executive function determinants of attention-deficit

83
University of Calgary PRISM: University of Calgary's Digital Repository Graduate Studies The Vault: Electronic Theses and Dissertations 2013-10-02 Executive Function Determinants of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Medication Response Kubas, Hanna Kubas, H. (2013). Executive Function Determinants of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Medication Response (Unpublished master's thesis). University of Calgary, Calgary, AB. doi:10.11575/PRISM/26036 http://hdl.handle.net/11023/1117 master thesis University of Calgary graduate students retain copyright ownership and moral rights for their thesis. You may use this material in any way that is permitted by the Copyright Act or through licensing that has been assigned to the document. For uses that are not allowable under copyright legislation or licensing, you are required to seek permission. Downloaded from PRISM: https://prism.ucalgary.ca

Upload: others

Post on 10-Feb-2022

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

University of Calgary

PRISM: University of Calgary's Digital Repository

Graduate Studies The Vault: Electronic Theses and Dissertations

2013-10-02

Executive Function Determinants of

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Medication

Response

Kubas, Hanna

Kubas, H. (2013). Executive Function Determinants of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder

Medication Response (Unpublished master's thesis). University of Calgary, Calgary, AB.

doi:10.11575/PRISM/26036

http://hdl.handle.net/11023/1117

master thesis

University of Calgary graduate students retain copyright ownership and moral rights for their

thesis. You may use this material in any way that is permitted by the Copyright Act or through

licensing that has been assigned to the document. For uses that are not allowable under

copyright legislation or licensing, you are required to seek permission.

Downloaded from PRISM: https://prism.ucalgary.ca

UNIVERSITY OF CALGARY

Executive Function Determinants of Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity

Disorder Medication Response

by

Hanna Alexandra Kubas

A THESIS

SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES

IN PARTIAL FULFILMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE

DEGREE OF MASTER OF SCIENCE

DEPARTMENT OF EDUCATIONAL PSYCHOLOGY

CALGARY, ALBERTA

SEPTEMBER, 2013

© Hanna A. Kubas 2013

ii

Abstract

Focusing on behavioural criteria for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) diagnosis

leads to considerable neuropsychological profile heterogeneity among diagnosed children and

variable response to methylphenidate (MPH) treatment. Documenting “cool” executive/working

memory (EWM) or “hot” self-regulation (SR) neuropsychological impairments could aid in

differential diagnosis of ADHD subtypes and may help determine the optimal MPH treatment

dose. In this study, children with ADHD Inattentive Type (n = 18) (IT) and Combined (n = 35)

(CT) underwent a randomized double-blind placebo-controlled 4-week MPH trial.

Neuropsychological, behavioural, and observational data were collected to evaluate medication

response. Results from individual neuropsychological tests suggest that performance was not

uniform; those with moderate or significant baseline EWM/SR impairment showed robust MPH

response, while response for those with lower baseline executive impairment was minimal.

Implications for medication titration, academic achievement, and long-term treatment efficacy

were examined.

iii

Acknowledgements

I would like to take this opportunity to sincerely thank all of those who helped make this

thesis a reality. First, I would like to thank my committee members, Dr. James B. Hale, Dr.

Gabrielle Wilcox, and Dr. Deborah Dewey, for agreeing to share this exciting educational

milestone with me.

To all of the former and current BrainGain Laboratory members: Andrea Schneider,

Emilie Crevier-Quintin, Jessica Carmichael, Kim Fitzer, and Erica Backenson, your constant

encouragement and unwavering support helped me get through the tough times. To my UVic and

U of C cohorts, thanks for the memories; I was lucky enough to be a part of two amazing groups.

Thank you to the Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council for their continued

support throughout my studies. Without their support I would not have been able to dedicate as

much of my time to developing and growing as a researcher and an academic.

To my family, Jacek, Joanna, Karol, Pisiu, and Mila: thank you for putting up with my

grumpiness, and school work obligations on evenings, weekends, and holidays, for reminding me

to relax during the stressful times, and for helping me realize that work is not always the most

important part of life.

And last, but certainly not least, I would like to express my deepest gratitude to my

supervisor, Dr. James B. Hale, for his continued guidance and patience, for always believing in

me and pushing me to be best that I can be, and for never being afraid to challenge current

practice. Together we will accomplish great things.

iv

Dedication

To my family and friends,

I could not have done this without you.

Enjoy.

v

Table of Contents

Abstract ............................................................................................................................... ii!

Acknowledgements ........................................................................................................... iii!

Dedication .......................................................................................................................... iv!

Table of Contents .................................................................................................................v!

List of Tables ................................................................................................................... vii!

List of Figures .................................................................................................................. viii!

List of Symbols, Abbreviations and Nomenclature ........................................................... ix

Co-Authorship Statement ................................................................................................. ixi

Chapter 1: Introduction ....................................................................................................1!

1.1 Background ................................................................................................................1!

1.2 Frontal-subcortical circuits and the biological basis of ADHD .................................3!

1.3 “Hot” versus “Cold” frontal-subcortical circuits .......................................................5!

1.4 Methylphenidate treatment in ADHD ........................................................................6!

1.5 Methylphenidate effects on cognitive and neuropsychological functioning .............8!

1.6 Methylphenidate effects on academic functioning ..................................................10!

1.7 Purpose of Current Study .........................................................................................12!

Chapter 2: Method ...........................................................................................................15!

2.1 Participants ...............................................................................................................15!

2.2 Procedure .................................................................................................................16!

2.3 Instrumentation ........................................................................................................20!

2.3.1 Go No-Go Test ................................................................................................20!

2.3.2 Stroop Color-Word Test ..................................................................................21!

2.3.3 Test of Memory and Learning-Digits Backwards ...........................................22!

2.3.4 Wisconsin Selective Reminding Test of Memory ...........................................23!

2.3.5 Trail Making Test–Part B ................................................................................24!

2.3.6 Conners’ Continuous Performance Test–II .....................................................24!

2.3.7 Hale-Denckla Cancellation Test ......................................................................26!

vi

Chapter 3: Results ............................................................................................................27!

3.1 Overview ..................................................................................................................27!

3.2 Individual neuropsychological assessment measure results ....................................30!

3.2.1 Go No-Go Test ................................................................................................30!

3.2.2 Stroop Color-Word Test ..................................................................................31!

3.2.3 Test of Memory and Learning-Digits Backwards ...........................................32!

3.2.4 Wisconsin Selective Reminding Test of Memory ...........................................33!

3.2.5 Trail Making Test Part B .................................................................................33!

3.2.6 Conners’ Continuous Performance Test-II ......................................................34!

3.2.7 Hale-Denckla Cancellation Test ......................................................................36

Chapter 4: Discussion ......................................................................................................37!

4.1 Overview of findings ...............................................................................................37!

4.2 Implications for “hot” and “cool” circuit executive functions ................................38!

4.3 Implications for academic achievement in ADHD ..................................................39!

4.4 Limitations ...............................................................................................................40!

4.5 Future research .........................................................................................................41!

Chapter 5: Conclusion .....................................................................................................43!

References ..........................................................................................................................46

Appendix A: Summary of Means, Standard Deviations, and MPH Dose-Response

Relationships for Executive Working Memory “Cool” Circuit Neuropsychological

Measures Across Impairment Groups .......................................................................69

Appendix B: Summary of Means, Standard Deviations, and MPH Dose-Response

Relationships for Self-Regulation “Hot” Circuit Neuropsychological Measures Across

Impairment Groups ...................................................................................................70

Appendix C: Copyright Permission Letter .......................................................................71!

vii

List of Tables

Table 1. MPH Dose-Response Relationships for EWM/SR Impairment Groups for

Individual Tests ......................................................................................................28

viii

List of Figures

Figure 1. Confirmatory Factor Analysis Frontal-Subcortical Circuit Loadings ....................14

Figure 2. Procedure for the Double-Blind Placebo Protocol ................................................17!

Figure 3. Dose-Response Relationships for Neuropsychological Tests by Impairment Group

................................................................................................................................29

!

ix

List of Symbols, Abbreviations and Nomenclature

Abbreviation Definition ADHD Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder

EF Executive functions LD Learning Disability DA Dopamine NE Norepinephrine

MPH Methylphenidate EWM Executive Working Memory or “cool” dorsolateral-dorsal cingulate circuit

SR Self-Regulation of “hot” orbital-ventral cingulate circuits ADHD-IT ADHD-Inattentive Type Subtype

ADHD-HIT ADHD-Hyperactive-Impulsive Subtype ADHD-CT ADHD-Combined Type Subtype

SCT Sluggish Cognitive Tempo APA American Psychiatric Association

DSM-IV-TR Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders–Fourth Edition–Text Revision

SD Standard Deviation M Mean

NPStat Non-Parametric Randomization Test for Statistical Ranks MANOVA Multivariate Analysis of Variance

ODD/CD Oppositional Defiant Disorder/Conduct Disorder A/D Anxiety/Depression

IQ Intelligence PI Principal Investigator B Baseline Condition P Placebo Condition L Low Dose Condition H High Dose Condition

N/A No Apparent Baseline Executive Impairment Low Low Baseline Executive Impairment

Moderate Moderate Baseline Executive Impairment High High Baseline Executive Impairment RCT Randomized Controlled Trial SEM Structural Equation Modeling fMRI Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging

DTI Diffusion Tensor Imaging

x

Test Abbreviation

Assessment Measure

TEA-CH Test of Everyday Attention for Children CELF-3 Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals 3

WIAT-II Wechsler Individual Achievement Test-II Stroop Stroop Color-Word Test

TOMAL-DB Test of Memory and Learning-Digits Backwards WSRTM Wisconsin Selective Reminding Test of Memory

TMT-B Trail Making Test-Part B CPT-II Conners’ Continuous Performance Test-II HDCT Hale-Denckla Cancellation Test CBCL Child Behavior Checklist

TRF Teacher Report Form CPRS-R:L Conners’ Parent Rating Scales–Revised: Long Form CTRS-R:L Conners’ Teacher Rating Scales–Revised: Long Form

APRS Academic Performance Rating Scale SSQ-R School Situations Questionnaire-Revised SERS Side Effects Rating Scale RAT Restricted Academic Task

xi

Co-Authorship Statement

The results presented in Chapter Three of this thesis have been previously published. The

citation for the journal article publication is:

Kubas, H. A., Backenson, E. M., Wilcox, G., Piercy, J. C., & Hale, J. B. (2012). The effects of

methylphenidate on cognitive functions in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder. Postgraduate Medicine, 124(5), 33–48. doi:10.3810/pgm.2012.09.2592

As the first author, I was in charge of the preparation and submission of the manuscript, as well

as the formulation of research questions, literature review, data analyses, and write-up of results.

I was also in charge of writing the manuscript, and the final edits required by the publisher.

The results presented in Chapter Three were also disseminated as a poster presentation at an

international conference. The citation for the poster presentation is:

Kubas, H. A., Backenson, E. M., Wilcox, G., Piercy, J. C., Carmichael, J. A., Fitzer, K. R., &

Hale, J. B. (2013, February). Differentiating frontal-subcortical circuit executive

dysfunction in ADHD medication response. Poster presentation at the 41st annual

International Neuropsychological Society meeting.

As the first author, I was in charge of compiling and preparing all aspect of the presentation, as

well as presenting the poster at the conference.

1

Chapter 1: Introduction

1.1 Background

Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder

encompassing a heterogeneous group of children who display persistent age-inappropriate

symptoms of hyperactivity, impulsivity, and inattention across multiple domains of functioning

(American Academy of Pediatrics [AAP], 2011; American Psychiatric Association [APA],

2000). Prevalence estimates suggest that approximately 5-7% of children and adolescents are

affected worldwide (Polanczyk, de Lima, Horta, Biederman, & Rhode, 2007; Willcutt, 2012),

making ADHD one of the most common childhood neuropsychiatric disorders (Barkley, 2006a).

Although still considered a disruptive behaviour disorder (APA, 2000), it is now widely

acknowledged that ADHD is a frontal-subcortical circuit disorder contributing to deficits in

executive functions (EF) including planning, organization, inhibition, working memory,

problem-solving, mental flexibility, monitoring, and evaluation (Biederman et al., 2004; Hale et

al., 2009a; Sonuga-Barke, Sergeant, Nigg, & Willcutt, 2008).

Although ADHD is now widely recognized as a frontal-subcortical circuit disorder

(Castellanos et al., 2002; Hale et al., 2009b), behavioural criteria and rating scales remain the

most commonly used diagnostic tools, leading to heterogeneous grouping of children with very

different manifestations of symptomology (Hale et al., 2012) and ratings that are insensitive to

neurocognitive functioning (Manor et al., 2008). Further, since most frontal-subcortical circuit

disorders lead to impaired attention (Lichter & Cummings, 2001), differential diagnosis of

ADHD becomes difficult when only behavioural criteria are used (Hale, Fiorello, & Brown,

2005).

2

ADHD often shows comorbidity with a wide variety of psychiatric conditions (Taylor,

2011; Willcutt et al., 2012), including externalizing (e.g., oppositional defiant, conduct disorder)

and internalizing (e.g., depression, anxiety) psychopathology (Barkley, 2006b), as well as

associations with specific learning and developmental problems (Thapar, Cooper, Eyre, &

Langley, 2012). ADHD tends to have an especially profound impact on academic functioning

(DuPaul & Stoner, 2003), and children with ADHD often exhibit significantly lower grades and

achievement scores, and higher rates of grade retention and school dropout, when compared to

peers without ADHD (Barkley, 2006a; Loe & Feldman, 2007).

It has been suggested that poor academic achievement in children with ADHD may be

the result of executive function deficits (Biederman et al., 2004), with prevalence rates of

comorbid ADHD and learning disabilities (LD) estimated to be around 31% (DuPaul & Stoner,

2003). Of the cognitive processes that fall under the umbrella of executive functions, working

memory deficits have been the strongest and most consistent impairments found in children with

ADHD (Castellanos, Sonuga-Barke, Milham, & Tannock, 2006; Martinussen, Hayden, Hogg-

Johnson, & Tannock, 2005; Willcutt, Doyle, Nigg, Faraone, & Pennington, 2005). Since

working memory is particularly important for memory encoding and retrieval, it should not be

surprising that impairment in these areas affects classroom learning and academic achievement

(Barkley, 1997; Wilcutt et al., 2005). While the ADHD–LD association may be the result of

common underlying neural pathways (Semrud-Clikeman, 2005), it remains unclear whether

learning difficulties associated with academic skill and/or performance deficits result from a

shared genetic etiology (Isles & Humby, 2006), a predisposition to comorbid disorders such as

conduct problems inadvertently leading to poor academic performance (Rhee, Willcutt, Hartman,

Pennington, & DeFries, 2008), behavioural interference with performance in the classroom (e.g.,

3

noncompliance, limited on-task behaviour) (DuPaul & Stoner, 2004), and/or cognitive and

neuropsychological deficits (Goldstein & Naglieri, 2008; Roth & Saykin, 2004).

Psychotropic medication is the most common form of treatment for children with ADHD

(Barkley, 2006b), and numerous studies have documented significant short-term benefits of

medication on objective measures of academic functioning (Chacko et al., 2006; Evans et al.,

2001; Pelham et al., 2001; Powers, Marks, Miller, Newcorn, & Halperin, 2008). However, the

effects of long-term medication use on academic outcomes in children with ADHD remain

unclear (Langberg & Becker, 2012). The long-term benefits of medication use are important

issues for families of children with ADHD (Hansen & Hansen, 2006), especially since grades

and achievement scores largely determine student acceptance into post-secondary programs and

strongly predict academic performance in college (Zwick & Sklar, 2005). As such, physicians

and other healthcare professionals need to be able to make evidence-based recommendations for

families of children and youth with ADHD that will maximize academic performance, while at

the same time managing problematic behaviours.

