environment and sdmprocess review 2014

Upload: johnalis22

Post on 02-Jun-2018

214 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/10/2019 Environment and SDMProcess REVIEW 2014

    1/25

    The Influence of Context on the StrategicDecision-Making Process: A Review of

    the Literature

    Neil Gareth Shepherd and John Maynard RuddMarketing Group, Aston Business School, Aston University, Aston Triangle, Birmingham B4 7ET, UK

    Corresponding author email:[email protected]

    This paper critically reviews the strategic decision-making process literature, with aspecific focus on the effects of context. Context refers to the top management team,

    strategic decision-specific characteristics, the external environment and firm charac-

    teristics. This literature review also develops an illustrative framework that incorpo-rates these four different categories of contextual variables that influence the strategicdecision-making process. As a result of the variety and pervasiveness of contextualvariables featured within the literature, a comprehensive and up-to-date review is

    essential for organizing and synthesizing the extant literature to explicate an agenda forfuture research. The purpose of this literature review is threefold: first, to criticallyreview the strategic decision-making process literature to highlight the underlying

    themes, issues, tensions and debates in the field; second, to identify the opportunitiesfor future theory development; and third, to state the methodological implications

    arising from this review.

    Introduction

    Strategic decisions (SDs) can be ill-structured, non-

    routine, uncertain and pervasive. They cut across

    organizational functions, entail a significant financial

    outlay, and have profound, long-term implications

    for the organization (Eisenhardt and Zbaracki 1992;

    Mintzberg et al. 1976; Shrivastava and Grant 1985).

    While SDs are not always entirely different from

    other organizational decisions, they are towards one

    end of a continuum, at the other end of which are the

    trivial everyday questions (Hickson et al. 1986, p.

    27). The strategic decision-making process (SDMP)

    is described as a set of different characteristics, such

    as rational, comprehensive, political; or as a

    sequence of activities (Goll and Rasheed 2005) that

    involves information gathering, developing alterna-tives and choosing among alternatives (Wally and

    Baum 1994). Strategic decision-making process

    research is of great importance, because the insights

    that it provides can improve the effectiveness of SDs

    made by executives, which ultimately contribute to

    the success of organizations.

    Context refers to the top management team

    (TMT), strategic decision-specific characteristics,

    the external environment and firm characteristics

    (Sharfman and Dean 1997b), and the lack of a sys-

    tematic treatment of contextual variables has resulted

    in an incomplete, and perhaps inaccurate, picture ofSDM (strategic decision-making) (Hough and

    White 2003, p. 488). Most existing SDMP studies

    have adopted an incremental approach to theory

    development, and focused only on a limited number

    of contextual variables. This has resulted in a frag-

    mented understanding, and left SDMP scholars

    unable to identify the key contextual influences on

    The authors would like to thank Kamel Mellahi, Editor-in-Chief, and the anonymous reviewers for their helpful andinsightful comments on earlier drafts of this work.

    bs_bs_banner

    International Journal of Management Reviews, Vol. 16, 340364 (2014)

    DOI: 10.1111/ijmr.12023

    2013 British Academy of Management and John Wiley & Sons Ltd. Published by John Wiley & Sons Ltd, 9600 GarsingtonRoad, Oxford OX4 2DQ, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA

    mailto:[email protected]:[email protected]
  • 8/10/2019 Environment and SDMProcess REVIEW 2014

    2/25

    the SDMP (Papadakis and Barwise 1997). Inconsist-

    encies among existing studies highlight the need for

    future research to pay closer attention to context

    (Elbanna and Child 2007b). For example, the effects

    of SDMP comprehensiveness on organizational

    performance remain unclear, with Fredrickson and

    Mitchell (1984) finding negative effects, andBourgeois and Eisenhardt (1988) finding positive

    effects. Closer scrutiny of context will reveal insights

    that can help to reconcile such contradictory results.

    Elbanna (2006, p. 14) emphasizes the need for this

    review, stating that we still know little about the role

    of other contextual variables in the SDMP, and that

    a next logical step in this line of critical review

    would be to review the role of contextual variables in

    the SDMP. Moreover, a priority for SDMP research

    is to identify the extent to which variance in the

    characteristics of the SDMP is explained by the

    context in which it takes place (Papadakis et al.1998; Rajagopalan et al. 1997). Similarly, Elbanna

    and Child (2007b) and Pettigrew (2003) emphasize

    that the rationality of the SDMP cannot be fully

    understood without comprehension of its context.

    Furthermore, it is vitally important to examine inter-

    actions between contextual variables and SDMP

    characteristics, because such interactions have sig-

    nificant implications for SDMP outcomes, such as

    the overall quality of a decision (Papadakis et al.

    2010).

    This paper critically reviews the SDMP literature

    with a specific focus on the effects of contextualvariables, to provide an in-depth analysis of the

    underlying themes, issues, tensions and debates in

    the domain, and to identify priorities for future

    research, together with the important methodological

    implications. An illustrative framework of the con-

    textual variables and their relationships with SDMP

    characteristics and outcomes is presented (Figure 1).

    The illustrative framework provides a scheme around

    which this review is structured. Such a review

    scheme allows the systematic identification of

    themes and contributions, and allows the similarities

    and discrepancies from such a diverse set of studies

    to be discerned (Ginsberg and Venkatraman 1985).

    Approach to the review

    To ensure inclusiveness with regard to the literature

    reviewed, this paper adopts an ontological framework

    whereby the concept of a decision is viewed as valu-

    able for understanding aspects of organizational

    behaviour (Miller 2010). However, certain authors

    (e.g. Mintzberg and Waters 1990) suggest that exam-

    ining decisions can be a hindrance to understanding

    organizational processes, because individual deci-

    sions can be troublesome to identify. Actions can

    occur without a formal decision having been made,

    and organizations can take a particular course ofaction in response to the external environment, rather

    than as a result of a systematic decision process.

    Ultimately, whether decisions are suitable subjects

    for empirical enquiry or not, and whether researchers

    can obtain objective knowledge of decision pro-

    cesses, rests upon the ontology of the researcher

    (Pettigrew 1990). The position taken in this paper is

    that an understanding of SDMPs is possible, and is

    useful for explaining differences in organizational

    performance. However, such research is challenging,

    in great part because of the complex influence of

    context.To conduct this review, and in keeping with

    prior classifications (e.g. Papadakis et al. 1998;

    Rajagopalan et al. 1993; 1997), we derived four cat-

    egories of contextual variables from an extensive lit-

    erature search: the TMT; SD-specific characteristics;

    the external environment; and firm characteristics.

    Each has either a direct effect on the characteristics

    of the SDMP or a moderating effect on the relation-

    ship between SDMP characteristics and SDMP out-

    comes (see Figure 1). Some studies also specify

    the direct effects of contextual variables on SDMP

    outcomes.Variables pertaining to each of the four categories

    were identified through keyword searches of top

    peer-reviewed academic journals in the ProQuest,

    EBSCO, Emerald Full Text, JSTOR Business,

    Science Direct and PsycArticles databases. Each

    article was then allocated to one of the four catego-

    ries of contextual variables. To ensure reliability, the

    classification decisions were independently verified

    by a senior academic familiar with the subject. To

    assure the completeness of the search, the Social

    Science Citation Index Journal Impact Factor was

    used to identify top peer-reviewed journals that

    commonly address the topic of strategic decision-

    making; these journals were then searched indivi-

    dually for articles featuring contextual variables.

    Manual searches of multiple reference lists were also

    conducted, and an electronic library catalogue was

    searched to identify relevant books.

    The search procedures generated a substantial

    working list of articles. This list was narrowed to

    specifically relevant articles by applying three strict

    Context in Strategic Decision-Making 341

    2013 British Academy of Management and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

  • 8/10/2019 Environment and SDMProcess REVIEW 2014

    3/25

    SDM

    PCharacteristics

    SDMPOutcomes

    ContextualVariab

    les

    Rationality

    Compreh

    ensiveness

    Political

    behaviour/politicization

    Intuition/intuitivesynthesis

    Lateralcommunication

    Hierarchicaldecentralization

    Problem

    solvingdissension

    Useoffinancialreporting

    Ruleform

    alization

    Flexibility

    Agreeme

    ntaboutcomprehensiveness

    Changei

    ncomprehensiveness

    Conflict

    Participation

    Levelof

    aggression

    Toleranceofambiguity/uncertainty

    Assessment/perceptionsofrisk

    Technocratic

    Sporadic

    Fluid

    Constricted

    Effectiveness

    Quality

    Speed

    Commitment

    Erraticstrategic

    decisions

    Organizational

    Performance

    Decisiveness

    Newproduct

    performance/quality

    TopManagementTea

    m

    (Table1)

    Demographics(tenure,

    education,diversity,age)

    Cognitivediversity/style

    Personality

    Other(meta-cognition,potency,

    polychronicity,aggressiv

    e

    philosophy)

    StrategicDecisionSpec

    ific

    Characteristics

    (Table2)

    Matter

    Uncertainty

    Motive

    Importance

    Timepressure

    TheExternalEnvironm

    ent

    (Table3)

    Dynamism

    Velocity

    Munificence

    Hostility

    Instability

    Uncertainty

    FirmCharacteristics

    (Table4)

    Powercentralization

    Structure

    Size

    Performance/Slackresources

    Other(externalcontrol,

    corporatecontrol,formal

    planningsystems)

    Directeffects

    Moderatingeffects

    Figure

    1.Contextua

    lvariab

    lesinSD

    MPresearch:anillustrative

    framewor

    kofe

    xtantresearch

    342 N.G. Shepherd and J.M. Rudd

    2013 British Academy of Management and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

  • 8/10/2019 Environment and SDMProcess REVIEW 2014

    4/25

    criteria, and articles were included in the final list

    only if they: (i) explicitly examine the direct effects

    of a particular contextual variable on SDMP charac-

    teristics, or (ii) explicitly examine the effects of a

    particular contextual variable on the relationship

    between SDMP characteristics and SDMP outcomes,

    or (iii) explicitly examine the effects of a particularcontextual variable on SDMP decision-level out-

    comes (e.g. decision speed, decision quality).

