default mode network in depression: a pathway to impaired affective cognition?

5
Clinical Neuropsychiatry (2013) 10, 5, 212-216 DEFAULT MODE NETWORK IN DEPRESSION: A PATHWAY TO IMPAIRED AFFECTIVE COGNITION? Fabio Sambataro, Nadine Donata Wolf, Pietro Giusti, Nenad Vasic, Robert Christian Wolf Abstract Major depressive disorder (MDD) is associated with alterations in multiple brain networks including the default mode network (DMN). The DMN is a set of large-scale connected brain regions that oscillate synchronously and are more active during rest relative to goal-directed tasks. Several functions have been hypothesized for the DMN, including cognitive, self-referential and social functions. Widespread alterations in the core and the subsystems of the DMN as well as in their interplay have been reported in MDD. We will review the evidence supporting DMN alterations in patients with MDD and underscore how they can contribute to social and affective cognitive impairments in this disorder. Key words: default mode network, cognition, emotion, major depressive disorder, functional magnetic resonance imaging Declaration of interest: all the authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest relevant to this manuscript. Fabio Sambataro 1,* , Nadine Donata Wolf 2 , Pietro Giusti 3 , Nenad Vasic 4 , Robert Christian Wolf 5 1 Brain Center for Motor and Social Cognition, Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia@UniPR, Parma, Italy; 2 Department of Addictive Behavior and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Mannheim, Germany; 3 Department of Scienze del Farmaco, Universita’ degli Studi di Padova, Padova, Italy; 4 Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy III, Ulm University, Ulm, Germany; 5 Center of Psychosocial Medicine, Department of General Psychiatry, University of Heidelberg, Germany. Corresponding author Fabio Sambataro MD, PhD Brain Center for Motor and Social Cognition, Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia@UniPR, Via Universita’ 12, Parma, Italy. Phone: +39-0521-903963; Fax: +39-0521-033900. E-mail: [email protected] Introduction Major depressive disorder (MDD) is a common (17% lifetime prevalence in the U.S., Andrade et al. 2003) and severe psychiatric disorder with disabling consequences, which make this disorder as the foremost contributor to the global burden of disease (Whiteford et al. 2013). MDD entails a multifaceted pattern of symptoms, which span across multiple domains including mood, cognition, motor and vegetative functions. Consistent with the heterogeneity of symptoms in MDD, studies investigating the neural correlates of this disorder (Vasic et al. 2009) have suggested that brain networks rather than individual regions could underlie the pathogenesis of MDD (Northoff et al. 2011). In particular, recent neuroscientific and clinical interest sparked on the role of the default mode network (DMN). In this paper, we propose that altered DMN can contribute to the development of MDD symptoms, which also include altered affective cognition. To set the stage for this proposal, we will illustrate the anatomical boundaries and the functional characterization of the DMN. Next, the article describes the evidence of altered DMN in MDD. Then, we will illustrate how DMN alterations in MDD could result in impairments of affective cognition. The Default-mode network: anatomy and function The DMN is a large-scale interacting brain system, which includes a set of brain regions whose activity is relatively increased during rest and is attenuated during a goal-directed behavior (Raichle et al. 2001). The DMN was originally identified during functional neuroimaging tasks by looking at those brain areas, whose activity increased during rest relative to task performance, so-called “task-induced deactivations”, TID (Raichle et al. 2001). Recently, resting state scanning which is an experimental setting where the participant is not required to perform any goal-directed tasks was introduced as more reliable approach to estimate spatial and temporal characteristics of the DMN. Among several techniques, seeded-correlation with the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) allows the estimation of the network of brain regions whose signal fluctuates synchronously, which includes the DMN. Alternatively, model-free analyses such as independent component analysis can allow the decomposition of the imaging data in spatio-temporal patterns (Sambataro et al. 2010), which could reflect resting state networks including the DMN. Irrespective of the methodological approach, imaging studies on the DMN converge on a similar 212 SUBMITTED OCTOBER 2013, ACCEPTED OCTOBER 2013 © 2013 Giovanni Fioriti Editore s.r.l.

