ethicality in negotiation: an analysis of attitudes, …€¦ · ethicality in negotiation: an...

30
ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business American University 4400 Massachusetts Ave., NW Washington, D.C. 20016 202-885-6193 (T) 202-885-1992 (F) [email protected] Denise Fleck Instituto de Pos-Graduacao e Pesquisa em Administracao - COPPEAD Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro – UFRJ Rio de Janeiro, Brazil Agnes Hofmeister-Toth Marketing Department Budapest University of Economic Sciences Budapest, Hungary 1

Upload: others

Post on 20-Jun-2020

5 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION:

AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES

Roger J. Volkema

Kogod School of Business American University

4400 Massachusetts Ave., NW Washington, D.C. 20016

202-885-6193 (T) 202-885-1992 (F)

[email protected]

Denise Fleck Instituto de Pos-Graduacao e Pesquisa em Administracao - COPPEAD

Universidade Federal do Rio de Janeiro – UFRJ Rio de Janeiro, Brazil

Agnes Hofmeister-Toth Marketing Department

Budapest University of Economic Sciences Budapest, Hungary

1

Page 2: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION:

AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES

Abstract

The study reported in this paper examines the role of invalid information (e.g.,

exaggerated offers, false promises, misrepresented facts) in a two-party, property leasing

negotiation in which participants negotiated seven issues via electronic mail. Prior to

negotiating, attitudes and intentions towards questionable or unethical tactics were measured,

and perceived behavior was measured through a post-negotiation questionnaire and compared

with actual behavior and negotiated outcomes (distributive and integrative). The results suggest

that the pre-negotiation questionnaire was a modest predictor of actual behavior, with general

attitudes effective in predicting overall behavior. Ethical behavior of the negotiator, ethical

behavior of the other party, and perceived honesty of the other party were the best predictors of

performance (perceived and actual), while likely use of unethical tactics and perceived honesty

of the other party predicted whether or not an agreement was reached.

ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION:

2

Page 3: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES

Negotiation is the decision-making process by which two or more people seek to reach an

agreement on the exchange or distribution of benefits, burdens, roles, or responsibilities

(Cramton & Dees, 1993; Thompson, 1998). As such, bargaining and negotiation are central to

the functioning of all human interaction, personal as well as professional (Lax & Sebenius, 1986;

Mintzberg, 1973; Volkema, 1999b).

Much of the early research on bargaining and negotiation took the form of game theory,

where the parties were asked to choose from a finite set of strategies that effected specific, pre-

determined outcomes. The focus of this research was on understanding and predicting rational

behavior (Luce & Raiffa, 1957; Nash, 1950; von Neumann & Morgenstern, 1947). Subsequent

research has focused more on how negotiators actually behave in situations where information is

incomplete or asymmetrically shared, the relationship between strategies and outcomes is

uncertain, and the value of an outcome is relative or unknown (Harsanyi, 1962; Putnam & Jones,

1982; Thompson, 1991).

Often cited as one of the essential elements in understanding and managing negotiation is

the flow of information (McMillan, 1992; Pruitt & Carnevale, 1993; Rubin & Brown, 1975;

Shell, 1999; Thompson, 1998). In theory, information exchange can help build trust between

parties, which in turn can lead to a fuller, richer exchange of information and, ultimately, better

joint/integrative outcomes. On the other hand, information is a source of power, so the more

information one party has about a counterpart=s strengths and weaknesses (preferences,

priorities, alternatives), the stronger his/her bargaining position (Brodt, 1994; French & Raven,

1959; Lewicki, Saunders & Minton, 1999; Schelling, 1960; Walton & McKersie, 1965).

Consequently, negotiators are faced with the challenge of determining how much and what types

3

Page 4: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

of information to reveal while assessing their negotiating counterpart=s honesty and

forthrightness.

While a number of studies have focused on the effects of valid (factual) information

exchange on negotiating performance (cf. Pruitt & Lewis, 1975; Olekalns, Smith & Walsh, 1996;

Roth & Murnighan, 1982; Thompson, 1991), few studies have focused on the exchange of

invalid information such as exaggerated offers, misrepresented facts, false promises, and bluffs,

despite their reported prominence in everyday bargaining and negotiation (Boles, Croson &

Murnighan, 2000; Murnighan, Babcock, Thompson & Pillutla, 1999; O’Connor & Carnevale,

1997). This paper reports on a study of the use of invalid information in a two-party, property

leasing negotiation, which was conducted via electronic mail. Invalid information was measured

in three ways: first, through a questionnaire (the “Incidents in Negotiation Questionnaire”) to

assess attitudes and intentions towards questionable or unethical tactics; second, through

participants’ self-reporting of their behavior in this commercial property leasing negotiation; and

third, through third-party measures of actual behavior. The purpose of the study was to assess

the questionnaire’s utility in predicting actual use of these tactics, and the impact of these tactics

on distributive and integrative outcomes (perceived and actual). A review of the literature

related to these issues is presented in the next section (along with the development of specific

hypotheses), followed by a description of the study’s design, analysis/results, and conclusions.

Background and Hypotheses

Information exchange is generally considered one of the keys to understanding and

managing the negotiation process (Pruitt & Carnevale, 1993; Pruitt & Rubin, 1986; Shell, 1999;

Thompson, 1998; Walton & McKersie, 1965). Information can take a number of forms, based

on its relevancy and validity. Relevant information, for example, might include a negotiator’s

needs, alternatives, resources, utilities, and style (Chatterjee & Ulvila, 1982; Fisher, Ury &

4

Page 5: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

Patton, 1991; Harsanyi, 1962; Pinkley, Griffith & Northcraft, 1995). In addition, negotiators

frequently share valid information that is not directly relevant to the issues of a negotiation (e.g.,

discussions of traffic, weather, current events, hobbies), which can assist the parties in getting

comfortable, building rapport, and reaching agreement (Lewicki, et al., 1999; Thompson,

Peterson & Kray, 1995).

The exchange of valid, relevant information has been found to predict both distributive

and integrative outcomes. Roth and Murnighan (1982), Brodt (1994), and Olekalns, Smith and

Walsh (1996) all found that parties with an informational advantage performed better with

respect to distributive outcomes. Likewise, several researchers have found that information

exchange is positively associated with higher integrative outcomes, most notably when that

information is about the parties’ interests or priorities (Olekalns, et al., 1996; Pruitt & Lewis,

1975; Thompson, 1991).