    Articles were excluded if the main focus was not the

    process of strategic decision-making.

    This review is structured as follows. First, we criti-

    cally review the empirical literature relating to each

    of the four categories of contextual variables to high-

    light the underlying themes, issues, tensions and

    debates in the field. We structure this review accord-

    ing to studies examining the direct effects of contex-

    tual variables on SDMP characteristics, to those

    examining the direct effects on SDMP outcomes andto those examining the moderating effects of contex-

    tual variables on the relationship between SDMP

    characteristics and outcomes. Second, we discuss the

    priorities for future theory development, and, third,

    we state the methodological implications arising

    from this review.

    The top management team

    The TMT is the dominant coalition of the most senior

    executives who have responsibility for setting theoverall direction of the organization (Hambrick and

    Mason 1984). Articles reviewed here are predicated

    on the concept of bounded rationality, whereby SDs

    are the product of behavioural influences, rather than

    economic utility maximizing processes (Cyert and

    March 1963; March and Simon 1958). Bounded

    rationality acknowledges inherent cognitive limita-

    tions of decision-makers that restrict their ability to

    collect and analyse all relevant information and iden-

    tify all possible alternatives (Griffith et al. 2012).

    The TMT variables reviewed in this section include

    demographic (tenure, education and diversity) and

    psychometric variables (cognitive diversity, cognitive

    style and personality) (see Figure 1 and Table 1). As

    well, TMT variables feature predominantly as ante-

    cedents of SDMP characteristics and outcomes, with

    no studies examining their moderating influence on

    the SDMP characteristicsoutcomes relationships.

    In the current section, we review articles that

    examine the direct effects of TMT demographic and

    psychometric variables on SDMP characteristics, as

    well as articles that examine the direct effects of

    these TMT variables on SDMP outcomes. Consistent

    with prior treatment in the literature (e.g. Papadakis

    and Barwise 2002; Papadakis et al. 1998), CEO

    variables are included in this section, because the

    CEO is cited as the most influential TMT member

    (Hambrick and Mason 1984).

    Direct effects of TMT demographic variables on

    SDMP characteristics

    Tenure. Tenure of the TMT and the CEO,

    operationalized as length of service with an organi-

    zation, has been found to influence the level of

    rationality, comprehensiveness and the extent of

    middle management participation in the SDMP. Goll

    and Rasheed (2005) found that long-tenured TMTs

    adopt rational SDMPs they engage in continuous

    proactive searches, undertake extensive analysis andconduct formal planning. Empirical evidence also

    indicates that, as the tenure of the TMT increases,

    so too does comprehensiveness. Fredrickson and

    Iaquinto (1989) term this phenomenon creeping

    rationality, and, as tenure increases,TMTs engage in

    ever more thorough situation diagnosis, generation

    and evaluation of alternatives, and integration of the

    decision into the overall strategy of the firm.

    Though middle management in organizations led

    by long-tenured CEOs have a higher level of involve-

    ment in the SDMP (termed hierarchical decentrali-

    zation), CEO tenure does not significantly affectother important and frequently studied SDMP char-

    acteristics such as comprehensiveness (Papadakis

    and Barwise 2002; Papadakis et al. 1998). Hence,

    the CEO may be the most powerful member of the

    TMT (Hambrick and Mason 1984), yet the effect of

    CEO tenure on SDMP comprehensiveness appears to

    be less influential than that of TMTs.

    TMT education level. TMT education level (e.g.

    high school, undergraduate degree, Masters degree)

    influences rationality and comprehensiveness in

    SDMP. Highly educated TMTs are more rational in

    SDM, owing to the strengthened analytical ability

    that results from increased education level (Goll and

    Rasheed 2005). Similarly, Papadakis and Barwise

    (2002) found that highly educated TMTs are more

    comprehensive in situation diagnosis, alternatives

    generation and evaluation, and integrating the

    decision into the firms overall strategy. While TMT

    education level influences rationality and com-

    prehensiveness, Papadakis et al. (1998) determined

    Context in Strategic Decision-Making 343

    2013 British Academy of Management and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

  • 8/10/2019 Environment and SDMProcess REVIEW 2014

    5/25

  • 8/10/2019 Environment and SDMProcess REVIEW 2014

    6/25

    Forbes(2005)

    Founder-managerage

    andpriorventure

    experience

    98founder-managers

    (entrepreneurs)ofInternet

    start-ups

    Field-based;cross-sectional;

    on-lineandmailsurvey;

    archivaldata;recentSDsmade

    bythefirms

    Regression

    analysis

    FirmsmakefasterSDswhenmanage

    dbyolder

    founder-managersandbythosewithpriorventure

    experience

    GollandRasheed

    (2005)

    TMTage,tenure

    ,and

    educationleve

    l

    159Manufacturingfirms

    Field-based;cross-sectional;mail

    survey;archivaldata;actual

    SDMPs

    Regression

    analysis

    AveragetenureandeducationleveloftheTMTispositively

    relatedtorationality,butaveragea

    geisnot

    Houghandogilvie

    (2005)

    Cognitivestyle

    749executives

    Laboratory-basedbehavioural

    simulation

    Structurale

    quation

    modelling

    Executiveswhousebothintuitionandobjectiveinformation

    makehigherqualitydecisions,whilethosewhotaketimeto

    makesociallyacceptabledecisions

    areindecisiveand

    perceivedtobeineffective

    Olsonetal.(2007a)

    TMTcognitived

    iversity

    TMTsfrom85hospitals

    Field-based;cross-sectional;mail

    survey;recentSDmadebythe

    firms

    Regression

    analysis

    Cognitivediversityhasapositiverelationshipwithtask

    conflict,andcompetence-basedtru

    ststrengthensthis

    relationship.Taskconflictmediatestherelationshipbetween

    cognitivediversityandSDunderstanding,commitmentand

    quality

    Olsonetal.(2007b)

    TMTcognitived

    iversity

    252Chineseexecutives

    Field-based;crosssectional;

    survey;recentSDmadebythe

    firms

    Regression

    analysis

    CognitivediversityhasanegativerelationshipwithSD

    commitmentandquality,andtherelationshipsare

    moderatedbyaffect-basedandcog

    nition-basedtrust

    Souitarisand

    Maestro(2010)

    TMTpolychronicity

    129TMTsfromnewtechnology

    ventures

    Field-based;cross-sectional;mail

    survey;recentSDmadebythe

    firms

    Regression

    analysis

    TMTpolychronicityhasapositiveef

    fectonSDspeedanda

    negativeeffectonSDMPcomprehensiveness.SDspeedand

    comprehensivenesspartiallymedia

    tetherelationship

    betweenTMTpolychronicityandfinancialperformance

    Mitchelletal.

    (2011)

    CEOmeta-cognition

    64CEOsoftechnologyfirms

    Field-basedexperiment;

    hypotheticalchoices;mail

    survey

    Conjointan

    alysis

    TheSDsmadebyCEOswithgreatermetacognitiveexperience

    arelesserratic

    ClarkandMaggitti

    (2012)

    TMTpotency

    54TMTsofhigh-technology

    companies

    Field-based;cross-sectional;

    interviews;mailsurveyof

    actualSDMPs

    Regression

    analysis

    TMTpotencyispositivelyrelatedto

    SDspeed.TMTpotency

    partiallymediatestherelationshipbetweenTMTexperience,

    knowledge,interactionprocessand

    SDspeed

    Context in Strategic Decision-Making 345

    2013 British Academy of Management and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

  • 8/10/2019 Environment and SDMProcess REVIEW 2014

    7/25

    that CEO education level affects only the use of

    financial reporting information. Highly educated

    CEOs make greater use of financial reporting infor-

    mation in the SDMP; however, CEO education level

    does not significantly influence any other character-

    istics of the SDMP, such as comprehensiveness

    (Papadakis and Barwise 2002; Papadakis et al.1998).