Upload: others

Post on 24-Mar-2022

4 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Clinical Neuropsychiatry (2013) 10, 5, 212-216

DEFAULT MODE NETWORK IN DEPRESSION: A PATHWAY TO IMPAIRED AFFECTIVE COGNITION?

Fabio Sambataro, Nadine Donata Wolf, Pietro Giusti, Nenad Vasic, Robert Christian Wolf

Abstract

Major depressive disorder (MDD) is associated with alterations in multiple brain networks including the default mode network (DMN). The DMN is a set of large-scale connected brain regions that oscillate synchronously and are more active during rest relative to goal-directed tasks. Several functions have been hypothesized for the DMN, including cognitive, self-referential and social functions. Widespread alterations in the core and the subsystems of the DMN as well as in their interplay have been reported in MDD. We will review the evidence supporting DMN alterations in patients with MDD and underscore how they can contribute to social and affective cognitive impairments in this disorder.

Key words: default mode network, cognition, emotion, major depressive disorder, functional magnetic resonance imaging

Declaration of interest: all the authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest relevant to this manuscript.

Fabio Sambataro1,*, Nadine Donata Wolf2, Pietro Giusti3, Nenad Vasic4, Robert Christian Wolf5

1Brain Center for Motor and Social Cognition, Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia@UniPR, Parma, Italy; 2Department of Addictive Behavior and Addiction Medicine, Central Institute of Mental Health, Mannheim, Germany; 3Department of Scienze del Farmaco, Universita’ degli Studi di Padova, Padova, Italy; 4Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy III, Ulm University, Ulm, Germany; 5Center of Psychosocial Medicine, Department of General Psychiatry, University of Heidelberg, Germany.

Corresponding author

Fabio Sambataro MD, PhDBrain Center for Motor and Social Cognition, Istituto Italiano di Tecnologia@UniPR, Via Universita’ 12, Parma, Italy. Phone: +39-0521-903963; Fax: +39-0521-033900. E-mail: [email protected]

IntroductionMajor depressive disorder (MDD) is a common

(17% lifetime prevalence in the U.S., Andrade et al. 2003) and severe psychiatric disorder with disabling consequences, which make this disorder as the foremost contributor to the global burden of disease (Whiteford et al. 2013). MDD entails a multifaceted pattern of symptoms, which span across multiple domains including mood, cognition, motor and vegetative functions. Consistent with the heterogeneity of symptoms in MDD, studies investigating the neural correlates of this disorder (Vasic et al. 2009) have suggested that brain networks rather than individual regions could underlie the pathogenesis of MDD (Northoff et al. 2011). In particular, recent neuroscientific and clinical interest sparked on the role of the default mode network (DMN). In this paper, we propose that altered DMN can contribute to the development of MDD symptoms, which also include altered affective cognition. To set the stage for this proposal, we will illustrate the anatomical boundaries and the functional characterization of the DMN. Next, the article describes the evidence of altered DMN in MDD. Then, we will illustrate how DMN alterations in MDD could result in impairments of affective cognition.

The Default-mode network: anatomy and function

The DMN is a large-scale interacting brain system, which includes a set of brain regions whose activity is relatively increased during rest and is attenuated during a goal-directed behavior (Raichle et al. 2001). The DMN was originally identified during functional neuroimaging tasks by looking at those brain areas, whose activity increased during rest relative to task performance, so-called “task-induced deactivations”, TID (Raichle et al. 2001). Recently, resting state scanning which is an experimental setting where the participant is not required to perform any goal-directed tasks was introduced as more reliable approach to estimate spatial and temporal characteristics of the DMN. Among several techniques, seeded-correlation with the posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) allows the estimation of the network of brain regions whose signal fluctuates synchronously, which includes the DMN. Alternatively, model-free analyses such as independent component analysis can allow the decomposition of the imaging data in spatio-temporal patterns (Sambataro et al. 2010), which could reflect resting state networks including the DMN.

Irrespective of the methodological approach, imaging studies on the DMN converge on a similar

212

Submitted OctOber 2013, accepted OctOber 2013

© 2013 Giovanni Fioriti Editore s.r.l.

