effect of stress and glucose on self-control mary redding and h. anna han, phd department of...

1
Effect of Stress and Glucose on Self-Control Mary Redding and H. Anna Han, PhD Department of Psychology, St. Mary’s College of Maryland References Conclusions Results Introduction Methods * Acknowledgements Special thanks to Dr. Anna H. Han and Angela Draheim for all their help and support. Self-control is the effortful process of refraining from behaviors that may be initially rewarding, but have long- term costs. 1,2,6 Ego Depletion explains that a person’s self-control depends on a limited cognitive resource, which can be depleted after use. 1,2,3,4,6 Stress is a factor that can contribute to self-control failures by consuming the resource required for self- control. 5,6 Controlled mental processes (such as self-control) require more glucose than automatic processes. 3 Specifically, an increase in blood glucose levels resulted in fewer self-control errors after an initial ego depletion task. 3,4 Present Research Further examined the depleting effect of stress as well as the attenuating role of glucose on self-control. 2 (stress/no stress) x 2 (glucose/ no glucose) design 1. Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Tice, D. M. (2007). The strength model of self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(6), 351-355. 2. Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 1252-1265. 3. Gailliot, M. T., Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Maner, J. K., Plant, E. A., Tice, D. M., et al. (2007). Self-control relies on glucose as a limited energy source: Willpower is more than a metaphor. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(2), 325-336. 4. Masicampo, E. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (2008). Toward a physiology of dual-process reasoning and judgment: Lemonade, willpower, and expensive rule-based analysis. Psychological Science, 19(3), 255-260. 5. Mayzner, M. S., & Tresselt, M. E. (1966). Anagram solution times: A function of multiple-solution anagrams. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(1), 66-73. 6. Oaten, M., & Cheng, K. (2005). Academic examination stress impairs self- control. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 24(2), 254-279. Hypotheses Participants under stress would have worse self-control performance (slower Stroop RT) compared to those not under stress. Participants who receive sugar will have better self-control performance (faster Stroop RT) than those who do not. Stress + no sugar=worst self-control No stress + sugar= best self-control. Participants 58 (45 female, 13 male) students from St. Mary’s College of Maryland. Students were excluded from participation if they had medical conditions related to sugar or sugar substitutes, such as diabetes. Procedures Participants randomly assigned a condition by session. Participants in the stressful conditions had 4 minutes to complete a solvable anagram task 5 , while those in the non- stressful conditions had 15 minutes to complete the same task. After competing the anagrams, the participants in the glucose conditions received regular “Life Savers” brand candy with sugar, and those in the no glucose conditions received sugar-free version. While participants ate the piece of candy, all participants answered 3 filler questionnaires (were not analyzed) which on average took about 10 minutes (the time to metabolize the sugar). Self-control was measured by reaction times (RT) on a computerized Stroop task, which randomly presented 16 congruent trials and 16 incongruent trials of a color/word pairing to the participants. At the end of the study, participants answered the questions from the 10-item Perceived Stress Scale. Congruent Incongruent Red Red Blue Blue Yellow Yellow Green Green Reaction times (RT) on the Stroop task was calculated by subtracting the average RT on the congruent trials from the incongruent. ANOVA showed there was no main effect of stress on self- control F < 1, nor main effect of glucose on self-control, F < 1. Moreover, there was no interaction between stress and glucose F < 1. By removing the outliers (data with z-scores >4 and <-4) from the data set (n=53), there was no significant main effect of either stress on self-control, F < 1, or glucose on self-control, F < 1. Results failed to support previous research that stress has detrimental effects on self-control and that glucose would have attenuating effects on self-control. Limitations Anagram task as a stress-inducer: The anagrams may have been equally stressful because both stressed and non- stressed conditions had relatively high perceived stress scale scores which were not significantly different between the two conditions. Form of glucose: One piece of candy may not have been enough to produce an effect (previous studies used 8-oz. glass of sugar water). Also, it may take more than 10 minutes to metabolize the sugar. Measure of self-control: The Stroop task is normally used in cognitive studies on selective attention, cognitive flexibility or processing speed, for example. Future Research: Continue to use anagrams to induce stress, but have a control condition where participants are acclimated to the lab setting and/or an activity that does not require controlled mental processing (i.e. reading a magazine). • Use a larger or more efficient amount of sugar than the one piece of Life Savers’ candy. Stress No stress 0 50 100 150 200 250 Glucose No Glucose Mean RT (ms) Figure 1. The mean Stroop RT scores in each condition excluding outliers. Error bars represent 1 standard error of the mean.

Upload: eileen-carpenter

Post on 12-Jan-2016

212 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Effect of Stress and Glucose on Self-Control Mary Redding and H. Anna Han, PhD Department of Psychology, St. Mary’s College of Maryland References Conclusions

Effect of Stress and Glucose on Self-ControlMary Redding and H. Anna Han, PhD

Department of Psychology, St. Mary’s College of Maryland

References

Conclusions

ResultsIntroduction

Methods

*

AcknowledgementsSpecial thanks to Dr. Anna H. Han and Angela Draheim for all their help and support.