    TMT demographic diversity. TMT demographic

    diversity refers to heterogeneity in the age, tenure,

    experience and education in the TMT, and has only

    weak direct effects on comprehensiveness defined

    as thoroughness and inclusiveness in making and

    integrating SDs (Simons et al. 1999). The same

    study identified that interactions between job-related

    demographic diversity measures (e.g. company

    tenure diversity) and TMT debate (discussions

    concerning how to approach the decision) are moresignificant predictors of comprehensiveness than

    interactions between non-job-related diversity meas-

    ures (e.g. age diversity) and TMT debate.

    The findings of the studies featuring TMT and

    CEO tenure, education and demographic diversity

    reviewed here show that demographic variables such

    as tenure and education do significantly affect char-

    acteristics of the SDMP such as rationality, compre-

    hensiveness, hierarchical decentralization and the

    use of financial reporting information. However, job-

    related TMT demographic diversity variables appear

    to influence comprehensiveness more than non-job-related demographic diversity variables. It is also

    apparent that TMT and CEO demographics exert dif-

    ferent influences on SDMP characteristics. Certainly,

    TMT demographic variables appear to be a stronger

    influence on SDMP comprehensiveness than those of

    the CEO.

    A lack of significant results from studies examin-

    ing CEO demographics brings into question the

    focus on individual CEOs for explaining SDMP

    characteristics. Hambrick and Mason (1984)

    acknowledge that the TMT as a unit of analysis has

    more explanatory power than the individual CEO,

    because tasks such as SDM are rarely undertaken

    exclusively by the CEO alone, but more often are

    shared between members of the TMT. Also, the con-

    struct validity of demographic variables has been

    questioned in the literature, prompting calls to use

    direct psychometric measures of TMT cognitive and

    behavioural traits (e.g. cognitive diversity instead of

    demographic diversity) to enhance the validity and

    explanatory capability of research (e.g. Hough and

    ogilvie 2005; Priemet al. 1999). While methodologi-

    cally convenient, using demographic variables leaves

    a black box (Lawrence 1997) of unexplained TMT

    cognitions, values and perceptions that influence

    SDs.

    Direct effects of TMT psychometric variables on

    SDMP characteristics

    Cognitive diversity. Cognitive diversity refers to

    differences in the preferences and beliefs of

    TMT members concerning the strategic goals and

    priorities of the organization (Miller et al. 1998).

    Cognitive diversity has been found to reduce com-

    prehensiveness (Milleret al. 1998) and to increase

    task conflict (Olsonet al. 2007a). Milleret al. (1998)

    found that cognitively diverse TMTs are less com-

    prehensive; instead, their SDMPs are characterized

    by limited brainstorming, the consideration of anarrow range of alternatives and restricted use of

    quantitative analyses. Cognitive diversity also causes

    task conflict, and, when TMT members disagree

    about the strategic goals and priorities of the organi-

    zation, the SDMP is characterized by discord and

    differences in judgment (Olson et al. 2007a). The

    same study also found that, when TMT members

    trust one anothers competence, the effects of cogni-

    tive diversity on task conflict are stronger. Hence,

    these two studies show that cognitive diversity

    diminishes comprehensive analysis and the consid-

    eration of multiple decision options and, instead,gives rise to conflict and disagreement during the

    SDMP.

    Cognitive style. Cognitive style describes how

    people perceive, think, solve problems, learn, and

    relate to each other (Hough and ogilvie 2005, p.

    421). Using laboratory simulations and measuring

    cognitive style by applying the MyersBriggs Type

    Indicator, Henderson and Nutt (1980) and Nutt

    (1993) discovered that executive cognitive style

    influenced the level of aggression, tolerance of ambi-

    guity and uncertainty, and assessment of risk in the

    SDMP. Nutt (1990) also used a laboratory-based

    simulation, measuring the cognitive style of 79

    senior executives and 89 middle managers through

    the MyersBriggs Type Indicator. The study shows

    that cognitive style determines the perceived risk in

    making the SD, as well as the likelihood of taking

    strategic action. The study also indicates that cogni-

    tive style determines the type of data and the mode of

    data processing used in the SDMP. However, while

    346 N.G. Shepherd and J.M. Rudd

    2013 British Academy of Management and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

  • 8/10/2019 Environment and SDMProcess REVIEW 2014

    8/25

    these studies of cognitive style highlight its potential

    importance, it remains unclear how cognitive style

    influences the SDMP outside laboratory conditions

    in situations where multiple members of the TMT are

    involved in the SDMP, rather than just one indi-

    vidual. It is important for future research to deter-

    mine whether the cognitive styles of TMT membersinteract to reinforce a particular style, or interact

    such that a balance of styles is achieved (Hough and

    ogilvie 2005). Also, the effects of cognitive style on

    important and frequently studied SDMP characteris-

    tics, such as comprehensiveness, are unknown.

    Personality. Research has examined the effects of

    CEO personality on the characteristics of the SDMP,

    and has produced equivocal results. Miller et al.

    (1988) found that CEOs with a higher need for

    achievement (desire to attain success and accomplish

    difficult tasks) are more rational in the SDMP(increased planning, systematic scanning of the envi-

    ronment and having explicit strategies). However,

    Papadakis et al. (1998) found no significant effects

    of CEO need for achievement on any of the SDMP

    characteristics. Papadakis et al. (1998) do find,

    however, that CEO risk propensity (willingness to

    take risks) diminishes rule formalization in the

    SDMP (standardized procedures and processes), but

    does not significantly affect any other SDMP char-

    acteristic. Furthermore, Papadakis and Barwise

    (2002) were unable to find any significant effects of

    CEO need for achievement or risk propensity on anyof the characteristics of the SDMP. The contradictory

    conclusions of Milleret al. (1988) and Papadakis

    et al. (1998) concerning CEO need for achievement

    could be attributable to differences in the size of the

    organizations sampled. Milleret al. (1988) focused

    on small organizations, where the influence of the

    CEO is likely to be pervasive, and the average

    number of employees in organizations in this sample

    was 111, compared with an average number of

    employees of 730 in the organizations in the

    Papadakis et al. (1998) and Papadakis and Barwise

    (2002) studies.

    Other TMT variables. Other TMT variables fea-

    tured in the literature include polychronicity and

    aggressive philosophy. Souitaris and Maestro (2010)

    studied TMT polychronicity, which refers to the

    TMTs tendency to move focus simultaneously and

    intermittently from one task to another. The results

    show that TMT polychronicity leads to faster deci-

    sions, but the TMTs are less comprehensive in their

    SDMP. Also, an aggressive TMT philosophy (deter-

    mination to beat competition) has been found to

    result in increased SDMP comprehensiveness,

    hierarchical decentralization, rule formalization and

    lateral communication (participation of major

    departments) (Papadakis and Barwise 2002;

    Papadakis et al. 1998).Theory development has clearly been hindered by

    the fragmented nature of the studies reviewed here,

    and many have focused solely on a single SDMP

    characteristic, often comprehensiveness or rational-

    ity. Top management team tenure, education, demo-

    graphic diversity, cognitive diversity, polychronicity

    and aggressive philosophy all significantly influence

    the degree of comprehensiveness. However, the

    effects of the CEO on SDMP characteristics are

    unclear and, while the effects of TMT demographic

    variables on multiple different SDMP characteristics

    have been examined (e.g. Papadakis et al. 1998),many of the psychometric variables such as cognitive

    diversity have not been subjected to such systematic

    scrutiny. Hence, given the limited number of studies

    examining TMT variables such as cognitive diversity,

    future research may extend this line of enquiry by

    studying their effects on other important and fre-

    quently studied SDMP characteristics such as

    political behaviour, because strong disagreement

    concerning strategic issues could conceivably result

    in executives forming coalitions and using power to

    promote their own perspectives.

    The research reviewed in this section raises impor-tant questions concerning whether a focus on an indi-

    vidual, such as in the studies of cognitive style, is

    appropriate when the SDMP is more often a group

    decision process (Hambrick 2007). Therefore, to

    complement the laboratory studies on cognitive

    style, field-based research focused on teams appears

    necessary. Future research examining the influence

    of the CEO should focus on small organizations,

    where SDM power is more centralized (Milleret al.

    1988).

    Direct effects of TMT variables on SDMP outcomes

    Cognitive style. Hough and ogilvies (2005) labo-

    ratory experiment, using the MyersBriggs Type

    Indicator to measure executive cognitive style, shows

    that cognitive style influences decision quality, deci-

    siveness (the number of problems addressed) and

    perceived effectiveness (team members perceptions

    of one anothers ability to complete tasks). Accord-

    ing to Hough and ogilvies (2005) study, executives

    Context in Strategic Decision-Making 347

    2013 British Academy of Management and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

  • 8/10/2019 Environment and SDMProcess REVIEW 2014

    9/25

    who use both intuition and objective information

    make higher-quality decisions, while those who take

    the time to make socially acceptable decisions are

    indecisive and are perceived to be ineffective.