Negotiators also communicate invalid or misleading information in the form of

exaggerated offers, misrepresented facts, and false promises of future business opportunities

(Anton, 1990; Scouller, 1972). These communications, in fact, have been found in several

studies to occur with some degree of frequency. O=Connor and Carnevale (1997), for example,

discovered that participants misrepresented information in 28% of their negotiations in a

laboratory study. Murnighan, et al. (1999) compared experienced and naive bargainers, and

found that 34% of the former both lied and were deceptive. Boles, et al. (2000) found that

participants in a laboratory negotiation were deceptive about 13% of the time, with more

deception occurring in the early rounds of negotiation. In addition, they noted that the

individuals making propositions were far more likely to outright lie than were the individuals

responding to propositions.

5

Page 6: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

There are a number of reasons why individuals may choose to use invalid information in

a negotiation, including the financial stakes (Boles, et al., 2000; Kelley, Beckman & Fischer,

1967; Tenbrunsel, 1998), perceived time pressure (Yukl, Malone, Hayslip & Pamin, 1976),

environmental competitiveness (Hegarty & Sims, 1978), and their own personal

characteristics/values (Hegarty & Sims, 1978; Trevino & Youngblood, 1990).

Invalid information can take several different forms, from generally accepted behaviors

like exaggerating an offer or demand to less accepted behaviors such as making false promises.

Lewicki (1983), building on the work of Bok (1978) and others, developed a questionnaire and

typology of lying and deception in negotiation that consists of five categories: traditional

competitive bargaining (e.g., exaggerating an initial offer or demand), misrepresentation of

information (i.e., lying about an informational item), bluffing (i.e., leading your counterpart to

believe that you are in control of consequences which you are not, such as through promises you

may not be able to fulfill), inappropriate information collection (e.g., various forms of bribery,

such as paying others for information), and influencing a counterpart=s professional network

(e.g., encouraging others to defect). In subsequent studies, Lewicki and Robinson (1998) and

Volkema (1999a) employed factor analyses to confirm these five distinct categories of behavior

and examined differences in perceptions due to demographic factors.

Although the “Incidents in Negotiation Questionnaire” developed by Lewicki and his

colleagues has been employed in a number of settings, there have been no studies to confirm the

predictive validity of the instrument. The questionnaire measures individuals’ perceptions of the

appropriateness of questionable negotiating tactics and their likelihood of using those tactics, not

actual behavior. There is, however, a long history of research that links attitudes, intentions, and

behaviors (for a review, see Ajzen, 1988). In their meta-analysis of over one hundred studies,

Kim and Hunter (1993) found significant support for each of the three linkages, particularly

6

Page 7: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

where an individual’s actions fall within his/her control. Given that this is the case for most of

the negotiating tactics identified by Lewicki and his colleagues, we propose the following:

Hypothesis 1: Attitudes and intentions towards questionable or unethical negotiating

tactics (high perceived appropriateness and likely use, as measured by the “Incidents in

Negotiation Questionnaire”) will be positively associated with self-reported and actual use of

those tactics.

Despite the apparent frequency with which invalid information appears to be employed,

there have been relatively few studies that have examined the influence of invalid information on

negotiation outcome. Roth and Murnighan (1982) found that the party with the larger outcome

(payoff) and an informational advantage was inclined to misrepresent his or her outcome to

his/her ultimate advantage, while O=Connor and Carnevale (1997) found that subjects who

misrepresented information achieved higher outcomes. These studies, however, focused only on

distributive outcomes (i.e., outcomes based on a single party’s performance), not joint or

integrative outcomes (which are based on the performance of both parties in a dyadic

negotiation), and only on misrepresentation of information. Other tactics in the typology

identified by Lewicki and his colleagues (e.g., bluffing) were not examined.

It would seem likely that individual/distributive outcomes would generally increase with

the use of unethical tactics, as one’s counterpart is making decisions based on erroneous

information. The communication of false or deceptive information might even create a sense of

openness and trust on the part of the other party, causing him/her to share additional valid

information. The use of such tactics would likely have the opposite effect on joint or integrative

outcomes, decreasing with the use unethical tactics and vice versa. For example, by not

exaggerating an initial offer or misrepresenting information, the parties may actually negotiate a

7

Page 8: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

settlement that comes closer to their mutual benefit rather than benefiting one party (the party

exaggerating an offer or misrepresenting information) at the expense of the party.

Given these arguments, the following hypothesis is offered:

Hypothesis 2: The more questionable or unethical tactics an individual employs, the

higher will be his/her actual outcome, the higher his/her distributive outcome, and the lower the

joint/integrative outcome.

In addition, it would be valuable to know what affect questionable or unethical tactics

have on perceived outcomes. Negotiators seldom know the actual payoff (distributive or

integrative outcome) in real-world negotiations, since they seldom know the utilities and

winnings of the other party (Putnam & Jones, 1982). Instead, they make their decisions

regarding whether or not to engage in future negotiations based on perceived outcomes. Brodt

(1994) did one of the few studies that examined the effects of information on perceived outcome,

and found that informed parties were significantly more positive about their performance than

were their uninformed peers. However, the inside information that was shared with these parties

came from a third party, so the validity of the information might not have been questioned.

We might assume that the individual using questionable tactics believes he/she has an

advantage over the other party, since the other party is making decisions based on erroneous

information. Therefore, the negotiator would expect that his/her outcome would exceed the

other party’s performance, and that their joint outcome might suffer. Thus,

Hypothesis 3: The more questionable or unethical tactics an individual employs, the

higher will be his/her perceived outcome, the higher his/her perceived distributive outcome, and

the lower the perceived joint/integrative outcome.

Finally, many of the studies conducted to date assume that an agreement will be reached

by the negotiating parties. In reality, this is not always the case. In fact, many authors advise

8

Page 9: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

negotiators to develop alternatives to a negotiated agreement as a way of protecting themselves

against a bad deal; sometimes the best alternative is to walk away (Fisher, Ury & Patton, 1991;

Thompson, 1998; Volkema, 1999b).