1.2 Frontal-subcortical circuits and the biological basis of ADHD

The main cortical areas implicated in ADHD include the prefrontal cortex – in particular,

the dorsolateral prefrontal and inferior prefrontal cortices (Dickstein, Bannon, Castellanos, &

Milham, 2006) – and their associated frontal-subcortical circuits and structures, including the

striatum (caudate, putamen), the thalamus (Castellanos et al., 2002; Hale et al., 2009b; Lichter &

Cummings, 2001), the limbic regions (e.g., nucleus accumbens), the corpus callosum and related

white matter tracts (Valera, Faraone, Murray, & Seidman, 2007), and the cerebellum (Vaidya &

Stollstroff, 2008). For some children with ADHD, other cortical regions, including the temporal

4

and parietal lobes, have also been implicated (Arnsten, 2009a).

Extant meta-analyses document a consistent pattern of frontal hypoactivity in individuals

with ADHD that is widely distributed in the dorsolateral and orbital prefrontal cortices and

related subcortical regions (Dickstein et al., 2006). According to most nomenclatures, there are at

least five major frontal-subcortical circuits, including the dorsolateral, orbitofrontal, anterior

cingulate, motor, and occulomotor circuits (Lichter & Cummings, 2001).

The circuits affected in ADHD are largely governed by catecholamine (e.g., dopamine

[DA] and/or norepinephrine [NE]) neurotransmitters (Arnsten, 2009b; Arnsten & Li, 2005), with

dysregulation affecting optimal frontal-subcortical circuit functioning (Arnsten & Pliszka, 2011;

Castellanos et al., 2002). DA is a critical neurotransmitter for regulatory frontal-subcortical

circuit functioning and is associated with motivation and reward, providing for sustained task

interest and improved performance, especially during inherently unmotivating tasks (Volkow et

al., 2001; Volkow et al., 2009). Insufficient DA in the prefrontal cortex may be the consequence

of an excess of dopamine transporters, which facilitate DA reuptake into the presynaptic

membrane, thereby decreasing DA availability in the synapse (Hood, Baird, Rankin, & Isaacs,

2005). This DA paucity in the synaptic cleft has been associated with both cognitive and

behavioural ADHD symptoms (Voeller, 2001). Furthermore, different frontal-subcortical circuits

are responsible for mediating different aspects of cognition and behaviour. While the dorsolateral

prefrontal circuit has been consistently implicated in mediating EFs, the orbital prefrontal circuit

governs emotional and behavioural regulation (Lichter & Cummings, 2001).

Frontal-subcortical circuit dysregulation not only affects behaviour during cognitive and

neuropsychological assessment, it also affects EFs that are directly or indirectly required for

optimal test performance (Hale et al., 2009b; Hale et al., 2012). Thus, a child with ADHD may

5

perform adequately on many intellectual and cognitive tasks yet show subset variability resulting

in performance decrements due to his or her EF dysfunction (Hale et al., 2012).

1.3 “Hot” versus “Cold” frontal-subcortical circuits

The dorsolateral prefrontal circuit mediates traditional task-related EFs while the orbital

prefrontal circuit controls self-governed emotional and behavioural regulation (Lichter &

Cummings, 2001). Consistent with this position, Zelazo and Muller (2011) suggest a distinction

between the development of “hot” and “cool” EFs. The former represents relatively affective

(reward/punishment) EF aspects more associated with the orbital and medial prefrontal cortices,

and the latter represents cognitive aspects more associated with the dorsolateral circuit.

Accordingly, “hot” EFs are required for problem situations that necessitate high affective

involvement, such as socioemotional and behavioural functioning. Conversely, relatively abstract

or decontextualized problems, such as those requiring automatic response suppression or

preservation of information in working memory for effective problem solving, require the use of

“cool” EFs (Zelazo & Muller, 2011).

It is now widely acknowledged that ADHD is a heterogeneous disorder, with various

neural pathways and different neuropsychological profiles leading to the manifestation of ADHD

(Castellanos & Tannock, 2002; Nigg, Willcutt, Doyle, & Sonuga-Barke, 2005; Willcutt et al.,

2005). Given our understanding of frontal-subcortical circuit functioning, it is likely that circuit

dysfunction explains the cognitive and behavioural manifestations associated with ADHD

(Dickstein et al., 2006; Hale et al., 2009b; Lichter & Cummings, 2001; Volkow et al., 2001).

Expanding on the notion of “hot” versus “cool” EFs, Castellanos and colleagues (2006) suggest

that deficits in “cool” EF tasks may be implicated in inattentive symptomology and cognitive

6

tasks such as response inhibition and working memory, whereas “hot” EF deficits may be related

to hyperactive and impulsive symptoms and associated with more externalizing and risk-taking

behaviours. Further, “hot” EFs, including delay aversion and emotional regulation, tend to be

more associated with a broad range of ADHD behavioural characteristics, whereas “cool” EFs,

such as sustained attention and inhibitory control, appear to be more related to executive control

dimensions of ADHD (Solanto et al., 2001), This distinction suggests that “hot” and “cool” EFs

may be independent predictors of ADHD subtypes (Zelazo & Muller, 2011).

1.4 Methylphenidate treatment in ADHD

Although meta-analyses support the effectiveness of behavioural treatments for ADHD

(Fabiano et al., 2009), psychostimulant medications remain the most common and efficacious

treatment, with methylphenidate (MPH; Ritalin) being the most researched and prescribed form

of ADHD medication (Barkley, 2006b; Voeller, 2001). Extant treatment literature suggests that

MPH is effective in reducing symptoms of inattentiveness, impulsivity, and hyperactivity in

about 70% of children with ADHD while the child is on the medication (Engert & Pruessner,

2008; Van der Oord, Prins, Oosterlaan, & Emmelkamp, 2008). Psychostimulants directly

increase DA and indirectly increase NE availability in the prefrontal cortex and associated

circuits (Berridge et al., 2006). MPH is a DA agonist that increases the overall concentration of

this neurotransmitter in the prefrontal and associated subcortical areas by blocking the

transporter and hindering DA reuptake in the striatum (Hale et al., 2005).

MPH appears to have the most influence on DA and NE availability in the frontal-subcortical

circuits that control attention and executive functions (Lichter & Cummings, 2001), which are

consistently hypoactive in patients with ADHD (Dickstein et al., 2006; Li, Sham, Owen, & He,

7

2006). Increased availability of DA and NE has been shown to lead to both cognitive and

behavioural improvements in children with ADHD treated with MPH (Engert & Pruessner,

2008). For example, MPH helps to maintain adequate levels of DA in the striatum, enabling

children with ADHD to control their attention effectively (Hood et al., 2005). While MPH

enhances executive modulation of behaviour and cognition, emerging evidence suggests that

differences among DA receptors (Floresco & Magyar, 2006) may lead to differential MPH

effects, with low and clinically relevant doses (i.e., doses that produce clinically relevant plasma

concentrations) improving working memory and sustained attention and higher doses impairing

these cognitive-enhancing actions (Arnsten, 2006; Arnsten, 2009b; Berridge et al., 2006;

Berridge & Devilbiss, 2011). Low and clinically relevant MPH doses produce a preferential

elevation in extracellular DA and NE specific to the prefrontal cortex, whereas higher doses

potently increase extracellular doses of both neurotransmitters widely throughout the brain

(Kuczenski & Segel, 1992). Furthermore, lower doses of MPH improve behavioural and

cognitive processes mediated by the prefrontal cortex, without pronounced tolerance or

sensitization associated with extended treatment (Berridge & Devilbiss, 2011).

Consistent with these findings, higher MPH doses may be necessary for reducing

behavioural disruption and intensity in children and adolescents with ADHD, while lower MPH

doses may be more efficient at improving executive control of attention (Konrad, Gunther,

Hanisch, & Herpertz-Dahlmann, 2004). This could account for variable MPH response found in

children with behaviourally-diagnosed ADHD. Consistent with the hypothesis that varying

medication doses may differentially affect behaviour and cognition, differential MPH effects on

the “cool” (executive working memory; dorsolateral-dorsal cingulate) and “hot” (self-regulation;

orbital-ventral cingulate) frontal-subcortical circuits have been documented in the literature

8

(Castellanos et al., 2006; Kelly, Sonuga-Barke, Scheres, & Castellanos, 2007). Further,

numerous studies have found differential dose-response relationships for behaviour and

cognition in children with ADHD (Hale et al., 2011; Chacko et al., 2005; Pliszka et al., 2007),

suggesting that the best MPH dose for cognition may be lower than the best dose for behaviour,

and MPH may have a stronger linear effect on the “hot” circuit and a stronger curvilinear effect

on the “cool” circuit (Hale et al., 2011).

1.5 Methylphenidate effects on cognitive and neuropsychological functioning

Although an abundance of research documents the efficacy of MPH in reducing

noncompliant and disruptive behaviours in children with ADHD (Pearson et al., 2003; Van der

Oord et al., 2008; Waxmonsky et al., 2008), the effects of MPH on cognition and

neuropsychological functioning have been less consistent (Conners, 2002; Hale et al., 2011).

While some propose that improvements in cognitive and behavioural functioning follow a linear

pattern, with ADHD symptom improvement noted at successively higher stimulant doses

(Pearson et al., 2004; Rapport, Denney, DuPaul, & Gardner, 1994), others have found a

curvilinear response, whereby lower doses of stimulants resulted in initial improvements relative

to placebo, followed by deterioration at higher doses (Hale et al., 2011; Hoeppner et al., 1997;

Sprague & Sleator, 1976). A recent meta-analysis on the effects of MPH on various

neuropsychological tasks found that higher doses of MPH resulted in greater improvements than

lower doses for some tasks but provided no additional improvements on others (Pietrzak,

Mollica, Maruff, & Snyder, 2006). Thus, MPH dose response studies suggest that the optimal

dosing varies across individuals and is related to the functional domain – with high doses

producing greater enhancements in some areas (e.g. attention and vigilance) but providing no

additional improvements, or even resulting in deterioration, in other areas (e.g. planning and

9

cognitive flexibility) (Hale et al., 2011; Pietrzak et al., 2006; Swanson, Baler, & Volkow, 2011).

Inconsistent findings also emerge when investigating the effects of MPH on

neuropsychological functioning in children with ADHD. While some argue that MPH does not

result in any cognitive benefits (e.g., Kemner et al., 2004; Kobel et al., 2008), MPH related

improvements have been documented across a wide range of cognitive and neuropsychological

functions (Pietrzak et al., 2006; Swanson, et al., 2011). For instance, MPH improvements have

been documented for response inhibition and mental flexibility as measured by the Stroop Color-

Word Test (Stroop; Langleben et al., 2006), sustained attention as measured by the Test of

Everyday Attention for Children (TEA-CH; Hood et al., 2005), verbal working memory as

measured by the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (CELF-3), ability to plan and

organize as measured by the writing subtest on the Wechsler Individual Achievement Test

(WIAT-II; Semrud-Clikeman, Pliszka, & Liotti, 2008), and visual memory and impulsivity as

measured by the Go-No Go, and Delayed-Matching-To-Sample tasks (Wilson, Cox, Merkel,

Moore, & Coghill, 2006). However, others have found mixed results, with improvement in some

areas but not others (DeVito et al., 2008; Rhodes, Coghill, & Matthews, 2006). Inconsistent

findings may be due to the monitoring of medication response in terms of their effects on

observed behaviour rather than on cognition (Hood et al., 2005). They may also be due to

differential MPH dose-response effects on cognitive and behavioural functioning (e.g., Hale et

al., 2005; Konrad et al., 2004; Pearson et al., 2004).

In addition to above optimal doses of MPH exacerbating cognitive dysfunction in some

individuals (e.g., Kuhle et al., 2007), some studies have found that high MPH doses may produce

“zombie effects” whereby in addition to poor cognitive functioning, children also become

unresponsive, hypoactive, and hyperfocused in the classroom (Tannock, Shachar, & Logan,

10

1995); however, others found no evidence to support this claim (Douglas, Barr, Desilets, &

Sherman, 1995). Due to response curve inconsistency across and within variables and individual

variability among children diagnosed with ADHD, Hoeppner and colleagues (1997) suggested

that careful examination of behavioural and cognitive MPH dose-response relationships is

warranted, especially for children who have been found to be poor responders to stimulant

treatment (e.g. Barkley, DuPaul, & McMurray, 1991).

1.6 Methylphenidate effects on academic functioning

Similar to cognitive and neuropsychological findings, studies exploring the long-term

effects of MPH on academic functioning and cognition have yielded mixed results (Advokat,

2009; Barbaresi, Katusic, Colligan, Weaver, & Jacobsen, 2007; Raggi & Chronis, 2006). While

some studies have found that MPH improves academic performance in children with ADHD

(Chacko et al., 2005; Powers, Marks, Miller, Newcorn, & Halperin, 2008), recent follow-up

studies have provided data that have generated speculations about the long-term cognitive effects

of MPH on academic functioning (Swanson et al., 2011). Although stimulant medication had a

positive impact on reading and math at the end of a 14-month study phase (MTA, 1999), follow-

up assessments revealed that after three years the initial relative benefits of treatment with

stimulant medication were no longer apparent (Jensen et al., 2007; Molina et al., 2009).

While the nature of the relationship between ADHD and academic underachievement

remains unclear, it is commonly believed that academic difficulties stem from the behavioural

manifestations of ADHD (e.g., inattention, hyperactivity, and impulsivity in the classroom)

(Corkum, McGonnell, & Schachar, 2010). An abundance of research demonstrates the efficacy

of MPH in reducing core symptoms of ADHD (Connor, 2006), in improving cognitive processes

11

considered important for learning (Rhodes et al., 2006), and in enhancing general classroom

functioning including academic accuracy and productivity (Evans et al., 2001). Many clinicians

believe that a reduction in core behaviour symptoms and improvements in underlying cognitive

processes, including attention, working memory, and response inhibition, may improve academic

performance over time by allowing children with ADHD to be more available to learn (Corkum

et al., 2010).

Although the long-term benefits of stimulant treatment have been equivocal (Swanson et

al., 2011), Volkow and colleagues (2009) have argued that MPH improves motivation for the

maintenance of academic tasks. Increased motivation may improve school performance by

increasing DA in the striatum and nucleus accumbens, thereby allowing for greater maintenance

of academic task performance. This hypothesis is consistent with Powers et al.’s (2008) report

that children and adolescents treated with psychostimulant medication achieved better

academically – as measured by standardized test and grades – than those not treated with

medication. Additionally, recent longitudinal research found positive associations between

medication use and standardized math and reading scores (Scheffler et al., 2009), with positive

long-term academic outcomes suggesting that MPH increases the availability for learning

through increased motivation, interest, and reward sensitivity (Volkow et al., 2009).

Despite evidence for increased motivation resulting from MPH treatment, differences in

cognitive and behavioural dose-response relationships may also explain the inconsistencies in

MPH effects on academic functioning (Hale et al., 2011). When differential MPH dose-response

relationships have been evaluated, higher doses often fail to offer additional benefit over lower

doses (Hale, et al., 2011). For example, Chacko et al. (2005) found academic and social

improvements for children following MPH treatment; however, few children showed significant

12

improvement with increased dosage. Similarly, a meta-analysis found that 63.5% of studies

showed improvement in cognitive functioning following MPH treatment with higher doses

producing no additional improvements over lower doses on various tasks (Pietrzak et al., 2006).

This pattern replicates earlier studies showing lower MPH doses leading to academic gains with

increasing dosage producing little additional cognitive and academic benefit (Greenhill et al.,

2001; Hale et al, 2011).

1.7 Purpose of Current Study

The present study builds upon previous research documenting cognitive and behavioural

dose-response relationships at single subject (Hale et al., 2011; Hale et al., 1998; Reddy & Hale,

2007) and group (Hale et al., 2005) levels of analyses. In this investigation, a double-blind

placebo controlled study of MPH response in children with ADHD was conducted. This research

examined whether differential MPH treatment effects emerged on various neuropsychological

measures when children with ADHD were grouped by level of “cool” executive working

memory (EWM) and “hot” self-regulation (SR) frontal-subcortical circuit impairment.