    Other TMT variables. The field experiment using

    hypothetical choices developed by Mitchell et al.(2011) found that CEO meta-cognition (reflection on

    his or her own thinking) is negatively associated with

    erratic SDs (inconsistent judgments that shape the

    direction of the firm). It follows, therefore, that

    CEOs who reflect on and attempt to control their

    cognitive processes are likely to make more consist-

    ent SDs. Also, Olsonet al. (2007b) show that cogni-

    tive diversity has a negative effect on SD quality and

    commitment. Top management teams that disagree

    about the strategic goals and priorities of the organi-

    zation, therefore, are less likely to be committed to a

    SD, and tend to make SDs that fail to achieve thedesired results. However, these effects are reduced

    for TMTs who trust one anothers ability and

    competence.

    As well, several studies have examined the effects

    of TMTs and CEOs on the speed of SDM, that is,

    how rapidly all aspects of the SDMP are executed,

    from the initial consideration of alternative courses

    of action to the final commitment to act (Forbes

    2005). Top management team polychronicity leads to

    speedy SDM (Souitaris and Maestro 2010), and

    TMT potency (TMTs perceptions of their ability to

    perform tasks effectively) is also positively related toSDM speed (Clark and Maggitti 2012). Contradic-

    tory to the studies of CEO demographic variables

    that found largely non-significant effects, Wally and

    Baum (1994) set up a field-based experiment that

    used hypothetical choices, and found that CEO tol-

    erance for risk, cognitive ability, use of intuition and

    propensity to act are all positively associated with

    SDM speed. Furthermore, Forbes (2005) revealed

    that younger executives (in their twenties and early

    thirties) of new ventures make slower SDs than their

    older counterparts. Hence, both TMT and CEO vari-

    ables appear to be significant predictors of SD speed,

    although the mediating processes that convert

    these variables into outcomes (such as speed) remain

    unexplained.

    TMT summary

    This section has revealed the very fragmented nature

    of the findings from studies that examine the influ-

    ence of the TMT on the SDMP. Theory development

    has been hindered because each TMT variable

    usually features in no more than one or two studies,

    and most studies focus on the effects of TMT vari-

    ables on only one SDMP characteristic, often com-

    prehensiveness or rationality. These problems are

    compounded by the omission of mediating decision

    processes, and variance in the level of analysisadopted, with some at the organization level, some at

    the individual and others at the decision level. While

    studies reviewed here each make a valuable contri-

    bution individually, a coherent body of theory has not

    developed, and overall implications for theory and

    practice remain unclear. The major conceptual and

    methodological issues arising from this section are

    each now discussed in further detail.

    A significant conceptual issue for SDMP research

    is the extent to which, relative to other contextual

    factors, the TMT influences SDMP characteristics.

    Yet few studies examine the influence of TMT vari-ables alongside other categories of contextual vari-

    ables. Studies examining the relative influence of

    multiple categories of contextual variables either

    show that the TMT and CEO have less influence on

    SDMP characteristics (Papadakis and Barwise 2002)

    or omit the TMT as a category as in the case of

    Elbanna and Child 2007b), who examined the rela-

    tive influence of the external environment, firm

    characteristics and SD-specific characteristics on

    rationality. Determining the relative importance of

    the TMT for explaining the SDMP compared with

    SD-specific characteristics, the external environ-ment, and firm characteristics should be a priority for

    future research so as to identify the most pertinent

    theories for SDMP research.

    The use of demographic variables as proxies for

    the underlying cognitive and behavioural traits of the

    TMT has attracted criticism, and authors have ques-

    tioned how reliably these demographic proxies actu-

    ally represent the traits that they are purported to

    (Milleret al. 1998; Priem et al. 1999). Despite the

    difficulty of obtaining psychometric data from

    TMTs, avoiding the use of demographic variables

    in future research is recommended. Instead, to attain

    greater construct validity and to improve the

    explanatory ability of research, direct measures of

    the constructs of interest should be obtained.

    Another conceptual issue is that no existing

    studies examine the moderating effects of the TMT

    on the relationship between SDMP characteristics

    and SDMP outcomes. This gap is notable, given the

    empirical evidence showing how other contextual

    variables interact with SDMP characteristics to

    348 N.G. Shepherd and J.M. Rudd

    2013 British Academy of Management and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

  • 8/10/2019 Environment and SDMProcess REVIEW 2014

    10/25

    influence SDMP outcomes (Elbanna and Child

    2007a). It is highly likely that the effects of SDMP

    characteristics on SDMP outcomes are subject to the

    influence of TMT variables (Papadakis et al. 2010).

    For example, the potential for intuitive SDMPs to

    positively influence SDMP outcomes may rest upon

    the expertise of the TMT (Khatri and Ng 2000).A final conceptual issue is omission of the medi-

    ating SDMP characteristics in studies that specify

    direct effects of the TMT on SDMP outcomes. It is

    fundamentally important to measure actual mediat-

    ing decision processes to fully understand the

    causal relationships between contextual vari-

    ables, SDMP characteristics and SDMP outcomes.

    Studies specifying direct effects of the TMT on

    SDMP outcomes do not account for the actual pro-

    cesses that convert TMT characteristics into SDMP

    outcomes.

    Methodological issues include the need for futureresearch to examine the influence of TMTs, rather

    than just CEOs, to increase the likelihood of obtain-

    ing significant results (Hambrick 2007; Papadakis

    and Barwise 2002). Research focused on CEOs

    should select samples of small organizations where

    SDM power is more centralized. Cognitive style

    appears to be an important construct for explaining

    SDMP characteristics and outcomes, but existing

    studies are laboratory-based and use an individual

    level of analysis. Field-based studies focused on the

    cognitive styles of teams would shed further light on

    its influence.

    SD-specific characteristics

    Strategic decision-specific characteristics are the

    labels and categories that decision-makers attribute

    to an SD, based on perceptions of stimuli

    (Papadakis et al. 1998). Empirical evidence demon-

    strates the significant effects that SD-specific char-

    acteristics have on SDMP characteristics, relative to

    the effects of other contextual variables (Elbanna

    and Child 2007b; Hickson et al. 1986; Papadakis

    et al. 1998). In this section, we review studies

    of SD-specific characteristics that have featured

    decision matter, uncertainty, motive, importance

    and time pressure (see Figure 1 and Table 2).

    Studies focusing on the direct effects of SD-specific

    characteristics on SDMP characteristics are

    reviewed first, followed by a review of the literature

    examining the moderating effects of SD-specific

    characteristics.

    Direct effects of SD-specific characteristics on

    SDMP characteristics

    Decision matter. One of the major conclusions of

    the seminal Bradford Studies is that the complexity

    and politicality inherent in an SD determine the

    process by which it is made (Hickson et al. 2001).

    Three types of decision matter are identified

    vortex, tractable and familiar that have differing

    levels of complexity and politicality (Astley et al.

    1982). Each of the three different decision matters

    was found to lead to a different decision process,

    characterized by varying levels of scrutiny, nego-

    tiation, discontinuity, centralization and duration.

    However, while the Hicksonet al. (1986) categoriza-

    tion conceptualizes politicality as being an anteced-

    ent, other studies have modelled politicality as a

    characteristic to describe the actual SDMP (e.g.

    Dean and Sharfman 1996; Eisenhardt and Bourgeois1988; Elbanna and Child 2007a; Papadakis et al.

    1998). To ensure sufficient discriminant validity

    between focal constructs, future research should

    specify precisely whether politicality is a contextual

    antecedent or a characteristic of the SDMP.

    SD uncertainty. This diminishes rationality (Dean

    and Sharfman 1993) and rule formalization

    (Papadakis et al. 1998), but promotes flexibility

    (Sharfman and Dean 1997a), politicization and

    problem-solving dissension (Papadakis et al. 1998).

    Thus, when there is uncertainty concerning theactions that should be taken or the information

    required to make a decision (Sonenshein 2007),

    decision-makers are less inclined to gather and

    analyse information (Dean and Sharfman 1993).

    When making uncertain SDs, as well, coalitions are

    formed, and bargaining takes place to overcome

    internal resistance and disagreement (Papadakis

    et al. 1998). However, decision-makers have also

    been found to respond to SD uncertainty by being

    open to new sources of information (Sharfman and

    Dean 1997a).

    SD motive. This refers to whether the SD is made in

    response to an opportunity or a threat. Papadakis et al.

    (1998) discovered that, when the SD is a response to a

    threat, the SDMP is characterized by hierarchical

    decentralization, as middle management become

    involved to a greater extent. However, they found no

    other significant effects of SD motive. Fredricksons

    (1985) laboratory experiment showed that, while the

    MBA sample were more comprehensive when faced

    Context in Strategic Decision-Making 349

    2013 British Academy of Management and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

  • 8/10/2019 Environment and SDMProcess REVIEW 2014

    11/25

  • 8/10/2019 Environment and SDMProcess REVIEW 2014

    12/25

    with a problem than when faced with an opportunity,

    no significant effects were identified in the executive

    sample. Similarly, Ashmoset al. (1998) were unable

    to find any significant effects of SD motive on the

    level of participation in the SDMP (the number of

    people involved and the extent of their involve-

    ment). Therefore, taken as a whole, these collectiveresults demonstrate only moderate support for SD

    motive being a significant influence on SDMP

    characteristics.

