effects of strabismic amblyopia and strabismus without amblyopia...

11
Eye Movement, Strabismus, Amblyopia, and Neuro-Ophthalmology Effects of Strabismic Amblyopia and Strabismus without Amblyopia on Visuomotor Behavior, I: Saccadic Eye Movements Ewa Niechwiej-Szwedo, 1 Manokaraananthan Chandrakumar, 1 Herbert C. Goltz, 1,2 and Agnes M. F. Wong 1,2 PURPOSE. It has previously been shown that anisometropic amblyopia affects the programming and execution of saccades. The aim of the current study was to investigate the impact of strabismic amblyopia on saccade performance. METHODS. Fourteen adults with strabismic amblyopia, 13 adults with strabismus without amblyopia, and 14 visually normal adults performed saccades and reach-to-touch movements to targets presented at 658 and 6108 eccentricity during binocular and monocular viewing. Latency, amplitude, and peak velocity of primary and secondary saccades were measured. RESULTS. In contrast to visually normal participants who had shorter primary saccade latency during binocular viewing, no binocular advantage was found in patients with strabismus with or without amblyopia. Patients with amblyopia had longer saccade latency during amblyopic eye viewing (P < 0.0001); however, there were no significant differences in saccade amplitude precision among the three groups across viewing conditions. Further analysis showed that only patients with severe amblyopia and no stereopsis (n ¼ 4) exhibited longer latency (which was more pronounced for more central targets; P < 0.0001), and they also had reduced amplitude precision during amblyopic eye viewing. In contrast, patients with mild amblyopia (n ¼ 5) and no stereopsis had normal latency and reduced precision during amblyopic eye viewing (P < 0.001), whereas those with gross stereopsis (n ¼ 5) had normal latency and precision. There were no differences in peak velocity among the groups. CONCLUSIONS. Distinct patterns of saccade performance accord- ing to different levels of visual acuity and stereoscopic losses in strabismic amblyopia were found. These findings were in contrast to those in anisometropic amblyopia in which the altered saccade performance was independent of the extent of visual acuity or stereoscopic deficits. These results were most likely due to different long-term sensory suppression mecha- nisms in strabismic versus anisometropic amblyopia. (Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2012;53:7458–7468) DOI:10.1167/ iovs.12-10550 A mblyopia is a neural disorder caused by inadequate visual stimulation during the early critical period of develop- ment. 1 Amblyopia is commonly associated with two risk factors: strabismus (eye misalignment) and anisometropia (difference in refractive errors between the eyes). 2 Patients with amblyopia have reduced visual acuity and contrast sensitivity, as well as other perceptual deficits, 3–10 which are most pronounced during amblyopic eye viewing; however, they are also evident during fellow eye viewing, albeit to a lesser extent. 11–13 The effects of amblyopia on visuomotor behavior have not been examined as extensively as the sensory/perceptual deficits. This is surprising because a fundamental function of the brain is to use sensory information from all modalities to make purposeful, goal-directed behaviors through the process of sensorimotor integration. Vision provides critical informa- tion about the location and properties of objects that we want to interact with or avoid. To see an object in detail, the fovea— the area of highest resolution on the retina—has to be directed toward the object, which is achieved via saccadic eye movements. Visual information is combined across eye movements to form internal spatial representations of the external world. 14,15 Thus, saccadic eye movements are an essential component of the action–perception loop, and play an important role in guiding flexible behaviors while people interact with objects in dynamic environments. Our group has been studying the effects of impaired spatiotemporal visual functions in amblyopia on motor behavior. In a series of detailed investigations, we have recently reported the impact of anisometropic amblyopia on saccadic eye movements, 16 reaching movements of the upper limb, 17,18 and eye–hand coordination 19 during visually guided reaching. Specifically, we have shown that patients with anisometropic amblyopia had significantly longer and more variable saccade latency during amblyopic eye viewing, 16 lower peak acceleration and a longer acceleration phase during reaching, 18 and a different temporal pattern of eye–hand coordination. 19 Importantly, the effects of amblyopia on reaching movements were evident not only during amblyopic eye viewing, but also during binocular and fellow eye viewing. As a next step in our systematic investigations, we examined whether different subtypes of amblyopia affect motor behaviors differentially. Previous studies have demon- strated several differences in perceptual deficits among patients with anisometropic versus strabismic amblyopia. For example, patients with anisometropic amblyopia exhibited From the 1 Department of Ophthalmology and Vision Sciences, The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; and the 2 University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada. Supported by Canadian Institutes of Health Research Grant MOP 106663, a Leaders Opportunity Fund grant from the Canadian Foundation for Innovation, and a grant from the Department of Ophthalmology and Vision Sciences at The Hospital for Sick Children. Submitted for publication July 9, 2012; revised August 22 and October 1, 2012; accepted October 2, 2012. Disclosure: E. Niechwiej-Szwedo, None; M. Chandrakumar, None; H.C. Goltz, None; A.M.F. Wong, None Corresponding author: Agnes M. F. Wong, Department of Ophthalmology and Vision Sciences, The Hospital for Sick Children, 555 University Avenue, Toronto, Ontario, Canada M5G 1X8; [email protected]. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, November 2012, Vol. 53, No. 12 7458 Copyright 2012 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, Inc.

Upload: others

Post on 05-Feb-2020

2 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Effects of Strabismic Amblyopia and Strabismus without Amblyopia …individual.utoronto.ca/goldentamizhan/73.pdf · 2012-11-20 · Eye Movement, Strabismus, Amblyopia, and Neuro-Ophthalmology

Eye Movement, Strabismus, Amblyopia, and Neuro-Ophthalmology

Effects of Strabismic Amblyopia and Strabismus withoutAmblyopia on Visuomotor Behavior, I: Saccadic EyeMovements

Ewa Niechwiej-Szwedo,1 Manokaraananthan Chandrakumar,1 Herbert C. Goltz,1,2

and Agnes M. F. Wong1,2

PURPOSE. It has previously been shown that anisometropicamblyopia affects the programming and execution of saccades.The aim of the current study was to investigate the impact ofstrabismic amblyopia on saccade performance.

METHODS. Fourteen adults with strabismic amblyopia, 13 adultswith strabismus without amblyopia, and 14 visually normaladults performed saccades and reach-to-touch movements totargets presented at 658 and 6108 eccentricity duringbinocular and monocular viewing. Latency, amplitude, andpeak velocity of primary and secondary saccades weremeasured.

RESULTS. In contrast to visually normal participants who hadshorter primary saccade latency during binocular viewing, nobinocular advantage was found in patients with strabismuswith or without amblyopia. Patients with amblyopia had longersaccade latency during amblyopic eye viewing (P < 0.0001);however, there were no significant differences in saccadeamplitude precision among the three groups across viewingconditions. Further analysis showed that only patients withsevere amblyopia and no stereopsis (n ¼ 4) exhibited longerlatency (which was more pronounced for more central targets;P < 0.0001), and they also had reduced amplitude precisionduring amblyopic eye viewing. In contrast, patients with mildamblyopia (n ¼ 5) and no stereopsis had normal latency andreduced precision during amblyopic eye viewing (P < 0.001),whereas those with gross stereopsis (n¼5) had normal latencyand precision. There were no differences in peak velocityamong the groups.

CONCLUSIONS. Distinct patterns of saccade performance accord-ing to different levels of visual acuity and stereoscopic losses instrabismic amblyopia were found. These findings were incontrast to those in anisometropic amblyopia in which thealtered saccade performance was independent of the extent ofvisual acuity or stereoscopic deficits. These results were most

likely due to different long-term sensory suppression mecha-nisms in strabismic versus anisometropic amblyopia. (Invest

Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2012;53:7458–7468) DOI:10.1167/iovs.12-10550

Amblyopia is a neural disorder caused by inadequate visualstimulation during the early critical period of develop-

ment.1 Amblyopia is commonly associated with two riskfactors: strabismus (eye misalignment) and anisometropia(difference in refractive errors between the eyes).2 Patientswith amblyopia have reduced visual acuity and contrastsensitivity, as well as other perceptual deficits,3–10 which aremost pronounced during amblyopic eye viewing; however,they are also evident during fellow eye viewing, albeit to alesser extent.11–13

The effects of amblyopia on visuomotor behavior have notbeen examined as extensively as the sensory/perceptualdeficits. This is surprising because a fundamental function ofthe brain is to use sensory information from all modalities tomake purposeful, goal-directed behaviors through the processof sensorimotor integration. Vision provides critical informa-tion about the location and properties of objects that we wantto interact with or avoid. To see an object in detail, the fovea—the area of highest resolution on the retina—has to be directedtoward the object, which is achieved via saccadic eyemovements. Visual information is combined across eyemovements to form internal spatial representations of theexternal world.14,15 Thus, saccadic eye movements are anessential component of the action–perception loop, and playan important role in guiding flexible behaviors while peopleinteract with objects in dynamic environments.

