17 12 Χ2 =2.01;p=0.1563Neither 4 3 0 0 Fisher’s exact test; p=0.1223

(a)=Risk of immediate and serious harm to self or others.

(b)=Risk of serious deterioration etc.

*statistically significant difference

Table 4. Outcome where admission order was not signed

n %

Voluntary admission 22 69

Discharge 8 25

Other admission (e.g. medical) 2 6

Involuntary admission within one week 2 6

Note: those who were admitted as involuntary patients within one week are also included in the numbers of patients admitted voluntarily,

discharged or admitted medically.

Page 22: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Objective: To determine the prevalence of mental illness amongthe residents of a homeless hostel in inner city Dublin.

Method: A cross-sectional survey was carried out among hostelresidents, as previous studies have indicated that homeless hostel-

dwelling men in Dublin constitute the largest single grouping of

homeless Irish people. All agreeable residents were interviewed by

the authors over an eight-week period using the Structured Clinical

Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-I) Clinical Version. For

each disorder, the current (30-day) and past prevalence was

determined.

Results: A total of 38 residents were interviewed, resulting in aresponse rate of 39.2% for the study. A total of 81.6% of residents

had a current Axis I diagnosis; this number increased to 89.5%

when combining current and past diagnoses. Only four residents

had no diagnosis. There was considerable comorbidity between

disorders, with a significant number of residents experiencing both

mental illness and substance use problems. When considering

lifetime diagnoses, 31.6% had a single diagnosis only; 57.9% had

two/more diagnoses. Twelve residents (31.6%) had been admitted

to a psychiatric hospital during their lifetime. The most prevalent

disorders during the past month were Alcohol Dependence

(23.7%), Opioid Dependence and Major Depressive Disorder (both

18.4%), Opioid Abuse and Alcohol-Induced Depression (both

7.9%). Only 23.7% of interviewed residents were attending

psychiatric or addiction services. A significant number of residents

who did not wish to participate in the study were identified by

hostel staff as having a confirmed psychiatric diagnosis.

Conclusion: The survey demonstrated a very high prevalence ofmental disorders among homeless hostel residents. The high

prevalence of dual diagnosis highlights the need for greater

collaboration between psychiatric services and addiction services.

The outcome also points to the importance of providing mental

health training to emergency shelter/hostel staff. Research into the

mental health status of the homeless should be undertaken

regularly if services are to be planned to meet the needs of this

vulnerable group.

Key words: Homeless, Hostel, Mental illness, Dual diagnosis

Introduction

PREVALENCE OF MENTAL ILLNESS AMONG THE HOMELESS

Studies have consistently demonstrated high rates of mental

disorders among people who are homeless.1 The rates of

psychiatric morbidity among homeless adults will vary according

to the type of homelessness: sleeping rough, using a night shelter,

staying in special hostels, and using temporary leased

accommodation.2 International comparisons also demonstrated

significant cross-cultural differences in the prevalence of mental

disorders among the homeless.1

In an Australian study 73% of homeless men and 81% of homeless

women in inner Sydney met criteria for at least one mental disorder

in the past year and 40% of the men and 50% of the women had

at least two mental disorders.1 Among the homeless population in

Utrecht, The Netherlands, 32% of people had depression, 15% a

schizophrenic disorder and 52% an antisocial personality disorder.3

In Madrid, Spain, 67% of homeless people had some type of

mental disorder.4 In Stockholm, Sweden, 47% of the homeless

suffered from mental illness.5 A survey in Belfast indicated that

approximately 25% of the homeless in Belfast hostels have a

diagnosed mental disorder.6

The most prominent mental disorders among the homeless are

depression, affective disorders, substance abuse, psychotic

disorders, schizophrenia, and personality disorders.7 Significant

numbers of homeless people experience the coexistence of mental

health issues and substance use problems (dual diagnosis).8

THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN HOMELESSNESS AND MENTAL

ILLNESS

The causal link between homelessness and mental ill-health is the

subject of an ongoing debate. Some argue that the psychiatric

problems of many of the homeless may result directly from their

poverty and associated lack of accommodation. Others contend

that the majority first experienced their symptoms of mental

disorder before becoming homeless.2

22

*Bernice Prinsloo,Registrar,Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, Our Lady’s Children’s Hospital,Crumlin, Dublin 12, Ireland.E-mail [email protected]

Catherine Parr,Registrar in Psychiatry,James Connolly Memorial Hospital,Blanchardstown, Dublin 15, Ireland.

Joanne Fenton,Consultant Psychiatrist,ACCES Team,Dublin 8, Ireland.

Submitted December 15th 2010Accepted March 21st 2011

Ir J Psych Med 2012; 29 (1): 22-26

Original Paper

Mental illness among the homeless: prevalence studyin a Dublin homeless hostelBernice Prinsloo, Catherine Parr, Joanne Fenton

* Correspondence

Page 23: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

According to Dr Joe Fernandez, former Director of the Programme

for the Homeless in St Brendan’s Hospital in Dublin, a distinction

must be drawn between “mental health problems” induced byhomelessness, and “mental illness” which may be a factor inbecoming or remaining homeless; both of which should be

attended to by primary care agents i.e. general practitioners, or by

the mental health services.9 Research suggests that the experience

of being homeless may contribute to anxiety or depressive

illnesses.10 On the other hand, a significant factor in precipitating

homelessness is serious mental illness, and alcohol or drug

addiction. It is also accepted that homelessness may exacerbate a

previous existing mental condition.11

CONSEQUENCES

Homelessness amongst the mentally ill is associated with a range

of other factors, including substance abuse, younger age at first

hospitalisation, and greater severity of symptoms compared to

individuals with mental illness who are not homeless.12 Many

homeless people with a dual diagnosis of mental illness and drug

or alcohol misuse find themselves without a service, since existing

services consider themselves to be either psychiatric services or

addiction services, with poorly defined collaborative functions.13

The homeless mentally ill also appear to encounter a range of

specific, additional problems in relation to housing including, for

example, difficulties accessing community care services following

discharge from hospital.14 A Crisis survey performed in 2002 found

that the homeless people interviewed were almost 40 times more

likely not to be registered with a GP than the average person. Poor

access to primary care services has a direct impact on other health

care services, with an increased likelihood of people attending

A&E.15

Social integration and participation, as well as employment and

meaningful life activities are compromised by the presence of

persistent and untreated psychiatric and physical health problems

for individuals who are homeless. The ability to take steps forward

may be limited due to a combination of poor life skills, ongoing

mental health issues and/or substance use problems, as well as

disruptive life events (lack of stable residence, lack of regular

income, victimisation related to street life).15 Individuals with mental

illness also tend to encounter substantial problems adapting to

housing following lengthy periods of homelessness and many of

these problems go on to complicate their broader re-integration

into society.16

HOMELESSNESS AND MENTAL ILLNESS IN IRELAND

Holohan conducted the first study to examine the health of the

adult homeless population in Dublin in 1997. Sixty six per cent of

people had at least one physical or psychiatric problem. Chronic

physical disease was reported by 41%. The prevalence of

depression in this study was 32.5% and for anxiety disorders

27.6%.17 In 1999 Feeney et al built on this work and examined the

health status and perception of health service access of homeless

hostel-dwelling men in Dublin. In this study 52% of men suffered

from depression, 50% from anxiety and 4% from other mental

health problems. Fifty percent of hostel residents were defined as

alcohol dependent, with 29% having severe alcohol dependence.

More than one third of respondents reported having engaged in

illicit use of at least one drug in the previous year.10

O’Neill et al conducted an audit in 2003 to determine the

proportion of those attending the Mater Misericordiae Hospital’s

psychiatric service that were homeless, including those presenting

to accident and emergency who were homeless. Of all the A&E

referrals to psychiatry, 34.8% were homeless. The homeless

presented most commonly in suicidal crisis (26.6%) compared with

12.5% in the non-homeless group. Substance-abuse disorders

were the primary diagnosis in 42.3% of the homeless group,

accounting for 14.2% in the housed sample.18

Considerable difficulty arises for homeless people in accessing

mental health care due to the sectorisation of services into

catchment areas.13 In Dublin this has resulted in increased pressure

on the two assertive community mental health teams providing

service to the homeless population, based in North Dublin and

South Dublin respectively.

The burden resulting from the gaps in statutory service provision for

homeless people with, or at risk of mental illness, falls on voluntary

homeless service providers and on the wider health and social

services, all of which are already under enormous strain. The

consequences impact severely on the quality of life for homeless

people with mental illness. The Simon Communities of Ireland, a

voluntary homeless agency has said: “[we] are extremely concerned

at the increase … witnessed in the numbers of people who are

homeless who are presenting with mental ill health. The lack of

access to assessment and treatment services by people who are

homeless further exacerbates the problem – leaving individuals very

vulnerable, and homeless services struggling to ensure they meet

service users’ needs.”13

In A Vision for Change several recommendations were made inrelation to mental health services for homeless individuals.19 There

is however still a long way to go towards implementing this policy

in the context of homelessness.

Objective

The objective of the study was to determine the prevalence of

mental illness among the residents of an emergency hostel located

in inner city Dublin. The hostel provides direct access, emergency

and short term accommodation for 74 single homeless men aged

over 26. The hostel is managed in partnership by Depaul Trust and

SVP.20 Previous studies have indicated that homeless hostel-

dwelling men in Dublin constitute the largest single grouping of

homeless Irish people.10

Method

A cross-sectional survey was carried out among hostel residents.

Posters were placed on a public notice board at the hostel inviting

residents to participate in the study; they were asked to inform

their key workers should they wish to participate. All agreeable

residents were then interviewed at the hostel by the authors over

an eight-week period from June to August 2010. All participants

23

Page 24: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

24

were given €10 as an incentive; this was provided on completion

of each interview. This procedure has been used in other studies to

maximise response rates.10

Residents were interviewed using the Structured Clinical Interview

for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-I) Clinical Version. For each

disorder, the 30-day (current) and past prevalence was determined.

The interviewers were psychiatrically trained and familiar with the

DSM-IV diagnoses. Inter-rater reliability was determined between

the interviewers.

Results

A total of 38 residents were interviewed. During the study period

128 men used beds at the hostel; of these 31 were emergency bed

users, simply occupying rooms with no key worker input. Not

taking these 31 men into account, a response rate of 39.2% was

achieved for the study. Characteristics of participating residents are

displayed in Table 1, with SCID-CV Diagnoses summarised in Table 2.

Table 1. Demographic profile of study participants

n (%)Age26-39 12 (31.6)

40-54 18 (47.4)

≥55 8 (21.1)

Mean 45.87 years

Marital statusNever married 22 (57.9)

Married/Cohabiting 4 (10.5)

Separated 6 (15.8)

Divorced 6 (15.8)

ChildrenYes 26 (68.4)

No 12 (31.6)

Educational statusPrimary level 5 (13.2)

Secondary level without graduating 28 (73.7)

Graduated secondary level 2 (5.3)

Higher qualification 3 (7.9)

EmploymentRegular 1 (2.6)

Casual/Volunteer work 2 (5.3)

Unemployed 35 (92.1)

Ethnic originIrish 35 (92.1)

UK 3 (7.9)

Table 2. Prevalence of DSM-IV Axis I Disorders among hostelresidents

*Full criteria have been met during the past month

Mood DisordersBipolar I Disorder, Most Recent Episode Mixed

Bipolar II Disorder, Hypomanic

Major Depressive Disorder

Single Episode

Recurrent, Moderate

Recurrent, In Partial Remission

Recurrent, In Full Remission

Depressive Disorder NOS

Alcohol-Induced Mood Disorder, Depressed

Other Substance-Induced Mood Disorder, Depressed

Schizophrenia and Other Psychotic DisordersSchizophrenia, Paranoid Type

Substance Use DisordersAlcohol Dependence

Alcohol Abuse

Cannabis Dependence

Cocaine Dependence

Opioid Dependence

Sedative, Hypnotic, or Anxiolytic Dependence

Amphetamine Abuse

Cannabis Abuse

Cocaine Abuse

Hallucinogen Abuse

Inhalant Abuse

Opioid Abuse

Phencyclidine Abuse

Sedative, Hypnotic, or Anxiolytic Abuse

Other (or Unknown) Substance Use

Anxiety DisordersPanic Disorder With Agoraphobia

Posttraumatic Stress Disorder

Anxiety Disorder NOS

Other Substance-Induced Anxiety Disorder

With Generalised Anxiety

With Phobic Symptoms

Adjustment DisordersAdjustment Disorder With Anxiety

Current*n (%)

1 (2.6)

1 (2.6)

1 (2.6)

4 (10.5)

1 (2.6)

1 (2.6)

1 (2.6)

3 (7.9)

2 (5.3)

2 (5.3)

9 (23.7)

2 (5.3)

7 (18.4)

1 (2.6)

2 (5.3)

1 (2.6)

3 (7.9)

1 (2.6)

2 (5.3)

2 (5.3)

1 (2.6)

1 (2.6)

1 (2.6)

Pastn (%)

1 (2.6)

1 (2.6)

1 (2.6)

11 (28.9)

3 (7.9)

2 (5.3)

1 (2.6)

1 (2.6)

2 (5.3)

2 (5.3)

4 (10.5)

2 (5.3)

1 (2.6)

3 (7.9)

1 (2.6)

1 (2.6)

2 (5.3)

Page 25: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

A total of 81.6% of residents had a current Axis I diagnosis; this

number increased to 89.5% for lifetime diagnoses (when

combining current and past diagnoses). Only four residents had no

diagnosis. When considering lifetime diagnoses, 31.6% had a

single diagnosis only; 57.9% had two/more diagnoses. Twelve

residents (31.6%) had been admitted to a psychiatric hospital

during their lifetime. Sixteen residents (42.1%) had a lifetime

diagnosis of any depressive disorder, two residents (5.3%) of a

psychotic disorder, 30 residents (78.9%) of any substance use

disorder, and 7 residents (18.4%) of an anxiety/adjustment

disorder.

The most common current and past diagnosis was Alcohol

Dependence, with 52.6% of residents meeting the criteria for

either current or past dependence. The most prevalent current

diagnoses are displayed in Figure 1.

Figure 1. Most prevalent Axis I Disorders during the pastmonth

Only 23.7% of interviewed residents were attending an outpatient

department (either psychiatric or addiction services); in addition

one resident was attending a counsellor and one was attending

AA. The majority were not linked in with any service.

The Specialist Support Worker at the hostel was able to provide

some information on the 59 hostel residents who did not wish to

participate in the study. Of these men, 14 (23.7%) had evidence of

mental illness. Six of these residents reportedly had a diagnosis of

Schizophrenia; five of them were linked in with psychiatric services.

An additional eight residents suffered from Depression (one of

them having a diagnosis of Bipolar Affective Disorder); of these six

were under the care of their GP, one was linked in with psychiatric

services and the resident with Bipolar Affective Disorder had been

refusing intervention in recent years. Another resident had no clear

diagnosis but had previously been admitted to a psychiatric

hospital.

Discussion

The survey demonstrated a very high prevalence of mental

disorders among homeless hostel residents. Comparing prevalence

rates with other cities and countries is difficult though due to

different definitions used for homelessness, differing diagnostic

criteria/interview techniques and different timeframes used (e.g.

lifetime/12-month/30-day prevalence).

The response rate of 39.2% in this study is low compared to other

studies.17,21 A possible explanation for this could be that the

interviews were conducted over a period of eight weeks, as

opposed to some studies where interviews were conducted

together within a few days. It is worth noting that reasons for

nonparticipation in research may also be related to severe mental

illness.21

There was considerable comorbidity between disorders, with a

significant number of residents experiencing both mental illness

and substance use problems. Both alcohol and substance use

disorders were highly prevalent, as has also been found in other

studies. A systematic review of the prevalence of mental disorders

among the homeless found that alcohol dependence and drug

dependence were the most common disorders in this group.21

Combining depressive disorders (including those that are alcohol

and substance-induced) results in a current prevalence of 34.2%.

This is similar to the rate found in a previous study examining the

health of the adult homeless population in Dublin; in that study

32.5% of homeless individuals reported depression.17 The current

prevalence of anxiety disorders was 10.5%, which is much lower

than the rate of 27.6% in the previous study.17 This low rate is

surprising as many men have been exposed to trauma and

violence. A possible explanation for this could be stricter diagnostic

criteria used in this study.

Of note only 5.3% of the sample were identified as having a

psychotic disorder, which is lower than expected compared to

some other studies.1,3,22 The prevalence would increase slightly if

one were to take into account those mentioned residents who did

not want to participate in the study. The systematic review

previously mentioned found that the prevalence rates for psychotic

illness among the homeless in western countries ranged from

2.8% to 42.3%. The review found that lower participation in

surveys was associated with lower prevalence of psychosis.21

The low number of residents attending services is cause for

concern. There could be various reasons for this, including the

differentiation between psychiatric and addiction services,

sectorisation of services into catchment areas, stigma of psychiatric

care, and shelter/hostel staff having limited experience in assessing

for or managing mental illness or addiction. In addition both

physical and psychiatric care rank low in homeless people’s list of

priorities. The demands of securing immediate needs for survival –

food, shelter, money – are more pressing than appointments with

doctors or nurses.23

A relatively small sample of residents were interviewed and the

study only focused on one particular group of homeless men, thus

limiting generalisability. The Specialist Mental Health Worker

actively encouraged those men with mental health problems to

attend for interview, possibly introducing selection bias. The study

was unique though in that, to the best of the authors’ knowledge,

it was the first study in Ireland assessing the complete spectrum of

Axis I Disorders; previous studies focused primarily on depression

and anxiety disorders.10,17 Interviews were conducted by

psychiatrically trained raters using a well validated measure.

25

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!! !"#$%&'''()'''*+,-'''.%&/01&2-'''34",'''5'''6",+%7&%,'''7$%"2#'''-8&'''.0,-'''9+2-8!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!! ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' !!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!! ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' !!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!! ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' !!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!! ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' !!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!! ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' !!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!! ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' !!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!! ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' !!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!! ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' !!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!! ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' !!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!! ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' !!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!! ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' !!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!! ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' !!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!! ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' !!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!!

!!! ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' ''' !!!

!!!

!!!

Page 26: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Conclusion

The study highlights the need to address mental illness in the

homeless population, as it is experienced by so many and may be

contributing to maintaining homelessness through sustained

unemployment and social isolation. The high prevalence of dual

diagnosis highlights the need for greater collaboration between

psychiatric and addiction services. The outcome further points to

the importance of providing mental health training to emergency

shelter/hostel staff. Such training offers the potential to significantly

improve staff’s ability to respond to the needs of residents with

mental illness, and to the behavioural problems some of these

individuals may pose for shelter/hostel operation.24 Research into

the mental health status of the homeless should be undertaken

regularly if services are to be planned to meet the needs of this

vulnerable goup.13

Conflict of interest

None.

Acknowledgement

We would like to thank the following people for their assistance:

• Ms Sharon Harvey, Specialist Support Worker

• Ms Margaret Freeney, Administrative Assistant

• Dr Some Onuoha, Locum Consultant Psychiatrist

References

1. Teesson M, Hodder T, Buhrich N. Psychiatric disorders in homeless men andwomen in inner Sydney. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry 2004; 38: 162-168.

2. Meltzer H. State-of-Science Review: SR-B6 The Mental Ill-Health of HomelessPeople. Foresight Mental Capital and Wellbeing Project. The Government Office for Sciences, 2008: 1-8.

3. Reinking DP, Wolf JR, Kroon H. High prevalence of mental disorders and addiction problems among the homeless in Utrecht. Ned Tijdschr Geneeskd. 2001; 145(24): 1161-6.

4. Vázquez C, Muñoz M, Sanz J. Lifetime and 12-month prevalence of DSM-III-R mental disorders among the homeless in Madrid: a European study using the CIDI. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 1997; 95(6): 523-30.

5. Halldin J, Eklöv L, Lundberg C, Åhs S. Mental health problems among homeless people in Sweden: focus on Stockholm. International Journal of Mental Health 2001; 3: 74-83.

6. McAuley A, McKenna HP. Mental disorders among a homeless population inBelfast: an exploratory survey. Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health Nursing 2008; 2(6): 335-342.

7. Martens WH. A review of physical and mental health in homeless persons. Public Health Rev. 2001; 29(1): 13-33.

8. Holohan T. Health Status, Health Service Utilisation and Barriers to Health Service Utilisation among the Adult Homeless Population of Dublin. Dublin:Eastern Health Board, 1997.

9. Fernandez J. Caring for the Homeless Mentally Ill: Status and Directions. Seminar presentation for the Irish Psychiatric Association at St Patrick’s Hospital, April 2003.

10. Feeney A, McGee HM, Holohan T, Shannon W. Health of Hostel-dwelling Men in Dublin. ERHA & RCSI 2000.

11. Barry S. The Homeless Mentally Ill – discussion document, Apendix A, CluainMhuire Adult Mental Health Service Proposal: The Homeless Mentally Ill. Dublin, 2002.

12. Opler LA, White L, Caton CL et al. Gender differences in the relationship of homelessness to symptom severity, substance abuse, and neuroleptic compliance in schizophrenia. J Nerv Ment Dis 2001; 189: 449-556.

13. Crowley F. Mental Illness: The Neglected Quarter – Homelessness. Amnesty International (Irish Section), Dublin, 2003.

14. Melzer D, Hale AS, Malik SJ et al. Community care for patients with schizophrenia one year after hospital discharge. BMJ 1991; 303: 1023-1026.

15. Critical Condition: Vulnerable single homeless people and access to GPs. Crisis UK, London, 2002.

16. Yanos PT, Barrow SM, Tsemberis S. Community integration in the early phaseof housing among homeless persons diagnosed with severe mental illness. Community Ment Health J 2004; 40: 133-15.

17. Holohan TW. Health and Homelessness in Dublin. Ir Med J 2000; 93: 41-43.18. O’Neill A, Casey P, Minton R. The homeless mentally ill: an audit from an inner

city hospital. Ir J Psychol Med 2007; 24: 62-66.19. Expert Group on Mental Health Policy. A Vision for Change: Report of the

Expert Group on Mental Health Policy. Dublin: The Stationery Office, 2006.20. Homelessness Directory 2007/2008. Suffolk: Homeless Agency and Resource

Information Centre, 2006: 10.21. Fazel S, Khosia V, Doll H, Geddes J. The Prevalence of Mental Disorders among

the Homeless in Western Countries: Systematic Review and Meta-RegressionAnalysis. PLoS Medicine 2008; 5(12): 1670-1681.

22. Shanks NJ, Priest RG, Bedford A, Garbett S. Use of the delusions-symptoms-state inventory to detect psychiatric symptoms in a sample of homeless men.British Journal of General Practice 1995; 45: 201-203.

23. Timms P, Balázs J. ABC of mental health: Mental health on the margins. BritishMedical Journal 1997; 315: 536-539.

24. Vamvakas A, Rowe M. Mental health training in emergency homeless shelters. Community Mental Health J 2001; 37(3): 287-95.

26

Page 27: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Abstract

Objectives: The aim of this study was to determine the views ofboth individuals attending the mental health services (attendees)

and mental health professionals in relation to how attendees and

staff should be addressed, how attendees should be described, and

how staff should be attired.

Methods: We surveyed 132 attendees of the West Galway MentalHealth Services and 97 mental health professionals in relation to

how they prefer to be addressed (first name/ title and surname/ no

preference) the description of attendees (patient / client / service

user / no preference) and the attire of mental health staff (casual /

smart / no preference). We also ascertained how mental health

professionals believed attendees would view these issues.

Results: Attendees preferred to be described as patients ratherthan clients or service users by all mental health professionals, with

46-54% of attendees preferring this term “patient” compared to

14-17% preferring the term “client”, 11-13% preferring the term

“service user” and 20-25% having no preference (p < 0.001).

They preferred to address doctors by their title and surname (61%)

but other mental health professionals by their first names (60-69%)

(p < 0.001). Attendees had a strong preference for being

addressed by their first names by all the mental health professionals

(86-91%) (p < 0.001). Doctors preferred to be attired formally

(88%), compared to nurses (50%) or other mental health

professionals (42%) (p = 0.002). Attendees had no preference in

relation to the attire of doctors but preferred other mental health

professionals to be attired informally.