    SD importance. Papadakiset al. (1998) studied the

    magnitude of impact of an SD, and discovered that,

    when the SD is expected to have profound and per-

    vasive implications for the organization and its

    operations, decision-makers engage in comprehen-

    sive SDMPs, using financial information, involving

    management from multiple levels and communicat-

    ing across functions. Contrary to this, Dean andSharfman (1993) revealed that decision importance

    was not significantly related to rationality. Hence,

    whether or not the decision is critical to the organi-

    zations future does not appear to determine the

    extent of information gathering and analysis, but

    does influence comprehensiveness, as well as the use

    of financial reporting information, hierarchical

    decentralization and lateral communication.

    Time pressure. This leads to problem-solving

    dissension in the SDMP, and reduces hierarchi-

    cal decentralization and lateral communication(Papadakis et al. 1998). Thus, when the SD is under

    time pressure, there will be dissent, less involvement

    of middle managers and restricted communication

    across departments.

    Overall, empirical evidence supports the view that

    SD-specific characteristics have a significant influ-

    ence on the characteristics of the SDMP (Elbanna

    and Child 2007b; Papadakis et al. 1998). This has

    important implications for studies adopting an

    organizational level of analysis, which assumes that

    organizations have SDMPs that are consistent

    from one decision to the next. Also, the effects of

    SD-specific characteristics appear to be nuanced. For

    example, the magnitude of impact of the SD is posi-

    tively related to comprehensiveness (Papadakiset al.

    1998), but the importance of the SD is not related to

    rationality (Dean and Sharfman 1993). Such contra-

    dictory results highlight the need for the careful

    and consistent definition and operationalization of

    not only SD-specific characteristics, but also SDMP

    characteristics.

    Moderating effects of SD-specific characteristics

    on the relationship between SDMP characteristics

    and SDMP outcomes

    Operationalizing rationality as the collection and

    analysis of information, Elbanna and Child (2007a)

    conclude that the positive relationship between

    rationality and SD effectiveness (the extent to which

    the decision achieves its objectives and avoids nega-

    tive unintended consequences) is stronger for SDs

    perceived as crises than for opportunities. Also, the

    negative relationship between political behaviour and

    SD effectiveness is stronger for SDs perceived as

    crises than for opportunities. This evidence suggests

    that rational SDMPs work best when faced with a

    crisis and that, in such situations, political behaviour

    will be even more detrimental to the effectiveness of

    the SD. Finally, the authors determine that SD uncer-

    tainty moderates the effects of rationality on SDeffectiveness, such that the positive influence of

    rationality on SD effectiveness is weaker for low

    uncertainty SDs than for high uncertainty SDs.

    Therefore, when decision-makers are uncertain

    about the information required and the likely out-

    comes, rational processes are more likely to produce

    effective SDs.

    SD-specific characteristics summary

    Empirical evidence suggests that SD-specific charac-

    teristics may be one of the most significant influences

    on SDMP characteristics, relative to other contextualvariables (Elbanna and Child 2007b; Hickson

    et al. 1986; Papadakis et al. 1998). Despite this,

    SD-specific characteristics are featured in only a

    handful of studies, and only one empirical study spe-

    cifically addresses their moderating influence on the

    relationship between SD-specific characteristics and

    outcomes. Hence, it is doubtful that the findings of

    the studies reviewed here are generalizable. Also, it

    remains unclear why, for example, the magnitude of

    impact from an SD and SD importance each has

    different implications for comprehensiveness and

    rationality (Dean and Sharfman 1993; Papadakis

    et al. 1998).

    It is also apparent that many of the SD-specific

    characteristics used in the studies reviewed here

    suffer from low reliabilities, and several studies have

    reported Cronbach alphas of between 0.54 and 0.63

    (e.g. Elbanna and Child 2007a; Dean and Sharfman

    1993; Papadakis et al. 1998). Low reliabilities indi-

    cate high levels of random error in the measures,

    which reduces the probability of detecting significant

    Context in Strategic Decision-Making 351

    2013 British Academy of Management and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

  • 8/10/2019 Environment and SDMProcess REVIEW 2014

    13/25

    relationships (Bagozzi 1994). Therefore, research

    focusing on SD-specific characteristics will benefit

    from measure development work to improve their

    reliabilities.

    Given the significant interactions detected in

    Elbanna and Childs (2007a) study and the rela-

    tive importance of SD-specific characteristics asantecedents of SDMP characteristics, a systematic

    programme of research examining SD-specific char-

    acteristics is warranted. Such research should model

    SD-specific characteristics as antecedents of differ-

    ent SDMP characteristics, and as moderators of the

    relationships between SDMP characteristics and

    SDMP outcomes, which would significantly help to

    develop a more coherent body of theory.

    The external environment

    The external environment is the third category of

    contextual variables, and in this section we first

    review those empirical studies that examine the

    direct effects of the external environment on SDMP

    characteristics. Second, we review studies that test

    the direct effects on SDMP outcomes and, finally, we

    review those that feature the moderating effects of

    the external environment on the relationship between

    SDMP characteristics and SDMP outcomes. The

    dimensions of the external environment reviewed in

    this section include environmental hostility, velocity,

    dynamism, instability, munificence and uncertainty(see Figure 1 and Table 3).

    Direct effects of the external environment on

    SDMP characteristics

    Hostile environments. Hostile environments are

    threatening and dangerous, making it difficult to

    stay afloat (Miller and Friesen 1983), and studies

    have produced confounding results. Dean and

    Sharfman (1993) and Sharfman and Dean (1997a)

    conclude that in such environments, decision-

    makers are less likely to gather and analyse infor-

    mation, and are less open to new information.

    However, Papadakis et al. (1998) were unable to

    detect significant effects of environmental hostility

    on any of the SDMP characteristics. Aside from

    being contradictory, the results of these studies are

    unexpected, given that in such environments the

    implications of making an ineffective SD are severe

    and, hence, more rational or comprehensive SDMPs

    would be expected.

    High-velocity, dynamic and unstable environ-

    ments. High-velocity environments are character-

    ized by rapid and discontinuous change in demand,

    competitors, technology and/or regulation, such that

    information is often inaccurate, unavailable, or

    obsolete (Bourgeois and Eisenhardt 1988, p. 816).

    Eisenhardt (1989) uses eight case studies of firms inthe high-velocity micro-computer industry to high-

    light how organizations use real-time information,

    and simultaneously consider multiple SD alterna-

    tives in such environments. Dynamic environments

    are very similar to high-velocity environments

    (Baum and Wally 2003) and are characterized by a

    highly unpredictable and unstable rate of change

    and high levels of uncertainty about the state of

    context, the meansends relationships, and/or the

    outcomes of the actions (Mitchell et al. 2011, pp.

    687688). However, Papadakis et al. (1998) is the

    only empirical study to examine the direct effects ofenvironmental dynamism on SDMP characteristics,

    and this study produced no significant effects. Envi-

    ronmental instability, defined as the extent to which

    market demand and technology are rapidly changing

    in a given industry (Dean and Sharfman 1996, p.

    376), has been found to cause TMT members to

    exhibit more agreement about the comprehensive-

    ness (the actual process was not reported) of the

    SDMP than in stable environments (Iaquinto and

    Fredrickson 1997). Hence, in unstable environments,

    executives are more likely to focus on the SDMP and

    not become distracted by disagreements and debates.Based on the studies reviewed here, it may be true

    that the external environment is a less significant

    influence on SDMP characteristics compared with

    the other categories of contextual variables. Cer-

    tainly, the lack of significant findings in Papadakis

    et al. (1998) for both environmental hostility and

    dynamism raises doubts over the veracity of the envi-

    ronmental determinism perspective (Aldrich 1979;

    Hannan and Freeman 1977). Also, the work of

    Elbanna and Child (2007b) challenges the signifi-

    cance of the external environment as a means for

    explaining the characteristics of the SDMP; the

    authors determine that SD-specific characteristics

    and firm characteristics are more significant predic-

    tors of rationality than the external environment is.

    Direct effects of the external environment on

    SDMP outcomes

    Environmental dynamism. Mitchell et al. (2011)

    use a field experiment with hypothetical choices to

    352 N.G. Shepherd and J.M. Rudd

    2013 British Academy of Management and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

  • 8/10/2019 Environment and SDMProcess REVIEW 2014

    14/25

  • 8/10/2019 Environment and SDMProcess REVIEW 2014

    15/25

  • 8/10/2019 Environment and SDMProcess REVIEW 2014

    16/25

    determine that environmental dynamism causes

    CEOs to make fewer erratic SDs, that environmental

    hostility causes CEOs to make more erratic SDs, and

    that dynamism and hostility interact to cause CEOs

    to make fewer erratic SDs. Thus, not only do envi-

    ronmental dynamism and hostility exert different

    effects, they interact to influence the outcomes of theSDMP. Baum and Wally (2003) also use a field

    experiment with hypothetical choices, and find that

    environmental dynamism and environmental munifi-

    cence (capacity to support growth) both cause CEOs

    to make faster SDs. Both the Mitchell et al. (2011)

    and Baum and Wally (2003) studies omit mediating

    decision processes, which leaves unexplained the

    actual decision processes that mediate the effects of

    the external environment on decision speed and

    whether the SD is erratic.

