how big five relates to electroencephalographic and behavioural … · 2018-05-21 · as individual...

1
As individual differences in social cognition may underlie several Big Five traits we investigated how personality relates to behavioural and electroencephalographic (EEG) correlates of gaze direction perception. Previous findings give reason to suggest that direct and averted gazes are associated with the brain activity indicative of approach and avoidance, respectively. There is also reason to believe that in order to activate “mentalizing” and self-awareness processes live models have to be used instead of pictures (Hietanen et al., 2008). In numerous studies frontal alpha asymmetry, presumably an inverse index of underlying cortical activity, has been associated with approach-avoidance motivation. More right sided alpha power has been related to (both trait and state) approach and more left sided alpha to withdrawal tendencies (for reviewes see Coan & Allen, 2004; Harmon-Jones et al., 2010). Recently a new EEG index of motivational tendencies has been proposed posterior versus frontal delta/theta power, hypothetically an inverse index of rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC) activity. The rACC is part of dopaminergic reward circuit. It is also important in the “default network” activated during resting states and deactivated during tasks that require self-relevant mental explorations and provide the means to anticipate upcoming events (Wacker et al., 2011). In several studies positive correlations between resting state posterior-frontal delta/theta index and trait BAS/Agentic Extraversion have been observed (for review see Wacker et al., 2011). How Big Five Relates to Electroencephalographic and Behavioural Correlates of Eye Contact Helen Uibo¹, Jüri Allik¹ & Jari Hietanen² ¹ Institute of Psychology, University of Tartu, Estonia ² Human Information Processing Laboratory, University of Tampere, Finland Correspondence concerning this study should be addressed to [email protected] Introduction Sample 41 students (13 males); mean age 22,51 years (SD = 2,95). Participants were recruited from a larger sample (N=147) according to their personality profiles. Final sample was balanced for all B5 traits, with Neuroticism and Extraversion as a priority. Experiment Stimuli: Live faces of two female models with neutral expression were presented through a 40 cm × 30 cm liquid crystal (LC-TEC Displays Ab) shutter that switched between opaque and transparent states. Distance from the shutter: model 30 cm, participant70 cm; faces levelled (figure 1). The identity of models was approximately counter- balanced across male and female participants as well as personality profiles (no significant differences, in traits or subscales). Two conditions: 1) Fixed timing: Gaze direction - direct, averted, closed eyes. 3 x 9 presentations for 5 seconds in pseudo-randomized order. No task was required, except to watch the stimuli as naturally as possible. 2) Participant controlled timing: Gaze direction - direct, averted. 2 x 8 presentations. Instruction was to look as long as it felt natural. Personality The ‘Short Five’ (S5) - 60-item questionnaire that measures 30 facets of the Five-Factor Model, Finnish version (Konstabel et al., 2012). Subjective ratings After second condition model’s face was presented again three times (direct and averted gaze, closed eyes) in random order. Each time the participant was asked to recall the experiment and rate accordingly the valence and arousal (9p SAM scales; Bradley & Lang, 1994) of his/her subjective experience, the level of basic emotions (fear, happiness, anger, sadness, surprise, disgust) expressed by and the approachability, dominance and pleasantness of the face (7p scales). Note: There were no significant differences between the models in any scale. EEG Recording: 16 scalp electrodes (F3,F4,F7,F8,C3,C4,P3,P4), 4 ocular electrodes (channels HEOG & VEOG), online reference to Cz, offline rereferecne to linked ears (A1, A2). Online 1200 Hz band-pass filter (50Hz notch filter enabled). Pre-processing: Removal of ocular activity - manually detected independent components corresponding to blinks, vertical and horizontal movements (EEGLAB Infomax ICA). Artefact rejection threshold -75 to 75 μV , movement and muscle artefacts (EEGLAB rejspec 20 40 Hz). On average 73,53% of trials remained (SD = 8,55%). Event-related spectral perturbations (ERSP) were calculated using wavelet analyses (EEGLAB newtimef) with -2500 to -500 ms prestimulus as baseline (10*log10 power). For individual electrodes ERSP-s were averaged within theta (5-7 Hz) and alpha (8-13Hz) bands from 0 to 4500 ms poststimulus. Only fixed timing condition EEG is analysed. Indexes: Frontal alpha asymmetry scores were calculated for midfrontal electrode pair F4/F3 by subtracting the mean power of the left site from the mean power of the right site . Posterior versus frontal theta activity was calculated by averaging the mean power of midfrontal electrodes (F4/F3) and of posterior electrodes (P4/P3) and subtracting the former from the latter. Results Conclusions Method Behavioural and self-report measures People with higher Neuroticism are prone to avoid social contact - they prefer to look at averted gaze and they rate it to be more positive and less arousing. As indicated by frontal alpha asymmetry, in response to eye contact emotionally stable and extraverted people have stronger approach response (reciprocal to partners motivation), while more neurotic and introverted people tend to withdrawal. Considering the hypothetical relationship between posterior vs. frontal theta and the “default network”, it can be speculated that for emotionally stable, extraverted and conscientious people seeing face with close eyes provides an opportunity to rest while neurotic, introverted and impulsive people see the situation as ambiguous and engage in more mental exploration. Alternative explanation is that for the latter reduced contact is pleasant. N E O A C 35 40 45 50 55 60 65 70 mean score more frontal theta (lower post.-front. scores) less frontal theta (higher post.-front.scores) direct gaze A N E O A C 35 40 45 50 55 60 65 70 mean score closed eyes N E O A C 35 40 45 50 55 60 65 70 mean score averted gaze B Figure 7. Differences in personality profile according to posterior vs. frontal theta power of (A) direct gaze, (B) averted gaze and (C) closed eyes; lines denote median split groups; whiskers denote st. errors; Fisher’s LSD test; °- p < 0,1; *- p < 0,05; C _* _ _° _ _°_ _° _ _° _ N E O A C 35 40 45 50 55 60 65 70 mean score alpha less rightsided (lower right-left scores) alpha more rightsided (higer right-left scores) direct gaze _° _ _* _ Figure 5. Differences in personality profile according to frontal alpha asymmetry of direct gaze condition; lines denote median split groups; whiskers denote st. errors; Fisher’s LSD test; °- p<0,1; *- p<0,05 direct valence arousal averted valence arousal closed valence arousal N Figure 1. Experimental setup. Posterior vs. frontal theta direct averted closed -0,5 -0,4 -0,3 -0,2 -0,1 0,0 0,1 0,2 0,3 0,4 0,5 dB Figure 6. Posterior and frontal theta difference of three gaze directions; whiskers denote st. errors There was no main effect of gaze direction on frontal alpha asymmetry scores (F=0,37; p=0,69). On trend level averted gaze elicited most negative and closed eyes most positive asymmetry (figure 4). Participants who had more positive asymmetry (more alpha on the right side) in response to direct gaze had significantly higher Extraversion and lower Neuroticism (figure 5). There was no main effect of gaze direction (F=2,14; p=0,12). Post hoc comparison using the Fisher’s LSD test revealed a difference between closed eyes and direct gaze (p<0,1). Theta during closed eyes was significantly more posterior (figure 6). There was a positive correlation between Neuroticism and posterior vs. frontal theta during closed eyes (r=0.44; p<0,05), indicating more posterior theta. Also the analysis of personality profiles of median split groups for three gaze directions was performed. Participants with more frontal theta (smaller posterior-frontal index) in response to ... ... direct gaze had higher Extraversion (figure 6A). ... averted gaze had higher Neuroticism (figure 6B). ... closed eyes had lower Neuroticism but also higher Extraversion and higher Conscientiousness (figure 6C). Participant controlled looking times Figure 3. Correlations between Neuroticism and SAM scales (within subject standardization). Pearson correlation; *- p<0,05 Subjective ratings On average participants looked at the model for 8,67 seconds (SD=7,52). Average looking times for averted and direct gaze were 9,59 (SD= 8,78) and 7,74 (SD=6,89) seconds respectively. Because of large variations relative differences in looking times were analysed. For that within subject Cohen’s d’s were calculated. They correlated negatively with Neuroticism (r=-0,34; p <0,05) indicating that people with high Neuroticism looked at averted gaze longer than direct gaze. Neuroticism: low average high 42 44 46 48 50 52 54 56 58 standardized looking time averted direct Figure 2. Standardized looking times (within subject standardization) for participants with low (t-score < 45), average (t-score 45-55) and high (t-score > 55) Neuroticism. Personality t-scores were calculated using a larger sample (N=147). Whiskers denote standard errors. Fisher’s LSD test; *- p<0,05; **-p<0,001 _** _ ______* ______________ _____* ______ Subjective ratings of valence and arousal as well as dominance, approachability and pleasantness were analysed using within subject standardization to reveal relative preferences. Several correlations with Neuroticism emerged (figure 3). People with high Neuroticism rated averted gaze to be more pleasant and less arousing and direct gaze to be more negative. Frontal alpha asymmetry Figure 4. Average frontal alpha asymmetry scores of three gaze directions; whiskers denote st. errors direct averted closed -0,25 -0,20 -0,15 -0,10 -0,05 0,00 0,05 0,10 0,15 dB EEG indexes - fixed timing condition We thank Mia Huolman, Ilkka Kirjavainen, Karoliina Manninen, Laura Pönkänen and Stefanie Sheid for their help in conducting the experiment. References: Bradley, M. M.,&Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuringemotion: The Self-Assessment Manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy & Experimental Psychiatry, 25, 4959. Coan, J. A., & Allen, J. J. B. (2004). Frontal EEG asymmetry as a moderator and mediator of emotion. Biological Psychology, 67(1-2), 750. Harmon-Jones, E., Gable, P. A., & Peterson, C. K. (2010). The role of asymmetric frontal cortical activity in emotion-related phenomena: A review and update. Biological Psychology, 84(3), 451462. Hietanen, J. K., Leppänen, J. M., Peltola, M. J., Linna-aho, K., & Ruuhiala, H. J. (2008). Seeing direct and averted gaze activates the approach-avoidance motivational brain systems. Neuropsychologia, 46(9), 24232430. Konstabel, K., Lönnqvist, J., Walkowitz, G., Konstabel, K., & Verkasalo, M. (2012). The ‘Short Five’ (S5): Measuring personality traits using comprehensive single items. European Journal of Personality. 26, 13-29. Wacker, J., Chavanon, M.-L., & Stemmler, G. (2010). Resting EEG signatures of agentic extraversion: New results and meta-analytic integration. Journal of Research in Personality, 44(2), 167179.

Upload: others

Post on 12-Mar-2020

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: How Big Five Relates to Electroencephalographic and Behavioural … · 2018-05-21 · As individual differences in social cognition may underlie several Big Five traits we investigated

As individual differences in social cognition may underlie several Big Five traits we investigated how personality relates to behavioural and electroencephalographic (EEG) correlates of gaze direction perception.

Previous findings give reason to suggest that direct and averted gazes are associated with the brain activity indicative of approach and avoidance, respectively. There is also reason to believe that in order to activate “mentalizing” and self-awareness processes live models have to be used instead of pictures (Hietanen et al., 2008).

In numerous studies frontal alpha asymmetry, presumably an inverse index of underlying cortical activity, has been associated with approach-avoidance motivation. More right sided alpha power has been related to (both trait and state) approach and more left sided alpha to withdrawal tendencies (for reviewes see Coan & Allen, 2004; Harmon-Jones et al., 2010).

Recently a new EEG index of motivational tendencies has been proposed – posterior versus frontal delta/theta power, hypothetically an inverse index of rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC) activity. The rACC is part of dopaminergic reward circuit. It is also important in the “default network” activated during resting states and deactivated during tasks that require self-relevant mental explorations and provide the means to anticipate upcoming events (Wacker et al., 2011). In several studies positive correlations between resting state posterior-frontal delta/theta index and trait BAS/Agentic Extraversion have been observed (for review see Wacker et al., 2011).

How Big Five Relates to Electroencephalographic and Behavioural Correlates of Eye Contact

Helen Uibo¹, Jüri Allik¹ & Jari Hietanen² ¹ Institute of Psychology, University of Tartu, Estonia

² Human Information Processing Laboratory, University of Tampere, Finland

Correspondence concerning this study should be addressed to [email protected]

Introduction

Sample

41 students (13 males); mean age 22,51 years (SD = 2,95).

Participants were recruited from a larger sample (N=147) according to their personality profiles. Final sample was balanced for all B5 traits, with Neuroticism and Extraversion as a priority.

Experiment

Stimuli:

Live faces of two female models with neutral expression were presented through a 40 cm × 30 cm liquid crystal (LC-TEC Displays Ab) shutter that switched

between opaque and

transparent states. Distance from the shutter: model 30 cm, participant70 cm; faces levelled (figure 1). The identity of models was approximately counter- balanced across male and female participants as well as personality profiles (no significant differences, in traits or subscales).

Two conditions:

1) Fixed timing: Gaze direction - direct, averted, closed eyes. 3 x 9 presentations for 5 seconds in pseudo-randomized order. No task was required, except to watch the stimuli as naturally as possible.

2) Participant controlled timing: Gaze direction - direct, averted. 2 x 8 presentations. Instruction was to look as long as it felt natural.

Personality The ‘Short Five’ (S5) - 60-item questionnaire that measures 30 facets of the Five-Factor Model, Finnish version (Konstabel et al., 2012).

Subjective ratings

After second condition model’s face was presented again three times (direct and averted gaze, closed eyes) in random order. Each time the participant was asked to recall the experiment and rate accordingly the valence and arousal (9p SAM scales; Bradley & Lang, 1994) of his/her subjective experience, the level of basic emotions (fear, happiness, anger, sadness, surprise, disgust) expressed by and the approachability, dominance and pleasantness of the face (7p scales).

Note: There were no significant differences between the models in any scale.

EEG

Recording:

16 scalp electrodes (F3,F4,F7,F8,C3,C4,P3,P4), 4 ocular electrodes (channels HEOG & VEOG), online reference to Cz, offline rereferecne to linked ears (A1, A2). Online 1–200 Hz band-pass filter (50Hz notch filter enabled).

Pre-processing:

Removal of ocular activity - manually detected independent components corresponding to blinks, vertical and horizontal movements (EEGLAB Infomax ICA). Artefact rejection – threshold -75 to 75 μV, movement and muscle artefacts (EEGLAB rejspec 20 – 40 Hz). On average 73,53% of trials remained (SD = 8,55%).

Event-related spectral perturbations (ERSP) were calculated using wavelet analyses (EEGLAB newtimef) with -2500 to -500 ms prestimulus as baseline (10*log10 power). For individual electrodes ERSP-s were averaged within theta (5-7 Hz) and alpha (8-13Hz) bands from 0 to 4500 ms poststimulus. Only fixed timing condition EEG is analysed.

Indexes:

Frontal alpha asymmetry scores were calculated for midfrontal electrode pair F4/F3 by subtracting the mean power of the left site from the mean power of the right site .

Posterior versus frontal theta activity was calculated by averaging the mean power of midfrontal electrodes (F4/F3) and of posterior electrodes (P4/P3) and subtracting the former from the latter.

Results

Conclusions

Method

Behavioural and self-report measures

People with higher Neuroticism are prone to avoid social contact - they prefer to look at averted gaze and they rate it to be more positive and less arousing. As indicated by frontal alpha asymmetry, in response to eye contact emotionally stable and extraverted people have stronger approach response (reciprocal to partners motivation), while more neurotic and introverted people tend to withdrawal. Considering the hypothetical relationship between posterior vs. frontal theta and the “default network”, it can be speculated that for emotionally stable, extraverted and conscientious people seeing face with close eyes provides an opportunity to rest while neurotic, introverted and impulsive people see the situation as ambiguous and engage in more mental exploration. Alternative explanation is that for the latter reduced contact is pleasant.

N E O A C

35

40

45

50

55

60

65

70

mean s

core

more frontal theta (lower post.-front. scores)

less frontal theta (higher post.-front.scores)

direct gaze

A

N E O A C35

40

45

50

55

60

65

70

mean s

core

closed eyes

N E O A C

35

40

45

50

55

60

65

70

mean s

core

averted gaze

B

Figure 7. Differences in personality profile according to posterior vs. frontal theta power of (A) direct gaze, (B) averted gaze and (C) closed eyes; lines denote median split groups; whiskers denote st. errors; Fisher’s LSD test; °- p < 0,1; *- p < 0,05;

C

_*_ _°_ _°_ _°_ _°_

N E O A C

35

40

45

50

55

60

65

70

mean s

core

alpha less rightsided (lower right-left scores)

alpha more rightsided (higer right-left scores)

direct gaze

_°_ _*_

Figure 5. Differences in personality profile according to frontal alpha asymmetry of direct gaze condition; lines denote median split groups; whiskers denote st. errors; Fisher’s LSD test; °- p<0,1; *- p<0,05

direct valence

arousal

averted valence

arousal

closed valence

arousal

N

Figure 1. Experimental setup.

Posterior vs. frontal theta

direct averted closed-0,5

-0,4

-0,3

-0,2

-0,1

0,0

0,1

0,2

0,3

0,4

0,5

dB

Figure 6. Posterior and frontal theta difference of three gaze directions; whiskers denote st. errors

There was no main effect of gaze direction on frontal alpha asymmetry scores (F=0,37; p=0,69). On trend level averted gaze elicited most negative and closed eyes most positive asymmetry (figure 4).

Participants who had more positive asymmetry (more alpha on the right

side) in response to direct gaze had significantly higher Extraversion and lower Neuroticism (figure 5).

There was no main effect of gaze direction (F=2,14; p=0,12). Post hoc comparison using the Fisher’s LSD test revealed a difference between closed eyes and direct gaze (p<0,1). Theta during closed eyes was significantly more posterior (figure 6).

There was a positive correlation between Neuroticism and posterior vs. frontal theta during closed eyes (r=0.44; p<0,05), indicating more posterior theta. Also the analysis of personality profiles of median split groups for three gaze directions was performed.

Participants with more frontal theta (smaller posterior-frontal index) in response to ...

... direct gaze had higher Extraversion (figure 6A).

... averted gaze had higher Neuroticism (figure 6B).

... closed eyes had lower Neuroticism but also higher Extraversion and higher Conscientiousness (figure 6C).

Participant controlled looking times

Figure 3. Correlations between Neuroticism and SAM scales (within subject standardization). Pearson correlation; *- p<0,05

Subjective ratings On average participants looked at the model for 8,67 seconds (SD=7,52). Average looking times for averted and direct gaze were 9,59 (SD= 8,78) and 7,74 (SD=6,89) seconds respectively.

Because of large variations relative differences in looking times were analysed. For that within subject Cohen’s d’s were calculated. They correlated negatively with Neuroticism (r=-0,34;

p <0,05) indicating that people with high Neuroticism looked at averted gaze longer than direct gaze.

Neuroticism: low average high

42

44

46

48

50

52

54

56

58

sta

nd

ard

ize

d lo

okin

g t

ime

averted

direct

Figure 2. Standardized looking times (within subject standardization) for participants with low (t-score < 45), average (t-score 45-55) and high (t-score > 55) Neuroticism. Personality t-scores were calculated using a larger sample (N=147). Whiskers denote standard errors. Fisher’s LSD test; *- p<0,05; **-p<0,001

_**_ ______*______________

_____*______

Subjective ratings of valence and arousal as well as dominance, approachability and pleasantness were analysed using within subject standardization to reveal relative preferences.

Several correlations with Neuroticism emerged (figure 3).

People with high Neuroticism rated averted gaze to be more pleasant and less arousing and direct gaze to be more negative.

Frontal alpha asymmetry

Figure 4. Average frontal alpha asymmetry scores of three gaze directions; whiskers denote st. errors

direct averted closed-0,25

-0,20

-0,15

-0,10

-0,05

0,00

0,05

0,10

0,15

dB

EEG indexes - fixed timing condition

We thank Mia Huolman, Ilkka Kirjavainen, Karoliina Manninen, Laura Pönkänen and Stefanie Sheid for their help in conducting the experiment.

References:

Bradley, M. M.,&Lang, P. J. (1994). Measuringemotion: The Self-Assessment Manikin and the semantic differential. Journal of Behavior Therapy & Experimental Psychiatry, 25, 49–59.

Coan, J. A., & Allen, J. J. B. (2004). Frontal EEG asymmetry as a moderator and mediator of emotion. Biological Psychology, 67(1-2), 7–50.

Harmon-Jones, E., Gable, P. A., & Peterson, C. K. (2010). The role of asymmetric frontal cortical activity in emotion-related phenomena: A review and update. Biological Psychology, 84(3), 451–462.

Hietanen, J. K., Leppänen, J. M., Peltola, M. J., Linna-aho, K., & Ruuhiala, H. J. (2008). Seeing

direct and averted gaze activates the approach-avoidance motivational brain systems. Neuropsychologia, 46(9), 2423–2430.

Konstabel, K., Lönnqvist, J., Walkowitz, G., Konstabel, K., & Verkasalo, M. (2012). The ‘Short Five’ (S5): Measuring personality traits using comprehensive single items. European Journal of Personality. 26, 13-29.

Wacker, J., Chavanon, M.-L., & Stemmler, G. (2010). Resting EEG signatures of agentic extraversion: New results and meta-analytic integration. Journal of Research in Personality, 44(2), 167–179.