Hippocampus

mPFC/ACC

TPCx

PreC/PCC

Hippocampus

mPFC/ACC/ACC

PreC/PCC

TPCxTPCxTPCx

HippocampusHippocampus

Figure 1. The Default Mode Network (DMN). Axial slices depict the spatial extent of the DMN estimated using seeded connectivity (with the PCC; left) and using independent component analysis (right). Yellow arrows indicate DMN regions. mPFC/ACC, medial Prefrontal Cortex/ anterior cingulare; PreC/PCC, precuneus/ posterior cingulate; TPCx, lateral temporo-parietal cortex

Default mode in depression: the affective cognition pathway

Clinical Neuropsychiatry (2013) 10, 5 213

spatial pattern (see fi gure 1). The DMN is composed by midline areas organized in an anterior “hub”, which is localized in the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), and a posterior “hub” encompassing the PCC and the precuneus; laterally, this network includes bilateral inferior temporo-parietal cortexes and ventrally, the medial temporal lobes (Andrews-Hanna et al. 2010). Although all these regions oscillate synchronously, recent studies suggest that the DMN could be decomposed in anatomical-functionally separate subsystems (Andrews-Hanna et al. 2010). Most studies subdivide the DMN in a core, comprising the two midline hubs, and distinct subsystems including anterior, posterior, and ventral regions (Uddin et al. 2009; Andrews-Hanna et al. 2010a, 2010b; Sambataro et al. 2010; Whitfi eld-Gabrieli et al. 2011; Damoiseaux et al. 2012; Sambataro et al. 2013). Responses in the brain regions within anterior subsystem have been found during tasks tapping on the theory of mind, mentalizing, moral decision making, social narrative understanding, social reasoning and conceptual processing thus suggesting a role in introspection about mental states. The ventral subsystem has been associated with episodic and autobiographical memory, navigation and imagery that are involved in memory-based simulations. The posterior DMN has been implicated in visual processing, motor planning, cognitive functions, memory retrieval, and also self-perception and internal mentation (Sambataro et al. 2010). The core is implicated in emotional, self-referential processing, mentalizing autobiographical memory, moral decision making, anticipation of value, and cognition which can globally contribute to the evaluation of motivationally salient and personally-signifi cant information (Andrews-Hanna et al. 2010).

This overall picture suggests the possibility that the DMN can have a pivotal role in the processing of affective cognition: fi rst, in the attribution of social/personal value to stimuli, then in the planning of social behavioral responses using memory information via

prospective simulation of social scenarios (Andrews-Hanna 2012). Also, the DMN could interact with other executive networks thus modulating the allocation of attentional resources for the process at hand (Sambataro et al. 2010).

Affective Cognition Affective cognition entails those brain processes

that lie at the interface between cognition and emotion, including emotion perception and recognition, and social and moral reasoning (Elliott et al. 2011). Classical paradigms used to probe this function investigate emotional processing and its interaction with concurrent cognitive demands: implicit and explicit processing of facial stimuli (Sambataro et al. 2006); emotional stimuli perception and recognition during executive control tasks (Schulz et al. 2009); passive and explicit moral evaluation (Moll et al. 2009). The brain regions usually implicated in the affective/cognition function belong to limbic system, such as amygdala, insula, striatum and the hippocampus, and to the cortical systems such as anterior cingulate (ACC) including subgenual cingulate (sgCC), mPFC/orbitofrontal cortex (OFC) and lateral PFC. Among different multi-compartmental models proposed to explain affective cognition, the model by Philips (Phillips et al. 2003, 2008) modifi ed after Ochsner’s PFC theorization (Ochsner et al. 2005, Ochsner et al. 2007) offers an interesting framework for interpreting affective cognition that distinguishes a ventral and a dorsal system. The ventral system includes amygdala, insula, ventral striatum, ventral ACC and ventral mPFC/OFC and mediates emotional appraisal and the generation of emotional states via bottom-up processing. The dorsal system includes hippocampus, dorsal ACC, and PFC with a role in emotional regulation with a top-down control and more specifi cally a dorsal part involved in the reappraisal of emotional contexts and the ventral part critical for

Fabio Sambataro et al.