    Moderating effects of the external environment on

    the relationship between SDMP characteristics and

    SDMP outcomes

    High-velocity, dynamic and unstable environments.

    A common theme in the literature has been to iden-

    tify interactions between the external environment

    and SDMP characteristics, and their influence on

    SDMP outcomes. Several studies have shown that in

    high-velocity or dynamic environments, rational and

    comprehensive SDMPs are positively associated

    with organizational performance (Bourgeois and

    Eisenhardt 1988; Eisenhardt 1989; Priem et al.1995). The basic tenet of these studies is that the

    uncertainty inherent in such environments can be

    mitigated by comprehensively gathering and analys-

    ing information, and developing and evaluating mul-

    tiple different decision options. However, there is

    also empirical evidence to suggest that the opposite

    is true, and that in such environments rational or

    comprehensive approaches to SDM are of little

    value, because information is unavailable, is incom-

    plete or becomes obsolete rapidly. The field-based

    experiments by Fredrickson and colleagues that use

    hypothetical scenarios with executives show that, in

    unstable environments, comprehensive SDMPs have

    a negative effect on organizational performance.

    These findings are also supported by Hough and

    White (2003), who use a laboratory simulation to

    show how rational SDMPs have no effect on decision

    quality in dynamic environments. It should also be

    noted, however, that no significant interactions were

    found between rationality and environmental insta-

    bility (Dean and Sharfman 1996) and rationality and

    environmental uncertainty (Elbanna and Child

    2007a).

    There is a clear tension in thefindings of Eisenhardt

    and colleagues studies, as compared with those of

    Fredrickson and colleagues. This may be explained

    by the sharp and discontinuous change (Bourgeois

    and Eisenhardt 1988, p. 816) that distinguishes high-velocity environments from the unstable environ-

    ments studied by Fredrickson and colleagues.Also, in

    research that has revealed positive effects of SDMP

    comprehensiveness in high-velocity environments, a

    common theme is the importance of SD speed.

    Eisenhardt (1989) highlights the ways in which

    decision-makers consider multiple decision options

    simultaneously, using real-time information to make

    speedy SDs and to achieve superior organizational

    performance. Similarly, Judge and Miller (1991) also

    determine SD speed to be positively related to

    organizational performance though only in high-velocity environments, and not in medium- or low-

    velocity environments. A tentative conclusion may be

    reached that an organizations ability to engage in

    SDMPs that are both comprehensive and fast leads to

    high levels of organizational performance in rapidly

    changing environments.

    Environmental munificence. Other studies have

    focused on how environmental munificence interacts

    with rationality to influence SDMP outcomes, and

    again no clear consensus exists. While Goll and

    Rasheed (2005) show that rationality leads to highlevels of organizational performance in munificent

    environments, Elbanna and Child (2007a) report that

    rationality is most likely to result in an effective SD

    in environments with low levels of munificence

    where few opportunities for growth exist.

    Empirical evidence also suggests that interactions

    between the external environment and SDMP char-

    acteristics may be even more complex than most

    existing research allows for. Goll and Rasheed

    (1997) show that not only does rationality benefit

    organizational performance in dynamic and in

    munificent environments, but the positive effects of

    rationality on performance are strongest in environ-

    ments high in both dynamism and munificence.

    Environmental uncertainty. Atuahene-Gima and Li

    (2004) adopt a sophisticated approach to modelling

    the external environment to examine two types of

    uncertainty: demand uncertainty and technology

    uncertainty. Their results indicate that comprehen-

    sive SDMPs benefit new product performance and

    Context in Strategic Decision-Making 355

    2013 British Academy of Management and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

  • 8/10/2019 Environment and SDMProcess REVIEW 2014

    17/25

    quality in the presence of demand uncertainty,

    because organizations are able to mitigate such

    uncertainty by searching for and analysing informa-

    tion relating to customer demand and preferences.

    However, in the presence of technology uncertainty,

    comprehensive SDMPs are of little benefit, as

    information relating to technology uncertaintyis dense, highly equivocal and difficult to collect

    (Atuahene-Gima and Li 2004). These findings point

    to a more nuanced interaction between SDMP char-

    acteristics and the external environment than most

    studies have modelled.

    Other interaction effects involving the external

    environment. Studies have also examined how the

    external environment interacts with other SDMP

    characteristics such as political behaviour and intui-

    tion. Bourgeois and Eisenhardt (1988) reveal that

    political behaviour causes poor organizational per-formance in high-velocity environments, because it

    distracts executives and causes delays (Eisenhardt

    and Bourgeois 1988). Also, Khatri and Ng (2000)

    studied intuitive synthesis (reliance on judgment,

    experience, and gut feelings) in the SDMP and

    found a positive relationship with organizational

    performance in unstable environments, but they

    observed a negative relationship in stable environ-

    ments. These results suggest that intuitive synthesis

    can help to overcome the limitations of rational and

    comprehensive SDMPs in unstable environments,

    perhaps because of its speed and ability to synthesizeinformation, but intuitive synthesis needs to be used

    cautiously in stable environments (Khatri and Ng

    2000).

    External environment summary

    Theory development has been hindered by the

    absence of a single approach to conceptualizing and

    measuring the external environment (Sharfman and

    Dean 1991). Studies have used environmental veloc-

    ity, instability and dynamism to capture the extent to

    which the external environment is subject to rapid

    and unpredictable change, which has produced

    conflicting results concerning the effects of com-

    prehensiveness on performance. Similarly, studies

    examining the implications of rationality in munifi-

    cent environments have produced contradictory

    results. Also, relative to other contextual variables,

    the external environment appears to have limited

    direct effects on SDMP characteristics (Elbanna and

    Child 2007b; Papadakis et al. 1998), and its influ-

    ence seems to be primarily a moderating one, influ-

    encing the relationship between SDMP charac-

    teristics and outcomes. In part, the contradictory

    findings of the studies reviewed in this section

    may be caused by some studies adopting an

    organizational level of analysis and others focusing

    on the decision level. Matters have been complicatedby differences in the SDMP characteristics used,

    with some examining rationality and others compre-

    hensiveness, and with the varying way in which these

    constructs are operationalized.

    There is a clear need to reconcile the conflicting

    results of the Eisenhardt and Fredrickson studies,

    concerning the relationship between comprehen-

    siveness and organizational performance. Such

    research should focus on the implications of com-

    prehensiveness and SD speed combined, under dif-

    ferent environmental conditions, because, despite

    the apparent importance of speed in fast-changingenvironments (Eisenhardt 1989), most studies omit

    it. Also, future research may try to reconcile the

    inconsistencies of other studies, such as those con-

    cerning the implications of rationality in munificent

    environments (Elbanna and Child 2007a; Goll and

    Rasheed 2005) through measurement of multiple

    different dimensions of the external environment

    within a single study. This would enable the detec-

    tion of three-way interactions shown to be impor-

    tant by Goll and Rasheed (1997). Furthermore,

    future research may also benefit from careful atten-

    tion to modelling the external environment. Theapproach adopted by Atuahene-Gima and Li (2004)

    is rare, in that it examines the effects of alternative

    types of environmental uncertainty (technology

    and demand) on the relationship between com-

    prehensiveness and SDMP outcomes. Therefore, a

    focus on the different types of uncertainty, instabil-

    ity, dynamism and velocity, such as customers,

    competitors and technology, may provide fresh

    insights.

    Firm characteristics

    In this section, we review the literature examining the

    direct effects of firm characteristics on SDMP char-

    acteristics and outcomes. Also, we review studies

    that examine the moderating effects of firm charac-

    teristics on the relationship between SDMP charac-

    teristics and outcomes. Firm characteristics featured

    in this section include power centralization, struc-

    ture, size, performance, slack resources, external

    356 N.G. Shepherd and J.M. Rudd

    2013 British Academy of Management and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

  • 8/10/2019 Environment and SDMProcess REVIEW 2014

    18/25

    control, corporate control and planning formality

    (see Figure 1 and Table 4).

    Direct effects of firm characteristics on

    SDMP characteristics

    Power centralization. Power centralized in thehands of the CEO leads to political behaviour

    (Bourgeois and Eisenhardt 1988; Eisenhardt and

    Bourgeois 1988). Hence, when the CEO does not

    distribute SDM power among members of the TMT,

    team members form coalitions, pursue their own

    agendas and, overall, act in a way that is destructive

    to SDs (Bourgeois and Eisenhardt 1988).

    Structure. Structure influences participation and

    rationality in the SDMP. Ashmos et al. (1998) show

    that organizations with fewer rules and standardized

    operating procedures have greater participation ofmultiple different internal stakeholders in the SDMP,

    whereas, in organizations with formalized rules, the

    SDMP is consistent and formulaic, with the same

    small number of individuals involved. Furthermore,

    Miller (1987) concludes that organizations with for-

    malized systems and processes that use task forces

    and committees will also favour rational SDMPs. For

    the study, Miller (1987) adopts an organizational

    level of analysis, and defines rationality as analysis,

    systematic scanning of the environment and having

    explicit strategies.