Conclusions: The study demonstrates that despite the increaseduse of several non-medical terms to describe attendees of mental

health services; the preferred term of attendees of the psychiatric

services in both in-patient and out-patient settings remains

‘patient’. However, this is not universally the case, and the

ascertainment of the preference of the attendee at the first

encounter with the health professional should be ascertained. We

also demonstrated that attendees preferences in relation to both

“dress and address” of doctors is significantly different to their

preference for nurses or other allied mental health professionals;

which may reflect a wish for a less familiar and more formal

interaction with doctors.

Key words:Dress, Address, Mental health professionals, Psychiatricservices

Introduction

A positive therapeutic relationship between mental health

professionals and individuals attending mental health services

(attendees) is a fundamental component of care in psychiatry,1, 2

having important implications for an attendee’s clinical

management and future prognosis.3-5 The interaction between

attendee and mental health professional incorporates many

important aspects, a number of which we analyse in this study.

These include individual preferences as to how attendees are

referred to by mental health staff (patient/ client/ service user); how

attendees are addressed by mental health staff (first name/ title

and surname); how attendees wish to address mental health staff

(first name/ title and surname); and how attendees prefer mental

health staff to be attired.

In recent years, there has been pressure from some stakeholders to

avoid using the term “patient” for attendees with the terms

“client” and “service user” in particular often utilised to describe

individuals who attend mental health services.6-10 The first of these

“non-medical” terms that became utilised was “client”, which

appears to have been adopted from Carl Rogers’ book in 1951,

“Client-Centred Therapy”. Since the 1970s “client” became

increasingly utilised to describe attendees of psychiatric services,11, 13

and appears to have arisen as “patient” had become associated

with negative connotations of sickness, paternalism, autho-

ritarianism, disability, dependence and powerlessness and

considered by some as outdated, patronising and stigmatising to

individuals with mental illness.12, 14 Thus the term “patient” has

been disparaged as the product of a narrow reductionist medical

27

Christina Sim, Registrar in Psychiatry, Departmentof Psychiatry, University College Hospital Galway,Ireland.

*Brian Hallahan,Consultant Psychiatrist, Department of Psychiatry,Roscommon County Hospital, Co. Roscommon,Ireland and Honorary Research Fellow, ClinicalScience Institute, National University of Ireland,Galway, Co Galway, Ireland.E-mail [email protected]

Colm McDonald, Professor of Psychiatry,Department of Psychiatry, Clinical Science Institute,National University of Ireland, Galway, Co. Galway, Ireland.

Submitted 15th October 2010Accepted 15th February 2011

Ir J Psych Med 2012; 29 (1): 27-32

Original Paper

Preferences of dress and address: views of attendees and mentalhealth professionals of the psychiatric services Christina Sim, Brian Hallahan, Colm McDonald

* Correspondence

Page 28: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

model of medical illness, which fails to appreciate the individual

attendee’s mental health problems in the context of their personal

narrative and psychosocial environment. The term “client” was

viewed as de-medicalising the interaction between attendee and

medical professionals. Despite this, a number of recent studies

have found that the majority of individuals attending psychiatric

services (67% to 83%) still prefer to be referred to as “patients”,2,

15-17 although one large multi-site study noted “client” to be the

term of preference.18

In relation to addressing attendees, a number of studies have noted

that most attendees (50-79%) of the psychiatric service prefer to

be addressed by their first name,2, 19, 20 although this issue has

predominantly assessed attendees’ wishes only in relation to how

doctors address them. It has previously been noted that attendee’s

prefer to address doctors (80-95%) by their title and surname,1, 2,

20 but address allied mental health professionals (e.g. nurses, social

workers, occupational therapists) by their first names.1, 2 Little

information is available to date detailing how the seniority of the

treating clinician may impact on how attendees wish to address

their treating doctor.

Conflicting data has been attained to date in relation to the

preference of attendees regarding doctors’ attire. A number of

studies have noted that smart (formal) attire by the treating

clinician is associated with increased trust and perceived

competence by the attendee leading to an increased ability to

discuss their social, sexual and psychological problems,2, 21 however

other studies, albeit in other medical disciplines, have not

corroborated these findings.22-24 Evidence also exists that the

presence of nurse uniforms is associated with perceptions of

professionalism,25 although there is limited evidence in relation to

psychiatric nursing attire and perceptions of professionalism by

attendees or their families.

In this study, we investigated the preference of both attendees and

a range of mental health professionals (consultant doctors, non-

consultant hospital doctors (NCHD), psychiatric nurses,

occupational therapists, social workers and psychologists) in

relation to how they prefer to be addressed (first name/ title and

surname/ no preference), how they prefer to describe attendees

(patient / client / service user / no preference) and how they prefer

mental health staff to be attired (casual / smart / no preference).

The views of attendees of both the acute services (acute in-patient

and day hospital) and sub-acute services (day centres and long stay

residential unit) were ascertained as were mental health

professionals’ beliefs about how attendees view these issues.

Methods

We included 56 individuals attending the acute psychiatric services,

all consecutively admitted individuals in the acute in-patient unit

in University College Hospital, Galway and the psychiatric day

hospital in Galway, between the 17th – 30th May, 2009 and 76

individuals attending the sub-acute services, i.e. 2 day centres and

a rehabilitation unit over the same time period. Each attendee was

approached by a researcher (CS or BH) at the above sites and the

study was explained to them, and consent was attained. They filled

in the questionnaires anonymously and handed them to a staff

nurse. If they had any queries in relation to any questions, they

were able to approach a staff nurse for clarification. At the end of

the study period, a staff nurse at each location posted the

completed anonymous questionnaires to the researcher (CS). For

each attendee, we obtained data in relation to the age, gender

and diagnosis, which were corroborated by their treating clinician.

Data was analysed blind to attendee’s personal details. Attendees

during the above time period completed a one page questionnaire

detailing:

1) How they preferred to be described by mental health

professionals treating them (patient / client / service user / no

preference).

2) How they preferred mental health professionals to address them

(first name/ title and surname/ no preference).

3) What they preferred to call mental health professionals (first

name/ title and surname/ no preference).

4) How they believed mental health professionals should be attired

(casual/ smart/ no preference).

The response rate of the attendees was 97%, with all day hospital

attendees, and all individuals attending the sub-acute services

completing the questionnaires, however four attendees of the

acute in-patient unit refused to fill in the questionnaire.

A number of meetings were held with mental health professionals

where the study was explained. Mental health professionals were

informed that they could contact one of the researchers (CS or BH)

for further clarification in relation to any of the questions. All

questionnaires were filled in anonymously and were posted to the

researcher (BH). Mental health professionals, including 33 doctors,

52 nurses, four social workers, four psychologists, two

occupational therapists, two other mental health team members

working in these centres (78 acute, and 19 sub-acute) completed

the one page questionnaire detailing:

1) How they wished to describe attendees of the mental health

services (patient/ client/ service user/ no preference).

2) What they believed attendees preferred to be described as

(patient/ client/ service user/ no preference).

3) How they preferred to be addressed by attendees (first name/

title and surname/ no preference).

4) What they believed attendees prefer to address mental health

professionals by (first name/ title and surname/ no preference).

5) How they liked to address attendees (first name/ title and

surname/ no preference).

6) What they believed attendees prefer to be addressed as (first

name/ title and surname/ no preference).

7) How they believed they should be attired (smart/ casual/ no

preference).

8) Have they believed attendees prefer them to be attired (smart

(formal) / casual (smart informal)/ no preference). These terms

were explained explicitly to staff and attendees.

28

Page 29: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Information on their profession, gender and age was also attained.

All mental health professionals in the region were invited to

complete the questionnaire; however three doctors and 20 nurses

did not complete the questionnaire. All allied mental health

professionals completed the questionnaire.

We used SPSS 15.0 for Windows (SPSS 15.0 for Windows, SPSS

Inc, Chicago, Illinois, USA) to analyse all data. We utilised the chi

squared test to compare all group data in relation to the dress and

address of attendees and mental health professionals.

Ethical approval was attained prior to the commencement of this

study from the Research Ethics Committee of Galway University

Hospital.

Results

One hundred and thirty-two attendees of the mental health services

and 97 mental health professionals participated in this study and

their demographic and clinical data are described in Table 1.

Attendees had a preference for all mental health professionals to

refer to them as “patients” rather than “clients” or “service users”,

with 46-54% of attendees preferring this term “patient”

compared to 14-17% preferring the term “client”, 11-13%

preferring the term “service user” and 20-25% having no

preference (p < 0.001) (Table 2). Doctors, but not other mental

health professionals, preferred to use the term “patient” (p <

0.00001) (Table 3). Doctors, but not other mental health

professionals believed that attendees would prefer this term,

however doctors over-estimated this preference (p < 0.00001) and

nurses under-estimated this preference (p < 0.0001).

Attendees of the mental health services had a strong preference for

being addressed by their first name by all mental health

professionals (86-91%) (Table 2). Nurses and allied mental health

professionals preferred to address attendee’s by their first name

(94%) more frequently than doctors (49%), although this was still

the most common preference of doctors (p < 0.0001) (Table 3).

Doctors preferred to be addressed by their title and surname

(61%), unlike nurses (98%) or other mental health professionals

(100%) (p < 0.00001) (Table 3) and attendees preferred to address

doctors by their title and surname (67%), whereas they preferred

to address nurses and allied mental health professionals by their

first name (60-69%) (p < 0.001) (Table 2). Attendees did not

differentiate between consultants and NCHDs in how they wished

to address them.

Attendees had no preference in relation to the attire of doctors,

however they expressed a preference for both nurses and allied

mental health professionals to be attired in casual clothes (p < 0.05)

(Table 2). Doctors (88%), but not nurses (50%) or allied mental

health professionals (42%) preferred to be attired formally (p <

0.0001) (Table 3). Doctors (p < 0.00001) and nurses (p < 0.01) over-

estimated attendee’s wishes in relation to them being attired formally.

No significant differences were noted in our findings in relation to

gender (attendee / professional), age (attendee / professional),

location (acute / sub-acute setting) or attendee diagnosis.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this is the first comprehensive study examining

the preference of “dress and address” of both attendees and a

variety of mental health professionals in the Irish psychiatric service.

In this study, we found that the term “patient” remains the

predominant preference of attendees of mental health services,

including those attending acute and sub-acute services. This

corroborates some previous research in other countries,2, 15-17, 26 and

suggests that despite the burgeoning use of a variety of other

terms in recent years,7-10 attendees themselves continue to prefer

the term “patient”. Previously, a UK study noted that attendees of

mental health services preferred mental health professionals other

than doctors to describe them as “clients” rather than “patients”,2

however we noted no such disparity in attendees views in relation

to mental health professionals, and this may reflect a cultural

difference in the populations studied. Nevertheless, nurses and

allied mental health professionals preferred to use terms such as

“client” and “service user” more than doctors, which has been

interpreted as indicating a less hierarchical form of interaction and

more collaborative type of relationship with attendees.2 Attendees

of mental health services span a range of disorders from disabling

severe enduring mental illnesses to milder forms of illness where

non-medical therapies predominate. The term “client” implies that

an attendee is a customer and voluntarily seeks services and is

aware of their difficulties or illness.27 Our sample of attendees had

a relatively severe level of mental illness with only 10% of

individuals not having a significant psychosis or mood disorder, and

thus one may postulate that this group with significant psycho-

pathology may be more likely to consider themselves as suffering

from a medical disorder and prefer to be regarded as “patients”

rather than “clients” or “service users”. Previous research however

has not noted an effect of diagnosis and preference of address.18, 26

Our finding that attendees prefer to address doctors by their title

and surname corroborates previous findings,2, 20, 22 and suggests

that attendees and doctors have a preference for a more hierarchal

form of interaction, in contrast to the more collaborative

relationship with nurses or allied mental health professionals.

Whilst several previous studies have found that attendees prefer

doctors to be dressed formally (smartly),19-22 we found that

attendees were equally split in their views on doctor’s attire.

Previous research in a mental health service in the UK, noted that

attendees of mental health services preferred doctors to be

formally attired although they viewed this as “less friendly”.19

Whilst it has previously been suggested that individuals when more

acutely psychiatrically unwell prefer to emphasize boundaries

between themselves and doctors including preferring doctors to

be attired more formally,19 our findings did not corroborate this.

The preference of attendees for other mental health professionals

to be less formally attired again suggests a “more familiar” type of

relationship compared to the relationship with their treating doctor.

We noted a discrepancy between attendees preferences and

doctors perception of attendees preferences. Doctors over-

estimated attendees’ preference for the term “patient”, and for

doctors to be formally attired and under-estimated attendees’

preferences for being addressed by their first name. Therefore, we

29

Page 30: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

believe that doctors and other mental health professionals can be

incorrect in relation to the preferences of attendees of the mental

health services and should ascertain their preference in relation to

how attendees wish to be addressed through direct enquiry during

the first encounter.

Our main limitation in this study was the limited data we were able

to attain from allied mental health professionals (e.g. occupational

therapists, social workers and psychologists) and thus data attained

from these groups must be interpreted with caution. Whilst we

attained data from both acute and sub-acute attendees from both

an urban and rural setting, our findings may not be generalisable

to other jurisdictions or those with less severe mental illness such as

neurotic disorders. We also only utilised descriptive terms for the

attire of mental health professionals, and despite the researchers

explaining the three options (casual, smart or no preference), these

terms are still quite subjective and the use of photographs to further

explain these terms may have been useful for individuals in

answering this question.

Conclusion

The study demonstrates that despite the increased use of several

terms to describe attendees of mental health services, the term

‘patient’ remains the preferred option of attendees of the

psychiatric services in both in-patient and out-patient settings, with

50-55% of attendees stating a clear preference for this term

compared to 14-17% preferring the term “client”, and 11-13%

preferring the term “service user”. Attendees of the mental health

services have different preferences in relation to how they should be

addressed by mental health professionals; and we believe that it is

the responsibility of the care provider to ascertain these preferences

by direct enquiry during the first encounter. Despite 88% of doctors

stating a preference for formal attire, only 30% of attendees stated

that they preferred their treating doctor to be addressed formally.

We also demonstrated that attendees preferences in relation to

both “dress and address” of doctors is significantly different to their

preference for nurses or other allied mental health professionals;

which may reflect a wish for a less familiar and more formal

interaction with doctors.

Conflict of interest

None.

30

Table 1: Demographic and Clinical Data

Attendees Mental HealthProfessionals

n (%) n (%)

Site of attendee’ treatment or mental health professional predominant workplaceAcute Services (Acute Unit / Day Hospital) 56 (42.4) 78 (80.4)Acute Psychiatric Unit 28 (21.2) 65 (67.0)Day Hospital 28 (21.2) 13 (13.4)

Sub-acute Services (Day Centre / Rehabilitation Unit) 76 (57.6) 19 (19.6) Day Centre 58 (44.0) 12 (12.4) Rehabilitation Unit 18 (13.6) 7 (7.2)

GenderMale 64 (48.5) 31 (32.0)Female 68 (51.5) 66 (68.0)

Age < 40 42 (31.8) 66 (68.0)≥ 40 90 (68.2) 31 (32.0)

Principal Diagnosis (ICD-10 classification)Organic (F00-09) 4 (3)Psychoactive substance misuse (F10-19) 10 (7.6) Schizophrenia spectrum (F20-29) 64 (48.5)Mood (Affective) Disorders (F30-39) 39 (29.5)Neurotic / Stress Related Disorders (F40-49) 3 (2.3)Personality Disorders (F60-69) 9 (6.9)Mild Intellectual Disability (F70) 1 (0.8)No formal psychiatric disorder 2 (1.5)

Mental Health Professionals OccupationDoctor 33 (34.0)Nurse 52 (53.6)Psychologist 4 (4.1)Occupational Therapist 2 (2.1)Social Worker 4 (4.1)Other team members 2 (2.1)

Page 31: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

31

Consultant NCHD Nurse Social Worker Occupational PsychologistTherapist

n (%) n (%) n (%) n (%) n (%) n (%)

Preferred description by health professionalPatient 68 (51.5) 71 (53.8) 66 (50.0) 60 (45.5) 66 (50.0) 66 (50.0)Client 22 (16.7) 20 (15.2) 18 (13.6) 22 (16.7) 20 (15.2) 20 (15.2)Service User 15 (11.4) 15 (11.4) 17 (12.9) 17 (12.9) 16 (12.1) 16 (12.1)No Preference 27 (20.5) 26 (19.7) 31 (23.5) 33 (25.0) 30 (22.7) 30 (22.7)

Preferred address by health professionalFirst Name 115 (87.1) 113 (85.6) 120 (90.9) 116 (88.6) 117 (88.6) 117 (88.6)Title and Surname 11 (8.3) 9 (6.8) 6 (4.5) 9 (6.8) 9 (6.8) 10 (7.6)No Preference 6 (4.5) 10 (7.6) 6 (4.5) 7 (5.3) 6 (4.5) 5 (3.8)

Addressing health professionals, attendee preference*First Name 32 (24.2) 32 (24.2) 91 (68.9) 86 (65.2) 89 (67.4) 79(59.8)Title and Surname 89 (67.4) 88 (66.7) 31 (23.5) 33 (25.0) 30 (22.7) 41 (31.1)No Preference 11 (8.3) 12 (9.1) 10 (7.6) 13 (9.8) 13 (9.8) 12 (9.1)

Preferred attire of health professionalCasual (Informal) 46 (34.8) 45 (34.1) 55 (41.7) 59 (44.7) 58 (43.9) 55 (41.7)Smart (Formal) 39 (29.5) 40 (30.3) 31 (23.5) 25 (18.9) 27 (20.5) 30 (22.7)No Preference 47 (35.6) 47 (35.6) 46 (34.8) 48 (36.4) 47 (35.6) 47 (35.6)

* A significant difference was detected in relation to attendees preferences between the groups, with attendees preferring to address doctors by their title and surname and to address other mental health professionals by their first name (x2 = 137.66, df = 10, p = 1.3x10-24)

Table 2: Preferences of Attendees in relation to the dress and address of mental health professionals

Doctor Nurse Allied Mental x2 df PHealth Professionals

n (%) n (%) n (%)Preferred description of attendee 29.484 6 0.00005Patient 30 (90.9) 19 (36.5) 4 (33.3)Client 0 (0) 19 (36.5) 3 (25.0)Service User 1 (3.0) 7 (13.5) 3 (25.0)No Preference 2 (6.1) 7 (13.5) 2 (16.7)

Perceived preferred description of attendee 24.512 6 0.0004Patient 24 (72.7) 27 (51.9) 4 (33.3)Client 1 (3.0) 11 (21.2) 1 (8.3)Service User 3 (9.1) 6 (11.5) 7 (58.3)No Preference 5 (15.2) 8 (15.4) 0 (0)

Preferred address of health professional 72,447 4 6.9X10-15

First Name 5 (15.2) 51 (98.1) 12 (100)Title and Surname 20 (60.6) 0 (0) 0 (0)No Preference 8 (24.2) 1 (1.9) 0 (0)

Perceived preferred address of health professional 58.735 4 5.3x10-12

First Name 5 (15.2) 47 (90.4) 12 (100)Title and Surname 23 (69.7) 3 (5.8) 0 (0)No Preference 5 (15.2) 2 (3.8) 0 (0)

Preferred address of attendee 26.858 4 0.00002First Name 16 (48.5) 49 (94.2) 11 (91.7)Title and Surname 10 (30.3) 1 (1.9) 1 (8.3)No Preference 7 (21.2) 2 (3.8) 0 (0)

Perceived preferred address of attendee 11.250 4 0.024First Name 22 (66.7) 47 (90.4) 12 (100)Title and Surname 6 (18.2) 2 (3.8) 0 (0)No Preference 5 (15.2) 3 (5.8) 0 (0)

Preferred attire of health professional 16.528 4 0.002Casual (Informal) 4 (12.1) 25 (48.1) 6 (50.0)Smart (Formal) 29 (87.9) 26 (50) 5 (41.7)No Preference 0 (0) 1 (1.9) 1 (8.3)

Perceived preferred attire of health professional 14.643 4 0.006Casual (Informal) 1 (3.0) 20 (38.5) 3 (25.0)Smart (Formal) 26 (78.8) 26 (50.0) 6 (50.0)No Preference 6 (18.2) 6 (11.5) 3 (25.0)

Table 3: Preferences of mental health professionals, and their perceptions of attendees’ preferences

Page 32: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

References

1. McGuire R, McCabe R, Priebe S. Theoretical frameworks for investigating and

understanding the therapeutic relationship in psychiatry. Soc Psych and

Psychiatric Epidem 2001; 36: 557-64.

2. McGuire-Snieckus, R., McCabe R, Priebe S. Patient, client or service user? A

survey of patient preferences of dress and address of six mental health

professions. Psych Bull 2003; 27: 305-8.

3. Day JC, Bentall RP, Roberts C, et al. Attitudes toward antipsychotic

medication: The impact of clinical variables and relationships with health

professionals. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005; 62: 717-24.

4. Fakhoury WK, White I, Priebe S. Be good to your patient: how the therapeutic

relationship in the treatment of patients admitted to assertive outreach affects

re-hospitalization. J Nerv Ment Dis 2007; 195: 789-91.

5. Gaudiano BA, Miller IW. Patients’ expectancies, the alliance in

pharmacotherapy, and treatment outcomes in bipolar disorder. J Consult Clin

Psychol 2006, 74: 671-76.

6. Swift G, Zachariah M, Casey PR. A rose by any other name: psychiatric

outpatients’ views on dress and address. Ir J Psychol Med, 2000; 17: 132-34.

7. Nursing and Midwifery Council. The Code: Standards of conduct,

Performance and Ethics for Nurses and Midwives, NMC 2008.

(http://ww.nmc-uk.org/aDisplay.Document.aspx?documentID=5982).

8. British Association of Social Workers. The Code of Ethics for Social Work.

BASW, 2002. (http://www.basw.co.uk/Portals/O/CODE%200F%20ETHICS.pdf).

9. British Psychological Society. Code of Ethics and Conduct. BPS, 2006.

(http://www.bps.org.uk/downloadfile.cfm?File_uuid=5084A882-

1143-DFDO-7E6C-F1983A65C242&ext=pdf).

10. College of Occupational Therapists. Code of Ethics and Professional Conduct

(Revised). College of Occupational Therapists, 2005.

(http://www.cot.org.uk/MainWebsite/Resources/Documents/Code-of-Ethics.pdf).

11. Davis C, Davis J. “Nurse”: dynamic term for a dynamic relationship. Nurse

Manager 1992; 23: 42-3.

12. Herzberg SR. Client or patient: which term is more approprioate for use in

occupational therapy? Am J Occup Ther 1990 44: 561-4.

13. Keaney F, Strang J, Raga-JM, Spektor D, Manning V, Kelleher M, et al. Does

anyone care about names? How attendees at substance misuse services like

to be addressed by health professionals. Eur Addict Res 2004; 10: 75-9.

14. Neuberger J. Do we need a new word for patients? Let’s do away with

“patients”. BMJ 1999; 318: 1756-8.

15. Ritchie CW, Hayes D, Ames DJ. Patient or client? The opinions of people

attending a psychiatric clinic. Psych Bull 2000; 24: 447-50.

16. Upton MWM, Boer GH, Neale AJ. Patients or clients? - a hospital survey. Psych

Bull 1994; 18, 142-3.

17. Simmons P, Hawley CJ, Gale TM, Sivakumaran T. Service user, patient, client,

user or survivor: describing recipients of mental health services. The

Psychiatrist 2010; 34: 20-3.

18. Covell NH, McCorkle BH, Weissman EM, Summerfelt T, Essock SM. What’s in

a name? Preferred by service recipients. Adm Policy Ment Health 2007; 34:

443-7.

19. Gledhill JA, Warner JP, King M. Psychiatrists and their patients: views on forms

of dress and address. BJPsych 1997; 171: 228-32.

20. Gallagher J, Waldron Lynch F, Stack J, Barragry J. Dress and address: Patient

preferences regarding doctor’s style of dress and patient interaction. IMJ

2008; 101: 211-3.

21. Rehman SU, Nietert PJ, Cope DW, Kilpatrick AO. What to wear today? Effect

of doctor’s attire on the trust and confidence of patients. Am J Med 2005;

118: 1279-86.

22. Lill MM, Wilkinson TJ. Judging a book by its cover: descriptivesurvey of

patients’ preferences from doctors’ appearance and mode of address. BMJ

2005; 331:1524-7.