Our group has been studying the effects of impairedspatiotemporal visual functions in amblyopia on motorbehavior. In a series of detailed investigations, we haverecently reported the impact of anisometropic amblyopia onsaccadic eye movements,16 reaching movements of the upperlimb,17,18 and eye–hand coordination19 during visually guidedreaching. Specifically, we have shown that patients withanisometropic amblyopia had significantly longer and morevariable saccade latency during amblyopic eye viewing,16

lower peak acceleration and a longer acceleration phase duringreaching,18 and a different temporal pattern of eye–handcoordination.19 Importantly, the effects of amblyopia onreaching movements were evident not only during amblyopiceye viewing, but also during binocular and fellow eye viewing.

As a next step in our systematic investigations, weexamined whether different subtypes of amblyopia affectmotor behaviors differentially. Previous studies have demon-strated several differences in perceptual deficits amongpatients with anisometropic versus strabismic amblyopia. Forexample, patients with anisometropic amblyopia exhibited

From the 1Department of Ophthalmology and Vision Sciences,The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, Ontario, Canada; and the2University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada.

Supported by Canadian Institutes of Health Research GrantMOP 106663, a Leaders Opportunity Fund grant from the CanadianFoundation for Innovation, and a grant from the Department ofOphthalmology and Vision Sciences at The Hospital for SickChildren.

Submitted for publication July 9, 2012; revised August 22 andOctober 1, 2012; accepted October 2, 2012.

Disclosure: E. Niechwiej-Szwedo, None; M. Chandrakumar,None; H.C. Goltz, None; A.M.F. Wong, None

Corresponding author: Agnes M. F. Wong, Department ofOphthalmology and Vision Sciences, The Hospital for Sick Children,555 University Avenue, Toronto, Ontario, Canada M5G 1X8;[email protected].

Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science, November 2012, Vol. 53, No. 12

7458 Copyright 2012 The Association for Research in Vision and Ophthalmology, Inc.

Page 2: Effects of Strabismic Amblyopia and Strabismus without Amblyopia …individual.utoronto.ca/goldentamizhan/73.pdf · 2012-11-20 · Eye Movement, Strabismus, Amblyopia, and Neuro-Ophthalmology

deficits in contrast detection8 and spatial localization across theentire visual field,20 whereas patients with strabismic ambly-opia exhibited more pronounced deficits in the central visualfield than those in the peripheral visual field. A study of 427amblyopic patients has also shown distinctive patterns of visualdeficits among different amblyopia subtypes.13 Patients withanisometropic amblyopia and moderate loss of acuity hadnormal/subnormal contrast sensitivity and were more likely tohave gross stereopsis, whereas those with strabismic ambly-opia and moderate loss of acuity had better than normalcontrast sensitivity at low spatial frequencies and were morelikely to have reduced/absent stereopsis.

The effects of strabismic amblyopia on saccadic eyemovements have only been investigated in two previousstudies. Schor21 examined saccades in five patients using apredictable, square-wave stimulus to elicit saccades. Hereported no difference in latency and a significant increase inamplitude variability when patients viewed with their ambly-opic eye. Ciuffreda and colleagues22 tested six patients usingan unpredictable stepping target and found longer saccadelatencies in some patients. However, they21,22 did not include avisually normal control group and their sample size was small,which precluded a more detailed analysis of the effects ofvisual acuity or stereoacuity on saccade performance. Previousstudies of children with strabismus without amblyopia foundno difference in saccade latency between children with andwithout binocular vision23 and in comparison to children withnormal vision.24 However, adults with strabismus showed animpairment in binocular coordination of saccades that wasmost pronounced in patients without binocular vision.25

The objective of the current study was to investigatesaccadic eye movements during a visually guided reaching taskin patients with strabismic amblyopia, as well as the effects ofvisual acuity and stereoacuity deficits on saccade performance.We hypothesized that impairments in saccade performancewould be largest in patients with amblyopia during amblyopiceye viewing. We also hypothesized that patients’ performancewould be affected by their level of visual acuity andstereoacuity deficits. Specifically, we hypothesized that sac-cades will be delayed and have reduced precision in patientswith poorer acuity and negative stereopsis. Results of thereaching movement are the focus of our next study.

METHODS

Participants

All participants were adults and underwent a complete orthoptic

assessment by a certified orthoptist, which included visual acuity

(Snellen chart), prism cover test (simultaneous and alternate) of eye

alignment, and measurement of stereoacuity using the Titmus test.

Exclusion criteria were any ocular cause for reduced visual acuity,

previous intraocular surgery, or any neurologic disease. All participants

were right-handed to reduce the variability in motor performance.26

Fourteen patients with strabismic amblyopia (6 females; age: 31.7

6 9.9 years; see Table 1 for clinical characteristics) were recruited.

Strabismic amblyopia was defined as an interocular acuity difference

‡2 lines on a Snellen chart, and subjects with a history of childhood

strabismus and manifest eye deviation. Visual acuity was tested with

current refractive correction. The difference in refractive error

between the two eyes was �1 diopter (D) of spherical or cylindrical

power, to rule out a potentially amblyogenic astigmatic component

(i.e., to rule out mixed-mechanism amblyopia). When stereopsis was

absent, the presence or absence of sensory fusion was determined

using Worth’s 4-dot test and Bagolini-striated glasses. Ten patients had

mild amblyopia, with acuity in the amblyopic eye ranging from 20/30

to 20/60. Five of the patients with mild amblyopia had gross stereopsis

(range: 120–800 seconds of arc), whereas the other five patients had

negative stereopsis. Four patients had severe amblyopia (20/200 in the

amblyopic eye) and negative stereopsis. Visual acuity in the fellow eye

was 20/20 or better in all patients.

Thirteen patients with strabismus without amblyopia (acuity 20/25

or better in both eyes) were also recruited (6 females; age: 30.1 6 4.0

years; see Table 1 for clinical characteristics). All patients had manifest

eye deviation. Nine patients were tested negative for stereopsis, two

had stereoacuity of 3000 seconds of arc, and the remaining two had

stereoacuity of 50 and 80 seconds of arc.

Fourteen visually normal participants (6 females; age: 31.7 6 9.9

years) with corrected-to-normal visual acuity (20/20 or better) in both

eyes and stereoacuity � 40 seconds of arc were recruited. Eye

dominance in visually normal participants was determined using the

Dolman ‘‘hole-in-card’’ test.27

The study was approved by the Research Ethics Board at The

Hospital for Sick Children, and all protocols adhered to the guidelines

of the Declaration of Helsinki. Informed consent was obtained from

each participant.

Apparatus and Experimental Protocol

Details of the apparatus and experimental procedure have been

described in a previous article.16 Briefly, the visual target was a white

square (visual angle: 0.58) presented on a black background on a

cathode ray tube computer monitor (Diamond Pro 2070SB, resolution

1600 3 1200 at 85 Hz; NEC/Mitsubishi Electric Visual Systems, Tokyo,

Japan). Testing was conducted in a dimly lit room. Eye movements

were recorded binocularly at 200 Hz using a video-based pupil/iris

tracking system (Chronos Vision, Berlin, Germany). Reaching move-

ments of the upper limb were also recorded simultaneously at 200 Hz

using an infrared illumination-based motion-capture system (Optotrak

Certus; Northern Digital, Waterloo, Canada).

Participants were seated at a table with their heads stabilized on a

chin rest. They fixated a cross at the beginning of each trial. The

viewing distance was 42 cm. After a variable delay of 1.5 to 3 seconds,

the fixation cross was extinguished and one visual target appeared

randomly at one of four eccentricities at 658 or 6108 along the

horizontal axis. There was no temporal delay between the offset of the

fixation point and the presentation of the target (simultaneous fixation

offset and target onset). Participants were instructed to look at the

target and to make a reaching movement to touch the target with their

index finger as quickly and as accurately as possible. Details of the

reaching task have been described previously.18 In 50% of the trials, the

target was switched off at the onset of hand movement. For the other

50% of the trials, the target remained on the screen. Trials with and

without visual feedback of the target were randomized on a trial-by-trial

basis.