It was hypothesized that the level and pattern of baseline data obtained from the

EWM/SR neuropsychological measures described below would differentiate MPH responders

from nonresponders. In a previous study, Hale et al. (2005) used confirmatory factor analysis to

identify factor loadings and hypothesized relations to one of two frontal-subcortical circuits for a

number of different neuropsychological measures. The authors found that the number of correct

responses on the Stroop Color-Word Test (Stroop), the time on the Trail Making Test-Part B

(TMT-B), the number of omission errors on the Conners’ Continuous Performance Test-II (CPT-

II), the time on the Hale-Denckla Cancellation Test (HDCT), and the number of correct

responses on the Test of Memory and Learning-Digits Backwards (TOMAL-DB) were all related

13

to the EWM or “cool” dorsolateral-dorsal cingulate circuits, while the number of errors on the

Stroop, the number of errors on the TMT-B, the number of commission errors on the CPT-II, the

number of correct responses on the HDCT, and the number of correct responses on the Go No-

Go Test were related to the SR or “hot” orbital-ventral cingulate circuits (Hale et al., 2005; See

Figure 1 and Appendices 1 and 2 for breakdown). In this study, participants were administered

the same neuropsychological measures described above during the baseline unmedicated week.

Children’s raw scores from the baseline week were then multiplied by a factor loading obtained

by Hale and colleagues in 2005, with resultant z-scores used to determine individual baseline

executive impairment. It was predicted that neuropsychological test performance on the

measures indicated above would vary based on the level of baseline executive impairment.

Specifically, it was hypothesized that the lower medication doses would produce better

neuropsychological performance than the higher medication doses for children with moderate to

high baseline cognitive impairment, since previous research has shown that cognition begins to

deteriorate at higher medication doses (Hale et al., 2011).

!

14

.73

– .55

– .53

– .62

.48 – .76 .40

.67

.47

– .68

– .78

!!! ! ! !!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!!

Figure 1. Confirmatory Factor Analysis Frontal-Subcortical Circuit Loadings Note. Adapted from Hale et al., 2005; Stroop = Stroop Color-Word Test; CPT-II = Conners’ Continuous Performance Test-II; HDCT = Hale-Denckla Cancellation Task; TOMAL-DB = Test of Memory and Language – Digits Backwards

Executive/ Working Memory

(Dorsolateral “Cool” Circuit)

Stroop Correct

Trails-B Time

CPT-II Omissions

HDCT Time

TOMAL-DB

Self-Regulation

(Orbital “Hot”

Circuit)

Stroop Errors

Trails-B Errors

CPT-II Commissions

HDCT Correct

Go-No Go Correct

15

Chapter 2: Method

2.1 Participants

Participants were drawn from a sample of 65 elementary and high school students that

were diagnosed by pediatricians with ADHD. Pediatricians used a semi-structured interview,

Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders–Fourth Edition–Text Revision (DSM-IV-

TR; APA, 2000) criteria, and parent and teacher behaviour rating scales to diagnose ADHD-

Inattentive Type (IT), ADHD-Hyperactive-Impulsive Type (HIT), and ADHD-Combined Type

(CT) subtypes. DSM-IV-TR diagnoses were independently confirmed by a licensed psychologist

through a semi-structured interview with parent, child, and/or teacher and review of medical,

developmental, social, and academic histories. Included participants also exhibited significant

inattention, hyperactivity, and/or impulsivity difficulties that interfered with adaptive functioning

at home and school, as conveyed by parent and/or teacher informant reports. Additionally,

participant behaviour ratings were at least 1.5 standard deviations (SDs) above the mean (M =

50; SD = 10) on one or more of the following subscales: Attention Problems of the Achenbach

(1991) Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) or Teacher Report Form (TRF), or the DSM-IV-TR

Inattention and/or Hyperactive-Impulsive subscales of the Conners’ Parent Rating Scales–

Revised: Long Form (CPRS-R:L) or Conners’ Teacher Rating Scales–Revised: Long Form

(CTRS-R:L; Conners, 1997). Participants with more than one secondary diagnosis or those with

a diagnosis of intellectual disability, seizure disorder, brain injury, or other medical condition

affecting neuropsychological performance were excluded.

The final sample included 53 participants ranging in age from 74 to 200 months (M =

119.72 months, SD = 29.97); the majority were in grades one through five (n = 39; 74%). There

were 37 male and 16 female participants. Congruent with epidemiological studies (Barkley,

16

2006b), children diagnosed with ADHD-CT (n = 35) comprised the largest group, followed by

IT (n = 18). Common comorbid diagnoses of LD (n = 13), oppositional defiant disorder/conduct

disorder (ODD/CD; n = 9), and anxiety/depression (n = 5) were noted. Similar to other research

on the comorbidity of ADHD and internalizing disorders (Biederman, Faraone, & Lapey, 1992),

all children diagnosed with anxiety/depression were in the IT group (n = 5). Participants were all

either medication naïve, or they received a wash-out period of two days prior to beginning the

medication trial.

2.2 Procedure

Following pediatrician diagnosis, parents were referred to the Principal Investigator (PI;

last author) for a MPH medication trial. After receiving information regarding the medication,

potential side effects, and medication trial protocol, interested parents saw a licensed

psychologist. The licensed psychologist proceeded to conduct a semi-structured interview,

obtaining informed consent as well as confirming DSM-IV-TR and parent behaviour rating scale

inclusion criteria. During a teacher meeting, the treatment protocol and classroom observation

were described and scheduled. At baseline assessment only, the TRF (Achenbach, 1991) was

used for classroom behaviour assessment. Classroom behaviour assessment at baseline and

treatment follow up included four other forms: the Academic Performance Rating Scale (APRS;

DuPaul, Rapport, & Perriello, 1991) the Conners’ Teacher Rating Scales–Revised: Long Form

(CTRS-R:L; Conners, 1997), the School Situations Questionnaire-Revised (SSQ-R; DuPaul &

Barkley, 1992), and the Side Effects Rating Scale (SERS; Barkley, 1990). After the initial parent

and teaching meetings, participants received a 45-minute classroom observation and a one-hour

17

baseline assessment at the beginning of each four-week trial. During the baseline (B) assessment,

participants were not medicated (See Figure 2).

Figure 2. Procedure for the Double-Blind Placebo Protocol Note. First published in [Postgraduate Medicine, 124(5), 2012, doi:10.3810/pgm.2012.09.2592] © Postgraduate Medicine, a division of JTE Multimedia, LLC. The study pharmacist prepared the placebo and medication conditions and randomly

assigned children to one of six trial orders of the placebo (P), low dose (L), and high dose (H)

conditions (P-L-H, P-H-L, L-P-H, L-H-P, H-L-P, H-P-L). In the medication conditions, low

doses were calculated as 0.15mg/kg/dose, while the high dose was calculated at 0.30mg/kg/dose,

and rounded to the nearest 2.5 mg (range 2.5 mg to 30 mg per dose). Lactose-filled opaque

Pediatrician evaluation and

diagnosis!

Referral to PI for medication trial !

Confirmation of diagnosis by Psychologist!

Parent and teacher meeetings;

Informed consent!

!Baseline: !

TRF, classroom observations, non-

medicated asessment, parent and teacher ratings !

!

Placebo:!classroom

observations, neuropsych

assessment, parent and teacher ratings !

Low dose: classroom

observations, neuropsych

assessment, parent and teacher ratings !

High dose: classroom

observations, neuropsych

assessment, parent and teacher ratings !

18

tablets were administered for the placebo condition, while in the active drug conditions the

lactose-filled opaque capsules contained a ground MPH tablet. Both placebo and active drug

conditions were delivered twice per day. To ensure the safety of the patient and to monitor

quality control, the physician, pharmacist, and PI were not blind to the order of conditions.

However, the research assistants, teachers, parents, and participants were all unaware of the

order of conditions.

A series of neuropsychological instruments were used to assess attention, working

memory, inhibition, and self-regulation through auditory, visual, verbal, and motor domains.

Graduate students, trained and supervised by the PI, administered the tests in the same order on

the last day of each condition. The neuropsychological assessments occurred within one to two

hours after medication was administered to the children. Classroom observations took place on

the same day as the assessments, within one to two hours of the second daily dose of medication.

Off-task, fidgeting, vocalizing, playing with objects, and out of seat observable behaviours were

determined using an adaptation of the Restricted Academic Task (RAT; Barkley, 1990). During

the observational period of classroom instructional activities, a 20-second momentary time

sampling technique was utilized. In order to ensure inter-rater reliability of observational

methods, videotaped classroom recordings were used for training purposes prior to data

collection, with inter-rater reliability measured at .90 or higher for all graduate students after

receiving training in the observational procedures.

To determine MPH dose-response patterns, the ordinal data was subjected to a non-

parametric randomization test for ranks (NPStat; May, Mason, Hunter, & Wells, 1990) used to

approximate repeated measures of a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) in the absence

of normal data. At the conclusion of the study, the order of conditions was revealed and brief

19

reports were provided to parents and pediatricians for subsequent clinical decision-making.

In 2005, Hale and colleagues used Structural Equation Modeling (SEM) to develop a

model of the Executive/Working Memory (EWM) and Self-Regulation (SR) factors, which were

hypothesized to reflect dorsolateral-dorsal cingulate and orbital-ventral cingulate frontal-

subcortical circuit functioning. This model was based on non-medicated, neuropsychological test

performance of children with ADHD and included the same neuropsychological measures that

were used in this study, including the: Go-No Go Test, Stroop-Color Word Test, Trails-B,

Conner’s Continuous Performance Test-II, Hale-Denckla Cancellation Task, Wisconsin

Selective Reminding Test, and Test of Memory and Language – Digits Backwards as well as the

Wisconsin Card Sorting Test and the Controlled Oral Word Association Test at baseline only.

See Figure 1 for a breakdown of the SEM derived factor scores for each of the

neuropsychological measures analyzed in this study.

In the current sample, raw scores from baseline (non-medicated) neuropsychological test

performance were multiplied by the Hale et al. (2005) derived factor scores for the same

neuropsychological measures to produce EWM dorsolateral “cool” and SR orbital “hot” scores.

Although Hale and colleagues (2005) hypothesized that EWM factors would be correlated with

DSM-IV-TR inattentive symptoms and the SR factors would be correlated with DSM-IV-TR

hyperactive/impulsive symptoms, they found that both the EWM (r = .502, p = .001) and SR (r =

.327. p = .034) correlated only with the hyperactive/impulsive symptoms. In addition, the EWM

and SR scores were highly correlated (r = -.76, p < .001). As such, the regression-based saved

EWM and SR factor scores were added to produce a combined EWM/SR impairment score.

This impairment score was then converted to a z-score for each participant. It was assumed that

positive z-scores reflected low levels of impairment and negative z-scores reflected higher levels

20

of impairment since the original Hale et al. (2005) analyses with the entire ADHD sample

yielded only moderate differences between the standardization sample and the total ADHD one.

As a result, z-scores were used to calculate no apparent (N/A; +1.01 or higher; n = 8), low (0.01

to +1.00; n = 20), moderate (0.00 to –1.00; n = 15), and high (–1.01 or lower; n = 10) executive

impairment groups.

2.3 Instrumentation

A variety of neuropsychological measures were used over the course of four-week trial to

identify medication effects on test performance over time. The utility of these reliable and valid

instruments when diagnosing ADHD and determining treatments effects is well documented in

the literature (Hale & Fiorello, 2004; Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996; Sergeant, Geurts, &

Oosterlaan, 2002; Willcutt et al., 2005), and previous studies have shown no significant practice

effects during medication trials (Hale et al., 2005, 2006; Hoeppner et al., 1997).

2.3.1 Go No-Go Test (Trommer, Hoeppner, & Zecker, 1991).

The Go No-Go test is a clinical tool with a long history of use in neuropsychological

assessments. It is used as a measure of motor inhibition, and has been used to measure

inattention and impulsivity in individuals with ADHD (Trommer et al., 1991). In this audiotaped

version, children raise and lower their index finger following a single beep sound or “Go” signal

and refrain from movement when presented with the double beep sound or “No-Go” signal. The

number of correct responses out of a total possible of 30 is recorded and used as a measure of

“hot” SR functioning. Research suggests that consistent group differences in performances are

found between controls and children with ADHD on measures of motor response inhibition,

including the Go No-Go (Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996), with consistent replicated evidence

21

demonstrating a deficit in executive motor inhibition in ADHD (Nigg, 2001). In a meta-analysis

conducted in 2005, signification differences between groups with and without ADHD were

obtained most consistently for response inhibition tasks similar to the Go No-Go (Willcutt et al.,

2005). The Go No-Go test has also been shown to be sensitive to medication effects in children

with ADHD, with even modest doses improving performance by decreasing the tendency to

make impulsive commission errors (failure to inhibit response to the No-Go signal) (Trommer et

al., 1991). It is also one of the measures used in studies that show changes in ADHD brain

functioning following stimulant treatment (see Vaidya et al., 1998). Neuroimaging research

suggests that children with ADHD do not activate frontostriatal regions in the same manner as

control participants, and tend to rely on a more diffuse network of regions, including more

dorsolateral and posterior brain regions (Durston et al., 2003; Tamm et al., 2004). Imaging

research pinpoints the right inferior prefrontal cortex as a crucial region for the ability to stop a

prepotent motor response (Rubia et al., 2005), and several studies report greater activation in the

dorsolateral and orbital prefrontal cortices during the inhibitory trials of the Go No-Go task

(Casey et al., 1997; Schulz et al., 2005). Imaging research also suggests that lower than normal

activation of the inferior prefrontal cortex may be responsible for the poor inhibitory control

often displayed by individuals with ADHD (Rubia et al., 1999).

2.3.2 Stroop Color-Word Test (Stroop; Golden, 1978).

Children are presented with colour words (e.g. “red”) that are printed in non-matching

coloured ink (e.g. blue ink). Participants name the colour of the ink rather than read the word that

is printed. Scoring consists of the number of correct word responses, used as a measure of “cool”

EWM functioning, and the number of errors (i.e., reading word instead of naming ink), used as a

22

measure of “hot” SR functioning, completed in 45 seconds. Test-retest reliability was found to be

.91, and some practice effects have been shown in college students (Spreen & Strauss, 1998).

However, the researchers suggest that increases in performance may not affect interpretation of

results if interpretation is based on the pattern rather than the level of performance. Results from

several meta-analysis indicate that children and adolescents with ADHD consistently exhibit

pooper performance on the Stroop task when compared to individuals without clinical diagnoses

(Homack & Riccio, 2004; Van Mourik et al., 2005), and neuroimaging studies have reported

greater activation in the lateral prefrontal cortex in ADHD patients during trials requiring

inhibitory control in the Stroop task (Bush et al., 1999).

2.3.3 Test of Memory and Learning-Digits Backwards (TOMAL-DB; Reynolds & Bigler, 1994).

This digit span task requires children to listen to orally presented numbers with spans

increasing in length and to repeat the digits in reverse order. The number of correct digits

recalled in the correct reverse order is used for scoring purposes. The Digit Span test is a

commonly used working memory measure, and the backward version has been found to sensitive

to ADHD neuropsychological and behavioural impairment (Hale, Hoeppner, & Fiorello, 2002).

Meta-analyses suggest that significant differences between groups with and without ADHD

emerge in the majority (55%) of studies that include verbal working memory tasks (Willcutt et

al., 2005). In another comprehensive meta-analysis of the relation between ADHD and working

memory, Martinussen et al. (2005) suggest that children with ADHD exhibit deficits in multiple

component of working memory, including verbal storage (effect size = 0.47) and verbal working

memory requiring manipulation (effect size = 0.43). Neuroimaging research has also

demonstrated that during task conditions requiring and increase in working memory, children

23

and adolescents with ADHD exhibit greater and more varied prefrontal cortex activation

compared to age-matched control participants (Rypma & D’Esposito, 2000; Sheridan, Hinshaw,

& D’Esposito, 2007).