23. Hennessy N, Harrison DA, Aitkenhead AR. The effect of the anaesthetist’s

attire on patient attitudes. The influence of dress on patient perception of

the anaesthetist’s prestige. Anaesthesia 1993; 48: 219-2.

24. Baevsky RH, Fisher AL, Smithline HA, Salzberg MR. The influence of physician

attire on patient satisfaction. Acad Emerg Med 1998; 5: 82-4.

25. Albert NM, Wocial L, Meyer KH, et al. Impact of nurses’ uniforms on

patient and family perceptions of nurse professionalism. Appl Nurs Res 2008;

21: 181-90.

26. Sharma V, Whitney D, Kazarian SS, Manchanda R. Preferred terms for users

of mental health services among service provides and recipients. Psychiatric

Services 2000; 51: 203-9.

27. Torrey EF. Patients, clients, consumers, survivors’ et al: What is in a name?

Schiz Bull 2010, in press.

32

Page 33: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Abstract

Drawings can be used as an important tool to measure children’s

perception and emotions. Using a qualitative design, we asked a

group of 24 school children (10 boys and 14 girls) aged 11-12 to

draw their impressions of psychiatrists. In the majority of drawings,

psychiatrists were portrayed as a friendly or kind figure. The art

work was analysed by the coordinator of the Arts Initiative in

Mental Health, Niamh O’Connor. Psychiatrists were portrayed

positively by this group of young people. This reflects a strong

influence of the media on children’s perception.

Introduction

Public perception of psychiatry is influenced by several factors such

as personal experience, the media and the experience of family

members and friends.1 A positive attitude towards the profession

will help reduce the stigma of mental illness. Several studies have

examined the public attitude towards mental illness,2, 3 patients

with mental health problems,2, 4 and psychiatric treatments.5-7 Less

is known about how psychiatrists are perceived by the general

public. A telephone survey in New Zealand found that psychiatrists

were perceived negatively compared with dentists, family doctors

and lawyers. Forty eight percent of the study population did not

know that psychiatrists are medical doctors, and 60% did not

know the difference between psychiatrists and psychologists.8 In

the UK , 412 adults were given a list of 26 professions and were

asked to rank according to which they most respected, psychiatrists

came 8th above jobs such as bank managers, accountants and

MPs. However, psychiatrists had the lowest ranking compared to

other medical professions such as GPs, pathologists and nurses.9

Drawings could provide useful information about children’s feelings

and emotions. In health settings, children’s drawings have been

used for various purposes including as diagnostic aides for

headaches,10, 11 as an index of self-esteem and anxiety in children

undergoing plastic surgery,12 and to describe the quality of care

children receive in hospitals.13

Few studies have addressed the question of how doctors are

perceived in children’s drawings. Marshall studied drawings of 40

children aged six to nine years in New Zealand and found that 92%

of doctors were depicted as happy or normal and only one child

drew a doctor with an ‘unfavourable facial attitude’.14 Another

study of 50 school children aged seven to eleven years in the UK

found that in 50% of the drawings the doctor was portrayed as a

benign or a kindly figure, in 44% as gruesome or threatening, and

in six percent as neutral.15 In the present study we set out to explore

children’s perception of psychiatrists by inviting a group of primary

school children to draw what they thought a psychiatrist looks like.

MethodologyAs part of the intergenerational art project, six school children aged

11-12 years, from Carraroe Primary School in Sligo, attended

Liscarney house, a day hospital for the old age psychiatry service in

Sligo, once a week for a period of seven weeks. The children

formed pairs with patients and participated in artwork with the

emphasis being on exchanging ideas and skills to promote

understanding. At the end of the period we asked the school

principal if the whole class, including those who did not participate

in the project, could visually represent an image of a psychiatrist.

This was a qualitative study combined with quantitative methods;

The art work was analysed by the coordinator of the Arts Initiative

in Mental Health (Niamh O’Connor). When studying the children's

drawings a comparative analysis of the themes and approaches of

the creators' was determined.

The primary concern was to ascertain the relative friendliness of

each psychiatrist and to then categorise and quantify all the other

information. Drawings were categorised under the following

headings - friendliness, sex, dress, setting, doctor with patient,

doctor's couch, other paraphernalia etc.

A formal analysis of the images - the creator's use of line, shape,

colour, texture, and composition was considered; particularly in

relation to any unusual characteristics and to the categories listed

above. Given the small size of our sample it was not possible to

conduct any meaningful statistical analysis.

33

*Sami Omer,Consultant Psychiatrist,Department of Old Age Psychiatry, Sligo, Ireland.E-mail [email protected]

Niamh O’Connor, Coordinator of the Arts Initiative in Mental Health,Sligo/Leitrim Mental Health Services, Clarion Road,Sligo, Ireland.

Gavin Sweeney,Clinical Nurse Manager,Department of Old Age Psychiatry,Liscarney House, Pearse Road, Sligo, Ireland.

Geraldine McCarthy,Department of Old Age Psychiatry,Liscarney House, Pearse Road, Sligo, Ireland.

Submitted August 11th 2009Accepted February 13th 2012

Ir J Psych Med 2012; 29 (1): 33-35

Original Paper

Psychiatrists in their eyes: Children’s drawings ofwhat a psychiatrist looks likeSami Omer, Niamh O’Connor, Gavin Sweeney, Geraldine McCarthy

* Correspondence

Page 34: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

34

ResultsTwenty four school children (10 boys and 14 girls) took part in the

study. One child submitted two drawings making the total number

of drawings 25.

Their artwork was made on A4 photocopier paper using coloring

pencils and was an immediate response to the question posed,

with no prior sketching or preparatory work. The main features of

the drawings are summarized in Table 1.

Feature Frequency of representation Number (%)

Female psychiatrist 18 (72)

Friendly psychiatrist 21 (84)

Psychiatrist with patient 12 (48)

Doctor’s Couch 14 (56)

Psychiatrist offers help 5 (20)

No context and/or sparse context 14 (56)

Psychiatrist with paraphernalia(Pen, file, book, bag/briefcase, stethoscope) 12 (48)

Table 1: Main features of the drawings

There was no difference in the quality of the drawings between

the six children who attended the day hospital and the 18 children

who did not attend.

GenderMost children depicted the psychiatrist as female 18 (72%), while

in seven (28%) drawings the psychiatrist was portrayed as male.

Only one girl depicted the psychiatrist as male while four boys drew

a female doctor.

Facial expressionThe majority of the drawings showed the psychiatrist as friendly,

with 16 (64%) of them smiling broadly. Five (20%) drawings

showed a less openly smiling face but a kind expression

nonetheless, while three (12%) showed the psychiatrist’s facial

expression as worried, shocked and unhappy. Of the 12 (48%)

drawings, which showed the person receiving treatment, six out

of 12 (50%) were smiling, three out of 12 (25%) were unhappy

and a further three (25%) were not apparent or hidden.

Clothing and accessoriesIn the majority of pictures the psychiatrist was dressed in normal

attire, while three children (12%) had them wearing a type of

uniform. Many of the doctors wore colorful clothing of stripes and

florals with careful detailing of buttons, collars and hair accessories.

The female psychiatrists wore stylish colour-coordinated outfits

with an emphasis on fashion, while the male doctors were dressed

more soberly. A small number showed the doctor with a handbag,

one(4%) carrying a book and seven (28%) wore spectacles,

perhaps denoting studiousness and maturity. One drawing

depicted the psychiatrist as a very mature, perhaps elderly woman.

Two (8%) psychiatrists wore a doctor’s headband. One child (4%)

depicted a female psychiatrist in an apparently religious scheme,

wearing robes similar in colour to the Virgin Mary, hovering above

the ground with an arc of stars encircling the woman.

The contextWhile many children took great care to detail the surrounding

environment a full 56% did not represent any context or included

only one piece of furniture with no floor, walls, ceiling or other

reference to place.

In seven (28%) drawings the psychiatrist was portrayed alone,

without any other reference to context. A further 17 (68%)

showed interior furniture of one kind or another (desks, chairs,

couches) with one drawing locating the psychiatrist in a garden.

In one drawing (4%) the psychiatrist was shown in a home

environment with a row of shoes, a kitchen and a rug etc. The

doctor’s couch was an overwhelming feature, apparent in 14

(56%) of the drawings. While the drawings were colourful, almost

all showed sparse and anonymous surroundings with limited or no

decorative features. The exception is two (8%) pictures where

much decorative detail was evident (cupboards, rugs, a fish tank,

a plant) both drawn by girls.

Doctor-patient relationshipOf the 12 drawings, which also incorporate the person receiving

treatment, 11 out of 12 (92%) showed an engagement between

doctor and patient. The doctor either stands nearby, sits facing the

patient or is speaking with the patient. Only one drawing showed

little relationship between the two subjects-the psychiatrist

disproportionately bigger than and facing away from the patient.

eleven of the drawings depicted the patient on a couch, with the

doctor standing nearby in seven of the pictures. Four images

showed the doctor sitting beside the patient who is always, with

the exception of one drawing, lying down.

Interestingly, a number of children added text to the drawing in

the form of speech, giving us a more layered understanding of the

relationship between doctor and patient. Questions like “how are

you feeling?”, “tell me your thoughts?” and statements like “of

course (I’ll help)”, “it’s ok I’ll help” and “I will help you” are

evident. A psychiatrist possibly using hypnosis to relax the patient

- “stare into this as you fall asleep” is shown in Figure 1. Discussion

about medication did not feature in any of the drawings.

Figure 1. Drawing by Sophie

Page 35: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Figure 2. Drawing by Laura.

In Figure 2 there is an ominous black cloud or speech bubble over

the patient’s head.

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge this is the first study that describes

children’s perception of psychiatrists. In the majority of drawings

the psychiatrist was portrayed as a friendly or kind figure. Television

had inevitably played a significant role in forming children’s

opinions about ‘What a psychiatrist looks like’. The doctor’s couch,

a familiar scene from the private clinics of hundreds of television

and film ‘shrinks’, was a recurring feature. The role of television in

influencing children’s perception and imagination is well-

recognised in the literature.16-18 Furthermore, it had been suggested

that while children can make the distinction between reality and

fictional TV when they have the real-world information about an

occupation, for unfamiliar occupations children may perceive what

they see on TV as real ‘perceived reality’.18

In Ireland a limited number of studies examined how psychiatry is

portrayed in the media. An audit of print media found that mental

health issues were generally portrayed in either a neutral or positive

way.19 A study of a small number of Irish films found that

psychiatrists were portrayed as humane, caring and occasionally

conflicted individuals.20

The colorful and fashionable attire may be attributed to the

children’s personal preferences and practiced modes of visual

representation rather than their knowledge of the dress sense of

psychiatrists.

The psychiatrist in most drawings was depicted as being female.

This is probably a mere reflection of the gender difference in our

study sample.

Bearing in mind that the task was to make a drawing to illustrate

‘What I think a psychiatrist looks like’ and not ‘What I think a

psychiatrist does’, significantly, almost 50% of the children, of their

own initiative, determined to expand upon this, to show the

psychiatrist in a supportive relationship with patients.

The six children who attended the day hospital had mainly worked

with patients. They were only briefly introduced to a psychiatrist

at the end of their art project. This would probably explain the lack

of difference in their impressions of psychiatrists compared to those

who did not attend the day hospital.

ConclusionPsychiatrists were portrayed positively by this group of young

people. They were all primary school children and although six had

attended a mental health setting over a seven week period, the

majority projected a positive/friendly image of psychiatrists in their

artwork. This reflects a strong influence of the media on children’s

perception. It also suggests that health promotion helping to

diminish or avoid stigma about mental health should be addressed

at an early stage. Further work looking at perceptions of teenagers

could help to elucidate this further.

Conflict of interest

None.

Acknowledgement

We wish to thank Dr. Maria Morgan, Dr. Caroline Ang, Dr. Kate

Johnson and Professor John Waddington from the Department of

Molecular and Cellular Therapeutics, Royal College of Surgeons in

Ireland. As part of an outreach programme, they asked a group of

school children to draw their impressions of scientists before and

after they took part in an interactive hands-on science workshop.

Their work inspired us to undertake this project. The authors are

immensely grateful to the principal of Carraroe National School

and to the children who took part in the study.

References

1. Philo G (ed.). Media and mental distress. London; Longman, 1996.

2. Wolff G, Pathare S, Craig T, Leff J. Community knowledge of mental illness

and reaction to mentally ill people. Br J Psychiatry 1996; 168(2):191-8.

3. Crisp AH, Gelder MG, Rix S, Meltzer HI, Rowlands OJ. Stigmatisation of

people with mental illness. Br J Psychiatry. 2000; 177:4-7.

4. Mehta N, Kassam A, Leese M, Butler G, Thornicroft G. Public attitudes

towards people with mental illness in England and Scotland 1994-2003. Br

J Psychiatry 2009; 194(3):278-84.

5. Angermeyer MC, Breier P, Dietrich S, Kenzine D, Matschinger H. Public

attitudes toward psychiatric treatment. An international comparison. Soc

Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2005; 40(11): 855-64.

6. Burke S, Kerr R, McKeon P. Male secondary school students’ attitudes towards

using mental health services. Ir J Psychol Med 2008; 25(2):52-56.

7. Mojtabai R. Americans’ attitudes toward mental health treatment seeking:

1990-2003. Psychiatr Serv. 2007; 58(5):642-651.

8. Sellman D, Pearson G. The public perception of psychiatrists: a telephone

survey. N Z Med J. 1985; 98(785):699-703.

9. Luty J, Fekadu D, Gallagher J. Umoh O. The professional status of

psychiatrists: good but not great. Psychiatric Bulletin (2006) 30: 314.

10. Wojaczyńska-Stanek K, Koprowski R, Wróbel Z, Gola M. Headache in children’s drawings. J Child Neurol. 2008; 23(2):184-91.

11. Stafstrom CE, Rostasy K, Minster A. The Usefulness of children’s drawings in

the diagnosis of headache. Pediatrics 2002; 109(3):460-472.

12. Lukash F. Children’s art as a helpful index of anxiety and self-esteem with

plastic surgery. Plast Reconstr Surg. 2002; 109(6):1777-86.

13. Pelander T, Lehtonen K, Leino-Kilpi H. Children in the Hospital: Elements of

quality in drawings. J Pediatr Nurs. 2007; 22(4):333-41.

14. Marshall CS. Looking at the doctor through the eyes of a child. N Z Fam Phys.

1985; 12:17-19.

15. Philipp R, Philipp E, Pendered L, Barnard C, Hall M. Can Children’s paintings

of their doctors be interpreted? J R Coll Gen Pract. 1986; 36(288):325-7.

16. Gash H, Domínguez Rodríguez P. Young people’s heroes in France and Spain.

Span J Psychol. 200; 12(1):246-57.

17. Comer JS, Furr JM, Beidas RS, Babyar HM, Kendall PC. Media use and

children’s perception of societal threat and personal vulnerability. J Clin Child

Adolesc Psychol. 2008; 37(3):622-30.

18. Pecora N, Murray JP, Wartella EA (eds.). Children and television. Fifty years of

research. Laurence Erlbaum Associates, Inc. New Jersey, 2007.

19. O’Connor A, Casey P. What it says in the papers: an audit. Ir J Psychol Med.

2001; 18:68-71.

20. Kelly B. Psychiatry in contemporary Irish cinema. Ir J Psychol

Med. 2006; 23(2): 74-79.

35

Page 36: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Abstract

Objective: This study was carried out to determine the prevalenceof selective mutism (SM) in an urban school population and to

assess comorbidity and family characteristics.

Method: Teachers of primary schools, teaching children betweenthe ages of four and 12, were sent a description of SM and asked

to complete the selective mutism questionnaire (SMQ) if they

believed a child in their class met criteria. Thirty-nine schools were

sampled, covering a catchment area of 10,927 children. Children

who screened positive on the SMQ were offered a full psychiatric

assessment. Parent, child and clinicians completed various rating

scales.

Results: A response rate of 100% from schools was obtained. Theprevalence rate of SM was 0.18% (20/10927). fourteen (70%)

attended for further evaluation. All children scored within the

clinical range on the Clinical Global Assessment Scale (CGAS),

indicating moderate to severe impairment. fifty percent (7)

reported a family history of social anxiety disorder, and 43% (6)

autistic spectrum disorders.

Conclusion: This is the first Irish based prevalence study of SM.Results indicate that SM is not as rare as previously believed.

Children with SM were found to have significant functional

impairment along with a strong family history of anxiety and

autism.

Key words: Comorbidity, Family psychopathology, Prevalence,Selective mutism.

Introduction

Selective mutism is considered a rare disorder of communication,

characterised by an emotionally determined consistent failure to

speak in select social situations, where speaking is expected, for

example school, despite demonstrating language competence in

more familiar situations, for example, home. The disorder is

persistent, lasting longer than one month, and causes impairment

in communication, educational and occupational achievement.1

Reported prevalence rates for selective mutism range between

0.033-2% of primary school children.2,3,4,5 Comparison between

different studies is difficult due to different populations sampled

and criteria for diagnosis used. For example, studies examining a

narrow age band looking at children between the ages of 4-6

years27 (when SM is more likely to present), yield higher prevalence

rates than those examining a wider age band4 (7-15 years). In

addition studies using DSM IV criteria yield smaller prevalence rates

than those using ICD 10 or DSM III R1 due to the inclusion of

impairment as a criterion for diagnosis in DSM IV.5 School and

community based studies typically yield consistently higher rates

than those cited in clinical samples, reflecting the fact that many

children with SM do not come to clinical attention.

The conceptualisation of SM has undergone a number of changes

in recent years. A wide variety of child characteristics have been

reported, including oppositional behaviour, developmental delay,

speech disorder, anxiety and shyness.6,7,8,9,10 Recent research

describes SM as closely related or an early developmental

expression of social phobia.11,12,13 Other authors have reported a

high frequency of neurodevelopmental delay, affecting motor,

linguistic and cognitive functioning. 9,14,15,16

A high frequency of family psychopathology has also been reported

in SM families.17,12,18,19 Previous studies have suggested an

association between SM and parental social anxiety.12,19 Two studies

suggest a family link with Asperger Disorder. 20,21

This study aimed to carry out a community study and follow cases

up with a clinical evaluation to try and establish an accurate

prevalence rate in a defined geographical catchment area in

Ireland. It also aimed to document any comorbidity or family

pathology.

36

* Louise Sharkey, Consultant Child and Adolescent Psychiatrist,Mater Child and Adolescent Mental Health Service,Mater Misericordiae University Hospital,Dublin 7, Ireland.E-mail [email protected]

Fiona McNicholas,Consultant Child Psychiatrist and Professor of ChildPsychiatry, Lucena Clinic, Rathgar, Dublin 6, Ireland.Our Lady’s Children’s Hospital, CrumlinUniversity College Dublin, Ireland.

Submitted September 2010Accepted August 2011

Ir J Psych Med 2012; 29 (1): 36-40

Brief Report

Selective Mutism: A prevalence study of primary school children in the Republic of Ireland

Louise Sharkey, Fiona McNicholas

* Correspondence

Page 37: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Method

Participants:The study group consisted of children aged 4-12 years attending

primary school in a defined geographical urban child and

adolescent mental health service catchment area in the Republic

of Ireland. The catchment area covers a population of 350 000,

primarily belonging to the lower socio-economic group.24 There are

39 primary schools in this area and all were sampled, covering a

total population of 10,927 students.

The study was approved by the ethical review board of St John of

God’s Hospital. Full informed written consent was obtained from

parents or guardians of all participants before the study.

Procedure:The study was conducted late in the academic year (April/ May), in

order to ensure a six month duration and to eliminate transient

mutism, common in the first term post school entry. A description

of selective mutism according to DSM-IV criteria22 was sent to the

principal and teachers of each school. If the principal or teacher

suspected that a pupil met criteria, they completed and returned

the Selective Mutism Questionnaire,2 in the stamped addressed

envelope enclosed. In addition, the principal was asked to indicate

on a separate form if there were no children in the school with

symptoms suggestive of SM. Children who met DSM-IV criteria for

SM were invited to participate in a clinical assessment with a

consultant child psychiatrist, to confirm the diagnosis and to assess

for additional comorbidity. The child and parent filled out some

additional questionnaires and the clinician completed the Children’s

Global Assessment Scale (CGAS) 23 (see Table 1).

Results

A total of 39 schools (100%) responded to the study

questionnaires. Schools who did not return data within a specified

time frame were followed up via telephone contact. Up to three

telephone calls were required before all data was returned for

analysis. Twenty children were identified by 19 teachers of 12

schools as having symptoms suggestive of SM. Twenty seven

schools returned forms or indicated through telephone contact

that there were no children in the school with symptoms suggestive

of the disorder. All parents of children who met criteria on the SMQ

were contacted by a clinician and offered a clinical assessment.

Parents of 14 children (70%), consented to an assessment.

The ages of children with SM ranged from 5.2-10.6 years (average

age 6.9 years). There was a preponderance of female subjects

(female to male ratio was 4:1). The spoken language in 11 of the

families was English. Three of the pupils were of ethnic minority

origin, but had been living in the country and attending school for

more than two years. All three pupils were reported to be bilingual

and demonstrated SM symptoms in both native and non-native

languages. Eleven children (79%) lived with both biological

parents. All children had regular contact with both parents. Ten

(71%) were the eldest in their family. All families belonged to the

lower socio-economic group.24

Clinical presentation:All children assessed met diagnostic criteria for Selective Mutism.22

The onset of their symptoms dated back to school entry (average

age 4.6 years). One child was previously assessed by the psychiatric

services and three received therapy for dyspraxia. Nine children

(64%) had a history of speech and language delay. One child had

a diagnosis of Tourette’s Syndrome. All children were considered

by their school to be functioning within the average range of

cognitive ability. All children with SM presented with moderate to

severe impairment in functioning (CGAS = 32-61) (Figure 1), as

rated by clinicians. (Table 2)

Only 21%, one fifth of the sample, scored within the clinical range

on the SCAS25 as rated by the children or parents (>42.48) (Figure

2). The most frequent items scored by these children related to

social phobia and separation anxiety disorder. None of the children

met criteria for obsessive compulsive disorder, generalized anxiety

disorder or panic disorder, as rated by the SCAS. (Table 2)

Although only four children (29%) received a total score within the

abnormal range on the SDQ,26 eight (57%) presented with

emotional difficulties and five (36%) with problems with peer

interaction and 2 (14%) with behaviour difficulties (Table 4).

Family psychopathology:Family psychopathology and neuro-developmental delay were

assessed by a child psychiatrist according to detailed information

about family history obtained during clinical interview. Results were

included only if a formal diagnosis had been given by a qualified

professional, for example a psychiatrist, psychologist or speech and

language therapist. According to parental report, there was a

positive family history in nine children (64.3%) of speech and

language delay or articulation difficulties in a first degree relative

which required clinical intervention. Similarly, there was a high

frequency of social anxiety disorders among family members with

two siblings from two families and seven (50%) parents who

reported receiving treatment for either selective mutism or social

phobia. All children had at least one parent who was portrayed as

‘very shy’. Surprisingly, almost half the sample, six (43%) reported

a diagnosis of autistic spectrum disorder in a first degree relative.

(Table 3)

Discussion

To the authors’ knowledge, this is the first prevalence study of

selective mutism in Ireland, and suggests a prevalence rate of

0.18% in a sample of primary school children aged 4-12 years. This

is a two stage study, using questionnaire screening and diagnostic

interview to achieve a diagnosis of SM using DSM IV criteria. As

such the prevalence rate found (0.18%) may be lower than that

found in other studies with less stringent diagnostic criteria. In

addition, this study included children aged 4-12 years, as compared

to other studies sampling a narrower age band (4-6 years) when

SM is most likely to present. For example, Kopp & Gillberg4 who

used criteria that are stricter than DSM-IV, found a rate of 0.18%

comparable to our results, in their study of Swedish school children

(7-15 years), while the relatively high rate of 1.9% reported by

Kumpulainen et al,5 was based on DSM III R criteria1 that do not

include impairment.

37

Page 38: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

More recently Bergman et al11 reported a prevalence rate of 0.76%

among their sample of kindergarten, first and second grade pupils

using DSM-IV22 criteria. Their relatively higher prevalence rate is

comparable to the rate reported in the United Kingdom by Brown

& Lloyd,27 and probably reflects the narrow age band studied,

when SM is more likely to present.