Participants performed the experiment in three viewing conditions:

(1) binocular viewing; (2) monocular viewing with the dominant eye

(i.e., the fellow eye for patients with amblyopia, and the nondeviating

eye for patients with strabismus without amblyopia); and (3)

monocular viewing with the nondominant eye (i.e., the amblyopic

eye for patients with amblyopia, and the deviating eye for patients with

strabismus without amblyopia). Data were collected in blocks for each

viewing condition, and the order of viewing conditions was

randomized across participants. All participants completed 10 trials

in each combination of the experimental conditions for a total of 240

trials/session. Practice trials were completed before the experiment

was begun to familiarize the participants with the experimental

procedure. All data were collected in one session (1–1.5 hours), which

included calibration of the apparatus, practice, and experimental trials.

Analysis: Saccadic Eye Movements

Eye position data were low-pass filtered using a second-order dual-pass

Butterworth filter with a cutoff frequency of 50 Hz. Eye velocity was

obtained using a two-point differentiation method. A custom-written

IOVS, November 2012, Vol. 53, No. 12 Saccadic Eye Movements in Strabismic Amblyopia 7459

Page 3: Effects of Strabismic Amblyopia and Strabismus without Amblyopia …individual.utoronto.ca/goldentamizhan/73.pdf · 2012-11-20 · Eye Movement, Strabismus, Amblyopia, and Neuro-Ophthalmology

TA

BLE

1.

Ch

arac

teri

stic

so

fPat

ien

tsw

ith

Stra

bis

mic

Am

bly

op

iaan

dSt

rab

ism

us

wit

ho

ut

Am

bly

op

ia

IDA

ge,

ySex

Sn

ell

en

Vis

ual

Acu

ity

(lo

gM

AR

)R

efr

acti

ve

Err

or

Devia

tio

no

nSim

ult

an

eo

us

Pri

smC

over

Test

(Alt

ern

ate

Pri

smT

est

)Ste

reo

acu

ity

(seco

nd

so

farc

)F

usi

on

No

tes

Rig

ht

Left

Rig

ht

Left

Ey

eN

ear

(PD

)D

ista

nce

(PD

)

Pat

ien

tsw

ith

stra

bis

mic

amb

lyo

pia

12

7F

20

/15

(�0

.10

)2

0/4

0(0

.30

)�

1.0

0.5

0�

1.0

0.5

0Left

ET

8(E

E1

0)

ET

2(E

E4

)4

00

23

7F

20

/40

(0.3

0)

20

/20

(0.0

0)�

4.0

0�

4.0

0R

igh

tE

T4

(ETþ

E6

)E

T4

80

0

31

9F

20

/20

(0.0

0)

20

/40

(0.3

0)�

2.7

5�

3.0

0Left

ET

2(E

E1

4)

ET

24

00

Hx

occlu

sio

n

42

9M

20

/30

(0.1

8)

20

/20

(0.0

0)�

2.2

3.0

03

11

0�

2.5

2.2

53

75

Rig

ht

XT

2(X

X4

0)

XT

2(X

X4

5)

20

0

52

8F

20

/20

(0.0

0)

20

/30

(0.1

8)þ

0.7

0.2

53

45þ

0.2

0.7

53

95

Left

ET

1E

T2

12

0H

xo

cclu

sio

n

63

8M

20

/30

(0.1

8)

20

/15

(�0

.10

)�

3.5

0�

3.5

0.5

03

25

Rig

ht

ET

35

,h

ypo

4E

T3

5,

hyp

o6

Negat

ive

Sup

pre

ssH

xo

cclu

sio

n

71

9M

20

/50

(0.4

0)

20

/20

(0.0

0)

Pla

no

Pla

no

Rig

ht

ET

16

ET

14

Negat

ive

Sup

pre

ssH

xo

cclu

sio

n

83

0M

20

/15

(�0

.10

)2

0/3

0(0

.18

)P

lan

oP

lan

oLeft

ET

35

ET

35

Negat

ive

Sup

pre

ssIE

T

95

1F

20

/50

(0.4

0)

20

/20

(0.0

0)þ

1.7

2.0

03

74þ

2.5

1.2

53

90

Rig

ht

ET

14

,h

ypo

14

ET

8,

hyp

o1

2N

egat

ive

Sup

pre

ssIE

T,H

xsu

rgery

/

occlu

sio

n

10

21

F2

0/2

0(0

.00

)2

0/3

0(0

.18

1.5

1.5

03

65þ

1.2

1.0

03

11

0Left

ET

2,

hyp

o5

bilat

era

lD

VD

ET

6,

hyp

o5

bilat

era

lD

VD

Negat

ive

W4

DIE

T,H

xsu

rgery

/

occlu

sio

n

11

25

M2

0/2

0(0

.00

)2

0/2

00

(1.0

0)�

0.7

0.5

03

90�

0.2

5Left

ET

12

ET

10

Negat

ive

Sup

pre

ssN

on

-cen

tral

fix

atio

n,

Hx

occlu

sio

n

12

37

M2

0/2

0(0

.00

)2

0/2

00

(1.0

0)

Pla

no

Pla

no

Left

XT

4(X

X8

)X

T4

(XTþ

X6

)N

egat

ive

No

n-c

en

tral

fix

atio

n

13

48

M2

0/1

5(�

0.1

0)

20

/20

0(1

.00

)P

lan

oP

lan

oLeft

XT

40

XT

30

Negat

ive

Sup

pre

ssN

on

-cen

tral

fix

atio

n,

Hx

surg

ery

/

occlu

sio

n

14

35

M2

0/2

0(0

.00

)2

0/2

00

(1.0

0)�

0.7

5P

lan

oLeft

XT

59

,H

T1

2X

T4

0,

HT

10

Negat

ive

Sup

pre

ssN

on

-cen

tral

fix

atio

n,

Hx

surg

ery

/

occlu

sio

n

Pat

ien

tsw

ith

stra

bis

mu

sw

ith

ou

tam

bly

op

ia

15

26

F2

0/1

5(�

0.1

0)

20

/20

(0.0

0)þ

1.0

0.2

53

35þ

2.0

0Left

ET

2(E

E4

)E

T2

(ETþ

E4

)5

0H

xo

cclu

sio

n

16

21

M2

0/1

5(�

0.1

0)

20

/15

(�0

.10

)P

lan

oP

lan

oR

igh

tX

4,

H2

X4

,H

28

0H

xsu

rgery

17

31

F2

0/2

0(0

.00

)2

0/2

5(0

.10

)P

lan

oP

lan

oR

igh

tE

T4

(ETþ

E1

2)

ET

4(E

E1

0)

30

00

Hx

occlu

sio

n

18

27

M2

0/2

0(0

.00

)2

0/2

0(0

.00

)P

lan

oP

lan

oR

igh

tE

T2

,H

T3

(ETþ

E4

)

ET

2,

HT

3

(ETþ

E4

)

30

00

Hx

occlu

sio

n/

surg

ery

19

29

F2

0/1

5(�

0.1

0)

20

/20

(0.0

0)

Pla

no

�2

.00þ

0.5

03

17

0R

igh

tX

T1

6,

hyp

o2

XT

14

,H

T3

Negat

ive

Sup

pre

ssH

xsu

rgery

20

33

F2

0/1

5(�

0.1

0)

20

/15

(�0

.10

)P

lan

oP

lan

oLeft

AE

T5

0E

T4

(ETþ

E2

5)

Negat

ive

Sup

pre

ss

21

33

M2

0/1

5(�

0.1

0)

20

/15

(�0

.10

)�

1.5

0P

lan

oLeft

ET

20

,b

ilat

era

l

HT

ET

20

,b

ilat

era

l

HT

Negat

ive

W4

DA

RC

,H

xsu

rgery

22

32

F2

0/2

0(0

.00

)2

0/2

0(0

.00

)�

2.5

0.5

03

17

5�

4.0

1.2

53

11

5R

igh

tE

T2

0,

HT

3E

T1

8,

HT

3N

egat

ive

Peri

ph

era

lfu

sio

n

on

Bag

olin

i

AR

C

23

31

M2

0/1

5(�

0.1

0)

20

/15

(�0

.10

)�

7.5

3.0

03

80�

4.2

2.0

03

95

Left

AE

T4

5,

HT

18

R/A

ET

40

,H

T1

6N

egat

ive

Sup

pre

ssH

xo

cclu

sio

n/

surg

ery

24

34

M2

0/1

5(�

0.1

0)

20

/15

(�0

.10

1.5

0.5

03

94

plþ

1.0

03

65

Rig

ht

AE

T1

8E

T1

2N

egat

ive

Sup

pre

ssO

cclu

sio

n,

surg

ery

25

34

F2

0/1

5(�

0.1

0)

20

/25

(0.1

0)þ

2.7

4.0

1.0

03

11

5Left

ET

20

ET

25

Negat

ive

Int.

fusi

on

on

W4

Dfu

sio

n

26

21

M2

0/1

5(�

0.1

0)