There are a number of factors that might lead one or both negotiators to back away from

an agreement, including the terms of the agreement (which may not allow a negotiator to reach

his/her breakeven point) or the perceived ethicality of either party. As Tenbrunsel (1998) noted,

the use of unethical tactics can actually create a cycle of distrust when individuals project their

own circumstances and unethical behavior onto others. If either party perceives the other party

as being untrustworthy, the deal may seem too risky to consummate.

Given this reasoning, the following is hypothesized:

Hypothesis 4: The more questionable or unethical tactics an individual employs, the

greater the likelihood of not reaching an agreement.

Method

Participants

The participants in this study were sixty-six graduate business students taking courses in

negotiation. One course was offered in Rio de Janeiro, Brazil, and included individuals from

Brazil and France. Sixty-one percent were male. Each of these students was paired with a

student taking a comparable course at another university in Budapest, Hungary. The students

taking the course in Budapest came from a number of different countries, including Hungary,

Germany, the Netherlands, Greece, Russia, Mexico, and the United States. All courses were

taught in English.

Procedure

The subjects participated in a two-party, property leasing negotiation, as described in

Volkema (1999b). The thirty-three graduate students in Brazil represented a company called

9

Page 10: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

Logan Telecommunications, which was interested in expanding its operations in a new

geographic area. As such, they were interested in leasing 300 square meters of commercial

space. The other party (graduate students in Hungary) represented RJW Properties, Inc., a real

estate firm with properties in the desired area.

In addition to background information about their company, each representative was

given a scoring table with seven critical issues to be negotiated. The seven issues included cost

per square meter, duration of lease, renovations, cost of utilities, parking space, furnishings, and

advanced payment. Depending upon the outcome negotiated for an issue, the individual earned a

certain number of points (Table 1). Because some of the issues were worth more points to the

representative of Logan Telecommunications than to the representative of RJW Properties, and

vice versa, negotiators had an opportunity to maximize their joint/integrative score as well as

their distributive score. Multi-issue simulations of this type, with both distributive and

integrative potential, have been used effectively in a number of prior studies (cf. Olekalns, et al.,

1996; Thompson, 1991).

____________________________

Insert Table 1 about here.

____________________________

The negotiation took place via electronic mail. All communications were in English, and

participants had fourteen days to complete the negotiation. This time frame was deemed more

than sufficient, based on prior testing of the simulation, both in person and via the internet. All

negotiating dyads remained the same throughout the study.

10

Page 11: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

Three weeks prior to commencing the negotiation, the thirty-three individuals

representing Logan Telecommunications were asked to complete the “Incidents in Negotiation

Questionnaire.” This questionnaire, developed by Lewicki and his colleagues (Lewicki &

Robinson, 1998; Robinson, et al., 2000), asks respondents to rate the appropriateness and

likelihood of using eighteen tactics or behaviors, rating each behavior on a 7-point Likert scale

(for appropriateness: 1=not at all appropriate, 7=very appropriate; for likelihood of use: 1=not

at all likely, 7=very likely). The tactics consist of a range of behaviors with respect to ethicality,

from generally accepted competitive tactics (e.g., exaggerating an offer or demand, hiding one’s

bottom line) to tactics involving the other party’s professional network (e.g., seeking to discredit

one’s opponent with his or her supervisor). The questionnaire indicates that there are no Aright

answers@ regarding the right or wrong thing to do, so respondents are encouraged to be candid in

their answers. All respondents were assured anonymity.

Following conclusion of the negotiation, participants completed a second questionnaire in

which they were asked to indicate how well they thought they had performed in the negotiation

and how well they thought the other party had done (both on 7-point Likert scales), which tactics

they had used (i.e., misrepresentation of factual information, making promises that could not be

kept, exaggerating an opening demand or offer, pretending not to be in a hurry), and overall how

honest they had been and how honest they thought the other party had been in the negotiation (on

7-point Likert scales). The method of asking participants to report in a post-experiment

questionnaire on tactics and information exchanged has been employed successfully in other

studies of bargaining and negotiation (cf. Boles, et al., 2000; Murnighan, et al., 1999). All post-

negotiation questionnaires were anonymous.

Independent and Dependent Measures

11

Page 12: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

To determine the predictive validity of the “Incidents in Negotiation Questionnaire,” the

7-point Likert ratings of appropriateness and likely use were taken for eighteen tactics, including

the four focal tactics - pretending not to be in a hurry (when you were), exaggerating an offer or

demand, misrepresenting information, and making promises that could not or would not be kept.

These four focal tactics were deemed representative of the original eighteen, and each had the

potential of being employed during the actual negotiation. Other tactics, such as talking to the

people to whom an opponent reports in order to undermine the negotiation or intentionally

misrepresenting the progress of negotiations to the press, are tactics that could not be effected by

participants in this negotiation. Still other tactics, such as hiding one’s bottom line, could not be

easily measured by third parties reviewing transcripts of the negotiation. Overall measures were

calculated as the sums of the ratings for the four focal tactics as well as for all eighteen tactics for

appropriateness and likely use, respectively. Self-reported use of the four focal tactics was taken

from the post-negotiation questionnaire. In addition, participants also were asked to indicate on

this questionnaire their overall honesty in the negotiation on a 7-point Likert scale (1=not very

honest/ethical, 7=very honest/ethical), as well as the perceived honesty of the other party.

Actual behavior for each negotiator (Logan Telecommunications and RJW Properties)

was determined by three individuals independently reviewing printed transcripts of the email

negotiations. These individuals, who did not know the value of the negotiated outcomes at the

time of their review, identified incidents of the four focal tactics. Using the Estimate-Discuss-

Estimate approach (Nutt, 1992), their independent assessments were compared and any

differences were resolved through discussion. An overall measure of ethicality for each

participant was determined by adding the occurrences of these questionable or unethical

behaviors across categories.

12

Page 13: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

To determine the influence of ethics on performance, several measures of outcome were

taken. Perceived outcomes were based on self-assessments recorded on the post-negotiation

questionnaire, where subjects were asked to indicate how well they thought they had done in the

negotiation (7-point Likert scale) and how well they thought the other party had done, again on a

7-point Likert scale (from 1 to 7, where 1=not well and 7=very well). Perceived distributive

outcome was the difference in these point values (self minus other), while perceived

joint/integrative outcome was calculated as the sum of these values. Actual outcome was

measured by totaling the point values for each of the seven issues negotiated (Table 1). Given

that the simulation allowed for both distributive and integrative outcomes, the difference in

scores between an individual and his/her counterpart (distributive outcome) was calculated, as

well as the sum of their scores (joint/integrative outcome).