Although SM is generally considered to be a rare disorder, most of

the recent evidence does not support this. The figure of 0.06% cited

by Fundudis et al28 is based on the use of very vague criteria for

diagnosing SM, that are not comparable to the current

conceptualization of SM in DSM-IV. This figure however is similar to

a more recent publication by Karakaya et al,3 in their study of

kindergarten to 3rd grade pupils using DSM-IV criteria. Their initial

prevalence rate following screening by teachers of 0.83% was

reduced to 0.033% following clinical evaluation by a child psychiatrist.

Gender differences have been noted in past studies of SM and a

suggestion that gender differences in prevalence increase with

age.16 In our study, we found a male: female prevalence ratio of

1:4, with more females than typically reported (ratio of 1:1.2),6

which may reflect the older age range sampled.

Comorbidity:Comorbid conditions were highly prevalent in our sample of

children with selective mutism. Similar to previous studies,11,12,13 all

children in the study were rated by clinicians as moderately to

severely impaired in social, academic and overall functioning.

Speech and language delay was the most common comorbid

problem, reported in 64% of the sample studied. This association

is in line with findings of 30-60% language disorder/delay recorded

in previous studies of SM. 9,16,29,30

Twenty one percent of the children with SM met criteria for

dyspraxia, a rate slightly lower than that reported by Kristensen15

for DCD. Only one fifth of the sample met criteria for an anxiety

disorder on a self-rating scale. This finding is inconsistent with

previous studies,12,13, 16 that suggest that SM is an anxiety disorder,

closely related to social phobia, but consistent with other studies

using self-rating scales to measure anxiety symptoms in children,

who manage anxiety symptoms and the self-consciousness

associated with speaking by avoiding speech.30 A teacher rating

scale would probably yield a much higher rate of anxiety disorder

in this population. The finding that only four children (29%)

received a total score in the abnormal range on the SDQ26 as rated

by parents, reflects the contextual nature of the disorder, where

SM may not be perceived as a problem in the home environment,

and the family’s tolerance and acceptance of mute behaviour (Table 4).

Family characteristics:There was a high loading for speech and language delay and

anxiety symptoms in the families of selectively mute children. Fifty

percent of the sample reported a family history of social anxiety

disorders and at least one parent of all children described

themselves as ‘excessively’ shy. This is consistent with earlier

results.32,33,34 Sixty-four percent of our sample reported a history of

speech and language delay in first degree relatives, comparable to

results from previous studies (21% - 78%).16,35,36.

Although an association between SM and autistic spectrum

disorder has been queried in previous studies,20 almost half of our

sample had a first degree relative with a diagnosis of ASD. Our

findings extend previous reports and describe the important role

family psychopathology plays in the aetiology of SM.

Limitations:This current study suffers from a number of methodological

limitations. Information from teachers was limited to a screening

questionnaire, and as children with SM are more likely to manifest

symptoms within the school environment, additional information

from teachers, in particular in relation to anxiety symptoms, would

have contributed significantly to this study. Second, our largely

working class sample, while representative of our geographically

defined catchment area, is probably not representative of the

population as a whole, and while attendance at school is mandatory,

it is possible that children with SM who are not attending school

were missed by this methodology. However principals were reminded

to consider all children enrolled in the school. Thirdly, as there is no

direct control group, comorbidity and family characteristics of this

SM sample can only be compared to those found in other published

studies. Whilst the structure of the family interview method would

have led to increased reliability and could have been done by an

independent researcher, the expertise of the clinician in taking family

histories must not be underestimated.

Despite these shortcomings the study has a number of

methodological qualities. The study of a non-referred population of

school children aged 4-12 years provides a more representative

sample than that obtained in clinical studies, and identifies the

population of ‘hidden SM’, children who do not come to clinical

attention as they do not cause a disturbance in the classroom, and

the disorder is not perceived as problematic in the home

environment. In addition, our study included a relatively large

population of school-aged children. All children whose parents

consented received a psychiatric evaluation, by a senior clinician,

confirming the diagnosis and identifying comorbidity. The use of

validated rating scales completed by multiple informants, emphasized

the situational nature of the disorder and the degree of impairment

experienced by children who have not yet come to clinical attention.

Conclusion

This study provides data that concur with other studies in

suggesting that SM is not as rare as previously reported, and that

a significant number of children with considerable impairment do

not come to clinical attention until later years, requiring more

intensive intervention. This suggests that efforts to identify and

treat SM in the early school year need to be increased, so that an

increased awareness of the disorder and early intervention

treatment programmes can be delivered in the school environment.

Following this study the authors developed a SM manual which

has been sent to all primary schools in the catchment area.37

Our findings suggest an association between SM and a family

history of social anxiety disorder, speech and language delay and

autistic spectrum disorder. It is significant that seven (50%) parents

reported receiving treatment for either selective mutism or social

phobia. While these results should be viewed as preliminary, further

studies of the etiological nature of the disorder and more specific

evidence based treatment interventions are clearly warranted.

38

Page 39: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Conflict of interest

None.

References

1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders – Third Edition Revised. Washington. American Psychiatric

Association, 1987.

2. Bergman RL, Keller M, Wood J, Piacentini J, McCracken J. Selective Mutism

Questionaire (SMQ): development and findings. Poster session presented at

the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Honolulu, USA.

2001

3. Karakaya I, Sismanlar SG, Oc OY, Memik NC, Coskun A, Agaoglu B, Yavuz

CI. (2007). Selective Mutism: A school- based cross-sectional study from

Turkey. Euro Child Adolesc Psychiatry 2007; 17:114- 117.

4. Kopp S, Gillberg C. Selective mutism: a population-based study: a research

note. J Child Psychol and Psychiatry 1997; 38:257-262.

5. Kumpulainen K, Rasanen R, Raaska H, Sompii V. Selective Mutism among

second- graders in an elementary school. Euro Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1998;

7: 24-29.

6. Tancer N. Elective Mutism: A Review of the Literature. Advances in Clin Child

Psychol 1992; 14:265-288.

7. Bradley S, Sloman L. Elective mutism in immigrant families. J Am Acad Child

Psychiatry 1975; 14: 510-514.

8. Hayden TL. The Classification of elective mutism. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry

1980;19: 118-133.

9. Kolvin I, Fundudis T. (1981). Electively mute children: Psychological

Development and background factors. J Clin Psychol Psychiatry 1981;

22:219-232.

10. Leonard HL. Selective Mutism. In Kaplan & Sadock’s Comprehensive Textbook

of Psychiatry (Vol 1, 7th Ed) Sadock BJ, Sadock VA.

11. Bergman RL, Piacentini J, Mc Cracken J. Prevalence and Description of

Selective Mutism in a school-based sample. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry 2002;

41: 238-246.

12. Black B, Uhde TW. Psychiatric characteristics of children with selective mutism:

A pilot study. J Am Acad of Child Psychiatry 1995; 34:847-855.

13. Dummit ES, Klein RG, Tancer NK, Asche B, Martin J, Fairbanks JA. Systematic

assessment of 50 children with selective mutism. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry

1997; 36:653-660.

14. Kristensen H. Elective mutism associated with developmental disorder/

delay:two case studies. Euro Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1997; 6:234-239.

15. Kristensen, H. Selective mutism and comorbidity with developmental

disorder/delay, anxiety disorder and elimination disorder. J Am Acad Child

Adolesc Psychiatry 2000; 39: 249-256.

16. Steinhausen HC & Juzi C. Elective Mutism: An analysis of 100 cases. J Am

Acad Child Psychiatry 1996; 35:606-614.

17. Brown BJ, Lloyd H. A controlled study of children not speaking at school. J Ass

Workers with Maladjusted Children 1975; 3: 49- 63.

18. Browne E, Wilson V, Laybourne P. Diagnosis and treatment of elective mutism

in children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1963; 2:605-617.

19. Kristensen H & Torgersen S. MCMI-II personality traits and symptom traits in

parents of children with selective mutism: A case-controlstudy. J Abn Psychol

2001; 110:648-652.20. Gillberg IC & Gillberg C. Asperger syndrome – some epidemiological

considerations: a research note. J Child Psychol and Psychiatry 1989; 30:631- 638.

21. Brix Andersson C, Hove Thomsen P. Electively mute children: an analysis of 37

Danish cases. Nordic J Psychiatry 1998;52:231-238.

22. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders – Fourth Edition. Washington. American Psychiatric Association 1994.

23. Shaffer D, Gould MS, Brasic J, Ambrosini P, Fisher P, Bird H, & Aluwahlia S.

Childrens Global Assessment Scale. Archives of General Psychiatry

1983; 40:1228-1231.

24. National Statistics Socio-economic Classification: users manual

NS-SEC. 2005: Office for National Statistics.

25. Spence S. The Spence children’s Anxiety Scale. The Child Psychology

Portfolio1997: NFER-NELSON.

26. Goodman, R. The Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire: a research note.

J Child Psychol and Psychiatry 1997; 38:581-586.

27. Brown BJ, Lloyd H. A controlled study of children not speaking at school.

Journal of Association of Workers with Maladjusted Children, 1975; 3, 49- 63.

28. Fundudis T, Kolvin I, Gariside RF. Speech Retarded and Deaf Children their

Psychological Development. 1979; London:Academic Press.

29. Rosler M.Befunde beim neurotischen Mutismus der Kinder cine

Untersuchungan 32 mutistischen Kindern. Praisx der Kinderpsychologie und

Kinderpsychiatier 1981; 30, 187- 194.

30. Wilkens, R. A comparison of elective mutism and emotional disorders in

children. British Journal of Psychiatry, 1985;146, 198-203.

31. DiBartolo PM, Albano AM, Barlow DH, Heimberg RG. Cross-informant

agreement in the assessment of social phobia in youth. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology 1998; 26, 213-20.

32. Lorand B. Katamnese elektiv mutistischer Kinder. Acta Paedopsychiatrica

1960; 27, 273-289.

33. Popella, E. Psychogener Mutismus bei Kindern. Nervenarzt, 1960;31, 257-263.

34. Steinhausen HC, Adamek R. The family history of children with elective

mutism. European Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 1997; 6, 107-111.

35. Carmody L. The Power of Silence: Selective Mutism in Ireland – a speech and

language perspective. Journal of Clinical Speech and Language Studies,

2000;1, 41-60.

36. Remschmidt H., Poller M, Herpertz-Dahlmann B, Hennighausen K.,

Gutenbrunner, C.A follow-up study of 45 patients with elective mutism.

European Archives of Psychiatry and Clinical Neuroscience, 2001; 251, 284-296.

37. Sharkey L, McNicholas F, Begley M. Elective Mutism Manual. Dublin. Mac

Communications, 2004.

39

Figure 1: CGAS Scores Figure 1: CGAS Scores

Page 40: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

40

Table 1: Measures:

Name of questionnaire Rater Description

Children’s Global Clinician

Assessment Scale (CGAS)

Selective Mutism Parent/Teacher

Questionaire (SMQ)

Strengths and Difficulties Parent

Questionaire (SDQ)

Spence Children’s Anxiety Child

Scale (SCAS) (age 8-12 years)

Parental report

completed for

children

age 4-8 years

A measure of psychosocial and psychiatric functioning for children aged 4-16 years.

A score less than 61 is used to identify subjects with definite disorders and a score

between 61 and 71 to identify subjects with probable disorders.

A 36-item multiple-choice scale. Parental or teacher report of child’s speaking

behaviours and interference associated with failure to speak.

A 24-item questionnaire of children aged 4-16 years. It identifies children with

conduct, hyperactivity, depressive and anxiety disorders.

A self-report measure of anxiety. Thirty-eight anxiety items, six filler items and one

open-ended non-scored item assessing social desirability. Scores on six sub-scales

relating to separation anxiety, social phobia, panic/agoraphobia, generalized anxiety,

obsessions/compulsions and fear of physical injury. The child is asked to rate on a

four point scale – ‘never’= 0, ‘sometimes’ = 1, ‘often’ = 2, or ‘always’ = 3 – how

often each item happens to them. This yields a maximum score of 114.

Table 2: Clinical Descriptives N=14

Age 6.9 (5.2-10.6)

Females 11

Primary language 11 english (78.5%)

3 bilingual

Onset symptoms Average 4.6 years (range 2.9 – 5.2 years)

Past child psychiatry history 1=assessed

1=Tourette’s Syndrome (same child as above)

Treatment Dyspraxia 3 (21%)

Speech & Language delay 9 (64%)

CGAS 14 (100%) >moderate impairment

Range 36-61

SCAS 3 (21%) clinical range for anxiety

Table 3: Family History

Mother Father Siblings 1st relative

SM 3 2

SP 3 1 1

Shy 11 3

SL delay 1 1 3 4

ASD 1 5(6)

Depression 1

Hearing Imp’d 1

Dyslexia 1

Table 4: SDQ

(abnormal range) Average score Normal Borderline Abnormal

Emotional* (5-10 abN) 5 5 1 8 (57%)

HA/Imp (7-10 abN) 4 11 0 3 (21%)

CD (4-10 abN) 2 10 2 2 (14%)

Prosocial (0-4 abN) 6.7 11 2 1 (7%)

Peer* (4-10 abN) 2 9 0 5 (36%)

Total* (17-40 abN) 12.6 8 2 4 (29%)

Page 41: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Abstract

Objectives: Psychotherapeutic skills are essential to practicepsychiatry effectively, but previous surveys of trainees in Ireland

show that they have not been getting adequate training in

psychotherapy. This survey of college tutors was carried out to

coincide with the establishment of the College of Psychiatry of

Ireland which is introducing new psychotherapy training

requirements. The survey aims to ascertain if trainees had fulfilled

the Royal College of Psychiatrists’ psychotherapy training

requirements, models of psychotherapy available and the

availability of psychotherapy qualifications amongst consultants

and senior registrars.

Methods: A questionnaire was posted to all registered tutors inthe Republic of Ireland.

Results: The postal questionnaire was sent to the 62 registeredcollege tutors in the 13 training schemes. There is one tutor in each

training hospital. The response rate was 79%. Our survey reveals

that no psychotherapy training was available according to 16.3%

of tutors. Only 22.5% of tutors were aware of trainees who had

met college training requirements in the previous two years. 79.8%

of tutors reported that there were consultants and senior registrars

with qualifications in psychotherapy who could offer training if

time and resources permitted.

Conclusions: Current training requirements are not being fulfilled.There are consultants and senior registrars who have

psychotherapy qualifications to provide psychotherapy training but

there are inadequate resources and time to formalise training. It is

unlikely that the implementation of training requirements by the

new college will be realisable without a review of training delivery.

Key words: Psychotherapy; Training; Resources

Introduction

Psychotherapeutic skills are an important component of general

psychiatric practice: to enhance awareness of one’s emotional

responses to situations, to improve communication skills and in

understanding group dynamics.1 The biopsychosocial model is

widely accepted and endorsed by A Vision for Change 2 and yet

evidence from surveys of trainees in Ireland shows that trainees are

not getting adequate experience or training in the psychological

components of this model of psychiatric practice.3,4,5 This has been

largely attributed to supply of trainers and supervisors rather than

low demand from trainee psychiatrists.4

The new College of Psychiatry of Ireland was established in 2009

and a new training curriculum for basic specialist training has been

published, though this will be reviewed. This could be viewed as an

opportunity to have a fresh look at psychotherapy training and

how it could realistically be implemented given the ever increasing

workload of psychiatrists.

The Royal College of Psychiatrists has consistently emphasised the

importance of psychotherapy training by making it mandatory as

part of basic psychiatric training. The practice of psychotherapy is

considered a key aspect of psychiatric practice.1, 6 In 1993 the Royal

College of Psychiatrists published guidelines for psychotherapy

training as part of general professional training in psychiatry.7 In

2001 the College published requirements that were more specific

and were intended to be mandatory for eligibility for the MRCPsych

examinations.8 Psychotherapeutic knowledge and skills feature

prominently in the Royal College of Psychiatrists’ 2009 CoreModule of a Competency Based Curriculum for Specialist Trainingin Psychiatry.9 Trainees are expected to have delivered “basicpsychological treatment in at least two modalities of therapy and

over longer and shorter durations.”

Guidelines and the mandatory requirements of the Royal College

of Psychiatrists have proved difficult to implement with repeated

41

* Angela Noonan, Consultant Psychiatrist,St. Vincent’s Hospital,Richmond Road,Dublin 3, Ireland.E-mail [email protected]

Alyson Lee,Consultant Psychiatrist,Lakeview Unit,Naas General Hospital,Naas, Co. Kildare, Ireland.

Submitted July 20th 2010Accepted August 31st 2011

Ir J Psych Med 2012; 29 (1): 41-45

Brief Report

Psychotherapy training in Ireland: A survey of college tutorsAlyson Lee, Angela Noonan

* Correspondence

Page 42: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

42

studies showing that trainees have often been unable to meet

them.10,11,12 A survey on psychotherapy conducted on trainees in

the Eastern region of Ireland in 1997 also showed failure to meet

requirements.4 In this study the authors found that 31.4% of

trainees surveyed received formal training in psychotherapy and

47% delivered psychotherapy. Only 4% of trainees met the College

requirements which were in place at the time.7 Trainees rate

psychotherapy training as relevant to their overall training but have

highlighted the lack of training opportunities and supervision as a

problem.3,4,5

Tutors were surveyed for several reasons: knowledge of availability

and access to training at a local level; knowledge of trainees who

are fulfilling training requirements; identification of psychotherapy

skills among trainers and supervisors.

The Royal College of Psychiatrists’ psychotherapy training

requirements8 published in 2001 which were in place at the time

of the survey were:

• Development of interview skills

• Psychotherapeutic formulation of psychiatric disorder

• A minimum of three short term cases (12-16 sessions), each

using a different psychotherapeutic model

• One long-term individual case (12-18 months)-any model

• Some experience of either group psychotherapy or couple,

family or systemic therapy

Method

A list of registered college tutors was obtained from the College of

Psychiatry of Ireland. An anonymous questionnaire was posted to

the 62 registered college tutors in the 13 training schemes. The

tutors were asked to complete and return the questionnaire to the

College of Psychiatry of Ireland in an envelope provided. The

respondents were asked the following questions:

1. Is there psychotherapy training available on your scheme?

2. If there is training available who provides this? (Respondents

were asked to identify disciplines that provided training.)

3. If there is training available what models of psychotherapy can

the trainees access?

4. Within the last two years (2006-2008) did any trainees meet

the Royal College of Psychiatrists’ training requirements?

5. Are there consultants and senior registrars trained in

psychotherapy who, if time and resources permitted, could

provide training?

There was a free text section for comments.

(A copy of the questionnaire is available from the authors on

request).

Results

The results were recorded and collated by the authors and are

presented in tables 1-5. The response rate was 79% with 49 tutors

out of a possible 62 responding to the questionnaire. Some

psychotherapy training was available to trainees according to

83.7% of tutors, with 16.3% reporting no training available. CBT

was the most commonly available modality of psychotherapy. In

36.7% of training centres only psychiatrists were involved in

delivering psychotherapy training. Table 3 shows how some centres

have a limited range of modalities of psychotherapy training, where

it exists at all. A noteworthy finding is that 79.8% of tutors

reported that there were senior clinicians with training in

psychotherapy who could be involved in the delivery of training if

time and resources permitted. 67.3% of tutors stated that trainees

had not met the training requirements and 10.2% did not know if

trainees had met the requirements.

Table 1: Provision of Psychotherapy Training

Provider Number of sites

None 8 (16.3%)

Psychiatry only 18 (36.7%)

Psychology only 6 (12.2%)

Family therapy only 1 (2%)

Psychiatry and psychology 5 (10.2%)

Psychiatry and social work 1 (2%)

Psychiatry and family therapist 2 (4.1%)

Psychiatry and nurse specialist 2 (4.1%)

Psychiatry and CBT therapist 1 (2%)

Psychiatry, psychology and nurse therapist 3 (6.1%)

Psychiatry, psychology, nurse therapist 1 (2%)

and occupational therapist

Psychology and nurse therapist 1 (2%)

Table 2: Modalities of Training Available

Modality Number of sites where available

CBT 31 (63.3%)

Supportive 21 (42.9%)

Psychoanalytic 19 (38.8%)

Systemic/Family Therapy 15 (30.6%)

CAT 6 (12.2%)

IPT 4 (8.2%)

DBT 3 (6.1%)

Other 2 (4.1%)

Table 3: Number of Psychotherapy Modalities Available

Number of modalities Number of tutors who reported availability

None 8 (16.3%)

1 modality 9 (18.4%)

2 modalities 16 (32.7%)

3 modalities 9 (18.4%)

4 modalities 3 (6.1%)

5 modalities 4 (8.2%)

Page 43: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

43

Table 4: Trainees meeting Royal College of Psychiatrists guidelines

Yes 11 (22.5%)

No 33 (67.3%)

Don’t know 5 (10.2%)

Table 5: Consultants and Senior Registrars trained inpsychotherapy who could provide training if time andresources permitted

Yes 39 (79.6%)

No 10 (20.4%)

Discussion

To our knowledge this is the first survey of college tutors about

psychotherapy training in Ireland. However, the small percentage of

trainees meeting college training requirements is in keeping with

a previous survey of trainees in the Eastern region of Ireland.4 As

the larger training schemes have several tutors they may be over

represented and the finding that 16.3% of tutors report no

psychotherapy training could be an underestimate as it is likely that

in smaller schemes there are fewer resources.

Our survey shows that 79.6% of respondents affirmed that training

could be provided if there were sufficient time and resources

available. In Ireland there are approximately 400 trainees in basic

training and approximately 100 in higher training. There are only

two approved consultant psychiatrist posts with a special interest

in psychotherapy in the public health service.19 In contrast to the

UK there are no subspecialty placements in psychotherapy at basic

specialist training or higher training level.

This survey highlights the fact that psychiatrists are obtaining

psychotherapy training but are not using these skills to train junior

colleagues. Furthermore, whilst consultants may update their

theoretical knowledge through attendance at courses and

workshops, they possibly are not utilising their formal

psychotherapy skills due to other commitments which can lead to

their attrition.18

In June 2008 the Irish Psychiatric Training Committee produced a

Handbook for Basic Specialist Training in Psychiatry13 which was

valid at the time of our survey (and a slightly updated version is still

valid for all but the foundation year basic specialist trainees who

commenced training in July 2011). The handbook includes the

following psychotherapy training guidelines:

• Understanding of psychodynamics and team work

• Development of good communication and interview skills

• Participation in an experiential reflective group (e.g. Balint

group) minimum 30 sessions

• One long term case (minimum 24 sessions)

• Three short term cases (8-16 sessions)

The clinical cases should be in at least two different psychotherapy

modalities. It also states that training must include both theoretical

and practical training and must be effectively supervised. Since the

establishment of the College of Psychiatry of Ireland, a Basic

Specialist Training Blueprint based on achievement of competencies

has been published.6 This includes yet another set of

psychotherapeutic goals. The requirement of the Handbook

regarding four cases is suggested as desirable. It is stated that

training schemes must provide Balint-style groups. Additionally the

Blueprint states that each trainee must complete a minimum of 18

sessions of brief psychological intervention and demonstrate

competence in psychotherapeutic aspects of advanced history

taking and formulation. As one of the competencies for child

psychiatry, it is stated that the trainee must be able to utilise play

therapy. The basic principles of the new curriculum are to be

welcomed, as indeed is the introduction of a training portfolio, but

in our view what a trainee is expected to know and to be able to

demonstrate at the end of training remains onerous and the

obstacles to training delivery remain the same.

There are other challenges. Many training schemes cover wide

geographical areas so seeing a patient over a long period of time

is impractical. One solution would be for trainees to remain in the

same hospital for one year. Many trainees do spend their first year

in one hospital but as they advance in their training when

undertaking psychotherapy may be more appropriate, they rotate

through subspecialty posts every six months and lack of continuity

in terms of patient and trainee tutor contact are further obstacles.

Some tutors were not aware of how many, if any, trainees were

meeting training requirements. One tutor responded that

psychotherapy training was “virtually non-existent leaving a

massive chasm in service provision.” Another respondent said that

the training arrangements were “very piecework.” The

establishment of a national training programme with designated

psychotherapy tutors in each training scheme would create greater

structure and continuity to training. This was suggested as a

possible solution by a previous survey of trainees and consultants.3

The European Working Time Directive means that trainees have

less time than before to fulfil training requirements. The high court

settlement on 22nd of January 2010, between the Irish Medical

Organisation and the Health Service Executive decreed that time

rostered for training would not constitute working time under the

EWTD but would qualify for payment.14 This may be a solution;

however, it would depend on the motivation of trainees and the

willingness of employers to pay.

