cultural adaptation of international students: … · study examines how stereotypes and...

74
CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: STEREOTYPES AND INTERCULTURAL COMMUNICATION COMPETENCE _______________ A Thesis Presented to the Faculty of San Diego State University _______________ In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Master of Arts in Communication _______________ by Ariel D. Baumbaugh Summer 2015

Upload: others

Post on 30-May-2020

4 views

Category:

Documents


1 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS:

STEREOTYPES AND INTERCULTURAL

COMMUNICATION COMPETENCE

_______________

A Thesis

Presented to the

Faculty of

San Diego State University

_______________

In Partial Fulfillment

of the Requirements for the Degree

Master of Arts

in

Communication

_______________

by

Ariel D. Baumbaugh

Summer 2015

Page 2: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the
Page 3: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

iii

Copyright © 2015

by

Ariel D. Baumbaugh

All Rights Reserved

Page 4: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

iv

DEDICATION

I would like to dedicate this to my family and friends. First, my mom and dad, who

have always supported me in everything and anything I have done. Not to mention the

countless hours they have both put in helping me proofread and edit. Without them, I most

likely would not have gotten to where I am today. Next, my friends, who have been there for

me when I needed them the most and have continued to encourage me throughout this

process. Last, I would also like to dedicate this to my dog, Lacey. Without her and the

abundance of dog therapy she has provided, I would probably not be sane.

Page 5: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

v

ABSTRACT OF THE THESIS

Cultural Adaption of International Students: Stereotypes and

Intercultural Communication Competence

by

Ariel D. Baumbaugh

Master of Arts in Communication

San Diego State University, 2015

The trend of studying abroad is rapidly expanding worldwide. Even though research

has examined the impact of studying abroad, the effects of stereotypes on students’ overall

cultural adaptation into the new culture has not been notably assessed. The present study

examines the stereotypes of warmth and competence that international students attribute to

U.S. Americans and the effect these stereotypes have on these students’ preparedness for

change, intercultural communication competence, and overall cultural adaptation to

American society. Utilizing Kim’s (2001) Cross-Cultural Adaptation Theory as a framework,

this study investigates the process of cultural adaptation by including preparedness for

change, stereotypes warmth and competence, and intercultural communication competence.

A cross-sectional study was conducted on 218 international students at a large southwestern

university. First it was shown that preparedness for change positively affects intercultural

communication competence. However, it was then illustrated that preparedness for change

did not relate to international students’ adaptation into the American culture. International

students’ perceived perception of U.S. Americans was then tested. Through this, it was found

that U.S. Americans are considered to be more competent than warm. From this finding, the

relationship between the two stereotype dimensions proved to correlate. This indicates that if

Americans are perceived as warm, that will positively associate that with perceptions of

competence. Next, stereotypes warmth and competence were shown to relate to preparedness

for change. The more prepared for change an international student was, the more warmth and

competence toward U.S. Americans was felt. When examining the relationship between

intercultural communication competence and the two stereotype dimensions, only stereotype

competence was shown to fully relate. The warmth dimension was only partially related to

intercultural communication competence. Following, only stereotype warmth was shown to

be a predictor for cultural adaptation. Stereotype competence was not positively related to an

international student’s cultural adaptation. Last, intercultural communication competence

positively related to an international students’ cultural adaptation into the American culture.

Through gaining this knowledge, researchers can benefit by having a better understanding of

the implications that preparedness for change, stereotypes, and intercultural communication

competence can have on international students’ overall adaptation into the American society.

Keywords: stereotypes, intercultural communication competence, cultural adaptation,

and international students

Page 6: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

vi

TABLE OF CONTENTS

PAGE

ABSTRACT ...............................................................................................................................v

LIST OF TABLES ................................................................................................................. viii

LIST OF FIGURES ................................................................................................................. ix

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS .......................................................................................................x

CHAPTER

1 INTRODUCTION .........................................................................................................1

Literature Review.....................................................................................................2

Cross-Cultural Adaptation Theory ...........................................................................3

Preparedness for Change....................................................................................6

Preparedness for Change and Intercultural Communication

Competence..................................................................................................6

Preparedness for Change and Cultural Adaptation ......................................9

Stereotypes .......................................................................................................10

Stereotype Content Model .........................................................................11

Stereotypes and Preparedness for Change .................................................16

Stereotypes and Intercultural Communication Competence......................17

Stereotypes and Cultural Adaptation .........................................................20

Intercultural Communication Competence and Cultural Adaptation ........23

2 METHOD ....................................................................................................................25

Procedures ..............................................................................................................25

Participants .............................................................................................................25

Measures ................................................................................................................27

Demographics ..................................................................................................27

Preparedness for Change..................................................................................28

Cultural Adaptation ..........................................................................................28

Stereotype Content Model ...............................................................................28

Page 7: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

vii

Intercultural Communication Competence ......................................................29

Data Analysis .........................................................................................................30

3 RESULTS ....................................................................................................................33

Hypothesis 1...........................................................................................................33

Hypothesis 2...........................................................................................................33

Research Question 1 ..............................................................................................33

Research Question 2 ..............................................................................................34

Research Question 3 ..............................................................................................34

Hypothesis 3...........................................................................................................34

Hypothesis 4...........................................................................................................35

Hypothesis 5...........................................................................................................35

4 DISCUSSION ..............................................................................................................36

Implications............................................................................................................42

Limitations and Future Research ...........................................................................44

Conclusion .............................................................................................................45

REFERENCES ........................................................................................................................46

APPENDIX

SURVEY......................................................................................................................54

Page 8: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

viii

LIST OF TABLES

PAGE

Table 1. Participants’ Characteristics: Sex, Age, Nationality, and Class Standing .................26

Table 2. Intercorrelation Matrix of All Constructed Variables................................................31

Page 9: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

ix

LIST OF FIGURES

PAGE

Figure 1. Structural model of cross-cultural adaptation. ............................................................4

Figure 2. Model of proposed hypotheses and research questions ..............................................6

Page 10: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

x

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

First and foremost I want to give a huge thank you to the wonderful committee I had.

Thank you Dr. Spitzberg for giving me the assistance and support needed to complete this

project. Without your guidance and helpfulness, this thesis would not be as accomplished as

it is now. You are truly a profound educator and I am thrilled I got the chance to work and

learn from you. Next, I would also like to thank Dr. Snavely. Thank you for being a part of

my committee and leading me through the process of acquiring a master degree here at San

Diego State University. The journey has been intense but needless to say amazing and worth

every drop of sweat. I would also like to thank Dr. Devos, for your contribution and great

ideas with making this the best thesis it possibly could be. Last, I would like to thank Dr.

Lee, her help was crucial in the execution of this thesis.

Next, I would like to thank my fellow communication master students. Our cohort is

so close and everyone really has been such a huge support system to me. Specifically,

Jazmyne for always being there to answer my many quantitative methods questions. Ari, for

working many days and nights with me on our theses, inspiring each other to keep going!

And last Madison, who since day one has been my person in the program. Without her

constant support, work ethic, and encouragement, this program would have been way more

difficult to complete. I am happy to say that not only have I gained so much knowledge from

this program, but I have also gained a life-long friend. Thank you to an amazing cohort.

Last, I would like to thank my family. They have always pushed me to do what I am

passionate about, while still encouraging me to be my very best. First, my dad who since day

one has instilled in me the importance of getting a high-level education. If he could get two

masters, then I can at least get one! Thank you for always being there for me and being my

motivation. Last my mom, who has watched me struggle and thrive throughout these two

years. With her endless advice and support, she has helped me more than anyone I know.

Thank you.

Page 11: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

1

CHAPTER 1

INTRODUCTION

The trend of studying abroad is expanding globally and continuing to rise in the

United States. From 2012 to 2013 there was a 7% increase of students studying abroad within

the U.S., resulting in an overall total of 819,644 international students (Institute of

International Education [IIE], 2013). Within the U.S., the states of California, Texas, and

New York encompass the majority of students who study abroad (IIE, 2013). These three

states have up to an estimated 112,000 international students (IIE, 2013). Moreover, the U.S.

Department of Commerce claimed that in 2012 international students provided about 24

billion dollars to the U.S. economy (IIE, 2013). Such data illustrates some of the tangible

impacts that studying abroad programs have on the United States.

There is considerable research examining the process of adapting to a new

environment while studying abroad. Prior research has mainly focused on specific variables

that influence international students’ adaptation such as second language acquisition skills

(Isabelli-García, 2006; Sablina & Kopiatina, 2013; Segalowitz et al., 2004; Yu & Shen,

2012), conflict management (Adrian-Taylor, Noels, & Tischler, 2007; Oommen, 2013;

Shupe, 2007), expectations (Martin, Bradford, & Rohrlich, 1995; Pitts, 2009), culture shock

(Chapdelaine & Alexitch, 2004; Edwards-Joseph & Baker, 2012; Lin, 2006; Zhou, Jindai-

Snape, Topping, & Todman, 2008) and re-entry culture shock (Arthur, 2003; Gaw, 2000;

Rohrlich & Martin, 1991; Sussman, 2002). Even with this extensive research on all the

factors found to influence international students’ adaptation into the new culture, there are

additional factors to be explored that might influence the adaptation process.

By investigating how specific factors such as stereotypes and intercultural

communication competence influence international students’ cultural adaptation, we can gain

a better understanding of the trials international students go through. Specifically, examining

stereotypes associated with U.S. Americans, international students’ cultural adaptation can be

Page 12: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

2

assessed through the pre-dispositional views they have before their travel even begins. By

examining intercultural communication competence, the progression of cultural adaptation

can be evaluated through successful interactions with U.S. Americans. Therefore, the present

study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to

international students’ adaptation to the United States culture.

Utilizing Kim’s (2001) Cross-Cultural Adaptation Theory as a framework, this study

will examine the effects international students’ predispositions and perceived stereotypes of

U.S. Americans have on their communication competence and cultural adaptation to U.S.

culture. International students’ predispositions will be considered in relation to their

preparedness for change. Specifically, it is argued that international students’ preparedness

for change affects their intercultural communication competence and cultural adaptation. To

obtain the necessary data for perceived stereotypes, the dimensions of warmth and

competence found in Fiske, Cuddy, Glick, and Xu’s (2002) stereotype content model will be

assessed. The degree that the stereotypes are associated to U.S. Americans will be discussed

and the connection between both dimensions of warmth and competence will be evaluated.

From this assessment, the link between the stereotypes warmth and competence will be

explored. Then it will be illustrated how the two stereotypes positively influence

international students’ preparedness for change and cultural adaptation. Last, the relationship

between intercultural communication competence and cultural adaptation will be

demonstrated.

LITERATURE REVIEW

With the popularity of study abroad programs in the U.S., researchers indicate that

international students are faced with a variety of factors that affect their adaptation process,

such as interaction (or lack thereof) with the host culture, a different educational system,

living arrangements (on or off campus), financial stresses, and length of stay (Mustaffa &

Ilias, 2013; Selltiz, Christ, Havel, & Cook, 1963). Even with all these factors influencing

cultural adaptation, stereotypes are also instrumental in international students’ adaptation to

the American culture. Stereotypes are attitudes that are thought to stem from predispositions

(Gertsen, 1990) that are likely held by international students before their travels begin. The

extent to which stereotypes distort or interfere with accurate interpretations of intercultural

Page 13: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

3

and intergroup interactions will probably effect if international students engage in less

competent communication and cultural adaptation. Thus, an international student’s

predisposition of preparedness for change, stereotypes of warmth and competence, and

intercultural communication competence are expected to influence foreign students’ cultural

adaptation. One way to explore the affects of these concepts is through Kim’s (2001)

theoretical model, which is detailed below.

CROSS-CULTURAL ADAPTATION THEORY

Kim’s (2001) Cross-Cultural Adaptation Theory attempts to bridge the gap between

the individual and the environment in the adaptation process through three overall

assumptions. First, humans appear to have an instinctive need to adapt to the environment

surrounding them (Kim, 1995, 2001). Second, adapting to the new environment is achieved

through communication (Kim, 1995, 2001). Third, through this multifaceted process of

adaptation, the individual is transformed (Kim, 1995, 2001). Based on these assumptions, the

individual may have the opportunity to achieve cross-cultural adaptation. Cross-cultural

adaptation is “the dynamic process by which individuals, upon relocating to new, unfamiliar,

or changed cultural environments, establish (or reestablish) and maintain relatively stable,

reciprocal, and functional relationships with those environments” (Kim, 2001, p. 31).

This theory is an appropriate framework for understanding what might enable an

effective adaptation. Kim’s (2001) structural model is utilized, which indicates specific

elements that impact cross-cultural adaptation (see Figure 1). The model can illustrate

systematically how preparedness for change, perceived U.S. American stereotypes, and

intercultural communication competence are all interconnected and influence cultural

adaptation.

Kim’s (2001) structural model is based on many different core dynamics and

conditions. A variety of the dimensions are external, establishing restrictions on the adaptive

conduct of the individual, while others are internal and found within the individual’s actions

(Kim, 2005). These differences enable adaptations to be interactive processes between the

dimensions, the individual, and the environment (Kim, 2005). The dynamics shown

(predisposition, environment, host communication competence, and intercultural

transformation) shape the ability to culturally adapt to a new environment (Kim, 2001). The

Page 14: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

4

Figure 1. Structural model of cross-cultural adaptation. Source: Kim, Y. Y.

(2001). Becoming intercultural: An integrative theory of communication and

cross-cultural adaptation. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

variables represented in Kim’s (2001) model that are discussed in this study are

predispositions, host communication competence, and cultural adaptation. Host

communication competence will be use interchangeably throughout with intercultural

communication competence. Thus, some predispositions and intercultural communication

competence are examined in regard to international students who come to study aboard

within the U.S. culture.

Predispositions are internal conditions, that are characteristics or beliefs individuals

have before their travels (Kim, 1995). These predispositions illustrate the degree to which

individuals differ because of diverse backgrounds, which establish limits to their overall

internal adaptive ability (Kim, 2005). By coming into contact with certain elements of the

environment, an individual’s internal dynamic changes, bringing the person closer to

incorporating new cultural aspects of behavior (Kim, 2005). According to Kim (2001), an

individual’s internal condition is based on their preparedness for change, ethnic proximity,

and adaptive personality. Even though all three components are significant, the present study

will focus on preparedness for change because preparedness for change is the perceptual and

mental readiness an individual has to grasp new cultural aspects (Kim, 2005). Since this

propensity stems from previous background knowledge, some individuals will be able to

overcome challenges found in the new culture (Kim, 2001, 2005). Thus, due to the many

Page 15: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

5

challenges international students face when studying abroad (Mustaffa & Ilias, 2013), it can

be suggested that preparedness for change has an impact on international students’ cultural

adaptation into the U.S. culture.

The next and core internal condition to this model is host communication

competence, which refers to an individual’s ability to analyze and collect knowledge through

cognitive, behavioral, and affective means (Kim, 2001, 2005). This ability is dependent on

interactions between individuals and locals of the new culture (Kim, 1995, 2001).

Furthermore, successful adaptation to a new culture can only occur when communication

style is congruent with the locals’ style (Kim, 2001, 2005). Through this, it is thought that by

effectively interacting with American citizens, international students will be able to adapt

more quickly to the U.S. culture.

The internal conditions listed above influence the last condition, intercultural

transformation (also known as cultural adaptation). Through experiencing internal

transformations due to the new environment, individuals’ mental, emotional, and behavioral

reactions start to become routine (Kim, 2005). Intercultural transformation is made up of

three components (i.e., function fitness, psychological health, and intercultural identity) that

bring all the necessary aspects of adaptation together (Kim, 2003). Cross-cultural adaptation

is a continuous and developing process, dependent on these varying levels that make up

one’s intercultural transformation (Kim, 2001). Therefore, the present study will focus on the

overall combination of these three aspects (functional fitness, psychological health,

intercultural identity) that Kim (2001) indicates influences and could bring about

international students’ cultural adaptation.

