eye tracking i socijalne situacije

8

Click here to load reader

Upload: djordje-djurica

Post on 02-Jun-2018

213 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Eye Tracking i Socijalne Situacije

8/10/2019 Eye Tracking i Socijalne Situacije

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/eye-tracking-i-socijalne-situacije 1/8

Visual Fixation Patterns During Viewingof Naturalistic Social Situations as Predictorsof Social Competence in Individuals With Autism Ami Klin, PhD; Warren Jones, BA; Robert Schultz, PhD; Fred Volkmar, MD; Donald Cohen, MD

Background: Manifestations of core social deficits inautism are more pronounced in everyday settings thaninexplicit experimental tasks. Tobring experimental mea-sures in line with clinical observation, we report a novelmethod of quantifyingatypical strategies of social moni-toring in a setting that simulates the demands of dailyexperience. Enhanced ecological validity was intended

to maximize between-group effect sizes and assess thepredictive utility of experimentalvariablesrelative to out-come measures of social competence.

Methods: Whileviewing socialscenes,eye-tracking tech-nology measured visual fixations in 15 cognitively ablemales with autism and 15 age-, sex-, and verbal IQ–matchedcontrolsubjects. We reliably coded fixationson4 regions: mouth, eyes, body, andobjects. Statisticalanaly-ses compared fixation time on regions of interest be-tween groups and correlation of fixation time with out-come measures of social competence (ie, standardized

measures of daily social adjustment and degree of autis-tic social symptoms).

Results: Significant between-group differences were ob-tained for all 4 regions. The best predictor of autism wasreduced eyeregion fixation time. Fixation on mouths andobjects was significantly correlated with social function-

ing: increasedfocus on mouths predicted improvedsocialadjustment and less autistic social impairment, whereasmore time on objects predicted the opposite relationship.

Conclusions: When viewing naturalistic social situa-tions, individuals with autism demonstrate abnormalpat-terns of social visual pursuit consistent with reduced sa-lienceofeyes andincreasedsalienceofmouths, bodies, andobjects. Fixation times on mouths and objects but not oneyes are strong predictors of degree of social competence.

Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59:809-816

ADVANCES IN psychologicalresearch of the core socialdeficits in autism have in-creasingly focused on dis-ruptions in early-emerging

skills that seemtoderail theprocesses ofso-cialization. 1 Typically developing infantsshow preferentialattention to social ratherthan inanimate stimuli, 2 and they alsopre-fer to focus on the more socially revealingfeatures of the face, such as the eyes ratherthan the mouth 3; in contrast, individualswith autism seem to lack these early socialpredispositions. 4-8 This attenuation to the

social world is accompanied by docu-mented abnormalities in face perception,both in face recognition and in identifica-tion of facialexpressions. 9-12 Inaddition, in-dividuals with autism use atypical strate-gies when performing such tasks,relying onindividual piecesof the face rather than ontheoverall configuration. 5,8 Alongsidetheseperceptual anomalies, individuals withautism have deficits in conceiving otherpeople’s mental states (inhavinga “theory”

that other people havemindsand then us-ing this knowledge to predict their socialbehavior). 13

Although these findingsoffer specifichypotheses fortheprofound impairment insocial adjustment exhibited by individualswith autism, there is no performance-based method for directly quantifying theimpactof these underlying processesonso-cialfunctioningin everydaysituations.Foran individualwith autism, thereal worldisfraughtwithsocial challenges andambigu-ities that are largely minimized within thenarrow parameters of experimental situa-

tions. Individuals with autism encountergreater difficulty when trying to spontane-ously imposesocialmeaningontowhattheysee than they do when solving explicittasks. 14 This is particularly evident in cog-nitively able individuals,whomayusetheirvisual-perceptual or language abilities toscaffold their performance, thus achievingrelatively high scoresona giventask butdo-ing so in ways that differ from normativestrategies. 15 This hypothesis was recently

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

From the Yale Child StudyCenter, Yale University,New Haven, Conn.

(REPRINTED) ARCH GEN PSYCHIATRY/VOL 59, SEP 2002 WWW.ARCHGENPSYCHIATRY.COM809

©2002 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. on December 27, 2011www.archgenpsychiatry.comDownloaded from

Page 2: Eye Tracking i Socijalne Situacije

8/10/2019 Eye Tracking i Socijalne Situacije

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/eye-tracking-i-socijalne-situacije 2/8

substantiated in a neurofunctional study 16 of face percep-tion inautismin whichadequate task performancewas ac-

companied by abnormal ventral temporal cortical activ-ity, whichin turn suggested that participantshad “treated ”faces as objects.