Even where training is available, feedback from some tutors

indicates that interest and take up of psychotherapy cases can be

poor. This may be because of fear and apprehension about feeling

de-skilled12 and of psychotherapy not being immediately pertinent

to training and career progression as the focus of trainees may be

on passing examinations.13 UK based trainees are expected to have

been assessed formally in having achieved competencies in

psychotherapy and the trainee must have received structured,

documented feedback on their performance to be eligible to

undertake the CASC exam, the final component of the

membership exam.16 Tutors based in Ireland are completing

sponsorship forms for the CASC examination without these

competencies being demonstrated.

Page 44: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Another challenge is bringing about a shift in the culture of training

so that more emphasis is placed on psychotherapy experience in

becoming a good psychiatrist. One respondent stated that we

require psychotherapy role models. The majority of trainees aspire

to do their higher training in Ireland and in order to be considered

for interview for higher training, candidates will be awarded marks

for research or audit that has been published or presented.

According to the College of Psychiatry of Ireland, qualifications in

psychotherapy will be rated for shortlisting for higher training in

psychiatry.17 However, obtaining a qualification in psychotherapy

is a time consuming undertaking and is unlikely to be attained

during basic training. Perhaps the College could consider rewarding

trainees who have documentary evidence of cases undertaken for

psychotherapy as part of basic specialist training so that trainees

selected for higher training have a range of both academic and

psychotherapeutic skills.

Conclusion

Limited psychotherapy training is available in most training

schemes but 16.3% of respondents reported that no

psychotherapy training of any kind was available in their training

scheme. Although only 22.5% of respondents stated that their

training scheme met the Royal College of Psychiatrists training

requirements, almost 80% of tutors said that there were a

sufficient number of senior trainees and consultants who could

provide training if they had enough time to do so. Increased

resources are needed so that consultants have protected time to

provide psychotherapy supervision. Provision of training by our

psychology colleagues needs to be considered further although

their resources are also very limited. The establishment of a national

training programme would allow for greater structure and

coordination in delivery of training. We have suggested other

changes to improve delivery of psychotherapy training which

include one year hospital placements in the latter part of basic

training and shifting the training culture by placing a greater value

on psychotherapy experience in the shortlisting process for higher

specialist training. The formation of the College of Psychiatry of

Ireland and the development of a new curriculum provides a

wonderful opportunity to enshrine the central importance of

psychotherapy in psychiatric training, but given the many

competing demands on consultants, our view is that unless there

are increased resources, the proposed requirements will merely

represent unrealisable ideals.

Conflict of interest

None.

Acknowledgements

Thanks to Grace Smyth of the College of Psychiatry of Ireland for

her invaluable assistance.

References

1. Mitchison S. Next year in Jerusalem: Psychotherapy training for tomorrow’s

consultants. Advances in Psychiatric Treatment 2007;13:276-283

2. A Vision for Change. Report of the expert group on mental health policy. The

Stationery Office.2006. http://www.hse.ie/eng/services/Publications/services/

Mentalhealth/Mental_Health_-_A_Vision_for_Change.pdf

3. Rooney S, Kelly G. Psychotherapy experience in Ireland. Psychiatr Bull 1999;

23:89-94.

4. Byrne P, Meagher D. Psychotherapy and trainees. Psychiatr Bull

1997; 21:707-710.

5. O’ Mahoney E, Corvin A. The attitudes of Irish trainees to their training and

its supervision: a five year follow up study. Ir J Psych Med 2001; 18(4): 120-

125.

6. http://www.irishpsychiatry.ie/Libraries/Postgraduate_

Training_Documents/BST_Blueprint_Final.sflb.ashx (accessed 25th July 2011)

7. Royal College of Psychiatrists. Guidelines for psychotherapy training as part

of general professional training. Psychiatr Bull 1993;17:695-698.

8. Royal College of Psychiatrists. Requirements for psychotherapy training as

part of basic specialist psychiatric training. Royal College of Psychiatrists.

2001.

9. http://www.rcpsych.ac.uk/pdf/CORE%20CURRICULUM

%20October%202010.pdf (accessed 26th July 2011)

10. McCrindle D, Wildgoose J, Tillett R. Survey of psychotherapy training for

psychiatric trainees in south west England. Psychiatr Bull 2001; 25:140-143.

11. Pretorius W, Goldbeck R. Survey of psychotherapy experience and interest

among psychiatric specialist registrars. Psychiatr Bull 2006; 30:223-225.

12. Agarwal S, Singh Y, Palanisamy V, Basker R, Van der Speck R. Psychotherapy

requirements as recommended by the college: awareness and achievement

by senior house officers. Psychiatr Bull 2007; 31:394-396.

13. http://www.irishpsychiatry.ie/Libraries/2010_Press_Releases

/CPsychi_BST_handbook_sept_2010.sflb.ashx (accessed 26th July 2011)

14. http://www.hse.ie/eng/about/Who/Board_Members/ceorpts

/feb10ceorpt.pdf (accessed 25th July 2011)

15. Malcolm M. Reflections in psychotherapy: a personal view.

Pscyhiatr Bull 1977; 1:8-10.

16. http://www.rcpsych.ac.uk/pdf/Information%20Pack%20

for%20the%20Psychotherapy%20section%20of%20the

%20C&G%20curriculum.pdf (accessed 26th July 2011)

17. http://www.irishpsychiatry.ie/Libraries/Postgraduate_

Training_Documents/Psy868_Shortlisting_Criteria.sflb.ashx (accessed 26th

July 2011)

18. Denman C. The place of psychotherapy in modern psychiatric practice.

Advances in psych treatment 2011;17:243-249.

19. Consultant Appointment Unit, Health Service Executive

45

Page 45: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Abstract

Objective: The aim of this exploratory study was to investigate theextent of suicide ideation, psychological maladjustment and views

of mental health service support in a sample of secondary school

pupils.

Method: A cross-sectional survey was conducted on a purposivesample of participants (n=93) recruited from a secondary school

located in the south east of Ireland. Participants completed a

Background Information Questionnaire (BIQ); the Suicide Ideation

Questionnaire (SIQ); and the Reynolds Adolescence Adjustment

Screening Inventory (RAASI).

Results: Approximately ten percent of participants displayed highlevels of suicide ideation whilst one third reported having previously

had suicidal thoughts; one quarter reported psychological

adjustment difficulties, although these varied by age and sex.

Participants’ drug use and their levels of parental closeness were

both individual factors that significantly predicted both suicide

ideation and psychological adjustment. Females reported higher

levels of parental closeness than males. Forty percent of

respondents rated mental health support services as insufficient to

meet their needs.

Conclusion: The findings raise serious concerns about the extentof suicidal thoughts amongst young people in Ireland; they also

highlight a potentially important role for parents in this regard.

Further research should ascertain national prevalence rates whilst

appropriate school-based mental health education/promotion and

support services should also be implemented.

Key words: Suicide ideation, Young people, Adolescents, Schools,Mental health.

Introduction

The increasing levels of suicide in Ireland, particularly amongst

young people, have attracted much attention in recent years.

Ireland has the fifth highest rate of youth suicide in the European

Union1 and suicide is the principal cause of death amongst young

men in this country.2 In 2006, 409 people in the Republic of Ireland

(9.6 per 100,000) reportedly completed suicide.3 Crucially, these

figures do not include parasuicide or deliberate self harm, for which

the National Parasuicide Registry (NPR) recorded 11,000 hospital

attendances in 2004. Recent data released by the Institute of Public

Health (IPH) (Barron et al 2008)4 indicate an overall suicide rate for

the Republic of Ireland of 11.2/100,000 (2001-2004), although this

figure conceals considerable regional variations.

A number of factors have been identified as antecedents in suicidal

behaviour and suicide attempts,5, 6, 7 whilst a stable progression has

also been found between suicide ideation and suicidal behaviour.8, 9 However, relatively little research on suicide ideation has been

conducted amongst young people in non-clinical settings in

Ireland, particularly within school-based populations. O’Sullivan10

and Lynch2 have conducted research among young school-going

adolescents (13-15 years old), whilst other studies have based their

research on older adolescents.11, 12 Suicide ideation is evident in

both age groups.

The principal aim of this exploratory study was to investigate the

extent of suicide ideation in a sample of secondary school pupils

and to assess their overall psychological adjustment. The specific

study objectives were to: (1) ascertain overall levels of suicide

ideation; (2) assess the prevalence of common psychological

adjustment problems; (3) explore the relationship between suicide

ideation, psychological adjustment and a number of key

background variables including parental closeness; and (4) assess

overall views of mental health service provision.

46

* Sinéad McGilloway,Senior Lecturer and Director Mental Health andSocial Research Unit,Department of Psychology,NUI Maynooth, Maynooth,Co. Kildare, Ireland.E-mail [email protected]

Ciara Brennan,HDip Student and Classroom Assistant,Sandford Parish National School,Ranelagh, Dublin 6, Ireland.

Submitted August 22nd 2008Accepted August 4th 2010

Ir J Psych Med 2012; 29 (1): 46-51

Original Paper

Suicide ideation, psychological adjustment and mental health service support: A screening study in an Irish secondary school sample

Ciara Brennan, Sinéad McGilloway

* Correspondence

Page 46: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Method

Participants and SettingsA purposive sample of 93 participants (46 males and 47 females)

aged 15 to 18 years (Mn=16.73; SD=0.75), was recruited from a

large, co-educational secondary school located in an urban area of

Wexford in the south east of Ireland. Both younger (15-16 years) and

older (17-18 years) age groups were targeted in order that age-

related comparisons could be made. The school principal identified

four classes in which there were prospective participants of a suitable

age and which were deemed to be broadly representative of the

different age groups within the school; each class included

approximately 25 pupils. All of those present at the time of the study

agreed to take part. Whilst we were unable to gauge the exact

number of pupils who were absent at the time of questionnaire

administration, we would estimate, from the final sample size and

the average class size, that the proportion missing was approximately

5%-7%.

MeasuresParticipants completed (anonymously) a questionnaire booklet

containing three self-report questionnaires including: 1) ABackground Information Questionnaire (BIQ); 2) the ReynoldsAdolescents Adjustment Screening Inventory (RAASI); and 3) theSuicide Ideation Questionnaire (SIQ). The BIQ was designedspecifically for purposes of this study to obtain key sociodemographic

and background information, such as experience of suicidal thoughts

(direct questions), parental closeness (Likert scale) and views of

mental health provision (open-ended questions). A number of

questions on the use and abuse of alcohol and drugs were also

included.

The RASSI13 is a 32-item, easy-to-administer and psychometrically

robust measure that is commonly used to screen for psychological

adjustment problems (e.g. antisocial behaviour; anger control

problems; emotional distress) in adolescents aged 12-19 years.

Participants are asked to rate the extent to which they have

experienced a particular thought in the last six months. All items use

a three-point response format ranging from ‘never’ through ‘almost

never’ to ‘nearly all the time’. The frequency of symptoms of adjusted

problems is recorded for each sub-scale. A ‘Total Adjustment’ score

may also be calculated, based on the scores for all four sub-scales

together.

The SIQ-JR,14 (Reynolds, 1987) is a brief, 15-item screening measure

(with good psychometric status) designed to assess thoughts about

suicide amongst adolescents aged 12 to 15 years. Item content

ranges from general thoughts of death and wishes to die, to serious

and more specific thoughts and ideas. Each item is rated on a 7-point

scale (0-6) in order to assess the frequency of occurrence during the

previous month (maximum score 90); higher scores indicate more

numerous, regular suicidal thoughts. Respondents are considered to

be ‘at risk’ if they obtain scores above 31.

Ethical considerationsThis study was conducted in accordance with the Codes of Conduct

of the British Psychological Society and Psychological Society of

Ireland. It was also reviewed internally and discussed in detail with

relevant school staff prior to commencement. Parental consent

forms, devised for purposes of the study, were distributed to each

pupil and parents were asked to provide their written informed

consent on an ‘opt-out’ basis. Participants were also given the

opportunity not to take part in the study, although all agreed to take

part. Whilst participants were of an age that did not require the

questions to be read aloud, the researcher was on hand to answer

any questions and address any concerns. No literacy problems were

reported. All participants also received a self-help information booklet

(e.g. on local mental health services) following questionnaire

completion, and were reminded of the counselling service provided

by the school. In addition, the school principal and the school

counsellor were identified to the pupils as appropriate support

persons. Thes two individuals were alerted to the findings (in writing)

as soon as they became available and indicated their commitment to

provide support to the relevant classes in whom ‘at risk’ pupils were

identified.

Results

Participant profile Most participants were living in rural settings (65%), had parents

who were married (79%) and came from families with three or fewer

siblings (63%). Three quarters consumed alcohol and 48% of males

and 21% of females respectively were classified as ‘binge drinkers’

(i.e. those who consume more than 5 drinks in one setting) (World

Health Organisation, 2004).15 One third confirmed using drugs such

as cannabis, ecstasy, cocaine and ‘speed’, 42% of whom stated that

they did so to relieve stress. Seventy per cent indicated that they had

no trouble in acquiring drugs. Whilst females tended to report higher

levels (M=6.94, SD=2.9) of parental closeness than their male

counterparts (M=5.59, SD=2.5) (t(91)=2.4, p=0.18; η2= .06), this

failed to reach statistical significance.

Suicide Ideation and Experiences with Suicide Approximately one third of participants (32%, 30/93) reported that

they had, at some stage, experienced suicidal thoughts (Figure 1)

(defined as thoughts of death and wanting to die), more than one

quarter of whom (27%, 8/30) met the ‘at risk’ criteria (i.e. scores

above 31) on the SIQ; 67% (20/30) of this group were in the older

age group (17-18 years). We were particularly interested in exploring

any age and sex differences due to previous research that suggests

that these are important concerns in this area.10,16 Participants

experienced suicidal thoughts as young as 10 years, although these

most commonly occurred between the ages of 14 and 16 years

(78%). More young (15-16 year olds) females (44%) than males (8%)

reported being bothered by suicidal thoughts, although this had

levelled off in the older age groups. A series of independent t-tests

showed no significant gender or age group (younger versus older)

differences in SIQ scores (p>0.05).

All students identified as ‘high risk’ (8/93) (as measured by the SIQ)

were currently using drugs and more commonly reported binge

drinking (43%, 13/30) than those without suicidal thoughts (29%,

18/63). Similarly, over half of this group (57%, 17/30) reported

consuming drugs in the past, compared to only 14% (9/63) of those

who reported no suicidal thoughts. Furthermore, 70% of all

participants (64/93) knew someone who had either attempted, or

completed suicide and this group was significantly more likely to

experience suicide ideation than those without such knowledge (χ2=

5.4, df= 93 p=0.02).

47

Page 47: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Figure 1: Proportion of participants (n=93) who had, at somestage, experienced suicidal thoughts

Psychological adjustment Clinically relevant levels of adjustment problems were reported in

one quarter (23/93) of participants and most commonly included

negative self-esteem and emotional distress respectively (Figure 2).

Unsurprisingly, moderate to strong positive correlations were found

between the SIQ and all sub-scales of the RAASI. One third of those

experiencing maladjustment (i.e. high total adjustment scores) also

reported ‘high-risk’ levels of suicide ideation. Parental closeness

was negatively and strongly correlated with both suicide ideation

[r = -.466, n = 92, p<0.000] and total adjustment [r=-.437, n=92,

p<0.000]. None of the male participants showed signs of

emotional distress, although one-quarter (6/24) of the 17-18 year

old females reported clinically significant emotional problems. Poor

anger control and antisocial behaviour respectively were seen in

9%-12% of older males and 5%-9% of younger females. None of

the younger males or older females reported these problems. No

significant age or gender differences were found with respect to

the RAASI total adjustment scores, or each of the sub-scales, with

the exception of emotional distress scores, which were significantly

higher for females (M=9.66, SD=5.1) than for males (M=6.11,

SD=3.8) (t(91)=4.12, p=0.000, eta squared= 0.6).

Figure 2: Levels of clinically significant adjustment problemsin the sample (n=93)

A number of predictor variables were regressed upon suicide

ideation and overall adjustment problems in two standard multiple

regression analyses (Table 1). These variables included drug use,

binge drinking, parental closeness and knowledge of someone

who had completed suicide (age and sex were not included as

these were only weakly correlated with SIQ and total RAASI scores).

In the first analysis, 41% of the variance in SIQ scores (adjusted R2

=.38) was predicted by the model and both drug use and parental

closeness respectively, emerged as significant predictors of suicide

ideation. Thus, the size and direction of the relationships suggested

that those who engage in drug use and have low levels of

perceived parental closeness, were more likely to experience suicide

ideation. Neither binge drinking, nor knowing a suicide victim,

contributed significantly to this model. The second regression

model (Table 1) explained 36% (adjusted R2 =.32) of the variability

in adjustment scores and again, drug use, followed closely by

parental closeness were the most influential variables and were of

a similar size and direction to those indicated above. As in the case

of SIQ scores, neither of the other two independent variables made

a significant unique contribution to the model.

Table 1: Summary of findings from two standard multipleregression analyses of key variables on suicide ideation (SIQ)and total adjustment (RAASI) scores respectively

Service provisionAlmost one third of participants reported that they had received

professional help, primarily for anger management, stress and

family problems. Forty per cent of those who had experienced

suicide ideation (12/30) reported that they had never received any

professional help whilst only one quarter of the ‘high risk’ group

(2/8) were receiving counselling. Forty per cent of the total sample

reported that they would like to receive additional information and

education on mental health and that they needed more support

services to help them during times of stress. Some suggestions

included: the provision of younger counsellors to whom pupils

could more easily relate; stress management classes; advice on

supporting others with mental health difficulties; problem solving;

and regular counselling sessions available to everyone.

Discussion

The principal aim of this study was to obtain a ‘snapshot’ of suicide

ideation in an opportune sample of secondary school students.

However, overall levels of psychological adjustment and

participants’ views of mental health service provision were also

assessed. The findings suggest that a significant proportion of

teenagers struggle with suicide ideation at some point in their lives

and yet, most of those deemed to be currently at risk, do not

appear to be seeking or receiving appropriate professional support.

Nonetheless, it is interesting to note the relatively high proportion

of the total sample overall, who reported that they had received

professional help, primarily for anger management, stress and

family problems. No information was sought on precisely the type

of services used, although it is likely that a proportion of this group

48

50

40

30

20

10

015-16YRS 15-16YRS

Age Group

% of participan

ts

Males

Females

Antisocialbehaviour

AngerControl

EmotionalDistress

NegativeSelf-esteem

TotalAdjustment

% ofparticipants

50

45

40

35

30

25

20

15

10

5

0

B S.E. Beta

SIQ RAASI SIQ RAASI SIQ RAASI

Drug Use 4.5 2.9 3.7 9.63 .442 .378***

Parent closeness 1.77 .43 .54 1.28 .379 .309**

Binge drinking .45 1.3 1.7 .008 .034 .001ns

Know suicide 2.69 2.6 3.3 3.07 .102 .125ns

Victim

Note: SIQ: R2=.41, Adj R2 = .376; RAASI: R2 = .363, Adj R2 = .324***p<0.001; **p<0.01; ns = non-significant

Page 48: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

sought help from the school counsellor, given the relative dearth of

youth mental health services. Similarly, Sullivan and colleagues17

report that almost one in five teenagers who were experiencing

problems, had received professional help.

Previous research conducted in Ireland suggests that the proportion

of young people with suicide ideation has not changed

substantially during the last ten years, despite greater suicide

awareness and ongoing attempts to improve both formal and

informal service provision.2,11,18 For example, an early study by

O’Sullivan and Fitzgerald11 found that suicide ideation and self

harm rates within a sample of 13-14 year-old Dublin schoolchildren

(n=88-101) ranged from 29% to 44% respectively. Another Irish

study,18 conducted with younger adolescents (13-15, n=195) in a

school setting, found that 15% reported suicidal thoughts – much

lower than the one third seen in the current study, which also

included older adolescents. In 2004, Lynch and colleagues2

identified almost 20% of a sample of Irish 12-15 year-olds (n=723)

to be at risk of possible suicide ideation and mental ill health. This

is consistent with Sullivan et al17 who reported that 20% of pupils

showed signs of possible depression. They also found that just over

30% of girls had serious thoughts of harming themselves. Thus,

it is clear that there is considerable variation in suicide ideation/self

harm rates amongst adolescents in Ireland. It is also difficult, on

the basis of available data, to make comparisons with other

countries. For instance, rates in Poland amongst adolescents

appear to compare favourably to Ireland,7 although Italy has lower

levels than reported in either country.10 It is likely, of course, that

such disparity may be due to methodological differences across

studies, as well as variations in sample size and overall quality.

Whilst the above studies focus on younger adolescents, the current

research also included older adolescents, but more large-scale

research with this group is needed, particularly as these young

people are moving into an important transition phase in their lives.

Furthermore, the current study was based in county Wexford, an

area in which there has been a recent spate of family suicides and

which has a suicide rate (13-15/100, 00) that is higher than the

national average;4 consequently, the researchers were interested in

establishing (albeit in an exploratory manner) any possible effects

of this on young people living in the area. The findings reported

here confirm that most of the young people surveyed knew

someone who had attempted, or completed suicide and, in line

with work from elsewhere, this group was more likely to

experience suicidal thoughts and ideas.6, 12, 19 Therefore, it would

appear that the young people living in this area represent a

particularly important target group for mental health professionals

and schools, in terms of developing effective suicide prevention

strategies and appropriate support services, including school-based

initiatives.

However, young people must also be prepared to seek and receive

help and recent qualitative research has shown that young student

males, in particular, may not be willing to seek help for a mental

health problem due to a lack of understanding, stigma and

confidentiality issues.12 This may explain, at least in part, the lower

levels of emotional distress reported in the young males in this

study. It is also possible that there is a lack of openness among

young males to recognise or admit to these feelings. Reassuringly,

the Irish Mental Health Initiative of 200320 highlights the

importance of education and positive mental health promotion in

suicide prevention. One of its aims is to educate adults and young

people on the prevention and recognition of mental illness, to

teach them how to deal with stressful situations and create their

own support network. For instance, the National University of

Ireland, Galway (one of the seven universities in Ireland), introduced

a ‘mind body and soul programme’ in 2002 to promote general

well-being amongst its student population. This approach has since

been used in several other third level institutions and may offer a

useful model for secondary level schools. Similarly, Eckert and

colleagues21 found that implementing curriculum-based

programmes to educate students about mental illness and suicide

was viewed (by American students) as less invasive than school

screening procedures. The Finnish government22 (which is well

known for its innovative mental health service provision), has also

implemented a national strategy which has effectively reduced

suicide deaths across all age groups, by involving local communities

in regional decision making strategies and procedures.

Research has shown that negative family environments contribute

to emotional distress which, in turn, can affect suicide ideation.23,

24 Thus, those who have a supportive family environment are less

likely to report suicidal behaviour.25 Irish society, particularly in rural

areas of the country, is characterised by strong family ties, and

parental closeness was one factor that was examined in this study;

this has not previously been reported in Irish studies. The findings

outlined here suggest, in line with previous cross-cultural research,26, 27, 28 that parental closeness is a strong negative correlate of

suicide ideation, and second only to substance abuse. This, coupled

with drug use, predicted a sizeable proportion of the variance in

both suicide ideation and psychological adjustment scores,

although other factors are clearly at play. This highlights the

potentially critical role of parenting and of strong parent-child

relationships in preventing suicide, but this merits further research,

both nationally and internationally, in order to tease apart the

precise mechanisms involved in this relationship.

The current sample reported high levels of alcohol and drug abuse

similar to those seen in other larger samples,29 particularly in young

people deemed to be at risk. Substance abuse has been identified

as an important risk factor in suicide ideation30, 31 and research

would suggest that the provision of mental health education and

promotion in schools, should incorporate some information on

substance abuse as well as depression, anxiety and some of the

other more common adjustment problems seen in this study.

Interestingly, the one quarter of participants in the current study

who experienced some form of adjustment problem, is marginally

higher than the 20 percent prevalence of mental ill health typically

found amongst young people elsewhere,1,32 although no clinical

diagnoses were used in this study.