2.3.4 Wisconsin Selective Reminding Test of Memory (WSRTM; Newby, 1999).

The examiner reads a word list, and children repeat all words remembered. The examiner

then repeats only the words missed on the previous trial and asks participants to repeat all words

recalled, including words recalled on the previous trial. This selective reminding continues until

the word list is completely recalled for two consecutive trials or until all ten trials are completed.

Words that are recalled without immediate reminding for two or more trials are assumed to have

entered long-term memory storage. The consistency of retrieval of words that have entered long-

term storage is also measured over successive trials. Four equivalent alternate forms were used,

and long-term storage and consistent long-term retrieval were analyzed in this study. The

alternate lists were found to be equivalent on frequency of occurrence in written English,

imagery, concreteness, and meaningfulness (all overall F-ratios and all pairwise t-tests > .05)

(Newby, 1999). Literature on similar selective reminding tests suggests minimal practice effects,

and increasing performance with age, especially in children (Spreen & Strauss, 1998). The

SRTM has been used in previous research on ADHD (Barkley, Anastopoulos, Guevremont, &

Fletcher, 1991), with equivocal effects of stimulants on SRTM noted in some studies (Barkley,

DuPaul, & McMurray, 1991; Hoeppner et al., 1997), but in others MPH response has been

positive for long-term storage and consistent long-term retrieval (Barkley, McMurray,

Edelbrock, & Robbins, 1989; Evans, Gualtieri, & Amara, 1986).

24

2.3.5 Trail Making Test–Part B (TMT-B; Reitan & Wolfson, 1985).

The Trail Making Test (TMT) is one of the most popular neuropsychological tests,

providing information on visual search, scanning, processing speed, and mental flexibility

(Tombaugh, 2004). Part B requires participants to shift between numbers and letters in ascending

order (i.e. 1-A-2-B). With Part A and Part B correlating only .49 with each other, research

suggests that the two alternate forms measure different functions, with Part B including more

visual interference and requiring more visual-perceptual processing ability than Part A

(Heilbronner et al., 1991).With alternate forms constructed by Hale (1997), only Part B was

utilized for the purposes of this study, as it has been found to be closely related to other tests of

timed executive function, suggesting that it measures frontal lobe dysfunction (Libon et al.,

1994). Both completion time and errors are recorded, with completion time used as a measure of

“cool” EWM functioning, and errors used as a measure of “hot” SR functioning. Meta-analyses

have found that TMT-B completion time can discriminate between children with ADHD and

controls (effect size range 0.55 to 0.75) (Pennington & Ozonoff, 1996; Willcutt et al., 2005), and

research suggests that TMT-B errors are sensitive to frontal-executive impairment (Hale et al.,

2009; Stuss et al., 2001). Interrater reliability for TMT-B has been reported as .90 (Fals-Stewart,

1991), and although some practice effects have been noted, these were found only for Part A,

and not for Part B (Lezak, 1995).

2.3.6 Conners’ Continuous Performance Test–II (CPT-II; Conners & MHS Staff, 2004).

This computerized continuous performance test requires sustained attention to visually

presented stimuli on a computer screen. The child watches the screen and responds each time a

letter appears (non-target stimulus) with the exception of one letter (target stimulus) for which no

25

response is required. Omissions were used as a measure of “cool” EWM functioning, and

commissions were used as a measure of “hot” SR functioning. Split-half reliability is .83 for

commission, and .94 for omissions, whereas test-retest reliability was found to be .65 (p < .01)

for commissions and .84 (p < .01) for omissions. The test-retest data suggests that the CPT-II is

relatively unaffected by practice effects, and all of the CPT measures used in this study yielded

non-significant p-values in a pre-post analysis designed to measure systematic improvement or

decline in performance over repeated administrations (Conners & MHS Staff, 2004). Meta-

analyses have found that stimulant medication improves CPT performance for both control

subjects and ADHD populations (Riccio et al., 2001), with CPT Omission errors consistently

producing the most significant differences between groups with and without ADHD (Willcutt et

al., 2005). Computerized CPT Omission measures have also been found to be sensitive to ADHD

medication response (Hale et al., 2005; Willcutt et al., 2005). A recent meta-analytic review

revealed large effect sizes for number of commissions (0.98), omissions (1.34), and variability in

response times (0.61) between children with ADHD and controls, suggesting that children with

ADHD committed more errors and had slower and/or more variable reaction times than controls

without ADHD. Performance over time effects were more moderate for all variables and the

authors suggested that these differences could be attributed entirely to sampling errors (Huang-

Pollock et al., 2012). Thus, for the purposes of this study we decided to focus on the most

consistent findings with CPT use in the ADHD population, and focus on commission and

omission errors.

26

2.3.7 Hale-Denckla Cancellation Test (HDCT; Hale, 1997).

This paper and pencil continuous performance test requires participants to cross out the

target stimulus embedded in a group of distractor stimuli. It is a measure of visual attention,

discrimination, scanning, tracking, memory, and graphomotor speed. Completion time and

number correct out of 30 is recorded. The HDCT is an adaptation and extension of the

Cancellation of Rapidly Reoccurring Target Figures Test (Rudel, Denckla, & Broman, 1978).

The HDCT has been found to discriminate children with ADHD from controls and children with

SLD, and was found to be sensitive to medication dose-response relationships in previous

ADHD research (Hale et al., 1998, 2005, 2006, Hoeppner et al., 1997). Previous research also

suggests that children with ADHD make more omission and commission errors on cancellation

tasks, suggesting that cancellation tasks measure aspects of executive attention (Fischer, Barkley,

Smallish, & Fletcher, 2005; Woods & Mark, 2007).

27

Chapter 3: Results

3.1 Overview

A repeated-measures MANOVA was computed for each of the seven neuropsychological

variables to determine individual response, with Drug Condition (B, P, L, H) serving as the

within-subjects factor and Impairment Level (N/A, Low, Moderate, High), based on Hale et al.

(2005), serving as the between-subjects factor. The results for each of the variables are presented

in Table 1 and graphically depicted in Figure 2. A summary of means, standard deviations, and

dose-response relationships for the individual tests can be found in Appendices 1 and 2.

Table 1 provides a breakdown of the frontal-subcortical circuit (dorsolateral-dorsal

cingulate [“cool”] and orbital-ventral cingulate [“hot]) that each of the neuropsychological tests

used in this study are hypothesized to reflect, according to the model developed by Hale at al.

(2005). The F-statistic and p-value associated with the repeated-measures MANOVAs for test

performance was broken down into each of the four Impairment groups (N/A, Low, Moderate,

and High). The post-hoc column specifically identifies the test scores that differed significantly

from one another across Drug conditions (B, P, L, H) at each Impairment group level. For

example, looking at the TOMAL-DB test, children with no apparent (N/A) baseline impairment

showed no significant test performance differences across the four drug conditions. However,

children with Low baseline executive impairment had Baseline (identified with a “1”) test

performances that were significantly lower than both the Placebo (identified with a superscripted

“b”) and the High dose (identified with a superscripted “d”) Drug Conditions.

28

Table 1. MPH Dose-Response Relationships for EWM/SR Impairment Groups for Individual Tests “Cool” Circuit Tests

“Hot” Circuit Tests F

F

p

p

Post-Hoc

Post-Hoc

F p

p

Post-Hoc

Post-Hoc TOMAL-DB

TOMAL-DB

Go No-Go

Go No-Go N/A 1.79 .266 N/S N/A 2.09 .220 N/S Low 6.04 .004 1bd Low 12.05 <.001 1bcd;2d Moderate 4.68 .022 1c Moderate 19.33 <.001 1bcd High 4.21 .029 1c;2cd High 10.32 .006 1bcd;2cd

Stroop Word Stroop Errors N/A 6.19 .039 1bcd

N/A 1.03 .453 N/S Low 8.94 <.001 1bcd;2d;3d Low 13.89 <.001 3a;4abc Moderate 19.93 <.001 1bcd Moderate 6.81 .006 3ab;4ab High 3.30 .088 1cd High 4.26 .035 3ab;4a

Trails Time Trails Errors N/A 1.48 .266 N/S N/A .64 .624 N/S Low 9.39 <.001 2a;3a;4a Low 3.97 .026 3b;4b Moderate 14.85 <.001 2a;3a;4a Moderate 6.86 .006 3a;4a High 3.14 .105 3bd High 9.18 .008 3ab;4ab

CPT Omissions CPT Commissions N/A 3.13 .126 4ab N/A .483 .708 N/S Low 6.61 .001 2a;3ab;4a Low 4.45 .018 3a;4a Moderate 6.64 .007 2a;3ab;4ab Moderate 2.49 .111 3ab;4c High 4.16 .065 3a High 1.83 .229 3a;4a

WSRTM HDCT N/A 1.59 .243 N/S N/A .725 .579 N/S Low 4.30 .015 1cd Low 1.61 .207 N/S Moderate 4.94 .019 1bcd Moderate 5.75 .011 1bcd High 8.82 .009 1bcd High 14.78 .002 1bcd;2cd Note. First published in [Postgraduate Medicine, 124(5), 2012, doi:10.3810/pgm.2012.09.2592]

© Postgraduate Medicine, a division of JTE Multimedia, LLC. N/S = not significant; N/A = no apparent baseline executive impairment (n = 8); Low = low baseline executive impairment (n = 20); Moderate = moderate baseline executive impairment (n = 15); High = high baseline executive impairment (n = 10); 1 = Baseline; 2 = Placebo; 3 = Low dose; 4 = High dose aLess than Baseline; bLess than Placebo; cLess than Low dose; dLess than High dose. Means and standard deviations of test performance for all measures across all conditions can be found in Appendices 1 and 2. CPT Commissions was not displayed in a graph as no relevant significant differences emerged.

29

Figure 3. Dose-Response Relationships for Neuropsychological Tests by Impairment Group. Note. First published in [Postgraduate Medicine, 124(5), 2012, doi:10.3810/pgm.2012.09.2592] © Postgraduate Medicine, a division of JTE Multimedia, LLC. No Apparent = No apparent baseline executive impairment (n = 8); Low = low baseline executive impairment (n = 20); Moderate = moderate baseline executive impairment (n = 15); High = high baseline executive impairment (n = 10). CPT Commissions was not displayed in a graph as no significant differences emerged.

30

3.2 Individual neuropsychological assessment measure results

The data were subjected to repeated measures MANOVA using Pillai’s Trace to

determine treatment effects. Mauchly Sphericity tests were used to determine whether a

multivariate or univariate approach to the data was warranted. Mauchly’s Test of Sphericity

assesses the null hypothesis that the error covariance matrix of the orthonormalized transformed

neuropsychological test performance was proportional to an identity matrix. Box’s M test for the

equality of covariance matrices was used to test the homogeneity of variance assumption, and

Levine’s Test of Equality of Error Variances was used to assess the null hypothesis that the error

variance of the neuropsychological test performance was equal across groups.

3.2.1 Go No-Go Test

Although Mauchly’s test of sphericity assumption for Drug was met (χ2(5) = 7.39, p =

.193), as was Levene’s test for the equality of error variances (p range .088 to .600), a

multivariate approach could not be completed due to an equality of covariance matrices violation

as determined by Box’s M test (F(30, 2,905.05) = 2.02, p = .001). Huynh-Feldt univariate tests

of within-subjects effects showed a highly significant Drug effect (F(3, 147) = 42.51, p < .001,

η2 = .47, power = 1.00), and Drug by Impairment Group interaction (F(9, 147) = 3.70, p < .001,

η2 = .19, power = .99). Tests of within-subjects orthogonal/polynomial contrasts revealed linear

and quadratic effects for both Drug and the Drug by Impairment interaction, suggesting that

dose-response curves were not uniform across conditions and impairment levels. A main effect

for the Impairment group (F(3, 49) = 4.59, p = .007, η2 = .22) was also observed, suggesting

significant group differences when collapsed across drug conditions.

31

With MANOVA results suggesting that test performance varied based on the level of

EWM/SR impairment, repeated-measures MANOVAs were then computed for each of the four

impairment groups separately. As indicated in Table 1, and depicted in Figure 2, post-hoc tests

revealed a significant medication response for the Low, Moderate, and High impairment groups,

with performance improving across Drug conditions.

3.2.2 Stroop Color-Word Test (Stroop)

For the Stroop test, we evaluated both the overall word score and the number of errors

made. For the overall word score, while Mauchly’s test of sphericity assumption for Drug was

met (χ2(5) = 7.40, p = .193), as was Levene’s test for the equality of error variances (p range

.211 to .979), a multivariate approach to the data could not be completed due to a violation of the

equality of covariance matrices as determined by Box’s M test (F(30, 2,905.05) = 1.56, p =

.028). While Huynh-Feldt univariate tests of within-subjects effects showed a highly significant

effect for Drug (F(3, 147) = 30.31, p < .001, η2 = .38, power = 1.00), the Drug by Impairment

Group interaction was not significant. Tests of within-subjects orthogonal/polynomial contrasts

revealed a quadratic effect for the Drug condition, suggesting that dose-response curves were not

uniform across conditions. There was also a main effect for Impairment group, (F(3, 49) = 12.91,

p < .001, η2 = .44), suggesting that there were group differences regardless of medication

condition. Post-hoc analyses presented in Table 1 revealed significant differences between Drug

conditions in all four Impairment conditions.

For Stroop errors, there were no violations of MANOVA assumptions, with Box’s M test

(F(30, 2,905.05) = 1.23, p = .183), Mauchly’s test of sphericity (χ2(5) = 10.60, p = .060), and

Levene’s test for the equality of error variances (p range .07 to .70) all nonsignificant. Using the

32

multivariate approach, Pillai’s Trace revealed a highly significant Drug effect (F(3, 47) = 17.14,

p < .001, η2 = .52, power = 1.00), and a significant Drug by Impairment Group interaction (F(9,

147) = 2.07, p = .036, η2 = .11, power = .85). Tests of within-subjects orthogonal/polynomial

contrasts revealed a linear effect for Drug and a cubic effect for the Drug by Impairment Group

Interaction indicating different response curves for different levels of impairment. However, no

main effect was found for Impairment Group. Post-hoc analyses reported in Table 1 revealed

significant Drug effects for the Low, Moderate, and High Impairment groups.

3.2.3 Test of Memory and Learning-Digits Backwards (TOMAL-DB)

For the TOMAL-DB test, a multivariate approach to the data could not be used as

Mauchly’s test of sphericity assumption for drug was violated (χ2(5) = 15.54, p = .008), as was

Levene’s test for the equality of error variances (p range .035 to .091), and the equality of

covariance matrices as determined by Box’s M test (F(30, 2,905.05) = 1.90, p = .002). Huynh-

Feldt univariate tests of within-subjects effects showed a highly significant effect for Drug (F(3,

147) = 10.46, p < .001, η2 = .18, power = 1.00), with the Drug by Impairment Group interaction

not reaching significance. Tests of within-subjects orthogonal/polynomial contrasts revealed a

linear and quadratic effect for Drug indicating that dose-response curves were not uniform across

conditions, which could suggest individual response curve differences. However, no main effect

was found for Impairment suggesting that there was no defining overall drug trial performance

pattern between impairment groups. As revealed in Table 1, post-hoc analyses revealed a

significant performance differences across Drug Conditions for the Low, Moderate, and High

Impairment groups.

33

3.2.4 Wisconsin Selective Reminding Test of Memory (WSRTM)

For the WSRTM, we looked at the storage-consistent retrieval ratio of task performance.