214 Clinical Neuropsychiatry (2013) 10, 5

learning the associations between emotionally-relevant outcomes and prior events. Interestingly, the midline regions, which spatially overlap with the DMN, are posited to mediate the automatic aspects of emotional regulation (Phillips et al. 2008).

Affective cognition impairments in MDDA number of alterations of affective cognition have

been described in MDD, including reduced sensitivity to emotional faces, cognitive dysfunction, and impaired social and moral reasoning. In particular, cognitive biases have been associated with the development, maintenance and relapse of depression (Gotlib and Jooroman 2010). Patients with MDD have an increased sensitivity for negative emotional stimuli, increased ability to remember negative items (Bradley et al. 1996), increased overgeneralized autobiographical memory, and pathological sense of guilt. These symptoms have been traditionally included in the seminal Beck’s cognitive triad, which comprises a schema, which is a deep cognitive structures, that biases information processing thus causing negative thoughts about abnormal the self, the world and the future (Beck et al. 1979). Interestingly, altered information processing includes attention, memory and prospection that are all functions mediated by the DMN.

DMN alterations in MDDAlterations in the DMN have been associated with

several neurological (Wolf et al. 2012, 2013) and psychiatric disorders (Sambataro et al. 2010, Wolf et al. 2011). Studies in MDD have indicated widespread increases of the DMN activity during both tasks and rest: in the anterior DMN including ACC (both subgenual and perigenual portions) and mPFC, in posterior regions including the PCC and the precuneus, and ventrally in the hippocampal formation and parahippocampus (Broyd et al. 2009, Price et al. 2010). Interestingly, these changes were associated with clinical severity. Increased activity in the anterior and posterior DMN in patients during emotional face perception and labeling with MDD predicted depression severity and hopelessness (Grimm et al. 2009). Similarly, Sheline (2009) found a failure to deactivate anterior, posterior and ventral DMN regions during passive viewing and reappraisal of negative emotional pictures. Consistently, increased subgenual cingulate activity in MDD at rest correlated in the dopaminergic system with depressive episode duration (Greicius et al. 2007) as well as with illness severity (Sheline et al. 2010, Hamilton et al. 2011). This region has been implicated in affect and viscero-motor function regulation as well as will negative emotion experience thus suggesting its relationship with altered emotional processing in MDD (Price et al. 2010). Within the ventral DMN subsystem, increased hippocampal responses to sad facial stimuli and to cognitive tasks have also been reported (Price et al. 2010).

Moreover, the DMN can interact with other resting state networks. The connectivity between the DMN and the striatum (Sambataro et al. 2013) that is involved in reward and cognitive processing is altered in MDD. Reduced connectivity of between these networks could underlie anhedonia and lack of motivation (Bluhm et al. 2009). Recent studies have also shown that connectivity both within the regions of the task-positive

network (network of regions correlated with a seed in a region recruited during a task) and of the task-negative network (i.e., the DMN) is increased in MDD (Zhou et al. 2010). Interestingly, the temporal anti-correlation (negative correlation) between these networks is increased in patients with MDD and the transition between predominance of one network vs another is altered in MDD with the dominance of the DMN being associated with maladaptive and depressive rumination (Hamilton et al. 2011). A recent hypothesis on the role of the DMN in MDD posits that the persistence of the DMN during task performance together with a reduced engagement of the task-related network are pivotal for MDD and for its recurrence (Marchetti et al. 2012).

Recent studies suggest that not only the onset of altered DMN subsystems per se, but also their interplay could underlie the MDD (Hamilton et al. 2011, Sambataro et al. in press). In particular, using a granger causality approach combined with independent component analysis our group found reduced effective connectivity at rest from the anterior subsystem to the ventral subsystem of the DMN in patients with MDD (Sambataro et al. in press), and this could contribute to the limbic and paralimbic hyperactivity. Increased ventral to anterior DMN connectivity has also been reported (Hamilton et al. 2011), thus confirming a pivotal role of altered connectivity between these two sub-systems, which mediate memory and self-referential processing, in the pathogenesis of MDD.