Conclusion

This exploratory screening study was limited by a relatively small

sample drawn from a single large, urban-based school and the

results do not include clinical/diagnostic interviews. While the

sample was one of convenience, there is no reason to expect that

the 5th and 6th year classes included in this study were in any way

49

Page 49: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

atypical of young people generally in this age group and indeed

the findings would tend to support this; for instance, the results are

consistent with those found elsewhere with both similar-sized and

larger samples.

Overall, the findings raise some serious concerns about the extent

of suicidal thoughts and ideas in young people whilst also

highlighting, amongst other things, the importance of the parent-

child relationship into the teenage years. The latter is something

about which very little is known. However, it would be useful to

assess regional variations within a larger national longitudinal study

in order to describe and track the overall prevalence of, and

changes in, suicide ideation amongst young people over time and

the extent to which these relate to actual suicide rates.

It is widely acknowledged that young people on the island of

Ireland lack specialist mental health service provision33 whilst

mental health services spending is also well below what is

required.34 Nonetheless, it is imperative that vulnerable, at risk

young people can be identified as early as possible and appropriate

prevention strategies put in place in order that potential deaths

can be avoided into the future. The Irish National Strategy for

Action on Suicide Prevention 2005-201435 advocates the

development of counselling services and crisis response protocols

in all primary and secondary schools throughout Ireland. Likewise,

one of the aims enshrined in the recent Programme for

Government document in Northern Ireland,36 is to establish a

suicide prevention helpline, as well as initiatives aimed at improving

the life and coping skills of all those at risk of suicide.

Importantly, a significant proportion of the young people in the

current study were not satisfied with mental health support and

indicated a high level of unmet need. While some of their

suggestions for improvement in this regard have significant

resource implications (e.g. regular counselling), others could be

more easily implemented in schools (e.g. younger counsellors). It is

also important to note that the Irish school curriculum includes a

Social Personal Health Education (SPHE) module aimed at:

developing self-awareness and personal skills; emotional health;

relationship skills; stress management; and promoting a greater

awareness/knowledge of mental illness. Other elements of the

programme are aimed at helping students to develop the

confidence to protect their mental health and well being, and to

examine the factors that might impact on these. It is difficult to

know to what extent this programme has been effective, but at

the very least, it is providing a forum to discuss the many issues

that affect young people.

The prevention of suicide amongst young people continues to be

controversial due, in large part, to the paucity of population-based

studies and a failure to take account of the multiple risk factors

involved.37 The results of the research were conveyed to the school

that was the focus of this study and in this way, research findings

such as those reported here, may help to develop mental health

promotion initiatives in schools, thereby potentially preventing

deaths from suicide in ‘at risk’ young people into the future.

However, it remains to be seen to what extent the current

downturn in the Irish economy will impact upon the much needed

implementation of school and other community-based mental

health promoting initiatives and support services for our young

people.

Acknowledgements

We would like to thank the school principal and all of the pupils for

participating in this study.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

1. WHO. World Health Organisation, European Ministerial Conference 2005.

Mental health: facing the challenges, building solutions (2005).

2. Lynch F, Mills C, Daly I, Fitzpatrick C. Challenging times: a study to

detect Irish adolescents at risk of psychiatric disorders and suicidal ideation.

J Adolesc 2004; 27(4): 441-451.

3. Central Statistics Office. Vital Statistics Fourth Quarter & Yearly Summary

2006. Released 31 July 2007.

http://www.cso.ie/newsevents/pr_vitalstatsq4andyear2006.htm

4. Barron S, Balanda K, Graham A, Fahy L. Irish Health Poverty Index (interim

release). INIsPHO Data Briefing No.2008:1. Dublin: Institute of Public Health,

2008.

5. Friedman AC. Prevalence and correlates of suicidal ideation and self-

reported attempts in an adolescent community population. Dissertation

Abstracts International Section A: Humanities and Social Sciences 1998; 59

(1): 0322.

6. Leo DD, Cerin E, Spathonis K, Burgis S. Lifetime risk of suicide ideation and

attempts in an Australian community: Prevalence, suicidal process,and help-

seeking behaviour. J Affective Disorders 2005; 86: 215–224.

7. Gmitrowicz A, Szymczak W, Kotlicka-Antczak M, Rabe-Joblonska J.

Suicidal ideation and suicide attempts in Polish adolescents: Is it a suicidal

process? Int J Adolesc and Med Health 2003; 15 (2): 113-124.

8. Miros NJ. Depression, anger, and coping skills as predictors of suicidal ideation

in young adults: Examination of the diathesis-stress-hopelessness theory.

Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering

2000; 61(6): 3286.

9. Wong JPS, Stewart SM, Ho SY, Rao U, Lam TH. Exposure to suicide and

suicidal behaviors among Hong Kong adolescents. Soc Sci & Med 2005; 61:

591–599.

10. O’Sullivan M, Fitzgerald M. Suicidal ideation and acts of self-harm among

Dublin school children. J Adolesc 1998; 21: 427-433.

11. Miotto, P., De Coppi, M., Frezza, M., Petretto, D., Masala, C., & Preti, A.

(Suicidal ideation and aggressiveness in school-aged youths. Psychiatry

Research 2003, 120, 247-255.

12. Burke S, Kerr R, McKeon P. Male secondary school students’ attitudes towards

using mental health services. Ir J Psych Med 2008; 25(2): 52-56.

13. Reynolds WM. Reynolds Adolescent Adjustment Screening Inventory.

Professional Manual Psychological Assessment Resources. Odessa FL:

Psychological Assessment Resources, 2001.

14. Reynolds WM. Suicide Ideation Questionnaire. Odessa, FL: Psychological

Assessment Resources, 1987.

15. World Health Organisation. Global Status on Alcohol – Binge drinking

defined. Geneva: World Health Organisation, 2004.

16. Bensley, L S, Van Eenwyk, J, Spieker, S J, Schoder, J. Self-

reported abuse history and adolescent problem behaviours I: Antisocial and

suicidal behaviours, Journal of Adolescent Health, 1999, 24, 163-172.

17. Sullivan C, Arensman E, Keeley HS, Corcoran P, Perry IP. Young People’s

Mental Health: a report of the Lifestyle and Coping Survey. National Suicide

Research Foundation, 2004.

18. Rowley J, Ganter K, Fitzpatrick C. Suicidal thoughts and acts in Irish

adolescents. Ir J Psych Med 2001; 18(3): 82-86.

19. Fergusson DM, Beaturais AL, Horwood LJ. Vulnerability and resiliency to

suicidal behaviour in young people. Psych Med 2003; 33: 61-73.

50

Page 50: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

20. Kracen, A. The Mental Health Initiative: a resource manual for mental health

promotion and suicide prevention in third level institutions. A partnership

initiative between Trinity College Dublin and the Northern Area Health

Board. Supported by the National Suicide Review Group, the Department of

Health and Children, and the Department of Education and Science, 2003.

21. Eckert TL, Miller DN, Riley-Tillman TC, DuPaul GJ. Adolescent suicide

prevention: Gender differences in students’ perceptions of the acceptability

and intrusiveness of school-based screening programs. J School Psychol 2006;

44: 271–285.

22. Jenkins R, Singh B. Suicide prevention strategies – an international

perspective. Int Rev Psychiatr 2000; 12(1): 7-14.

23. Connor JJ, Rueter MA. Parent–Child Relationships as Systems of Support or

Risk for Adolescent Suicidality. J Family Psych 2006; 20(1): 143–155.

24. Stanley H. Family characteristics that influence and differentiate adolescent

non-single, and multiple suicide attempts. Dissertation Abstracts

International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering 2004; 65(5-B), 2008.

25. Evan DL, Foa EB, Gur RE, Hendin H, O’Brien CP, Seligman MEP, Walsh BT.

Treating and Preventing Adolescent Mental Health Disorders: What we know

and what we don’t know. London: Oxford University Press, 2005.

26. Borowsky IW, Ireland M, Resnick MD. Adolescent suicide attempts: Risks and

protectors. Pediatrics 2001; 107: 485-493.

27. Séguin M, Lynch J, Labelle R, Gagnon A. Personal and family risk factors for

adolescent suicidal ideation and attempts. Arch Suicide Res 2004; 8(3): 227-

238.

28. Bridge JA, Goldstein TR, Brent DA. Adolescent suicide and suicidal behaviour.

J Child Psych and Psychiatry 2006; 47(3): 372–394.

29. McCrystal P, Percy A, Higgins K. Drug use patterns and behaviours of young

people at an increased risk of drug use during adolescence. Int J Drug Policy

2006; 17(5): 393-401.

30. Fombonne E. Suicidal behaviours in vulnerable adolescents. Time trends and

their correlates. Br J Psychiatry 1998; 173: 556–7.

31. Cleary A, Nixon E, Fitzgerald M. Psychological health and well-being among

young Irish adults. Ir J Psych Med 2007; 24(4): 139-144.

32. Department of Health and Children. A Vision for Change. Dublin: Department

of Health and Children, 2006.

33. McGilloway S, Donnelly M, Scott D. Teenagers in adult psychiatric inpatient

care in Northern Ireland. Ir J Psych Med 2000; 17(2): 54-58.

34. O’Shea E, Gannon B, Kennelly B. Eliciting preferences for resource allocation

in mental health care in Ireland, Health Policy 2008;

doi:10.1016/j.healthpol.2008.03.018.

35. Health Service Executive. Reach Out: National Strategy for Action on Suicide

Prevention 2005-2014. Dublin: The Health Service Executive, 2005.

36. Northern Ireland Executive. Building a Better Future – Programme for

Government 2008-2011. Belfast: Northern Ireland Executive, 2008.

37. Donnelly M, McGilloway S. Mental Disorders. In: Yarnell J, Ed. Epidemiology

and Prevention: A Systems-Based Approach. London: Oxford University Press,

2007.

51

Page 51: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Abstract

Objective: To review the role of handwriting analysis in psychiatry.

Method: Case-report and review of key papers.

Results: M, a 27-year-old man, presented with incoherent speech,palilalia, logoclonia, incongruous affect, paranoid delusions and

auditory hallucinations. M was diagnosed with schizophrenia and

cannabis misuse, complicated by speech and language difficulties.

M spent long periods writing on pieces of paper; towards the start

of his admission, his writing was unintelligible but became more

intelligible as his psychosis resolved. M’s handwriting demonstrates

clinical features of psychosis (e.g. clang associations) and

graphological abnormalities associated with schizophrenia in the

literature (rigidity in letter-formation, mechanical expressions, and

tendency toward over-use of straight lines).

Conclusion: Analysis of handwriting is likely to play a limited rolein psychiatric diagnosis but may prove useful in monitoring clinical

improvement in certain patients.

Key words: Psychoses; Cannabis; Psychomotor dysfunction;Review of the literature

Introduction

The study of handwriting as a diagnostic tool has a lengthy history

in psychiatry.1-4 Sulner reflected the position of many in the

psychiatric and legal professions when she wrote that handwriting

was closely related to brain function and was, thus, likely to reflect

disordered psychological functioning and mental illness.2

Consistent with this idea, authors such as Lewinson1 and Privat4

provide detailed analyses of samples of handwriting from

individuals with mental illness.

While this field was the subject of considerable research and clinical

interest in the 1940s, 1950s and 1960s, it has not commanded

similar attention in recent decades. We aimed to review key papers

in this field in light of a specific clinical case in which disturbances

of handwriting were a central feature.

Case Report

M was a 27-year-old man who was brought to the Emergency

Department by the police, having been found setting fires outside

his home. He presented in a confused, dishevelled and unkempt

state. On admission, M’s speech was incoherent and demonstrated

palilalia (repetition of a word with increasing frequency) and

logoclonia (repetition of the last syllable of the last spoken word).

His affect was incongruent with his situation (i.e. in police custody).

M had multiple delusions that famous actors were involved in his

daily life (e.g. making him cups of tea) and that he was being

pursued by unknown, threatening forces. M described auditory

hallucinations in a “deep, deep voice” but was unable to detail

what the voice said. While he was oriented in time, place and

person, M’s insight was limited: he did not agree that he was ill

and was legally detained in hospital as an involuntary patient.

Background history from M’s brother revealed that M and his

brother, who lived together, were frequent users of large amounts

of cannabis and that M had been in this mental state for many

months or possibly years. M’s brother confirmed that M’s delusions

persisted even when M was abstinent from cannabis and had

preceded M’s use of cannabis in the first instance.

M was diagnosed with schizophrenia, according to Diagnostic and

Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (Fourth Edition, Text

Revision)5 criteria, and substance misuse, complicated by significant

difficulties with speech and language. Following admission to

hospital, M was prescribed olanzapine orodispersible tablet (10

miligrams per day, administered orally) and as he became less

agitated it became apparent that he had significant difficulties with

speech and language. Formal assessment by a speech and

language therapist revealed that M had minimal verbal output

which was generally unintelligible; this caused him considerable

frustration. M appeared able to understand the words of others

but was poorly cooperative with more detailed speech and

language assessments.

M’s brother revealed that, prior to admission, M had spent long

periods writing letters and words on small pieces of paper.

Throughout his one-month hospital stay, M continued to write

52

* Brendan D. Kelly,Department of Adult Psychiatry, University College Dublin, Mater Misericordiae University Hospital, 62/63 Eccles Street, Ireland. E-mail [email protected]

Mary Davoran,Senior Registrar and Lecturer in Forensic Psychiatry,Central Mental Hospital, Dundrum, Dublin 14,Ireland.

Eugene Breen,Consultant Psychiatrist, Mater MisericordiaeUniversity Hospital, Eccles Street, Dublin 7, Ireland.

Natalie Sherrard, Liaison Psychiatry Clinical NurseSpecialist, Department of Psychiatry, MaterMisericordiae University Hospital, Eccles Street,Dublin 7, Ireland.

Submitted October 3rd 2008Accepted August 11th 2011

Ir J Psych Med 2012; 29 (1): 52-54

Case Report

Graphology and psychiatric diagnosis: Is the writing on the wall?Mary Davoren, Natalie Sherrard, Eugene Breen, Brendan D. Kelly

* Correspondence

Page 52: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

intensively on sheets of note-paper, the back of cigarette cartons,

etc. Towards the start of his admission, M’s writings were

fragmented and difficult to understand (Figure 1, during first week

of admission) but they became more intelligible as his psychosis

resolved (Figures 2 and 3, during third and fourth weeks of

admission, respectively).

Three weeks after admission, M’s psychotic symptoms had

decreased significantly and, one week later, he was discharged

from hospital. At ten-month follow-up, M remained well in the

community on continued antipsychotic medication, although he

continued to abuse cannabis.

Discussion

M’s diagnoses were schizophrenia and substance misuse,

complicated by significant difficulties with speech and language. It

is not possible to determine if the difficulties with communication

predated his psychotic illness, but it was readily apparent that

improvement in psychotic symptoms was associated with

improvement in communication behaviours, including handwriting.

Samples of M’s handwriting reproduced in this paper demonstrate

both clinical features of psychosis (e.g. clang association, in Figure2) and some of the more specific graphological abnormalitiesdescribed in the literature on mental illness and disturbances of

handwriting.

Sulner, for example, listed a range of “abnormalities” which she

suggested “indicate mental disease or mental or emotional

disturbance”, including repetition of letters or words, omission of

letters or words (or parts of letters or words), transposition of

letters, incorrect spelling, scribbling in the midst of an otherwise

readable text, lack of control of writing, meaningless marks, or

interruptions between letters.2 Many of these “abnormalities” are,

however, common, and Sulner’s list does not appear aimed at

assisting in reaching a clinical diagnosis, but rather achieving a legal

aim: i.e. determining, in a general sense, if an individual was

suffering from any “mental disease or mental or emotional

disturbance”.2

Hilton was somewhat more circumspect about the proposed

relationship between handwriting and mental disorders,

questioning both the sensitivity and specificity of handwriting

abnormalities by posing two key questions: Are such abnormalities

always present in the writing of individuals with mental disorder?

And are such abnormalities specific to mental disorders? 3

It is exceedingly unlikely that any of the anomalies outlined by

Sulner2 are only found in the handwriting of persons with mental

disorder. For example, logoclonus/logoclonia is also found in

Parkinson’s disease and here represents a spastic repetitive

phenomenon, e.g. “I went to the cinema...maa...maaa...maa...”

Against this background, Lewinson1 drew on the work of the

German philosopher and graphologist Ludwig Klages (1872-1956)

to develop a more detailed approach based on the presence of

different constellations of features in the writing of individuals withpsychosis.1,6

Lewinson studied and reproduced handwriting samples from

individuals with schizophrenia, the “paranoid condition” and the

“manic-depressive condition”, and concluded that the handwriting

of individuals with psychosis was especially characterised by

disturbances in all three dimensions of handwriting: height,

breadth and depth.1 Lewinson’s handwriting samples included

twenty individuals with schizophrenia and these tended to

demonstrate “narrowness of letters” (not present in M’s

handwriting), slant “toward the right” (not present in M’s

handwriting), “rigid school-copy forms” (present in Figures 1 to 3),

an “empty-mechanical” expression (present in Figures 1 to 3) and

a “tendency for straight line” (see the letter ‘O’ in Figure 3, which

resembles a square more than a circle).1

Consistent with the presence of schizophrenia and “paranoid

condition” on a single diagnostic continuum, Lewinson described

considerable overlap between the handwriting features of these

two conditions.1 Lewinson reported that handwriting in the

“paranoid condition” was characterised by “lack of connection in

printed writing” (present in Figures 1 to 3), “inhibited, constricted”

character (present in Figures 1 to 3), “alteration between wide and

narrow letters” (not present in M’s handwriting), “wide writing”

(present in Figures 1 to 3) and “irregular slant toward the right”

(not present in M’s handwriting).1

Handwriting of individuals in the “depressive phase” of manic-

depression was characterised by “smallness of the writing” (not

present in M’s handwriting), “lack of rhythm” (not present in M’s

handwriting), “narrowness in letters” (not present in M’s

handwriting) and “vertical to right slanting” (present in Figure 3).1

Handwriting in individuals with “mania” was characterised by

“irregular and increasing” size (not present in M’s handwriting),

“lack of rhythm” (not present in M’s handwriting) and “school

copy forms with additions and ornamentations” (see drawings of

hands in Figure 2, especially at the end of the fifth last line, where

the word “handsome” is presented as a picture of a hand followed

by the letters “sume”).1

Overall, M’s handwriting was most consistent with the

graphological features that Lewinson1 associated with

schizophrenia and the “paranoid condition”, although M’s

handwriting also demonstrated selected features of both the

depressive and elated phases of manic-depression.

Notwithstanding these similarities to Lewinson’s typologies, analysis

of M’s handwriting did not make a significant contribution to the

diagnostic process. The increased intelligibility of his writing as his

psychotic symptoms resolved, however, suggests that changes in

the content and/or form of handwriting may, in certain patients,

assist in monitoring clinical improvement over time. The

longitudinal course and predictive usefulness of this approach has

not yet been studied.

As a result, it remains the case that graphology can, at best, provide

only “supplementary assistance to accepted psychiatric

determinations”.3 This position may change in future years if and

when novel graphological approaches and analytic technologies

improve the sensitivity and specificity of handwriting analysis in the

context of psychiatric diagnosis and practice.

Conflict of interest

None.

53

Page 53: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Figure 1First sample of handwriting (first week of M’s admission), showing

“rigid school-copy forms” and “empty-mechanical” expression,

consistent with schizophrenia; “lack of connection in printed

writing”, “inhibited, constricted” character and “wide writing”,

consistent with Lewison’s “paranoid condition”.1

Figure 2Second sample of M’s handwriting (third week of M’s admission),

showing “rigid school-copy forms” and an “empty-mechanical”

expression, consistent with schizophrenia; “lack of connection in

printed writing”, “inhibited, constricted” character and “wide

writing”, consistent with “paranoid condition”; “school copy

forms with additions and ornamentations” consistent with mania

(see the end of the fifth last line, where the word “handsome” is

presented as a picture of a hand followed by the letters “sume”).1

Figure 3Third sample of handwriting (fourth week of M’s admission),

showing “rigid school-copy forms”, an “empty-mechanical”

expression and a “tendency for straight line” (see the letter ‘O’,

which resembles a square more than a circle), consistent with

schizophrenia; “lack of connection in printed writing”, “inhibited,

constricted” character and “wide writing”, consistent with

“paranoid condition”; and “vertical to right slanting”, consistent

with the “depressive phase” of manic-depression.1

References

1. Lewinson TS. Dynamic disturbances in the handwriting of

psychotics. Am J Psychiatry 1940; 97: 102-135.

2. Sulner HF. Mental disorders: their effects upon handwriting. Am

Bar Assoc J 1959; 45: 931-934.

3. Hilton O. Handwriting and the mentally ill. J Forensic Sci 1962;

7: 131-139.

4. Privat S. Contribution de la graphologie au diagnostic et au

pronostic des psychoses. Gaz Med Fr 1965; 72: 2501-2523.

5. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical

Manual of Mental Disorders (Fourth Edition, Text Revision).

Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association, 2000.

6. Lewinson TS. An introduction to the graphology of Ludwig

Klages. Character and Personality 1938; 6: 163-176.

54

Page 54: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Introduction

One of the most important questions facing psychiatry today

concerns its relationship to the emerging international service user

movement. I believe that this movement presents not only the

greatest challenge to psychiatry, but also the greatest opportunity.

As it becomes more organised and influential this movement is

starting to play a major role in shaping the sort of questions that

are being asked about mental health services and their priorities.

Yet there is limited reflection in our profession about how we, as

doctors, might engage positively with it. It seems that while we are

comfortable working with individuals and organisations who

accept the medical framing of mental problems, we are less willing

to contemplate working with critical service users. These are people

who reject the medical model because they feel harmed by a

system that describes their problems using the language of

psychopathology. If we are serious about having an inclusive

debate on mental health we will have to overcome this impasse.

We need to entertain the idea that people who reject the medical

framing of their problems are nevertheless legitimate stakeholders.

It is time that we learned how to talk to them and to listen to their

ideas. The user movement, with its substantial critical component,

is not going to go away.

One of the most important elements of the relationship between

psychiatric services and the people who use them is the reality of

psychiatric power. Many critical service users accept that legally

sanctioned interventions may be necessary when individuals lose

capacity to care for themselves and perhaps put themselves or

others at risk. However they do not accept the fact that the Mental

Health Act in Ireland gives sole authority to doctors to take

decisions on such interventions without any obligation to consult

other interested parties.

Under the terms of the 2001 Act in Ireland, while an application for

involuntary detention may be made by a relative or other named

individual and a GP is required to support this with a

recommendation order, once a patient is detained all power is put

into the hands of the consultant psychiatrist. As a result the Act

effectively privileges the voice of psychiatry. When an individual is

detained, the psychiatrist is endowed with the authority to

determine the nature of his or her problems and the vocabulary

that will be used to describe them. Moreover, the psychiatrist has

the power to determine what treatment will be used, how it will be

used and for how long. It is also within the power of the

psychiatrist to decide what side-effects of medication will be taken

into account and what risks to the patient’s health will be tolerated

and to order ECT for a patient even if the patient and his/her family

refuse it. The patient is seen for a second opinion shortly after

admission, but this is also carried out by a psychiatrist. The three-

person tribunal team that reviews the admission order always

includes a psychiatrist.

There is evidence that many people who have undergone

involuntary admission and treatment continue to feel hurt and even

violated by this process.1 Stefan Priebe and his colleagues reported

on a study in which they were able to follow up more than half of

a cohort of patients who were detained under the Mental Health

Act in England.2 After one year they asked these patients if their

involuntary detention was justified. Only 40% said that it was. The

authors comment that ‘this percentage might have been even

smaller if all patients had been re-interviewed’. This would indicate

that a majority of patients are currently unhappy with the process

of involuntary detention.

A review of the 2001 Irish Mental Health Act will be taking place

shortly. In my opinion, this presents us with an opportunity to

engage creatively with the user movement in all its diversity,

something that many psychiatrists genuinely welcome. However, in

order to do so we will have to reflect critically on the ‘micro-politics’

of our current clinical encounters. In this paper I will argue that the

decision-making powers that are currently given to psychiatry

cannot be justified on either scientific or moral grounds. I will go

on to argue that shedding these powers (and subsequent

responsibilities) would be a positive move for our profession.

Justifying Psychiatric Power

It is usual to justify psychiatric power by asserting that we have an

expertise about mental illness that allows us to diagnose accurately,

classify logically and treat efficiently. The assumption is that no

other group in society has such knowledge and therefore can be

trusted to make decisions about people who are mentally

deranged for one reason or another.