Analyses

Hypotheses 1-3 were tested using regression analyses, while Hypothesis 4 was tested

using discriminant analysis. Due to dependency concerns with data from negotiating dyads (e.g.,

highly correlated outcome measures within dyads), only data from Logan Telecommunications

representatives were analyzed in testing hypotheses (Kenny, 1995).

Results

From the “Incidents in Negotiation Questionnaire,” respondents indicated that pretending

to be in no hurry was the most appropriate of the four focal tactics examined (mean=5.94,

SD=1.39), followed closely by exaggerating an offer or demand (mean=5.52, SD=1.44),

misrepresenting information (mean=3.85, SD=2.15), and making promises that could not or

would not be kept (mean=2.15, SD=1.37) (Table 2). Of these four tactics, participants indicated

13

Page 14: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

that they would be most likely to pretend to be in no hurry (mean=5.21, SD=1.75), followed by

exaggerate an offer or demand (mean=5.15, SD=1.52), misrepresent information (mean=3.88,

SD=1.98), and make false promises (mean=1.79, SD=1.02). As these numbers suggest,

participants felt that it was more appropriate to use each tactic than they indicated they were

likely to do.

____________________________

Insert Table 2 about here.

____________________________

Consistent with findings reported elsewhere (cf. Volkema, 1997), the correlations

between attitudes (appropriateness) and intentions (likely use) were all significant. Specifically,

the correlation for pretending to be in no hurry was .43 (p<.05), the correlation for exaggerating

an offer was .71 (p<.001), the correlation for misrepresenting information was .85 (p<.001), and

the correlation for making a promise that could not or would not be kept was .65 (p<.001). The

correlation between attitudes and intentions for all eighteen tactics in the questionnaire was .70

(p<.001).

Eight of the thirty-three participants (24.2%) indicated on the post-negotiation

questionnaire that they had pretended not to be in a hurry (mean=.24, SD=.44), while twenty-

four participants (72.7%) felt that they had exaggerated an offer or demand during the

negotiation (mean=.73, SD=.45). Fifteen individuals (45.5%) indicated that they had

misrepresented information, from one to three times (mean=.73, SD=.91). One person indicated

that he/she had made a promise that could not or would not be kept (mean=.03, SD=.17). On a

scale from one to seven, where seven means very honest/ethical, participants indicated a mean

honesty in their negotiations of 6.12 (SD=1.02). Their mean perceived honesty of the other party

was 4.81 (SD=1.94). Thus, overall the thirty-three representatives of Logan

14

Page 15: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

Telecommunications felt that they had been more honest than their negotiating counterparts at

RJW Properties.

In terms of actual behavior (as determined from transcripts), two individuals (6.1%)

pretended to be in no hurry (when they actually were in a hurry), while ten individuals (30.3%)

were found to have exaggerated an offer or demand. Twenty-four (72.7%) misrepresented

information, from one to ten times (mean=2.67, SD=2.57). No one was found to have made a

promise that could not be kept. Overall, twenty-seven (81.8%) of the thirty-three participants

representing Logan Telecommunications used at least one of the four focal tactics. The numbers

were comparable for those individuals representing RJW Properties: twenty-five (75.8%) used

at least one of the tactics.

There were strong correlations between several of the self-reported behaviors and actual

behaviors. The correlation between participants’ perception of pretending to be in no hurry and

their actual use of this tactic was .45 (p<.01), while the correlation between perceived

misrepresentation of information and actual misrepresentation was .53 (p<.001). The correlation

between perceived exaggeration and actual exaggeration, while positive (.11), was not

significant. (The fact that more individuals reported exaggerating offers than were actually

counted is likely due to the fact that only offers or demands that were outside the specified

ranges shown in Table 1 were counted as exaggerated offers, while some individuals may have

exaggerated within a specified range for a given issue.) Overall, participants’ perceptions of

their honesty were highly correlated with actual use of these tactics (r=-.52, p<.01). That is, the

more honest a participant judged himself/herself to be in this negotiation, the less he or she

actually used these questionable or unethical tactics in the negotiation.

The correlations between participants’ specific attitudes and their actual behavior and

between intentions and actual behavior were all positive, but at best marginally significant

15

Page 16: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

(Hypothesis 1). The only significant findings were for exaggerating an offer (attitude: r=.32,

p<.10) and misrepresenting information (attitude: r=.31, p<.10; intention: r=.33, p<.10).

However, overall both attitudes and intentions were significantly correlated with overall behavior

(attitude: r=.48, p<.01; intentions: r=.35, p<.05).

In terms of performance, twenty-two of the thirty-three pairs of negotiators completed

their negotiations. For those reaching an agreement, the scores for the negotiators representing

Logan Telecommunications ranged from 2550 to 3000, with a mean of 2777.27 (SD=119.25),

while the scores for their counterparts (representing RJW Properties) ranged from 2400 to 3050

(mean=2675.00, SD=183.71). The distributive outcome for the focal participants ranged for –

400 to 600 (mean=102.27, SD=284.30). The integrative outcome ranged from 5300 to 5700

(mean=5452.27, SD=122.94).

Regression analyses revealed that the “Incidents in Negotiation Questionnaire” did

predict both self-reported and actual behavior. With appropriateness and likelihood of use of the

four focal tactics as the independent variables, participants’ appropriateness ratings for the sum

of the four tactics was positively associated with actual unethical behavior (β=.32, t=1.90, p<.10;

F1,32=3.59, p<.10, R2=.10). In addition, participants’ ratings for overall appropriateness of tactics

(all eighteen tactics) was negatively associated with perceived honesty in the negotiation (β=-.30,

t=-1.76, p<.10; F1,32=3.11, p<.10, R2=.09) and positively associated with actual unethical

behavior (β=.48, t=3.08, p<.01; F1,32=9.49, p<.01, R2=.23). There were no significant results for

perceived honesty of the other party. These significant findings suggest that the higher the

perceived appropriateness of the eighteen questionable or unethical tactics from the

questionnaire, the less honest the participant perceived himself/herself to be in the negotiation

and the more questionable or unethical tactics the individual actually employed in the

negotiation.