By utilizing Kim’s (2001) cross-cultural adaptation theory, and more specifically her

structural model, as the theoretical framework to this present study, we can start to

understand how international students’ perceived stereotypes of warmth and competence and

intercultural communication competence affect their cultural adaptation into the American

society (See Figure 2).

Page 16: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

6

Figure 2. Model of proposed hypotheses and research questions.

Preparedness for Change

Preparedness for change refers to the level of readiness or willingness individuals

have prior to coming into contact with a new culture (Kim, 1995, 2001). Individuals who

lack this readiness will struggle to adapt because they do not understand the social rules and

behaviors associated with the new culture (Furnham & Bochner, 1982). Preparedness for

change provides awareness of the differences between the new culture and the individual’s

home culture, making them more prepared for better adaptation into the new culture

(Kitsantas, 2004). Overall preparedness for change involves several different factors such as

formal education received, prior training in the new culture’s language and overall culture,

previous cross-cultural experiences, and if the travel was voluntary (Kim, 1995, 2001). These

factors seem to be the foundation needed in order to establish how prepared international

students are to study abroad and have a successful cross-cultural experience.

PREPAREDNESS FOR CHANGE AND

INTERCULTURAL COMMUNICATION

COMPETENCE

According to Cross-Cultural Adaptation Theory, preparedness for change influences

intercultural communication competence (Kim, 2001). Spitzberg and Changnon (2009)

define intercultural communication competence as “the appropriate and effective

management of interaction between people who, to some degree or another, represent

different or divergent affective, cognitive, and behavioral orientations to the world” (p. 7). In

Page 17: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

7

other words, interaction between culturally diverse individuals is most likely to be competent

to the extent such behavior fits positively valence expectancies associated with each culture

(Spitzberg & Changnon, 2009). More specifically, intercultural communication competence

is focused around attributes of “motivation, knowledge, and skills” (Spitzberg & Changnon,

2009, p. 35). From these attributes, individuals seem to have the power to create their own

experience through successful interactions, facilitating a quicker adjustment into the new

environment.

Previous research has connected intercultural communication competence to factors

such as willingness to communicate (Hamad & Lee, 2013; MacIntyre, Baker, Clément, &

Conrod, 2001; Yashima, Zenuk-Nishide, & Shimizu, 2004), cultural sensitivity (Ellenwood

& Snyders, 2010; Knutson, Komolsevin, Chatiketu, & Smith, 2003; Liu, 2014; Williams,

2005) and empathy (Arasaratnam, 2006; Gertsen, 1990; Herfst, van Oudenhoven, &

Timmerman, 2008). Prior research, however, does not appear to have assessed Kim’s (2001)

proposed relationship between intercultural communication competence and preparedness for

change.

Even though research has indicated that there are specific concepts that separately

affect an individual’s level of preparedness for change and intercultural communication

competence, it seems to not visibly have illustrated how together they influence one’s overall

preparedness for change and their intercultural communication competence. For example,

Belz (2003) conducted a study that illustrated the influence formal education has on

competent communication with locals of the new culture. It was shown that the more cultural

education individuals had, the less cultural misinterpretations occurred (Belz, 2003). It is

expected that those individuals with fewer cultural misunderstandings would be better

communicators because of the perceived fluidity they would have while conversing with the

locals by already knowing the new culture’s social norms (Kim, 2001). Also, since formal

schooling is known to enable a higher level of intellectual capability, this higher academic

ability aids individuals in educating themselves in the new aspects of the culture (Kim,

2001).

The more knowledge individuals have about a culture prior to traveling, the more

successful their communication with the locals might be. De Verthelyi (1995) conducted a

study on the challenges that international students’ spouses have when their significant other

Page 18: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

8

comes to study abroad in the United States. From this study, it was suggested that there was

less cultural understanding if the individuals in the new culture did not learn about the

American culture and language before their travels (De Verthelyi, 1995). Further, from this

lack of prior knowledge and cultural understanding, individuals were then missing the

communication skills needed to bridge that gap between their home culture and the new one

(De Verthelyi, 1995). This gap could also be expanded due to language barriers because

training in the new culture’s language prior to travel was not completed (De Verthelyi, 1995).

Ultimately, it appears that the more equipped individuals are about the culture they are

visiting; the more successful their communication will be with the host culture (Kim, 2001).

Prior training programs for international students could enable them to have a higher

level of preparedness for change and better intercultural communication competence. For

example, Kitsantas (2004) suggests that the training study abroad programs offer before

travel are too short, making them ineffective learning tools for international students. By

being prepared in the general expectations and communicative behaviors of the new culture,

the individuals will be able to progress faster to cultural understanding (Brislin & Kim,

2003). This is because most individuals are experts at interacting in their own culture

(Furnham & Bochner, 1982), but lack the knowledge of proper communicative behaviors to

with members of different cultures (Mizera, Tulviste, Konstabel, & Lausa, 2013). To

illustrate, individuals who are new to a culture are usually untrained at interacting with the

locals (Furnham & Bochner, 1982). This is because they are expected to learn the new

accepted behaviors informally through trial and error; they are rarely clarified through

training courses prior to travel (Furnham & Bochner, 1982). With a training program,

individuals could be given the necessary skills to become competent communicators in the

new environment (Furnham & Bochner, 1982; Shackleford, 2011). Due to this, many

intercultural studies have suggested a need for prior culture training before traveling begins

in order to become more competent communicators (Brislin & Kim, 2003; Furnham &

Bochner, 1982; Kim, 2001, 2003, 2005; Kitsantas, 2004). Even though there are many

aspects of preparedness for change, together those aspects seem to have the power to create

successful interactions between international students and locals of the host culture. Thus,

international students’ overall level of preparedness for change could affect their

Page 19: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

9

communication competency in the new culture. From this, the following hypothesis is

proposed:

H1: Preparedness for change is positively related to intercultural communication

competence.

PREPAREDNESS FOR CHANGE AND

CULTURAL ADAPTATION

Preparedness for change has the power to affect international students’ adaptation

into American culture. Previous researchers have examined how preparedness for change

affects cultural adaptation (De Verthelyi, 1995; Goldstein & Kim, 2006; Harris & Moran,

1987; Tomich, McWhirter, & King, 2000). When studying abroad in a foreign country, it is

apparent there are many different aspects individuals could prepare for that might influence

their adaptation into the new culture (Mustaffa & Ilias, 2013). Those aspects would include,

but are not limited to: new obligations, new environmental settings, different native

languages, and different cultural norms (Mustaffa & Ilias, 2013; Selltiz et al., 1963). Not to

mention that each university around the world has its own organization, expectations,

workload, and staff-student relations to which the international students are expected to adapt

(Klineberg & Hull, 1979). Nevertheless, the more prepared an international student is with

knowledge of these new aspects before travel, the easier their understanding and adaptation

into the new culture will be (Klineberg & Hull, 1979; Selltiz et al., 1963). Tomich et al.

(2000) indicates that prior education of the new culture before traveling begins would be

beneficial and speed along the adaptation development. This is because knowledge before

travel enables the individual to have an early start at confronting and understanding the new

environment’s expectations (Tomich et al., 2000). De Verthelyi (1995) also suggests that an

individual’s motivation or readiness for facing aspects of a new culture is contingent on the

way prior cultural experiences have shaped them to have an adaptable approach to new

cultures. Simply, the more cultural experience an individual has had prior to travel, the more

flexible their personality will be to adapting to a new culture (De Verthelyi, 1995). Through

this, individual can begin to gain understanding of the host intentions and actions, making for

an easier adaptation to occur (Tomich et al., 2000).

Preparedness for change can specifically affect adaptation to the American culture.

Chen (2000) conducted a study by examining the ways preparedness for change impacts

Page 20: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

10

international students’ cultural adaptation into the American culture. She investigated three

aspects (cultural knowledge, language proficiency, and self-confidence), which are aspects of

preparedness for change thought to impact cultural adaptation (Chen, 2000). All three factors

were shown to significantly affect cultural adaptation (Chen, 2000). International students

felt that without speaking English fluently, without knowing the American culture, and

without having the confidence to do so, it was harder for them to adapt, especially if their

culture was very distinct from the United States’ culture (Chen, 2000). Another study also

illustrates how international students, especially those from diverse cultural backgrounds like

Asian countries, struggle to adapt due to the lack of preparedness for change (Lin & Yi,

1997). It was found that the international students find it difficult to understand their

professors and fellow students’ accents (Lin & Yi, 1997). This then makes them disinclined

to interact with locals from the host culture (Lin & Yi, 1997). According to Kim (2001), to

have successful adaptation, individuals need to interact with the locals of the host culture.

Thus, if international students do not feel generally prepared before travel, it appears they

will not adapt as quickly or easily to the new culture. Since the international students

struggled to adapt because they lacked this aspect of preparedness for change, Lin and Yi

(1997) suggest that training prior to studying abroad should take place. Brislin and Kim

(2003) also recommend that the most successful abroad experience would include in-depth

cross-cultural training regarding the new culture the individuals are going to visit. It is

proposed that individuals would then know the differences of the new culture compared to

their own before their traveling took place (Brislin & Kim, 2003). Ultimately, it seems

apparent that some general level of preparedness for change is important for individuals to

have before their travels begin in order to have a smoother adaptation into the new culture

(Church, 1982; Kim 2001, 2005; Rawlings & Sue, 2013). Therefore, the following

hypothesis is proposed:

H2: Preparedness for change is positively related to cultural adaptation.

Stereotypes

Each culture around the world has its own perceived ideas of people in other societies

(Cuddy et al., 2009). These perceived ideas are otherwise known as stereotypes, embellished

assumptions about specific groups or people (Gertsen, 1990). Vast cultural and social

disparities enable such universal stereotypes to occur (Cuddy et al., 2009; Glock & Krolack-

Page 21: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

11

Schwerdt, 2013). This means that individuals from certain countries have specific worldwide

stereotypes associated to them just because they originate from that country (Lee & Fiske,

2006). These global perceptions become even more solidified because people tend to view

individuals that are not members of their group as dissimilar to their particular group, with

more stereotypical characteristics associated to them (Park, Ryan, & Judd, 1992; Ryan &

Bogart, 1997). Due to this, national stereotypes seem to be a common occurrence (Bender,

Gidlow, & Fisher, 2013), making it probable that international students studying in the U.S.

will have prominent stereotypical views towards the U.S. and U.S. Americans which in turn

could affect their experience.

Positive or negative stereotypes international students have associated to the country

they are visiting are thought to affect their study abroad experience. It was been assessed that

the more positive the expectation of studying abroad programs are, the more individuals

would want to participate (Goldstein & Kim, 2006). This is consistent with other research

indicating that positive expectations are anticipated when an individual interacts with other

individuals who they perceive to be fellow group members (Hoyle, Pinkley, & Insko, 1989;

Park et al., 1992). Likely stemming from those expectations are stereotypes, which also seem

to influence an individual’s behavior and experience. Many studies suggest that stereotypes

have the power to influence an individual’s behavior (Dovidio, Hewstone, Glick, & Esses,

2010; Fiske, 1998; Krings, Sczesny, & Kluge, 2011). Thus, it is expected in this present

study that the stereotypes of warmth and competence might have positive or negative aspects

associated to them that could have the power to influence an international students’ behavior

and overall adaptation into the American culture.

STEREOTYPE CONTENT MODEL

Although a considerable amount of research assessing stereotypes has examined

overall stereotyping behavior, scholars have also considered the multi-dimensional aspects of

stereotypes. Those dimensions include how individuals utilize stereotypes in their social

exchanges (stereotype processes) and the stereotypes that individuals make on specific

groups or people (stereotype content; Ruble & Zhang, 2013). This present research will be

implementing both of these aspects, which illustrate stereotypes as an individual’s perceived

opinion or belief (good or bad) of another group (Cuddy et al., 2009). More specifically, this

Page 22: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

12

study will be utilizing the stereotype content model (SCM) that implements both aspects and

has been proven to be a systematic structure (Cuddy et al., 2009; Fiske et al., 2002).

The contents and processes of stereotypes are created and further adjusted through

social stresses that individuals are faced with in everyday life (Fiske et al., 2002). In other

words, the foundations of stereotypes are shaped through daily experiences (Fiske et al.,

2002). For example, since international students in study abroad programs at universities are

constantly tackling new cultural differences and behavioral norms (Mustaffa & Ilias, 2013), it

can be assumed that those studying in the U.S. would have the same problems when

communicating with U.S. Americans. Analyzing the processes and content of stereotypes

allows for definite examination and widespread comparison of certain groups (Cuddy, Fiske

& Glick, 2008; Cuddy et al., 2009; Eckes, 2002; Fiske et al., 2002), such as international

students and U.S. Americans. Since this present study is not focusing on international

students from one distinct culture, examining general stereotype processes and content might

allow for a more generalized approach to understanding stereotypes of U.S. Americans.

The stereotypes of warmth and competence are components of an individual’s every

day life (Fiske et al., 2002). When individuals encounter another person or group of people,

they will internally ask themselves two questions: What is the other person’s reason for

conversing with me?; and are they qualified to follow through with their reason for

conversing with me? (Cuddy et al., 2008; Cuddy et al., 2009; Fiske, 2012; Fiske, Cuddy, &

Glick, 2007). Specifically, when coming into contact with someone new, one wants to know

the other’s intent (warmth) and their ability to carry out this intent (competence; Binggeli,

Krings, & Sczesny, 2014; Cuddy et al., 2008; Fiske et al., 2002). Individuals will be

perceived as warm if they are friendly, sympathetic, and appear dependable throughout a

conversation (Abele, Cuddy, Judd, & Yzerbyt, 2008; Fiske et al., 2002). Individuals will be

perceived as competent if they are smart, skillful, and effectual in keeping the conversation

going throughout an interaction (Abele et al., 2008; Fiske et al., 2002). Additionally, through

the order of these questions, warmth seems to be considered the primary of the two because

of the importance initial perceptions (warmth) have on an individual, when compared to the

ability the individual has to act out their intentions (competence; Cuddy et al., 2008).

Demonstrating how the warmth stereotype tends to dominate over one’s perceived level of

competence (Cuddy et al., 2008; Fiske et al., 2007), which seems to create an inverse

Page 23: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

13

relationship between the two. Nevertheless, these two dimensions seem to be an essential

foundation for an individual’s social interactions (Fiske et al., 2007), which makes them

valuable aspects to examine.

These two universal dimensions of warmth and competence seem to have the power

to influence the way individuals of the host culture are perceived, which could affect

adaptation into a new culture. SCM predicts that groups that have high status and are

competitive with other groups will be perceived as competent but not warm (Glick et al.,

2006). This is because other groups view competitive groups with resentment due to their

aggressive nature (Fiske et al., 2007; Glick et al., 2006). The stereotype content model also

predicts that if groups are thought to be powerful but cooperate with others, they will be

perceived as both competent and warm (Glick et al., 2006). This is because a high status

group like this illustrates how they cooperate with other groups, enabling the other groups to

have a sense of admiration or respect towards them (Fiske, Xu, Cuddy, & Glick, 1999; Glick

et al., 2006). To test this, Glick et al. (2006) and Trifiletti, Andrighetto, Rattazzi, Visintin,

and Falvo (2011) conducted studies that examined how the stereotypes of warmth and

competence are associated with the United States. The findings from both of these studies

correspond with the above predictions of SCM, which indicate that many think of the United

States as being competent but cold (Glick et al., 2006; Trifiletti et al., 2011). However, since

most international students chose to study abroad in the U.S., it is thought that the stereotypes

of warmth and competence associated to U.S. Americans might be different. Therefore, the

following research question is proposed:

RQ1: To what degree are U.S. Americans perceived as (a) warm and (b)

competent?