The logic for psychiatric power would appear to be:

• States of madness and distress are the result of episodes of

mental illness.

• Mental illness can be fully grasped in the logic of medicine and

thus doctors are uniquely positioned to explain and to predict

the outcome of such episodes.

• Psychiatry possesses a range of treatments that are un-

controversially of benefit to its patients.

• It is therefore justified to give psychiatrists the power to both

detain and to treat patients without their consent.

The assertion here is that psychiatric science leads to psychiatricpower.

55

Pat Bracken, Consultant Psychiatrist and Clinical Director, Centre for Mental Health Care and Recovery, Bantry General Hospital, Bantry, Co Cork, Ireland.E-Mail [email protected]

Submitted October 20th 2010 Accepted January 13th 2012

Ir J Psych Med 2012; 29 (1): 55-58

Opinion

Psychiatric power: A personal viewPat Bracken

Page 55: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Thomas Szasz has challenged this position for over half a century.3

He argues that it is actually the other way round. In his opinion the

history of psychiatry is very different from the history of medicine.

Western medicine traces its origins back to the Greeks, and has

always been primarily concerned with the suffering caused by

diseased organs, and the various interventions that can be made to

ease or cure this suffering. Until the 19th century most mental and

emotional problems were not treated by physicians. Disturbances

of thinking and emotion were understood mainly as spiritual or

moral problems. When people were detained on account of such

disturbances, this was the domain of clerics rather than doctors.

In the 19th century a new sort of physician was born: the ‘mad-

doctor’, the ‘alienist’, and in the end, the psychiatrist. What

characterised all these doctors was not their knowledge but the

location of their work. Psychiatrists were simply doctors who

worked in asylums. There is general agreement amongst historians

that the enormous asylums were not the creation of the medical

profession, less still of psychiatry.

As Roy Porter says: ‘It would be wrong to regard this drive over the

last three centuries towards institutionalizing insanity

fundamentally as the brainchild of “psychiatry”. In the first instance

the sequestration of ‘lunatics’ was primarily an expression of civil

policy: rather an initiative from magistrates, philanthropists and

families than the achievement (for good or ill) of the doctors.

Indeed, the rise of psychological medicine was more the

consequence than the cause of the rise of the insane asylum.

Psychiatry could flourish once, but not before, large numbers of

inmates were crowded into asylums’.4

Szasz argues that ‘as the clergyman’s power diminished, the mad-

doctor’s increased, and theological coercion was replaced by

psychiatric coercion’.3 Furthermore, according to Porter, the

emergence of a psychiatric science only took place once individuals

had already been separated from society and brought together in

the asylums of the early 19th century. In other words, according to

Szasz and historians like Roy Porter, psychiatric power led topsychiatric science.

This analysis has major implications for the situation in which we

find ourselves today. Historically, there was no real science of

psychiatry that could justify the power that was given to the

profession. In the early 19th century, there were disparate,

contradictory theories of madness; there were no clear

classification systems and the treatments used were often akin to

torture. Porter gives us an account of Johann Christian Reil’s

approach to treatment (Reil is credited with being the first person

to coin the term ‘psychiatry’). Reil, says Porter:

‘proposed an idiosyncratic variant on moral treatment: the

charismatic alienist would master the delinquent mind; a staff

trained in play-acting would further the alienist’s efforts to break

the patient’s fixed ideas – and all would be combined with salutary

doses of therapeutic terror (sealing-wax dropped onto the palms,

immersion in a tub of eels, etc.)’.5

Psychiatry did not have any answers. In fact, the one regime that

genuinely seemed to possess therapeutic efficacy, the moral

treatment practiced at the Quaker York Retreat, was devised by

the Tuke family who were tea merchants, not doctors. Yet,

psychiatry was invested with the power to detain, to explain and

to treat.

In our own era, the powers of psychiatry have been refined but

not lessened. The question still faces us: is the science of psychiatric

treatment robust enough to justify psychiatric power?

In their defense of involuntary commitment, in The Reality ofMental Illness, Martin Roth and Jerome Kroll make an importantpoint:

‘Since most people agree that civil commitment, i.e. involuntary

incarceration of a person who has not committed a crime, and

involuntary treatment (based upon what others believe is best for

a person) represent massive [their emphasis] infringements of thatperson’s civil liberties and personal integrity, it follows that the

factual basis and the ethico-legal justification for such a course

must be suitably strong and unambiguous’.6

According to Roth and Kroll, if psychiatrists are ever going to be

able to justify their role in such ‘massive infringements’ of civil

liberty we will need to be very confident that our science is

disinterested and robust and that our treatments are transparently

effective. We will need to be confident that we can predict

outcomes, and happy that we understand how our treatments

work and for whom. Furthermore, we will need to be very clear

that the benefit of these treatments completely outweighs their

negative effects. Remember, no other branch of medicine has the

power that psychiatry possesses. We will need to be at least as

confident as the rest of medicine about the veracity of our science

if we are to justify this power. Do we have such a science? Let us

briefly look at a couple of issues.

Is the science of psychiatric treatment ‘strong andunambiguous’?

While mainstream psychiatry still holds to the idea that

antidepressants work through their pharmacological effects on

specific neurotransmitters in the CNS, critical psychiatrists like

Joanna Moncrieff and David Healy have been pointing out for

many years that most of the therapeutic effect of anti-depressants

can be explained by the placebo effect. In the past two years the

evidence for this has become indisputable. Two major meta-

analyses of US Federal Drug Administration (FDA) data by Turner et

al7 and Kirsch et al8 concluded that over 80% of the improvement

seen in the drug groups was duplicated in the placebo groups.

Irving Kirsch published a book based upon this research in 2009.

He concludes:

‘The belief that antidepressants can cure depression chemically is

simply wrong’.9

The placebo phenomenon is not peculiar to psychiatry.10 However,

a great deal of theoretical reflection in psychiatry, such as the

monoamine theory of affective disorders, has been based upon the

56

Page 56: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

supposed biological effects of these drugs. Thus these recent meta-

analyses represent not only a challenge to our prescribing but also

question the foundations of contemporary psychiatric science itself.

Rather than embrace the therapeutic reality and the theoretical

implications of the placebo phenomenon and explore ways of

incorporating these positively and transparently, a number of

prominent psychiatrists have sought to justify current prescribing

practices by dismissing the results of these meta-analyses. Some of

them have done so by arguing that we should abandon the

evidence-based medicine (EBM) approach. They say something like:

‘actually science isn’t that important in debates about

psychopharmacology, what is really important is the experience of

the doctor’. So we hear one prominent psychopharmacologist,

MacAllister Williams, insisting that:

‘it matters little whether the patient responds due to the placebo

effect or the specific pharmacological actions of the drug, as long

as they get better’.11

But the whole logic of EBM, of double blind controlled trials, is to

identify what part of therapeutic improvement is due to the

placebo response. MacAllister Williams is telling us that this is not

important: we know how to get our patients better with

antidepressants and that is all that matters.

The psychiatric discourse around antidepressants is far from the

‘strong and unambiguous science’ demanded by Roth and Kroll.

Is the science of anti-psychotics any more robust? The development

of second generation antipsychotics was heralded as one of the

great achievements of modern psychopharmacology. For many

years after their introduction, psychiatrists told patients and

relatives how safe and effective these drugs were. There was talk

about a ‘breakthrough’ in the treatment of schizophrenia. Several

years on, and the picture does not look so rosy. We now know that

these drugs are possibly more toxic than the earlier ones and there

is no evidence that they are more effective. Furthermore, in a major

paper in the Lancet last year, evidence was presented to show thateven the notion that these drugs represented a significantly

different grouping was false.12 In an editorial comment on thispaper, Peter Tyrer and Tim Kendall wrote:

‘The spurious invention of the atypicals can now be regarded as

invention only, cleverly manipulated by the drug industry for

marketing purposes and only now being exposed. But how is it

that for nearly two decades we have, as some have put it, “been

beguiled” into thinking they were superior?’.13

Antidepressants and antipsychotics are cornerstones of

psychopharmacology. These are the drugs that are administered to

patients when they are detained. Psychopharmacology is the usual

form of treatment given to patients on an involuntary basis.

My argument is that the science we have available to us now, with

its explanations and treatments, is simply not of the standard set by

Roth and Kroll to justify the power that psychiatrists have been

given.

Conclusion

As in most other countries, the Mental Health Act in Ireland puts

a great deal of power and responsibility into the hands of

psychiatrists. Advance directives, advocates, carers, other

professionals and other interested parties play little if any part in

the decision-making process regarding detention and treatment.

In other words, the current legal framework governing these

interventions endorses the singular authority of psychiatric science.

In this paper, I have argued that psychiatric power did not develop

logically from the explanatory and therapeutic abilities of

psychiatric science in the 19th century. Indeed, historians suggest

that this science was given energy and direction from the priorincarceration of thousands of patients across the western world.

Furthermore, I have argued that, in our own time, we simply do not

possess the sort of medical science with explanatory, predictive and

therapeutic powers that might justify the legal authority invested

in us. Our knowledge does not pass the test set by Roth and Kroll.

Indeed, I believe that because psychiatry is tasked to deal

specifically with problems of beliefs, feelings, behaviours and

relationships, its knowledge has to be qualitatively different to that

upon which a medicine of the tissues is built.14

But even if we did have such a knowledge, the current privileged

position given to psychiatry in the Mental Health Act directly

contradicts the fundamental ethic of the ‘recovery approach’ to

mental health which is promoted by ‘A Vision for Change’ and bythe Mental Health Commission. One of the Commission’s most

recent documents contains the statement: ‘the recovery approach

challenges the privileging of one theoretical perspective as the

primary explanation for and the treatment of mental distress and

the privileging of professional interpretations and expertise over

expertise by experience and personal meaning. The biomedical

model and medical treatments may have an important place for

some people in the recovery process, but as an invited guest, rather

than the overarching paradigm’.15 A key element of the recovery

approach is the promotion of ‘empowerment’.16 This includes the

power to define the nature of one’s problems and to be involved

in decisions about treatment.

Some psychiatrists have yet to embrace the ‘recovery approach’

and continue to think and act as though the psychopathological

framework was the only legitimate and valid way of understanding

states of madness and distress.17 But many are now responding to

the calls of service users to create a different sort of psychiatry. A

key element of any mental health service involves the management

of risk. But there is clearly room to rethink how we do this. The

EUNOMIA study has demonstrated ‘huge variation’ in the rates of

involuntary admissions across Europe.18 Differences in ‘socio-

cultural contexts’ appear to be responsible for this variation rather

than rates of mental illness. I am not arguing that there should be

no mechanisms in place to intervene when individuals are mentally

disturbed. I am not denying that medical practitioners have an

important role to play. However, I am suggesting that the current

powers and responsibilities of psychiatry are not warranted on

either empirical or philosophical grounds.

57

Page 57: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

In their recent qualitative study of the impact of coercive

interventions, Sibitz et al19 found that while some service users,

who had received such interventions in the past, felt that

involuntary treatment was sometimes needed; many felt that their

crises could have been managed differently. They found that ‘the

ways that problems were formulated by mental health staff as

psychiatric issues were sometimes contrary to the ways patients

saw their problems and what was needed to solve them’. As a

result, participants in this study reported that the experience of

involuntary treatment had left them with a ‘general distrust of

others, particularly of medical professionals’. They reported living

their lives as if ‘on probation’ in the aftermath.

This has implications for the personal relationships between

individual psychiatrists and their patients but also for the profession

and the society it serves. At present, psychiatry continues to be

feared. In spite of all the anti-stigma campaigns, as long as the

profession is bestowed with powers to incarcerate and to treat on

an involuntary basis, this fear will continue. The forthcoming review

of the Mental Health Act provides an opportunity for psychiatrists

to shed some of these powers and to engage with service-users in

a positive debate about how and when force should be used in

mental health crises.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

1. Katsakou C, Priebe S. Patients’ experiences of involuntary hospital admission

and treatment: a review of qualitative studies. Epidemiol Psichiatr Soc 2007;

16: 172-8.

2. Priebe S, Amos T, Leese M et al. Patients’ views and readmissions 1 year after

involuntary hospitalisation. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 94: 49-54.

3. Szasz T. The medicalization of everyday life. New York: Syracuse University

Press; 2007.

4. Porter R. A social history of madness: Stories of the insane. London:

Weidenfeld and Nicolson, 1987.

5. Porter R. Madness: A brief history. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2002.

6. Roth M, Kroll J. The reality of mental illness. Cambridge: Cambridge University

Press; 1986

7. Turner EH, Matthews AM, Linardatos E et al. Selective publication of

antidepressant trials and its influence on apparent efficacy. NEJM 2008; 358:

252-60.

8. Kirsch I, Deacon BJ, Huedo-Medina TB et al. Initial severity and antidepressant

benefits: a meta-analysis of data submitted to the Food and Drug

Administration. PLoS Med 2008; 5: e45.

9. Kirsch I. The Emperor’s new drugs. Exploding the antidepressant myth.

London: The Bodley Head; 2009.

10. Moerman D. Meaning, medicine and the ‘placebo effect’. Cambridge:

Cambridge University Press; 2002.

11. McAllister-Williams RH. Do antidepressants work? A commentary on “Initial

severity and antidepressant benefits: a meta-analysis of data submitted to

the Food and Drug Administration” by Kirsch et al. Evidence Based Mental

Health 2008; 11: 66-68.

12. Leucht S, Corves C, Arbter D et al. Second-generation versus first-generation

antipsychotic drugs for schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Lancet, 2009; 373:

31-34.

13. Tyrer P, Kendall T. The spurious advance of antipsychotic drug therapy. Lancet

2009; 373: 4-5.

14. Bracken P. Thomas, P. Postpsychiatry: mental health in the postmodern world.

Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2005.

15. Mental Health Commission. A recovery approach within the Irish mental

health services. A framework for development. Dublin: MHC; 2008.

16. Warner R. Does the scientific evidence support the recovery model? The

Psychiatrist 2010; 34: 3-5.

17.Goss C, Moretti F, Mazzi MA, Del Piccolo L, Rimondini M, Zimmermann C.

Involving patients in decisions during psychiatric consultations. Br J Psych

2008; 193: 416-421.

18. Fiorillo A, De Rosa C, Del Vecchio V et al. How to improve clinical practice on

involuntary hospital admissions of psychiatric patients: suggestions from the

EUNOMIA study. Eur Psychiatry 2011; 26(4): 201-207.

19. Sibitz I, Scheutz A, Lakeman R, Schrank B, Schaffer M, Amering M. Impact of

coercive measures on life stories: qualitative study. Br J Psych 2011; 199:

239-244.

58

Page 58: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

If the shelves of my local bookshop are anything to go by, the self-

help genre has never been more popular. Everything from career,

to love life, to weight, to self esteem is seemingly malleable with

the help of a little paperback tome filled with wisdom. But there

is a catch – not all self help is of the same quality and faithful

readers do not always know the difference. For clinicians, it can be

equally difficult to know what reading materials to recommend to

patients and relatives with the aim of expediting recovery.

Consequently, it is always a relief when a reputable professional

with a proven track record decides to update material that has

already proved enormously successful. One such example is Dr

Tony Bates, the founding Director of Headstrong (the National

Centre for Youth Mental Health in Ireland), who has many years

additional experience as a Principal Clinical Psychologist at St

James’s Hospital, Dublin. His original self-help book, entitled

Depression: A Common Sense Approach (1999), was so wellreceived it sold some thirty thousand copies. His updated version

could leap off the shelves just as quickly, particularly as it

incorporates mindfulness, a technique that has become

increasingly popular over the past decade.

Coming through Depression: A Mindful Approach to Recovery iswritten in clear and concise English. This is worth mentioning first,

lest we forget that, in the context of mental illness, not all of its

potential readers will have optimum concentration levels. This

book is practical, however. Dr Bates utilises vignettes and case

histories to which the reader can easily relate, while diagrams and

tables illustrate concepts such as the manner in which thoughts,

feelings, physical symptoms and behaviours interact. The book is

divided into three parts, with Part One providing a simple

explanation of what it might feel like to experience depression,

along with common signs and symptoms of the illness and a

differentiation between mild and severe depression. There are

sections on childhood and postnatal depression, and a checklist

that the reader can use to assess their own depressive symptoms.

Dr Bates also explores the causes of depression, from childhood

experiences through to genetic theories.

In Part Two, Dr Bates examines approaches that can aid recovery,

including daily structure, mood tracking, lifestyle changes and

appropriate help-seeking. The author explains in detail how to set

up a mood diary, an activity log and a recovery journal, while this

section also includes both a chapter on building self-esteem, and

a chapter dedicated to carers.

Finally, in Part Three, Dr Bates explores the concept and practice of

mindfulness. Much of what he writes is based on the work of

three pioneers of the area, Thich Nhat Hanh, Jon Kabat-Zinn and

Mark Williams, all of whom were one-time teachers of Dr Bates.

According to the author, “Mindfulness means resting in the present

moment, with full awareness. When we are mindful, we bring

attention back to whatever is happening in our lives”. This, he

suggests, allows us to enjoy the simple things in daily life we might

otherwise overlook such as “the smile on a child’s face” or “the

sensation of the breeze against our skin”. Mindfulness, Dr Bates

says, “gives us a gentle way to respond to distress” in which we

“learn to notice and to accept difficulties as they emerge, rather

than trying to push them away and suppress them”. The book

benefits in particular from the inclusion of a mindfulness CD,

allowing the theory explained in the book to be practiced with

relative ease.

Coming through Depression: A Mindful Approach to Recovery is anarticulate, sensitive and practical guide to recovery from depression

that at no point encroaches upon other components of the

multidisciplinary recovery plan. Perhaps, indeed, it provides the ray

of hope individuals need when finding themselves trapped in the

dark chamber of depression. Well worth recommending to

patients and relatives alike.

Stephen McWilliams,Consultant Psychiatrist,St John of God Hospital,Stillorgan, Co. Dublin, Ireland.E-mail [email protected]

59

Book review

Coming through Depression: A Mindful Approach to Recovery Tony Bates Gill & Macmillan: 2011 (160pp). ISBN: 978-071714780-9

Page 59: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Leadership is a complicated concept. Leadership can be difficult to

define in words, yet most people can agree when it is present -

and virtually all can agree when it is absent. While many books

have been written about leadership, there are few that stand out

as providing the in-depth insight necessary truly to understand the

qualities of a good leader.

In “Leadership With Consciousness”, Tony Humphreys strides

fearlessly into many of the great debates about leadership today,

and comes up with an admirably clear-sighted set of

recommendations about “training for raising consciousness”,

amongst other measures. Dr Humphreys is well-placed to

undertake such an endeavour: he a consultant clinical psychologist,

national and international speaker, and accomplished author. To

date, Dr Humphreys has written fourteen best-selling books on a

range of themes, including the intriguing “Power of Negative

Thinking” (Newleaf, 2004).

“Leadership With Consciousness” will undoubtedly join the ranks

of Dr Humphrey’s other successful books: it is clear, incisive and

timely. Dr Humphreys argues that various economic processes,

such as those which contributed to the economic crisis of 2008

onward, are enmeshed with defensive emotional processes at

individual level. At least some of these processes are unconscious

but powerful, and their potential effects on leadership, decision-

making and other matters are explored throughout the book.

Placing these arguments in a strongly contemporary framework,

Dr Humphreys draws much-needed attention to individual

psychological processes which underpin society in general and the

financial “system” in particular – a system which is so often blamed

for recent economic and social turmoil. This system does not, of

course, appear out of a vacuum, and Dr Humphreys examines the

role of undetected individual emotional processes in producing and

shaping it. In essence, Dr Humphreys argues that consciousness of

the need to resolve unconscious defences is critical for effective

leadership. The final chapter, “Training for Consciousness”,

focuses on specific steps necessary for this task, and provides a list

of qualities for “leadership with consciousness”.

Quite apart from these conclusions, there is much else that is of

interest along the way. There is, for example, an involving passage

about “the place of dignity and compassion in the medical care of

people”. This discussion provides a useful focus on compassion at

individual level in the delivery of patient care. This is clearly an

essential element in health services, but I would have liked to see

greater exploration of the some of the less-discussed complexities

of compassion: How, for example, does a health-planner

incorporate compassion into the decision-making process when

there are difficult choices to be made about the distribution of

scarce resources? Evaluating legitimate but competing claims on

one’s compassion is difficult but necessary: sometimes, hard-nosed

decision-making may be needed in order to be compassionate.

“Leadership With Consciousness” is good but not perfect. The

book would, for example, have benefitted from more extensive

referencing: there are many points at which the author makes quite

specific statements or provides quotations, and it would be helpful

if there were more pointers to sources. In terms of navigating the

volume itself, it would have been helpful if the contents page

included page numbers for the chapter sub-sections listed, and not

just for the start of each chapter.

To counter-balance these deficits, this book’s index is truly a rare

joy: extensive, accurate and sensibly arranged. A book like this is

not only to be read through from start to finish, but will also be

consulted from time to time for reference and study. A good index

assists greatly with these tasks.

Overall, this is a good, if imperfect, book. Dr. Humphrey’s emphasis

on the effects of individual psychological processes on leadership

and decision-making is greatly to be welcomed. His suggestions on

“training for consciousness” are also likely to prove thought-

provoking for at least some readers.

This book might, perhaps, be best approached in conjunction with

two other volumes both of which provide additional, if contrasting,

perspectives on leadership. Readers with interests in psychiatric

and psychological perspectives on political leadership will

undoubtedly enjoy the classic “Psychological Assessment of

Political Leaders” (University of Michigan Press, 2005) by Jerrold

M. Post, Professor of Psychiatry, Political Psychology, and

International Affairs at George Washington University, and founder

of the CIA's intriguing Center for the Analysis of Personality and

Political Behavior. Professor Post’s penetrating volume is both a joy

to read and a fascinating window into the analysis of political

leaders from a psychiatric perspective.

60

Book review

Leadership With ConsciousnessTony Humphreys Atrium/Attic Press/Cork University Press: Cork, 2011 (202pp).

ISBN 978-1-85594-218-9

Page 60: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

This reading would be well complemented by another classic of

the leadership literature: “The Prince” (1532), by Niccolò

Machiavelli (1469-1527). Despite the ubiquitous use of the term

“Machiavellian” in a rather pejorative fashion, there is much in

“The Prince” that is informative and interesting, and the work as

a whole is more subtle that it is often given credit for, especially in

its recommendations for leaders. The literature on leadership is

full of such books: intelligent, insightful and very often

misunderstood.

Tony Humphrey’s “Leadership With Consciousness” is a useful

addition to the leadership literature. It is clearly written, admirably

focussed, and provides welcome insight into the role of individual

psychological processes in producing the social and economic

predicaments in which much of Europe finds itself. The solutions

to these predicaments may be complex, but more “leadership with

consciousness”, as suggested by Dr Humphreys, would certainly

be a good start.

Brendan D. Kelly, Consultant Psychiatrist and Senior Lecturer in Psychiatry,

Department of Adult Psychiatry,

University College Dublin,

Mater Misericordiae University Hospital,

62/63 Eccles Street,

Dublin 7,

Ireland.

E-mail [email protected]

61

Page 61: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

The wonderfully named French physician Louis-Alexandre-

Hippolyte Leroy-Dupré wrote that acute homesickness “becomes

more rare each day thanks to rapid communications which modern

industry is beginning to establish among people who will soon be

nothing more than one big happy family.” One might imagine that

this observation was written for the age of Facebook, Skype and

Twitter, but it is fact over one hundred and fifty years old, dating

from 1846.

Susan J Matt is a historian at Weber State University in Utah; her

specialty is the history of the emotions (a previous book is entitled

“Keeping Up With The Joneses: Envy in American Consumer

Society 1890-1930”) This admirably lucid book, based on primary

sources such as diaries, letters and personal interviews, is an

overview of the history of a particular emotion, homesickness.

American society is famously built on the archetype of the pioneer,

the rugged individualist, cheerfully moving on from place to place

without demur. This archetype finds different forms; the

immigrant, the cowboy, the “Organisation Man”, the pilgrim

settler, but all have in common a sense of perpetual motion and

freedom from ties.

As with all archetypes and grand narratives, the details of reality

were very different. Very many pioneers and immigrants returned,

despite the social pressures to remain. Matt places centre stage

the men and women who actually lived these experiences, and

who were often beset by overwhelming homesickness. This was

especially so for women, less in control of their destiny than men.