A multivariate approach to the data could not be used as Mauchly’s test of sphericity assumption

for drug was violated (χ2(5) = 15.20, p = .010), as was Levene’s test for the equality of error

variances (p range .025 to .855), and there was also a violation of the equality of covariance

matrices as determined by Box’s M test (F(30, 2,905.05) = 2.27, p < .001). Huynh-Feldt

univariate tests of within-subjects effects showed a highly significant effect for Drug (F(2.84,

139) = 16.24, p < .001, η2 = .25, power = 1.00); however, the Drug by Impairment group effect

was not significant. Tests of within-subjects orthogonal/polynomial contrasts revealed linear and

quadratic effects for Drug, suggesting that dose-response curves were not uniform across

conditions. However, no main effect for Impairment group (F(3, 49) = 0.55, p = .653, η2 = .03)

was found. Post-hoc analyses revealed a significant Drug effect for the Low, Moderate, and High

Impairment as depicted in Table 1.

3.2.5 Trail Making Test Part B (TMT-B)

For the TMT-B, analyses were conducted for the number of errors made and for total

completion time. Looking first at errors, while Mauchly’s test of sphericity assumption for drug

was not violated (χ2(5) = 7.31, p = .199) and there was also no violation of the equality of

covariance matrices (Box’s M test, F(30, 2,905.05) = 1.13, p = .291), a univariate approach was

required because of a violation in the equality of error variances according to Levene’s test (p

range .025 to .715). Huynh-Feldt univariate tests of within-subjects effects showed highly

significant Drug (F(3, 147) = 15.58, p < .001, η2 = .24, power = 1.00), and Drug by Impairment

interaction effects (F(9, 147) = 6.07, p < .001, η2 = .27, power = 1.00). Tests of within-subjects

34

orthogonal/polynomial contrasts revealed linear and cubic effects for Drug, and the Drug by

Impairment interaction, suggesting non-uniform dose-response curves across conditions.

Analyses also revealed a main effect for Impairment (F(3, 49) = 12.30, p < .001, η2 = .43).

Referring to Table 1, post-hoc analyses revealed a significant Drug effect for the Low, Moderate,

and High Impairment groups.

For TMT-B time, a multivariate approach to the data could not be used as Mauchly’s test

of sphericity assumption for drug was violated (χ2(5) = 132.95 p < .001) as was Levene’s test for

the equality of error variances (p range < .001 to .172), and there was also a violation of the

equality of covariance matrices as determined by Box’s M test (F(30, 2,905.05) = 5.35, p <

.001). Huynh-Feldt univariate tests of within-subjects effects showed a highly significant effect

for Drug (F(1.34, 147) = 10.89, p = .001, η2 = .18, power = .95). The Drug by Impairment group

interaction was also significant (F(4, 147) = 2.79, p = .034, η2 = .15, power = .73). Tests of

within-subjects orthogonal/polynomial contrasts revealed a linear and quadratic effect for Drug,

suggesting that dose-response curves were not uniform across conditions. There was also a main

effect for Impairment group (F(3, 49) = 6.93 p = .001, η2 = .30). Significant differences in

performance time were noted across Drug conditions for the Low, Moderate, and High

Impairment Groups during post-hoc analysis, as shown in Table 1.

3.2.6 Conners’ Continuous Performance Test-II (CPT-II)

For the CPT-II task we looked at both omission and commission errors made. Looking

first at omission errors, a multivariate approach to the data could not be used as Mauchly’s test of

sphericity assumption for drug was violated (χ2(5) = 84.62, p < .001) as was Levene’s test for the

equality of error variances (p range .006 to .205), and there was also a violation of the equality of

35

covariance matrices as determined by Box’s M test (F(30, 2,905.05) = 3.53, p < .001). Huynh-

Feldt univariate tests of within-subjects effects showed a highly significant effect for Drug

(F(1.61, 79) = 14.84, p < .001, η2 = .23, power = 1.00) and the Drug by Impairment group

interaction (F(4.83, 79) = 2.70, p = .028, η2 = .14, power = .78). For the Drug condition,

orthogonal/polynomial contrasts revealed both linear and quadratic effects suggesting varying

dose-response curves across conditions and groups. There was also a main effect for Impairment

group (F(3, 49) = 3.49, p = .023, η2 = .18). Post-hoc analyses revealed significant performance

differences across Drug conditions for each of the four Impairment levels, as indicated in Table

1.

For CPT-II commission errors, a multivariate approach to the data was used as Mauchly’s

test of sphericity assumption for drug was not violated (χ2(5) = 8.03, p = .155) neither was

Levene’s test for the equality of error variances (p range .060 to .126), and there was also no

violation of the equality of covariance matrices as determined by Box’s M test (F(30, 2,905.05)

= .88, p = .662). Although Pillai’s Trace multivariate tests showed a highly significant effect for

Drug (F(3, 47) = 6.21, p < .001, η2 = .28, power = .95), the interaction between Drug and

Impairment group was not significant. While tests of within-subjects orthogonal/polynomial

contrasts revealed a linear effect for Drug, suggesting that dose-response curves were not

uniform across conditions, no main effect for Impairment group was found. As outlined in Table

1, post-hoc analyses revealed a significant Drug effect for the Low, Moderate, and High

Impairment groups.

36

3.2.7 Hale–Denckla Cancellation Test (HDCT)

For the HDCT number of correct responses, although the equality of covariance matrices

assumption was not violated (Box’s M F(30, 2,905.05) = 1.25, p = .167), a multivariate approach

to the data could not be used as Mauchly’s test of sphericity assumption for Drug was violated

(χ2(5) = 23.37, p < .001) as was Levene’s test for the equality of error variances (p range < .001

to .677). Huynh-Feldt univariate tests of within-subjects effects showed a highly significant

effect for Drug (F(2.60, 147) = 27.89, p < .001, η2 = .36, power = 1.00) and for the interaction of

Drug and Impairment group (F(7.80, 147) = 6.14, p < .001, η2 = .27, power = 1.00). Tests of

within-subjects orthogonal/polynomial contrasts revealed a linear and quadratic effect for both

the Drug and the Drug by Impairment level interaction, suggesting that dose-response curves

were not uniform across conditions and impairment levels. A main effect for Impairment group

(F(3, 49) = 8.54, p < .001, η2 = .34), was also found, suggesting that there was a performance

pattern between impairment groups. Post-hoc analyses revealed a significant Drug effect for the

Moderate and High Impairment groups as outlined in Table 1.

37

Chapter 4: Discussion

4.1 Overview of findings

In the present study, children with behaviourally diagnosed ADHD underwent double-

blind placebo controlled MPH trials with cognitive/neuropsychological, data collected during

four different conditions (B, P, L, H) throughout the randomized controlled trial (RCT). Results

revealed highly significant MPH treatment effects with differences in performance emerging for

children based upon the structural equation modeling (SEM)-determined level of EWM/SR

impairment on a number of the cognitive/neuropsychological measures as depicted in Figure 3,

and summarized in Table 1 and Appendices 1 and 2. Orthogonal polynomial within-subjects

contrasts for Drug revealed that not all individuals had a uniform linear MPH response across

variables, with quadratic and cubic effects found. For those with no apparent (N/A) and low

baseline EWM/SR impairment, MPH response was often poor; while for those with moderate or

high baseline impairment, MPH response was often dramatic. In addition, differential cognitive

MPH response patterns emerged for those with moderate and high baseline impairment with

performance deteriorating on some measures at the higher medication dose.

Although statistically significant performance differences did not emerge between the

low dose and high dose Drug conditions for all of the tests, the graphs presented in Figure 2

suggest that performance began to deteriorate at the higher dosage for the Go No-Go, Stroop

errors, TOMAL-DB, HDCT errors, and the TMT-B errors and time, a trend that was especially

salient at Moderate and High levels of impairment. Furthermore, test performance for children

with N/A and Low baseline executive impairment showed minimal improvement across the Drug

conditions for the Go No-Go, Stroop word, TOMAL-DB, HDCT correct, and TMT-B time,

suggesting that children with minimal baseline executive impairment may not respond well to

38

stimulant medication – at least as measured by performance on these neuropsychological

measures.

4.2 Implications for “hot” and “cool” circuit executive functions

While a neurophysiological explanation for the differential cognitive MPH dose-response

relationships is beyond the scope and evidence provided here, some speculation appears to be

warranted given similar empirical findings (Arnsten, 2006; Berridge et al., 2006; Konrad et al.,

2004). As suggested earlier, it is possible that MPH has a differential effect on the “hot” (e.g.,

SR; orbital-ventral cingulate) and “cool” (e.g., EWM; dorsolateral-dorsal cingulate) frontal-

striatal-thalamic circuits (Castellanos et al., 2006; Kelly et al., 2007). The phylogenetically

younger “cool” dorsal circuits, necessary for deliberative executive processing, working

memory, and attention control, may respond best to low dose stimulants; while the

phylogenetically older “hot” ventral circuits, necessary for affective decision making, self-

regulation, and behavioural control, may respond best to high dose stimulants (Figner,

Mackinlay, Wilkening, & Weber, 2009; Roiser et al., 2009; Steinberg, 2008).

In this study, while most of the children with moderate to high baseline impairment

responded cognitively to stimulant medication, quadratic and/or cubic effects were observed with

the high MPH dose often resulting in deteriorating test performance. For example, inspection of

Go No-Go, TOMAL-DB, HDCT, and TMT-B test means (See Appendices 1 and 2) revealed

better performance on the lower MPH dosage. Additionally, the Stroop and TMT-B errors were

greater on the higher dosage, especially in the high impairment group. For those with ADHD and

high baseline impairment, the “cool” neuropsychological circuits appear to be optimized with

low dose stimulants with deterioration occurring with increased medication. With clinician focus

39

on behavioural criteria for ADHD diagnosis and for judging treatment response, affected

children may be receiving the correct dose for behavioural and self-regulatory control, but this in

turn may limit “cool” executive functions necessary for effective problem-solving and academic

learning.

4.3 Implications for academic achievement in ADHD

Using MPH to alleviate overt ADHD behavioural problems may be the preeminent

concern of parents and teachers, but the MPH RCT results presented here are consistent with the

cognitive and academic achievement MPH literature, suggesting that the best dose for cognition,

neuropsychological functioning, and academic achievement may be lower than the best dose for

behavioural control (Chacko et al., 2005; Hale et al., 2011; Pliszka et al., 2007; Teicher, Polcari,

& McGreenery, 2008). Although behaviour was not directly assessed for the purposes of this

study, previous research has found that MPH has a linear effect on behaviour – as measured by

behaviour rating scales – suggesting that increasing medication doses reduces problematic

behaviours (Hale et al., 2005; Hale et al., 2011). The differential dose-response relationships

reported here could explain why long-term treatment MPH efficacy remains limited in ADHD

(Jensen et al., 2007) and the inconsistent findings of MPH effects on academic achievement

(Langberg & Becker, 2012; Van der Oord et al., 2008). It is possible that by focusing our clinical

attention on the “hot” behavioural/self-regulation circuit for diagnosis and MPH titration, we are

limiting the long-term academic gains in many children with ADHD because higher medication

doses may result in deleterious cognitive and neuropsychological effects. If the optimal dose is

chosen solely on behavioural criteria, children will likely struggle with learning and academic

40

achievement because their “cool” dorsal executive and attention control/working memory

functions may be impaired as a result of (Berridge et al., 2006).

“Cool” dorsolateral-dorsal cingulate executive functions such as sustained attention,

flexible problem solving, fluid reasoning, and working memory are important predictors of

academic areas such as math reasoning, reading comprehension, written expression, higher level

implicit language, and reading, math, and writing fluency (Biederman et al., 2004; Decker, Hill,

& Dean, 2007; Denckla, 1996; Goldstein & Naglieri, 2008; Hale & Fiorello, 2004; Loe &

Feldman, 2007). However, concerns over whether measurement of these executive functions is

useful in ADHD diagnosis (Brown & LaRosa, 2002) has led to few protocols addressing these

neuropsychological-academic relationships in ADHD, especially in relation to judging MPH

treatment efficacy.

MPH titration that focuses on overt behaviour could increase short-term academic

compliance and performance gains (DuPaul & Stoner, 2004), but it is unlikely to produce long-

term academic treatment efficacy because neuropsychological functioning may be impaired in

children who appear to be receiving optimal titration given their behavioural concerns. However,

if medication titration was based on maximizing neuropsychological and academic functioning,

with concurrent behaviour therapy offered to reduce remaining problematic behaviours, perhaps

both academic and behavioural long-term improvements would be realized in children with

ADHD (Hale et al., 2009b; Hale et al., 2012).

4.4 Limitations

These findings should be evaluated in the context of several study limitations. Although

the sample largely consisted of children in grades one through five, children aged six to 16 were

41

included. Since ADHD developmental differences in neuropsychological and cognitive

functioning are well established (Barkley, 1997), future research should examine MPH effects on

test performance across age ranges. The sample size in this study was small, which likely limited

the reliability and power of results. Although baseline and placebo conditions partially guarded

against intelligence differences, future research should consider including a cognitive screening

of all children. Finally, the ratio of males to females is approximately 3-to-1 in community

ADHD samples, and as high as 9-to-1 in clinical ADHD samples (APA, 2000), so the current

sample is atypical for a clinical population.

4.5 Future research

Future research is needed to examine the long-term MPH effects on cognition and

academic performance in relation to levels of executive impairment and how these important

relationships are affected by differing medication dosages. Research examining

neuropsychological and behavioural MPH response over time will allow researchers to evaluate

how dosage and length of MPH treatment affects outcomes. Additionally, functional magnetic

resonance imaging (fMRI) and diffusor tensor imaging (DTI) research is needed to explore MPH

response in the different “hot” orbital-ventral cingulate and “cool” dorsolateral-dorsal cingulate

circuits and how this impacts SR and EWM respectively. This future empirical work may benefit

the field in determining whether MPH dose response curves differ for the circuits and ultimately

lead to better titration practices and outcomes for children with ADHD (Hale et al., 2011).

Future studies should consider the underlying differences between MPH treatment

responders and non-responders. A valuable question that arises from the present study is whether

children who are MPH non-responders truly have ADHD or if the symptoms of ADHD are the

42

result of other diagnoses (e.g., depression). Further increasing the heterogeneity of the disorder,

some research suggests that the current DSM-IV-TR diagnostic subtypes of ADHD miss a

constellation of symptoms described as “sluggish cognitive tempo” (SCT; Carlson, 1986). SCT

symptoms include hypoactivity, daydreaming, lethargy, limited alertness, and mental confusion,

with many researchers arguing that SCT is a distinct disorder of inattention (Garner, Marceaux,

Mrug, Patterson, & Hodgens, 2010; Harrington & Waldman, 2010; Jacobson et al., 2012) with

fewer EF deficits but higher rates of depression than DSM-IV-TR identified ADHD subgroups

(Barkley, 2012). Thus, future research should examine medication response across the different

ADHD subtypes, as some subtypes may be less responsive to medication than others (Hale et al.,

2009).

43

Chapter 5: Conclusion

Although the neurobiological basis of ADHD is well established, and ADHD is known to

affect the frontal-subcortical circuits responsible for executive function and self-regulation,

(Dickstein et al., 2006; Lichter & Cummings, 2001), it continues to be diagnosed primarily on

the basis of overt behaviour and indirect summative informant reports. Making diagnoses and

judging treatment efficacy on the basis of overt behavioural symptoms is problematic as ADHD

may also interfere with cognitive, neuropsychological, academic and social functioning across

multiple environments (Hale et al., 2009b; Hale et al., 2012). Further, most frontal-subcortical

circuit disorders lead to impaired attention (Lichter & Cummings, 2001), making differential

diagnosis difficult if only behavioural criteria are used (Hale et al., 2005).