Past research has examined the dimensions of warmth and competence in terms of

four mixed stereotypes (i.e., typologies) that exhibit high and low socially desired traits:

paternalistic, envious, contemptuous, and admiration (Fiske et al., 2002). Although the four

mixed stereotypes are not the main focus of this present study, they are an important aspect

to stereotype content and processes and are therefore briefly discussed below.

The first of the four mixed stereotypes are paternalistic stereotypes, which occur

when individuals are perceived as having low-competence and high-warmth (Fiske et al.,

2002; Glick & Fiske, 2001). In the U.S. paternalistic stereotypes are related to an individual’s

gender, age, language, and ethnic group (Fiske et al., 2002). This is because certain

Page 24: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

14

individuals within these groups (elderly individuals, underprivileged African Americans,

traditional women, and non-native English speakers) tend to not be respected and thus,

overlooked by society (Fiske et al., 2002). Due to the tendencies to overlook individuals

typically associated to this stereotype, are feelings of kindness, which enable onlookers to

have a sense of warmth linked to individual within this category (Fiske et al., 2002). In sharp

contrast is the second mixed stereotype known as envious. The envious stereotype results

from a perception of high-competence but low-warmth (Fiske et al., 2002; Glick & Fiske,

2001). In the U.S., this stereotype is applicable to men, nontraditional women (e.g., working

women, athletes, etc.), wealthy individuals, minority authorities, Jews, and Asians (Fiske et

al., 2007; Fiske et al., 2002; Glick & Fiske, 2001). It seems that individuals within this

category are seen as too industrious, self-interested, and only task proficient (Fiske et al.,

2002). Ultimately, they are more skillful with work than they are with interpersonal

relationships (Fiske et al., 2002).

The third mixed stereotype, contemptuous, is perceived when members have low-

competence and low-warmth (Fiske et al., 2002). Individuals that are perceived to hold this

stereotype are considered to be scrounging individuals who do not add or contribute to

society (Fiske et al., 2002). In the U.S., groups such as these would contain underprivileged

white and black individuals, those on welfare, the disabled, and immigrants (Fiske et al.,

2002; Glick & Fiske, 2001). Since it is believed that individuals in this category do not

enhance society, feelings of anger, disapproval, revulsion, and antipathy are attached to them

(Fiske et al., 2002; Glick & Fiske, 2001). The last type of mixed stereotype is admiration,

which occurs when members are perceived as having high-competence and high-warmth

(Fiske et al., 2002). Admiration can bring about feelings of pride and positive attitudes

towards individuals within this group (Cuddy et al., 2008; Fiske et al., 2002). Specifically in

the U.S., Christian individuals, U.S. citizens (middle class), and white individuals are

considered to be within this group (Fiske et al., 2007; Fiske et al., 2002). This group has

close ties with other members of society and is thought to be a communal group (Fiske et al.,

2002). Due to being such a collective group, many individuals communicate aspects of

satisfaction and admiration towards members of it (Cuddy et al., 2008). These four

stereotypes seem to be influential in cross-cultural experiences and enable a variety of

perceptions on specific groups of people.

Page 25: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

15

The extent to which these typologies can be assessed within the present study greatly

depend on the dimensional aspects of the responses. Moreover, typologies (which are

categorical) restrict our understanding of the relationship between stereotypes and other

factors. Therefore, the present study will assess the dimensional aspects of warmth and

competence. Yet previous research seems to not have fully assessed how these dimensional

aspects relate to one another.

There seem to be a unique relationship between the dimensions warmth and

competence. Research recently has suggested that the relationship between the two as either a

halo effect or a compensation effect (Kervyn, Yzerbyt, Judd, & Nunes, 2009; Yzerbyt,

Kervyn, & Judd, 2008). When examining individuals, the halo effect seems to come into

play, which illustrates a positive relationship between the two stereotype dimensions

(Yzerbyt et al., 2008). So, if individuals are perceived as friendly (i.e., warm), then they will

most likely also be perceived as intelligent (i.e., competent; Yzerbyt et al., 2008). However,

when examining the social perceptions of groups, there seems to be a compensation effect,

which results in perceiving some out-group members as positive on one dimension but the

negative on the other dimension (Fiske et al., 2002; Kervyn et al., 2009; Yzerbyt et al.,

2008). For example, if someone is perceived as friendly (warm), they are less likely to be

perceived as intelligent (competent).

These contradictory views illustrate a need for a closer examination of the

complicated relationship between the dimension warmth and competence. Yzerbyt et al.

(2008) conducted a study examining the relationship between the two dimensions. The

resulting association illustrated a compensation effect with participants tending to favor one

group over the other on both of the dimensions (Yzerbyt et al., 2008). It was illustrated that

the relationship between the dimensions warmth and competence is unlike any other

stereotype grouping (Yzerbyt et al., 2008). It appears that the dimensions of warmth and

competence are both significant and independent to each other (Yzerbyt et al., 2008). For

example, if a country’s culture is observed as warm, that will impact its observed competence

and vice versa (Kervyn et al., 2009). Further, there appears to be a distinction between the

two dimensions in their predominance and the consequence of this primacy (Kervyn et al.,

2009). Research has indicated the warmth dimension is often more important than the

competence dimension, especially when examining social perceptions (Cuddy et al., 2008).

Page 26: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

16

Even in gender related stereotypes, women are viewed as more warm, whereas men are

viewed as more competent (Ebert, Steffens, & Kroth, 2014). Given this, knowledge on the

warmth stereotype, or perceived constraints of this stereotype, enables deductions to occur on

individuals’ degree of competence (Yzerbyt et al., 2008). This complex relationship between

the two dimensions is important to interpret because of how fundamental the stereotype

dimensions are to understanding the intricate nature of social perceptions (Kervyn, Dolderer

Mahieu, & Yzerbyt, 2010; Kervyn, Fiske, & Yzerbyt, 2013). Therefore, the following

research question is proposed:

RQ2: What is the relationship between the stereotypes of warmth and

competence?

STEREOTYPES AND PREPAREDNESS FOR

CHANGE

The stereotype content model could enable for a structural way to interpret the effects

preparedness for change have on individuals traveling. Prior research has examined

expectations (Hoyle et al., 1989), stereotypes (Cuddy et al., 2009; Fiske et al., 2007), and

aspects of preparedness for change (Chen, 2000; Goldstein & Kim, 2006; Kim, 2001; Lin &

Yi, 1997), but has not fully assessed how these aspects affect one another. Most stereotypes

do hold some valence, but habitually stunt the potential cognitive growth of individuals

(Beamer, 1992) before their travel even begins. This is because individuals seem to create

expectations of interactions based on these stereotypes, even though these views only

represent part of the culture (Beamer, 1992). However, through the readiness and motivation

associated with preparedness for change, accurate expectations or stereotypes are more likely

to be formed of the new culture (Black & Gregersen, 1990; Kim, 2001). These stereotypes

could have the power to create an optimistic or sanguine study abroad experience for

international students in the United States. Conversely, if individuals are not prepared or

have improper knowledge of the country they are planning to visit, the perceived stereotypes

might hinder their intercultural interactions (Gertsen, 1990). This makes the relationship

between preparedness for change and perceived stereotypes unclear.

This contends that stereotypes might be related to preparedness for change, but there

seems to be a lack of research directly examining this. Nevertheless, related studies do help

in the understanding of the relationship between the two. For example, Ruble and Zhang

Page 27: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

17

(2013) examined the stereotypes associated to Chinese students by Americans. From their

study, it was apparent that there were generational and national stereotypes about the Chinese

population that made Americans expect certain behaviors (Ruble & Zhang, 2013). It was

suggested that if Americans were more prepared and had more experience with the Chinese

culture, their stereotypes might be more realistic and not as detrimental to the interaction

process (Ruble & Zhang, 2013). This seems to illustrate how the lack of preparedness or

knowledge of another culture enables stereotypes to take over and influence an individual’s

behaviors. Other work also demonstrates this occurrence but through the stereotypes of

warmth and competence. Cuddy et al. (2008) investigated and reviewed the universal effects

the stereotypes of warmth and competence have on social interactions. In their chapter, they

suggest that the stereotypes of warmth and competence create emotions, which are associated

to individuals and specific groups (Cuddy et al., 2008). These emotional associations could

then have the power to affect an individual’s cognition, which in turn is thought to influence

their preparedness for change (Cuddy et al., 2008). This is because the stereotypes of warmth

and competence influence various configurations of predispositions to occur emotionally,

rationally, and behaviorally (Cuddy et al., 2008). So the more preparedness for change an

individual has, the less impactful the effects of the stereotypes warmth and competence could

have, making the individual better suited for experiencing and adapting to the new

environment. However, to what extent these universal stereotypes could affect one’s

preparedness is unclear. Thus, the following research question is proposed:

RQ3: What is the relationship between stereotype (a) warmth and (b) competence

and preparedness for change?

STEREOTYPES AND INTERCULTURAL

COMMUNICATION COMPETENCE

The stereotypes of warmth and competence are connected to an individual’s

intercultural communication competence, making these concepts important to examine.

Stereotypes can influence successful intercultural communication exchanges (Dovidio et al.,

2010; Fiske, 1998). This is because categorizing individuals into groups enables stereotypical

behaviors to be associated to them (Dovidio et al., 2010; Fiske et al., 2002), which seems to

influence effective communication between groups. For instance, in order for individuals to

be interculturally competent, they must be able to manage the social interaction in

Page 28: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

18

appropriate and effective ways (Spitzberg & Changnon, 2009). In order to do this, the

individual needs to have the necessary set of skills to successfully implement the correct

cultural behaviors into the interaction (Spitzberg & Changnon, 2009). Some of those

competent abilities would include the individual’s view of how similar their culture is to the

new one, language skills, mindfulness, prior knowledge about the new culture, and

motivation to have a successful interaction (Spitzberg & Changnon, 2009). Since most

groups determine the stereotype (warm and/or competent) of another group by their

relationship with each other (Lin, Kwan, Cheung, & Fiske, 2005), perceptions of

communication competence might fluctuate depending on the new culture. This is because

these abilities may be perceived as competent in one culture while not competent in another

(Spitzberg, 2007). Through the anticipation of these stereotypical behaviors in interactions

(Dovidio et al., 2010) it can be proposed that if those expectancies are not met, social or

communication difficulties might arise that could affect the competency of the interaction.

Stereotypes can adversely affect intercultural communication competence in a variety

of ways. For example, Olaniran (1996) conducted a study that investigated the problems

international students face and how those problems affect their competence when

communicating with locals of a host culture. Some of those difficulties included the

language, age, and dissimilarities between the international students’ home culture and the

new culture (Olaniran, 1996). It was found that regardless of the difficulties felt, the more the

international students interacted with locals of the host culture, the more apt both parties

were at understanding the dissimilarities of each other’s culture (Olaniran, 1996). More

specifically, the more similar the cultures were, the less difficulty was felt in the interaction

(Olaniran, 1996). This could be due to that fact that stereotypes are not only limited to the

dimensions of warmth and competence; beliefs and morals also seem to influence one’s

attitudes towards other groups (Guan, Deng, & Bond, 2010). Thus, parallel to Spitzberg and

Changnon (2009) from Olaniran’s (1996) study, it appears that the more similar the new

culture is to the individual, the more competent the individual will be when communicating.

From these similarities, it is speculated that the two dimensions of warmth and competence

would have a positive outcome in comparable cultures, making for a prosperous intercultural

communication exchange to occur.

Page 29: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

19

Countries all around the world are starting to socialize their citizens to other cultures

(Beamer, 1992), which could help to eliminate stereotypes and enable more successful

intercultural communicators. Stereotypical difference associated to other cultures restricts the

travelers’ perceptions and ultimate understanding of the culture (Beamer, 1992). By being

mindful of differences, individuals could strengthen their intercultural communication

competence (Chang, 2013). In order to do this, universities are now creating more classes

directed towards building students’ intercultural communication competence (Beamer, 1992).

In these classes, students progress towards intercultural competence by moving past the

preconceived stereotypes, and test their own meanings of interaction through asking

questions about the other culture to gain more knowledge (Beamer, 1992). One way they are

specifically doing this is through teaching the students English, which is considered the

universal language (Alptekin, 2002; Rawlings & Sue, 2013). By learning such a versatile

language, the students will be more prepared when interacting with individuals from a variety

of cultures (Alptekin, 2002). After all, language proficiency was illustrated to be one of the

most important traits to conversing successfully with individuals from other cultures (Chen,

2000). Also, when the individuals arrive to the new culture, by interacting with the locals,

they can further their progress of becoming competent communicators (Lee & Chen, 2000).

This is because constant interaction and communication with the locals of the new culture

allows for faster cultural learning and skills to be acquired (Lee & Chen, 2000). Through

being motivated to recognize and understand the vast differences each culture around the

world has to offer, it is hoped that international students will acknowledge the cultural

stereotypes and develop the tools to become successful intercultural communicators

(Beamer, 1992).

Although there are communication difficulties that make intercultural communication

competence difficult to achieve (Spencer-Rodgers & McGovern, 2002), this present study

speculates that the more friendly (warm) and skillful (competent) the individual is, the more

successful their intercultural exchange would be. A seemingly easy way to gain these

objectives would be through interacting with individuals from the host culture. An

individual’s degree of warmth will be perceived if they are friendly or appear trustworthy

throughout the conversation (Abele et al., 2008). The more warmth felt towards an

individual, the more likely the other individual would be motivated to have a conversation or

Page 30: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

20

interact with them (Cuddy et al., 2008). Thus, the warmth stereotype seems to be crucial for

the initial stage of interaction, illustrating that without the desire to initially converse, the

later skill of competence might not be obtained. Individuals’ competence will be derived

from how skillful they are at keeping the conversation going throughout the interaction

(Abele et al., 2008). So where it seems the warmth stereotype draws individuals together to

have an interaction, the competence stereotype furthers the process of communication along

(Cuddy et al., 2008). Thus, it can be assumed that if individuals are lacking the friendliness

or skill level to keep the conversation going and maintain their goal, they will not have a

successful intercultural interaction. Based on this, the stereotypes of warmth and competence

seem to have a significant relation to whether or not an individual is a competent

communicator in cross-cultural settings. From this, the following hypotheses are proposed:

H3a: Stereotype warmth is positively related to intercultural communication

competence.

H3b: Stereotype competence is positively related to intercultural communication

competence.

STEREOTYPES AND CULTURAL ADAPTATION

Just as preconceived social perceptions affect intercultural communication

competence (Fiske, 1998), they could also affect international students’ cultural adaptation to

the American culture. This is because the dimensions of warmth and competence offer

information about certain stereotypical characteristics associated to individuals and groups

(Fiske et al., 2007; Fiske et al., 1999). Since stereotypes associated to individuals outside of

one’s social group have the power to influence that individual’s expectations and behavior

(Hoyle et al., 1989), social biases could be created (Dovidio et al., 2010) which could affect

cultural adaptation. Through affecting an individual’s behavioral outcome, the adaptation

process could become jeopardized depending on the stereotype associated. This problem of

adaptation not being obtained makes it is important to narrow down and analyze how

stereotypes affect this process.