Thedevelopmentofmorenaturalisticparadigms thatcapitalize on the challenging nature of open-ended socialscenarios for individuals with autism could result in sev-eral advantages.First, theeffectsize ofbetween-group find-ings may increase, bringing experimentally measured ab-normalities in autism to a level more commensurate withclinicalobservationsofsocial impairment. Second, byplac-inga performance-basedmethod into thecontext of natu-ralistic socialfunctioning, there is increased likelihood thattheexperimentalmethodwill better predict level of socialcompetence.Abnormalperformanceon social tasksin au-

tism is typically reported as a group finding relative to acontrol sample, with little attempt to use the given mea-sure as a predictor of level of social adjustment or of levelof social impairment.Thelack ofquantifiable indicesofso-cial competence that coulddefinea spectrumofsocial out-comes, from normality to varying manifestations of au-tism, hindersgeneticandneurofunctional research, whichpartially rely on such indices for interpretation of herita-bility and neuroimaging data. 17,18

To create an experimental paradigm to measure so-cial functioning in a context that more closely resemblessocial demands in naturalistic situations, we used eye-trackingtechnologytostudyspontaneousviewingpatternsof cognitively able individuals with autism and age- and

verbalIQ –matchedcontrolsubjects.Previouswork19,20

hasusedstaticimages toassessaspectsofface scanning;to ourknowledge, this is the first study of eye tracking as a dy-namicphenomenon.Theparadigminvolvesviewing digi-tizedvideotapeclipsofcomplexsocialsituationswhilewear-inga noninvasive eye-trackingdevice that superimposesthe viewer ’s point of regard onto the viewed scenes. Theresultant videotape can then be coded for patterns of vi-sual fixation that canbe measuredin termsofpercentageofviewingtimespentondifferentaspectsofthesocialscenes.Forthisexploratory study, several linesof researchguided

ourdecision as to thescheme to useforcodingfixations.First, we were interested in the relative salience of majorcomponentsof theviewedscenes. 4,8,14,15 Thus, wedividedthetotalon-screenarea intoface,body,andobjectregions.Second,giventhe researchresults suggestingthat individu-als with autism have difficulty interpreting social infor-mation conveyedthrough the eyes, 6,7 andthat they focuspreferentially on mouths when performing face percep-tiontasks, 5 we subdivided theface into an eyeregion and

a mouth region. Theresultant codingscheme, therefore,consisted of4 clearlydelineatedviewingareas of interest:mouth,eye,body,andobjects(seethe “CodingProcedures ”subsection forfurtherdetails). Wewerealso interested inexploringthe relationship between patternsofvisualfixa-tion and outcome measures of social competence. Weoperationalized socialcompetence in termsof levelof real-life socialadjustment (asdefined byscoreson thesocial-izationdomainof theVinelandAdaptive Behavior Scales,ExpandedEdition [VABS-E] 21) and intermsof degreeof autisticsocialsymptoms (asdefinedbythesocialdomainoftheAutismDiagnosticObservationSchedule[ADOS] 22). Whereas the VABS-E providesa measure of social ability(higher scores mean higher levels of social adjustment),

theADOSprovidesameasureofsocial impairment(higherscores mean higher levels of autistic social behaviors).On the basis of the extant literature, we expected

that individuals with autism would preferentially focuson the mouth region rather than on the eye region andon objects relative to controls. In addition, given thenor-mative pattern of preferentially using information gath-ered from the eyes to understand others, we also pre-dicted that within the autistic group, higher percentageof viewing time on the eye region would be positivelycorrelated with level of social adjustment andnegativelycorrelated with level of social impairment.

PARTICIPANTS AND METHODS

PARTICIPANTS

Fifteen male adolescents and young adults with autism wererecruited througha large, federally funded research project onthe neurobiology of autism carried out in the developmentaldisabilities section of theYale Child Study Center. This projectincludes a 3-day protocol consisting of extensive diagnostic,neuropsychological, neuroimaging, and genetic studies. Be-fore participation, all individuals or their legal guardians sup-plied written informed consent. The protocol wasapproved bythe Human Investigations Committee of the Yale UniversitySchool of Medicine. Diagnoses were assigned on the basis of parental interview (Autism Diagnostic Interview –Revised23) anddirect observations of participants ’ social and communicative

behaviors (ADOS22

). All participants with autism met DSM-IV 24 diagnostic criteria for autistic disorder as operationalizedthrough standardized algorithms derived from the Autism Di-agnostic Interview –RevisedandtheADOS. Intellectual level wasmeasured using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children 25

or the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. 26 All 15 participantswith autism were cognitively able and yet severely impaired intheir social functioning, as measured using theVABS-E 19:adis-crepancy of more than 3.5 SD was obtained between their ver-bal IQ and standard scores on the socialization domain of theVABS-E (see Table 1 for a summary of participant character-ization data). Participantswithautism were individually matched

Table 1. Participant Characterization Data *

AutismGroup

(n = 15)

ControlGroup

(n = 15)t

ValueP

Value

Age, y 15.4 (7.2) 17.9 (5.6) −1.070 .29VIQ† 101.3 (24.9) 102.5 (20.4) −0.136 .89Vineland socialization

standard score†46.7 (12.7) . . . . . . . . .