20

/15

(�0

.10

3.5

1.2

53

11

3.0

1.0

03

95

Rig

ht

ET

25

,h

ypo

6E

T1

4,

hyp

o6

Negat

ive

Sup

pre

ssIE

T

27

25

M2

0/1

5(�

0.1

0)

20

/20

(0.0

0)�

1.0

0.5

03

75�

0.7

0.2

53

13

0Left

XT

4,

HT

4X

T2

5,

hyp

o5

Negat

ive

Sup

pre

ssO

cclu

sio

n

PD

,p

rism

dio

pte

r;H

x,h

isto

ry;W

4D

,W

ort

h4

Do

tte

st;B

ago

lin

i,B

ago

lin

ist

riat

ed

gla

sses;

ET,eso

tro

pia

;X

T,ex

otr

op

ia;X

,ex

op

ho

ria;

E,eso

ph

ori

a;H

T,h

yp

ert

rop

ia;H

yp

o,h

yp

otr

op

ia;H

,h

yp

erp

ho

ria;

Int.

fusi

on

,in

term

itte

nt

fusi

on

;IE

T,in

fan

tile

eso

tro

pia

;A

RC

,an

om

alo

us

reti

nal

co

rresp

on

den

ce;

AE

T,al

tern

atin

geso

tro

pia

;D

VD

,d

isso

cia

ted

vert

ical

devia

tio

n;

surg

ery

,su

rgic

altr

eat

men

to

fst

rab

ism

us.

7460 Niechwiej-Szwedo et al. IOVS, November 2012, Vol. 53, No. 12

Page 4: Effects of Strabismic Amblyopia and Strabismus without Amblyopia …individual.utoronto.ca/goldentamizhan/73.pdf · 2012-11-20 · Eye Movement, Strabismus, Amblyopia, and Neuro-Ophthalmology

script (Matlab; The MathWorks, Natick, MA) was used to identify

primary saccades using a velocity threshold of 20 deg/s. All trials were

inspected visually to ensure that saccades were identified correctly by

the computer script. We examined the recordings of the right and left

eyes during the binocular viewing condition and found no difference in

saccade kinematics between eyes. Therefore, only one eye was

included in the statistical analysis. During monocular viewing

conditions, only the viewing eye was analyzed. Outcome measures

for primary saccades were mean latency, amplitude, and peak velocity.

The variability (SD) of primary saccade amplitude was also calculated.

All trials were inspected for the presence of secondary saccades

that were marked manually for each trial. Secondary saccades that

occurred within 250 ms of the primary saccades were defined as

corrective saccades. The outcome measures for secondary saccades

were frequency in each viewing condition, as well as mean saccade

latency, amplitude, and peak velocity. In addition, we calculated the

precision (i.e., variability) of the final amplitude after secondary

saccades were executed. The final amplitude was defined as the

algebraic sum of primary and secondary saccade amplitudes. The final

saccade amplitude precision was used to examine whether secondary

saccades were generated to correct the error in position that remained

after the primary saccade.

Statistical Analysis

All continuous dependent variables were submitted to repeated-

measures ANOVA with one between-subjects factor: Group (strabismic

amblyopia, strabismus without amblyopia, visually normal) and three

within-subjects factors: Viewing Condition (binocular, monocular

dominant eye [fellow eye for patients with amblyopia], and monocular

nondominant eye [amblyopic eye for patients with amblyopia]), Target

Location (58, 108), and Visual Feedback of Target (on, off).

The frequency of corrective saccades was compared using

Pearson’s v2 statistic. The frequency of saccades was first compared

between patients and visually normal participants, then the effect of

Viewing Condition was examined within each group, both using

Pearson’s v2 statistic.

All statistical analyses were performed using a commercial statistical

analysis software program (SAS Software, version 9.2; SAS Institute Inc.,

Cary, NC). Descriptive statistics were reported as the mean and

corresponding SD. All main effects and interactions were analyzed

further using Tukey–Kramer post hoc tests to adjust for multiple

comparisons. The significance level was set at P < 0.05. Preliminary

analysis of all the data showed that Visual Feedback of Target had no

significant effect on any outcome measures; therefore, data with or

without visual feedback were collapsed for subsequent analysis and

reporting. Saccades to the left and right targets at each eccentricity

were pooled together for statistical analysis.

Effects of Severity of Amblyopia and Stereopsis. To investigate

further the effects of severity of amblyopia and stereopsis, a separate

repeated-measures analysis was performed on each outcome measure.

For this analysis, patients with amblyopia were stratified into three

subgroups: (1) mild amblyopia (i.e., acuity �20/60) and gross

stereopsis (mild�stereoþ; Table 1: patients 1–5); (2) mild amblyopia

and negative stereopsis (mild�stereo�; Table 1: patients 6–10); and (3)

severe amblyopia (i.e., acuity 20/200) and negative stereopsis (Table 1:

patients 11–14). Since the number of subjects in each subgroup was

small, a nonparametric approach was used. All data were transformed

to ranks and then submitted to a repeated-measures ANOVA following

the procedure developed by Conover and Iman.28 The ANOVA had

Subgroup as a between-subjects factor (i.e., mild�stereoþ,

mild�stereo�, severe) and two within-subjects factors: Viewing

Condition (binocular, monocular fellow eye, and monocular amblyopic

eye) and Target Location (58, 108).

Effects of Stereopsis in Patients with Strabismus without

Amblyopia. To investigate the effects of stereopsis in patients with

strabismus without amblyopia, a separate repeated-measures ANOVA

on the ranked data28 was performed on each outcome measure. For

this analysis, patients with strabismus without amblyopia were divided

into two subgroups: stereo positive (n¼4), and stereo negative (n¼9).

The ANOVA had Subgroup as a between-subjects factor (i.e., stereoþ,

stereo�) and two within-subjects factors: Viewing Condition (binocular,

monocular dominant eye, and monocular nondominant eye) and Target

Location (58, 108).

Effects of Strabismic Eye Deviation. Spearman correlation

analysis was performed to investigate the relation between the amount

of eye deviation and primary saccade outcome measures (latency,

amplitude accuracy [mean] and precision [SD], peak velocity). The

correlation coefficient was calculated separately for patients with

strabismic amblyopia and patients with strabismus without amblyopia

for the different viewing conditions.

RESULTS

Primary Saccades

Figure 1 shows representative eye velocity tracings from avisually normal participant and individual patients withstrabismic amblyopia (mild�stereoþ, mild�stereo�, severe),and strabismus without amblyopia (stereoþ, stereo�). Thevisually normal participant showed highly stereotypical sac-cades in all viewing conditions. The largest deviation from thestereotypical saccade behavior was seen in the patient withsevere amblyopia (negative stereopsis) during amblyopic eyeviewing and in the patient with strabismus without amblyopia(negative stereopsis) in all viewing conditions. Both patientsshowed delayed and highly variable saccades.

Latency. The main effect of Viewing Condition (F2,76 ¼16.81; P < 0.0001) and the interaction between Group andViewing Condition were significant (F4,76 ¼ 6.92; P < 0.0001;Fig. 2A). Post hoc tests revealed that mean saccade latencyincreased significantly when patients with strabismic ambly-opia viewed with their amblyopic eye (218 6 49 ms),compared with viewing with their fellow eye (172 6 36 ms)or binocularly (177 6 39 ms). Patients with strabismus withoutamblyopia had comparable saccade latency in all viewingconditions (binocular: 191 6 29 ms; dominant eye: 190 6 23ms; nondominant eye: 198 6 32 ms). In contrast, visuallynormal participants had significantly shorter saccade latencywhen viewing binocularly (173 6 27 ms), compared withmonocular viewing with the dominant eye (190 6 25 ms) ornondominant eye (191 6 26 ms).

The three-way interaction between Group (strabismicamblyopia, strabismus without amblyopia, visually normal),Viewing Condition, and Target Location was also significant(F6,76 ¼ 5.12; P ¼ 0.0002; Fig. 3A). Post hoc testing indicatedthat patients with strabismic amblyopia had significantly longerlatency during amblyopic eye viewing for saccades to the 58target (227 6 52 ms) compared with the 108 target (209 6 44ms). Target Location did not affect saccade latency in patientswith strabismus without amblyopia and visually normalparticipants in any viewing condition.