There are many ways that perceptions of groups can be formed based on the two

dimensions of warmth and competence (Fiske et al., 2007) that might affect international

students’ adaptation development. For example, Klineberg and Hull (1979) conducted a

study that suggested U.S. Americans were able to form relationships with individuals but that

those relationships were perceived as superficial. Since Kim (2005) suggests that in order to

Page 31: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

21

culturally adapt, individuals need to be able to develop deeper relationships, it appears that

even though U.S. Americans were perceived as friendly, they were not competent enough at

developing agreeable or satisfactory relationships with the other students. This illustrates that

the dimensions of warmth and competence can affect an individual’s adaptation into a new

environment. Additionally, Bond (1986) conducted a study that examined the stereotypes

between Americans and Chinese exchange students that interacted regularly. The stereotypes

were shown to be consistent with Americans both maintaining the interaction (warm) and

controlling its direction (competent), whereas the Chinese students were viewed as more

compliant and less assertive (Bond, 1986). Through this, it was apparent that the stereotypes

seemed to regulate the interaction (Bond, 1986), which might influence the adaptation

process. This is because stereotypes such as these may enable for levels of discrimination to

take place, affecting individuals’ attitudes and behaviors (Dovidio et al., 2010), impeding

their ability to adapt (Jung, Hecht, & Wadsworth, 2007). These findings seem to illustrate

how the warmth and competence of U.S. Americans regarded by other cultures are influential

in the adaptation process.

Coinciding with behavioral implications of these stereotypes are emotional effects

that also are thought influence cultural adaptation. These emotions are based on stereotypes

that most international students connect to the U.S. culture through American television and

movies (Chen, 2000). However, it seems that the stereotypical representation of the U.S. and

U.S. Americans portrayed to other countries end up giving international students a false

sense of American culture, ultimately affecting their adaptation (Rawlings & Sue, 2013).

Even though familiarity with American culture enables easier communication and adaptation

(Jung et al., 2007), the falsification the media portrays seems to create an incorrect reality for

international students to base their attitudes and emotions on. Through this misrepresentation,

the stereotypes of warmth and competence the media portrays could make it more difficult

for international students to adapt to the American culture.

Most individuals then might feel a sense of ineptness when communicating with

others of diverse cultural backgrounds because of the difficulties they encounter throughout

the interaction (Spencer-Rodgers & McGovern, 2002). For example, Spencer-Rodgers and

McGovern (2002) conducted a study that analyzed U.S. Americans’ mindsets toward

international students and how those attitudes affected the foreigner’s intercultural

Page 32: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

22

communication competence and adaptation. From this, it was suggested that many U.S.

Americans felt irritated or awkward when communicating with international students

(Spencer-Rodgers & McGovern, 2002). It is suggested that international students can identify

the attitudes Americans have toward them and that it might have the power to impact their

communication (Jung et al., 2007). If this is the case and the international students feel

negative emotions radiating from U.S. Americans, it is speculated that they will not feel as

inclined to interact with them. This is due in part to emotions being the clearest analyst for

behaviors and attitudes associated to international students (Spencer-Rodgers & McGovern,

2002). This illustrates that the emotions attached to the stereotypes of warmth and

competence could be very influential to the behaviors that affect the adaptation process.

Affecting cultural adaptation and emerging from these behavioral and emotional

outcomes of the stereotype’s warmth and competence, is the degree to which an individual is

perceived to fit these stereotypes. The relationship between groups creates a social structure

upon which to base the interaction (Cuddy et al., 2008). To analyze this though, it is essential

to examine the degree to which the members are thought to fit the assumed stereotype (Park

& Judd, 1990). Since the dimensions of warmth and competence are considered universal,

they help to account for the basic differences stereotypes create in groups (Cuddy et al.,

2008). For instance, Kim (2001) suggests that interaction with locals of the host culture is the

first step towards adapting. Therefore, if U.S. Americans are perceived as friendly (warm) or

skillful (competent), it can be speculated that international students would go out of their way

to interact and begin the process of adaptation. However, the reverse scenario might also hold

validity. This reasoning stems from out-group members being more likely to obey and adapt

to the new norms than members of the in-group (Park & Judd, 1990). Meaning that it seems

more probable for international students to adapt their behaviors to those of U.S. Americans

than the other way around. Moreover, this adaptation might be easier if the two groups have

similar characteristics of warmth and competence. This is because if the stereotypes between

groups are not distinctively different, then the groups regard themselves as less stereotypical

(Park & Judd, 1990) and then might adapt more easily. From this, it is suggested that the

more warmth and competence felt towards the host culture, the more likely international

students will change their behavior and culturally adapt. Based on the above reasoning, the

following hypotheses are proposed:

Page 33: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

23

H4a: Stereotype warmth is positively related to cultural adaptation.

H4b: Stereotype competence is positively related to cultural adaptation.

INTERCULTURAL COMMUNICATION

COMPETENCE AND CULTURAL ADAPTATION

As alluded to, intercultural communication competence is closely linked to

individuals’ intercultural transformation or more specifically, their cultural adaptation (Kim,

1988, 2005). When individuals arrive to a new environment, they immediately begin the

procedure of cross-cultural adaptation (Kim, 2001). This adaptation thrives and expands

through their interactions with the locals of the host culture (Kim, 2001, 2005). Although

cross-cultural adaptation is the process of adjustment into a new culture, conflicts might

occur between individuals due to differences between the new culture and their accustomed

culture (Koskinen & Tossavainen, 2004; Lee & Chen, 2000). Nevertheless, even if one group

feels threatened by the other, it appears that this threat can disappear if there are enough

successful interactions between the individuals (McLaren, 2003). If that threat can dissolve

through the willingness and ability to interact (McLaren, 2003), it seems that the capacity to

adapt is greatly associated with competent interactions between the locals of the host culture

and the individuals traveling to the new culture.

With the importance of intercultural communication competence in the adaptation

process, many studies have looked at the effects of interacting with the host culture has

(Carlson & Widaman, 1988; Lin & Yi, 1997; Mckay-Semmler & Kim, 2014; Redmond &

Bunyi, 1993; Sawyer & Chen, 2012). One analysis suggested that interactions with the host

culture are imperative because it builds the foreigner’s experience and overall

communication competence (Andrews & Henze, 2009). Without this experience, their

adaptation might never be complete (Andrews & Henze, 2009). Another study argues that

competent interactions with the locals of the host culture enable the individuals to gain

information about the host culture’s language and culture, viewpoints, everyday expectations,

and customs (Lee & Chen, 2000). By gaining this knowledge through communication and

interpersonal interactions such as these, individuals are then able to learn about the host

culture and acquire the tools needed to further their adaptation to the new culture (Lee &

Chen, 2000). Further, Ruben and Kealey (1979) suggest that an individual’s own

communication skills are critical for cross-cultural adaptation to occur, especially those in

Page 34: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

24

Western cultures. Illustrating that the more skilled an individual is at communicating in their

own culture, the more likely they will be a competent communicator and adapt to the new

culture (Ruben & Kealey, 1979). Therefore, communication seems to be the foundation for

building relationships and culturally adapting (Kim & Mckay-Semmler, 2013).

Intercultural communication competence also seems to be vital in regards to

international students’ adaptation into another culture that is distinct from their own.

Zimmermann (1995) conducted a study that examined the intercultural communication

competence of international students coming to America. From this study, even though there

was a slight correlation between speaking the English language and adaptation, it was found

to be just as important for international students to constantly be interacting and

communicating successfully with locals of the host environment (Zimmermann, 1995).

While the locals have had most of their lives to gain the communication skills needed for

successful interactions, most international students have only had a couple years of training

or none at all (Zimmermann, 1995). Therefore, by learning new communicative behaviors

through experimentation, international students might strengthen their intercultural

communication competence, and learn to adapt more quickly to the new environment.

Mckay-Semmler and Kim (2014) also conducted a study that analyzed the impact

communication had on adaptation. From their analysis of Hispanics’ adapting to the

American culture, it was suggested that communicating and interacting with U.S. Americans

directly relates to the Hispanics’ adaptation (Mckay-Semmler & Kim, 2014). Through

interacting with the locals, the Hispanics were able to learn from trial and error the

communicative norms of the American society, making them more competent

communicators (Mckay-Semmler & Kim, 2014). Thus, it appears that through

communication and interaction with the locals, individuals are able to detect the differences

and similarities between their accustomed culture and the new culture, while simultaneously

adapting to the new expectations (Kim, 2001). These studies jointly indicate how important

successful intercultural communication competence is to international students’ cultural

adaptation into the culture where they are studying abroad. From the above reasoning, the

following hypothesis is proposed:

H5: Intercultural communication competence is positively related to cultural

adaptation.

Page 35: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

25

CHAPTER 2

METHOD

PROCEDURES

International students were recruited in a variety of different ways. Primarily, they

were recruited from participating Communication and Psychology courses. However,

additional international students were obtained through various campus organizations that

service international students (e.g., Open University, American Language Institute, and the

International Student Center). This web-based survey was created using Qualtrics (a web-

based survey software). If enrolled in participating courses the study was presented on the

Research Participants Management website (SONA) and participants received credit for their

participation. International students not enrolled in participating courses were provided with

the link to the Qualtric survey through a recruitment flyer or email. All participants were

initially asked to complete a statement of informed consent and then they were provided with

instructions on how to complete the various parts of the survey. Since this study was only

applicable to international students, the only data excluded were from participants (n = 58)

who selected their nationality as being from the United States.

PARTICIPANTS

This study was composed of a convenience sample (N = 218) of international students

who were enrolled at a large public university in Southern California. Due to the participants

being international students, the national identity of the participants varied. Participants’

nationality included China (n = 31), India (n = 30), Mexico (n = 13), Sweden (n = 13), Japan

(n = 11), Saudi Arabia (n = 10), and the United Kingdom (n = 10) with more participants (n =

100) from other varied countries (n = 47). The participants also ranged in age from 18 to 45

years of age. Participants consisted of college freshman (n = 52), sophomores (n = 21),

juniors (n = 48), 4th

year seniors (n = 34), 5th

year seniors (N = 8), and graduate students (n =

55) (M= 3.41, SD=1.86). Every effort was made to have an equal representation of both male

Page 36: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

26

and female students in this study. However, there ended up being a higher number of females

(n = 137) than males (n = 81). For full distribution of participants’ characteristics see Table 1

below. This study was distributed only to international students who were studying abroad;

this included international students seeking four-year degrees, as well as those only studying

abroad for a semester or two.

Table 1. Participants’ Characteristics: Sex, Age, Nationality, and Class Standing

Characteristics N %

Sex

Male 81 37.2

Female 137 62.8

Age

18 20 9.2

19 26 11.9

20 24 11.0

21 27 12.4

22 28 12.8

23 24 11.0

24 19 8.7

25 15 6.9

26 11 5.0

Other 24 11.2

Nationality

China 31 14.2

India 30 13.8

Mexico 13 6.0

Sweden 13 6.0

Japan 11 5.0

Saudi Arabia 10 4.6

United Kingdom 10 4.6

Other countries 100 45.8

Page 37: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

27

Class Standing

Freshman (1st yr) 52 23.9

Sophomore (2nd

yr) 21 9.6

Junior (3rd

yr) 48 22.0

Senior (4th

yr) 34 15.6

5th

year Senior or beyond 8 3.7

Graduate Student 55 25.2

MEASURES

Pre-existing scales were used to measure cultural adaptation, stereotype content

model, and intercultural communication competence. A new scale was created to assess

preparedness for change based on qualitative attributes presented by Kim (2001).

Demographics

At the beginning of the questionnaire, participants are asked demographic questions.

These questions consist of asking the participants’ sex (male/female), age, class standing

(freshman, sophomore, junior, senior, etc.), ethnicity (Danish, Hispanic or Latino, Japanese,

African American, etc.) and nationality. Nationality was assessed through two questions. The

first question asked where the participants hold their nationality. The second question asked

the participants to indicate what country they normally resided in. For both questions,

participants were provided a list of countries ranging from Afghanistan to Zimbabwe.

Additionally, participants were asked to indicate their English language proficiency. A series

of items on a 5-point scale (1= poorly, 5= excellent) asked participants to rate their English

speaking/writing/comprehension skills. The last questions are open-ended to asses their

language proficiency (e.g. how many years the participants have been speaking English, what

their primary language is, what language do they speak most with their friends and family,

and what other languages they speak) and also their prior travel experiences (e.g. how many

times they have been to the United States before this study abroad semester/year).

Page 38: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

28

Preparedness for Change

Preparedness for change was measured with a newly created scale based on the

features Kim (2001) indicate reflect an individual’s readiness prior to travel: formal

education, training in host culture and language, previous cross cultural travel experiences,

and if their travel choice was voluntary. This uni-dimensional construct was measured

through 5 items. Participants were asked to indicate on a 5-point scale from 1 (strongly

disagree) to 5 (strongly agree) how true each statement is. Items were generated based on the

various features. Some item statements included “I have received a lot of schooling and

formal education,” “I have had training in the host culture’s language prior to traveling” and

“I have had prior traveling experiences with different cultures.” In the present study, the

overall scale was found to be reliable (α = .69).

Cultural Adaptation

Cultural adaptation was assessed utilizing the Sociocultural Adaptation Scale (SCAS;

Ward & Kennedy, 1999). According to Ward and Kennedy (1999), this is a comprehensive

and versatile adaptation scale. Originally a 29-item scale that measures participants’

behavioral (22 items) and cognitive (7 items) difficulty in adapting to a new culture (Ward &

Kennedy, 1999), the present study included four additional items that were specific to the

study abroad experience. Participants were asked to indicate on a 5-point scale from 1 (no

difficulty) to 5 (extreme difficulty) the amount of difficulty felt in different situations. The

sample items consisted of how difficult it is “Making friends,” “Taking an American

perspective on the culture,” and “Understanding what is required of you at University.” Also,

due to the point meaning of 1 (no difficulty) to 5 (extreme difficulty) all the items were

recoded to 1 (extreme difficulty) to 5 (no difficulty). In the present study, the subscales of

behavioral (α= .94), cognitive (α= .92), and studying abroad (α= .79) were found to be

reliable, as well as the overall scale (α= .97).

Stereotype Content Model

Stereotypes were measured utilizing the Stereotype Content Model (Fiske et al.,

2002). This scale measures the degree of warmth and competence felt by individuals toward

others (Fiske et al., 2002). In addition, the variants of warmth and competence illustrate four

stereotypes: paternalistic (low competence, high warmth), envious (low warmth, high

Page 39: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

29

competence), contemptuous (low warmth, low competence), and admiration (high warmth,

high competence; Fiske et al., 2002). Five items measured competence and four items

measured warmth (Fiske et al., 2002). Participants were asked to indicate on a 5-point scale

from 1 (not at all) to 5 (extremely) how true each statement is. Scale items were adjusted so

participants were responding about Americans as a group. Competence statements included

“Americans are competent” and “Americans are confident.” Warmth statements included

“Americans are warm” and “Americans are good-natured.” In the present study, both

competence (α = .68) and warmth (α = .78) were reliable sub-scales, with the overall scale

being reliable (α = .78).

Intercultural Communication Competence

Intercultural communication competence was measured in two ways. Previous

research has indicated that communication competence has both dispositional (i.e. trait-like)

and situational (i.e. state-like) characteristics (see Cupach & Spitzberg, 1983). In order to

assess the dispositional characteristics of communication competence, the Conversation

Skills Rating Scale (CSRS; Spitzberg, 2007) was used. This is a 30-item scale that assesses

interpersonal competence (Spitzberg, 2007); however for the present study it has been

adapted to analyze intercultural communication competence. The first 25-items measure

behavior skills (e.g., composure, attentiveness, coordination, and expressiveness), while the

last 5-items measure individuals’ molar communication competence (Spitzberg, 2007).