Current behavioural diagnostic practices cannot address the neuropsychological

heterogeneity found in children diagnosed with ADHD, because behaviour ratings are based on

subjective interpretation of an individual’s behaviour rather than brain-behaviour relationships

(Hale et al., 2012). Although positive MPH treatment effects on behaviour are well established

(Abikoff et al., 2004; Pearson et al., 2004; Van der Oord et al., 2008, Waxmonsky et al., 2008)

and MPH can improve a child’s readiness for classroom learning (DuPaul & Stoner, 2004),

long-term MPH treatment efficacy remains elusive (Jensen et al., 2007). Although research

supporting the association between long-term ADHD medication use and improved standardized

achievement scores is emerging (Langberg & Becker, 2012), the clinical and educational utility

of these statistically significant achievement gains remains unclear. The relationship between

long-term medication use and school grades is also inconclusive with attenuated treatment

outcomes found in some ADHD samples and minimal, equivocal, or even untoward response in

certain individuals (Hale et al., 2011).

44

It is unclear how MPH ameliorates the EWM/SR deficits experienced in this

heterogeneous population because medication use and titration are typically based solely on

behavioural criteria. Focusing on overt behaviour rather than underlying executive deficits

experienced by children with ADHD may explain why MPH produces equivocal effects on long-

term academic success (Van der Oord, 2008), and the reason that long-term treatment efficacy of

stimulants remains limited in all areas (Jensen et al., 2007), especially since the best medication

dose for cognition may be lower than the best dose for behaviour (Goldstein & Naglierli, 2008;

Hale, Mulligan, & Simmerman, 2006; Hale et al., 1998, 2005, 2011; Hoeppner et al., 1997;

Reddy & Hale, 2007). Thus, a new clinical orientation and alternative training approach is

needed for physicians and other practitioners working with children with attention problems and

ADHD. Education is needed to help practitioners attend to the neuropsychological, academic,

and behavioural characteristics of affected children for differential diagnosis and optimization of

treatment outcomes.

The results from this study suggest that the current use of indirect and subjective

behavioural approaches for ADHD differential diagnosis and judgment of MPH treatment

efficacy need to be re-examined. Rather than focusing solely on behavioural criteria, future

research and clinical practice should include direct assessment of cognitive, neuropsychological,

academic, and behavioural functioning to ensure children referred for attention problems indeed

have “true” ADHD – at least the kind that responds to medication (Hale et al., 2009a). The RCT

results presented here not only have direct implications for ADHD differential diagnosis but also

for understanding MPH dose-response relationships in ADHD. As a result, children with ADHD

and other attention problems would benefit from interdisciplinary coordination of service

delivery (Hale & Fiorello, 2004) using multimodal, multi-method approaches to address their

45

diverse needs (DuPaul & Stoner, 2004). Physicians, psychologists, teachers, and parents can

enhance treatment outcomes through regular communication and coordination of service delivery

to determine the child’s response to MPH and other interventions. By examining MPH response

in relation to, and in combination with other interventions, interdisciplinary teams can optimize

the neuropsychological, academic, and behavioural functioning of children with ADHD.

46

References

Achenbach, T. M. (1991). Integrative guide for the 1991 CBCL/4–18, YSR, and TRF profiles.

Burlington, VT: University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry.

Advokat, C. (2009). What exactly are the benefits of stimulants for ADHD? Journal of Attention

Disorders, 12(6), 495–498. doi:10.1177/1087054708329781

American Academy of Pediatrics, Subcommittee on Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder,

Steering Committee on Quality Improvement and Management. (2011). ADHD: Clinical

practice guideline for the diagnosis, evaluation, and treatment of attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents. Pediatrics, 128(5), 1–16.

doi:10.1542/peds.2011-2654

American Psychiatric Association. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders

(4th ed., Text Rev.). Washington, DC: Author.

Arnsten, A. F. (2006). Stimulants: Therapeutic actions in ADHD. Neuropsychopharmacology,

31, 2367–2383. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1301164

Arnsten, A. F. T. (2009a). The emerging neurobiology of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder:

The key role of the prefrontal association cortex. Journal of Pediatrics, 154(5), I–S43.

doi:10.1016/j.jpeds.2009.01.018.

Arnsten, A. F. T. (2009b). Toward a new understanding of attention-deficit hyperactivity

disorder pathophysiology. CNS Drugs, 23(1, Supplement), 33–41. doi:1172-

7047/09/0001-0033

Arnsten, A. F. T., & Li, B. (2005). Neurobiology of executive functions: Catecholamine

influences on prefrontal cortical functions. Biological Psychiatry, 57(11), 1377–1384.

doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.08.019

47

Arnsten, A. F. T., & Pliszka, S. R. (2011). Catecholamine influences on prefrontal cortical

function: Relevance to treatment of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and related

disorders. Pharmacology Biochemistry and Behavior, 99(2), 211–216.

doi:10.1016/j.pbb.2011.01.020

Barbaresi, W. J., Katusic, S. K., Colligan, R. C., Weaver, A. L., & Jacobsen, S. J. (2007).

Modifiers of long-term school outcomes for children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder: Does treatment with stimulant medication make a difference? Results from a

population-based study. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 28(4), 274–

287. doi:10.1097/DBP.0b013e3180cabc28

Barkley, R. A. (1990). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and

treatment. New York, NY: Guilford.

Barkley, R. A. (1997). Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions:

Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychological Bulletin, 121(1), 65–94.

doi:10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65

Barkley, R. A. (2006a). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A handbook for diagnosis and

treatment (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Barkley, R. A. (2006b). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. In D. A. Wolfe & E. J. Mash

(Eds.), Behavioral and emotional disorders in adolescents: Nature, assessment, and

treatment (pp. 91–152). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Barkley, R. A. (2012). Distinguishing sluggish cognitive tempo from ADHD in children and

adolescents: Executive functioning, impairment, and comorbidity. Journal of Clinical

Child & Adolescent Psychology, 42(2), 161–173. doi:10.1080/15374416.2012.734259

48

Barkley, R. A., Anastopoulos, A. D., Guevremont, D. C., & Fletcher, K. E. (1991). Adolescents

with ADHD: patterns of behavioral adjustment, academic functioning, and treatment

utilization. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 30(5),

752-761.

Barkley, R. A., DuPaul, G. J., & McMurray, M. B. (1991). Attention deficit disorder with and

without hyperactivity: Clinical response to three dose levels of methylphenidate.

Pediatrics, 87, 519–531.

Barkley, R. A., McMurray, M. B., Edelbrock, C. S., & Robbins, K. (1989). The response of

aggressive and nonaggressive ADHD children to two doses of methylphenidate. Journal

of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 28(6), 873.

Berridge, C. W., & Devilbiss, D. M. (2011). Psychostimulants as cognitive enhancers: The

prefrontal cortex, catecholamines, and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological

Psychiatry, 69(12), e101–e111. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.06.023

Berridge, C. W., Devilbiss, D. M., Andrzejewski, M. E., Arnsten, A. F., Kelley, A. E.,

Schmeichel, . . . Spencer, R. C. (2006). Methylphenidate preferentially increases

catecholamine neurotransmission within prefrontal cortex at low doses that enhance

cognitive function. Biological Psychiatry, 60, 1111–1120.

doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.04.022

Biederman, J., Faraone, S. V., & Lapey, K. (1992). Comorbidity of diagnosis in attention-deficit

hyperactivity disorder. In G. Weiss (Ed.), Child and adolescent psychiatry clinics of

North America: Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (pp. 335–360). Philadelphia, PA:

Saunders.

49

Biederman, J., Monuteaux, M. C., Doyle, A. E., Seidman, L. J., Wilens, T. E., Ferraro, F., . . .

Faraone, S. V. (2004). Impact of executive function deficits and attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) on academic outcomes in children. Journal of

Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72(5), 757–766. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.72.5.757

Brown, R. T., & La Rosa, A. (2002). Recent developments in the pharmacotherapy of attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Professional Psychology, Research and Practice,

33(6), 591–595. doi:10.1037/0735-7028.33.6.591

Bush, G., Frazier, J. A., Rauch, S. L., Seidman, L. J., Whalen, P. J., Jenike, M. A., ... &

Biederman, J. (1999). Anterior cingulate cortex dysfunction in attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder revealed by fMRI and the Counting Stroop. Biological

psychiatry, 45(12), 1542–1552.

Carlson, C. L. (1986). Attention deficit disorder with and without hyperactivity: A review of

preliminary experimental evidence. In B. B. Lahey & A. E. Kazdin (Eds.), Advances in

clinical child psychology (Vol. 9, pp. 153–175). New York, NY: Plenum.

Casey, B. J., Trainor, R. J., Orendi, J. L., Schubert, A. B., Nystrom, L. E., Giedd, J. N., ... &

Rapoport, J. L. (1997). A developmental functional MRI study of prefrontal activation

during performance of a go-no-go task. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(6), 835–

847.

Castellanos, F.X., & Tannock, R. (2002). Neuroscience of attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder: The search for endophenotypes. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3, 617–628.

doi:10.1038/nrn896

50

Castellanos, F. X., Patti, P. L., Sharp, W., Jeffries, N. O., Greestein, D. K., Clasen, L. S., . . .

Rapoport, J. L. (2002). Developmental trajectories of brain volume abnormalities in

children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the

American Medical Association, 288(14), 1740–1748. doi:10.1001/jama.288.14.1740.

Castellanos, F. X., Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Milham, M. P., & Tannock, R. (2006). Characterizing

cognition in ADHD: Beyond executive dysfunction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(3),

117–123. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.011

Chacko, A., Pelham, W. E., Gnagy, E. M., Greiner, A., Vallano, G., Bukstein, O., & Rancurello,

M. (2005). Stimulant medication effects in a summer treatment program among young

children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy

of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44(3), 249–257. doi:10.1097/00004583-200503000-

00009

Conners, C. K. (1997). Conners’ Rating Scales–Revised: Long Form. North Tonawanda, NY:

Multi-Heath Systems.

Conners, C. K. (2002). Forty years of methylphenidate treatment in attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Attention Disorders, 6, S17–S30.

Conners, K., & MHS Staff. (2004). Conners’ Continuous Performance Test–II user’s manual.

Toronto, Canada: Multi-Health Systems.

Connor, D. F. (2006). Stimulants. In R. A. Barkley (Ed.), Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder:

A handbook for diagnosis and treatment (3rd ed, pp. 608–648). New York, NY: Guilford

Press.

Corkum, P., McGonnell, M., & Schachar, R. (2010). Factors affecting academic achievement in

children with ADHD. Journal of Applied Research on Learning, 3(Article 9), 1–14.

51

Decker, S. L., Hill, S. K., & Dean, R. S. (2007). Evidence of construct similarity in executive

functions and fluid reasoning abilities. International Journal of Neuroscience, 117(6),

735–748. doi:10.1080/00207450600910085

Denckla, M. B. (1996). Biological correlates of learning and attention: what is relevant to

learning disability and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder? Journal of Developmental

& Behavioral Pediatrics, 17(2), 114–119.

DeVito, E. E., Blackwell, A. D., Kent, L., Ersche, K. D., Clark, L., Salmond, C. H., . . .

Sahakian, B. J. (2008). The effects of methylphenidate on decision making in attention-

deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 64, 636–639.

doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.04.017

Dickstein, S. G., Bannon, K., Castellanos, F. X., & Milham, M. P. (2006). The neural correlates

of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: An ALE meta-analysis. Journal of Child

Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(10), 1051–1062. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01671.x

Douglas, V. I., Barr, R. G., Desilets, J., & Sherman, E. (1995). Do high doses of stimulants

impair flexible thinking in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder? Journal of the

American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 34(7), 879–885.

DuPaul, G. J., & Barkley, R. A. (1992). Situational variability of attention problems:

Psychometric properties of the Revised Home and School Situations Questionnaires.

Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 21, 178–188. doi:10.1207/s15374424jccp2102_10

DuPaul, G. J., Rapport, M. D., & Perriello, L. M. (1991). Teacher ratings of academic skills: The

development of the Academic Performance Rating Scale. School Psychology Review,

20(2), 284–300.

52

DuPaul, G. J., & Stoner, G. (2004). ADHD in the schools: Assessment and intervention

strategies (2nd ed.). New York, NY: Guilford.

Durston, S., Tottenham, N. T., Thomas, K. M., Davidson, M. C., Eigsti, I., Yang, Y., . . . Casey,

B. J. (2003). Differential patterns of striatal activation in young children with and without

ADHD. Biological Psychiatry, 53(10), 871–878.

Engert, V., & Pruessner, J. C. (2008). Dopaminergic and noradrenergic contributions to

functionality in ADHD: The role of methylphenidate. Current Neuropharmacology, 6(4),

322–328. doi:10.2174/157015908787386069

Evans, R. W., Gualtieri, C. T., & Amara, I. (1986). Methylphenidate and memory: dissociated

effects in hyperactive children. Psychopharmacology, 90(2), 211–216.

Evans, S. W., Pelham, W. E., Smith, B. H., Bukstein, O., Gnagy, E. M., Greiner, A. R., . . .

Baron-Myak, C. (2001). Does-response effects of methylphenidate on ecologically valid

measures of academic performance and classroom behavior in adolescents with ADHD.

Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 9, 163–175. doi:10.1037//1064-

1297.9.2.163

Fabiano, G. A., Pelham, Jr., W. E., Coles, E. K., Gnagy, E. M., Chronis-Tuscano, A., &

O’Connor, B. C. (2009). A meta-analysis of behavioural treatments for attention-deficit/

hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Psychology Review, 29, 129–140.

doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2008.11.001

Figner, B., Mackinlay, R., Wilkening, F., & Weber, E. (2009). Affective and deliberative

processes in risky choice: Age differences in risk taking in the Columbia Card Task.

Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35(3), 709–

730. doi:10.1037/a0014983

53

Fischer, M., Barkley, R. A., Smallish, L., & Fletcher, K. (2005). Executive functioning in

hyperactive children as young adults: attention, inhibition, response perseveration, and

the impact of comorbidity. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27(1), 107–133.

Floresco, S. B., & Magyar, O. (2006). Mesocortical dopamine modulation of executive

functions: Beyond working memory. Psychopharmacology, 188, 567–585.

doi:10.1007/s00213-006-0404-5

Garner, A. A., Marceaux, J. C., Mrug, S., Patterson, C., & Hodgens, B. (2010). Dimensions and

correlates of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and sluggish cognitive tempo.

Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 38, 1097–1107. doi:10.1007/s10802-010-943-8

Golden, J. C. (1978). Stroop Color and Word Test. Chicago, IL: Stoelting.

Goldstein, S., & Naglieri, J. (2008). The school neuropsychology of ADHD: Theory, assessment,

and intervention. Psychology in the Schools, 45(9), 859-874.

Greenhill, L. L., Swanson, J. M., Vitiello, B., Davies, M., Clevenger, W., Wu, M., . . . Wigal, T.

(2001). Impairment and deportment responses to different methylphenidate doses in

children with ADHD: The MTA titration trial. Journal of the American Academy of Child

and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 180–187. doi:10.1097/00004583-200102000-00012

Evans, R. W., Gualtieri, C. T., & Amara, I. (1986). Methylphenidate and memory: Dissociated

effects in hyperactive children. Psychopharmacology, 90, 211-216.

Hale, J. B. (1997). Development of alternate forms of the Hale Cancellation Task and Trail-

Making Test for use in ADHD medication trials. Unpublished manuscript, Rochester

Institute of Technology.

Hale, J. B., & Fiorello, C. A. (2004). School neuropsychology: A practitioner’s handbook. New

York, NY: Guilford.

54

Hale, J. B., Fiorello, C. A., & Brown, L. (2005). Determining medication treatment effects using

teacher ratings and classroom observations of children with ADHD: Does

neuropsychological impairment matter? Educational and Child Psychology, 22, 39–61.

Hale, J. B., Hoeppner, J. B., DeWitt, M. B., Coury, D. L., Ritacco, D. G., & Trommer, B. (1998).