The comparison among patients within the amblyopiasubgroup (mild�stereoþ, mild�stereo�, severe) showed asignificant interaction between Subgroup and Viewing Condi-tion (F4,22¼9.34; P < 0.0001, Fig. 2B). Post hoc tests indicatedthat patients with severe amblyopia had significantly longersaccade latency when viewing with the amblyopic eye (273 636 ms), compared with binocular (165 6 41 ms) or fellow eyeviewing (160 6 33 ms), and to the other subgroups (i.e., mildamblyopia with and without stereopsis). Patients with mildamblyopia and negative stereopsis also had significantly longersaccade latency during amblyopic eye viewing (187 6 34 ms),compared with binocular (165 6 23 ms) or fellow eye viewing(165 6 35 ms). In contrast, patients with mild amblyopia and

IOVS, November 2012, Vol. 53, No. 12 Saccadic Eye Movements in Strabismic Amblyopia 7461

Page 5: Effects of Strabismic Amblyopia and Strabismus without Amblyopia …individual.utoronto.ca/goldentamizhan/73.pdf · 2012-11-20 · Eye Movement, Strabismus, Amblyopia, and Neuro-Ophthalmology

FIGURE 1. Representative eye velocity tracings from individual trials during monocular viewing with the nondominant/amblyopic eye (left

column), monocular viewing with the dominant/fellow eye (middle column), and binocular viewing (right column) when the target was shown108 to the right. Top row: a visually normal participant; second row: a patient with mild amblyopia and gross stereopsis (200 seconds of arc) (Table1, ID 4); third row: a patient with mild amblyopia and negative stereopsis (Table 1, ID 6); fourth row: a patient with severe amblyopia and negativestereopsis (Table 1, ID 14); fifth row: a patient with strabismus and stereopsis (80 seconds of arc) (Table 1, ID 16); last row: a patient withstrabismus without amblyopia and negative stereopsis (Table 1, ID 21).

7462 Niechwiej-Szwedo et al. IOVS, November 2012, Vol. 53, No. 12

Page 6: Effects of Strabismic Amblyopia and Strabismus without Amblyopia …individual.utoronto.ca/goldentamizhan/73.pdf · 2012-11-20 · Eye Movement, Strabismus, Amblyopia, and Neuro-Ophthalmology

gross stereopsis had similar saccade latencies across all viewing

conditions (binocular viewing: 200 6 43 ms; fellow eye

viewing: 190 6 35 ms; amblyopic eye viewing: 205 6 29 ms).

The three-way interaction between Amblyopia Subgroup

(mild�stereoþ, mild�stereo�, severe), Viewing Condition, andTarget Location was also significant (F6,22 ¼ 2.88; P ¼ 0.0315;

Fig. 3B). Post hoc testing indicated that only patients withsevere amblyopia had significantly longer latency during

amblyopic eye viewing for saccades to the 58 targets (294 6

7 ms) compared with the 108 targets (252 6 21 ms). Targetlocation did not affect saccade latency in patients with

amblyopia who had mild acuity deficits with and withoutstereopsis.

There was no significant difference among patients withstrabismus without amblyopia with and without stereopsis for

saccade latency in any viewing condition. There was no

relation between the extent of eye deviation and latency inboth patient groups.

Amplitude. There was a significant main effect of targetlocation for saccade amplitude (F1,38 ¼ 1255.91; P < 0.0001).Saccades to the 108 target had higher amplitude than those tothe 58 target in all experimental conditions for visually normalparticipants and all patients (Table 2). No other significantmain effects or interactions were present for mean saccadeamplitude. There were also no differences among patients withamblyopia. The distribution of primary saccade amplitude foreach target location across viewing conditions for a fewrepresentative participants in each subject group is shown inSupplemental Figure S1 (see Supplementary Material andSupplementary Fig. S1, http://www.iovs.org/lookup/suppl/doi:10.1167/iovs.12-10550/-/DCSupplemental).

Saccade amplitude precision (i.e., variability) was notsignificantly different when patients with amblyopia werecompared with visually normal participants and patients withstrabismus without amblyopia (F4,76¼ 3.12; P¼ 0.064). Therewas a main effect of target location (F1,38 ¼ 114.08; P <0.0001); however, interaction between Group and Target

FIGURE 2. Mean saccade latencies as a function of viewing condition. (A) Latencies were significantly longer for patients with strabismic amblyopia(shown in red) during amblyopic eye viewing (P < 0.0001). Visually normal participants (shown in black) had shorter saccade latencies duringbinocular viewing compared with monocular viewing. In contrast, all patients had comparable saccade latency during binocular and fellow(nondeviated) eye viewing (i.e., no binocular advantage). Patients with strabismus without amblyopia (shown in green) had similar saccade latencyin all viewing conditions. (B) Saccade latencies were significantly longer only for patients with severe amblyopia (blue lines, visual acuity 20/200,negative stereopsis) during amblyopic eye viewing (P < 0.0001).

FIGURE 3. Mean saccade latency as a function of target eccentricity across viewing conditions. (A) Patients with strabismic amblyopia (shown inred) had longer saccade latency during amblyopic eye viewing for the 58 target compared with the 108 target (P¼ 0.0002). (B) Further subgroupanalysis revealed that only patients with severe visual acuity deficits due to amblyopia and negative stereopsis (shown in blue) had a significantlonger saccade latency (P¼ 0.0315) during amblyopic eye viewing to targets located closer to fixation compared with more peripheral targets.

IOVS, November 2012, Vol. 53, No. 12 Saccadic Eye Movements in Strabismic Amblyopia 7463

Page 7: Effects of Strabismic Amblyopia and Strabismus without Amblyopia …individual.utoronto.ca/goldentamizhan/73.pdf · 2012-11-20 · Eye Movement, Strabismus, Amblyopia, and Neuro-Ophthalmology

Location did not reach significance (F2,38¼2.33; P¼0.056). Allsubjects exhibited larger variability for the 108 target (visuallynormal: 1.07 6 0.528; patients with strabismic amblyopia: 1.326 0.658; patients with strabismus without amblyopia: 1.50 60.688) compared with the 58 target (visually normal: 0.68 60.318; patients with strabismic amblyopia: 0.88 6 0.488;patients with strabismus without amblyopia: 0.85 6 0.428).

There was a significant interaction between AmblyopiaSubgroup and Viewing Condition (F4,22¼6.89; P¼0.0009; Fig.4A) for saccade amplitude precision. Post hoc testing showedthat patients with severe amblyopia had significantly reducedprecision of saccade amplitude during amblyopic eye viewing(2.08 6 0.868) compared with binocular (1.12 6 0.528) orfellow eye viewing (0.71 6 0.328). Similarly, patients with mildamblyopia and negative stereopsis had reduced saccadeamplitude precision during amblyopic eye viewing (1.62 60.558) compared with binocular (1.18 6 0.328) or fellow eyeviewing (1.07 6 0.468). In contrast, patients with mildamblyopia and gross stereopsis had similar saccade amplitudeprecision across viewing conditions (binocular viewing: 0.816 0.608; fellow eye viewing: 1.18 6 0.648; amblyopic eyeviewing: 0.77 6 0.258), which was comparable to visuallynormal participants (binocular viewing: 0.94 6 0.648; domi-nant eye viewing: 0.83 6 0.378; nondominant eye viewing:0.94 6 0.528) and to patients with strabismus withoutamblyopia (binocular viewing: 1.27 6 0.768; dominant eyeviewing: 1.21 6 0.738; nondominant eye viewing: 1.16 60.578).

There was no significant difference among patients withstrabismus without amblyopia with and without stereopsis forsaccade amplitude in any viewing condition. There was norelation between the extent of eye deviation and amplitude orprecision in both patient groups.

Peak Velocity. There was a significant main effect of targetlocation for saccade peak velocity (F1,38¼712.87; P < 0.0001).Saccades to the 108 target had higher peak velocity than thoseto the 58 target in all experimental conditions for visuallynormal participants and all patients. There was also a maineffect of viewing condition (F2,76 ¼ 4.67; P ¼ 0.012) for peakvelocity. Saccades had higher peak velocity during binocularviewing compared with monocular viewing, which was mostevident for saccades to the 108 target in all groups, except forpatients with strabismus without amblyopia and negativestereopsis (Table 2).

There was no significant difference among patients withstrabismus without amblyopia with and without stereopsis forpeak velocity in any viewing condition. There was no relationbetween the extent of eye deviation and peak velocity in bothpatient groups.

Secondary Corrective Saccades

Frequency. The overall frequency of corrective saccadeswas greater in patients with strabismic amblyopia (16.3%) andpatients with strabismus without amblyopia (15.1%) comparedwith visually normal participants (13.5%; v2

[df¼2] ¼ 8.61, P ¼0.013). However, there was no difference in the frequency ofsecondary saccades among viewing conditions for patientswith strabismic amblyopia (binocular viewing: 6.3%, fellow eyeviewing: 5.6%, amblyopic eye viewing: 4.4%), patients withstrabismus without amblyopia (binocular viewing: 5.8%;dominant eye viewing: 4.3%; nondominant eye viewing:4.9%), and visually normal participants (binocular viewing:4.1%; dominant eye viewing: 4.8%; nondominant eye viewing:4.6%).