Participants were asked to indicate on a 5-point scale from 1 (inadequate) to 5 (excellent)

how skillful they are at using various communicative behaviors (e.g. list). Statements

included “Speaking rate (neither too slow nor too fast)” and “Speaking fluency (pauses,

silences, “uh”, etc.).” The items “Shaking or nervous twitches,” “Unmotivated movements

(tapping feet, fingers, hair-twirling, etc.),” and “Interruption of partner speaking turns” were

recoded. Participants were then asked to indicate on a 7-point scale from 1 (poor

conversationalist, socially unskilled, incompetent communicator, inappropriate

communicator, ineffective communicator) to 7 (good conversationalist, socially skilled,

competent communicator, appropriate communicator, effective communicator) their overall

communication performance. In the present study, both the skills (α =.95) and the molar

scales (α =.92) were found to be reliable.

Page 40: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

30

In order to assess state-like competence, Wiemann’s (1977) communicative

competence scale was utilized. This state measure is a 30-item scale that assesses an

individual’s communication style in relation to communication competence (Wiemann,

1977). Participants were asked to indicate on a 5-point scale from 1 (strongly disagree) to 5

(strongly agree) how much the statements listed apply to their communication style.

Statements included “I find it easy to get along with others” and “I can adapt to changing

situations.” Also, the statements “My personal relations are cold and distant” and “I ignore

other people’s feelings” were recoded. In the present study, the scale was found to be reliable

(α =.93).

DATA ANALYSIS

Data was analyzed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS). First,

the data was screened for normality and outliers. As indicated in each measure section, items

in some of the scales were then recoded. Then, all scale items were assessed for frequency

and reliability. In an effort to examine superior reliability and structure on the preparedness

for change and adaptation scales an exploratory factory analysis was undertaken, and

examination of alpha-if-item-deleted were examined. In neither analysis did a better

structured emerge. After this, descriptives and correlational analyses were conducted (See

Table 2). In order to test the positive relationship between preparedness for change and

intercultural communication competence (H1) a bivariate correlation was run. To test the

positive relationship between preparedness for change and cultural adaptation (H2) a

bivariate correlation was also administered. RQ1 asked to what degree are U.S. Americans

perceived as (a) warm and (b) competent and was answered by assessing the rating on the

two dimensions (descriptive statistics). Further a paired t-test was conducted to assess the

distributions of stereotypes warmth and competence. Next, RQ2 asked what the relationship

between the stereotypes of warmth and competence were and was assessed with a bivariate

correlation. RQ3 inquired about the relationship between stereotype (a) warmth and

(b) competence and preparedness for change and was also assessed with a bivariate

correlation. RQ3 was further evaluated through a multiple stepwise regression. H3 examined

the relationship between intercultural communication competence and (a) stereotype warmth

and (b) stereotype competence. Both parts to H3 were assessed with bivariate correlations.

Page 41: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

31

Tab

le 2

. In

terc

orr

elati

on

Matr

ix o

f A

ll C

on

stru

cted

Vari

ab

les

P

rep

are

Ad

apt

Ste

reo

Tra

it

ICC

Skil

l

ICC

Mola

r

ICC

Adap

t

Cogn.

Adap

t

Beh

av.

Ad

apt

Ab

road

Ad

apt

Su

m

War

m

Pre

par

e -

Adap

t .1

3

-

Ste

reo

.21

**

.18

*

-

Tra

it I

CC

.3

0*

**

.25

**

.44***

-

Skil

l IC

C

.29

**

*

.38

**

*

.26**

.59***

-

Mola

r IC

C

.23

**

.46

**

*

.14

.45***

.62***

-

Adap

t C

ogn

. .1

2

.94

**

*

.17*

.19*

.35***

.37***

-

Adap

t B

ehav

. .1

4

.99

**

*

.18*

.26**

.38***

.46***

.91***

-

Adap

t A

bro

ad

.17

*

.90

**

*

.13

.25**

.39***

.48***

.79***

.88

**

*

-

Adap

t S

um

.1

5

.98

**

*

.17*

.25**

.38***

.43***

.94***

.99

**

*

.90

**

*

-

War

m

.16

*

.19

*

.85***

.28***

.17*

.04

.20*

.20

**

.12

.20

**

-

Com

p.

.19

*

.12

.84***

.46***

.27***

.19*

.09

.1

0

.10

.10

.43

**

*

*C

orr

elat

ion i

s si

gnif

ican

t at

the

p <

.05 l

evel

. *

*p

< .01 l

evel

. *

**p <

.001

lev

el

Page 42: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

32

H4 tested the positive relationship between cultural adaptation and (a) stereotype warmth and

(b) stereotypes competence. Both parts to H4 were examined through bivariate correlations.

Last, in order to test the positive relationship between intercultural communication

competence and cultural adaptation (H5) the data was assessed with a bivariate correlation.

Page 43: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

33

CHAPTER 3

RESULTS

HYPOTHESIS 1

Hypothesis 1 porposed that there is a positive relationship between preparedness for

change and intercultural communication competence. The preparedness for change measure

correlated (r= .30, p < .001) with Wiemann’s (1977) Communicative Competence measure

and (r= .29, p < .001) with Spitzberg’s (2007) Conversational Skills Rating Scale (CSRS). It

also correlated significantly with the molar portion of the CSRS (r = .23, p < .01). Therefore,

H1 is supported with a small-to-moderate effect size.

HYPOTHESIS 2

Hypothesis 2 proposed that there is a positive relationship between preparedness for

change and cultural adaptation. Preparedness for change did not correlate significantly with

the composite adaptation measure (r = .13, ns, p=.09). Nor did it correlate significantly with

the composite cognitive (r= .12, ns, p=.14) or behavior (r = .14, ns, p=.08) subscales of the

adaptation measure. It did however show a marginal correlation with the adapt abroad

subscale of the adaptation measure (r = .17, p < .05), and all of the correlations are positive

in sign, indicating a slight tendency for formal preparation to be associated with feelings of

adaptation. Nonetheless due most of the findings not being statistically significant, H2 is not

supported.

RESEARCH QUESTION 1

Research question 1 asked to what degree are U.S. Americans perceived as (a) warm

and (b) competent. Means of the individual stereotyping items range from 3.26 to 4.30 on a

5-point scale, with relatively small (below 1 scale interval) standard deviations. The warmth

mean (M = 3.49, SD = .80) and competence mean (M = 3.82, SD = .61) indicates that

Page 44: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

34

Americans are perceived as somewhat above true midpoint in both warmth and competence.

Conducting a paired t-test indicates that Americans are perceived as significantly more

competent than warm (t(164) = -5.49, p < .001).

RESEARCH QUESTION 2

Research question 2 asked what is the relationship between the stereotypes of warmth

and competence. The warmth and competence variables were significantly and positively

correlated (r = .43, p < .001). In general, this indicates that perceptions of Americans’

warmth are positively associated with perceptions of their competence.

RESEARCH QUESTION 3

Research question 3 asked what is the relationship between stereotype (a) warmth and

(b) competence and preparedness for change. Preparedness for change was significantly but

only slightly correlated with perceptions of warmth (r = .16, p < .05) and competence (r =

.19, p < .05). In general, there was a slight but significant tendency for perceptions of

preparedness for change to be associated with greater perceptions of American’s warmth and

competence. When stepwise regressed upon preparedness for change, only competence

entered the model, suggesting that warmth adds no statistically unique variance in predicting

preparedness for change.

HYPOTHESIS 3

Hypothesis 3a proposed that there is a positive relationship between stereotype

warmth and intercultural communication competence. There is a statistically significant

positive correlation between warmth and intercultural communication competence (Wiemann

(1977) scale, r = .28, p < .001), Spitzberg’s (2007) CSRS skills measure (r = .17, p < .05),

but not to the CSRS molar measure (r = .04, ns, p=.58). Therefore, H3a is partially supported.

Hypothesis 3b proposed that there is a positive relationship between stereotype

competence and intercultural communication competence. There is a statistically significant

and positive association with the Wiemann’s (1977) measure of intercultural communication

competence (r = .46, p < .001) and the CSRS skills measure (r = .27, p < .001), and the

CSRS molar measure (r = .19, p < .05). Thus, H3b is supported.

Page 45: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

35

HYPOTHESIS 4

Hypothesis 4a proposed that there is a positive relationship between stereotype

warmth and cultural adaptation. The correlation between stereotype warmth and cultural

adaptation revealed a statistically significant, small positive correlation (r = .20, p < .01),

which was consistent with both the behavioral (r = .20, p < .01) and the cognitive (r = .20,

p < .05), but not the adapt abroad scale (r = .12, ns, p= .14). In general, the perception of

Americans as warm is associated with a slight tendency to feel more well-adapted. Therefore,

H4a is supported.

Hypothesis 4b proposed that there is a positive relationship between stereotype

competence and cultural adaptation. The stereotyping of competence is not significantly

associated with the overall adaptation measure (r = .10, ns, p= .21), as well as the three

subscales of adaptation: behavioral (r = .10, ns, p= .19), cognitive (r = .09, ns, p= .23), and

adapt abroad (r = .10, ns, p= .20), Therefore, H4b is not supported.

HYPOTHESIS 5

Hypothesis 5 stated that there is a positive relationship between intercultural

communication competence and cultural adaptation. Cultural adaptation is positively related

to intercultural competence, as measured by the Weimann’s (1977) measure (r = .25, p <

.01), Spitzberg’s (2007) CSRS skills measure (r = .38, p < .001), and CSRS molar measure

(r = .43, p < .001). All three subscales of the adaptation measure displayed comparable

consistent correlations to intercultural communication competence. To Weimann’s (1977)

measure the adaptation subscales measured: behavioral (r = .26, p < .01), cognitive (r = .19,

p < .05), and adapt abroad (r = .25, p < .01). To Spitzberg’s (2007) skills CSRS scale the

adaptation subscales measured: behavioral (r = .38, p < .001), cognitive (r = .35, p < .001),

and adapt abroad (r = .39, p < .001). Last, to Spitzberg’s (2007) molar CSRS scale the

adaptation subscales measured: behavioral (r = .46, p < .001), cognitive (r = .37, p < .001),

and adapt abroad (r = .48, p < .001). In general, there is a moderately strong tendency for

cultural adaptation to be associated with greater perceived communication competence, and

therefore, H5 is supported.

Page 46: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

36

CHAPTER 4

DISCUSSION

In a world rapidly advancing in mobility, open borders, globalized business and

challenges, competence in intercultural interactions seems more important than ever. It

appears that numerous models and theories of competence in intercultural contact have been

proposed, but far fewer have been explicitly tested. This study sought to expand and test

Kim’s (2001) well-developed model of intercultural adaptation by testing already existing

measures (preparedness for change and intercultural communication competence) along with

extending the model to include additional variables such as stereotypes warmth and

competence as they influence cultural adaptation.

Kim (2001) indicates that preparedness for change affects host intercultural

communication competence. Kim (2001) proposed that five features facilitate preparedness

for change, which in turn promote intercultural adaptation and competence: Formal

education, training in host culture and language, previous cross cultural travel experiences,

and voluntary choice of cultural location. The findings illustrated that there was a

significantly positive relationship between all five features of preparedness for change and

intercultural communication competence (H1). Not only did preparedness for change

correlate with Wiemann’s (1977) state-like scale of communication competence, but it also

correlated with both parts of Spitzberg’s (2007) Conversation Skills Rating Scale (CSRS).

This is consistent with previous research that illustrated that increased prior knowledge

known about a culture decreases misunderstandings and increases the quality of individual’s

communicative patterns and overall interactions (De Verthelyi, 1995). This also parallels

prior research that has indicated how individuals’ conversations with locals would increase

with a structured course in cultural training of the new culture prior to travel (Brislin & Kim,

2003; Furnham & Bochner, 1982; Kitsantas, 2004). Thus, the higher an international

Page 47: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

37

student’s level of preparedness for change, the more successful interactions between

international students and U.S. Americans take place.

In her model, Kim (2001) also indicates that intercultural communication competence

affects cultural adaptation. The findings illustrated that there is a significant positive

relationship between intercultural communication competence and cultural adaptation (H5).

Prior research has extensively illustrated the connection between intercultural

communication competence and cultural adaptation to a new environment (Lin & Yi, 1997;

Redmond & Bunyi, 1993; Sawyer & Chen, 2012), in particular for international students to

have intercultural communication competence when trying to adapt to a new culture (Mckay-

Semmler & Kim, 2014; Zimmermann, 1995). Without successful and appropriate

conversations with locals, international students are less likely to acquire the necessary

communicative skills to prosper in the new environment (Kim, 2001; Lee & Chen, 2000).

Ultimately, intercultural communication competence is an essential trait for international

students to have when trying to culturally adapting to the American culture.

Preparedness for change and cultural adaptation are also linked in Kim’s (2001)

model. Although there was a positive relationship between preparedness for change and

cultural adaptation, that relationship was not significant (H2). Overall, international students’

level of preparedness for change did not substantially influence their cultural adaptation into

the American culture. This could be due to the disparity between how the American culture is

portrayed in other countries such as China, and the reality of how the American culture really

is (Rawlings & Sue, 2013). In other words, the knowledge international students are being

taught about the American culture through movies and television or even in the classroom

setting may not be consistent with how U.S. American culture actually is (Chen, 2000;

Rawlings & Sue, 2013). Even though previous research has indicated a need for in-depth

training courses prior to travel (Brislin & Kim, 2003; Lin & Yi, 1997), some countries seem

to be basing their training off unrealistic views of the American culture. So when

international students arrive to the United States, their expectations of our culture may be

skewed. A more speculative explanation may be that U.S. culture is so ubiquitously over-

exposed through mass media and mobility that preparation adds little to the sophistication of

foreigners in understanding the culture.

Page 48: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

38

When analyzing the subscales of Ward and Kennedy’s (1999) general adaptation

scale, however, the results varied. Although the composite subscales of cognitive and

behavioral were not significant, the third subscale, ‘adapt abroad,’ did illustrate a small

significantly positive correlation with preparedness for change. The subscale ‘adapt abroad’

was measuring specific aspects international students studying abroad face in university

settings. Prior research has indicated that each university around the world has its own

organization, expectations, workload, and staff-student relations which international students

are expected to adapt (Klineberg & Hull, 1979). Yet, the more prepared international students

are with knowledge of these new aspects before travel, the better their adaptation into the

new culture (Klineberg & Hull, 1979; Selltiz et al., 1963). Even though the findings illustrate

that in general cases, preparedness for change does not relate to cultural adaptation, it is

possible that in educational settings with more structured and specific new cultural aspects,

the knowledge learned prior to travel is easier to implement. If this is the case, it makes sense

that the international students’ level of preparedness for change would significantly affect

their adaptation to life at the new university. This is consistent with the present study’s

findings, which illustrated that even though in general preparedness for change does not

significantly influence cultural adaptation, specific to the university there is a slight

association.

After analyzing the variables illustrated in Kim’s (2001) model to affect cultural

adaptation, the present study added the stereotype dimensions of warmth and competence,

which are found in the stereotype content model (Fiske et al., 2002) and represent

fundamental dimensions of social perceptions (Fiske et al., 2007; Kervyn et al., 2010;

Kervyn et al., 2013). Through extending Kim’s (2001) model with the addition of these

variables, it is thought that a better understanding and awareness of how stereotypes affect

cultural adaptation could be gained. First, the degree to which U.S. Americans are perceived

as either warm or competent was tested (RQ1). The findings indicated that U.S. Americans

are perceived as both warm and competent. When the results were compared together,

however, it was illustrated that U.S. Americans are perceived to be significantly more

competent than warm. Due to the voluntary nature of study abroad programs, it was assumed

that the stereotypes warmth and competence associated to U.S. Americans might vary.

Page 49: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

39

Instead, the findings indicated that international students still view U.S. Americans as more

competent than warm.

