graphic organizers and students with learning …

22
GRAPHIC ORGANIZERS AND STUDENTS WITH LEARNING DISABILITIES: A META-ANALYSIS Douglas D. Dexter and Charles A. Hughes Abstract. This meta-analysis reviews experimental and quasi- experimental studies in which upper-elementary, intermediate, and secondary students with learning disabilities learned from graphic organizers. Following an exhaustive search for studies meeting specified design criteria, 55 standardized mean effect sizes were extracted from 16 articles involving 808 participants. Students at levels ranging from grade 4 to grade 12 used graphic organizers to learn in core-content classes (English/reading, sci- ence, social studies, mathematics). Posttests measured near and far transfer. Across several conditions, settings, and features, the use of graphic organizers was associated with increases in vocabulary knowledge, comprehension, and inferential knowledge. Mean effect sizes varied from moderate to large based on type of meas- ure, type of graphic organizer, and subject area. Conclusions, implications for future research, and practical recommendations are presented. DOUGLAS D. DEXTER, Ph.D., College of Education, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pennsylvania. CHARLES A. HUGHES, Ph.D., College of Education, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pennsylvania. As students enter the upper-elementary, intermediate, and secondary grades, academic demands are height- ened as material becomes more complex and the cur- riculum is driven by higher-order skills and advanced concepts (Fletcher, Lyon, Fuchs, & Barnes, 2007). Students at these levels typically receive less individual attention than in primary grades (Hughes, Maccini, & Gagnon, 2003), and are often required to learn prima- rily through didactic lecture and expository text presen- tation (Gajria, Jitendra, Sood, & Sacks, 2007; Minskoff & Allsopp, 2003). This shift in learning presentation, rife with abstract concepts, unfamiliar content, and technical vocabulary (Armbruster, 1984), may seem daunting to most students, but especially so to students with learning disabilities (LD). Students with LD often have difficulty with basic aca- demic skills (e.g., reading) and organizational/study skills (Deshler, Ellis, & Lenz, 1996). These students gen- erally have difficulty connecting new material to prior knowledge, identifying and ignoring extraneous infor- mation, identifying main ideas and supporting details, drawing inferences, and creating efficient problem-solv- ing strategies (Baumann, 1984; Holmes, 1985; Johnson, Graham, & Harris, 1997; Kim, Vaughn, Wanzek, & Wei, 2004; Williams, 1993). Because many textbooks are written above grade-level reading ability and lack orga- nizational clarity (Gajria et al., 2007), these learning difficulties make interpreting and comprehending expository text especially challenging (Bryant, Ugel, Thompson, & Hamff, 1999). Students with LD need explicit content enhance- ments to assist in verbal (e.g., text or lecture) compre- hension, and graphic organizers (GOs) have often been recommended as an instructional device to assist these 51 Learning Disability Quarterly © 2011 Council for Learning Disabilities 2011, Vol. 34, No. 1, 51-72

Upload: others

Post on 11-Dec-2021

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

GRAPHIC ORGANIZERS AND STUDENTS WITHLEARNING DISABILITIES: A META-ANALYSIS

Douglas D. Dexter and Charles A. Hughes

Abstract. This meta-analysis reviews experimental and quasi-experimental studies in which upper-elementary, intermediate,and secondary students with learning disabilities learned fromgraphic organizers. Following an exhaustive search for studiesmeeting specified design criteria, 55 standardized mean effectsizes were extracted from 16 articles involving 808 participants.Students at levels ranging from grade 4 to grade 12 used graphicorganizers to learn in core-content classes (English/reading, sci-ence, social studies, mathematics). Posttests measured near and fartransfer. Across several conditions, settings, and features, the useof graphic organizers was associated with increases in vocabularyknowledge, comprehension, and inferential knowledge. Meaneffect sizes varied from moderate to large based on type of meas-ure, type of graphic organizer, and subject area. Conclusions,implications for future research, and practical recommendationsare presented.

DOUGLAS D. DEXTER, Ph.D., College of Education, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pennsylvania.CHARLES A. HUGHES, Ph.D., College of Education, The Pennsylvania State University, University Park, Pennsylvania.

As students enter the upper-elementary, intermediate,and secondary grades, academic demands are height-ened as material becomes more complex and the cur-riculum is driven by higher-order skills and advancedconcepts (Fletcher, Lyon, Fuchs, & Barnes, 2007).Students at these levels typically receive less individualattention than in primary grades (Hughes, Maccini, &Gagnon, 2003), and are often required to learn prima-rily through didactic lecture and expository text presen-tation (Gajria, Jitendra, Sood, & Sacks, 2007; Minskoff & Allsopp, 2003). This shift in learning presentation,rife with abstract concepts, unfamiliar content, andtechnical vocabulary (Armbruster, 1984), may seemdaunting to most students, but especially so to studentswith learning disabilities (LD).

Students with LD often have difficulty with basic aca-demic skills (e.g., reading) and organizational/study

skills (Deshler, Ellis, & Lenz, 1996). These students gen-erally have difficulty connecting new material to priorknowledge, identifying and ignoring extraneous infor-mation, identifying main ideas and supporting details,drawing inferences, and creating efficient problem-solv-ing strategies (Baumann, 1984; Holmes, 1985; Johnson,Graham, & Harris, 1997; Kim, Vaughn, Wanzek, & Wei,2004; Williams, 1993). Because many textbooks arewritten above grade-level reading ability and lack orga-nizational clarity (Gajria et al., 2007), these learning difficulties make interpreting and comprehendingexpository text especially challenging (Bryant, Ugel,Thompson, & Hamff, 1999).

Students with LD need explicit content enhance-ments to assist in verbal (e.g., text or lecture) compre-hension, and graphic organizers (GOs) have often beenrecommended as an instructional device to assist these

51

Learning Disability Quarterly © 2011 Council for Learning Disabilities2011, Vol. 34, No. 1, 51-72

students in understanding increasingly abstract con-cepts (Bos & Vaughn, 2002; Hughes et al., 2003; Ives &Hoy, 2003; Kim et al., 2004; Nesbit & Adesope, 2007;Rivera & Smith, 1997).

What Are Graphic Organizers?GOs are visual and spatial displays that make rela-

tionships between related facts and concepts moreapparent (Gajria et al., 2007; Hughes et al., 2003; Kimet al., 2004). They are intended to promote more mean-ingful learning and facilitate understanding and reten-tion of new material by making abstract concepts moreconcrete and connecting new information with priorknowledge (Ausubel, 1968; Mayer, 1979). While thereis inconsistency in the definitions of types of GOs(Rice, 1994), we were able to classify all the studies onstudents with LD where effects could solely be attrib-uted to the GO into the following five general cate-gories.

Cognitive Mapping. Cognitive mapping assists inmaking major ideas and relationships explicit by using“lines, arrows, and spatial arrangements to describetext content, structure, and key conceptual relation-ships” (Darch & Eaves, 1996, p. 310). By graphicallydisplaying important information, cognitive mapping

may support and develop organizational strategiesessential to both reading and writing of content areamaterial by bridging the gap between idea organizationand written language structures (Pehrssen & Denner,1988). Further, Boyle and Yeager (1997) pointed outthat minimizing sentences and details in the GO is anessential component of cognitive mapping. They rec-ommend using keywords and simple drawings ratherthan complex sentences or elaborate drawings.Teachers often make cognitive mapping GOs to use asadvance organizers for challenging text. Students alsocan fill in teacher-prepared blank cognitive mappingGOs during or after attending to challenging verbalmaterial. An example of a cognitive mapping GO isfound in Figure 1.

Semantic Mapping. Semantic mapping (SM) is aheuristic that enables students to recognize relevantinformation from lecture and text (e.g., main ideas,important supporting details), delete isolated detailsthat may not be relevant to overall understanding, andhighlight key concepts that may have not been fullydeveloped in a lecture or text (Bos & Anders, 1990).

Unlike cognitive mapping, when using SM, studentsand the teacher actively create a visual representation(e.g., relationship map or web) to represent the rela-

Figure 1. Cognitive mapping example (science).

Downloaded from: http://www.studygs.net/mapping/

52 DEXTER & HUGHES

trees

earth30%

oceans97.5%plants

rain

clouds

atmosphere.001%

rivers, lakes,streams 2%

water onearth 70%

tionships among concepts. Typically, concepts arelisted, and the teacher and students make predictionsabout how the concepts can be arranged to demonstratethose relationships on the GO (Bos & Anders, 1992).

A well-made GO consists of a superordinate concept(e.g., main idea, topic) placed in an oval in the middleor top of the page. Coordinate concepts (e.g., categoriesrepresenting related concepts) are placed in ovals sur-rounding or underneath the superordinate concept andconnected by lines. Coordinate concepts can include avariety examples, functions, or characteristics of the

superordinate concept. Finally, subordinate concepts(e.g., concepts representing the coordinate concept) arelisted below each coordinate concept (Bos & Anders,1990, 1992; Pearson & Johnson, 1978). Figure 2 pro-vides an example of an SM GO.