Subgroup analysis of patients with amblyopia showed asignificant frequency difference in corrective saccade frequen-cy (v2

[df¼2]¼ 49.00, P < 0.001). Specifically, patients with mildTA

BLE

2.

Mean

Pri

mar

ySa

ccad

eA

mp

litu

de

and

Peak

Velo

cit

y

58

Targ

et

108

Targ

et

Bin

ocu

lar

Do

min

an

t

(Fell

ow

)E

ye

No

nd

om

inan

t

(Am

bly

op

ic)

Eye

Bin

ocu

lar

Do

min

an

t

(Fell

ow

)E

ye

No

nd

om

inan

t

(Am

bly

op

ic)

Eye

Am

pl,

8

Peak

Velo

cit

y,

deg/s

Am

pl,

8

Peak

Velo

cit

y,

deg/s

Am

pl,

8

Peak

Velo

cit

y,

deg/s

Am

pl,

8

Peak

Velo

cit

y,

deg/s

Am

pl,

8

Peak

Velo

cit

y,

deg/s

Am

pl,

8

Peak

Velo

cit

y,

deg/s

Vis

ual

lyn

orm

al4

.87

60

.29

23

96

28

4.6

76

0.2

22

29

62

74

.78

60

.25

23

16

28

9.4

56

0.3

73

49

64

89

.31

60

.67

32

86

49

9.3

56

0.4

33

38

64

2

Pat

ien

tsw

ith

stra

bis

mic

amb

lyo

pia

Mild

–st

ere

4.7

86

0.1

52

51

62

95

.34

61

.23

25

56

45

4.7

86

0.3

42

45

62

89

.54

60

.33

37

76

27

8.9

06

1.3

73

48

62

99

.67

60

.45

35

86

45

Mild

–st

ere

o�

5.1

66

0.5

52

63

66

44

.76

60

.78

25

36

70

5.0

26

0.8

82

56

68

79

.46

60

.46

37

16

91

8.7

66

1.3

13

44

68

18

.68

61

.09

32

46

98

Seve

re–st

ere

o�

5.1

36

0.3

62

72

65

35

.15

60

.94

25

16

34

4.8

36

0.5

12

46

66

89

.77

60

.48

37

86

74

9.1

46

0.7

33

51

64

69

.47

60

.94

34

06

65

Pat

ien

tsw

ith

stra

bis

mu

sw

ith

ou

tam

bly

op

ia

Stra

bis

mu

s–st

ere

4.6

86

0.1

32

41

63

74

.79

60

.40

23

56

25

4.6

06

0.3

32

32

63

29

.66

60

.71

36

46

48

9.2

16

0.4

63

42

64

59

.59

60

.77

35

16

50

Stra

bis

mu

s–st

ere

o�

4.6

96

0.5

32

42

62

94

.68

60

.21

24

86

50

4.8

36

0.4

92

48

64

28

.47

61

.69

34

26

56

9.1

46

0.6

23

55

63

28

.84

61

.15

34

36

25

7464 Niechwiej-Szwedo et al. IOVS, November 2012, Vol. 53, No. 12

Page 8: Effects of Strabismic Amblyopia and Strabismus without Amblyopia …individual.utoronto.ca/goldentamizhan/73.pdf · 2012-11-20 · Eye Movement, Strabismus, Amblyopia, and Neuro-Ophthalmology

amblyopia and gross stereopsis made fewer secondarysaccades (3.1%), compared with patients with mild amblyopiaand negative stereopsis (6.4%) and patients with severeamblyopia (6.8%). Differences among viewing conditions areshown in Table 3.

Latency. The latency of corrective saccades was compara-ble across all viewing conditions for patients with strabismicamblyopia, patients with strabismus without amblyopia, andvisually normal participants. The subanalysis of patients withamblyopia also yielded no significant differences.

Amplitude and Peak Velocity. There was a significantinteraction between Group and Viewing Condition forcorrective saccade amplitude (F4,62 ¼ 4.49; P ¼ 0.003). Posthoc testing indicated that patients with strabismic amblyopiahad significantly greater secondary saccade amplitudes duringamblyopic eye viewing (2.44 6 1.148) compared withbinocular (1.58 6 0.788) and fellow eye viewing (1.43 6

0.908), which were comparable to visually normal participants(binocular viewing: 1.39 6 0.928; dominant eye viewing: 1.346 0.618; nondominant eye viewing: 1.42 6 0.808) and topatients with strabismus without amblyopia (binocular view-ing: 1.72 6 0.928; dominant eye viewing: 1.37 6 0.668;nondominant eye viewing: 1.47 6 0.458). The interactionbetween Group and Viewing Condition for corrective saccadepeak velocity did not reach significance (F4,62 ¼ 1.97; P ¼0.111).

Further analysis of patients with amblyopia revealed a maineffect of Viewing Condition for secondary saccade amplitude(F2,11 ¼ 10.03; P ¼ 0.001) and peak velocity (F2,11 ¼ 4.70; P ¼0.024). Data presented in Table 3 indicate that patients withnegative stereopsis had significantly larger amplitude and peakvelocity of secondary saccades during amblyopic eye viewingcompared with binocular and fellow eye viewing.

Amplitude Variability after Secondary Saccade. Thecomparison between patients with amblyopia, patients withstrabismus without amblyopia, and visually normal participantswas not statistically significant. The mean precision of the finalamplitude after the secondary saccade was less than 18 for allgroups (range: 0.65–0.988). In contrast, analysis of patientswith amblyopia revealed a main effect of Subgroup (F2,11 ¼13.70; P ¼ 0.001). Post hoc analysis indicated that amplitudevariability after secondary saccade was larger in patients withmild amblyopia and negative stereopsis compared with theother groups in all viewing conditions, with the exception ofpatients with severe amblyopia during amblyopic eye viewing.There was also an interaction between Subgroup and ViewingCondition (F4,22¼ 8.91; P¼ 0.0002; Fig. 4B). Post hoc analysisindicated that patients with severe amblyopia had significantlylower precision (i.e., increased variability) of the finalamplitude after secondary saccade during amblyopic eyeviewing (1.33 6 0.228) compared with binocular (0.77 6

0.288) and fellow eye viewing (0.50 6 0.168), as well as with

TABLE 3. Frequency and Kinematics of Secondary Saccades for Patients with Strabismic Amblyopia

Mild�Stereoþ (n* ¼ 79) Mild–Stereo� (n* ¼ 163) Severe–Stereo� (n* ¼ 173)

Binocular

Fellow

Eye

Amblyopic

Eye Binocular

Fellow

Eye

Amblyopic

Eye Binocular

Fellow

Eye

Amblyopic

Eye

Frequency, % 5.0 2.0 2.4 6.5 3.9 7.3 7.5 11.4 3.0

Latency, ms 209 6 24 225 6 17 215 6 19 206 6 28 213 6 20 202 6 25 198 6 40 192 6 27 146 6 29

Amplitude, 8 1.65 6 0.71 1.27 6 0.24 1.78 6 0.59 1.90 6 0.83 1.78 6 1.30 2.79 6 0.96† 1.16 6 0.60 1.17 6 0.49 2.64 6 1.85†

Peak velocity, deg/s 122 6 43 102 6 24 131 6 39 125 6 43 112 6 48 161 6 48† 100 6 57 95 6 42 153 6 97†

* Total number of trials that contained secondary saccades in each group of patients.† Saccade amplitude and peak velocity were greater during amblyopic eye viewing for patients with amblyopia with negative stereopsis.

FIGURE 4. The precision (i.e., variability) of primary saccade amplitude across viewing conditions. (A) Patients with amblyopia and negativestereopsis (both mild [shown in green] and severe [shown in blue] acuity deficits) had significantly reduced primary saccade amplitude precision(i.e., greater SD) during amblyopic eye viewing (P < 0.0001) compared with patients with mild amblyopia and gross stereopsis (shown in red). (B)Only patients with severe amblyopia and negative stereopsis had lower final saccade amplitude precision after executing a secondary saccade,whereas patients with mild acuity deficits attained precision comparable to that of visually normal participants after executing a secondary saccade(P¼ 0.0002).

IOVS, November 2012, Vol. 53, No. 12 Saccadic Eye Movements in Strabismic Amblyopia 7465

Page 9: Effects of Strabismic Amblyopia and Strabismus without Amblyopia …individual.utoronto.ca/goldentamizhan/73.pdf · 2012-11-20 · Eye Movement, Strabismus, Amblyopia, and Neuro-Ophthalmology

patients with mild amblyopia with and without stereopsis, asillustrated in Figure 4B.