When testing the relationship between the stereotypes warmth and competence and

intercultural communication competence (H3), the results were also similar. Even though the

findings illustrated that there was a significant positive relationship between international

students perceptions of U.S. American friendliness (warm) and intercultural communication

competence, this positive relationship is a qualified one. This was because there was only a

significant relationship on Wiemann’s (1977) competence scale and Spitzberg’s (2007)

CSRS skills scale, not on the molar one attached to CSRS. Prior research has indicated that

the more warmth felt toward an interlocutor, the more likely the motivation to continue the

interaction (Cuddy et al., 2008). Although the findings suggest this, there appears to be a

change where being friendly (warm) does not indicate a successful interaction. Both the

perceived competence of U.S. Americans and the lack of warmth needed for successful

interaction might stem from deep-seated perspectives individuals from other countries have

of high status countries like the United States (Glick et al., 2006). And regardless of the

voluntary nature of their travel, will still view these countries with resentment for their

competitive and assertive disposition (Fiske et al., 2007; Glick et al., 2006). This is consistent

with prior research that illustrated U.S. Americans being viewed more competent than warm

(Glick et al., 2006; Trifiletti et al., 2011). In the present study, the only aspect that indicated a

definite successful interaction was the stereotype competence. This was shown to be the case

in all three scales of intercultural communication competence. This is consistent with prior

research that indicates the more an individual is perceived as being intelligent (competent),

the more successful the interaction will be (Abele et al., 2008). In addition to the classes

universities offer that generally prepare individuals for the new culture, there are now

specific classes offered to strengthen students’ intercultural communication competence

(Beamer, 1992). In the present study, through having more intercultural communication

competence, international students appear to be more motivated and successful when

conversing with just as skillfully perceived U.S. Americans. Consequently, where it seems

the warmth stereotype draws individuals together to have an interaction, the competence

stereotype furthers the process of communication along (Cuddy et al., 2008). However due to

the partial abnormality of stereotype warmth and intercultural communication competence

Page 50: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

40

finding, it seems to demand further research. Research and theory continue to unravel the

complementary and ambivalent ways in which the relatively parallel dimensions of warmth

and competence, affiliation and status, negative and positive face, communion and

autonomy, and appropriateness and effectiveness interact in facilitating competent interaction

(e.g. Kervyn, Yzerbyt, & Judd, 2010; Nicotera, Steele, Catalani, & Simpson, 2012).

The results also supported that preparedness for change is significantly related to

international student’s perceptions of U.S. Americans (RQ3). Overall, the more preparedness

for change international students have prior to their study abroad experience, the greater their

perceptions of U.S. Americans being regarded as friendly (warm) and intelligent

(competent). This is a consistent outcome because when the relationship between stereotypes

warmth and competence was tested (RQ2), the findings revealed that warmth and competence

are positively and significantly related. This demonstrates that the perception of U.S.

Americans being warm was positively related with perceptions of their competence. When

the data were further analyzed in the present study, however, preparedness for change did not

affect international students’ perceptions of U.S. Americans as warm, only their perceived

level of competence. This is an unexpected finding because past research has illustrated that

the warmth stereotype tends to dominate over an individual’s perceived level of competence

(Cuddy et al., 2008; Fiske et al., 2007). So if warmth is considered to be the initial stereotype

(Cuddy et al., 2008), it holds valence that U.S. Americans’ warmth, might then be seen as

more important than their competence. Nevertheless, the data to the present study illustrates

that competence is the more important of the two dimensions for preparedness for change,

even though it is correlated with warmth. Due to this, it appears that both dimensions might

serve different roles. Future research should be conducted to explore why this might have

been the end result.

The positive relationship between cultural adaptation and stereotypes warmth and

competence (H4) illustrated a small but significantly positive relationship between cultural

adaptation and stereotype warmth. In general, this finding indicates that the more

international students perceive U.S. Americans as friendly (warm), the more likely they will

adapt to the American culture. Previous research has indicated that when brought into a new

environment, out-group members naturally want to adapt (Park & Judd, 1990). Since,

interaction with locals of the host culture is the first step toward adapting (Kim, 2001), if

Page 51: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

41

U.S. Americans are perceived as friendly (warm), international students are likely to go out

of their way to interact and begin the process of adaptation. Conversely, through further

analyzing the data, it was shown that the subscale of ‘adapt abroad’ did not show a

significant relationship with cultural adaptation. This indicates that the U.S. Americans’

perceived level of friendliness did not influence the international students’ adaptation to the

new classroom environment. This might be because international students were more

concerned with getting an acceptable grade than conversing with the U.S. Americans in their

class.

Whereas there was a significant relationship between the stereotype warmth and

cultural adaptation, the findings of the present study illustrated that there was not a

meaningful one between stereotype competence and cultural adaptation. Overall, the

perceived competence of U.S. Americans did not affect international students’ perceived

cultural adaptation to the American culture. This is not consistent with past research. One

explanation is that in order to culturally adapt, individuals need to be able to develop deeper

relationships (Kim, 2005). It may be that U.S. Americans are able to develop relationships

with individuals from other countries, but international students see those relationships as

relatively superficial (Klineberg & Hull, 1979). Therefore, through not developing deeper

connections, the perceived competence associated to U.S. Americans might not significantly

influence the international students’ cultural adaptation into the American culture. Given this

unexpected finding, more research should be conducted to analyze why this might be the

outcome.

There is an alternative approach that could be taken to analyze the influence the

dimensions of warmth and competence have on international students’ cultural adaptation.

Instead of implementing the general dimensions of warmth and competence, the four

quadrants (cold/incompetent, cold/competent, warm/incompetent, and warm/competent)

could be analyzed in context (Fiske et al., 2002). These quadrants are created from the wide-

ranging dimensions of warmth and competence (Fiske et al., 2002). Through analyzing these

four quadrants in context, instead of general stereotype dimensions, a more comprehensive

understanding might emerge regarding the influence the underlying dimensions have on

cultural adaptation. Such quadrants might separate the sources of interdependent variance

between the dimensions, and elucidate the stylistic and motivational forms such perceptual

Page 52: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

42

schema contributes to intercultural interactions. Thus, through analyzing international

students’ cultural adaptation through these four quadrants, the variance might be restricted,

and a better consistency could be achieved.

IMPLICATIONS

The findings of the present study add to the vast research on cultural adaptation,

specifically, how preparedness for change, stereotypes, and intercultural communication

competence affects the difficult adaptation process that many international students face

when studying abroad. Research to date seems to have only compared one or two of these

variables. By combining these variables this study has provided a more comprehensive

understanding of the process international students undertake in order to culturally adapt.

Along with the general impact the findings of the present study have, there are specific

implications that can be gathered.

The present study provides a clear understanding for what might enable a successful

cultural adaptation. Through testing two variables Kim (2001) demonstrates to affect cultural

adaptation (preparedness for change and host communication competence), the present study

was able to expand knowledge of how other outside variables influence cultural adaptation.

The stereotype dimensions of warmth and competence were examined in relation to each

other, preparedness for change, intercultural communication competence, and cultural

adaptation. Through implementing these two stereotype dimensions, a more comprehensive

understanding can take place due to rudimentary and adaptive behaviors (Cuddy et al., 2008).

The fundamental yet constantly changing nature of these stereotype dimensions (Fiske et al.,

2002) could serve as important insights into the process of cultural adaptation. For example

in the present study, U.S. Americans were more likely to be seen as competent than warm.

Thus, when international students arrive to the United States, they are anticipating a more

skillful interaction with U.S. Americans than a friendly one. Through this anticipation,

expectations might be contrived that could affect their cultural adaptation into the American

culture.

Overall, international students’ preparedness for change was associated with their

perceived stereotypes of warmth and competence toward U.S. Americans. Therefore, the

more knowledge the individual gained prior to travel, the better their overall perceptions of

Page 53: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

43

U.S. Americans were. However, only the warmth stereotype was related to cultural

adaptation. Through structural equation modeling, research should look further into how

preparedness for change, cultural adaptation, and stereotype experiences affect their

associations with an assortment of perceptions toward groups of people. Such research would

facilitate generalization of Kim’s (2001) theory to be used in other types of contexts.

Along with the theoretical implications to the present study, there are also some

practical implications. A major implication is improved study abroad programs that focus on

integrating the international students into the new culture. Due to the increased globalization

of our world, study abroad programs appear to have become a popular college experience.

For most international students, studying abroad in the United States it is not just about the

educational opportunity (Zimmermann, 1995), but a chance to learn about U.S. American

culture and English language (Rawlings & Sue, 2013). In order for such programs to

succeed, their students need to be able to communicate competently with U.S. Americans

(Zimmermann, 1995). Future study abroad programs should consider a buddy or indigenous

mentor system where U.S. Americans are paired up with international students. Zimmermann

(1995) contends that this would be helpful for international students’ adjustment into the

American culture. Also, it is speculated that by being paired up, not only would international

students gain the knowledge they seek, but U.S. Americans might also benefit through the

process.

Another practical implication would be effective mandatory training or preparations

programs for international students prior to studying abroad. Previous research and the

present study have shown that training programs would be an efficient means to lessen some

of the problems international students face when studying abroad (Furnham & Bochner,

1982; Kitsantas, 2004). Even though some countries do have prior cultural training, the

training does not provide an accurate portrayal of how the new culture really is, which allows

international students to create unrealistic expectations (Rawlings & Sue, 2013) In addition,

although teaching in the classroom setting is important, actual interactions with individuals

from the new country is more valuable (Zimmerman, 1995). So, new training programs

should focus on getting the participants out of the classroom setting and into firsthand

experiences. It has also been suggested that the lack on contact between groups could enable

negative perceptions (Tan, Fujioka, & Lucht, 1997) Thus, with the correct type of training

Page 54: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

44

programs, international students can learn through trial and error before they travel and start

the beginning stages of effective communication in the new culture.

LIMITATIONS AND FUTURE RESEARCH

As with all research, there were several limitations to the current study. A main

limitation was that the sample was a convenience sample taken at a large southwestern

university. Through this type of sample, not only were there more female (n=137) than male

(n=81) participants, but it was also challenging to find a significant number of participants.

This could have been partially due to the fact that this study was only applicable to

international students and opportunities to recruit these types of students are limited. Next,

this was a cross-sectional study. Since this study was only distributed at one point in time, a

longitudinal study might show different results, especially in the sense that the amount of

time a student has been in the locale, or the degree to which the student has traveled

previously, could affect the participants’ intercultural competence and preparation. It would

also be noteworthy to see if the results from this cross-sectional study would vary from those

of a longitudinal study, which examined cultural adaptation of international students during

their entire study abroad experience. Another limitation was that the international students’

nationality ranged and varied only slightly. A lot of the participants came from either China

(n=31) or India (n=30). Research has shown that the more diverse the new culture is

compared to an individual’s home culture, the more those individuals will struggle to adapt

(Chen, 2000; Lin & Yi, 1997; Spencer-Rodgers & McGovern, 2002). Since a bulk of

participants were from cultures distinct to the United States, future researchers might want to

be consistent with the range of their participants’ nationality. This consistency might enable a

more widespread understanding of the adaptation process international students from various

cultures go through. The fact that the sample consisted of students who elected to study in the

U.S. may also indicate a restriction in range of their attitudes toward U.S. Americans,

compared to the attitudes of U.S. Americans of their home indigenous cultural population.

Although this study examined many different variables that affect cultural adaptation,

it only examined the effects the stereotypes of warmth and competence have on international

students’ culture adaptation. In order to extend the stereotype content model, future

researchers might focus on the typologies (paternalistic, envious, contemptuous, and

Page 55: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

45

admiration) that make up these two stereotype dimensions (Fiske et al., 2000). Even though

examination of those typologies in the present study would have constricted this analysis, in

another format they might bring about a more detailed understanding of how each stereotype

affects cultural adaptation. Also, due to the explicit emotional outcomes related to each

typology (Fiske et al., 2000), more information about the role of emotion and its affect within

cultural adaptation would bring more clarity to these results.

CONCLUSION

This research has illuminated the importance of cultural adaptation in today’s society.

It appears that humans have an innate ability to adapt to new environments (Kim, 2001). Due

to this, with study abroad programs gaining more popularity, this research is crucial to the

advancement of such programs. Through extending Kim’s (2001) structural model, new

variables were shown to affect international students’ cultural adaption into American

culture. By conducting studies such as this one, the relationship between various factors and

cultural adaptation could be implemented into new updated study abroad programs. By

highlighting the influence of preparedness for change, stereotypes, and intercultural

communication competence on cultural adaptation, a greater understanding of the trials and

errors international students go through when studying abroad may be obtained. Ultimately,

an individual’s perceptions, interaction capabilities, and expressive means influence cultural

adaptation (Kim, 2001). With such abilities one can begin to understand through verbal and

nonverbal cues of the new environment “what is real, what is true, what is right, what is

beautiful, and what is good” (Kim, 2001, p. 47).

Page 56: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

46

REFERENCES

Abele, A. E., Cuddy, A., Judd, C. M., & Yzerbyt, V. Y. (2008). Fundamental dimensions of

social judgment. European Journal of Social Psychology, 38(7), 1063–1065.

Adrian-Taylor, S., Noels, K., & Tischler, K. (2007). Conflict between international graduate

students and faculty supervisors: Toward effective conflict prevention and

management strategies. Journal of Studies in International Education, 11(1), 90-117.

Alptekin, C. (2002). Towards intercultural communicative competence in ELT. English

Language Teaching Journal, 56(1), 57-64.

Andrews, D. C., & Henze, B. (2009). Teaching professional writing to American students in

a study abroad program. Business and Professional Communication Quarterly, 72(1),

5–20.

Arasaratnam, L. A. (2006). Further testing of a new model of intercultural communication

competence. Communication Research Reports, 23(2), 93–99.

Arthur, N. (2003). Preparing international students for the re-entry transition. Canadian

Journal of Counseling, 37(3), 173-185.

Beamer, L. (1992). Learning intercultural communication competence. International Journal

of Business Communication, 29(3), 285-303.

Belz, J. (2003). Linguistic perspectives on the development of intercultural competence in

telecollaboration. Language Learning and Technology, 7(2), 68-99.

Bender, J., Gidlow, B., & Fisher, D. (2013). National stereotypes in tourist guidebooks: An

analysis of auto- and hetero- stereotypes in different language guidebooks about

Switzerland. Annals of Tourism Research, 40, 331-351.

Binggeli, S., Krings, F., & Sczesny, S. (2014). Stereotype content associated with immigrant

group in Switzerland. Swiss Journal of Psychology, 73(3), 123-133.

Black, J., & Gregersen, H. (1990). Expectations, satisfaction, and intention to leave of

American expatriate managers in Japan. International Journal of Intercultural

Relations, 14(4), 485-506.

Bond, M. (1986). Mutual stereotypes and the facilitation of interaction across cultural lines.

International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 10(3), 259-276.

Brislin, R. W., & Kim, E. S. (2003). Cultural diversity in people’s understanding and uses of

time. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 52(3), 363-382.

Page 57: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

47

Carlson, J. S., & Widaman, K. F. (1988). The effects of study abroad during college on

attitudes toward other cultures. International Journal of Intercultural Relations,

12(1), 1-18.

Chang, Y. (2013). Never mind? Journal of Intercultural Communication, 32, 5.

Chapdelaine, R., & Alexitch, L. (2004). Social skills difficulty: Model of culture shock for

international graduate students. Journal of College Student Development, 45(2), 167-

184.

Chen, Z. (2000). International students' preparation for and adaptation to the American

higher education system: A study of cross-cultural communication. World

Communication, 29, 25-48.

Church, A. (1982). Sojourner adjustment. Psychological Bulletin, 91(3), 540-572.

Cuddy, A., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2008). Warmth and competence as universal dimensions

of social perception: The stereotype content model and the BIAS map. In M. P. Zanna

(Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 61–150). New York, NY:

Academic Press.

Cuddy, A., Fiske, S. T., Kwan, V., Glick, P., Demoulin, S., Leyens, J., … Bond, M. (2009).

Stereotype content model across cultures: Towards universal similarities and some

differences. British Journal of Social Psychology, 48(pt 1), 1-33.

Cupach, W. R., & Spitzberg, B. H. (1983). Trait versus state: A comparison of dispositional

and situational measures of interpersonal communication competence. Western

Journal of Speech Communication, 47(4), 364-379.

De Verthelyi, R. (1995). International students’ spouses: Invisible sojourners in the culture

shock literature. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 19(3), 387-411.

Dovidio, J., Hewstone, M., Glick, P., & Esses, V. (2010). Prejudice, stereotyping and

discrimination: Theoretical and empirical overview. In J. Dovidio, M. Hewstone, P.

Glick, & V. Esses (Eds.), The Sage handbook of prejudice, stereotyping and

discrimination (pp. 3-28). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Ebert, I., Steffens, M., & Kroth, A. (2014). Warm, but maybe not so competent?-

Contemporary implicit stereotypes of women and men in Germany. Sex Roles, 70(9-

10), 359-375.

Eckes, T. (2002). Paternalistic and envious gender stereotypes: Testing predictions from the

stereotype content model. Sex Roles, 47(3-4), 99-114.

Edwards-Joseph, A., & Baker, S. (2012). Themes Caribbean overseas students perceive

influence their levels of culture shock. College Student Journal, 46(4), 716-729.

Ellenwood, A. E., & Snyders, F. J. A. (2010). Virtual journey coupled with face-to-face

exchange: Enhancing the cultural sensitivity and competence of graduate students.

Intercultural Education, 21(6), 549-566.

Fiske, S. (1998). Stereotyping, prejudice, and discrimination. In D. T. Gilbert, S. T. Fiske, &

G. Lindzey (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (4th

ed., Vol. 1, pp. 357–414).

New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

Page 58: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

48

Fiske, S. (2012). Managing ambivalent prejudices: Smart-but-cold and warm-but-dumb

stereotypes. The Annals of the American Academy of Political and Social Science,

639(1), 33-48.

Fiske, S., Cuddy, A., & Glick, P. (2007). Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth

and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 77-83.

Fiske, S., Cuddy, A., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype

content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and

competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(6), 878-902.

Fiske, S., Xu, J., Cuddy, A., & Glick, P. (1999). (Dis)respecting versus (dis)liking: Status and

interdependence predict ambivalent stereotypes of competence and warmth. Journal

of Social Issues, 55(3), 473-489.

Furnham, A., & Bochner, S. (1982). Social difficulty in a foreign culture: An empirical

analysis of culture shock. In S. Bochner (Ed.), Cultures in contact: Studies in cross-

cultural interaction (pp. 161-198). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon Press.

Gaw, K. (2000). Reverse culture shock in students returning from overseas. International

Journal of Intercultural Relations, 24(1), 83-104.

Gertsen, M. (1990). Intercultural competence and expatriates. International Journal of

Human Resource Management, 1(3), 341-362.

Glick, P., & Fiske, S. T. (2001). Ambivalent stereotypes as legitimizing ideologies:

Differentiating paternalistic and envious prejudice. In J. Jost & B. Major (Eds.), The

psychology of legitimacy: Emerging perspectives on ideology, justice, and intergroup

relations (pp. 278– 306). New York, NY: Cambridge University Press.

Glick, P., Fiske, S. T., Abrams, D., Dardenne, B., Ferreira, M. C., Gonzalez, R.,…Yzerbyt,

V. (2006). Anti-American sentiment and America’s perceived intent to dominate: An

11-nation study. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 28(4), 363-373.

Glock, S., & Krolak-Schwerdt, S. (2013). Does nationality matter? The impact of

stereotypical expectations on student teachers’ judgments. Social Psychology of

Education, 16(1), 111-127.

Goldstein, S., & Kim, R. (2006). Predictors of U.S. college student’s participation in study

abroad programs: A longitudinal study. International Journal of Intercultural

Relations, 30(4), 507-521.

Guan, Y., Deng, H., & Bond, M. H. (2010). Examining stereotype content model in a

Chinese context: Inter-group structural relations and mainland Chinese’s stereotypes

towards Hong Kong Chinese. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 34(4),

393-399.

Hamad, R., & Lee, C. M. (2013). An assessment of how length of study abroad programs

influences cross-cultural adaptation. Journal of Human Behavior in the Social

Environment, 23(5), 661-674.

Harris, P. R., & Moran, R. T. (1987). Managing cultural differences. Houston, TX: Gulf

Publishing Company.

Page 59: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

49

Herfst, S. L., van Oudenhoven, J. P., & Timmerman, M. E. (2008). Intercultural effectiveness

training in three Western immigrant countries: A cross-cultural evaluation of critical

incidents. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 32(1), 67-80.

Hoyle, R. H., Pinkley, R. L., & Insko, C. A. (1989). Perceptions of social behavior: Evidence

of differing expectations for interpersonal and intergroup interaction. Personality and

Social Psychology Bulletin, 15(3), 365-376.

Institute of International Education [IIE]. (2013). Open doors 2013: Report on international

educational exchange. Washington, DC: National Press Club.

Isabelli-García, C. (2006). Study abroad social networks, motivation and attitudes:

Implications for second language acquisition. In M. A. DuFon & E. Churchill (Eds.),

Language learners in study abroad contexts (pp. 231-258). Tonawanda, NY:

Multilingual Matters Ltd.

Jung, E., Hecht, M., & Wadsworth, B. (2007). The role of identity in international students’

psychological well-being in the United States: A model of depression level, identity

gaps, discrimination, and acculturation. International Journal of Intercultural

Relations, 31(5), 605-624.

Kervyn, N., Dolderer, M., Mahieu, T., & Yzerbyt, V. (2010). Atypicality and the two

fundamental dimensions: Applying the negativity effect on warmth to group

perception. European Journal of Social Psychology, 40(3), 484-489.

Kervyn, N., Fiske, S., & Yzerbyt, V. (2013). Integrating the stereotype content model

(warmth and competence) and the Osgood semantic differential (evaluation, potency,

and activity). European Journal of Social Psychology, 43(7), 673-681.

Kervyn, N., Yzerbyt, V., & Judd, C. (2010). Compensation between warmth and

competence: Antecedents and consequences of a negative relation between the two

fundamental dimensions of social perception. European Review of Social Psychology,

21, 155-187.

Kervyn, N., Yzerbyt, V., Judd, C., & Nunes, A. (2009). A question of compensation: The

social life of the fundamental dimensions of social perception. Journal of Personality

and Social Psychology, 96(4), 828-842.

Kim, Y.Y. (1988). Communication and cross-cultural adaptation: An integrative theory.

Philadelphia, PA: Multilingual Matters LTD.

Kim, Y. Y. (1995). Cross-cultural adaptation: An integrative theory. In R. L. Wiseman (Ed.),

Intercultural communication theory (pp. 170-193). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Kim, Y. Y. (2001). Becoming intercultural: An integrative theory of communication and

cross-cultural adaptation. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Kim, Y. Y. (2003). Adapting to an unfamiliar culture: An interdisciplinary overview. In W.

B. Gudykunst (Ed.), Cross-cultural and intercultural communication (pp. 243-257).

Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Page 60: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

50

Kim, Y. Y. (2005). Adapting to a new culture: An integrative communication theory. In W.

B. Gudykunst (Ed.), Theorizing about intercultural communication (pp. 375-400).

Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Kim, Y. Y., & Mckay-Semmler, K. (2013). Social engagement and cross-cultural adaptation:

An examination of direct- and mediated interpersonal communication activities of

educated non-natives in the United States. International Journal of Intercultural

Relations, 37(1), 99-112.

Kitsantas, A. (2004). Studying abroad: The role of college students’ goals on the

development of cross-cultural skills and global understanding. College Student

Journal, 38(3), 441-452.

Klineberg, O., & Hull, W. (1979). At a foreign university: An international study of

adaptation and coping. New York, NY: Praeger.

Knutson, T. J., Komolsevin, R., Chatiketu, P., & Smith, V. R. (2003). A cross-cultural

comparison of Thai and U.S. American rhetorical sensitivity: Implications for

intercultural communication effectiveness. International Journal of Intercultural

Relations, 27(1), 63-78.

Koskinen, L., & Tossavainen, K. (2004). Study abroad as a process of learning intercultural

competence in nursing. International Journal of Nursing Practice, 10(3), 111-120.

Krings, F., Sczesny, S., & Kluge, A. (2011). Stereotypical inferences as mediators of age

discrimination: The role of competence and warmth. British Journal of Management,

22(2), 187-201.

Lee, B., & Chen, L. (2000). Cultural communication competence and psychological

adjustment: A study of Chinese immigrant children’s cross-cultural adaptation in

Canada. Communication Research, 27(6), 764-792.

Lee, T., & Fiske, S. (2006). Not an outgroup, not yet an ingroup: Immigrants in the

stereotype content model. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 30, 751-

768.

Lin, C. (2006). Culture shock and social support: An investigation of a Chinese student

organization on a U.S. campus. Journal of Intercultural Communication Research,

35(2), 117-137.

Lin, J., & Yi, K. (1997). Asian international students’ adjustment: Issues and program

suggestions. College Student Journal, 31, 473–479.

Lin, M., Kwan, V., Cheung, A., & Fiske, S. (2005). Stereotype content model explains

prejudice for an envied outgroup: Scale of anti-Asian American stereotypes.

Personality & Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(1), 34-47.

Liu, S. (2014). Becoming intercultural: Exposure to foreign cultures and intercultural

competence. China Media Research, 10(3), 7-14.

MacIntyre, P. D., Baker, S. C., Clément, R., & Conrod, S. (2001). Willingness to

communicate, social support, and language-learning orientations of immersion

students. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 23(3), 369–388.

Page 61: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

51

Martin, J., Bradford, L., & Rohrlich, B. (1995). Comparing predeparture expectations and

post-sojourn reports: A longitudinal study of U.S. students abroad. International

Journal of Intercultural Relations, 19(1), 87-110.

McKay-Semmler, K., & Kim, Y. Y. (2014). Cross-cultural adaptation of Hispanic youth: A

study of communication patterns, functional fitness, and psychological health.

Communication Monographs, 81(2), 133-156.

McLaren, L. M. (2003). Anti-immigrant prejudice in Europe: Contact, threat perception, and

preferences for the exclusion of migrants. Social Forces, 81(3), 909-936.

Mizera, L., Tulviste, T., Konstabel, K., & Lausa, E. (2013). Silent and slow Estonians,

emotional and fast Russians: A comparative study of communication stereotypes in

two neighboring countries. Communication Quarterly, 61(3), 268-283.

Mustaffa, C., & Ilias, M. (2013). Relationship between students adjustment factors and cross

cultural adjustment: A survey at the northern university of Malaysia. Intercultural

Communication Studies, 22(1), 279-300.

Nicotera, A., Steele, J., Catalani, A., & Simpson, N. (2012). Conceptualization and test of an

aggression competence model. Communication Research Reports, 29(1), 12-25.

Olaniran, B. (1996). Social skills acquisition: A closer look at foreign students on college

campuses and factors influencing their level of social difficulty in social situations.

Communication Studies, 47(1-2), 72-88.

Oommen, D. (2013). The relationship between mental distress, assessed in terms of anxiety

and depression, and conflict management in the context of cultural adaptation.

Journal of Intercultural Communication Research, 42(3), 91-111.

Park, B., & Judd, C. M. (1990). Measures and models of perceived group variability. Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 59(2), 173-191.

Park, B., Ryan, C. S., & Judd, C. M. (1992). Role of meaningful sub-groups in explaining

differences in perceived variability for in-groups and out-groups. Journal of

Personality and Social Psychology, 63(4), 553-567.

Pitts, M. (2009). Identity and the role of expectations, stress, and talk in short-term student

sojourner adjustment: An application of the integrative theory of communication and

cross-cultural adaptation. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 33(6), 450-

462.

Rawlings, M., & Sue, E. (2013). Preparedness of Chinese students for American culture and

communicating in English. Journal of International Students, 3, 29-40.

Redmond, M., & Bunyi, J. (1993). The relationship of intercultural communication

competence with stress and the handling of stress as reported by international

students. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 17(2), 235-254.

Rohrlich, B., & Martin, J. (1991). Host country and reentry adjustment of student sojourners.

International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 15(2), 163-182.

Page 62: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

52

Ruben, B., & Kealey, D. (1979). Behavioral assessment of communication competency and

the prediction of cross-cultural adaptation. International Journal of Intercultural

Relations, 3(1), 15-47.

Ruble, R., & Zhang, Y. (2013). Stereotypes of Chinese international students held by

Americans. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 37(2), 202-211.

Ryan, C. S., & Bogart, L. M. (1997). Development of new group member’s in-group and out-

group stereotypes: Changes in perceived group variability and ethnocentrism. Journal

of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(4), 719-732.

Sablina, S., & Kopiatina, O. (2013). The role of language-game in sociocultural adaptation:

Russian students in France. International Education Studies, 6(4), 10-19.

Sawyer, R., & Chen, G. (2012). The impact of social media on intercultural adaptation.

Intercultural Communication Studies, 21(2), 151-169.

Segalowitz, N., Freed, B., Collentine, J., Lafford, B., Lazar, N., & Díaz-Campos, M. (2004).

A comparison of Spanish second language acquisition in two different learning

contexts: Study abroad and the domestic classroom. Frontiers: The Interdisciplinary

Journal of Study Abroad, 10, 1-18.

Selltiz, C., Christ, J., Havel, J., & Cook, S. (1963). Attitudes and social relations of foreign

students in the United States. Minneapolis, MN: Lund Press.

Shackleford, N. (2011). Japanese students in a New Zealand homestay programme: Issues of

linguistic and intercultural competence. Communication Journal of New Zealand,

12(1), 71-81.

Shupe, E. (2007). Clashing cultures: A model of international student conflict. Journal of

Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38(6), 750-771.

Spencer-Rodgers, J., & McGovern, T. (2002). Attitudes toward the culturally different: The

role of intercultural communication barriers, affective responses, consensual

stereotypes, and perceived threat. International Journal of Intercultural Relations,

26(6), 609-631.

Spitzberg, B. H. (2007). CSRS: The conversational skills rating scale—An instructional

assessment of interpersonal competence (NCA Diagnostic Series, 2nd

Edition).

Annandale, VA: National Communication Association.

Spitzberg, B. H., & Changnon, G. (2009). Conceptualizing intercultural competence. In D. K.

Deardorff (Ed.), The Sage handbook of intercultural competence (pp. 2-52).

Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage.

Sussman, N. (2002). Testing the cultural identity model of the cultural transition cycle:

Sojourners return home. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 26, 391-

408.

Tan, A., Fujioka, Y., & Lucht, N. (1997). Native American stereotypes, TV portrayals, and

personal contact. Journalism & Mass Communication Quarterly, 74(2), 265-284.

Tomich, P., McWhirter, J., & King, W. (2000). International student adaptation: Critical

variables. International Education, 29, 37-46.

Page 63: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

53

Trifiletti, E., Andrighetto, L., Rattazzi, M., Visintin, E., & Falvo, R. (2011). More competent

than warm: The implicit stereotypes of Americans. Testing, Psychometrics,

Methodology in Applied Psychology, 18(4), 199-209.

Ward, C., & Kennedy, A. (1999). The measurement of sociocultural adaptation. International

Journal of Intercultural Relations, 23(4), 659-677.