16

Page 17: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

When the perceived honesty of self and other as well as actual unethical behavior of other

were regressed on actual unethical behavior, the perceived honesty of self was found to be

negatively associated with actual unethical behavior of self (β=-.48, t=-3.37, p<.01) while actual

unethical behavior of the other party was found to be positively associated with actual unethical

behavior of self (β=.38, t=2.62, p<.05) (F2,31=9.74, p<.001, R2=.40). Thus, the more honest the

negotiator perceived himself/herself to be in the negotiation, the fewer questionable or unethical

tactics the individual employed during the negotiation. The lack of a significant correlation

between perceived honesty of self and the other party (r=-.01) suggests that participants did not

necessarily project their own ethicality onto their negotiating counterpart. However, the positive

relationship between actual unethical behavior of the two parties suggests that unethical behavior

might breed similar behavior from the other party.

As a follow-up analysis, the first use of a questionable or unethical behavior was

examined for each representative (Logan Telecommunications and RJW Properties). There was

a high correlation (r=.64, p<.001), suggesting that the first incidence of a questionable or

unethical behavior on the part of one negotiator corresponded to reciprocal behavior on the part

of the other negotiator. Furthermore, the earlier a Logan Telecommunications representative

used a questionable or unethical behavior in the sequence of email messages exchanged, the

more unethical the RJW representative was overall (r=-.40, p<.05), and the earlier the RJW

representative used a questionable or unethical behavior, the more unethical the Logan

Telecommunications representative was overall (r=-.36, p<.10).

17

Attitudes, intentions, perceived honesty, and actual behavior also were regressed on

outcome (actual and perceived). The more questionable or unethical tactics a party used (β=.37,

t=1.77, p<.10) and the less honest he/she thought the other party was in the negotiation (β=-.51,

t<-2.50, p<.05), the higher the negotiator’s score (consistent with Hypothesis 2) (F2,21=3.66,

Page 18: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

p<.05, R2=.28). These variables, however, did not predict actual distributive or integrative

outcomes.

The participant’s perception of the honesty of the other party and the actual unethical

behavior of the other party were the only variables significantly associated with perceived

outcomes. The more honest the other party was perceived to be, the better the negotiator

perceived he/she did in the negotiation (β=.30, t=1.70, p<.10; F1,31=2.89, p<.10, R2=.09) and the

better the negotiator perceived the other party did in the negotiation (β=.64, t=4.61, p<.001;

F1,31=21.23, p<.001, R2=.41). In addition, the more honest the other party was perceived to be

(β=-.58, t=-3.66, p<.001) and the more unethical the other party actually was (β=-.29, t=-1.83,

p<.10), the smaller the perceived distributive outcome (i.e., the difference between his/her

perceived performance and the perceived performance of the other party) (F2,31=7.11, p<.01,

R2=.33). Finally, the more honest the other party was perceived to be, the larger the perceived

joint outcome (β=.55, t=3.64, p<.001; F1,31=13.25, p<.001, R2=.31).

18

As previously noted, eleven pairs of negotiators were unable to reach an agreement in the

allotted time (a period of two weeks, which appeared substantial for completing the negotiation).

To determine if the ethics of the subjects might have played a role in predicting whether or not

an agreement was reached, a discriminant analysis was performed, with agreement/no agreement

as the dependent variable, and the mean score for appropriateness of tactics (attitude), mean

score for likely use of tactics (intentions), perceived honesty during the negotiation, perceived

honesty of the other party during the negotiation, and actual ethical behavior of each party during

the negotiation as the predictor variables. The analysis yielded a significant function (canonical

correlation=.54; Wilks’ lambda=.71; p<.01) involving two variables – likely use of tactics and

perceived honesty of the other party. Thus, the more likely the use of competitive or unethical

tactics, the greater the likelihood that no agreement would be reached (supporting Hypothesis 4).

Page 19: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

In addition, the less honest the other party was perceived to be, the greater the likelihood that no

agreement would be reached. The function correctly classified 78.1% of all cases into their

original groups, which exceeds the threshold suggested by Hair et al. (1998). In addition, Press’s

Q was significant (Q=10.1, p<.01), supporting the efficacy of the function.

Discussion

As reported elsewhere (Boles, et al., 2000; Murnighan, et al., 1999; O=Connor &

Carnevale, 1997; Scouller, 1972), the use of questionable or unethical tactics in negotiating is a

common phenomenon. In this study, nearly 82% of the participants were found to use one or

more of the four focal tactics. The tactic that was used most frequently was misrepresenting

information, which was used by 73.2% of the participants. In general, these percentages are

somewhat higher than the numbers reported in previous studies, which may be due to the

medium employed in this study – electronic mail – and the fact that most of the dyads

represented cross-cultural negotiations. At least one study has suggested that the use of unethical

tactics increases when negotiations occur via electronic mail, due in part to the inability of the

other party to read nonverbal cues (which can give away intentions to deceive) (Valley, Moag &

Bazerman, 1998). In addition, Volkema and Fleury (2002) report that the likelihood of unethical

behavior increases when an individual is negotiating with someone from another country,

particularly a country known for its skilled negotiators.

The “Incidents in Negotiation Questionnaire” was found to be modestly predictive of

perceived (self-reported) and actual behavior. While there were some marginally significant

relationships between perceived appropriateness or likely use of specific tactics and actual use of

those tactics, the overall measure of appropriateness for the eighteen behaviors was the best

predictor of actual ethical behavior (overall). Prior research on the linkages between attitudes,

intentions, and behavior has generally reported significant correlations between specific attitudes

19

Page 20: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

or intentions and specific behaviors, and between general attitudes or intentions and general

behaviors (Ajzen, 1988). The somewhat weak finding for the former (specific-to-specific

linkages) might suggest that the “Incidents in Negotiation Questionnaire” would benefit from

multiple measures of a single tactic, which could improve the validity of those measures. Each

of the eighteen tactics, including the four focal tactics, is measured with a single question (item).

The likely use of questionable or unethical tactics, the actual use of those tactics, and the

perceived honesty of the other party were the critical variables in predicting outcomes. The use

of questionable or unethical tactics by a party was positively associated with a party’s

performance, while the perceived honesty of the other party was negatively or inversely

associated with the negotiator’s score. Thus, a negotiator did better relative to others

representing Logan Telecommunications when he/she used these questionable tactics and when

he/she presumed the other party was using these tactics.