ConclusionsCurrent evidence amply supports a role of the DMN

function and its subsystems in major depression. One of the brain mechanisms through which the DMN could contribute to depressive symptoms could entail the modulation of affective cognition. Interestingly, alterations of affective cognition (Fritzsche et al. 2010) as well as of the DMN (Farb et al. 2011, Elliott et al. 2012) in remitted patients tend to support the view that these are trait rather than state markers of the disorder. Furthermore, another important point to be addressed is whether the DMN alterations at rest can predict altered DMN engagement during the performance of an affective or cognitive task, which may illustrate intrinsic and stable traits for depression (Sambataro et al., personal communication). The modulation of affective cognition by the therapeutic interventions focused on the DMN modulation using psychotherapy (e.g., mindfulness based treatments, see Chiesa et al. 2010), drugs (e.g., selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, (e.g., selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors, Pizzagalli 2011) or deep brain stimulation (Lozano et al. 2008) deserves further investigation. Also, the regions of the DMN (Sambataro et al. 2010, 2013) as well as affective cognition symptoms (Whitmer et al. 2012) are modulated by dopaminergic system. Future studies addressing how genetic variation contributes to this trait of MDD could open new therapeutic avenues for the treatment of this disorder.

AcknowledgmentsThis work was supported by Marie Curie

Reintegration grant (FP7-276981 to F.S.). All the authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest relevant to this manuscript.

Default mode in depression: the affective cognition pathway

Clinical Neuropsychiatry (2013) 10, 5 215

for adaptive and maladaptive rumination. Biol Psychiatry 70, 4, 327-333.

Lozano AM, Mayberg HS, Giacobbe P, Hamani C, Craddock RC and Kennedy SH (2008). Subcallosal cingulate gyrus deep brain stimulation for treatment-resistant depression. Biol Psychiatry 64, 6, 461-467.

Marchetti I, Koster EH, Sonuga-Barke EJ and De Raedt R (2012). The default mode network and recurrent depression: a neurobiological model of cognitive risk factors. Neuropsychol Rev 22, 3, 229-251.

Moll J and Schulkin J (2009). Social attachment and aversion in human moral cognition. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 33, 3, 456-465.

Northoff G, Wiebking C, Feinberg T and Panksepp J (2011). The ‘resting-state hypothesis’ of major depressive disorder-a translational subcortical-cortical framework for a system disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 35, 9, 1929-1945.

Ochsner KN and Gross JJ (2005). The cognitive control of emotion. Trends Cogn Sci 9, 5, 242-249.

Ochsner KN and Gross JJ (2007). The neural architecture of emotion regulation. In JJ Gross, Handbook of Emotion Regulation, p. 104. Guilford Press, New York, US.

Phillips ML, Drevets WC, Rauch SL and Lane R (2003). Neurobiology of emotion perception II: Implications for major psychiatric disorders. Biol Psychiatry 54, 5, 515-528.

Phillips ML, Ladouceur CD and Drevets WC (2008). A neural model of voluntary and automatic emotion regulation: implications for understanding the pathophysiology and neurodevelopment of bipolar disorder. Mol Psychiatry 13, 9, 829, 833-857.

Pizzagalli DA (2011). Frontocingulate dysfunction in depression: toward biomarkers of treatment response. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 1, 183-206.

Price JL and Drevets WC (2010). Neurocircuitry of mood disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 1, 192-216.

Raichle ME, MacLeod AM, Snyder AZ, Powers WJ, Gusnard DA and Shulman GL (2001). A default mode of brain function. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 98, 2, 676-682.

Sambataro F, Blasi G, Fazio L, Caforio G, Taurisano P, Romano R, Di Giorgio A, Gelao B, Lo Bianco L, Papazacharias A, Popolizio T, Nardini,M and Bertolino A (2010a). Treatment with olanzapine is associated with modulation of the default mode network in patients with Schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 35, 4, 904-912.