DISCUSSION

This study examined saccades in patients with strabismicamblyopia and compared their performance to that of patientswith strabismus without amblyopia and visually normalparticipants. The major findings are: (1) patients withstrabismic amblyopia and patients with strabismus withoutamblyopia showed no binocular advantage for saccadeinitiation; (2) latency and amplitude of primary saccades wereaffected by the severity of amblyopia and the presence ofstereopsis; (3) amblyopic patients without stereopsis initiatedsecondary saccades more frequently compared with visuallynormal participants. These secondary saccades improved thefinal precision of saccade amplitude; however, the precisionremained significantly worse in patients with severe amblyopiaduring amblyopic eye viewing; and (4) these findings wereunique to patients with strabismic amblyopia, because theywere not observed in patients with strabismus withoutamblyopia.

Patients Lack Binocular Advantage for SaccadeInitiation

Binocular advantage refers to improvements in performance(e.g., visual acuity, contrast sensitivity, and detection of dimstimuli) during binocular viewing compared with monocularviewing in normal people.29 It has been proposed that whensensory signals from the eyes are summated, uncorrelatedsignals (i.e., noise) cancel out and correlated signals areamplified (binocular summation).29,30 This binocular summa-tion, however, is impaired in patients with amblyopia. Levi andcolleagues31 found no improvement in contrast sensitivityduring binocular viewing compared with fellow eye viewing inthree patients with strabismic amblyopia and one patient withanisometropic amblyopia. Our findings provide additionalevidence that patients with strabismic amblyopia lack binoc-ular advantage in their oculomotor system; their saccadelatency was comparable during binocular and fellow eyeviewing.

We also found slightly higher peak velocity during binocularviewing compared with that during monocular viewing, whichwas evident in all groups, except for patients with strabismuswithout amblyopia and negative stereopsis. The difference wasstatistically significant, but it was quite small (~5% for 108saccades). Although we cannot explain this effect at present,we will continue to investigate it in the future by examiningthe saccade main sequence in patients to determine whetherviewing binocularly or monocularly affects the saturationvelocity of saccades.

Effects of Severity of Amblyopia and Stereopsis

Although the number of patients with strabismic amblyopia ineach subgroup was small in the current study, we foundsignificant differences in saccade latency and precision amongpatients with different levels of visual acuity and stereoacuitydeficits. With respect to acuity deficit, our analysis yielded twoimportant findings. First, saccade latency was prolonged onlyin patients with a severe acuity deficit (20/200) but not inpatients with a mild deficit (�20/60) during amblyopic eyeviewing. These results are in contrast to those in patients withanisometropic amblyopia who had significantly longer saccadelatencies during amblyopic eye viewing, irrespective ofwhether they had a mild or severe acuity deficit, using the

same criteria and experimental paradigm.16,32 Second, patientswith strabismic amblyopia and severe acuity deficits experi-enced more difficulty orienting to targets closer to centralfixation (i.e., the 58 vs. 108 target). This is in contrast topatients with anisometropic amblyopia whose saccade laten-cies were not affected by Target Location.16,32 Our current andprevious results16,32 can be interpreted as the motor conse-quences of different long-term sensory suppression mecha-nisms in strabismic versus anisometropic amblyopia.Suppression of the central field helps to eliminate centraldiplopia arising from eye misalignment and allows some degreeof peripheral fusion. In contrast, the prolonged saccade latencythat is independent of Target Location16,32 in patients withanisometropic amblyopia is consistent with sensory suppres-sion of a blurred image across the entire visual field.20

Interestingly, we found no correlation between the amountof strabismus and saccade latency for different target locations.A larger sample size is required for a more robust correlationanalysis.

The prolonged saccade latency for a more centrally locatedtarget is consistent with stronger sensory suppression of thecentral visual field in strabismic amblyopia as found in bothhumans33 and cats.34 This pattern of behavior is not likely to bedue to interocular suppression because longer latency wasfound only when patients were viewing with the amblyopiceye and not during binocular viewing. However, it is possiblethat the chronic suppression of the deviated eye duringbinocular viewing extends to the monocular viewing condi-tions.35 A previous brain imaging study36 showed a lower levelof cortical activation during foveal stimulation of the amblyopiceye, and may explain the longer saccade latency to morecentrally located targets.

A second important finding is that patients with negativestereopsis had significantly reduced precision of primarysaccades during amblyopic eye viewing, regardless of thevisual acuity deficit. In particular, we found that saccadeamplitude precision was significantly worse in patients withstrabismic amblyopia and negative stereopsis compared withpatients who had gross stereopsis and a similar acuity deficit. Itis possible that despite having good acuity in the amblyopiceye, the signal from the amblyopic eye in patients withnegative stereopsis may remain under suppression duringnatural binocular viewing, which also habitually extends tomonocular amblyopic eye viewing during the brief experimen-tal period. Our findings are consistent with a recentneuroimaging study37 on patients with strabismic amblyopia,which showed that the decrease in neural activation in V1/V2during amblyopic eye stimulation was dependent on thesuppressive effects of the fellow eye: activity was morereduced when the fellow eye was open than when it wasclosed. Taken together, our findings and those of others37

suggest that the amblyopic eye is under chronic suppressionduring both monocular and binocular viewing, albeit to adifferent extent.

Previous studies have used perceptual and psychophysicaltasks to demonstrate spatial localization deficits in ambly-opia.20,38,39 Patients showed a systematic localization bias inthe direction of the deviated eye, and exhibited increasedspatial uncertainty that was more pronounced in central visioncompared with the periphery. Using a saccade task, weprovide additional evidence that patients with amblyopia havedeficits in spatial localization as shown by increased variability(i.e., reduced precision) in primary saccade amplitudeespecially during amblyopic eye viewing. We also observed agradation of effects of strabismic amblyopia: in patients with asevere acuity deficit (and negative stereopsis), both detection(i.e., longer saccade latency) and localization (i.e., increasedsaccade amplitude variability) deficits were evident. In patients

7466 Niechwiej-Szwedo et al. IOVS, November 2012, Vol. 53, No. 12

Page 10: Effects of Strabismic Amblyopia and Strabismus without Amblyopia …individual.utoronto.ca/goldentamizhan/73.pdf · 2012-11-20 · Eye Movement, Strabismus, Amblyopia, and Neuro-Ophthalmology

with mild amblyopia and negative stereopsis, a localizationdeficit was evident but not a detection deficit (i.e., normalsaccade latency), whereas in those with mild amblyopia andgross stereopsis, no localization or detection deficit waspresent. The distinct pattern found in patients with mildamblyopia and negative stereopsis could be explained by aspeed–accuracy tradeoff: saccades were initiated with normallatency but with a greater than normal scatter of landingpositions. This behavior might have led to a large inaccuracy;however, the errors in primary saccade amplitude werecorrected by secondary saccades as discussed in the followingtext.

Secondary Saccades

Primary saccades have a tendency to undershoot the target byapproximately 10%.40,41 Secondary saccades are initiated tocorrect the amplitude error remaining after the primarysaccade. Two sources of error feedback have been proposedfor the generation of secondary saccades.40,42,43 One is basedon extraretinal information derived from the efference copy ofthe oculomotor command. Another is based on retinalfeedback derived from the position of the target image onthe retina at the end of the primary saccade.

Our data show two important findings. First, only patientswith amblyopia (mild and severe) and negative stereopsisinitiated secondary saccades more frequently. Second, saccadesinitiated by these patients with negative stereopsis duringamblyopic eye viewing had higher amplitudes and peakvelocities. Since we also found that these patients had lowerprimary saccade amplitude precision, these secondary sac-cades were initiated to correct the amplitude error remainingafter the primary saccade. Patients with mild amblyopia wereable to correct the error significantly more compared withpatients with severe amblyopia, as indicated by improved finalamplitude precision after the secondary saccade. These resultscan be interpreted by considering that the retinal feedback ismore impaired in patients with severe amblyopia. Consequent-ly, patients with less reliable retinal position error signals mayneed a larger visual error signal before initiating a secondarysaccade, which in turn leads to higher amplitude and, thus,peak velocity of the corrective eye movement. In addition, ourdata indicate that the error detection process is impaired inpatients with severe amblyopia because they made significantlyfewer secondary saccades during amblyopic eye viewing. Ourfindings highlight the importance of a reliable retinal feedbacksignal for the initiation of secondary corrective saccades.