• Self-control is the effortful process of refraining from behaviors that may be initially rewarding, but have long-term costs.1,2,6

• Ego Depletion explains that a person’s self-control depends on a limited cognitive resource, which can be depleted after use. 1,2,3,4,6

• Stress is a factor that can contribute to self-control failures by consuming the resource required for self-control. 5,6

• Controlled mental processes (such as self-control) require more glucose than automatic processes.3

• Specifically, an increase in blood glucose levels resulted in fewer self-control errors after an initial ego depletion task. 3,4

Present Research• Further examined the depleting effect of stress as well as the attenuating role of

glucose on self-control.

• 2 (stress/no stress) x 2 (glucose/ no glucose) design

1. Baumeister, R. F., Vohs, K. D., & Tice, D. M. (2007). The strength model of self-control. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(6), 351-355.

2. Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Muraven, M., & Tice, D. M. (1998). Ego depletion: Is the active self a limited resource? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(5), 1252-1265.

3. Gailliot, M. T., Baumeister, R. F., DeWall, C. N., Maner, J. K., Plant, E. A., Tice, D. M., et al. (2007). Self-control relies on glucose as a limited energy source: Willpower is more than a metaphor. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(2), 325-336.

4. Masicampo, E. J., & Baumeister, R. F. (2008). Toward a physiology of dual-process reasoning and judgment: Lemonade, willpower, and expensive rule-based analysis. Psychological Science, 19(3), 255-260.

5. Mayzner, M. S., & Tresselt, M. E. (1966). Anagram solution times: A function of multiple-solution anagrams. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 71(1), 66-73.

6. Oaten, M., & Cheng, K. (2005). Academic examination stress impairs self-control. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 24(2), 254-279.

7. Tice, D. M., & Baumeister, R. F. (1997). Longitudinal study of procrastination, performance, stress, and health: The costs and benefits of dawdling. Psychological Science, 8, 454-45.

Hypotheses• Participants under stress would have worse self-control

performance (slower Stroop RT) compared to those not under stress.

• Participants who receive sugar will have better self-control performance (faster Stroop RT) than those who do not.

• Stress + no sugar=worst self-control• No stress + sugar= best self-control.

Participants• 58 (45 female, 13 male) students from St. Mary’s College of Maryland.

• Students were excluded from participation if they had medical conditions related to sugar or sugar substitutes, such as diabetes.

Procedures• Participants randomly assigned a condition by session.

• Participants in the stressful conditions had 4 minutes to complete a solvable anagram task5, while those in the non-stressful conditions had 15 minutes to complete the same task.

• After competing the anagrams, the participants in the glucose conditions received regular “Life Savers” brand candy with sugar, and those in the no glucose conditions received sugar-free version.

• While participants ate the piece of candy, all participants answered 3 filler questionnaires (were not analyzed) which on average took about 10 minutes (the time to metabolize the sugar).

• Self-control was measured by reaction times (RT) on a computerized Stroop task, which randomly presented 16 congruent trials and 16 incongruent trials of a color/word pairing to the participants.

• At the end of the study, participants answered the questions from the 10-item Perceived Stress Scale.

Congruent IncongruentRed RedBlue Blue

Yellow YellowGreen Green

• Reaction times (RT) on the Stroop task was calculated by subtracting the average RT on the congruent trials from the incongruent.

• ANOVA showed there was no main effect of stress on self-control F < 1, nor main effect of glucose on self-control, F < 1. Moreover, there was no interaction between stress and glucose F < 1.

• By removing the outliers (data with z-scores >4 and <-4) from the data set (n=53), there was no significant main effect of either stress on self-control, F < 1, or glucose on self-control, F < 1.

• Results failed to support previous research that stress has detrimental effects on self-control and that glucose would have attenuating effects on self-control.

Limitations • Anagram task as a stress-inducer: The anagrams may have been equally stressful

because both stressed and non-stressed conditions had relatively high perceived stress scale scores which were not significantly different between the two conditions.

• Form of glucose: One piece of candy may not have been enough to produce an effect (previous studies used 8-oz. glass of sugar water). Also, it may take more than 10 minutes to metabolize the sugar.

• Measure of self-control: The Stroop task is normally used in cognitive studies on selective attention, cognitive flexibility or processing speed, for example.

Future Research:• Continue to use anagrams to induce stress, but have a control condition where

participants are acclimated to the lab setting and/or an activity that does not require controlled mental processing (i.e. reading a magazine).

• Use a larger or more efficient amount of sugar than the one piece of Life Savers’ candy.

Stress No stress0

50

100

150

200

250GlucoseNo Glucose

Mea

n RT

(ms)

Figure 1. The mean Stroop RT scores in each condition excluding outliers. Error bars represent 1 standard error of the mean.