From the first settlers on, thoughts of home contended with the

various religious, political and economic motives for perpetual

motion. While official rhetoric emphasised the importance of

forging on with the pioneer spirit, diaries and letters allow Matt to

reconstruct the emotional lives often lost to history.

In 1865, twenty –four Union soldiers officially died of nostalgia.

Among the American forces in World War 1, only one casualty had

a cause of death listed as nostalgia. Matt records the varying

opinions of psychiatrists, alienists on physicians on the causes and

management of nostalgia-as-an-illness. Contemporary concerns

such as racial and ethnic purity (“weaker” ethnicities such as the

Irish and Southern Europeans were often held to be more

susceptible) and venereal disease were implicated as risk factors

for nostalgia cases.

Over the later nineteenth century and into the twentieth, public

attitudes to homesickness hardened. Once, children who crossed

thousands of miles to return from boarding schools to families

were celebrated. Their attachment to home was seen as evidence

of a tender sensibility. How homesickness was addressed by the

military in the various wars in the era Matt’s history covers is

revealing. Armies have to balance the motivating power of

attachment to country with the demotivating power of separation

from that same country. In the American Civil War, homesickness

among soldiers was seen as evidence of a nobility of nature. This

attitude persisted through the century. The sole nostalgia fatality of

the Spanish-American War of 1898 was treated with great

sympathy bordering on glorification by the contemporary media.

The inter-war years saw the cultural shift gain momentum. This

was the era where the child rearing “expert” began to opine in

the popular press; no less a figure than the seminal behaviourist

John Watson weighs in on the importance of avoiding excessive

affection with one’s children. The following fifty years saw the

denigration of homesickness gain pace. Where the home-loving

children of previous eras were celebrated, now over attachment to

parents and to home was seen as “sissifying” and a manifestation

of “Momism.” An ethic of universal cheerfulness which celebrated

the “can-do” spirit further cast homesickness into disrepute. The

interests of corporate America were in creating a mobile workforce,

ready to cross the continent at short notice. While this is not a

matter that Matt discusses, this aspect did get me thinking how

the anti-family jeremiads of R D Laing and David Cooper ironically

dovetailed neatly with this corporate imperative. Perhaps, as the

Marxists say, there are no accidents.

Anti-homesickness rhetoric persists today, although the picture is

complicated by the rise of technologies which allow instantaneous

communication, and the global availability of familiar brands. Yet

these developments are palliatives for homesickness, not cures.

Skype, Facebook and similar technologies allow a certain abolition

of distance, and Matt shows how they have perhaps helped in the

rehabilitation of homesickness as a valid public emotion. Indeed,

one of her themes is “the surprising persistence of the extended

family” and how emotions and their expression can be moulded

and shaped by social forces, but are also strangely resistant to them

Indeed, this is a history of the resilience of homesickness, despite

everything. So many approaches in contemporary humanities

emphasise the contingent and socially constructed nature of

things; what Matt manages to do is to acknowledge the role of

social and economic pressures while making a strong case that

emotions are less fungible than theorists, pundits and social

engineers of all political hues would believe. There is also very little

of the jargon and theoretical ballast which many contemporary

historians freight their work

Matt’s title clearly indicates that this is an American history of

homesickness, but the book is of great interest to an Irish

62

Book review

Homesickness: An American HistorySusan J Matt. Oxford University Press 2011, (344 pp). ISBN 978-0-19-537185-7

Page 62: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

readership too. The Irish immigrant experience abroad is of course

familiar to most of us; a sizable chunk of Irish popular music is

eloquent testimony to the force of homesickness. More

fundamentally, homesickness is a universal emotion; all readers will

find someone to identify with among the lives Matt describes. We

may not always go through the same social transformations as

America at the same time, but we always seem to get round to

them sooner or later. In our age of ghost estates and resurgent

emigration, many of the concerns of the book seem all too

relevant.

Academic careers rival medical careers in demanding frequent

moves (and in requiring a certain insouciance as the proper

response.) In her acknowledgements, Matt salutes her husband

and observes “since we met in Ithaca, New York, in 1990, we have

lived in six different states and travelled many places, but no matter

where we are, when I am with him, I am home.” It is a poignant

note, and one which sets the tone for a humane and thought-

provoking work.

Séamus MacSuibhne,Consultant Psychiatrist,

St Lukes Hospital,

Co. Kilkenny, Ireland.

Visiting Fellow,

School of Medicine,

University College Cork,

Co. Cork, Ireland.

E-mail [email protected]

63

Hugh Freeman, editor of the British Journal of Psychiatry from 1983to 1993, died recently at the age of 81. A native of Salford in

Greater Manchester, Hugh attended Oxford University on a

scholarship and subsequently joined the Royal Army Medical

Corps. He went on to train in psychiatry at the Maudsley and

became a consultant psychiatrist in Salford. He was an early

advocate of community psychiatry, with retention of inpatient

facilities in general hospitals.

Hugh may best be remembered as an historian of his specialty. He

co-edited 150 Years of British Psychiatry, published in 1991 tocommemorate a century and a half of the Royal College of

Psychiatrists, and edited 1999’s A Century of Psychiatry. As well ashis decade at the helm of the British Journal, he was an editor of

History of Psychiatry and founding editor of Current Opinion inPsychiatry.

I got to know Hugh when I wrote, with Jane Falvey, a chapter on

the Richmond Asylum (St. Brendan’s Hospital) for the second

volume of 150 Years of Psychiatry. In those days Hugh wrote toyou in a small but legible hand asking you to cut and paste. He

was so calm and polite that you carried out his wishes

unhesitatingly despite not having a computer! Over the years I

received appreciative letters from Hugh for anything I sent him.

When he was ill some years ago his wife Joan Casket, a professor

of psychology, handled his correspondence until Hugh was fit

again. My abiding memory of Professor Freeman is one of unruffled

determination. Hugh had three sons and a daughter. His family,

and psychiatry, has lost an enormous presence in their, and our,

lives.

Brian O’ Shea, Editor, Psychiatry Professional,Editor, A Textbook of Psychological Medicine, Tribunal Consultant Psychiatrist with the Mental Health

Commission.

E-mail [email protected]

Appreciation

Professor Hugh Lionel Freeman (1930-2011) Brian O’ Shea Submitted November 8th 2011

Accepted February 2nd 2012

Page 63: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Interaction of duloxetine with warfarin; a cautionary report

64

* Mugtaba Osman, Psychiatric Registrar, St Patrick’s University Hospital, Dublin 8, Ireland. E-mail [email protected]

Elaine Greene, Consultant Old Age Psychiatrist, Jonathan Swift Clinic, St James’sHospital, Dublin 8, Ireland.

Dear Editor,

Drug-drug interactions are common in older adults. Four out of five

people aged over 75 years take at least one medicine and thirty-

six percent of this age group take four medicines or more. 1

Warfarin is primarily cleared by the liver through the cytochrome

P450 system. Many of the isoenzymes involved are also involved in

the metabolism of psychotropic agents.2 Drug interactions with

Warfarin are generally well described. Data regarding the

interaction of warfarin with duloxetine are limited. There are only

two case reports in the literature which report conflicting findings.3,4

Additionally, a small open-label study reported no significant

interaction between the two agents. 5

Warfarin is normally indicated for serious vascular conditions. So

an elderly patient who is attending the psychiatric service and is on

warfarin will have higher risk for serious interactions. This is exactly

the subject of this case report.

Case Report

An eighty seven year old gentleman was admitted for treatment of

a severe depressive episode with prominent anxiety symptoms.

Relevant medical co-morbidities included paroxysmal atrial

fibrillation, for which he was on lifelong warfarin prophylaxis.

Notably, his warfarin dose had been stable for several months prior

to admission. He presented with a treatment resistant depression

and failed to respond to trials of antidepressant medications

(including venlafaxine, bupropion, SSRI’s, mirtazapine and

agomelatine) and to augmentation with several different agents

(including risperidone, lithium, olanzapine, amisulpride and

aripiprazole). He refused the option of ECT. A trial of duloxetine

was moderately successful. The dose was increased cautiously to

90 mg with good effect. At this point it was noted that his INR had

increased significantly. This was temporally related to an increase in

duloxetine from 60 to 90 mg daily. There had been no other

significant changes in his management or his medical condition that

would explain the increased INR. Review of his INR readings and

comparison with his duloxetine treatment revealed a stepwise

increase in INR which coincided with the increase in duloxetine.

As this patient’s treatment resistant depression had responded to

duloxetine, a decision was made to continue duloxetine treatment

at this higher dose. This necessitated a forty percent dose reduction

in warfarin to stabilise the INR at a safe therapeutic level.

In light of the temporal relationship between the increase in

duloxetine and the rise of INR, the most likely explanation lies at

the cytochrome P450 level. It is likely that duloxetine may potentiate

the anticoagulant effect of warfarin through displacing warfarin

from CYP 1A2 isoenzymes.6 This would result in a net increase in

bioactive warfarin and a consequent increase in INR.

Discussion

A higher number of people enter old age nowadays thanks for

recent advances in health measures.7 Therefore, more people over

the age of 65 are expected to be on combination of warfarin plus

another one or more medications, especially psychiatric

medications. This report adds to the scarce evidence that currently

exists for possibility of drug-drug interactions between duloxetine

and warfarin. It is the first report, we know of, to describe a

potentiating effect for duloxetine upon the action of warfarin in

older adults. It stresses the need to closely monitor INR levels for

elderly patients on warfarin, especially if it is combined with

duloxetine for treatment of depressive illness or other psychiatric or

non-psychiatric disorder. It appears that duloxetine can

synergistically potentiate the effect of warfarin, thereby, leading to

a bleeding hazard. Further research is urgently needed in this area.

References

1. Department of Health (2001) National Service Framework for Older People.

London: Stationery Office.

2. Limke KK, Shelton AR, Elliott ES. Fluvoxamine interaction with warfarin. Ann

Pharmacother. 2002;36:1890-1892

3. Glueck C J, Khalil Q, Wang P. Interaction of Duloxetine and Warfarin Causing

Severe Elevation of International Normalized Ratio. JAMA, April 2006; 13:

1517-1518.

4. Monastero R. Potential drug-drug interaction between duloxetine and

acenocoumarol in a patient with Alzheimer's disease, Clinical Therapeutics,

December 2007; 29: 2706-2709

5. Frincu-Mallos C. Duloxetine Does Not Modify Anticoagulant Effects of

Warfarin: Presented at American College of Clinical Pharmacology (ACCP)

37th Annual Meeting, September 2008.

6. Glueck C J, Khalil Q, Wang P. Interaction of Duloxetine and Warfarin Causing

Severe Elevation of International Normalized Ratio. JAMA, April 2006; 13:

1517-1518.

7. Kapplan B J, Sadock’s V A, Psynopsis of Psychiatry Behavioural

Sciences/ Clinical Psychiatry, 10th edition. In Geriatric Psychiatry,

chapter 56, page 1348, Lippincott W&W, 2007.

Letter to the EditorMugtaba Osman, Elaine Greene

*Correspondence

Page 64: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Irish Journal of Psychological MedicineGuidelines for Authors

Aim and Scope of the Journal

The aim of the Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine is to publish

original scientific contributions in psychiatry, psychological medicine

(including surgery and obstetrics), and related basic sciences

(neurosciences, biological, psychological, and social sciences).

The scope of the Journal includes any subspecialties of the above,

including, but not limited to, behavioural pharmacology, biological

psychiatry, child and adolescent psychiatry, intellectual disability,

forensic psychiatry, psychotherapies, psychiatry of old age,

epidemiology, rehabilitation, psychometrics, substance misuse,

sexual studies, linguistics, and the history, philosophy and

economics of psychiatry.

The Journal is dedicated to providing reliable, valid clinical and

scientific information to inform mental health care decisions and

improve the quality of mental health care.

Editorial Standards

The Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine complies with the “Code

of Conduct and Best Practice Guidelines for Journal Editors” of the

Committee on Publication Ethics (2011) and the “Editorial Policy

Statements” of the Council of Science Editors (2009).

http://publicationethics.org/files/Code_of_conduct_for_journal_edit

ors_Mar11.pdf

http://www.councilscienceeditors.org/i4a/pages/index.cfm?pageid=

3286

Submission Guidelines

The Journal will accept for consideration original papers, brief

research reports, audits, clinical case reports, review articles,

historical papers, perspective articles, editorials, letters to the editor

and book reviews. Original data papers receive top priority for

speedy publication.

Manuscripts should be prepared in accordance with the “Uniform

Requirements for Manuscripts (URM) Submitted to Biomedical

Journals” of the International Committee of Medical Journal Editors

(2010). www.icmje.org

Title Page

The title page should include the paper title, author name(s), author

qualifications, author job title(s), author affiliation(s) and full address

(es). The name, address, email address and telephone number of

the corresponding author should be clearly and separately indicted.

Text

• The manuscript should be typed, double-spaced, in 12-point

Times New Roman font.

• Pages should be numbered but do not use any other automatedfeatures (such as endnotes, headers or footers), any form of

automated referencing software, or any mechanism to track

changes to various drafts of a manuscript.

• Numbers one to ten should be written as words in the text,

unless used as a unit of measurement; all numbers should be

written in digits in tables and figures.

• All numbers which start sentences should be written in words,

not digits.

• Bold type-face should be used for headings of sections and sub-

sections within the paper.

• Do not use tabs or indents within the text of the paper.

• SI units are required for all measurements.

• Means should be accompanied by standard deviations.

• Exact p values should be provided, unless p<0.0001.

• Recommended non-proprietary drug names should be used.

• Writing should be clear, simple and direct.

• Short sentences are preferred.

Abstract and Key-Words

The page following the title page should carry an abstract followed

by a list of three to ten key-words drawn, if possible, from the

medical subject headings (MeSH) list of the United States National

Library of Medicine and National Institutes of Health

(www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/mesh). The title and key-words should be

chosen to help future literature searchers.

The abstract, up to 150 words for an unstructured or 250 words

for a structured abstract (Haynes et al, 1991), should state

specifically the main purposes, procedures, findings and conclusions

of the study, emphasising what is new or important. For original

papers, brief research reports, audits and review articles, a

structured abstract is required, using the headings: Objectives,

Methods, Results and Conclusions.

Under the Abstract heading of Method, include, wherever

applicable the study design, setting, patients/participants (selection

criteria, description), interventions, observational and analytical

methods and main outcome measures. (For review articles specify

the methods of literature search and selection). Under the Abstract

heading of Results, give the most important specific data together

with their statistical significance.

Original Papers

Original papers should be divided into sections as follows:

Introduction, Methods, Results and Discussion. A Conclusions

section is not mandatory but may be included in the original

submission if the author wishes, or may be requested at a later

stage by peer-reviewers or editors. The Results section should

present a summary of main results and should not simply refer to

tables. Reports of randomised trials must conform to CONSORT

2010 guidelines.

http://www.consort-statement.org/consort-statement/overview0/

65

Page 65: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

Audit Papers

It is preferable that audit papers present the full cycle of clinical

audit, including audit, intervention and re-audit. In exceptional

circumstances, papers presenting one element of the audit cycle

may be published, but priority will be given to papers presenting

full audit cycles. The format for audit papers may differ from that

outlined for original papers, and may include, for example,

Introduction, Audit, Intervention, Re-Audit, Discussion and

Conclusions.

Clinical Case Reports

All clinical case reports must have the patient’s written, informed

consent before the paper is submitted.

References

Timely references should highlight the paper’s relevance to current

research or clinical practice. For references to journal articles

(International Committee of Medical Journal Editors, 2006; 2010;

Haynes et al, 1991; Bailar & Mosteller, 1998) and to books (Daly et

al, 1991; Gardner & Altman, 1989; American Psychiatric

Association, 1987) use the ‘Vancouver’ style, i.e. number references

in the order they appear in the text, do not alphabetise. Journal

titles should be abbreviated as outlined on PubMed by the United

States National Library of Medicine and National Institutes of Health

(www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/nlmcatalog/journals). Please see the sample

paper on the website of the College of Psychiatry of Ireland for

further details

http://www.irishpsychiatry.ie/Members/MembersInformationTools/

irishjournalofpsychologicalmedicine.aspx

Tables and Figures

Figures and graphs should be clear and of good quality, and should

be accompanied by relevant data to facilitate redrawing where

necessary. Clear and informative headings and captions should be

provided.

Submission Process

Manuscripts may be submitted electronically via email to

[email protected] or [email protected]

Full postal address, telephone and fax numbers should be included.

Where possible, tables, figures and text should be included in the

same document. There is no need to also submit by post or fax.

All submitted material will become the property of the Journal until,

and if, publication is refused. Material so referred should not be

sent elsewhere for publication.

Conflict of Interest

In the interest of accountability all financial and material support

for the research and the work should be clearly stated (DeAngelis

et al, 2001). Authors of original data must take responsibility for

the integrity of the data and accuracy of the data analysis. All

authors must have full access to all the data in the study (Davidoff

et al, 1986). Authors must declare any conflict of interest clearly, in

accordance with the guidance of the International Committee of

Medical Journal Editors (2006):

“Public trust in the peer review process and the credibility of publishedarticles depend in part on how well conflict of interest is handled duringwriting, peer review, and editorial decision making. Conflict of interestexists when an author (or the author's institution), reviewer, or editorhas financial or personal relationships that inappropriately influence(bias) his or her actions (such relationships are also known as dualcommitments, competing interests, or competing loyalties). Theserelationships vary from those with negligible potential to those withgreat potential to influence judgment, and not all relationshipsrepresent true conflict of interest. The potential for conflict of interestcan exist whether or not an individual believes that the relationshipaffects his or her scientific judgment. Financial relationships (such asemployment, consultancies, stock ownership, honoraria, paid experttestimony) are the most easily identifiable conflicts of interest and themost likely to undermine the credibility of the journal, the authors, andof science itself. However, conflicts can occur for other reasons, such aspersonal relationships, academic competition, and intellectual passion.”

Statement of Informed Consent

Where relevant, papers must include a statement regarding

informed consent, in accordance with the guidance of the

International Committee of Medical Journal Editors (2006):

“Patients have a right to privacy that should not be infringedwithout informed consent. Identifying information, includingpatients' names, initials, or hospital numbers, should not bepublished in written descriptions, photographs, and pedigreesunless the information is essential for scientific purposes and thepatient (or parent or guardian) gives written informed consent forpublication. Informed consent for this purpose requires that apatient who is identifiable be shown the manuscript to bepublished. Authors should identify Individuals who provide writingassistance and disclose the funding source for this assistance.Identifying details should be omitted if they are not essential.Complete anonymity is difficult to achieve, however, and informedconsent should be obtained if there is any doubt. For example,masking the eye region in photographs of patients is inadequateprotection of anonymity. If identifying characteristics are altered toprotect anonymity, such as in genetic pedigrees, authors shouldprovide assurance that alterations do not distort scientific meaningand editors should so note. The requirement for informed consentshould be included in the journal's instructions for authors. Wheninformed consent has been obtained it should be indicated in thepublished article.”

Statement of Human and Animal Rights

Where relevant, papers must include a statement regarding human

and animal rights, in accordance with the guidance of the

International Committee of Medical Journal Editors (2006):

“When reporting experiments on human subjects, authors shouldindicate whether the procedures followed were in accordance withthe ethical standards of the responsible committee on humanexperimentation (institutional and national) and with the HelsinkiDeclaration of 1975, as revised in 2000 (5). If doubt exists whetherthe research was conducted in accordance with the HelsinkiDeclaration, the authors must explain the rationale for theirapproach, and demonstrate that the institutional review bodyexplicitly approved the doubtful aspects of the study. Whenreporting experiments on animals, authors should be asked to

66

Page 66: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine

indicate whether the institutional and national guide for the careand use of laboratory animals was followed.”

Acknowledgements

Authors should obtain permission to acknowledge individuals

named in any Acknowledgments section, since readers may infer

endorsement.

Suggested Peer-Reviewers

Each submission must be accompanied by the names, professionaltitles, professional addresses and email addresses of three suggested

peer-reviewers. Authors should select these suggested peer-

reviewers to include individuals, in any part of the world, who are

recognized experts in the area to which the submission refers, and

whom the authors believe would provide useful, objective peer-

reviews of the manuscript. The editors will give consideration to

sending the manuscript to some or all of these peer-reviewers, but

are not under any obligation to do so.

Description of the Peer-Review Process

All submissions are acknowledged by email. Submissions are

initially considered by the Editor-In-Chief or Deputy Editor for

suitability for peer-review. Submissions selected for peer-review are

sent to three anonymous outside peer-reviewers. Where one or

more peer-reviewers recommend acceptance or acceptance after

revision, all peer-reviews are sent to the corresponding author, with

an invitation to revise the paper.

If the author chooses to revise the paper, the revised paper should

be accompanied by a detailed cover letter responding to each

comment made by each peer-reviewer, indicating precisely how the

revision deals with each comment, or why the author disagrees with

or cannot incorporate specific comments.

Each peer-reviewer will then receive the revised paper, cover letter

and comments of the other peer-reviewers. After the peer-

reviewers’ further comments have been received, a final decision

about publication will be made.

The editorial process may vary from the above under certain

circumstances, at the discretion of the Editor-In-Chief or Deputy

Editor.

Other Modes of Review

Some guidance on statistical matters for authors is provided by

International Committee of Medical Journal Editors (2006; 2010)

and Bailar & Mosteller (1998). Notwithstanding this guidance,

statistical review may be required for certain papers, and this will be

arranged by the Journal editors where indicated. Other, morespecialist forms of peer-review may also be required on occasion,

and these, too, will be arranged by the Journal editors where

indicated.

Fast-Track Publication

Papers which the editors feel warrant fast-track publication will be

expedited through the publication process. The decision to “fast-

track” papers lies with the editors.

Plagiarism and Duplicate Publication

Manuscripts are considered with the understanding that they have

not been published previously, either in print or electronic format.

In the event that plagiarism or duplicate publication is suspected,

the author will be invited to comment on the matter and a decision

will be taken by the editors.

Appeals

IIn the event that an author wishes to appeal an editorial decision,

the author can send a letter of appeal to the Editor-In-Chief. The

Editor-In-Chief will pass the relevant materials to the Consulting

Editor who may seek external opinion. The Consulting Editor will

advise the Editor-In-Chief in relation to the appeal but the final

decision on the matter rests with the Editor-In-Chief.

References

American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental

Disorders. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association, 1987.

Bailar JC, Mosteller F. Guidelines for statistical reporting in articles for medical

journals. Ann Intern Med 1988; 108: 266-273.

Committee on Publication Ethics. Code of Conduct and Best Practice Guidelines

for Journal Editors. London: Committee on Publication Ethics, 2011.

http://publicationethics.org/files/Code_of_conduct_for_journal_editors_Mar11.pdf

Council of Science Editors. Promoting Integrity in Scientific Journal Publications.

Wheat Ridge, CO: Council of Science Editors, 2009.

http://www.councilscienceeditors.org/i4a/pages/index.cfm?pageid=3286.

Daly LE, Bourke GJ, McGilvray J. Interpretation and Uses of Medical Statistics (4th

Edition). Oxford: Blackwell Scientific Publications, 1991.

DeAngelis CD, Fontanarosa PB, Flanagin A. Reporting financial conflicts of interest

and relationships between investigators and research sponsors. JAMA 2001; 286:

89-91.

Davidoff F, DeAngelis CD, Drazen JM, Hoey J, Højgaard L, Horton R, Kotzin S,

Nicholls MG, Nylenna M, Overbeke AJ, Sox HC, Van Der Weyden MB, Wilkes MS.

Sponsorship, authorship, and accountability. JAMA 2001; 286: 1232-1234.

Gardner MJ, Altman DG (editors). Statistics with Confidence: Confidence Intervals

and Statistical Guidelines. London: British Medical Journal, 1989.

Haynes RB, Mulrow CD, Huth EJ, Altman DG, Gardner MJ. More information

abstracts revisited. Ann Intern Med 1990; 113: 69-76.

International Committee of Medical Journal Editors. Uniform requirements for

manuscripts submitted to biomedical journals. Vancouver: International

Committee of Medical Journal Editors, 2006.

International Committee of Medical Journal Editors. Uniform requirements for

manuscripts submitted to biomedical journals. Vancouver: International

Committee of Medical Journal Editors, 2010. www.icmje.org

67

Page 67: Irish Journal of Psychological Medicine