In conclusion, our study adds to the growing body ofliterature that recognizes amblyopia as a heterogeneousdisorder of both visual and visuomotor functions.13,44–47 Wefound a distinct pattern of deficits in patients with strabismicamblyopia that, unlike that in anisometropic amblyopia, wasdependent on the level of visual acuity and stereoacuity losses.Gross stereopsis was associated with better saccade perfor-mance in terms of detection and localization, regardless ofviewing condition. In addition, these deficits were unique topatients with strabismic amblyopia, because they were notfound in patients with strabismus without amblyopia. Ourresults suggest that the evaluation of the effectiveness oftreatment regimens for amblyopia should also consider motorimprovement, rather than solely on visual acuity.

References

1. Daw NW. Visual Development. 2nd ed. New York: Springer;2006:145–164.

2. Hensch TK. Critical period plasticity in local cortical circuits.Nat Rev Neurosci. 2005;6:877–888.

3. Sireteanu R, Baumer CC, Sarbu C, Iftime A. Spatial andtemporal misperceptions in amblyopic vision. Strabismus.2007;15:45–54.

4. Simmers AJ, Ledgeway T, Mansouri B, Hutchinson CV, Hess RF.The extent of the dorsal extra-striate deficit in amblyopia.Vision Res. 2006;46:2571–2580.

5. Simmers AJ, Ledgeway T, Hess RF, McGraw PV. Deficits toglobal motion processing in human amblyopia. Vision Res.2003;43:729–738.

6. Holopigian K, Blake R, Greenwald MJ. Selective losses inbinocular vision in anisometropic amblyopes. Vision Res.1986;26:621–630.

7. Ho CS, Giaschi DE, Boden C, Dougherty R, Cline R, Lyons C.Deficient motion perception in the fellow eye of amblyopicchildren. Vision Res. 2005;45:1615–1627.

8. Hess RF, Wang YZ, Demanins R, Wilkinson F, Wilson HR. Adeficit in strabismic amblyopia for global shape detection.Vision Res. 1999;39:901–914.

9. Bonneh YS, Sagi D, Polat U. Spatial and temporal crowding inamblyopia. Vision Res. 2007;47:1950–1962.

10. Barrett BT, Pacey IE, Bradley A, Thibos LN, Morrill P.Nonveridical visual perception in human amblyopia. Invest

Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2003;44:1555–1567.

11. Woo GC, Irving EL. The non-amblyopic eye of a unilateralamblyope: a unique entity. Clin Exp Optom. 1991;74:1–5.

12. Mirabella G, Hay S, Wong AM. Deficits in perception of real-world scenes in patients with a history of amblyopia. Arch

Ophthalmol. 2011;129:176–183.

13. McKee SP, Levi DM, Movshon JA. The pattern of visual deficitsin amblyopia. J Vis. 2003;3:380–405.

14. Irving DE. Integrating information across saccadic eyemovements. Curr Direct Psychol Sci. 1996;5:94–100.

15. Prime SL, Vesia M, Crawford JD. Cortical mechanisms for trans-saccadic memory and integration of multiple object features.Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2011;366:540–553.

16. Niechwiej-Szwedo E, Goltz HC, Chandrakumar M, Hirji ZA,Wong AM. Effects of anisometropic amblyopia on visuomotorbehavior, I: saccadic eye movements. Invest Ophthalmol Vis

Sci. 2010;51:6348–6354.

17. Niechwiej-Szwedo E, Goltz HC, Chandrakumar M, Wong AM.The effect of sensory uncertainty due to amblyopia (lazy eye)on the planning and execution of visually-guided 3D reachingmovements. PLoS ONE. 2012;7:e31075.

18. Niechwiej-Szwedo E, Goltz HC, Chandrakumar M, Hirji Z,Crawford JD, Wong AM. Effects of anisometropic amblyopia onvisuomotor behavior, part 2: visually-guided reaching. Invest

Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2011;52:795–803.

19. Niechwiej-Szwedo E, Goltz HC, Chandrakumar M, Hirji Z,Wong AM. Effects of anisometropic amblyopia on visuomotorbehavior, III: temporal eye–hand coordination during reach-ing. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2011;52:5853–5861.

20. Hess RF, Holliday IE. The spatial localization deficit inamblyopia. Vision Res. 1992;32:1319–1339.

21. Schor C. A directional impairment of eye movement control instrabismus amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol. 1975;14:692–697.

22. Ciuffreda KJ, Kenyon RV, Stark L. Increased saccadic latencies inamblyopic eyes. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1978;17:697–702.

23. Bucci MP, Kapoula Z, Yang Q, Bremond-Gignac D. Latency ofsaccades, vergence, and combined movements in childrenwith early onset convergent or divergent strabismus. Vision

Res. 2006;46:1384–1392.

24. Kapoula Z, Bucci P. Distribution-dependent saccades inchildren with strabismus and in normals. Exp Brain Res.2002;143:264–268.

25. Kapoula Z, Bucci MP, Eggert T, Garraud L. Impairment of thebinocular coordination of saccades in strabismus. Vision Res.1997;37:2757–2766.

IOVS, November 2012, Vol. 53, No. 12 Saccadic Eye Movements in Strabismic Amblyopia 7467

Page 11: Effects of Strabismic Amblyopia and Strabismus without Amblyopia …individual.utoronto.ca/goldentamizhan/73.pdf · 2012-11-20 · Eye Movement, Strabismus, Amblyopia, and Neuro-Ophthalmology

26. Sainburg RL. Handedness: differential specializations forcontrol of trajectory and position. Exerc Sport Sci Rev. 2005;33:206–213.

27. Dolman P. Tests for determining sighting of the eye. Am J

Ophthalmol. 1919;2:287.

28. Conover WJ, Iman RL. Analysis of covariance using the ranktransformation. Biometrics. 1982;38:715–724.

29. Howard IP. Basic Mechanisms. Seeing in Depth. Toronto:University of Toronto Press; 2002:319.

30. Campbell FW, Green DG. Monocular versus binocular visualacuity. Nature. 1965;208:191–192.

31. Levi DM, Harwerth RS, Manny RE. Suprathreshold spatialfrequency detection and binocular interaction in strabismicand anisometropic amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci.1979;18:714–725.

32. Niechwiej-Szwedo E, Goltz HC, Wong AM. Deficits andadaptation of eye–hand coordination during visually-guidedreaching movements in people with amblyopia. In: Steeves J,Harris LR, eds. Plasticity in Sensory Systems. Cambridge, UK:Cambridge University Press; In press:3–27.

33. Sireteanu R, Fronius M. Naso-temporal asymmetries in humanamblyopia consequence of long-term interocular suppression.Vision Res. 1981;21:1055–1063.

34. Harrad R, Sengpiel F, Blakemore C. Physiology of suppressionin strabismic amblyopia. Br J Ophthalmol. 1996;80:373–377.

35. Li J, Thompson B, Lam CS, et al. The role of suppression inamblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2011;52:4169–4176.

36. Conner IP, Odom JV, Schwartz TL, Mendola JD. Retinotopicmaps and foveal suppression in the visual cortex of amblyopicadults. J Physiol. 2007;583:159–173.

37. Farivar R, Thompson B, Mansouri B, Hess RF. Interocularsuppression in strabismic amblyopia results in an attenuated

and delayed hemodynamic response function in early visualcortex. J Vis. 2011;11:1–12.

38. Bedell HE, Flom MC, Barbeito R. Spatial aberrations and acuityin strabismus and amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1985;26:909–916.

39. Fronius M, Sireteanu R. Monocular geometry is selectivelydistorted in the central visual field of strabismic amblyopes.Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 1989;30:2034–2044.

40. Becker W, Fuchs AF. Further properties of the human saccadicsystem: eye movements and correction saccades with andwithout visual fixation points. Vision Res. 1969;9:1247–1258.

41. Henson DB. Corrective saccades: effects of altering visualfeedback. Vision Res. 1978;18:63–67.

42. Prablanc C, Masse D, Echallier JF. Error-correcting mechanismsin large saccades. Vision Res. 1978;18:557–560.

43. Weber RB, Daroff RB. Corrective movements followingrefixation saccades: type and control system analysis. Vision

Res. 1972;12:467–475.

44. Grant S, Melmoth DR, Morgan MJ, Finlay AL. Prehensiondeficits in amblyopia. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2007;48:1139–1148.

45. Grant S, Moseley MJ. Amblyopia and real-world visuomotortasks. Strabismus. 2011;19:119–128.

46. Ho CS, Giaschi DE. Stereopsis-dependent deficits in maximummotion displacement in strabismic and anisometropic ambly-opia. Vision Res. 2007;47:2778–2785.

47. Agrawal R, Conner IP, Odom JV, Schwartz TL, Mendola JD.Relating binocular and monocular vision in strabismic andanisometropic amblyopia. Arch Ophthalmol. 2006;124:844–850.

7468 Niechwiej-Szwedo et al. IOVS, November 2012, Vol. 53, No. 12