The perceived honesty of the other party was the primary variable significantly related to

perceived outcomes. The more honest the other party was perceived to be, the better a negotiator

perceived he/she did compared to others representing Logan Telecommunications, the poorer

he/she perceived he/she did in competition with the other party (distributive outcome), but the

better he/she perceived he/she did in cooperation with the other party (joint/integrative outcome).

Interestingly, the more honest the other party was perceived to be, the better the negotiator

perceived the other party did in the negotiation. Conceivably, the other party’s perceived

honesty led to an increase in exchange of valid information, or accommodation on certain issues.

There were two variables that were most effective in differentiating between negotiations

that reached agreement and negotiations that did not reach agreement – likely use of questionable

or unethical tactics (as measured by the “Incidents in Negotiation Questionnaire”) and the

perceived honesty of the other party. The more likely the use of these tactics and the less honest

20

Page 21: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

one’s counterpart was perceived to be, the greater the likelihood that the parties would not reach

an agreement. It is important to note, however, that the rating of the other party’s honesty

occurred at the conclusion of the negotiating period (when it was clear no agreement would be

reached). Therefore, it is conceivable that the respondent was seeking to explain or justify the

failed negotiation by placing blame elsewhere. In future research, this perception might be

measured at several points throughout the negotiation, to determine if perceptions change when it

first appears that no agreement will be reached.

The significant positive relationship found between subjects’ self-reported behavior and

actual behavior is also worth noting, as such findings have not always been reported by

researchers. In general, restricting the recall of an event to a short and recent reference period

and providing specific recall cues can help improve recall (Gilbert, Fiske & Lindzey, 1998).

However, individuals still may choose to misreport events when social capital is at stake. The

high correlation between self-reported behavior and actual behavior found in this study may be

due to the fact that participants knew transcripts of their exchanges would be available to others

(e.g., the researchers) for verification. While this finding requires further study, it suggests one

way of increasing the validity of self-report data of this type, where the issue is more one of

honesty in reporting than accuracy in reporting behaviors.

These findings have some direct implications for practitioners. First, as reported in prior

research, the use of questionable or unethical tactics is commonplace among negotiators

(approximately 80% in this study indicated use of at least one such tactic), although some tactics

may be considered more acceptable than other tactics. Thus, it is not unreasonable to assume

that the other party in a negotiation will use a questionable or unethical tactic, especially

misrepresentation of information (which was the tactic used most frequently). Second, an

individual’s general attitude/intentions towards the appropriateness and use of questionable or

21

Page 22: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

unethical tactics will likely translate into actual use of such tactics. Therefore, the extent to

which another party’s general attitude or intentions towards tactics can be ascertained prior to the

negotiation (e.g., through demographic information, reputation, etc.) or early in the negotiation

(e.g., through background and anecdotal exchanges) could be valuable in predicting the degree of

unethical behavior. Third, the first use of questionable or unethical tactics will likely result in

reciprocal behavior on the part of the other party, and the earlier such tactics are employed, the

more unethical the other party is likely to be overall. Fourth, the more likely one is to use

questionable or unethical tactics and the less honest the other party is perceived to be, the greater

the likelihood that no agreement will be reached. This could be costly if a viable agreement is

lost due to inaccurate perceptions of the other party. Whether or not someone can easily project

honesty while in fact acting unethically is important to this finding. An ad hoc correlation

analysis of participants’ perceptions of the other parties’ honesty and the other parties’ actual

unethical behavior was in the expected direction (r=-.26) but not statistically significant (p=.15).

Fifth, perceived outcomes matter, since many times negotiators do not know how they actually

performed relative to their counterpart; a decision to engage in future negotiations with the same

party must therefore be based on perceptions. Furthermore, based on the findings of this study,

one’s perceived performance (distributive and integrative) is likely to be tied to the perceived

honesty of the other party. The more honest the other party is perceived to be, the less the

negotiator’s distributive advantage is perceived to be and the larger the joint outcome is

perceived to be. Finally, the use of questionable or unethical tactics by a negotiator and the

perceived dishonesty of the other party can positively influence the negotiator’s actual

performance. Exaggerating an initial offer, for example, is both a generally accepted tactic and a

tactic that can directly affect outcome by serving as an anchor around which a final settlement is

reached. Cramton and Dees (Cramton & Dees, 1993; Dees & Cramton, 1991), in fact, argue that

22

Page 23: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

a negotiator has a moral right to exaggerate an initial offer, since the other party will assume this

was the case in any event and demand the negotiator compromise on a sincere initial offer.

As with all research, there are several caveats that must be offered regarding this study

and our findings. First, it should be pointed out that this negotiation took place entirely via

electronic mail. While this medium has its benefits in terms of data analysis and has been

employed effectively elsewhere (Tinsley, O’Connor & Sullivan, 2002), as already noted the use

of questionable or unethical tactics may vary depending on the medium and its richness (Daft &

Lengel, 1986; Daniels, 1967; Tinsley, et al., 2002). Electronic mail, as an asynchronous form of

communication that is not always reliable, introduces other issues that can complicate the

development of rapport and trust between parties. Before generalizing beyond this medium,

further study is clearly warranted. Although electronic mail may be used increasingly in

negotiations over the coming years, the parties frequently have some face-to-face contact as well.

Researchers need to recognize that other media (or combination media) might influence ethical

behavior.

It should also be pointed out that the parties in this negotiation were from many different

cultures (as is the case in most multinational organizations these days) and had had no prior

contact (or the guarantee of future contact). These factors also can affect the extent to which a

party employs questionable tactics (Volkema & Fleury, 2002), and how individuals perceive the

actions (or inactions) of the other party. With the continued globalization of markets and the

reliance on technologies such as electronic mail to negotiate agreements, these factors will

become increasingly important.

Finally, future research needs to examine the intentions with which invalid information is

employed. Invalid information may provide a means of checking another party’s knowledge

base, uncovering the other party’s settlement range, determining a counterpart’s negotiating style

23

Page 24: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

(e.g., cooperative, assertive), building rapport and perceived trust, or countering another party’s

dishonesty, among other purposes. Future research might seek to isolate these intentions in order

to better understand their influence on behavior and performance.

24

Page 25: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

References Ajzen, I. (1988). Attitudes, personality, and behavior. Milton Keynes: Open University Press. Anton, R.J. (1990). Drawing the line: An exploratory test of ethical behavior in negotiation.

The International Journal of Conflict Management, 1 (3), 265-280. Bok, S. (1978). Lying: Moral choice in public and private life. New York: Pantheon. Boles, T.L., Croson, R.T.A., Murnighan, J.K. (2000). Deception and retribution in repeated ultimatum bargaining. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 83 (2), 235-259. Brodt, S.E. (1994). AInside information@ and negotiator decision behavior. Organizational

Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 58, 172-202. Chatterjee, K., & Ulvila, J.W. (1982). Bargaining with shared information. Decision

Sciences, 13, 380-403. Cramton, P.C., & Dees, J.G. (1993). Promoting honesty in negotiation: An exercise in practical ethics. Business Ethics Quarterly, 3 (4), 359-394. Dees, G.J., & Cramton, P.C. (1991). Shrewd bargaining on the moral frontier: Toward a theory of morality in practice. Business Ethics Quarterly, 1 (2), 135-167. Daft, R.L., & Lengel, R.H. (1986). Organizational information requirements, media richness, and structural design. Management Science, 32, 554-571.

Daniels, V. (1967). Communication, incentive, and structural variables in interpersonal

exchange and negotiation. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 3, 47-74. Fisher, R., Ury, W., & Patton, B. (1991). Getting to yes. New York: Penguin. French, J.R.P., & Raven, B. (1959). The bases of social power. In D. Cartwright (Ed.),

Studies in social power (pp. 150-167). Ann Arbor, MI: University of Michigan Institute for Social Research.

Gilbert, D.T., Fiske, S.T., & Lindzey, G. (1998). The handbook of social psychology (4th ed.). Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill. Hair, J.F., Jr., Anderson, R.E., Tatham, R.L., & Black, W.C. (1998). Multivariate data analysis (5th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Prentice Hall. Harsanyi, J. (1962). Bargaining in ignorance of the opponent=s utility function. Journal of

Conflict Resolution, 6, 29-38.

25

Page 26: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

Hegarty, W.H., & Sims, H.P. (1978). Some determinants of unethical behavior: An experiment. Journal of Applied Psychology, 63, 451-457.

Kelley, H.H., Beckman, L.L., & Fischer, C.S. (1967). Negotiating the division of a reward

under complete information. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 3, 361-391. Kenny, D.A. (1995). The effect of nonindependence on significance testing in dyadic research. Personal Relationships, 2, 67-75. Kim, M., & Hunter, J.E. (1993). Relationships among attitudes, behavioral intentions, and behavior: A meta-analysis of past research, part 2. Communication Research, 20 (3), 331-364. Lax, D.A., & Sebenius, J.K. (1986). The manager as negotiator: Bargaining for cooperation

and competitive gain. New York: The Free Press. Lewicki, R.J. (1983). Lying and deception: A behavioral model with applications to

negotiations. In M.H. Bazerman and R.J. Lewicki (Eds.), Negotiating in organizations (pp. 86-90). Beverly Hills, CA: Sage.

Lewicki, R.J., & Robinson, R.J. (1998). Ethical and unethical bargaining tactics: An

empirical study. Journal of Business Ethics, 17, 665-682.

Lewicki, R.J., Saunders, D.M., & Minton, J.W. (1997). Essentials of negotiation. Chicago, IL: Irwin.

Luce, D., & Raiffa, H. (1957). Games and decisions. New York: Wiley & Sons. McMillan, J. (1992). Games, strategies, managers: How managers can use game theory to

make better business decisions. New York: Oxford University Press. Mintzberg, H. (1973). The nature of managerial work. New York: Harper & Row. Murnighan, J.K., Babcock, L., Thompson, L., & Pullutla, M. (1999). The information

dilemma in negotiations: Effects of experience, incentives, and integrative potential. The International Journal of Conflict Management, 10 (4), 313-339.

Nash, J. (1950). The bargaining problem. Econometrica, 28, 155-162. Nutt, P.C. (1992). Formulation tactics and the success of organizational decision making, Decision Sciences, 23 (3), 519-540. O=Connor, K.M., & Carnevale, P.J. (1997). A nasty but effective negotiation strategy:

Misrepresentation of a common-value issue. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 504-515.

Olekalns, M., Smith, P.L, & Walsh, T. (1996). The process of negotiating: Strategy and

26

Page 27: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

timing as predictors of outcomes. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 68, 68-77.

Pinkley, R. Griffith, T., & Northcraft, G. (1995). AFixed pie@ a la mode: Information

availability, information processing, and the negotiation of sub-optimal agreements. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 62, 101-112.

Pruitt, D.G., & Carnevale, P. (1993). Negotiation in social conflict. Pacific Grove, CA:

Brooks-Cole. Pruitt, D.G., & Lewis, S.A. (1975). Development of integrative solutions in bilateral

negotiation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 621-630. Pruitt, D.G., & Rubin, J. (1986). Social conflict: Escalation, stalemate, and settlement. New

York: Random House. Putnam, L.L., & Jones, T.S. (1982). The role of communication in bargaining. Human

Communication Research, 8 (3), 262-280. Robinson, R.J., Lewicki, R.J., & Donahue, E.M. (2000). Extending and testing a five factor model of ethical and unethical bargaining tactics: Introducing the SINS scale. Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21, 649-664 Roth, A., & Murnighan, J.K. (1982). The role of information in bargaining: An

experimental study. Econometrica, 50, 1123-1142. Rubin, J.Z., & Brown, B.R. (1975). The social psychology of bargaining and negotiation.

New York: Academic Press. Schelling, T. (1960). The strategy of conflict. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Scouller, A. (1972). Disclosure of information in collective bargaining. Personnel

Management, 46, 22-24. Shell, G.R. (1999). Bargaining for advantage: Negotiation strategies for reasonable people.

New York: Viking. Tenbrunsel, A.E. (1998). Misrepresentation and expectations of misrepresentation in an

ethical dilemma: The role of incentives and temptation. Academy of Management Journal, 41 (3), 330-339.

Thompson, L. (1991). Information exchange in negotiation. Journal of Experimental Social

Psychology, 27 (2), 161-179. Thompson, L. (1998). The mind and heart of the negotiator. Upper Saddle River, NJ:

Prentice Hall.

27

Page 28: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

Thompson, L., Peterson, E., & Kray, L. (1995). Social context in negotiation: An information-processing perspective. In R.M. Kramer and D.M. Messick (Eds.), Negotiation as a social process (pp. 5-36). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage

Tinsley, C.H., O’Connor, K.M., & Sullivan, B.A. (2002). Tough guys finish last: The perils of a distributive reputation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 88, 621-642. Trevino, L.K., & Youngblood, S.A. (1990). Bad apples in bad barrels: A causal analysis of

ethical decision-making behavior. Journal of Applied Psychology, 75, 378-385. Valley, K.L., Moag, J., & Bazerman, M.H. (1998). ‘A matter of trust’: Effects of communication on the efficiency and distribution of outcomes. Journal of Economic Behavior and Organization, 34, 211-238. Volkema, R.J. (1997). Perceptual differences in appropriateness and likelihood of use of negotiation behaviors: A cross-cultural analysis. The International Executive, 39 (3), 335-350. Volkema, R.J. (1999a). Ethicality in negotiations: An analysis of perceived similarities and

differences between Brazil and the United States. Journal of Business Research, 45, 59-67.

Volkema, R.J. (1999b). The negotiation toolkit: How to get exactly what you want in any

business or personal situation. New York: AMACOM. Volkema, R.J., & Fleury, M.T.L. (2002). Alternative negotiating conditions and the choice of negotiation tactics: A cross-cultural comparison. Journal of Business Ethics, 36, 381- 398. von Neumann, J., & Morgenstern, O. (1944). Theory of games and economic behavior.

Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Walton, R., & McKersie, R. (1965). A behavioral theory of labor relations. New York:

McGraw-Hill. Yukl, G.A., Malone, M.P., Hayslip, B., & Pamin, T.A. (1976). The effects of time pressure

and issue settlement order on integrative bargaining. Sociometry, 39 (3), 277-281.

28

Page 29: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

TABLE 1 Issues, Negotiated Outcomes, and Point Values ________________________________________________________________________________ Issue Negotiated outcome Point value ________________________________ Logan Telecom. RJW Properties ________________________________________________________________________________ Cost per square meter $500. 900 300 $600. 750 450 $700. 600 600 $800. 450 750 $900. 300 900 Renovation of space No rooms renovated 150 250 One room 400 200 Two rooms 500 150 Three rooms 600 100 Utilities included None 100 300 Water/sewer 150 200 Water/sewer/electricity 200 100 Length of lease One year 500 200 Two years 450 500 Three years 300 700 Four years 200 900 Parking available No cars 100 300 One car 300 250 Two cars 500 200 Three cars 600 150 Four cars 650 100 Furnishings None 100 100 Refrigerator/stove 350 150

Advanced payment One month 500 200 Six months 350 600 One year 150 900 Maximum 3700 3700 Minimum 1100 1100 ________________________________________________________________________________

29

Page 30: ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, …€¦ · ETHICALITY IN NEGOTIATION: AN ANALYSIS OF ATTITUDES, INTENTIONS, AND OUTCOMES Roger J. Volkema Kogod School of Business

TABLE 2 Descriptive Statistics and Correlation Matrix for Attitudes, Intentions, and Behaviors a Variables Mean SD 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 Attitude 1. Pretend no hurry 5.94 1.39 2. Exaggerate offer 5.52 1.44 .24 3. Misrep. info. 3.85 2.15 .53** .20

4. Make promises 2.15 1.37 .05 -.07 .30 t 5. All tactics 66.82 16.92 .56*** .20 .68*** .52** Intention 6. Pretend no hurry 5.21 1.75 .43* .01 .34 t .13 .30 t 7. Exaggerate offer 5.15 1.52 .23 .71*** .12 -.06 -.05 .31 t 8. Misrep. info. 3.88 1.98 .53*** .10 .85*** .31 t .63*** .44** .11 9. Make promises 1.79 1.02 .06 .06 .10 .65*** .23 .11 .20 .20 10. All tactics 59.06 12.71 .48** .14 .47** .34* .70*** .61*** .28 .67*** .39* Self-reported Behavior 11. Pretend no hurry .24 .44 -.08 -.11 .14 .04 .02 .10 .08 .04 .33 t .08 12. Exaggerate offer .73 .45 -.03 .32 t .12 .02 -.07 -.24 .15 -.18 .01 -.42 * .19 13. Misrep. info. .73 .91 .23 .23 .54 *** -.02 .42 * .04 .14 .54*** .17 .48 ** .17 -.11 14. Make promises .03 .17 .14 -.56 *** .26 .24 .18 .18 -.49 ** .28 -.14 -.03 -.10 .11 -.14 15. Overall honesty - self 6.12 1.02 .16 -.11 -.28 -.17 -.30 t -.09 -.05 -.24 -.09 -.28 -.07 .14 -.33 t .15 16. Overall honesty – other 4.81 1.94 -.18 -.02 .13 .07 .24 .00 -.21 .12 .12 .20 .01 -.03 .32 t .11 -.01 Actual Behavior 17. Pretend no hurry .06 .24 .20 -.09 .38* -.12 .27 .19 -.03 .21 .05 .23 .45 ** -.13 .36 * -.05 -.41 * .03 18. Exaggerate offer .30 .47 .08 .32 t .23 .02 .28 .11 .29 .24 .01 .27 .09 .11 .13 -.12 -.21 .21 .11 19. Misrep. info. 2.67 2.57 .18 .20 .31 t -.02 .44 ** .09 -.06 .33 t -.08 .31 t -.04 -.27 .53 *** -.19 -.48** -.06 .39 * .11 20. Make promises .00 .00 -- -- -- -- -- -- -- -- -- -- -- -- -- -- -- -- -- -- -- 21. All Tactics 3.03 2.77 .20 .23 .36 * -.03 .48** .12 -.01 .37 * -.06 .35* .02 -.24 .55 *** -.20 -.52** -.02 .46** .28 .98*** --

a N=33. t p<.10, * p<.05, **p<.01, ***p<.001

30