    Size. Size is commonly operationalized as the

    number of full-time employees, and empirical evi-

    dence shows that certain SDMP characteristics vary

    according to the size of the organization. Large

    organizations are more comprehensive in their

    SDMPs than small organizations (Fredrickson and

    Iaquinto 1989; Papadakis et al. 1998), although the

    effects of size on other SDMP characteristics are less

    clear. Iaquinto and Fredrickson (1997) report that as

    firms increase in size, TMT agreement about the

    comprehensiveness of the SDMP diminishes which

    implies that levels of debate and disagreement

    regarding the SDMP will increase in large organiza-

    tions. However, Papadakis et al. (1998) found no

    association between size and politicization. Simi-

    larly, Dean and Sharfman (1993) were unable to

    detect any relationship between size and rationality.

    Overall, these findings suggest that, when firms

    increase in size, so too does the comprehensiveness

    of their SDMPs, but the implications of size for

    rationality and political behaviour remain unclear.

    Performance. Another line of empirical enquiry

    has been to examine the effects of organizational

    performance on SDMP characteristics, although

    confounding results have emerged. Papadakis et al.

    (1998) used two measures of organizational perfor-

    mance and found that organizations generating a

    high return on assets tend to have SDMPs that arecharacterized by comprehensiveness, the use of

    financial reporting information and hierarchical

    decentralization, whereas the SDMPs of organiza-

    tions experiencing profit growth are more political.

    Papadakis et al. (1998) suggest that high levels of

    organizational performance endow organizations

    with resources to invest in comprehensive and

    decentralized SDMPs that make greater use of

    financial reporting information. However, perfor-

    mance also provokes politics and conflict over how

    best to use these resources. Fredricksons (1985)

    laboratory experiment was unable to reach a con-clusion concerning the effects of performance on

    comprehensiveness. MBA students were more com-

    prehensive when organizational performance was

    poor compared with when performance was excel-

    lent, whereas the sample of executives exhibited no

    differences according to performance. Also,

    Iaquinto and Fredrickson (1997) found no signifi-

    cant effect of performance on TMT agreement

    about comprehensiveness. Finally, Sharfman and

    Dean (1997a) investigated the effects of slack

    resources (resources intentionally kept beyond those

    needed to meet operational commitments) on theflexibility of the SDMP. Organizations with high

    levels of slack resources were found to be open

    to new ideas, to new sources of information, and

    to decision-makers adopting new roles in the

    SDMP.

    Other firm characteristics. Other firm characteris-

    tics affecting SDMP characteristics include the

    degree of external control, type of corporate control

    and planning formality. When the SDMP is subject to

    the influence of individuals external to the organiza-

    tion, the SDMP becomes less rational (Dean and

    Sharfman 1993), thus highlighting the importance of

    managerial discretion for rationality. Papadakiset al.

    (1998) examined the type of corporate control and

    found that the SDMPs of state-controlled organiza-

    tions are comprehensive and political, whereas pri-

    vately owned organizations are less comprehensive,

    and rely less on financial reporting and formalized

    rules. Also, organizations with formal planning

    systems have greater lateral communication, and are

    Context in Strategic Decision-Making 357

    2013 British Academy of Management and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

  • 8/10/2019 Environment and SDMProcess REVIEW 2014

    19/25

  • 8/10/2019 Environment and SDMProcess REVIEW 2014

    20/25

    more comprehensive and political (Papadakis et al.

    1998).

    From this section of the review it is possible to

    reach a number of tentative conclusions: (1) Power

    centralization invokes political behaviour; (2) As

    organizations increase in size, their SDMPs become

    more comprehensive; (3) Organization structureinfluences rationality, as well as who participates in

    the SDMP, and; (4) Ownership, external control

    and formal planning influence comprehensiveness,

    rationality and political behaviour.

    One particularly ambiguous area concerns the

    effects of organizational performance on the SDMP,

    with Papadakis et al. (1998) concluding that return

    on assets and growth in profits each affect diff-

    erent SDMP characteristics. However, adopting

    measures of performance such as return on assets

    and growth in profits may not reliably reflect the

    actual levels of resources available for use by theorganization. For example, an organization could

    be highly profitable, but also highly geared, with

    significant annual cash outflows to repay its debt.

    Such cash outflows would not be reflected in meas-

    ures of performance such as profit growth, but

    would result in the organizations being resource

    constrained. Furthermore, performance is unlikely

    to be a valid proxy for other types of slack

    resources (such as human resource slack) (Voss

    et al. 2008).

    Direct effects of firm characteristics on

    SDMP outcomes

    Studies have examined the direct effects of firm char-

    acteristics on SD speed. While Eisenhardts (1989)

    case studies do not reveal any link between power

    centralization and SD speed, Wally and Baum (1994)

    and Baum and Wally (2003) both demonstrated that,

    when authority for SDM is centralized, SD speed

    increases.

    Moderating effects of firm characteristics on therelationship between SDMP characteristics and

    SDMP outcomes

    Covinet al. (2001) identified an interaction between

    intuitive and technocratic SDMPs, organization

    structure and the external environment, which influ-

    ences organization performance. In high-technology

    environments, intuitive SDMPs lead to high levels of

    organization performance among firms with organic

    structures (characterized by open communication

    and few rules and procedures). Covin et al. (2001)

    also determined that in low-technology environ-

    ments, technocratic SDMPs (using systematic

    and quantitative analysis) lead to high levels of

    organizational performance among firms with

    organic structures. Thus, organizations need to

    ensure that their SDMPs and organizational struc-tures are configured to best fit their external environ-

    ments (Covin et al. 2001).

    Elbanna and Child (2007a) found that the posi-

    tive influence of rationality on SD effectiveness is

    moderated by organizational performance (using a

    perceptual measure of both financial and non-

    financial organizational performance), such that in

    high-performing organizations the influence is

    weaker. Additionally, the negative influences of

    intuition and political behaviour on SD effective-

    ness are weaker for companies with high perfor-

    mance. While there is scant other empiricalevidence with which to compare these findings, it

    does highlight a view that the effects of SDMP

    characteristics on SD effectiveness are subject to

    the influence of firm characteristics.

    Firm characteristics summary

    Firm characteristics exert a significant influence on

    SDMP characteristics relative to other contextual

    variables (Elbanna and Child 2007b). For instance,

    power centralization affects political behaviour,

    structure influences rationality and participation, andsize is significantly related to comprehensiveness.

    External control, corporate control and formal plan-

    ning also influence rationality, comprehensiveness

    and political behaviour. However, organizational per-

    formance has produced confounding results, with

    return on assets and growth in profits each influenc-

    ing different SDMP characteristics. Careful theory

    development is required to disentangle the differing

    effects of alternative measures of organizational per-

    formance (Papadakis et al. 1998). Because perfor-

    mance has been used as a proxy for slack resources,

    future research should consider using direct meas-

    ures of slack resources to examine its effects on

    SDMP characteristics, as well as its moderating

    influence on the relationship between SDMP charac-

    teristics and SDMP outcomes. Additionally, future

    research should examine the moderating effects

    of other firm characteristics and, in particular,

    should consider the possibility of three-way interac-

    tions among firm characteristics and contextual

    variables such as the external environment that

    Context in Strategic Decision-Making 359

    2013 British Academy of Management and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

  • 8/10/2019 Environment and SDMProcess REVIEW 2014

    21/25

    are highlighted by Covin et al. (2001) as hav-

    ing significant implications for organizational

    performance.

    Despite calls in the literature for such research

    (e.g. Eisenhardt 1989; Molloy and Schwenk 1995), it

    is evident that no studies have examined firm capa-

    bilities such as real-time information systems. Theabsence of such research is particularly noteworthy,

    given that the ability to make comprehensive and fast

    SDs is vital to achieve superior levels of performance

    in high-velocity environments (Eisenhardt 1989).

    Such capabilities may rest upon the information

    systems and technologies of the firm.

    Conclusions

    This review has highlighted the underlying themes,

    issues, tensions and debates in the SDMP literatureregarding the direct and moderating influences of

    context. In this section, we discuss the priorities for

    future theory development. These include the need

    for fewer constructs and for more careful modelling,

    as well as the importance of examining multi-

    theoretic models and the moderating effects of con-

    textual variables. We also address the methodological

    implications, which include the importance of large

    samples and multivariate analysis, measurement reli-

    ability, issues surrounding levels of analysis, and

    qualitative research designs.

    Priorities for future theory development

    The need for fewer constructs and for more careful

    modelling. An incremental approach to theory

    building has resulted in a proliferation of constructs,

    especially SDMP characteristics, and there is con-

    sensus in the literature that too much invention of

    language (Bower 1997, p. 27) has hindered theory

    development (Papadakis and Barwise 1997). This

    review has identified significant differences in the

    definitions and operationalizations of rationality and

    comprehensiveness, and they are frequently used

    interchangeably (Goll and Rasheed 1997, 2005;

    Papadakis and Barwise 2002; Papadakiset al. 1998;

    Priemet al. 1995). This is despite evidence that con-

    textual variables such as SD importance and firm size

    affect rationality and comprehensiveness differently.