Evaluating medication response in ADHD. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 31(6), 595–

607. doi:10.1177/002221949803100610

Hale, J. B., Mulligan, C. A., & Simmerman, K. L. (2006, February). Quantifying medication

response in ADHD: Does neuropsychological impairment matter? Poster presentation at

the 34th Annual Meeting of the International Neuropsychological Society, Boston, MA.

Hale, J. B., Reddy, L. A., Decker, S. L., Thompson, R., Henzel, J., Teodori, A., . . . Denckla, M.

B. (2009a). Development and validation of an executive function and behavior rating

screening battery sensitive to ADHD. Journal of Clinical and Experimental

Neuropsychology, 1, 1–16. doi:10.1080/13803390802687423

Hale, J. B., Reddy, L. A., Semrud-Clikeman, M., Hain, L. A., Whitaker, J., Morely, J., . . . Jones,

N. (2011). Executive impairment determines ADHD medication response: Implications

for academic achievement. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 44(2), 196–212.

doi:10.1177/0022219410391191

Hale, J. B., Reddy, L. A., Wilcox, G., McLaughlin, A., Hain, L., Stern, A., . . . Eusebio, E.

(2009b). Assessment and intervention for children with ADHD and other frontal-striatal

circuit disorders. In D. C. Miller (Ed.), Best practices in school neuropsychology:

Guidelines for effective practice, assessment and evidence-based interventions (pp. 225–

280). New York, NY: John Wiley.

55

Hale, J. B., Yim, M., Schneider, A. N., Wilcox, G., Henzel, J. N., & Dixon, S. G. (2012).

Cognitive and neuropsychological assessment of ADHD: Redefining a disruptive

behavior disorder. In D. P. Flanagan & P. Harrison (Eds.), Contemporary intellectual

assessment: Theories, tests, and issues (3rd ed., pp. 687–707). New York, NY: Guilford

Press.

Hansen, D. L., & Hansen, E. H. (2006). Caught in a balancing act: Parents’ dilemmas regarding

their ADHD child’s treatment with stimulant medication. Qualitative Health Research,

16, 1267–1285. doi:10.1177/1049732306292543

Harrington, K. M. & Waldman, I. D. (2010). Evaluating the utility of sluggish cognitive tempo

in discriminating among DSM-IV ADHD subtypes. Journal of Abnormal Child

Psychology, 38, 173–185. doi:10.1007/s10802-009-9355-8

Heilbronner, R. L., Henry, G. K., Buck, P., Adams, R. L., & Fogle, T. (1991). Lateralized brain

damage and performance on trail making A and B, digit span forward and backward, and

TPT memory and location. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 6(4), 251–258.

Hoeppner, J. B., Hale, J. B., Bradley, A. M., Byrnes, M., Coury, D. L., Lennie, L., & Trommer,

B. L. (1997). A clinical protocol for determining methylphenidate dosage levels in

ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 2, 19–30. doi:10.1177/108705479700200102

Homack, S., & Riccio, C. A. (2004). A meta-analysis of the sensitivity and specificity of the

Stroop Color and Word Test with children. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19(6),

725–743.

56

Hood, J., Baird, G., Rankin, P. M., & Isaacs, E. (2005). Immediate effects of methylphenidate on

cognitive attention skills of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder.

Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 47(6), 408–414. doi:10.1111/j.1469-

8749.2005.tb01162.x

Huang-Pollock, C. L., Karalunas, S. L., Tam, H., & Moore, A. N. (2012). Evaluating vigilance

deficits in ADHD: A meta-analysis of CPT performance. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology, 121(2), 360–371.

Isles, A. R., & Humby, T. (2006). Modes of imprinted gene action in learning disability. Journal

of Intellectual Disability Research, 50(5), 318–325. doi:10.1111/j.1365-

2788.2006.00843.x

Jacobson, L. A., Murphy-Bowman, S. C., Pritchard, A. E., Tart-Zelvin, A., Zabel, T. A., &

Mahone, E. M. (2012). Factor structure of a sluggish cognitive tempo scale in clinically-

referred children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40(8), 1327–1337.

doi:10.1007=s10802-012-9643-6

Jensen, P. S., Arnold, L. E., Swanson, J. M., Vitiello, B., Abikoff, H. B., Greenhill, L. L. . . .

Hur, K. (2007). 3-year follow up of the NIMH MTA study. Journal of the American

Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 46, 989–1002.

doi:10.I097lchi.0b0r3e3I80686d48

Kelly, C., Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Scheres, A., & Castellanos, F. X. (2007). Functional

neuroimaging of the reward and motivational pathway in ADHD. In M. Fitzgerald, M.

Bellgrove, & M. Gill (Eds.), Handbook of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (pp.

209–235). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons.

57

Kemner, C., Jonkman, L. M., Kenemans, J. L., Bocker, K. B., Verbaten, M. N., & Engeland, H.

(2004). Sources of auditory selective attention and the effects of methylphenidate in

children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 55, 776–778.

doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2003.12.007

Kobel, M., Bechtel, N., Weber, P., Specht, K., Klarhofer, M., Scheffler, K., . . . Penner, I. K.

(2008). Effects of methylphenidate on working memory functioning in children with

attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. European Journal of Paediatric Neurology, 13,

516–523. doi:10.1016/j.ejpn.2008.10.008

Konrad, K., Gunther, T., Hanisch, C., & Herpertz-Dahlmann, B. (2004). Differential effects of

methylphenidate on attentional functions in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(2),

191–198. doi:10.1097/00004583-200402000-00015

Kuczenski, R., & Segal, D. S. (1992). Regional norepinephrine response to amphetamine using

dialysis: comparison with caudate dopamine. Synapse, 11(2), 164–169.

Kuhle, H., Kinkebur, J., Andes, K., Heidorn, F., Zeyer, S., Rautzenberg, P., . . . Jansen, F.

(2007). Self-regulation of visual attention and facial expression of emotions in ADHD

children. Journal of Attention Disorders, 10(4), 350–358.

doi:10.1177/1087054706289944

Langberg, J. M., & Becker, S. P. (2012). Does long-term medication use improve the academic

outcomes of youth with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Clinical Child and

Family Psychology Review, 15(3), 215–233. doi:10.1007/s10567-012-0117-8

58

Langleben, D. D., Monterosso, L., Elman, I., Ash, B., Krikorian, G., & Austin, G. (2006).

Effects of methylphenidate on Stroop Color-Word task performance in children with

attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Research, 141, 315–320.

doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2005.09.007

Lezak, M. D. (Ed.). (1995). Neuropsychological assessment (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Oxford

University Press.

Li, D., Sham, P. K., Owen, M. J., & He, L. (2006). Meta-analysis shows significant association

between dopamine system genes and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).

Human Molecular Genetics, 15(14), 2276–2284. doi:10.1093/hmg/ddl152

Libon, D. J., Glosser, G., Malamut, B. L., Kaplan, E., Goldberg, E., Swenson, R., & Prouty

Sands, L. (1994). Age, executive functions, and visuospatial functioning in healthy older

adults. Neuropsychology, 8(1), 38–43. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.8.1.38

Lichter, D. G., & Cummings, J. L. (Eds.). (2001). Frontal-subcortical circuits in psychiatric and

neurological disorders. New York, NY: Guilford.

Loe, I. M., & Feldman, H. M. (2007). Academic and educational outcomes of children with

ADHD. Ambulatory Pediatrics, 7, 82–90. doi:10.1093/jpepsy/jsl054

Manor, I., Meidad, S. Zalsman, G., Zemishlany, Z., Tyano, S., & Weizman, A. (2008). Objective

versus subjective assessment of methylphenidate response. Child Psychiatry and Human

Development, 39, 273–282. doi:10.1007/s10578-007-0087-0

Martinussen, R., Hayden, J., Hogg-Johnson, S., & Tannock, R. (2005). A meta-analysis of

working memory impairments in children with attention-deficit/hyperactive disorder.

Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 44, 377–384.

doi:10.1097/01.chi.0000153228.72591.73

59

May, R. B., Masson, M. E. J., Hunter, M. A., & Wells, J. (1990). NPStat 3.01 [Computer

software]. Victoria, Canada: University of Victoria.

Molina, B. S. G., Hinshaw, S. P., Swanson, J. M., Arnold, L. E., Vitiello, B., Jensen, P. S., . . .

Houck, P. R. (2009). The MTA at 8 years: Prospective follow-up of children treated for

combined-type ADHD in a multisite study. Journal of the American Academy of Child &

Adolescent Psychiatry, 48(5), 484–500. doi:10.1097/CHI.0b013e31819c23d0

The MTA Cooperative Group. (1999). A 14-month randomized clinical trial of treatment

strategies for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry,

56(12), 1073–1086. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.56.12.1073

Newby, R. F. (1999). Wisconsin Selective Reminding Test. Milwaukee, WI: Medical College of

Wisconsin.

Nigg, J. T. (2001). Is ADHD a disinhibitory disorder? Psychological Bulletin, 127(5), 571–598. Nigg, J. T., Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., & Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S. (2005). Causal heterogeneity

in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Do we need neuropsychologically impaired

subtypes? Biological Psychiatry, 57(11), 1224–1230.

doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.08.025

Pearson, D. A., Santos, C. W., Casat, C. D., Lane, D. M., Jerger, S. W., Roache, J. D., . . .

Cleveland, L. A. (2004). Treatment effects of methylphenidate on cognitive functioning

in children with mental retardation and ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of

Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(6), 677–685.

doi:10.1097/01.chi.0000124461.81324.13

60

Pearson, D. A., Santos, C. W., Roache, J. D., Casat, C. D., Loveland, K. A., Lachar, D., . . .

Cleveland, L. A. (2003). Treatment effects of methylphenidate on behavioral adjustment

in children with mental retardation and ADHD. Journal of the American Academy of

Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 209–216.

doi:10.1097/01.CHI.0000037009.34553.36

Pelham, W. E., Gnagy, E. M., Burrows-Maclean, L., Williams, A., Fabiano, G. A., Morrisey, S.

M., . . . Morse, G. D. (2001). Once-a-day Concerta methylphenidate versus three-times-

daily methylphenidate in laboratory and natural settings. Pediatrics, 107, e105.

doi:10.1542/peds.107.6.e105

Pennington, B. F., & Ozonoff, S. (1996). Executive functions and developmental

psychopathology. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 51–87.

doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01380.x

Pietrzak, R. H., Mollica, C. M., Maruff, P., & Snyder, P. J. (2006). Cognitive effects of

immediate-release methylphenidate in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder. Neuroscience and Behavioral Reviews, 30(8), 1225–1245.

doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.10.002

Pliszka, S. R., Liotti, M., Bailey, B. Y., Perez, R., Glahn, D., & Semrud-Clikeman, M. (2007).

Electrophysiological effects of stimulant treatment on inhibitory control in children with

attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent

Psychopharmacology, 17, 356–366. doi:10.1089/cap.2006.0081

Polanczyk, G., de Lima, M. S., Horta, B. L., Biederman, J., & Rohde, L. A. (2007). The

worldwide prevalence of ADHD: A systematic review and metaregression analysis. The

American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 942–948.

61

Powers, R. L., Marks, D. J., Miller, C. J., Newcorn, J. H., & Halperin, J. M. (2008). Stimulant

treatment in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder moderates adolescent

academic outcome. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 18(5), 449–

459. doi:10.1089/cap.2008.021

Raggi, V. L. & Chronis, A. M. (2006). Interventions to address the academic impairment of

children and adolescents with ADHD. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 9,

85–111. doi:10.1007/s10567-006-0006-0

Rapport, M. D., Denney, C., DuPaul, G. J., & Gardner, M. J. (1994). Attention deficit disorder

and methylphenidate: Normalization rates, clinical effectiveness, and response prediction

in 76 children. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry,

33(6), 882–893. doi:10.1097/00004583-199407000-00015

Reddy, L. A., & Hale, J. B. (2007). Inattentiveness. In A. R. Eisen (Ed.), Treating childhood

behavioral and emotional disorders: A step-by-step evidence-based approach (pp. 156–

211). New York, NY: Guilford.

Reitan, R. M., & Wolfson, D. (1985). Neuroanatomy and neuropathology: A guide for

neuropsychologists. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press.

Reynolds, C. R., & Bigler, E. D. (1994). Test of memory and learning. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.

Rhee, S. H., Willcutt, E. G., Hartman, C. A., Pennington, B. F., & DeFries, J. C. (2008) Test of

alternative hypotheses explaining the comorbidity between attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder and conduct disorder. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 36, 29–40.

doi:10.1007/s10802-007-9157-9

62

Rhodes, S. M., Coghill, D. R., & Matthews, K. (2006). Acute neuropsychological effects of

methylphenidate in stimulant drug-naïve boys with ADHD II – broader executive and

non-executive domains. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(11), 1184–1194.

doi:11 11/j.1469-7610.2006.01633.x.

Riccio, C. A., Waldrop, J. J., Reynolds, C. R., & Lowe, P. (2001). Effects of Stimulants on the

Continuous Performance Test (CPT) Implications for CPT Use and Interpretation. The

Journal of neuropsychiatry and clinical neurosciences, 13(3), 326–335.

Roiser, J. P., Cannon, D. M., Gandhi, S. K., Tavares, J. T., Erickson, K., Wood, S., . . . &

Drevets, W. C. (2009). Hot and cold cognition in unmedicated depressed subjects with

bipolar disorder. BipolarDdisorders, 11(2), 178–189. doi:10.1111/j.1399-

5618.2009.00669.x

Roth, R. M., & Saykin, A. J. (2004). Executive dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity

disorder: cognitive and neuroimaging findings. The Psychiatric Clinics of North America,

27(1), 83–96. doi:10.1016/S0193-953X(03)00112-6

Rubia, K., Overmeyer, S., Taylor, E., Brammer, M., Williams, S. C., Simmons, A., & Bullmore,

E. T. (1999). Hypofrontality in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder during higher-

order motor control: a study with functional MRI. American Journal of Psychiatry,

156(6), 891–896.

Rubia, K., Smith, A. B., Brammer, M. J., Toone, B., & Taylor, E. (2005). Abnormal brain

activation during inhibition and error detection in medication-naive adolescents with

ADHD. American Journal of Psychiatry, 162(6), 1067–1075.

Rudel, R. G., Denckla, M. B., & Broman, M. (1978). Rapid silent response to repeated target

symbols by dyslexic and nondyslexic children. Brain and language, 6(1), 52–62.

63

Rypma, B., & D'Esposito, M. (2000). Isolating the neural mechanisms of age-related changes in

human working memory. Nature Neuroscience, 3(5), 509–515.

Scheffler, R. M., Brown, T. T., Fulton, B. D., Hinshaw, S. P., Levine, P., & Stone, S. (2009).

Positive association between attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder medication use and

academic achievement during elementary school. Pediatrics, 123, 1273–1279.

doi:10.1542/peds.2008-1597

Schulz, K. P., Fan, J., Magidina, O., Marks, D. J., Hahn, B., & Halperin, J. M. (2007). Does the

emotional go/no-go task really measure behavioral inhibition?: Convergence with

measures on a non-emotional analog. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 22(2), 151–

160.

Semrud-Clikeman, M. (2005). Neuropsychological aspects for evaluating learning disabilities.

Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38(6), 563–568. doi:10.1177/00222194050380061301

Semrud-Clikeman, M., Pliszka, S., & Liotto, M. (2008). Executive functioning in children with

attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: Combined type with and without a stimulant

medication history. Neuropsychology, 22(3), 329–340. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.22.3.329

Sergeant, J. A., Geurts, H., & Oosterlaan, J. (2002). How specific is a deficit of executive

functioning for attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? Behavioural Brain Research,

130, 3–28. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00430-2

Sheridan, M. A., Hinshaw, S., & D'ESPOSITO, M. A. R. K. (2007). Efficiency of the prefrontal

cortex during working memory in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the

American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 46(10), 1357–1366.