Wiemann, J. (1977). Explication and test of a model of communicative competence. Human

Communication Research, 3(3), 195-213.

Williams, T. R. (2005). Exploring the impact of study abroad on students’ intercultural

communication skills: Adaptability and sensitivity. Journal of Studies in International

Education, 9(4), 356-371.

Yashima, T., Zenuk-Nishide, L., & Shimizu, K. (2004). The influence of attitudes and affect

on willingness to communicate and second language communication. Language

Learning, 54(1), 119–152.

Yu, B., & Shen, H. (2012). Predicting roles of linguistic confidence, integrative motivation

and second language proficiency on cross-cultural adaptation. International Journal

of Intercultural Relations, 36(1), 72-82.

Yzerbyt, V., Kervyn, N., & Judd, C. (2008). Compensation versus halo: The unique relations

between the fundamental dimensions of social judgment. Personality and Social

Psychology Bulletin, 34(8), 1110-1123.

Zhou, Y., Jindai-Snape, D., Topping, K., & Todman, J. (2008). Theoretical models of culture

shock and adaptation in international students in higher education. Studies in Higher

Education, 33(1), 63-75.

Zimmermann, S. (1995). Perceptions of intercultural communication competence and

international student adaptation to an American campus. Communication Education,

44, 321-335.

Page 64: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

54

APPENDIX

SURVEY

Statement of Informed Consent

You are being asked to partake in a research study. However, before you start the process, it is crucial to read

and understand the following information in order to be aware of what is being asked of you.

Investigators:

Ariel Baumbaugh, M.A. Candidate, School of Communication, San Diego State University

Carmen M. Lee, Ph.D., School of Communication, San Diego State University

Purpose of the Study: The purpose of this study is to assess the cultural adaptation of international students to American culture.

Specifically, this study seeks to assess the stereotypes held of Americans and intercultural communication

competence in relation to cultural adaptation. This study will involve approximately 300 international students.

Description of the Study:

Participation in this study will involve completion of a web-based self-report survey. You will be asked to

specify the attributes you associate with Americans. Additional questions will be asked that deal specifically

with how competent you are at communicating with the locals and your overall cross-cultural adaptation to the

culture. It is recommended that you complete this survey in one sitting. This survey should take no more than

60 minutes.

Risks or Discomforts:

There are no predicted risks seen by completing this survey. However, if you feel uncomfortable answering a

question, feel free to skip it. If some of the questions in the survey cause you to reflect on certain situations that

produce trouble, please feel free to contact Counseling and Psychological Services at S.D.S.U at 619-594-5220.

Incentive to Participate:

If you are enrolled in a participating communication and psychology courses, you may receive extra credit for

completion of this survey. However, if you are not enrolled in participating communication or psychology

courses, no extra credit or compensation will be presented to you. All participants, regardless of credit

awarding, can benefit from participation because this survey will bring individuals closer to understanding the

process of cultural adaptation.

Confidentiality and Anonymity:

Your participation in this survey is completely confidential. Your name will not be linked to the answers in any

way. Moreover, your responses from the questionnaire will be kept confidential and only viewed by the

Investigators.

Voluntary Nature of Participation:

Your participation in this survey is voluntary. Your decision to participate or not will not effect your relations

with SDSU. Also, if you decide not to finish the survey, you may stop immediately without penalty.

Page 65: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

55

Questions About the Study:

If you have any questions about this study, please contact the following individuals:

Ariel Baumbaugh, MA Candidate Carmen M. Lee, PhD

School of Communication School of Communication

San Diego State University San Diego State University

[email protected] [email protected]

If you have questions regarding your rights as a participant in this study, please contact the Division of

Research Administration San Diego State University (619-594-6622; [email protected]).

Consent to Participate:

The San Diego State University Institutional Review Board has agreed to this consent form, as indicated by the

Board’s stamp. The consent form needs to be evaluated annually and becomes invalid on the date that is shown

on the official stamp.

Take as much time as needed in completing this survey. Please answer the questions honestly.

Thank you in advance for your participation.

By checking the “I consent” box, you indicate that you have read the information provided in this document and

have had an opportunity to ask any questions about the study. It also indicates that you agree to be in the study

and have been told that you can change your mind and withdraw your consent to participate at any time.

Finally, by clicking on the “I consent”’ box below, you are only giving your consent to participate and not

giving up any of your legal rights. You can save or print out a copy of this consent form from your computer.

If you consent to participate in this study, please check the “I consent” box below. If you do not consent, you

may close this browser window.

____ I consent

Page 66: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

56

Demographics

In order for a more complete understanding of your responses to this survey, we’d like to ask

you for some general information about yourself. Please answer the following questions by

selecting the appropriate response or filling in the blanks.

What is your sex? (please check one) ___ Male ___ Female

What is your age (please indicate numerically; e.g., 18, 19, etc.)? ________

What is your current college standing?

___ Freshman (1st yr) ___ Sophomore (2

nd yr) ___ Junior (3

rd yr)

___ Senior (4th

yr) ___ 5th

Year Senior or Beyond ___ Other (please

specify):__________

Please indicate your ethnicity (For example: Danish, Hispanic or Latino, Japanese, African

American etc.):__________________

What is the country where you hold nationality? Feel free to select all that apply.

___Afghanistan ___Britain ___ China ___Canada

___ Italy ___Jordan ___Kenya ___New Zealand

___Turkey ___Vietnam ___Zimbabwe ___Other (please specify):_____

*NOTE: The list of nationalities comes from http://www.listofcountriesoftheworld.com/

What is your country of residence? (i.e. the country where you normally live)

___Australia ___Japan ___China ___Britain

___Canada ___Taiwan ___Indonesia ___India

___New Zealand ___Brazil ___France ___Italy ___Other (please specify): _______

Page 67: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

57

Please answer the following question in regard to your ability to speak English. If poorly,

indicate the number “1” and if you speak English excellently indicate the number “5.” Please

feel free to specify any number between 1 and 5.

Poorly (1) Excellently (5)

How well do you speak English? 1 2 3 4 5

How well do you write English? 1 2 3 4 5

How well do you read English? 1 2 3 4 5

How many years have you been speaking English? (please indicate numerically e.g., 1, 2, 3,

etc.) ________

What is your primary (first) language? ________________

What language do you speak with most of your friends? ________________

What language do you speak with most of your family? ________________

Do you speak any other languages besides [adaptive insert of primary language] and

English? ___No ___Yes (please specify languages): _______________

How many times have you been to the United States before this study abroad

semester/year? (Please indicate numerically; e.g., 1, 2, 3, etc.) ________

Page 68: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

58

For the following, please indicate overall how you would rate yourself when conversing with

others. Specifically, for the descriptions of behaviors listed below, we would like for you to

rate how skillfully YOU use, or do not use, the following communicative behaviors in

conversations with others. If inadequate (awkward or disruptive) indicate "1," if excellent

(smooth or controlled) indicate "5." Choose any response between “inadequate” and

“excellent.”

Inadequate (1) Excellent (5)

1. Speaking rate (neither too slow nor too fast) 1 2 3 4 5

2. Speaking fluency (pauses, silences, “uh”, etc.) 1 2 3 4 5

3. Vocal confidence (neither too tense/nervous nor

overly confident sounding)

1 2 3 4 5

4. Articulation (clarity of pronunciation and

linguistic expression)

1 2 3 4 5

5. Vocal variety (neither overly monotone nor

dramatic voice)

1 2 3 4 5

6. Volume (neither too loud nor too soft) 1 2 3 4 5

7. Posture (neither too closed/formal nor too

open/informal)

1 2 3 4 5

8. Lean toward partner (neither too forward nor too

far back)

1 2 3 4 5

9. Shaking or nervous twitches (aren’t noticeable or

distracting)

1 2 3 4 5

10. Unmotivated movements (tapping feet, fingers,

hair-twirling, etc.)

1 2 3 4 5

11. Facial expressiveness (neither blank nor

exaggerated)

1 2 3 4 5

12. Nodding of head in response to partner statements 1 2 3 4 5

13. Use of gestures to emphasize what is being said 1 2 3 4 5

14. Use of humor and/or stories 1 2 3 4 5

Page 69: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

59

15. Smiling and/or laughing 1 2 3 4 5

16. Use of eye contact 1 2 3 4 5

17. Asking of questions 1 2 3 4 5

18. Speaking about partner (involvement of partner as

a topic of conversation)

1 2 3 4 5

19. Speaking about self (neither too much nor too

little)

1 2 3 4 5

20. Encouragements or agreements (encouragement

of partner to talk)

1 2 3 4 5

21. Personal opinion expression (neither too passive

nor aggressive)

1 2 3 4 5

22. Initiation of new topics 1 2 3 4 5

23. Maintenance of topics and follow-up comments 1 2 3 4 5

24. Interruption of partner speaking turns 1 2 3 4 5

25. Use of time speaking relative to partner 1 2 3 4 5

For the next set of statements, please rate your overall communication performance.

I was a(n)…

26. Poor Conversationalist 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Good Conversationalist

27. Socially Unskilled 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Socially Skilled

28. Incompetent Communicator 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Competent Communicator

29. Inappropriate Communicator 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Appropriate Communicator

30. Ineffective Communicator 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 Effective Communicator

Page 70: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

60

Preparedness for change is the level of readiness for travel an individual has. This next

section deals with the preparedness for change one has before they study aboard. In this

section, try to remember back to before you came to the United States to study abroad. Please

respond to the following and indicate how true each statement is. If strongly disagree

indicate "1," if strongly agree indicate "5." Choose any response between “strongly disagree”

and “strongly agree.”

Strongly Strongly

Disagree Agree

31. I have received a lot of schooling and formal education 1 2 3 4 5

32. I have had training in the host culture’s cultural

practices/customs prior to traveling

1 2 3 4 5

33. I have had training in the host culture’s language prior

to traveling

1 2 3 4 5

34. I have had prior traveling experiences with different

cultures

1 2 3 4 5

35. I voluntarily studied abroad 1 2 3 4 5

Please respond to the following statements and indicate your general experience in the

United States. When answering, specify the amount of difficulty that is experienced. Choose

any response between "no difficulty" and "extreme difficulty".

No Extreme

Difficulty Difficulty

36. Making friends 1 2 3 4 5

37. Using the transport system 1 2 3 4 5

38. Making yourself understood 1 2 3 4 5

39. Getting used to the pace of life 1 2 3 4 5

40. Expressing your ideas in class 1 2 3 4 5

41. Going shopping 1 2 3 4 5

42. Going to social events/gatherings/functions 1 2 3 4 5

Page 71: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

61

43. Understanding the local value system 1 2 3 4 5

44. Talking about yourself with others 1 2 3 4 5

45. Understanding jokes and humor 1 2 3 4 5

46. Dealing with someone who is

unpleasant/cross/aggressive

1 2 3 4 5

47. Getting used to the local food 1 2 3 4 5

48. Following rules and regulations 1 2 3 4 5

49. Understanding the locals’ world view 1 2 3 4 5

50. Dealing with the bureaucracy 1 2 3 4 5

51. Making yourself understood 1 2 3 4 5

52. Adapting to local accommodation 1 2 3 4 5

53. Communicating with people of a different

ethic group

1 2 3 4 5

54. Understanding what is required of you at

university

1 2 3 4 5

55. Relating to members of the opposite sex 1 2 3 4 5

56. Dealing with unsatisfactory service 1 2 3 4 5

57. Seeing things from the locals’ point of view 1 2 3 4 5

58. Dealing with the climate 1 2 3 4 5

59. Coping with academic work 1 2 3 4 5

60. Dealing with people staring at you 1 2 3 4 5

61. Accepting/understanding the local political

system

1 2 3 4 5

62. Dealing with people in authority 1 2 3 4 5

63. Taking a local perspective on the culture 1 2 3 4 5

64. Worshipping in your usual way 1 2 3 4 5

65. Finding your way around 1 2 3 4 5

66. Understanding cultural differences 1 2 3 4 5

67. Dealing with foreign staff at the university 1 2 3 4 5

68. Being able to see two sides of an

intercultural issue

1 2 3 4 5

Page 72: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

62

69. How many conversations a week do you typically have with Americans since being in

the United States? ________

Please respond to the following and indicate how true each statement is. When answering,

specify YOUR perceptions of U.S. Americans you have interacted with since being in the

United States. If not at all, indicate "1," if extremely indicate "5." If helpful, think back to a

recent conversation with an US American. Choose any response between “not at all” and

“extremely.”

Not At All (1) Extremely (5)

70. Americans are competent 1 2 3 4 5

71. Americans are confident 1 2 3 4 5

72. Americans are independent 1 2 3 4 5

73. Americans are competitive 1 2 3 4 5

74. Americans are intelligent 1 2 3 4 5

75. Americans are tolerant 1 2 3 4 5

76. Americans are warm 1 2 3 4 5

77. Americans are good-natured 1 2 3 4 5

78. Americans are sincere 1 2 3 4 5

Please take a couple of moments to recall the most recent interaction YOU have had with an

American while studying abroad in the United States. Think about an interaction that has

lasted at least fifteen minutes. Try and imagine yourself there at that exact moment.

79. Where was it (i.e., place/location)? __________________

80. How long ago did this recalled conversation take place? __________________

81. What was the first initial of the person’s name that the conversation was with?______

Page 73: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

63

Keep the above memory and conversation in your mind when answering the following

statements.

Please respond and indicate how each statement depicts your style of communication in that

conversation. Again, when answering, specify how those statements apply to YOU. If

strongly disagree, indicate "1," if strongly agree indicate "5." Choose any response between

"strongly disagree" to "strongly agree”.

Strongly Strongly

Disagree Agree

82. I find it easy to get along with others 1 2 3 4 5

83. I can adapt to changing situations 1 2 3 4 5

84. I treat people as individuals 1 2 3 4 5

85. I feel as if I am rewarding to talk to 1 2 3 4 5

86. I can deal with others effectively 1 2 3 4 5

87. I am a good listener 1 2 3 4 5

88. My personal relations are cold and distant 1 2 3 4 5

89. I feel like I am easy to talk to 1 2 3 4 5

90. I wont argue with someone just to prove I am

right

1 2 3 4 5

91. I ignore other people’s feelings 1 2 3 4 5

92. I generally know how others feel 1 2 3 4 5

93. I let others know I understand them 1 2 3 4 5

94. I understand other people 1 2 3 4 5

95. I listen to what people say to me 1 2 3 4 5

96. I like to be close and personal with people 1 2 3 4 5

97. I generally know what type of behavior is

appropriate in any given situation

1 2 3 4 5

Page 74: CULTURAL ADAPTATION OF INTERNATIONAL STUDENTS: … · study examines how stereotypes and intercultural communication competence relate to international students’ adaptation to the

64

98. I usually do not make unusual demands of my

friends

1 2 3 4 5

99. I am supportive of others 1 2 3 4 5

100. I can easily put myself in another person’s

shoes

1 2 3 4 5

101. I am a likeable person 1 2 3 4 5

102. I am flexible 1 2 3 4 5

103. People can go to me with their problems 1 2 3 4 5

104. I generally say the right thing at the right time 1 2 3 4 5

105. I like to use my voice and body expressively 1 2 3 4 5

106. I am sensitive to others’ needs of the moment 1 2 3 4 5

107. I am relaxed and comfortable when speaking 1 2 3 4 5

108. I do not mind meeting strangers 1 2 3 4 5

109. I am generally relaxed when conversing with a

new acquaintance

1 2 3 4 5

110. I enjoy social gatherings where I can meet new

people

1 2 3 4 5

111. I am not afraid to speak with people in

authority

1 2 3 4 5

Thank you for your participation!

You have reached the end of the survey, please make sure to click on the submit button

below.

*Note if you are to receive extra credit, you will then be taken to the necessary form.