    To address problems with the overabundance of

    SDMP characteristics, Elbanna (2006) and Elbanna

    and Child (2007a) highlight how rationality, compre-

    hensiveness, intuition and political behaviour are a

    parsimonious set of constructs that adequately repre-

    sent two perspectives underpinning the SDMP litera-

    ture, namely synoptic-formalism and incremen-

    talism. Synoptic formalism stresses formal analysis

    in the SDMP, and incrementalism views SDMPs as

    subject to behavioural influences such as intuition

    and politics (Eisenhardt and Zbaracki 1992;

    Fredrickson and Mitchell 1984; Hart 1992; Hitt andTyler 1991). Elbanna (2006) and Elbanna and Child

    (2007a) suggest that rationality and comprehensive-

    ness are constructs that represent the synoptic formal

    perspective, and political behaviour and intuition are

    constructs that embody the incremental perspective.

    Certainly, a common terminology and consistency in

    the variables modelled would support the develop-

    ment of a more coherent body of theory (Papadakis

    and Barwise 1997). Furthermore, future research

    should precisely define and operationalize rationality

    and comprehensiveness, and should have a clear

    theoretical rationale for including one or the other.Including both in one study would enable researchers

    to discern the relative influence of context on each of

    them.

    Lack of replication. Lack of replication has hin-

    dered the coherent development of theory. Future

    research should seek to replicate, or at least control

    for, some of the significant variables and relation-

    ships identified in this review. For example, decision

    speed has significant implications for perfor-

    mance (Eisenhardt 1989; Judge and Miller 1991);

    despite this, most studies omit it. Similarly, studiesattempting to explain SDMP outcomes under certain

    environmental conditions have focused on com-

    prehensiveness or rationality. However, there is

    considerable empirical evidence that SDMPs are

    multi-dimensional, and SDMP outcomes are subject

    to the influence of SDMP characteristics other than

    just rationality, such as political behaviour (Dean and

    Sharfman 1996). Closer attention to control variables

    would lead to greater implicit replication.

    Direct measures. Using demographics as proxies

    for the underlying psychometric characteristics of

    the TMT has received strong criticism, and Priem

    et al. (1999) suggest that this approach sacrifices

    construct validity for measurement reliability, and

    forsakes explanation for prediction. Similarly, this

    review identifies organizational performance being

    used as a proxy for slack resources. To improve

    methodological rigour and to develop theory with

    greater explanatory power, future research should

    use direct measures of contextual variables.

    360 N.G. Shepherd and J.M. Rudd

    2013 British Academy of Management and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

  • 8/10/2019 Environment and SDMProcess REVIEW 2014

    22/25

    Mediating decision processes. Researchers should

    include measures for actual decision processes. For

    example, examining the effects of cognitive style on

    decision outcomes neglects the mediating role of the

    actual decision process. Because a decision-maker

    has a rational cognitive style, it does not necessarily

    follow that the actual SDMP will be rational; othercontextual variables may have a more significant

    effect in determining the decision process (Bakken

    and Haerem 2011). It is fundamentally important to

    measure actual mediating decision processes to fully

    understand the causal relationships between context,

    SDMP process characteristics and SDMP outcomes.

    The need for multi-theoretic models

    Some authors (e.g. Elbanna and Child 2007b; Hitt

    and Tyler 1991; Papadakis et al. 1998) have associ-

    ated each of the four categories of contextual vari-

    ables with a theoretical perspective. For example, the

    TMT is associated with the upper echelons perspec-

    tive (Hambrick and Mason 1984), and the external

    environment is associated with the environmental

    determinism perspective (Aldrich 1979; Hannan and

    Freeman 1977). By including contextual variables

    from each of the four categories, researchers can

    compare the overall impact and predictive power of

    each of these theoretical perspectives for explaining

    variance in SDMP characteristics. While most

    studies have focused on one category of contextual

    variable, there is no theoretical rationale, as yet, topropose that any category is more, or less, significant

    than any other (Rajagopalan et al. 1993).

    The moderating role of contextual variables

    With the exception of the external environment, there

    is scant empirical research examining the moderating

    effects of other contextual variables on the relation-

    ship between SDMP characteristics and outcomes,

    and even research examining the moderating effects

    of the external environment has produced conflicting

    results. Furthermore, TMT variables are yet to be

    featured as moderators. Models that incorporate bothSDMP characteristics and contextual variables as

    moderators can predict more variance in outcome

    variables, because the limited empirical evidence

    that exists has shown such interactions to be highly

    significant (Elbanna and Child 2007a).

    Methodological implications

    Large samples and multivariate analysis. Most of

    the managerially relevant SDM research is based on

    a fairly small number of cases studied in depth

    (Papadakis and Barwise 1997, p. 296). The insights

    provided by Bourgeois and Eisenhardt (1988),

    Eisenhardt (1989), Eisenhardt and Bourgeois (1988),

    Dean and Sharfman (1993, 1996), Papadakis and

    Barwise (2002) and Papadakiset al. (1998) all draw

    upon samples with 70 or fewer SDs from 38 or fewerorganizations. Therefore, to improve the external

    validity and generalizability of SDMP research, there

    is a need for large-sample field research (Papadakis

    et al. 2010; Rajagopalan et al. 1993). If multiple

    contextual variables, SDMP characteristics and

    SDMP outcomes are to be modelled, larger sample

    sizes will be essential to ensure that the ratio of cases

    to predictor variables is sufficient. Multivariate

    analysis comprises a set of statistical analysis tech-

    niques well suited to such analysis.

    Measurement reliability. Measures of SD-specificcharacteristics have suffered from low levels of inter-

    nal consistency, which indicates high levels of

    random error. Unreliable measures make detecting

    significant relationships less likely (Bagozzi 1994).

    Given the empirical evidence suggesting that

    SD-specific characteristics are potentially one of the

    most significant contextual influences on the

    SDMP, future research should seek to improve their

    reliabilities.

    Levels of analysis. Variance in the level of analysis

    adopted has hindered theory development and makescomparisons of findings difficult. While some studies

    do focus on individual decisions, a large number

    adopt an organizational level of analysis, assuming

    that organizations have consistent SDMPs, despite

    considerable empirical evidence showing that the

    SDMP varies according to the individual SD being

    made (Elbanna and Child 2007b; Hickson et al.

    1986; Papadakis et al. 1998). A focus on individual

    decisions is therefore recommended. Furthermore,

    there is a lack of significant results from studies

    examining the effects of CEO characteristics on the

    SDMP. Unless the focus of research is small organi-

    zations, it appears likely that the characteristics of the

    TMT as a whole will have more significant effects on

    the SDMP, rather than any one individual.

    Qualitative research designs. To complement large

    sample studies and to examine emerging con-

    cepts such as non-conscious cognitive processes

    (Hodgkinson and Healey 2011), SDMP researchers

    could adopt methods commonly used by scholars in

    Context in Strategic Decision-Making 361

    2013 British Academy of Management and John Wiley & Sons Ltd.

  • 8/10/2019 Environment and SDMProcess REVIEW 2014

    23/25

    other domains particularly cognitive task analysis

    (CTA) and critical incident technique (CIT) to

    probe incidents and uncover contextual influences on

    the SDMP. For example, techniques such as CIT and

    CTA can lead to a better understanding of the con-

    textual variables that contribute to intuitive hits and

    misses (Hodgkinson and Sadler-Smith 2011, p. 60).These types of techniques could be conducted with

    multiple members of the TMT, creating potential to

    uncover different perspectives from those involved in

    the SDMP.

    Summary

    This review has identified four categories of con-

    textual variables (TMT, SD-specific characteristics,

    the external environment and firm characteristics)

    that influence the SDMP, and provided an in-depth

    analysis of the underlying themes, issues, tensionsand debates in the literature. To develop theory in

    this important area, future research should take

    greater care with modelling, and include mediating

    decision processes, pay close attention to control

    variables and use direct measures of contextual

    variables. Future studies could also include contex-

    tual variables from each of the four categories to

    determine their relative influence, and also model

    contextual variables as moderators of the SDMP

    characteristicsoutcomes relationship, as well as

    antecedents of the SDMP. The important methodo-

    logical implications arising from this review high-light the importance that future research should use

    large sample sizes and multivariate techniques,

    improve measurement reliability, adopt a decisional

    level of analysis and complement quantitative

    studies with qualitative research.

    References

    Aldrich, H.E. (1979). Organizations and Environments.

    Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Ashmos, D.P., Duchon, D. and McDaniel, R.R. Jr (1998).

    Participation in strategic decision making: the role oforganizational predisposition and issue interpretation.

    Decision Sciences, 29, pp. 2551.

    Astley, W.G., Axelsson, R., Butler, R.J., Hickson, D.J. and

    Wilson, D.C. (1982). Complexity and cleavage: dual

    explanations of strategic decision-making. Journal of

    Management Studies, 19, pp. 357375.

    Atuahene-Gima, K. and