Semantic Feature Analysis. Semantic feature analy-sis (SFA) is similar to SM by helping students recognizerelevant information from lecture and text. This isdone through a presentation of related concept charac-teristics in a matrix form. In SFA, unrelated conceptscan be inferred directly from the chart (Darch &

GRAPHIC ORGANIZERS AND LD 53

Figure 2. Semantic mapping example (social studies).

Figure 3. Semantic feature analysis example (science).

Comparison of Dog Breeds

Basset Hound Old English Sheepdog Brittany Spaniel Border Collie

Energetic – + + +

Weekly Grooming – + – –

Good with + + ? –Kids/Other Pets

Note. + = feature present; - = feature not present; ? = not sure.

Nobles

Vassals

Serfs

People

Fiefs

Farming

Castles on hills

LandFeudalism in Middle AgesEurope

Gersten, 1986). Typically, a relationship matrix is con-structed with vocabulary representing the coordinateconcepts placed along the top of the matrix, and thevocabulary representing the subordinate conceptsplaced along the side (Bos & Anders, 1990) The teacherand students can then make predictions and confirma-tions of relationships (e.g., related, not related, notsure) between the coordinate and subordinate concepts(Bos &Anders, 1992). The superordinate concept servesas the title. Figure 3 provides an example of an SFA G

Syntactic/Semantic Feature Analysis. Syntactic/semantic feature analysis (SSFA) is nearly identical toSFA but with the addition of cloze-type sentences writ-ten based on the matrix (Bos & Anders, 1990). Clozesentences contain blank spaces replacing new vocabu-lary words. Students must use the context of the sen-tence and the SFA matrix to fill in the blanks. Anexample of an SSFA GO is found in Figure 4.

Visual Display. Visual displays present concepts orfacts spatially, in a computationally efficient manner.That is, relationships between concepts are madeapparent and clear by their location on the display.According to Hughes et al. (2003), in a visual display,facts or concepts are typically presented in one of five

ways: temporal (e.g., timeline), spatial (e.g., decisiontree), sequential (e.g., flowchart), hierarchal (e.g., tax-onomy), or comparative (e.g., Venn diagram). Anexample of a visual display GO is found in Figure 5.

Previous ResearchSeveral groups of researchers have conducted reviews

and meta-analyses of the effectiveness of using GOswith nondisabled students (e.g., Ausubel, 1968;Kulhavy, Stock, & Caterino, 1994; Mayer, 1979; Moore& Readence, 1984; Nesbit & Adesope, 2007; Robinson,Katayama, DuBois, & Devaney, 1998). Based on theseexaminations of both the benefits of GOs and theeffective design of GOs, several key findings are con-sistently replicated: (a) students with low verbal abilitygain more from GOs than students with high verbalability; (b) students with little or no prior knowledgein a subject gain more from GOs than students with anabundance of prior knowledge in a subject; (c) GOs areespecially helpful in assisting students with far-transfertasks, in addition to near-transfer tasks and factualrecall; (d) GOs should be explicitly taught to studentsfor maximum impact; (e) GOs should spatially grouptogether or connect concepts so readers are more likelyto perceive them as being interrelated and to draw

54 DEXTER & HUGHES

Figure 4. Syntactic/semantic feature analysis example (science).

Comparison of Dog Breeds

Basset Hound Old English Sheepdog Brittany Spaniel Border Collie

Energetic – + + +

Weekly Grooming – + – –

Good with Kids/ + + ? –Other Pets

Note. + = feature present; - = feature not present; ? = not sure.

A(n) ________________ is the least energetic breed we have discussed.

A(n) ________________ requires at least weekly grooming.

A(n) ________________ and _________________ are good with other pets.

perceptual inferences about their relationships; (f) GOsshould not be clustered with a lot of information; read-ers should easily perceive the phenomena or relationsthat are important; (g) GOs are effective because oftheir computational efficiency, minimizing stress onthe working memory; and (h) GOs can be effectivewhen used before, during, or after a lesson. Whilethese findings are promising, the vast majority of thestudies reviewed used college students as participants,and few comparisons were made to students identifiedas LD.

Two research syntheses of school-aged children withLD (Gajria et al., 2007; Kim et al., 2004) have focused onGOs. In Gajria et al., as part of an examination of severalcontent enhancements, GOs were found to have largeeffects for comprehension of expository text for upper-elementary, intermediate, and secondary students withLD. Likewise, Kim et al. found large effects for GOs onthe reading comprehension of upper-elementary, inter-mediate, and secondary students with LD.

Drawing definitive conclusions from these reviews isproblematic for several reasons, however. First, thereviews did not take sample sizes into considerationwhen calculating effect sizes and comparing studies.Estimations of effect were based on individual studycharacteristics without standardization. The use ofmeta-analysis allows for statistical standardization offindings, resulting in numerical values that are inter-pretable in a consistent pattern across all studies, thuscontrolling for an individual study’s sample size (Lipsey& Wilson, 2001). Furthermore, according to Lipsey andWilson, meaningful effects and relationships wheremultiple quantitative studies with varying sample sizesagree, as well as differential effects of study differences,are more likely to be identified by meta-analytic proce-dures than by less systematic and analytic approaches.

Second, the reviews focused solely on factual com-prehension measures, not on vocabulary, inference, or relational comprehension. While information on fac-tual comprehension is important, it may be useful to

GRAPHIC ORGANIZERS AND LD 55

Figure 5. Visual dislay example (English/language arts).

Compare twounlike thingsusing like or as

Ex. – the girl isas big as ahouse

Compareunlike things

Examples offigurative language

Compare twounlike thingswith is, are,etc.

Ex. – the girlis a house

Simile Metaphor

Similarities

measure the utility of GOs for students with LD on mul-tiple constructs rather than only on the ability toanswer fact-level questions about a topic. The utility ofan intervention (e.g., GO) on multiple constructs is con-sidered important for high-quality research (Gersten,Baker, & Lloyd, 2000). Thus, the effectiveness of anintervention should be examined on all possible con-structs.

Third, no systematic analysis of effects was conductedby type of measure (e.g., near or far transfer), type ofGO, subject area, or student stage of attending to verbalmaterial. This lack of clarity makes it difficult to identifywhy the GO interventions were so effective. A more sys-tematic analysis of GO studies may allow for more pre-cise explanations of effect based on multiple criteria,and lead to a more robust understanding of how andwhy GOs are effective for students with LD.

In the present study, we conducted a meta-analysis ofGO research to address the following questions:

1. What are the overall effects of GOs on the posttestperformance of students with LD?

2. Do these effects maintain over time?3. Are there differential effects by type of measure

(near or far transfer)?4. Are there differential effects by type of GO?5. Are there differential effects by subject area?6. Are there differential effects by stage of attending

to verbal material (before, during, after instruc-tion)?

MethodLiterature Search Procedure

A three-step process was used to identify studiesusing GOs with upper-elementary, intermediate, andsecondary students with LD. First, we conducted a com-prehensive computerized search of PsycInfo, ERIC, andSocial Science Citation Index databases for studies from1975 (e.g., year LD was officially recognized by passageof P.L. 94-142) to October 2009, using a list of searchterms generated from previous reviews of GO studies(e.g., Horton et al., 1993; Moore & Readence, 1984;Nesbit & Adesope, 2006). We used the following com-bination of descriptors: graphic organ*, expository, verbal,learning disab*, concept map, cognitive map, adolesc*,semantic map, semantic feature analysis, visual display.

Second, we conducted ancestral searches of identifiedarticles, as well as the two most recent reviews of con-tent enhancements used with students with LD (e.g.,Gajria et al., 2007; Kim et al., 2004).

Finally, we conducted hand searches of the followingjournals to locate the most recent literature: ExceptionalChildren, Journal of Educational Psychology, Journal ofLearning Disabilities, The Journal of Special Education,Learning Disability Quarterly, Learning Disabilities

Research & Practice, Remedial and Special Education, andReading Research Quarterly. This process yielded a totalof 27 published articles to analyze, many (e.g., 20)including more than one study.

Inclusion CriteriaWe used six criteria to evaluate the appropriateness

of each study. First, the study must have included a dependent measure of near or far transfer of verbal(e.g., text or lecture) material and a GO as the inde-pendent variable. Studies with a mnemonic illustrationrather than GO as the content enhancement (e.g.,Brigham, Scruggs, & Mastropieri, 1995; Mastropieri,Scruggs, & Levin, 1987) were excluded, as effects could not be attributed solely to the GO. Likewise,studies including GOs associated with Content En-hancement Routines (Bulgren, Deshler, Schumaker, &Lenz, 2000; Bulgren, Lenz, Schumaker, Deshler, &Marquis, 2002; Bulgren, Marquis, Lenz, Schumaker, & Deshler, 2009; Bulgren, Schumaker, & Deshler, 1988;Scanlon, Deshler, & Schumaker, 1996) were excluded,because effects could not be attributed solely to theGO, even though GOs were vital components of eachof these routines. Further discussion about ContentEnhance- ment Routines is included in the Limitationssection of this meta-analysis.

Second, the study must have taken place in upper-elementary-, intermediate-, or secondary-level class-rooms (e.g., grades 4-12). This grade range was selectedbecause it represents a time when curricula typicallybecome more complex and students are required tolearn primarily through didactic lecture and expositorytext presentation (Fletcher et al., 2007; Hughes et al.,2003; Minskoff & Allsopp, 2003).

Third, based on the recommendations of Rosenthal(1994) and Lipsey and Wilson (1993), only studiesusing experimental or quasi-experimental groupdesigns with control groups were included. Therefore,single-subject research studies (e.g., Gardill & Jitendra,1999; Idol & Croll, 1987), repeated-measures studies(e.g., Lenz, Adams, Bulgren, Poulit, & Laraux, 2007),and single-group studies (e.g., Bergerud, Lovitt, &Horton, 1988; Boon, Fore III, Ayres, & Spencer, 2005;Horton, Lovitt, & Bergerud, 1990; Lovitt, Rudsit,Jenkins, Pious, & Benedetti, 1986; Sinatra, Stahl-Gemake, & Berg, 1984; Sturm & Rankin-Erickson, 2002)were excluded.

Fourth, the study must have provided sufficient quan-titative information (e.g., group means and standarddeviations; F statistic) to permit calculation of an effectsize (ES). One experimental study (Boyle & Weishaar,1997) was excluded because it provided only multivari-ate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) data withoutgroup means and standard deviations. According to

56 DEXTER & HUGHES

Hunter and Schmidt (2004), there is no algebraicallyequivalent method to compute a comparable ES in such an instance.

Fifth, participants in the experimental and controlgroups must have included students with LD. Wedefined LD the same way as Kim et al. (2004) in theirresearch review and Swanson, Hoskyn, and Lee (1999)in their meta-analysis (e.g., average intelligence andpoor performance in at least one academic or relatedbehavioral area). In each of the studies included, allparticipants were identified as students with LD.

Finally, the study must have been published in apeer-reviewed journal and in English. This excludedany studies in Dissertation Abstracts International andunpublished studies from researchers in the field.While this criterion ensures that only the highest qual-ity research was included in the meta-analysis (Slavin,1995), it also represents a potential publication bias(Lipsey & Wilson, 1993, 2001). This will be discussedfurther in the Limitations section.

Study CodingThe first author coded pertinent study features,

including participant characteristics (i.e., grade level,disability classification), subject area, type of GO,stated purpose, study contrasts, dependent measures,and reported findings. A graduate research assistantdouble-coded this information, resulting in an inter-rater reliability of .97. After discussion and clarificationto resolve disagreements in coding, interrater reliabilityreached 1.00.

Individual Effect Size CalculationUsing methods described by Lipsey and Wilson

(2001), standardized mean difference effect size was computed using pooled standard deviation. The for-mula used was:

where “XG1 is the mean for group 1, XG2 is the meanfor group 2, and Sp is the pooled standard deviation”(Lipsey & Wilson, p. 48). When studies only providedan F statistic, effect size was computed using a formularecommended by Thalheimer and Cook (2002). Theformula used was:

where nt is the number of treatment subjects, and nc isthe number of control subjects.

Next, to correct for upwardly biased effect sizes dueto small samples, a Hedges correction (Hedges, 1981;Lipsey & Wilson, 2001) was utilized. The unbiased

effect size estimate was computed using the followingformulae:

where N is the total sample size, ESsm is the biased stan-dardized mean difference, and

is the inverse variance weight used to calculate theweighted mean effect size. According to Hedges,Shymansky, and Woodworth (1989), the inverse vari-ance weight is a better approach to accounting for thesample size of a given study than the simpler approachof weighting by sample size.

OutliersPrior to analyzing the weighted mean effect size,

extreme effect sizes that may have disproportionateinfluence on the analysis were eliminated (Lipsey &Wilson, 2001). Based on the recommendation of Burns(2004), eliminated outliers were effect sizes that weregreater than 1.5 times the mean effect size.

Data AnalysisFollowing transformations and outlier elimination,

data were analyzed by computing the weighted meaneffect size:

where ∑ (w x ES) is the summed product of the effectsize and inverse variance weight, and ∑ w is thesummed inverse variance weight.

Next, the standard error of the mean effect size wascomputed using the following formula:

GRAPHIC ORGANIZERS AND LD 57

where

ESsm = ,XG1 - XG2

Sp

d =nt + nc

ntnc )(F√ ,)nt + nc

nt + nc - 2(

SEsm =nG1 + nG2

nG1nG2√ ,(ES’sm)2

2(nG1 + nG2)+

34N - 9 ]1 -[ES’sm = ESsm

wsm =1

SE2sm

,2nG1nG2 (nG1 + nG2)2(nG1 + nG2)2 + nG1nG2(ES’sm)2

=

,

wsm =1

SE2sm

seES = w∑

ES = ∑ (w x ES)

w∑

√ 1 ,

w∑√ 1 ,

is the square root of one divided by the summed inversevariance weight. The z-test for the weighted mean effectsize was then computed by dividing the mean effect sizeby the standard error of the mean effect size, or in sta-tistical notation:

Finally, using the z-test, the 95% confidence interval forthe weighted mean effect size was computed using thefollowing formulae:

Homogeneity AnalysisHomogeneity analysis tests whether the assumption

that all of the effect sizes estimate the same populationmean is reasonable (Hunter & Schmidt, 2004). Lipseyand Wilson (2001) contend that single mean effect sizesby themselves are not sufficient descriptors of the dis-tribution. Therefore, we computed a Q statistic to testhomogeneity, using the formula:

The Q statistic is distributed as a chi-square with degreesof freedom (df) equaling number of ESs – 1. In ouranalysis of all studies, the critical value for a chi-squarewith df = 47 and p = .05 is 64. Because our calculated Qstatistic (57.2) is less than this critical value, we can failto reject the null hypothesis of homogeneity andassume a fixed-effects model, under which the variabil-ity across effect sizes does not exceed what would beexpected based on sampling error. However, Lipsey andWilson (2001) warned that a nonsignificant Q statistic“does not always provide great confidence that a fixedeffects model is justified” (p. 117). Because we do nothave a large number of effect sizes and the correspon-ding samples are relatively small, the Q statistic may nothave sufficient statistical power. Therefore, we also fit a random-effects model, which assumes sampling errorplus other sources of variability are randomly distrib-uted (Lipsey & Wilson). The random-effects model isalso a more conservative estimate than the fixed-effectsmodel of differences between moderating variables(Hunter & Schmidt, 2004).

Whereas the fixed-effects model weights each studyby the inverse of the sampling variance, the random-effects model weights each study by the inverse of the

sampling variance plus a constant that represents thevariability across the population effects (Lipsey &Wilson, 2001). The formulae are as follows:

is the random-effects variance component.We reran the analysis using this new weight to fit therandom-effects model.

The preceding analyses were conducted for posttestmeasures, maintenance measures, and differentialeffects of individual levels of independent variables(e.g., GO type, subject area) and dependent variables(e.g., near- or far-transfer measures). Because it was possible to fail to reject the null hypothesis of homo-geneity, we will present results for both the fixed-effects and random-effects model for comparison.Cohen’s (1988) criteria for interpreting strength ofeffect sizes (small ES < .20, medium ES = .50, large ES >.80) were used to gauge the magnitude of the findingsin this analysis.

ResultsA total of 55 unique posttest effect sizes were

extracted from studies in 16 published articles meetingour inclusion criteria. For the purposes of this analysis,each unique effect size was considered an individualestimate of effect. (Included articles are marked with anasterisk in the References section.) In addition, eight ofthe published articles included maintenance data ren-dering 29 more unique effect sizes. Table 1 includesdetailed information on each study, participants, vari-ables, measures, and individual effect sizes.

Instructional ContextEach of the studies included instruction on the use of

a GO. The majority of studies (Anders, Bos, & Filip,1984; Bos & Anders, 1990, 1992; Boyle, 1996, 2000;Darch & Carnine, 1986; Darch, Carnine, & Kame’enui,1986; Darch & Eaves, 1986; Darch & Gersten, 1986;DiCecco & Gleason, 2002; Englert & Mariage, 1991;Griffin, Simmons, & Kame’enui, 1991; Hudson, 1996;Ives, 2007; Reyes, Gallego, Duran, & Scanlon, 1989)incorporated aspects of direct, explicit instruction (e.g.,modeling, prompted practice). The authors of theremaining study (Bos, Anders, Filip, & Jaffe, 1989)reported that written guidelines for teaching the GOwere developed; however, these guidelines were notincluded in the article.

58 DEXTER & HUGHES

where

ESseES

Z =

Lower = ES - 1.96(seES)

Upper = ES + 1.96(seES)

2](w x ES)(w x ES2) -

[∑Q = ∑

w∑

θ

w∑w2∑

w -∑ ( ),Qr - k - 1

θ =

se2i +

,wi =1

θ

GRAPHIC ORGANIZERS AND LD 59

Table 1Independent Variables, Dependent Measures, and Effect Sizes of Individual Experiments

Subject/GO Near or Effect SizeType/Study/ Control Far Dependent Posttest MaintenanceParticipants Condition Transfer Measure M M

SOCIAL STUDIESSemantic Mapping

Bos & Anders (1992); Normative Near Researcher-generated .42 .62*Study 5: comprehension test26 upper-elementary students with LD

Normative Near Researcher-generated .30 .06

comprehension testSemantic Feature Analysis

Anders, Bos, & Filip Definition Near Researcher-generated 1.71*** N/A(1984); 62 high school instruction comprehension teststudents with LD

Definition Near Researcher-generated 1.49*** N/Ainstruction vocabulary test

Bos, Anders, Filip, & Dictionary Near Researcher-generated 1.67*** .00Jaffe (1989); 50 high method comprehension testschool students with LD

SM, SFA, SSFA Combination

Bos & Anders (1992); Definition Near Researcher-generated .81 .86Study 1: 42 upper- instruction comprehension testelementary studentswith LD

Definition Near Researcher-generated .50 1.15instruction vocabulary test

Bos & Anders (1992); Definition Near Researcher-generated 1.46 N/AStudy 3: 47 upper- instruction comprehension testelementary students with LD

Definition Near Researcher-generated 1.28 N/Ainstruction vocabulary test

Visual Display

Darch & Carnine Text-only Near Researcher-generated 1.73*** N/A(1986); 24 upper- comprehension testelementary students with LD

Text-only Far Researcher-generated .64 N/Afar-transfer

comprehension Testcontinued next page

60 DEXTER & HUGHES

Table 1 continuedIndependent Variables, Dependent Measures, and Effect Sizes of Individual Experiments

Subject/GO Near or Effect SizeType/Study/ Control Far Dependent Posttest MaintenanceParticipants Condition Transfer Measure M M

SOCIAL STUDIES continued

Visual Display continued

Darch, Carnine, & Directed reading Near Researcher-generated .79*** N/AKame’enui (1986); comprehension test84 junior high school students with LD

Directed reading Far Researcher-generated .82*** N/Afar-transfer comprehension test

DiCecco & Gleason Guided Near Researcher-generated .05 N/A(2002); 24 junior high discussion fact quizzesstudents with LD

Guided Far Relational statements .94*** N/Adiscussion in two written essays

Hudson (1996); 21 Note-taking Near Researcher-generated 1.58*** 2.37***junior high students guide knowledge testwith LD

Note-taking Far Researcher-generated 1.00** 2.08***guide inference test

SCIENCESemantic Mapping

Bos & Anders (1990); Definition Near Researcher-generated 1.27*** .94**61 junior high instruction vocabulary teststudents with LD

Definition Near Researcher-generated 1.33*** .54instruction comprehension test

Bos & Anders (1992); Normative Near Researcher-generated 2.16*** .78*Study 6: 22 junior comprehension testhigh students with LD

Reyes, Gallego, Duran, Definition Near Researcher-generated 1.27*** .94**& Scanlon (1989); instruction comprehension test61 junior high students with LD

Definition Near Researcher-generated 1.33*** .47instruction vocabulary test

continued next page

GRAPHIC ORGANIZERS AND LD 61

Table 1 continuedIndependent Variables, Dependent Measures, and Effect Sizes of Individual Experiments

Subject/GO Near or Effect SizeType/Study/ Control Far Dependent Posttest MaintenanceParticipants Condition Transfer Measure M M

SCIENCE continued

Semantic Feature Analysis

Bos & Anders (1990 Definition); Near Researcher-generated 1.03** .66*61 junior high instruction vocabulary teststudents with LD

Definition Near Researcher-generated 1.47*** .44instruction comprehension test

Bos & Anders (1992); Normative Near Researcher-generated .17 .73*Study 6: 22 junior comprehension testhigh students with LD

Reyes, Gallego, Duran, Definition Near Researcher-generated 1.03*** .66*& Scanlon (1989); instruction comprehension test61 junior high students with LD

Definition Near Researcher-generated 1.47*** .44instruction vocabulary test

Syntactic/Semantic Feature Analysis

Bos & Anders (1990); Definition Near Researcher-generated .64 1.38***61 junior high instruction vocabulary teststudents with LD

Definition Near Researcher-generated 1.18** 1.40***instruction comprehension test

Reyes, Gallego, Definition Near Researcher-generated .64 1.38***Duran, & Scanlon instruction comprehension test(1989); 61 junior high students with LD

Definition Near Researcher-generated 1.18*** 1.40***instruction vocabulary test

SM/SFA/SSFA Combination

Bos & Anders (1992); Definition Near Researcher-generated 1.22 .78Study 2: 61 junior instruction comprehension testhigh students with LD

Definition Near Researcher-generated .92 1.01instruction vocabulary test

Bos & Anders (1992); Definition Near Researcher-generated 1.51 1.51Study 4: 53 junior instruction comprehension test high students with LD

Definition Near Researcher-generated .83 .79instruction vocabulary test

continued next page

62 DEXTER & HUGHES

Table 1 continuedIndependent Variables, Dependent Measures, and Effect Sizes of Individual Experiments

Subject/GO Near or Effect SizeType/Study/ Control Far Dependent Posttest MaintenanceParticipants Condition Transfer Measure M M

SCIENCE continued

Visual Display

Darch & Eaves (1986); Text-only Near Researcher-generated 1.29*** .3522 high school comprehension teststudents with LD

Text-only Near Researcher-generated .64 N/Afar-transfer comprehension test

Griffin, Simmons, & No visual Near Researcher-generated .51 N/AKame’enui (1991); display comprehension test28 upper-elementary students with LD

SCIENCE/SOCIAL STUDIES

Visual Display

Darch & Gersten Basal Near Researcher-generated 1.72*** N/A(1986); 24 high school instruction comprehension teststudents with LD

Semantic Feature Analysis

Darch & Gersten Dictionary Near Researcher-generated 1.66*** .00(1986); 24 high school method vocabulary teststudents with LD

ENGLISH/LANGUAGE ARTS

Cognitive Mapping

Boyle (1996); 30 junior Typical Near Formal reading .34 N/Ahigh school students reading inventorywith LD or MMR instruction

Typical Near Below-grade-level .87** N/Areading literal comprehension

instruction test

Typical Near On-grade-level 1.33*** N/Areading literal comprehension

instruction test

Typical Far Below-grade-level .76** N/Areading inferential

instruction comprehension test

Typical Far On-grade-level .96*** N/Areading inferential

instruction comprehension testcontinued next page

GRAPHIC ORGANIZERS AND LD 63

Table 1 continuedIndependent Variables, Dependent Measures, and Effect Sizes of Individual Experiments

Subject/GO Near or Effect SizeType/Study/ Control Far Dependent Posttest MaintenanceParticipants Condition Transfer Measure M M

ENGLISH/LANGUAGE ARTS continued

Cognitive Mapping

Mastropieri & Peters List Near Research-generated 1.42*** N/A(2003); 20 junior high illustration featured item recallstudents with LD test

List Far Research-generated 1.06** N/Aillustration featured item recall

test

Semantic Mapping

Englert & Mariage Typical reading Near Written free recall 1.84*** N/A(1991); 28 upper- instructionelementary students with LD

Visual Display

Boyle (2000); 24 high No training Near Literal comprehension 1.14*** N/Aschool students with testLD or MMR

No training Near Relational .91** N/Acomprehension test

No training Far Inferential .51 N/Acomprehension test

MATHEMATICS

Visual Display

Ives (2007); Study 1: Control Near Teacher-generated .67* N/A30 high school teststudents with LD

Control Near Researcher-generated 1.06** .94**test (concepts)

Control Far Researcher-generated .16 .00test (system solving)

Ives (2007); Study 2: Control Near Researcher-generated .49 N/A20 high school teststudents with LD

***p < .001; **p < .05; *p < .1; all GOs were created by the researchers.

Generally, instruction for the experimental groupsincluded one to two sessions focused solely on how touse the GO, one to two sessions of prompted practiceusing the GO, and independent student use of the GOfor the remainder of sessions. During the initial ses-sions, the teacher or researcher presented the GO tostudents and described how it illustrated relationships.For example, Darch and Carnine (1986) presented theirvisual display via an overhead projector, and studentsfollowed along while the teacher used a script todescribe the various cells in the display and the inter-relationships between them.

The following sessions generally included the instruc-tor explicitly guiding the students in creating or fillingout the GO. For example, Bos and Anders (1990) explic-itly prompted the students in each step of creating ahierarchical semantic map from a vocabulary list. Thislevel of assistance was gradually faded. For instance,Darch and Gersten (1986) first presented a visual dis-play with all the cells labeled, and prompted the entiregroup in answering questions about specific facts in the GO. The researchers followed this by guiding thestudents through a visual display that did not providecell labels. Finally, individual students were promptedin labeling blank visual displays. Instruction in theremaining sessions generally focused on independentuse of the GO by the students, in addition to text or lec-ture presentations. However, in each of the visual dis-play studies all of the content was presented solelythrough the GO.

Each of the interventions lasted between one andseven weeks, with an additional one to four weeksbetween posttest and maintenance measures. All of the

studies were conducted in a resource classroom duringor after the regular school day.

What Are the Overall Effects of GOs on thePosttest Performance of Students With LD?

After the removal of six outliers, there was a largeoverall standardized effect of GOs on the posttest per-formance (e.g., multiple-choice comprehension, vocab-ulary, written recall) of students with LD across allstudies (ES = .91, SE = .062) for both random- and fixed-effects models and a 95% confidence interval of .79,1.03 for the random effects model. Table 2 provides thefull comparison between the random- and fixed-effectsmodels.

Do These Effects Maintain Over Time?Twenty-nine studies included maintenance measures.

In each of the studies, measures consisted of multiple-choice comprehension or vocabulary items. Thesemeasures were given to students one to four weeks afterthe conclusion of the intervention.

The test of homogeneity for overall maintenanceeffects produced a nonsignificant Q statistic (Q = 24.49,cv = 35.17). Therefore, similar to the overall posttesteffects, results of both the random- and fixed-effectsmodels are reported. After removal of five outliers, therewas a medium overall effect for maintenance across allstudies (ES = .56, SE = .074) with a 95% confidence inter-val of .41, .70 for the random-effects model. Table 3 pro-vides the full comparison between the random- andfixed-effects models.

Differential EffectsBased on number of effect sizes and significant Q-val-

ues, the remaining analyses of differential effects are

64 DEXTER & HUGHES

Table 2Overall ES for Fixed and Random Models

95% CIES SE of ES Z-test Lower Upper

Fixed Model .9127 .062 14.677* .79 1.03

Random Model .9061 .062 14.615* .78 1.02

*p < .001.

reported as the random-effects model. This provides amore conservative estimate of effect.

Are There Differential Effects by Type of Measure(Near or Far Transfer)? Forty-five individual estimatesof effect were calculated for near-transfer measures atposttest and 27 individual estimates of effect for main-tenance. In all but two articles, near-transfer measuresconsisted of researcher-generated multiple-choice ques-tions on material directly covered in the lessons. In theremaining two articles, Boyle (1996) used a standardizedmeasure of reading comprehension and Englert andMariage (1991) used a measure of written free recall,

respectively. Overall mean effect size was 1.07 forposttest and .78 for maintenance.

Ten individual estimates of effect were calculated forfar-transfer measures at posttest and two individual esti-mates of effect for maintenance. These measures testedstudents’ ability to apply knowledge to situations notdirectly covered in the text or lecture. For example,Hudson (1996) included the question “Name one waythe environment influenced the culture of the Arctictribes” (p. 81). The teacher never stated the causal rela-tion between environment and culture; only facts aboutenvironment and culture were stated.

GRAPHIC ORGANIZERS AND LD 65

Table 3Maintenance ES for Fixed and Random Models

95% CIES SE of ES Z-test Lower Upper

Fixed Model .5625 .077 7.305* .41 .71

Random Model .5614 .074 7.586* .41 .70

*p < .001.

Table 4Near- and Far-Transfer – Random-Effects Model

Posttest Maintenance95% CI 95% CI

ES Lower Upper ES Lower Upper

Near Transfer 1.065 .94 1.19 .7809 .63 .93n = 45 n = 27

Far Transfer .6127 .36 .87 .6886 .07 1.31n = 10 n = 2

Note. n = number of ESs.

As for near-transfer, far-transfer measures consisted ofresearcher-generated multiple-choice questions in allbut one study. Boyle (1996) used a standardized meas-ure for far transfer. The overall mean effect size was .61for posttest and .69 for maintenance. Table 4 providesthe full comparison between near- and far-transfermeasures.

Are There Differential Effects by Type of GO? Thetypes of GOs used in the studies matched the defini-tions in the introduction to this analysis (e.g., cognitivemapping, SM, SFA, SSFA, visual display). However, inone article containing eight studies (Bos & Anders,1992), the researchers used a combination of SM, SFA,and SSFA. The method they utilized to present theirresults prohibited disaggregation of the findings.Therefore, a sixth category (SM/SFA/SSFA Combination)was added to the analysis. Large posttest effects (e.g.,.74-1.2) were found for all types of GOs except visualdisplays. Visual displays had a moderate effect (e.g.,.74). There were no statistically significant differencesbetween GOs with large posttest effects. For mainte-

nance measures, SSFA and SM/SFA/SSFA Combinationhad significantly larger effects than the other GO types(e.g., 1.39, 1.01). Table 5 provides the full comparisonbetween types of GOs.

Are There Differential Effects by Subject Area?Posttest effects were calculated for the subject areas ofEnglish/writing/reading, mathematics, science, andsocial studies. Large posttest effects were found for allsubject (e.g., .96-1.05) areas except mathematics (e.g.,.59). Mathematics had a moderate posttest effect thatwas significantly smaller than the other subject areas.Maintenance effects were calculated for mathematics,science, and social studies. Science had a large mainte-nance effect (e.g., .80) that was significantly larger thanthe moderate effects for mathematics and social studies.Table 6 provides the full comparison between GOs bysubject area.

Are There Differential Effects by Stage ofAttending to Verbal Material (Before, During, AfterInstruction)? There was not enough information toquantify differential effects by stage of instruction. All

66 DEXTER & HUGHES

Table 5Type of Graphic Organizer – Random-Effects Model

Posttest Maintenance95% CI 95% CI

ES Lower Upper ES Lower Upper

Cognitive .8914 .58 1.21 N/A N/A N/A Mapping n = 7 n = 0

Semantic 1.251 .94 1.56 .6925 .38 1.01Mapping n = 7 n = 6

Semantic Feature 1.187 1.06 1.32 .3695 .23 .51Analysis n = 10 n = 8

Syntactic/Semantic .91 .82 .99 1.39 .95 1.83Feature Analysis n = 4 n = 4

SM/SFA/SSFA 1.062 .93 1.19 1.013 .86 1.17Combination n = 8 n = 6

Visual Display .7486 .56 .93 .7877 .42 1.16n = 19 n = 5

Note. n = number of ESs.

but one study included GOs before and during instruc-tion, and not enough information was provided to dis-aggregate these data. One study, Englert and Mariage(1991), used GOs after instruction. The unstandardizedeffect size for a near-transfer, written free recall measurewas large (ES = 1.84).

DiscussionAs was the casae in previous research syntheses (e.g.,

Gajria et al., 2007; Kim et al., 2004; Moore & Readence,1984), findings from this meta-analysis indicate thatGOs improve the factual comprehension of upper-elementary, intermediate, and secondary students withLD. Unlike these previous reviews, this analysis alsoindicates that GOs may improve vocabulary and infer-ence/relational comprehension for students with LD.

What Are the Overall Effects of GOs on thePosttest Performance of Students With LD?

Overall, there was a large mean effect for posttest per-formance (ES = .91, SE = .06) of students with LD usingboth the fixed-effects and random-effects models. Theimmediate posttest performance spanned multiple con-structs, including multiple-choice factual comprehen-sion, vocabulary, and written recall requiring relationalcomprehension. This suggests that GOs are effective innot only improving basic skills (e.g., factual recall) butalso higher-level skills (e.g., inference). This finding is

consistent with the theories of Ausubel (1968) andMayer (1979) that GOs may especially assist lower abil-ity learners in both basic and higher level skills by cre-ating an easier context for assimilating information intotheir memory.

Do These Effects Maintain Over Time?There was a moderate mean effect for maintenance

(ES = .56, SE = .07) of students with LD. The significantdrop-off from posttest to maintenance is consistentwith the findings of the other GO research syntheses.The drop has been attributed to lack of clarity regardingthe duration and length of intervention sessions neededto positively affect maintenance (Gajria et al., 2007;Gersten, Fuchs, Williams, & Baker, 2001). The relativelyshort duration of the intervention studies (e.g., 1-7weeks) may not have provided sufficient instructiontime for students to use GOs independently. However, acloser look at effects by type of GO shows that effectsizes for visual displays and SSFA were larger for main-tenance than posttest. This may lend support to thevisual argument hypothesis (Waller, 1981), which positsthat GOs that are structured in a way that facilitatesunderstanding and perception of concept relationshipsare superior to more complicated GOs that may requireinstruction to recognize conceptual relationships(Dexter, 2010). The visual displays and SSFA were morecomputationally efficient than the other GOs. That is,

GRAPHIC ORGANIZERS AND LD 67

Table 6Subject Area – Random-Effects Model

Posttest Maintenance95% CI 95% CI

ES Lower Upper ES Lower Upper

English/Writing/ .9612 .72 1.20 N/A N/A N/An = 11 n = 0

Mathematics .5942 .21 .98 .4559 .07 .99n = 4 n = 2

Science 1.052 .88 1.23 .8035 .64 .97n = 23 n = 20

Social Studies 1.037 .85 1.22 .6535 .38 1.03n = 19 n = 8

Note. n = number of ESs.

they were simple enough for students to recognize con-ceptual relationships without teacher instruction. Thismay explain why maintenance effects were larger forthese types of GOs.

Are There Differential Effects by Type ofMeasure (Near or Far Transfer)?

This meta-analysis also separated results into near-transfer and far-transfer measures. Near-transfer results(i.e., measures applying knowledge to situationsdirectly covered in the text or lecture) indicate thatGOs are effective strategies for improving factual recall,factual and relational comprehension, and vocabularyknowledge. Across all near-transfer studies, the meaneffect size was large (ES = 1.07), and maintenanceeffects were moderate (ES = .78). Students using GOssignificantly outperformed their peers receiving typicalclassroom instruction on near-transfer measures.Interestingly, more complicated GOs requiring inten-sive teacher instruction (e.g., SM, SFA) resulted in thelargest effects for near-transfer posttest measures. Thisindicates that while these GOs are difficult to under-stand independently, with appropriate instruction theyare superior to less complicated GOs for immediate fac-tual recall.

Far-transfer results (i.e., measures applying knowl-edge to situations not directly covered in the text orlecture) indicate that GOs may also improve the infer-ence skills and relational knowledge of secondary stu-dents with LD. Across all far-transfer studies, the meaneffect size was moderate (ES = .61), and maintenanceeffects were moderate (ES = .69).

It is interesting to note that for far-transfer measures,maintenance effect sizes were larger than posttest effectsizes. Previous research has indicated students with LDtypically perform poorly on far-transfer tasks due totheir inability to detect underlying concepts in verbalinformation due to difficulty assimilating verbal infor-mation with previous knowledge (Suritsky & Hughes,1991). The results of this analysis demonstrate thatGOs may bridge the gap between verbal informationand prior knowledge and assist students with LD in far-transfer tasks. This finding supports Mayer’s (1979)assimilation theory, which posits that GOs that assim-ilate material to a broader set of past experiences allowsuperior transfer to new situations.

The finding is also consistent with the research ofRobinson and colleagues (Robinson et al., 1998;Robinson & Kiewra, 1995; Robinson & Schraw, 1994;Robinson & Skinner, 1996), comparing visual displays(e.g., tree diagrams, matrices, network charts) with tra-ditional, non-graphic outlines. In each of the studies,groups of nondisabled college students using GOs andtraditional outlines equally outperformed text-only

groups in factual recall, but the GO groups significantlyoutperformed the outline and text-only groups in iden-tifying concept relations and making far-transfer con-cept comparisons.

Are There Differential Effects by Subject Area?This analysis also examined the effects of GOs by

subject area. All subject areas had moderate to largeeffects for posttest and maintenance measures. Thelargest effects were in science (ES = 1.05 for posttest, ES= .80 for maintenance) and the smallest effects inmathematics (ES = .59 for posttest, ES = .46 for mainte-nance). The large effects in science may be explained bythe unfamiliar, technical vocabulary and content oftenbased on relationships between concepts (Lovitt et al.,1986). This type of content lends itself to computa-tionally efficient GOs that make relationships explicitand clear. Also, students may rely more heavily on con-tent enhancements like GOs when content seemsstrange or foreign.

The small effects for mathematics may be explainedby the fact that the information was much moreabstract than the other subject areas. It may also beexplained by the fact that the mathematics study useda visual display, which only had a moderate overalleffect. Use of GOs with mathematics concepts and solv-ing systems of linear equations are only beginning inthe field. Ives (2007) offerred several implications forfuture research based on his initial study in this field. Itwill take time and more study to fully understand theeffects of GOs on mathematics understanding.

Are There Differential Effects by Stage ofAttending to Verbal Material (Before, During,After Instruction)?

Finally, a previous GO research synthesis (Moore &Readence, 1984) reported that GOs presented as textsummaries after instruction were more effective thanGOs presented before or during instruction. This find-ing cannot be corroborated by the present analysisbecause only one study (Englert & Mariage, 1991) used GOs after instruction. While this study had anextremely large effect size (1.84), more studies areneeded to confirm this finding. The current analysispoints to effective instruction and choice of GO to bemore important than stage of attending to verbal mate-rial in effectiveness of the intervention.

Methodological LimitationsThere are two methodological limitations to the con-

clusions of this analysis. First, there is the possibility ofa publication bias. According to Smith (1980), as well asLipsey and Wilson (1993), published articles have alarger mean effect size than unpublished studies. Thisbias, also known as a “file-drawer” effect, happensbecause studies with null findings are less likely to be

68 DEXTER & HUGHES

published by major journals, often leaving offendingdata in a researcher’s file drawer (Lipsey & Wilson,2001). In this analysis, based on the advice of Slavin(1995), we purposefully selected only published studiesto ensure the highest quality of research designs. Whilethis eliminated our ability to compare mean effect sizesof published studies versus unpublished studies, wewere able to utilize Rosenthal’s (1979) fail-safe N statis-tic, which was later adapted by Orwin (1983). This sta-tistic determines “the number of studies with an effectsize of zero needed to reduce the mean effect size to aspecified or criterion level” (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001, p. 166).

The formula is as follows:

where k0 is the number of effect sizes of zero needed toreduce the mean effect size to ESc (criterion effect level),k is the number of current studies, and ESk is the cur-rent weighted mean effect size. Using this statistic,reduction of our overall weighted effect size of .91 to.50 would take 45 additional unpublished studies withan effect size of zero. While this may be a possibility, it is unlikely that 45 additional null studies exist inresearchers’ file drawers.

Second, our 55 unique effect sizes or studies wereculled from only 16 published articles. While this is an acceptable practice (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001), it doeslimit the generalizability of the findings because therewere only 21 distinct samples of students with LD (total N = 808). Therefore, caution should be exercisedin generalizing these findings to all intermediate andsecondary students with LD.

Individual Study LimitationsThree limitations to the individual studies warrant

consideration. First, while all of the effect sizes in thepresent analysis were based on differences between atreatment group and a control group, it is not clear ifthe control conditions constituted an adequate stan-dard to measure the effects of GO interventions(Gersten, Baker, & Lloyd, 2000). The control conditionsin the studies used primarily typical classroom practices(e.g., dictionary instruction) rather than more closelycomparable practices (e.g., outlines, structured over-views). While this provides much evidence for GOeffects over typical classroom practice, it does not yieldinformation comparing GOs with other researchedpractices (Kim et al., 2004)

Second, while the results indicate large effects forvocabulary, inference, and comprehension, it is impor-

tant to note that all but one study used measures thatwere researcher-created and closely tied to the content.While these measures should have good content valid-ity, there is no way to measure broader construct valid-ity. Only Boyle (1996) used a standardized measure forreading comprehension. This fact may limit the gener-alizability of these findings and questions the actuallevel of understanding obtained by students in the GOconditions.

Finally, studies using Content EnhancementRoutines were not included in this meta-analysisbecause their effects could not be attributed solely tothe GO, even though GOs are a vital component ofeach routine. Research on Content EnhancementRoutines has been conducted for over two decades,focusing primarily on assisting all students, includingthose with LD, thrive in the often rigorous intermedi-ate and secondary general education content class-rooms (Bulgren, 2006; Bulgren, Deshler, & Lenz, 2007These routines have been shown to improve both basicskills (e.g., factual knowledge, comprehension) andhigher-order skills (e.g., manipulation, extension, gen-eralization) for students with LD (e.g., Bulgren et al.,2000; Bulgren et al., 2002; Bulgren et al., 2009; Scanlonet al., 1996). While results of these studies did not fitinto our analysis, they do serve as examples of effectiveinterventions utilizing GOs as a component of a largerroutine.

Implications for PracticeThe major implication of this study for applied prac-

tice is consistent with assimilation theory and thevisual argument hypothesis; that is, more instruction-intensive types of GOs (e.g., SM, SFA) are better forimmediate factual recall while more computationallyefficient GOs (e.g., visual display, SSFA) are better formaintenance and transfer. This knowledge can helpteachers in designing GOs for initial instruction and forre-teaching, studying, and retention purposes. Forinstance, a semantic map for initial instruction, fol-lowed by a simpler visual display for review and studywill potentially maximize the effects of recall, mainte-nance, and far-transfer for students with LD.

Another implication for practice is that, regardless ofGO type, a teacher must explicitly teach students howto use a given GO. Students with LD need explicitinstruction to understand how concepts are related, torecognize differences between main and subordinateideas, and to put all the pieces together to make a clearpicture of the content being learned no matter howimplicit a GO may seem. A teacher’s use of effectiveinstruction practices (e.g., modeling, corrective feed-back, etc.) will positively impact the intervention’seffectiveness.

GRAPHIC ORGANIZERS AND LD 69

k0 = k - 1ESk

ESc[ ] ,

Conclusions and Implications for FutureResearch

This meta-analysis found that, in comparison to activ-ities such as reading text passages, attending to lectures,and participating in typical classroom practice (e.g., dic-tionary instruction), GOs are more effective on posttest,maintenance, and transfer measures. However, this con-clusion must be tempered for several reasons.

First, each of the studies took place in self-containedresource classrooms. This may not be typical for today’supper-elementary, intermediate, and secondary stu-dents with LD. As many students with LD are now fullyincluded with nondisabled peers in core contentclasses, it is important to closely examine how GOs willwork in this setting. The feasibility and practicality ofGOs must be examined in general education settingsand recommendations for effective use put forth.

Second, there is great need for GO replication studies.Only three articles in the present meta-analysis werepublished in the past 10 years. More current groupdesign, randomized control trials, is needed to fully val-idate the benefits of GOs across all secondary studentswith LD.

Finally, for student independent practice, it was notalways clear from the studies if the GO was used cor-rectly or at all. For instance, when students independ-ently filled in a blank GO, there was no reportedprocedure for ascertaining if they were properly labelingmain and subordinate details. Likewise, several of thestudies (e.g., Anders, Bos, & Filip, 1984; Bos & Anders,1990; Bos & Anders, 1992) reported students had a GOand text to study for the posttests. They did not includea procedure for making sure the students were actuallyusing the GO to study. These students may have beenusing the text as their study guide. This lack of controlmay somewhat negate the attribution of effects to theGO. Future research must tightly control for thesepotential problem areas.

Taking the above issues into account, the evidence inthis analysis should still persuade educational practi-tioners to make well-planned and well-instructed use ofGOs. There were no significant negative effects acrossany of the categories of analysis and no other identifieddetrimental effect. A thoughtful combination of typesof GOs will help make the learning process more effi-cient for upper-elementary, intermediate, and second-ary students with LD.

References*Anders, P. L., Bos, C. S., & Filip, D. (1984). The effect of semantic

feature analysis on the reading comprehension of learning dis-abled students. In J. A. Niles & L. A. Harris (Eds.), Changing per-spectives on reading/language processing and instruction (pp.162-166). Rochester, NY: National Reading Conference.

Armbruster, B. B. (1984). The problems of “inconsiderate” text. InG. G. Duffy, L. R. Roehler, & J. M. Mason (Eds.), Comprehensioninstruction: Perspectives and suggestions (pp. 202-217). New York,NY: Longman.

Ausubel, D. P. (1968). Educational psychology: A cognitive view. NewYork, NY: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

Baumann, J. F. (1984). The effectiveness of a direct instruction par-adigm for teaching main idea comprehension. Reading ResearchQuarterly, 20, 93-115.

Bergerud, D., Lovitt, T. C., & Horton, S. (1988). The effectivenessof textbook adaptations in life science for high school studentswith learning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 21, 70-76.

Boon, R. T., Fore III, C., Ayres, K., & Spencer, V. G. (2005). Theeffects of cognitive organizers to facilitate content-area learningfor students with mild disabilities: A pilot study. Journal ofInstructional Psychology, 32, 101-117.

*Bos, C. S., & Anders, P. L. (1990). Effects of interactive vocabu-lary instruction on the vocabulary learning and reading com-pre-hension of junior-high learning disabled students. LearningDisability Quarterly, 13, 31-42.

*Bos, C. S., & Anders, P. L. (1992). Using interactive teaching andlearning strategies to promote text comprehension and contentlearning for students with learning disabilities. InternationalJournal of Disability, Development, and Education, 39, 225-238.

*Bos, C. S., Anders, P. L., Filip, D., & Jaffe, L. E. (1989). The effectsof an interactive instructional strategy for enhancing readingcomprehension and content area learning for students withlearning disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 22, 384-389.

Bos, C. S., & Vaughn, S. (2002). Strategies for teaching students withlearning and behavior problems (5th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn &Bacon.

*Boyle, J. R. (1996). The effects of a cognitive mapping strategy onthe literal and inferential comprehension of students with milddisabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 19, 86-98.

*Boyle, J. R. (2000). The effects of a Venn diagram strategy on theliteral, inferential, and relational comprehension of studentswith mild disabilities. Learning Disabilities: A MultidisciplinaryJournal, 10, 5-13.

Boyle, J. R., & Weishaar, M. (1997). The effects of expert-generatedversus student-generated cognitive organizers on the readingcomprehension of students with learning disabilities. LearningDisabilities Research & Practice, 12, 228- 235.

Boyle, J. R., & Yeager, N. (1997). Blueprints for learning: Using cog-nitive frameworks for understanding. Teaching ExceptionalChildren, 29, 26-31.

Brigham, F. J., Scruggs, T. E., & Mastropieri, M. A. (1995).Elaborative maps for enhanced learning of historical informa-tion: Uniting spatial, verbal, and imaginable information. TheJournal of Special Education, 28, 440-460.

Bryant, D. P., Ugel, N., Thompson, S., & Hamff, A. (1999).Instructional strategies for content-area reading instruction.Intervention in School and Clinic, 34, 293-302.

Bulgren, J. A. (2006). Integrated content enhancement routines:Responding to the needs of adolescents with disabilities in rig-orous inclusive secondary content classes. Teaching ExceptionalChildren, 38, 54-58.

Bulgren, J. A., Deshler, D. D., Schumaker, J. B., & Lenz, B. K.(2000). The use and effectiveness of analogical instruction indiverse secondary content classrooms. Journal of EducationalPsychology, 92, 426-441.

Bulgren, J., Deshler, D. D., & Lenz, B. K. (2007). Engaging adoles-cents with LD in higher order thinking about history concepts

70 DEXTER & HUGHES

using integrated content enhancement routines. Journal ofLearning Disabilities, 40, 121-133.

Bulgren, J. A., Lenz, B. K., Schumaker, J. B., Deshler, D. D., &Marquis, J. G. (2002). The use and effectiveness of a comparisonroutine in diverse secondary content classrooms. Journal ofEducational Psychology, 94, 356-371.

Bulgren, J. A., Marquis, J. G., Lenz, B. K., Schumaker, J. B., &Deshler, D. D. (2009). Effectiveness of question exploration toenhance students’ written expression of content knowledge andcomprehension. Reading and Writing Quarterly, 25, 271-289.

Bulgren, J. A., Schumaker, J. B., & Deshler, D. D. (1988). Effective-ness of a concept teaching routine in enhancing the perform-ance of LD students in secondary-level mainstream classes.Learning Disabilities Quarterly, 11, 3-17.

Burns, M. (1994). Empirical analysis of drill ratio research.Remedial and Special Education, 25, 167-173.

Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences(2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

*Darch, C., & Carnine, D. (1986). Teaching content area materialto learning disabled students. Exceptional Children, 53, 240-246.

*Darch, C. B., Carnine, D. W., & Kame’enui, E. J. (1986). The roleof graphic organizers and social structure in content areainstruction. Journal of Reading Behavior, 18, 275-295.

*Darch, C., & Eaves, R. (1986). Visual displays to increase compre-hension of high school learning-disabled students. The Journalof Special Education, 20, 309-318.

*Darch, C., & Gersten, R. (1986). Direction-setting activities inreading comprehension: A comparison of two approaches.Learning Disability Quarterly, 9, 235-243.

Deshler, D. D., Ellis, E., & Lenz, B. K. (1996). Teaching adolescentswith learning disabilities: Strategies and methods. Denver, CO:Love.

Dexter, D. D. (2010). Graphic organizers and their effectiveness forstudents with learning disabilities. Thalamus, 26, 51-67.

*DiCecco, V. M., & Gleason, M. M. (2002). Using graphic organiz-ers to attain relational knowledge from expository text. Journalof Learning Disabilities, 35, 306-320.

*Englert, C. S., & Mariage, T. V. (1991). Making students partnersin the comprehension process: Organizing the reading POSSE.Learning Disability Quarterly, 14, 123-138.

Fletcher, J. M., Lyon, G. R., Fuchs, L. S., & Barnes, M. A. (2007).Learning disabilities: From identification to intervention. New York,NY: The Guilford Press.

Gajria, M., Jitendra, A. K., Sood, S., & Sacks, G. (2007). Improvingcomprehension of expository text in students with LD: Aresearch synthesis. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 40, 210-225.

Gardill, M. C., & Jitendra, A. K. (1999). Advanced story mapinstruction: Effects on the reading comprehension of studentswith learning disabilities. The Journal of Special Education, 33, 2-17.

Gersten, R., Baker, S., & Lloyd, J. W. (2000). Designing high-qual-ity research in special education: Group experimental design.The Journal of Special Education, 34, 2-18.

Gersten, R., Fuchs, L. S., Williams, J. P., & Baker, S. (2001).Teaching reading comprehension strategies to students withlearning disabilities: A review of research. Review of EducationalResearch, 71, 279-320.

*Griffin, C. C., Simmons, D. C., & Kame’enui, E. J. (1991).Investigating the effectiveness of graphic organizers instructionon the comprehension and recall of science content by studentswith learning disabilities. Reading, Writing, and LearningDisabilities, 7, 355-376.

Hedges, L. V. (1981). Distribution theory for Glass’s estimator ofeffect size and related estimators. Journal of Educational Statistics,

6, 107-128.Hedges, L. V., Shymansky, J. A., & Woodworth, G. (1989). A prac-

tical guide to modern methods of meta-analysis. Washington, DC:National Science Teachers Association.

Holmes, B. C. (1985). The effects of a strategy and sequenced mate-rial on the inferential comprehension of disabled readers.Journal of Learning Disabilities, 18, 542-546.

Horton, P. B., McConney, A. A., Gallo, M., Woods, A. L., Senn, G.J., & Hamelin, D. (1993). An investigation of the effectiveness ofconcept mapping as an instructional tool. Science Education,77, 95-111.

Horton, S. V., Lovitt, T. C., & Bergerud, D. (1990). The effective-ness of graphic organizers for three classifications of secondarystudents in content area classes. Journal of Learning Disabilities,23, 12-22.

*Hudson, P. (1996). Using a learning set to increase the test per-formance of students with learning disabilities in social studiesclasses. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 11, 78-85.

Hughes, C. A., Maccini, P., & Gagnon, J. C. (2003). Interventionsthat positively impact the performance of students with learn-ing disabilities in secondary general education classes. LearningDisabilities, 12, 101-111.

Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (2004). Methods of meta-analysis:Correcting error and bias in research findings. Thousand Oaks, CA:Sage Publications, Inc.

Idol, L., & Croll, V. J. (1987). Story-mapping training as a means of improving reading comprehension. Learning DisabilityQuarterly, 10, 214-229.

*Ives, B. (2007). Graphic organizers applied to secondary algebrainstruction for students with learning disorders. LearningDisabilities Research & Practice, 22, 110-118.

Ives, B., & Hoy, C. (2003). Graphic organizers applied to higher-level secondary mathematics. Learning Disabilities Research &Practice, 18, 36-51.

Johnson, L., Graham, S., & Harris, K. R. (1997). The effects of goalsetting and self-instructions on learning a reading comprehen-sion strategy: A study with students with learning disabilities.Journal of Learning Disabilities, 30, 80-91.

Kim, A., Vaughn, S., Wanzek, J., & Wei, S. (2004). Graphic organ-izers and their effects on the reading comprehension of studentswith LD: A synthesis of research. Journal of Learning Disabilities,37, 105-118.

Kulhavy, R. W., Stock, W. A., & Caterino, L. C. (1994). Referencemaps as a framework for remembering text. In W. Schnotz & R.W. Kulhavy (Eds.). Comprehension of graphics (pp. 153-162). NewYork, NY: Elsevier Science.

Lenz, B. K., Adams, G. L., Bulgren, J. A., Pouliot, N., & Laraux, M.(2007). Effects of curriculum maps and guiding questions on thetest performance of adolescents with learning disabilities.Learning Disability Quarterly, 30, 235-244.

Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (1993). The efficacy of psychologi-cal, educational, and behavioral treatment. The AmericanPsychologist, 48, 1181-1201.

Lipsey, M. W., & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis (Vol.49). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications, Inc.

Lovitt, T., Rudsit, J., Jenkins, J., Pious, C., & Benedetti, D. (1986).Adapting science materials for regular and learning disabled sev-enth graders. Remedial and Special Education, 7, 31-39.

Mastropieri, M. A., Scruggs, T. E., & Levin, J. R. (1987). Learning-disabled students’ memory for expository prose: Mnemonic ver-sus nonmnemonic pictures. American Educational ResearchJournal, 24, 505-519.

Mayer, R. E. (1979). Can advanced organizers influence meaning-ful learning? Review of Educational Research, 49, 371-383.

GRAPHIC ORGANIZERS AND LD 71

Minskoff, E., & Allsopp, D. (2003). Academic success strategies foradolescents with learning disabilities and ADHD. Baltimore, MD:Brookes Publishing.

Moore, D. W., & Readence, J. F. (1984). A quantitative and quali-tative review of graphic organizer research. Journal of EducationalResearch, 78, 11-17.

Nesbit, J. C., & Adesope, O. O. (2006). Learning with concept and knowledge maps: A meta-analysis. Review of EducationalResearch, 76, 413- 448.

Orwin, R. G. (1983). A fail-safe N for effect size in meta-analysis.Journal of Educational Statistics, 8, 157-159.

Pearson, P. D., & Johnson, D. (1978). Teaching reading comprehen-sion. New York, NY: Holt, Rinehart, & Winston.

Pehrsson, R., & Denner, P. (1988). Semantic organizers:Implications for reading and writing. Topics in LanguageDisorders, 8, 24-37.

*Reyes, E. I., Gallego, M. A., Duran, G. Z., & Scanlon, D. J. (1989).Integration of internal concepts and external factors: Extendingthe knowledge of learning disabled adolescents. The Journal ofEarly Adolescence, 9, 112-124.

Rice, G. E. (1994). Need for explanations in graphic organizerresearch. Reading Psychology: An International Quarterly, 15, 39-67.

Rivera, D. P., & Smith, D. (1997). Teaching students with learningand behavior problems (3rd ed.). Boston, MD: Allyn & Bacon.

Robinson, D. H., Katayama, A. D., DuBois, N. F., & Devaney, T.(1998). Interactive effects of graphic organizers and delayedreview on concept acquisition. Journal of Experimental Education,67, 17-31.

Robinson, D. H., & Kierwa, K. A. (1995). Visual argument: Graphicorganizers are superior to outlines in improving learning fromtext. Journal of Educational Psychology, 87, 455-476.

Robinson, D. H., & Schraw, G. (1994). Computational efficiencythrough visual argument: Do graphic organizers communicaterelations in text too effectively? Contemporary EducationalPsychology, 19, 399-415.

Robinson, D .H., & Skinner, C. H. (1996). Why graphic organizersfacilitate search processes: Fewer words or computationally efficient indexing? Contemporary Educational Psychology, 21,166-180.

Rosenthal, R. (1979). The “file-drawer” problem and tolerance fornull results. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 638-641.

Rosenthal, R. (1994). Statistically describing and combining stud-ies. In H. Cooper & L. V. Hedges (Eds.), The handbook of researchsynthesis (pp. 231-244). New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation.

Scanlon, D., Deshler, D. D., & Schumaker, J. (1996). Can a strat-egy be taught and learned in secondary inclusive classrooms?Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 11, 41-57.

Sinatra, R. C., Stahl-Gemake, J., & Berg, D. N. (1984). Improvingreading comprehension of disabled readers through mapping.The Reading Teacher, 38, 22-29.

Slavin, R. E. (1995). Best evidence synthesis: An intelligent alter-native to meta-analysis. Journal of Clinical Epidemiology, 48, 9-18.

Smith, M. L. (1980). Publication bias and meta-analysis.Evaluation in Education, 4, 22-24.

Sturm, J. M., & Rankin-Erickson, J. L. (2002). Effects of hand-drawn and computer-generated concept mapping on the expos-itory writing of middle school students with learningdisabilities. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 17, 124-139.

Suritsky, S. K., & Hughes, C. A. (1991). Benefits of notetaking:Implications for secondary and postsecondary students withlearning disabilities. Learning Disability Quarterly, 14, 7-18.

Swanson, H. L., Hoskyn, M., & Lee, C. (1999). Interventions for stu-dents with learning disabilities. New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Thalheimer, W., & Cook, S. (2002, August). How to calculate effectsizes from published research articles: A simplified methodology.Retrieved from http://work-learning.com/effect_sizes.htm.

Waller, R. (1981, April). Understanding network diagrams. Paper pre-sented at the annual meeting of the American EducationalResearch Association, Los Angeles.

Williams, J. P. (1993). Comprehension of students with or withoutlearning disabilities: Identification of narrative themes and idio-syncratic text representations. Journal of Educational Psychology,85, 631-641.

* = Studies included in meta-analysis.

Please address correspondence about this article to: Douglas D.Dexter, 230 CEDAR, The Pennsylvania State University, UniversityPark, PA 16802; email:[email protected]

72 DEXTER AND HUGHES.