Sambataro F, Dimalta S, Di Giorgio A, Taurisano P, Blasi G, Scarabino T, Giannatempo G, Nardini M and Bertolino A (2006). Preferential responses in amygdala and insula during presentation of facial contempt and disgust. Eur J Neurosci 24, 8, 2355-2362.

Sambataro F, Fazio L, Taurisano P, Gelao B, Porcelli A, Mancini M, Sinibaldi L, Ursini G, Masellis R, Caforio G, Di Giorgio A, Niccoli-Asabella A, Popolizio T, Blasi G and Bertolino A (2013). DRD2 genotype-based variation of default mode network activity and of its relationship with striatal DAT binding. Schizophr Bull 39, 1, 206-216.

Sambataro F, Murty VP, Callicott JH, Tan HY, Das S, Weinberger DR and Mattay VS (2010b). Age-related alterations in default mode network: impact on working memory performance. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713002596 Published online by Cambridge University Press on 31 October 2013. Neurobiol Aging 31, 5, 839-852.

Sambataro F, Murty VP, Lemaitre HS, Reed JD, Das S, Goldberg TE, Callicott JH, Weinberger DR and Mattay VS (2010c). BDNF modulates normal human hippocampal ageing [corrected]. Mol Psychiatry 15, 2, 116-118.

Sambataro F, Wolf ND, Pennuto M, Vasic N and Wolf RC (in press). Revisiting default mode network function in major depression: evidence for disrupted subsystem connectivity. Psychol Med.

Schulz KP, Clerkin SM, Halperin JM, Newcorn JH, Tang CY

ReferencesAndrade L, Caraveo-Anduaga JJ, Berglund P Bijl, RV

De Graaf R, Vollebergh W, Dragomirecka E, Kohn R, Keller M, Kessler RC, Kawakami N, Kilic C, Offord D, Ustun TB and Wittchen HU (2003). The epidemiology of major depressive episodes: results from the International Consortium of Psychiatric Epidemiology (ICPE) Surveys. Int J Methods Psychiatr Res 12, 1, 3-21.

Andrews-Hanna JR (2012). The brain’s default network and its adaptive role in internal mentation. Neuroscientist 18, 3, 251-270.

Andrews-Hanna JR, Reidler JS, Sepulcre J, Poulin R and Buckner RL (2010). Functional-anatomic fractionation of the brain’s default network. Neuron 65, 4, 550-562.

Beck AT, Rush AJ, Shaw BF and Emery G (1979). Cognitive Therapy of Depression. The Guilford Press, New York.

Bluhm R, Williamson P, Lanius R, Theberge J, Densmore M, Bartha R, Neufeld R and Osuch, E (2009). Resting state default-mode network connectivity in early depression using a seed region-of-interest analysis: decreased connectivity with caudate nucleus. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci 63, 6, 754-761.

Bradley BP, Mogg K and Millar N (1996). Implicit memory bias in clinical and non-clinical depression. Behav Res Ther 34, 11-12, 865-879.

Broyd SJ, Demanuele C, Debener S, Helps SK, James CJ and Sonuga-Barke EJ (2009). Default-mode brain dysfunction in mental disorders: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 33, 3, 279-296.

Chiesa A and Serretti A (2010). A systematic review of neurobiological and clinical features of mindfulness meditations. Psychol Med 40, 8, 1239-1252.

Damoiseaux JS, Prater KE, Miller BL and Greicius MD (2012). Functional connectivity tracks clinical deterioration in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiol Aging 33, 4, 828 e819-830.

Elliott R, Lythe K, Lee R, McKie S, Juhasz G, Thomas EJ, Downey D, Deakin JF and Anderson IM (2012). Reduced medial prefrontal responses to social interaction images in remitted depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry 69, 1, 37-45.

Elliott R, Zahn R, Deakin JF and Anderson IM (2011). Affective cognition and its disruption in mood disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology 36, 1, 153-182.