Vineland socialization age-equivalent score 4.8 (2.6) . . . . . . . . .ADOS socialization

algorithm totals‡9.7 (2.9) . . . . . . . . .

* Data are given as mean (SD).†VIQ indicates verbal IQ and is derived from the Wechsler Intelligence Scale

for Children, 3rd edition (WISC-III), and the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale,3rd edition (WAIS-III). The WISC-III, WAIS-III, and Vineland have mean = 100and SD = 15.

‡On Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS)–3 and ADOS-4, thecut-off point for autism is 6 on the socialization algorithm items (maximumscore is 14).

(REPRINTED) ARCH GEN PSYCHIATRY/VOL 59, SEP 2002 WWW.ARCHGENPSYCHIATRY.COM810

©2002 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. on December 27, 2011www.archgenpsychiatry.comDownloaded from

Page 3: Eye Tracking i Socijalne Situacije

8/10/2019 Eye Tracking i Socijalne Situacije

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/eye-tracking-i-socijalne-situacije 3/8

Page 4: Eye Tracking i Socijalne Situacije

8/10/2019 Eye Tracking i Socijalne Situacije

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/eye-tracking-i-socijalne-situacije 4/8

stances, region-of-interest resolution was maximized: only thoseshots in which theon-screendimensionsof the character ’s headwere greater than or equal to 5 ° of the participant ’s field of viewcould be coded (this was the basis for selecting the 2 minutes42 seconds of codable videotape data from the 5 clips shown toeach participant; long-range shots were excluded). This crite-rion wasused to ensurevalidity of the eye-tracking data coding,particularly in relation to codingof theeye vs the mouth region(if an on-screen head is smaller than 5 ° of the visual field, thesystem ’s marginof error is large enough to potentiallycauseno-table discrepancies in thecodingof facial featurepreference). Fi-nally, several scenes were discarded because there were no ac-tors facing the camera or because the actors, the camera action,or both moved too rapidly for meaningful analysis of preferen-tial viewing patterns.

Some frames that were coded could not be coded as fixa-tion data. These included framesin which a participant blinkedor made a saccadic shift. Blinks were defined by a loss of point-of-regardcoordinate data (which was superimposed alongwithtime code at the bottom of each picture) and then further veri-fied by an inset videotaped image of the participant ’s eye (insuch a case revealing closed lids). Initiation and completion of saccadic movementswere defined bya rotational velocity thresh-old of 30° per second, 31 which translated in our experimentalsetting into a linear velocity of 15 or more pixels per frame (ie,d 15 pixels, where d= ( x 2)+( y2), over the course of 1 frame[33 milliseconds]). Saccadic movements were also confirmedby the inset eye image. In addition, some frames could not becoded for the following reasons: off-screen fixations (watches,hands, etc), rubbing of eyes,and obstruction of the scene viewcamera (hand in front of face, nose rubbing, etc). These frames,recorded as “no data, ” amounted to a mean (SD) 5.2% (6.6%)of total time for controls and 15.6% (15.9%) of time for view-ers with autism ( P=.03). Although this significant differencecould be related to decreased concentration and increaseddistractibility in participants with autism, we cannot infer anyspecific conclusionsfromthisstatistical differencebecause other,non –attention-related factors could also be at play, including,for example, rubbing the eyes or sneezing. Future studies willneed to pay more attention to coding schema for characteriza-tion of lost data.

STATISTICAL ANALYSES

T tests with Bonferroni corrections for number of between-group comparisons were used to test group differences in thepercentage of total viewing time spent on fixations on mouth,eye, body, and object regions of the scene images. Pearson cor-relations were used to explore the degree of the relationshipbetween fixation patterns and the 2 measures of social com-petence —level of social adjustment (operationalized as theage-equivalent score on the socialization domain of the VABS-E)anddegree of autistic social impairment (operationalized as thetotal algorithm score on the social section of the ADOS).

RESULTS

Consistent with our predictions, individuals with au-tism focused 2 times more on the mouth region, 2 timesless on the eye region, 2 times more on the body region,

and 2 times more on the object region relative to age-and verbal IQ –matched controls ( Table 2 ). Effect sizewas greatest for fixation on the eye region, making it thebest predictor ofgroup membership( d=3.19).Therewereno significant correlations between any of the measuresof fixation time andchronological age or verbal IQ in the2 groups except for a positive significant correlation be-tween percentage of fixation time on the mouth regionand age in the group with autism( r =0.62; P=.01). Whenone outlier (a participantwith autismaged 38 years) wasremoved from thesample, this correlation was no longersignificant. None of the other correlations were alteredby excluding this person. As shown in Figure 2 , therewas little overlap in the measures of fixation time across

the2 groups, although there wassome variabilitywithinthe groups, except for fixation on objects in the controlgroup.

We next explored the association between fixationtime measures andmeasures of social competence. Con-trary to our expectation, fixation time on the eye regionwasnot associated witheither social adaptation (VABS-Esocialization scores) or social disability (ADOS socialscores) ( r =−0.20 and r =0.14, respectively). In contrast,fixation times on the mouth region ( Figure 3 ) and onthe object region ( Figure 4 ) were strong predictors of

100

90

80

70

60

50

40

30

20

10

0Mouth Eyes Body Object

V i s u a l F i x a t i o n

T i m e ,

%

Viewers With AutismTypically Developing Viewers

Figure 2. Box plot comparison of visual fixation time on mouth, eyes, body,and object regions for 15 viewers with autism and 15 typically developingviewers (controls). The upper and lower boundaries of the standard boxplotsare at the 25th and 75th percentiles. The horizontal line across the box marksthe median of the distribution, and the vertical lines below and above the boxextend to the minimum and maximum, respectively.

Table 2. Percentage of Viewing Time SpentFocused on Mouth, Eyes, Body, and Object Regions *

Region

AutismGroup

(n = 15)

ControlGroup

(n = 15)t

ValueP

Value

Mouth 41.2 (15.0) 21.2 (12.1) 4.026 .001Eyes 24.6 (8.1) 65.4 (12.8) −10.455 .001Body 24.6 (12.4) 9.7 (5.7) 4.226 .001

Object 9.6 (6.5) 3.7 (2.4) 3.286

.003* Data are given as mean (SD).

(REPRINTED) ARCH GEN PSYCHIATRY/VOL 59, SEP 2002 WWW.ARCHGENPSYCHIATRY.COM812

©2002 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. on December 27, 2011www.archgenpsychiatry.comDownloaded from

Page 5: Eye Tracking i Socijalne Situacije

8/10/2019 Eye Tracking i Socijalne Situacije

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/eye-tracking-i-socijalne-situacije 5/8

social competence, albeit in different directions. Fixa-tion time on themouthregionwasassociated with greatersocial adaptation (ie, more socially able) and lower au-tistic social impairment (ie, less socially disabled). Go-ing in the opposite direction, fixation time on the objectregion was associated with lower social adaptation and

greater autistic social impairment. Fixation time on thebody region followed the same trend as for the object re-gion, but the correlations were not significant ( r =−0.49and r =0.34 for social adaptation and social disability,respectively). When the outlier (a participant with au-tismaged 38years)wasexcluded fromcorrelational analy-ses, none of the measures of association were altered ex-cept the positive correlation between fixation time onobjects and the measure of social adaptation, which waspreviously significant at P .10 and was now significantat P .05.

COMMENT

We found significant differences in percentages of vi-sual fixation time on mouth, eye, body, and object re-gions when viewing naturalistic social situationsamongcognitively able adolescents and young adults with au-tismrelative toage-and verbal IQ –matched controls. The

best predictor of group membership was percentage of fixation time on the eye region: the control group visu-ally fixated on the eye region 2 times more than did thegroup with autism, and there was no overlap in the dis-tribution of results. However, within the group with au-tism, this variable was unrelated to outcome measuresof social competence. In contrast, percentages of fixa-tion time on the mouth and object regions were strongpredictors of social competence measures, with highermouth fixation time associated with higher levels of so-cial adaptation and lower levelsof autistic social impair-

12.00

8.00

4.00

0.00

10 30 50 70Visual Fixation Time on Mouth, %

V A B S

- E S o c i a l i z a t i o n

A g e - E q u

i v a l e n t

S c o r e

A

S o c i a l A

d a p

t a t i o n

r = 0.729; P < .01

16

12

8

4

10 30 50 70Visual Fixation Time on Mouth, %

A D O S S o c i a l A

l g o r i t h m

S c o r e

B

S o c i a l D i s

a b i l i t y

r = –0.627; P < .05

Figure 3. Correlation of mouth fixation time and outcome measures of socialadaptation (A) and social disability (B) in 15 viewers with autism. VABS-Eindicates Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Expanded Edition; ADOS,Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule.

12.00

8.00

4.00

0.00

5 10 15 20 25Visual Fixation Time on Objects, %

V A B S

- E S o c i a l i z a t i o n

A g e - E q u

i v a l e n t

S c o r e

A

S o c i a l A

d a p

t a t i o n

r = –0.503; P < .10

B

17.5

15.0

12.5

7.5

10.0

5 10 15 20 25Visual Fixation Time on Objects, %

A D O S S o c i a

l A l g o r i t h m

S c o r e

S o c i a l D i s

a b i l i t y

r = 0.636; P < .05

Figure 4. Correlation of object fixation time and outcome measures of socialadaptation (A) and social disability (B) in 15 viewers with autism. VABS-Eindicates Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Expanded Edition; ADOS,Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule.

(REPRINTED) ARCH GEN PSYCHIATRY/VOL 59, SEP 2002 WWW.ARCHGENPSYCHIATRY.COM813

©2002 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. on December 27, 2011www.archgenpsychiatry.comDownloaded from

Page 6: Eye Tracking i Socijalne Situacije

8/10/2019 Eye Tracking i Socijalne Situacije

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/eye-tracking-i-socijalne-situacije 6/8

ment, and object fixation time associations going in theopposite direction.

This study shows the utility of developing perfor-mance-basedparadigmscapable ofquantifyingsocial func-tioning under more naturalistic conditions. Theeffect sizesobtained for between-group comparisons were mark-edly larger relativeto experimentalstudies 32 of social func-tioning in autism and more comparable to the few stud-ies14 available that focused on spontaneous responses

rather thanexplicit problem-solving tasks. Similarly, theenhancedecological validity aimedfor in thepresentstudyalso led to stronger associations between performance-based experimental measures and outcome measures of social competence, an important goal to be achieved inthe search for dimensional endophenotypes for geneticresearch. 17 Moregenerally, the paradigmpresentedhereinprovides a valuable windowintotheways individualswithautism search for meaning when confronted with com-plex social situations.

Given that this is the first study of its kind, com-parisons with other studies can only be indirect. Never-theless, the small number of available studies is gener-ally supportiveofourfindings.For example, somestudies

havesuggested increased relianceon mouths rather thaneyes when individuals with autism are required to per-form face perception tasks, 5,6 great difficulty in “read-ing” the meaning of eyeexpressions, 7 and increased ori-entation to objects relative to people. 4,8 A recent studyby Joseph 33 addressedthedifferentialrelianceon eyes andmouths in autism more directly than previous studies. When 10-to 12-year-old, cognitivelyable individualswithautismwere asked to perform a match-to-sample face rec-ognition task in which faces varied as a function of eyeor mouth changes, they exhibited a selective advantagefor mouths.

Although the present study wasnotdesigned to ex-plore the factors underlying visual fixation patterns in

autism, the strong predictive association between mouthand object, but not eye fixations and outcome measuresof social competence, suggests that specific hypothesesneed to be pursued in future, more-refined studies. Thefinding that a higher percentage of mouth fixation timepredicted a higher level of social competence is as in-triguing as it is counterintuitive, and, therefore, it de-serves special attention. Given the well-known associa-tion between level of verbal skills and better outcome inautism, 34 it is possible that the participants in our studywere focusing on mouths because that is where speechcomes from. By concentrating their efforts on some-thing that they can understand, they might attain betterunderstanding of social situations. Nevertheless, such a

compensatorystrategy is notwithout its limitations giventhat the meaning of language is often modified by non-verbal social cues such as eye expressions. The fact thatpercentage of time focused oneyeswas unrelated tomea-sures of social competence suggests the possibility thatfor individuals with autism, looking at eyes does not ac-crue considerable advantages in their efforts to under-stand social situations, or, in other words, that the eyesare not meaningful to them. 7,34

The hypothesis that increased fixation on mouthsresults from concentration on speech needs to be fur-

ther refined on the basis of what we know about visualattention to facial regions in speech perception tasks. 35

Whereas audiovisual speech perception (like the stimuliin thepresent study)drawsgaze towardthe talker ’s eyes,36

speech reading (ie, seeing someone speak with no ac-companying audio) is more likely to draw gaze towardthe talker ’s mouth. 37 However, visual fixations in speechreading depend on the nature of the task. If participantsare asked to perform a segmental speechperception task

(ie, to guesswhat the talker is saying), theviewer is morelikely to focus on the mouth region. 36-38 However, if par-ticipants areasked to perform a prosodic speechpercep-tion task (ie, to guess the inflection of the talker ’s voice,eg, sad, happy, or mad), then the viewer is more likelyto focus on the upper region of the face (eyes andeyebrows). 37 On thebasisof this literature, ourvisualfixa-tion results could suggest that our participants were notonly focusing on the verbal content of speech but werealso ignoringparalinguistic cues such as prosody, 39 whichare usually essential to understanding nonliteral aspectsof social situations such as intentions and attitudes. 40 Inother words, our findings could be related to the over-reliance of individuals with autism on the literal aspects

of speech at the expense of intonational cues associatedwith social meaning. 41 By pairingspeechperception taskswitheye-trackingmeasures, 37 these relationships are likelyto be clarified.

Another line of inquiry for future studies relates tothepossibility that increased fixation on mouths andde-creased fixation on eyes indicate that individuals with au-tism may acquire a degree of perceptual expertise onmouths but not on eyes. Findings of the previously de-scribed study by Joseph 33 seem to point in this direc-tion. When the face recognition task was presented inthe form of inverted faces, children with autism did notshow the typical decrement in performance 42 (the so-called inversion effect 43) in the eye condition, but did so

in themouthcondition. Given that an increasein themag-nitude of the inversion effect and associated transitionfrom feature-based to configural processing marks thedevelopment of perceptual expertise relative to a classof objects, 44-47 the findings of Joseph raise the possibilitythat children with autism have expertisewhen recogniz-ing faces that vary in mouth features but not when theyvary in eye features. Future studies that could manipu-late these variables more explicitly in still (ie, photo-graphs) anddynamic (ie, videotape) stimuli might clarifythese issues. Also, given the recent surge of neuroimag-ing research on the brain circuitry involved in the ac-quisition of perceptual expertise, 47 the coregistration of neurofunctional and eye-trackingdatamight prove to be

particularlyuseful in elucidating theunusual patternsof behavioral and brain functioning in regard to face per-ception in autism. 16,18,48

A final line of inquiry suggested by our results con-cerns the need to explore the association between morefixation time on objects and decreased social compe-tence in participants with autism. This finding is con-sistent with the notion that focus on objects means re-ducedsalienceofsocialstimuli 4,15 and, therefore, decreasedlikelihood of understanding the social situation. Theper-centage of fixation time on objects was small relative to

(REPRINTED) ARCH GEN PSYCHIATRY/VOL 59, SEP 2002 WWW.ARCHGENPSYCHIATRY.COM814

©2002 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. on December 27, 2011www.archgenpsychiatry.comDownloaded from

Page 7: Eye Tracking i Socijalne Situacije

8/10/2019 Eye Tracking i Socijalne Situacije

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/eye-tracking-i-socijalne-situacije 7/8

fixation time on the face (mouth and eyes). This is notsurprising given that our videotape clips were deliber-ately chosen to minimize inanimate distractions. Fu-ture studies may maximize the predictive utility of theassociationbetween fixation on objectsandoutcomemea-sures of social competence by manipulating the promi-nence of inanimate stimuli (eg, by making objects moveor by using objects known to attract the attention of in-dividualswithautism). Clinical observations haveshown

the devastating impact of object-rich environments onthe ability of children with autism to focus on sociallymeaningful aspects of educational environments. 49

The present study has several limitations. First, al-thoughclose to three quarters of all individuals with au-tism have a degree of mental retardation, 50 we studiedmore cognitively able individuals with autism. We do notknow if theseresults willextend toparticipants with lowerIQs.Neither doweknow what trendsto expect inyoungerchildrenwith autismor in individuals with milder mani-festations of the condition (eg, Asperger syndrome andPervasive DevelopmentalDisorder –Not Otherwise Speci-fied [also called “atypical autism ”]). Second, our designdoes not rule out possible contributions from abnormal

functioning in areas other than socialvisual pursuit (eg,underlying attentional or perceptual abnormalities). 51,52

The use of more traditional eye-tracking protocols fo-cused on the integrity of brain mechanisms associatedwith eye movement 53 alongside our novel application of this technology will be necessary to rule out explana-tions other than the ones explored in this study. Third,although the measures reported in this study were in-formative, they are unlikely to be the most sensitive in-dicators of abnormalities in social visual pursuit in au-tism. In a case study reported elsewhere, 54 we illustratea series of social visual tracking phenomena that seemto capture the abnormalities in autism in a much moredynamic andstark fashion thansummaries of visual fixa-

tion times. Themoment-by-momentvisual traces left be-hind by the saccadic movements and fixations of indi-viduals with autismillustrate more vividly their atypicalattempts to create social meaning out of what they see.Perhaps paradigms capable of quantifying the subtletiesof such data will soon emerge from theseas-yet only heu-ristic approaches.

Submitted for publication June 14, 2001; final revision re-ceived October 10, 2001; accepted October 24, 2001.

This work was partly supported by grants P01 HD03008 and P01 HD/DC35482 from the National InstituteofChildHealthandHumanDevelopment, Bethesda,Md,andby a grant from theNational Alliance for AutismResearch,

Princeton, NJ.This study was presented in part at the annual meet-ing of theNational Institute of ChildHealth andHuman De-velopment, Collaborative Projects of Excellence in Autism,New Haven, Conn, May 2, 2001.

We thank the participants and their families; Eliza-beth Levitan, BA, Tammy Babitz, MA, Sanno Zack, BA,Catalina Hooper, BA, Amy Augustyn, BA, and Chris-topher Abildgaard, BA, for their assistance with thisstudy; and Katarzyna Chawarska, PhD, for her contribu-tion to interpretation of findings. We also thank Warner

Bros, Elizabeth Taylor, George Segal, and the estates of Sandy Dennis and Richard Burton for permission to usetheir images.

This article is dedicated to the memory of our mentor,collaborator, and colleague, Donald Cohen, MD, whoselegacy embodies the best in clinical services, public advo-cacy, clinical science, bioethics, and mentoring.

Corresponding authorandreprints: AmiKlin, PhD, YaleChild Study Center, 230 S Frontage Rd, New Haven, CT

06520 (e-mail: [email protected]).

REFERENCES

1. Waterhouse L, Fein D, Modahl C. Neurofunctional mechanisms in autism. Psy- chol Rev. 1996;103:457-489.

2. Spelke ES, Phillips A, Woodward AL. Infants’ knowledge of object motion andhuman action. In: Sperber D, Premack D, Premack AJ, eds. Causal Cognition: AMultidisciplinary Debate. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press; 1995:44-78.

3. Haith MM, Bergman T, Moore MJ. Eye contact and face scanning in early in-fancy. Science. 1979;198:853-855.

4. DawsonG, Meltzoff AN,OsterlingJ, RinaldiJ, BrownE. Childrenwith autismfailto orient to naturally occurring social stimuli. J Autism Dev Disord. 1998;28:479-485.

5. Langdell T. Recognition of faces: an approach to the study of autism. J Child

Psychol Psychiatry. 1978;19:255-268.6. Baron-Cohen S,WheelwrightS, Jolliffe T.Is there a“languageof theeyes”? evi-

dence from normal adults, and adults with autism or Asperger syndrome. Vis Cogn. 1997;4:311-331.

7. Baron-Cohen S, Jolliffe T, Mortimore C, Robertson M. Another advanced test oftheory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or As-perger syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997;38:813-822.

8. Hobson RP, Ouston J, Lee A. What’s in a face? the case of autism. Br J Psychol.1988;79:441-453.

9. Klin A, Sparrow SS, de Bildt A, Cicchetti DV, Cohen DJ, Volkmar FR. A normedstudy of face recognition in autism and related disorders. J Autism Dev Disord.1999;29:497-507.

10. Tantam D, Monaghan L, Nicholson H, Stirling J. Autistic children’s ability to in-terpret faces: a research note. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1989;30:623-630.

11. Boucher J, Lewis V. Unfamiliar face recognition in relatively able autistic chil-dren. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1992;33:843-859.

12. Weeks S, Hobson R. Thesalience of facial expression for autisticchildren.J Child

Psychol Psychiatry. 1987;28:137-151.13. Baron-Cohen S, Tager-Flusberg H, Cohen D, eds. Understanding Other Minds:

Perspectives From Developmental Neuroscience. 2nd ed. Oxford, England: Ox-ford University Press; 2000.

14. Klin A. Attributing social meaning to ambiguous visual stimuli in higher func-tioning autism and Asperger syndrome: the social attribution task. J Child Psy- chol Psychiatry. 2000;41:831-846.

15. Klin A, Schultz R, Cohen D. Theory of mind in action: developmental perspec-tives on social neuroscience. In: Baron-Cohen S, Tager-Flusberg H, Cohen D,eds. Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives From Developmental Neurosci- ence. 2nd ed. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press; 2000:357-388.

16. Schultz RT, Gauthier I, Klin A, Fulbright R, Anderson A, Volkmar FR, SkudlarskiP,Lacadie C,CohenDJ, GoreJC. Abnormalventraltemporalcorticalactivityamongindividualswith autism and Aspergersyndromeduring facediscrimination.Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:331-340.

17. Rutter M, Silberg J, O’Connor T, Simonoff E. Genetics and child psychiatry, I:advances in quantitativeand molecular genetics. J ChildPsychol Psychiatry.1999;

40:3-18.18. Schultz RT, RomanskiLM, TsatsanisKD. Neurofunctionalmodelsof autistic dis-

order and Asperger syndrome: clues from neuroimaging. In: Klin A, Volkmar F,SparrowS, eds. AspergerSyndrome.NewYork, NY:Guilford Publications;2000:172-209.

19. Yarbus AL. Eye Movements and Vision. New York, NY: Plenum Press; 1967.20. Walker-Smith GJ, Gale AG, Findlay JM. Eye movement strategies in face per-

ception. Perception. 1977;6:313-326.21. Sparrow SS, Balla D, Cicchetti D. Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Expanded

Edition. Circle Pines, Minn: American Guidance Service; 1984.22. Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore P, Risi S. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule

(ADOS). Los Angeles, Calif: Western Psychological Services; 1999.23. Lord C, Rutter M, LeCouteur A. Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised: a revised

(REPRINTED) ARCH GEN PSYCHIATRY/VOL 59, SEP 2002 WWW.ARCHGENPSYCHIATRY.COM815

©2002 American Medical Association. All rights reserved. on December 27, 2011www.archgenpsychiatry.comDownloaded from

Page 8: Eye Tracking i Socijalne Situacije

8/10/2019 Eye Tracking i Socijalne Situacije

http://slidepdf.com/reader/full/eye-tracking-i-socijalne-situacije 8/8

version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible per-vasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 1994;24:659-685.

24. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association;1994.

25. Wechsler D. Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. 3rd ed. San Antonio, Tex:Psychological Corp; 1991.

26. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale. 3rd ed. San Antonio, Tex: Psy-chological Corp; 1997.

27. Klin A, Carter A, Volkmar FR, Cohen DJ, Marans WD, Sparrow SS. Assessmentissues in children with autism. In: Cohen DJ, Volkmar FR, eds. Handbook of Au- tismand PervasiveDevelopmentalDisorders.2nded.New York,NY: John Wiley& Sons Inc; 1997:411-418.

28. Hobson RP. Methodological issues for experiments on autistic individuals’ per-ception and understanding of emotion. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1991;32:1135-1158.

29. Cohen J. A coefficient of agreement for nominal scales. Educ Psychol Measure.1960;20:37-46.

30. Cicchetti DV, Sparrow SS. Developing criteria for establishing inter-raterreliability of specific items in a given inventory. Am J Ment Defic. 1981;86:127-137.

31. Leigh RJ, Zee DS. The Neurology of Eye Movements. 3rd ed. Oxford, England:Oxford University Press; 1999:94.

32. Yirmiya N,Erel O,ShakedM, Solomonica-Levi D.Meta-analysescomparingtheoryof mind abilities of individuals with autism, individuals with mental retardation,and normally developing individuals. Psychol Bull. 1998;124:283-307.

33. JosephRM. Facerecognitionprocesses in typicallydevelopingchildrenand chil-dren with autism. Paper presented at: Society for Research in Child Develop-ment Biennial Meeting; April 21, 2001; Minneapolis, Minn.

34. Lord C, Paul R. Language and communication in autism. In: Cohen DJ, VolkmarFR,eds.Handbookof Autism andPervasive Developmental Disorders.NewYork,NY: John Wiley & Sons Inc; 1997:195-225.

35. StorkeDG, Hennecke ME. Humanspeechreading: psychologyand cognition.In:Speechreading in Humans and Machines: Models, Systems, and Applications.Berlin, Germany: Springer-Verlag; 1996:525-535.

36. Vatikiotis-Bateson E, Eigsti I-M, Yano S, Munhall KG. Eye movement of perceiversduring audiovisual speech perception. Percept Psychophys. 1998;60:926-940.

37. Lansing CR, McConkie GW. Attention to facial regions in segmental and pro-sodic visual speech perception tasks. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1999;42:526-539.

38. Preminger JE,Lin H-B, Payen M, LevittH. Selectivevisualmasking inspeechread-ing. J Speech Lang Hear Res. 1998;41:564-575.

39. Scherer KR. Vocal affect expression: a review and a model for future research.Psychol Bull . 1986;99:143-165.

40. Bolinger DL. Intonation and Its Parts. Stanford, Calif: Stanford University Press;1986.

41. Landa R. Social language use in Asperger syndrome and high-functioning au-tism. In: Klin A, Volkmar, FR, Sparrow SS, eds. Asperger Syndrome. New York,NY: Guilford Publications; 2000:125-155.

42. Tanaka JW,Kay JB,GrinnellE, Stansfield B, SzechterL. Facerecognition in youngchildren: when the whole is greater than the sum of its parts. Vis Cogn. 1998;5:479-496.

43. ValentineT. Upside-downfaces: a reviewof theeffect of inversionuponfacerec-ognition. Br J Psychol. 1988;79:471-491.

44. GauthierI, Tarr MJ.Becoming a“Greeble” expert: exploring mechanisms for facerecognition. Vision Res. 1997;37:1673-1681.

45. GauthierI, TarrMJ, AndersonAW, Skudlarski P, GoreJC. Activation of the middlefusiform “face area” increases with expertise in recognizing novel objects. Nat Neurosci. 1999;2:568-573.

46. Diamond R, Carey S. Why faces are and are not special: an effect of expertise.J Exp Psychol Gen. 1986;115:107-117.

47. Gauthier I, Skudkarski P, Gore JC, Anderson AW. Expertise for cars and birdsrecruits brainareas involved in face recognition.Nat Neurosci.2000;3:191-197.

48. MarcusDJ, NelsonCA. Neuralbasesand development of face recognition inau-tism. CNS Spectrums. 2001;6:36-59.

49. Olley JG, Reeve C. Issues of curriculum and classroom structure. In: Cohen DJ,Volkmar FR, eds. Handbook of Autism and Pervasive Developmental Disorders.New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons Inc; 1997:484-508.

50. Fombonne E. The epidemiology of autism: a review.Psychol Med.1999;29:769-786.

51. Harris NS, Courchesne E, Townsend J, Carper RA, Lord C. Neuroanatomic con-tributions to slowed orienting of attention in children with autism. Cognit Brain Res. 1999;8:61-71.

52. Courchesne E, Townsend J, Akshoomoff NA, Saitoh O, Yeung-Courchesne R,Lincoln AJ, James H, Hass R, Schreibman L, Lau L. Impairments in shifting at-tention in autistic and cerebellar patients. Behav Neurosci. 1994;108:1-18.

53. Minshew NJ, Luna B, Sweeney JA. Oculomotor evidence for neocortical sys-tems but not cerebellar dysfunction in autism. Neurology. 1999;52:917-922.

54. Klin A, Jones W, Schultz R, Volkmar FR, Cohen DJ. Defining and quantifying thesocial phenotype in autism. Am J Psychiatry. In press.

(REPRINTED) ARCH GEN PSYCHIATRY/VOL 59, SEP 2002 WWW.ARCHGENPSYCHIATRY.COM816

©2002 American Medical Association All rights reserved on December 27, 2011www.archgenpsychiatry.comDownloaded from