64

Solanto, M. V., Abikoff, H., Sonuga-Barke, E., Schachar, R., Logan, G., Wigal, T., . . . Turkel,

E. (2001). The ecological validity of delay aversion and response inhibition as measures

of impulsivity in AD/HD: A supplement to the NIMH Multi-Modal Treatment Study of

AD/HD. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 29, 215–228.

doi:10.1023/A:1010329714819

Sonuga-Barke, E. J. S., Sergeant, J. A., Nigg, J., & Willcutt, E. (2008). Executive dysfunction

and delay aversion in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Nosologic and diagnostic

implications. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 17(2), 367–

384. doi:10.1016/j.chc.2007.11.008

Van Mourik, R., Oosterlaan, J., & Sergeant, J. A. (2005). The Stroop revisited: A meta!analysis

of interference control in AD/HD. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 46(2),

150–165.

Sprague, R. L., & Sleator, E. K. (1977). Methylphenidate in hyperkinetic children: Differences in

dose effects on learning and social behavior. Science, 198, 1274–1276.

doi:10.1126/science.337493

Steinberg, L. (2008). A social neuroscience perspective on adolescent risk-taking.

Developmental Review, 28(1), 78–106. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2007.08.002

Stuss, D. T., Bisschop, S. M., Alexander, M. P., Levine, B., Katz, D., & Izukawa, D. (2001). The

Trail Making Test: a study in focal lesion patients. Psychological Assessment, 13(2),

230–239. doi:10.1037/1040-3590.13.2.230

Swanson, J., Baler, R. D., & Volkow, N. D. (2011). Understanding the effects of stimulant

medications on cognition in individuals with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A

decade of progress. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36, 207–226. doi:10.1038/npp.2010.160

65

Tamm, L., Menon, V., Ringel, J., & Reiss, A. L. (2004). Event-related FMRI evidence of

frontotemporal involvement in aberrant response inhibition and task switching in

attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of the American Academy of Child &

Adolescent Psychiatry, 43(11), 1430–1440.

Tannock, R., Schachar, R., & Logan, G. (1995). Methylphenidate and cognitive flexibility:

Dissociated dose effects in hyperactive children. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology,

23(2), 235–266. doi:10.1007/BF01447091

Taylor, E. (2011). Antecedents of ADHD: A historical account of diagnostic concepts. Attention

Deficit and Hyperactivity Disorders, 3, 69–75. doi:10.1007/s12402-010-0051-x

Teicher, M. H., Polcari, A., & McGreenery, C. E. (2008). Utility of objective measures of

activity and attention in the assessment of therapeutic response to stimulants in children

with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Journal of Child and Adolescent

Psychopharmacology, 18(3), 265–270. doi:10.1089/cap.2007.0090

Thapar, A., Cooper, M., Eyre, O., & Langley, K. (2013). Practitioner Review: What have we

learnt about the causes of ADHD? Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(1), 3–

16. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02611.x

Tombaugh, T. N. (2004). Trail Making Test A and B: Normative data stratified by age and

education. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 19(2), 203–214.

Trommer, B. L., Hoeppner, J. B., & Zecker, S. G. (1991). The go-no go test in attention deficit

disorder is sensitive to methylphenidate. Journal of Child Neurology, 6(Suppl.), S128–

S131. doi:10.1177/0883073891006001131

66

Vaidya, C. J. & Stollstroff, M. (2008). Cognitive neuroscience of attention deficit hyperactivity

disorder: Current status and working hypotheses. Developmental Disabilities Research

Reviews, 14, 261–267. doi:10.1002/ddrr.40

Vaidya, C. J., Austin, G., Kirkorian, G., Ridlehuber, H. W., Desmond, J. E., Glover, G. H., &

Gabrieli, J. D. (1998). Selective effects of methylphenidate in attention deficit

hyperactivity disorder: a functional magnetic resonance study. Proceedings of the

National Academy of Sciences, 95(24), 14494–14499.

Valera, E. M., Faraone, S. V., Murray, K. E., & Seidman, L. J. (2007). Meta-analysis of

structural imaging findings in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biological

Psychiatry, 61(12), 1361–1369. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.06.011

Van der Oord, S., Prins, P. J., Oosterlaan, J., & Emmelkamp, P. M. (2008). Efficacy of

methylphenidate, psychosocial treatments and their combination in school-aged children

with ADHD: A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 783–800.

doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.007

Voeller, K. K. S. (2001). Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder as a frontal-subcortical

disorder. In D. G. Lichter & J. L. Cummings (Eds.), Frontal-subcortical circuits in

psychiatric and neurological disorders (pp. 334–371). New York, NY: Guilford Press.

Volkow, N. D., Wang, G-J., Fowler, J. S., Logan, J., Gerasimov, M., Maynard, . . . Franceschi,

D. (2001). Therapeutic doses of oral methylphenidate significantly increase extracellular

dopamine in the human brain. The Journal of Neuroscience, 21(2), 1–5.

Volkow, N. D., Wang, G., Kollins, S. H., Wigal, T. L., Newcorn, J. H., Telang, F., . . . Swanson,

J. M. (2009). Evaluating dopamine reward pathway in ADHD. JAMA: The Journal of the

American Medical Association, 302(10), 1084–1091. doi:10.1001/jama.2009.1308

67

Waxmonsky, J., Pelham, W. E., Gnagy, E., Cummings, M. R., O’Connor, B., Majumdar, A., . . .

Robb, J. A. (2008). The efficacy and tolerability of methylphenidate and behavior

modification in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and severe mood

dysregulation. Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychopharmacology, 18(6), 573–588.

doi:10.1089/cap.2008.065

Willcutt, E. G., Doyle, A. E., Nigg, J. T., Faraone, S. V., & Pennington, B. F. (2005). Validity of

the executive function theory of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A meta-analytic

review. Biological Psychiatry, 57(11), 1336–1346. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006

Willcutt, E .G., Nigg, J., Pennington, B. F., Solanto, M. V., Rohde, L. A., Tannock, R., . . .

Lahey, B. B. (2012). Validity of DSM-IV attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder

symptom dimensions and subtypes. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 121(4), 991–1010.

doi:10.1037/a0027347

Wilson, H. K., Cox, D. J., Merkel, R. L., Moore, M., & Coghill, D. (2006). Effect of extended

release stimulant-based medications on neuropsychological functioning among

adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Archives of Clinical

Neuropsychology, 21, 797–807. doi:10.1016/j.acn.2006.06.016

Woods, A. J., & Mark, V. W. (2007). Convergent validity of executive organization measures on

cancellation. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 29(7), 719–723.

Zelazo, P. D., & Muller, U. (2011). Executive function in typical and atypical development. In

U. Goswami (Ed.), The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive development

(2nd ed.) (pp. 574–603). Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.

68

Zwick, R., & Sklar, J. C. (2005). Predicting college grades and degree completion using high

school grades and SAT scores: The role of student ethnicity and first language. American

Educational Research Journal, 42, 439–464. doi:10.3102/00028312042003439

69

Note. TO

MA

L DB

= Test of Mem

ory and Learning – Digits Backw

ard; Stroop = Stroop Color W

ord Test; TMT-B

= Trail Making Test

Part B; CPT-II = C

onners’ Continuous Perform

ance Test-II; WSR

TM = W

isconsin Selective Reminding Test of M

emory; N

/S = not significant; N

/A = no apparent baseline executive im

pairment (n = 8); Low

= low baseline executive im

pairment (n = 20); M

oderate = m

oderate baseline executive impairm

ent (n = 15); High = high baseline executive im

pairment (n = 10); 1 = B

aseline; 2 = Placebo; 3 = Low

dose; 4 = High dose; aLess than B

aseline; bLess than Placebo; cLess than Low dose; dLess than H

igh dose.

Appendix A

. Summ

ary of Means, Standard D

eviations, and MPH

Dose-Response Relationships for Executive W

orking Mem

ory “Cool”

Circuit N

europsychological Measures Across Im

pairment G

roups

Baseline Placebo

Low D

ose H

igh Dose

“Cool” Circuit Tests M

SD

M

SD

M

SD

M

SD

F

p Post-H

oc !TO

MA

L-DB

!N

/A

17.13 8.27

23.50 14.38

22.38 14.62

22.63 14.60

1.79 .266

N/S

Low

15.50 8.45

21.75 12.03

21.50 12.77

24.65 13.91

6.04 .004

1bd

Moderate

11.20 4.96

13.47 5.84

16.87 4.63

14.13 6.64

4.68 .022

1c

High

9.50 5.48

12.70 11.64

17.30 12.76

15.00 11.89

4.21 .029

1c;2

cd Stroop

N/A

34.88

9.36 42.38

8.25 44.38

9.15 45.75

12.23 6.19

.039 1

bcd Low

25.90

6.49 31.30

8.14 29.75

7.77 34.30

8.29 8.94

<.001 1

bcd;2d;3

d M

oderate 19.20

5.10 24.27

7.34 26.67

6.32 26.07

5.39 19.93

<.001 1

bcd H

igh 20.10

6.21 24.90

9.41 27.30

11.86 28.00

9.90 3.30

.088 1

cd TM

T-B Time

N/A

46.00

51.65 43.88

48.92 32.63

26.10 39.75

33.97 1.48

.266 N

/S Low

55.15

19.04 37.90

14.50 40.40

14.04 37.85

16.77 9.39

<.001 2

a;3a;4

a M

oderate 77.87

28.33 50.07

21.63 47.27

27.42 55.13

21.40 14.85

<.001 2

a;3a;4

a H

igh 161.40

165.64 88.90

56.57 60.50

29.57 75.20

42.17 3.14

.105 3

bd CPT-II O

missions

N/A

51.50

5.66 50.38

8.09 46.88

7.51 45.75

6.18 3.13

.126 4

ab Low

62.50

18.06 55.90

13.72 49.85

8.62 51.25

14.92 6.61

.001 2

a;3ab;4

a M

oderate 68.53

14.74 62.27

10.27 56.33

10.70 52.07

9.52 6.64

.007 2

a;3ab;4

ab H

igh 100.00

72.20 68.30

24.71 67.00

37.38 54.20

8.73 4.16

.065 3

a W

SRTM

N/A

76.73

20.97 85.27

30.35 91.01

12.64 81.92

34.97 1.59

.243 N

/S Low

74.21

12.22 81.75

22.25 88.36

13.68 88.42

20.10 4.30

.015 1

cd M

oderate 64.91

16.16 80.33

22.26 89.09

16.96 83.20

21.13 4.94

.019 1

bcd H

igh 55.53

25.52 74.73

30.83 86.72

15.28 90.19

9.74 8.82

.009 1

bcd

70

Note. Stroop = Stroop C

olor Word Test; TM

T-B = Trail M

aking Test Part B; CPT-II = C

onners’ Continuous Perform

ance Test-II; HD

CT

= Hale-D

enckla Cancellation Test; N

/S = not significant; N/A

= no apparent baseline executive impairm

ent (n = 8); Low = low

baseline executive im

pairment (n = 20); M

oderate = moderate baseline executive im

pairment (n = 15); H

igh = high baseline executive impairm

ent (n = 10); 1 = B

aseline; 2 = Placebo; 3 = Low dose; 4 = H

igh dose; aLess than Baseline; bLess than Placebo; cLess than Low

dose; dLess than H

igh dose.

Appendix B. Sum

mary of M

eans, Standard Deviations, and M

PH D

ose-Response Relationships for Self-Regulation “Hot” C

ircuit N

europsychological Measures Across Im

pairment G

roups

Baseline Placebo

Low D

ose H

igh Dose

“Hot Circuit Tests”

M

SD

M

SD

M

SD

M

SD

F p

Post-Hoc

Go N

o-Go

N/A

26.13

3.44 27.00

4.93 27.00

4.93 27.63

5.13 2.09

.220 N

/S Low

25.30

2.39 27.70

1.75 27.80

1.32 28.70

1.72 12.05

<.001 1

bcd;2d

Moderate

22.87 2.90

27.13 2.33

27.53 2.20

27.20 2.54

19.33 <.001

1bcd

High

20.40

3.13 23.40

3.57 26.40

2.37 25.90

3.57 10.32

.006 1

bcd;2cd

Stroop Errors N

/A

2.13 .99

2.38 1.60

2.38 .92

1.63 1.06

1.03 .453

N/S

Low

3.15 1.50

2.50 1.79

1.95 1.61

1.20 1.28

13.89 <.001

3a;4

abc M

oderate 3.60

1.72 2.87

1.73 1.67

1.50 1.67

1.54 6.81

.006 3

ab;4ab

High

3.70 1.16

4.10 3.41

1.60 1.58

1.90 1.73

4.26 .035

3ab;4

a TM

T-B Errors N

/A

.88 .83

1.25 1.67

1.13 .64

1.63 1.19

.64 .624

N/S

Low

1.00 1.03

1.60 1.31

.80 .95

.65 1.14

3.97 .026

3b;4

b M

oderate 2.47

1.51 1.67

1.11 .87

1.13 .93

.70 6.86

.006 3

a;4a

High

4.50 1.78

3.70 1.25

1.20 .92

1.70 1.42

9.18 .008

3ab;4

ab CPT-II Com

missions

N/A

50.25

11.89 51.88

15.11 50.13

17.10 48.50

16.57 .483

.708 N

/S Low

51.25

11.78 47.00

11.77 45.90

11.69 43.55

12.43 4.45

.018 3

a;4a

Moderate

51.53 7.97

49.47 9.33

45.27 9.92

48.60 8.06

2.49 .111

3ab;4

c H

igh

55.20 7.90

48.60 9.40

45.50 13.43

43.80 15.09

1.83 .229

3a;4

a H

DCT Correct

N/A

27.13

3.04 28.50

1.51 28.38

3.46 27.88

3.18 .725

.579 N

/S Low

26.55

2.78 27.65

2.25 27.90

3.37 27.95

2.21 1.61

.207 N

/S M

oderate 22.00

4.04 26.60

2.47 27.60

1.99 26.80

2.91 5.75

.011 1

bcd H

igh 17.80

5.07 24.00

4.78 27.80

1.62 27.50

2.42 14.78

.002 1

bcd;2cd

71

Appendix C: Copyright Permission Letter

Global Multimedia Partners, Inc Permission Letter

Permission #: 127021 Date: 22 May 2013

Hanna Kubas ! 5212 32 Avenue NW Calgary, Alberta, T3B-0J6! Canada [email protected]

Reference to material to be used: Kubas, H. A., Backenson, E. M., Wilcox, G., Piercy, J. C., & Hale, J. B. (2012). The effects of methylphenidate on cognitive functions in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Postgraduate Medicine 124(5): 33-48.! Table 1. MPH Dose-Response Relationships for EWM/SR Impairment Groups for Individual Tests; Figure 1. Procedure for the double-blind placebo controlled protocol; and! Figure 2. Dose response relationships for neuropsychological tests by impairment group.

Proposed use: Author will be using Figure 1 &2, Table 1, as well as data presented in the Results of the article in thesis.

We hereby grant you permission to republish the material mentioned above for the purpose you have indicated only, subject to the following conditions:

1. Receipt of a copyright fee of [WAIVED] due upon receipt of our permission letter and invoice. All applicable taxes will be presented on the invoice.

2. If any part of the material to be used (for example, figures) has appeared in our publication with credit or acknowledgement to another source, permission must also be sought from that source. If such permission is not obtained then that material may not be included in your publication/copies.

3. Reproduction of this material is confined to the purpose for which permission is hereby given. 4. Suitable acknowledgement to the source must be made, either as a footnote or in a reference

list at the end of your publication, as follows: Reprinted from Publication title, Volume number, Author(s). Title of Article, Page No., Copyright (Year), with permission from JTE Multimedia.

5. This permission is for one time use and is granted for non-exclusive English world rights only. For other languages, please reapply for permission separately. Permission EXCLUDES use in any electronic form other than as specified.

Reprints/Permissions Manager! For and on behalf of JTE Multimedia! Agreed Global Multimedia Partners, Inc ________________________________ _____________________________