Farb NA, Anderson AK, Bloch RT and Segal ZV (2011). Mood-linked responses in medial prefrontal cortex predict relapse in patients with recurrent unipolar depression. Biol Psychiatry 70, 4, 366-372.

Fritzsche A, Dahme B, Gotlib IH, Joormann J, Magnussen H, Watz H, Nutzinger DO and von Leupoldt A (2010). Specificity of cognitive biases in patients with current depression and remitted depression and in patients with asthma. Psychol Med 40, 5, 815-826.

Greicius MD, Flores BH, Menon V, Glover GH, Solvason HB, Kenna H, Reiss AL and Schatzberg AF (2007). Resting-state functional connectivity in major depression: abnormally increased contributions from subgenual cingulate cortex and thalamus. Biol Psychiatry 62, 5, 429-437.

Grimm, S, Boesiger, P, Beck, J, Schuepbach, D, Bermpohl, F, Walter, M, Ernst, J, Hell, D, Boeker, H and Northoff, G (2009). “Altered negative BOLD responses in the default-mode network during emotion processing in depressed subjects.” Neuropsychopharmacology 34, 4, 932-943.

Hamilton JP, Chen G, Thomason ME, Schwartz ME and Gotlib IH (2011). Investigating neural primacy in Major Depressive Disorder: multivariate Granger causality analysis of resting-state fMRI time-series data. Mol Psychiatry 16, 7, 763-772.

Hamilton JP, Furman DJ, Chang C, Thomason ME, Dennis E and Gotlib IH (2011). Default-mode and task-positive network activity in major depressive disorder: implications

Fabio Sambataro et al.

216 Clinical Neuropsychiatry (2013) 10, 5

Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Moran JM, Nieto-Castanon A, Triantafyllou C, Saxe R and Gabrieli JD (2011). Associations and dissociations between default and self-reference networks in the human brain. Neuroimage 55, 1, 225-232.

Whitmer AJ and Gotlib IH (2012). Depressive rumination and the C957T polymorphism of the DRD2 gene. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 12, 4, 741-747.

Wolf RC, Sambataro F, Vasic N, Schmid M, Thomann PA, Bienentreu SD and Wolf ND (2011). Aberrant connectivity of resting-state networks in borderline personality disorder. J Psychiatry Neurosci 36, 6, 402-411.

Wolf RC, Sambataro F, Vasic N, Wolf ND, Thomann PA, Landwehrmeyer GB and Orth M (2013). Longitudinal task-negative network analyses in preclinical Huntington’s disease. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci.

Wolf RC, Sambataro F, Vasic N, Wolf ND, Thomann PA, Saft C, Landwehrmeyer GB and Orth M (2012). Default-mode network changes in preclinical Huntington’s disease. Exp Neurol 237, 1, 191-198.

Zhou Y, Yu C, Zheng H, Liu Y, Song M, Qin W, Li K and Jiang T (2010). Increased neural resources recruitment in the intrinsic organization in major depression. J Affect Disord 121, 3, 220-230.

and Fan J (2009). “Dissociable neural effects of stimulus valence and preceding context during the inhibition of responses to emotional faces. Hum Brain Mapp 30, 9, 2821-2833.

Sheline YI, Price JL, Yan Z and Mintun MA (2010). Resting-state functional MRI in depression unmasks increased connectivity between networks via the dorsal nexus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 107, 24, 11020-11025.

Uddin LQ, Kelly AM, Biswal BB, Xavier Castellanos F and Milham MP (2009). Functional connectivity of default mode network components: correlation, anticorrelation, and causality. Hum Brain Mapp 30, 2, 625-637.

Vasic N, Walter H, Sambataro F and Wolf RC (2009). Aberrant functional connectivity of dorsolateral prefrontal and cingulate networks in patients with major depression during working memory processing. Psychol Med 39, 6, 977-987.

Whiteford HA, Degenhardt L, Rehm J, Baxter AJ, Ferrari AJ, Erskine HE, Charlson FJ, Norman RE, Flaxman AD, Johns N, Burstein R, Murray CJ and Vos T (2013). Global burden of disease attributable to mental and substance use disorders: findings from the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet.