„follow my lead“: what follows after one child‘s ... · „follow my lead“: what follows...

1
„Follow my lead“: What follows after one child‘s initiative in preschooler triads in a cooperative task? Paula Döge & Heidi Keller Department Development & Culture Preschooler triads (N=20) of either girls or boys at the age of 62 months (SD = 6.2 months; range = 48-71 months) were videotaped during the task of cooperatively replicating a tangram figure. References: Fabes, R. A., Martin, C. L. & Hanish, L. D. (2011). Children’s Behaviors and Interactions with Peers. In K.H. Rubin, W. M. Bukowski & B. Laursen (Eds.). Handbook of Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups (pp. 45-62). New York/London: Guilford. Ishikawa, F. & Hay, D. F. (2006). Triadic interaction among newly acquainted 2-year-olds. Social Development, 15(1), 145-168. A total number of 179 initiatives was identified. There were more nonverbal initiatives than verbal in both boy and girl groups (figure 1). In the majority of cases group members did not react to one child‘s initiative (figure 2). Girls and boys did not dier in the percentage of reaction types [Χ 2 (2)=1.669, n.s.], but there were dierences between reaction types to verbal and nonverbal initiatives [Χ 2 (2) =7.539, p = .023]. Contact details: Paula Döge University of Osnabrück / Lower Saxonian Institute for early education and child development Email: [email protected] 1. Identification of initiatives 2. Coding of behavioral responses 3. Analyses of sequences Introduction Methods Results All verbal and nonverbal initiatives were identified. Behavioral responses of the other children within a 5 second interval following the initiative were coded as either support or rejection. The absence of any response by the other children was coded as no reaction. Behavioral sequences resulted when the initiating child acted again. 65% 35% female groups nonverbal verbal 73% 27% male groups Figure 1 Proportion of verbal and nonverbal initiatives in female and male groups Figure 2 Proportion of reaction codes after verbal and nonverbal initiatives reaction of initiating child no reaction repeating/ continuing new initiative Σ peer reaction no reaction 89 22 9 120 support 23 10 1 34 rejection 13 12 0 25 Σ 125 44 10 179 Table 1 Crosstabulation of peer and initiating child reaction Involvement (Figure 3): As a measure for children’s degree of interaction three levels of involvement were dierentiated. There was no dierence between boy and girl groups, but involvement levels diered significantly after verbal and nonverbal initiatives [Χ 2 (2)=13.067, p = .001]. Approximately one half of all initiatives were neither followed by a peer reaction nor any reaction of the initiating child. Peer support seldom lead to new initiatives. More often the initiating child showed no reaction, thus awaiting peer involvement. The initiating child did not suggest any new initiative after rejection of the first initiative. The child either did not show any further reaction or repeated / continued his or her first initiative. If there was not any reaction of peers, the initiating child more often repeated or continued the first initiative rather than suggesting a new one. Peer interactions play an important role in children‘s everyday life in institutional daycare. Sustaining social interactions requires skills as attending to the interaction partner(s), mastering turn-taking and prosocial behavior (Fabes, Martin & Hanish, 2011). If cooperation is needed to master a task, these skills become even more important. Peer triads represent a complex setting of interactional possibilities of all three children (Ishikawa & Hay, 2006). Initiatives constitute starting points to analyze how social interaction is negotiated in a cooperative task. By suggesting how to proceed one child oers opportunities for social practices. The other children’s reactions to the initiative are indicative for the involvement and social structure. We therefore ask: (1) To what extent and how are initiatives responded to by the other group members? (2) Are there dierences between boy and girl groups? (3) What behavioral interaction sequence follows each initiative? Behavioral sequences (Figure 4): The analysis focusses on the number of sequential steps that followed the initiative and how many peers contributed to the interaction. Significantly dierent behavioral sequences followed after verbal and nonverbal initiatives [Χ 2 (3)=25.655, p = .000], but there was no dierence between boy and girl groups [Χ 2 (3) =2.476, n.s.]. Discussion Figure 3 Percentage of high, medium and low involvement of the children 53% 22% 25% verbal initiatives no reaction support rejection 73% 10% 17% nonverbal initiatives Verbal and nonverbal initiatives to solve a cooperation task were analyzed in preschooler triads. Children generally showed little interaction after one child’s initiative and ‘no reaction’ dominated as response pattern in both boys and girls groups. However, the modus of the initiative was crucial: Peers reacted more often to verbal initiatives and more interaction between children resulted. Nonverbal initiatives seemed to be less identifiable as interaction prompts for children at this age. It is not known whether this characterizes a certain developmental stage in related socio-cognitive skills or whether these results reflect children’s preference for verbal over nonverbal communication in an interactive problem-solving task with peers. Figure 4 Percentage of dierent behavioral sequence types 52.8% 73% 20.8% 19.8% 26.4% 7.2% verbal nonverbal high medium low 24.5% 60.3% 28.3% 12.7% 20.8% 19.8% 26.4% 7.2% verbal nonverbal three-step two-step single no 22 nd ISSBD Biennal Meeting, July 8-12, 2012, Edmonton, Canada

Upload: ngotuyen

Post on 14-Nov-2018

216 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

„Follow my lead“: What follows after one child‘s initiative in preschooler triads in a cooperative task? Paula Döge & Heidi Keller

Department Development & Culture

Preschooler triads (N=20) of either girls or boys at the age of 62 months (SD = 6.2 months; range = 48-71 months) were videotaped during the task of cooperatively replicating a tangram figure.

References: Fabes, R. A., Martin, C. L. & Hanish, L. D. (2011). Children’s Behaviors and Interactions with Peers. In K.H. Rubin, W. M. Bukowski & B. Laursen (Eds.). Handbook of Peer Interactions, Relationships, and Groups (pp. 45-62). New York/London: Guilford. Ishikawa, F. & Hay, D. F. (2006). Triadic interaction among newly acquainted 2-year-olds. Social Development, 15(1), 145-168.

A total number of 179 initiatives was identified. There were more nonverbal initiatives than verbal in both boy and girl groups (figure 1). In the majority of cases group members did not react to one child‘s initiative (figure 2). Girls and boys did not differ in the percentage of reaction types [Χ2 (2)=1.669, n.s.], but there were differences between reaction types to verbal and nonverbal initiatives [Χ2 (2)=7.539, p = .023].

Contact details: Paula Döge University of Osnabrück / Lower Saxonian Institute for early education and child development Email: [email protected]

1. Identification of initiatives

2. Coding of behavioral responses

3. Analyses of sequences

Introduction

Methods

Results

All verbal and nonverbal initiatives were identified. Behavioral responses of the other children within a 5 second interval following the initiative were coded as either support or rejection. The absence of any response by the other children was coded as no reaction. Behavioral sequences resulted when the initiating child acted again.

65%

35%

female groups

nonverbal verbal

73%

27%

male groups

Figure 1 Proportion of verbal and nonverbal initiatives in female and male groups

Figure 2 Proportion of reaction codes after verbal and nonverbal initiatives

reaction of initiating child no reaction repeating/

continuing new

initiative Σ

peer reaction

no reaction 89 22 9 120 support 23 10 1 34 rejection 13 12 0 25 Σ 125 44 10 179

Table 1 Crosstabulation of peer and initiating child reaction Involvement (Figure 3): As a measure for children’s degree of interaction three levels of involvement were differentiated. There was no difference between boy and girl groups, but involvement levels differed significantly after verbal and nonverbal initiatives [Χ2 (2)=13.067, p = .001].

Approximately one half of all initiatives were neither followed by a peer reaction nor any reaction of the initiating child.

Peer support seldom lead to new initiatives. More often the initiating child showed no reaction, thus awaiting peer involvement.

The initiating child did not suggest any new initiative after rejection of the first initiative. The child either did not show any further reaction or repeated / continued his or her first initiative.

If there was not any reaction of peers, the initiating child more often repeated or continued the first initiative rather than suggesting a new one.

Peer interactions play an important role in children‘s everyday life in institutional daycare. Sustaining social interactions requires skills as attending to the interaction partner(s), mastering turn-taking and prosocial behavior (Fabes, Martin & Hanish, 2011). If cooperation is needed to master a task, these skills become even more important. Peer triads represent a complex setting of interactional possibilities of all three children (Ishikawa & Hay, 2006). Initiatives constitute starting points to analyze how social interaction is negotiated in a cooperative task. By suggesting how to proceed one child offers opportunities for social practices. The other children’s reactions to the initiative are indicative for the involvement and social structure. We therefore ask:

(1)  To what extent and how are initiatives responded to by the other group members? (2)  Are there differences between boy and girl groups? (3)  What behavioral interaction sequence follows each initiative?

Behavioral sequences (Figure 4): The analysis focusses on the number of sequential steps that followed the initiative and how many peers contributed to the interaction. Significantly different behavioral sequences followed after verbal and nonverbal initiatives [Χ2 (3)=25.655, p = .000], but there was no difference between boy and girl groups [Χ2 (3)=2.476, n.s.].

Discussion

Figure 3 Percentage of high, medium and low involvement of the children

53% 22%

25%

verbal initiatives

no reaction

support

rejection 73%

10%

17%

nonverbal initiatives

Verbal and nonverbal initiatives to solve a cooperation task were analyzed in preschooler triads. Children generally showed little interaction after one child’s initiative and ‘no reaction’ dominated as response pattern in both boys and girls groups. However, the modus of the initiative was crucial: Peers reacted more often to verbal initiatives and more interaction between children resulted. Nonverbal initiatives seemed to be less identifiable as interaction prompts for children at this age. It is not known whether this characterizes a certain developmental stage in related socio-cognitive skills or whether these results reflect children’s preference for verbal over nonverbal communication in an interactive problem-solving task with peers.

Figure 4 Percentage of different behavioral sequence types

52.8%

73%

20.8%

19.8% 26.4%

7.2%

verbal nonverbal

high medium low

24.5%

60.3% 28.3%

12.7% 20.8%

19.8% 26.4% 7.2%

verbal nonverbal

three-step two-step single no

22nd ISSBD Biennal Meeting, July 8-12, 2012, Edmonton, Canada