contingency learning deficits and generalization in chronic unilateral hand pain patients

11
Contingency Learning Deficits and Generalization in Chronic Unilateral Hand Pain Patients Ann Meulders,* ,y Daniel S. Harvie, z Jane K. Bowering, z Suzanne Caragianis, x Johan W. S. Vlaeyen,* ,y,jj and G. Lorimer Moseley z *Research Group on Health Psychology, University of Leuven, Leuven, Belgium. y Center for Excellence on Generalization in Health and Psychopathology, University of Leuven, Leuven, Belgium. z Sansom Institute for Health Research, University of South Australia, Adelaide, Australia. x SA Hand Therapy, Daw Park/Mawson Lakes, South Australia, Australia. jj Department of Clinical Psychological Science, Maastricht University, Maastricht, the Netherlands. Abstract: Contingency learning, in particular the formation of danger beliefs, underpins conditioned fear and avoidance behavior, yet equally important is the formation of safety beliefs. That is, when threat beliefs and accompanying fear/avoidance spread to technically safe cues, it might cause disability. Indeed, such overgeneralization has been advanced as a transdiagnostic pathologic marker, but it has not been investigated in chronic pain. Using a novel hand pain scenario contingency learning task, we tested the hypotheses that chronic hand pain patients demonstrate less differential pain ex- pectancy judgments because of poor safety learning and demonstrate broader generalization gradi- ents than healthy controls. Participants viewed digitized 3-dimensional hands in different postures presented in random order (conditioned stimulus [CS]) and rated the likelihood that a fictive patient would feel pain when moving the hand into that posture. Subsequently, the outcome (pain/no pain) was presented on the screen. One hand posture was followed by pain (CS1), another was not (CS). Generalization was tested using novel hand postures (generalization stimuli) that varied in how similar they were to the original conditioned stimuli. Patients, but not healthy controls, demon- strated a contingency learning deficit determined by impaired safety learning, but not by exaggerated pain expectancy toward the CS1. Patients showed flatter, asymmetric generalization gradients than the healthy controls did, with higher pain expectancy for novel postures that were more similar to the original CS. The results clearly uphold our hypotheses and suggest that contingency learning def- icits might be important in the development and maintenance of the chronic pain–related disability. Perspective: Chronic hand pain patients demonstrate 1) reduced differential contingency learning determined by a lack of safety belief formation, but not by exaggerated threat belief formation, and 2) flatter, asymmetric generalization gradients than the healthy controls. ª 2014 by the American Pain Society Key words: Generalization, hand pain, chronic pain, unconditioned stimulus–expectancy, contingency learning. C ontingency learning is adaptive—the ability to identify cues in our environment that signal threat or negative outcomes promotes survival by initi- ating avoidance (and other defensive responses); the ability to identify cues that signal reward or positive out- comes promotes approach behavior. Basic contingency learning, and more specifically the formation of danger expectancy beliefs, is indeed known to play a causal Received December 21, 2013; Revised July 3, 2014; Accepted July 8, 2014. A.M. is a postdoctoral researcher of the Research Foundation Flanders (FWO-Vlaanderen), Belgium (12E33714N). The study was also supported by the Odysseus Grant ‘‘The Psychology of Pain and Disability Research Program’’ funded by the Research Foundation Flanders (FWO-Vlaanderen), Belgium, to J.W.S.V. (G090208N) and by an EFIC-Grunenthal Research Grant to A.M. G.L.M. is supported by the National Health and Medical Research Council of Australia (ID 1061279). This work was also supported by National Health and Medical Research Council grant (ID 1047317). The authors report no conflict of interest. The data in this study were presented at the symposium on ‘‘Recent Devel- opments in Understanding and Treating Chronic Pain: From Fundamental Research to Novel Targets for Treatment’’ at the 43rd European Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Therapies (EABCT), Marrakech, Morocco, September 2013, and at PainAdelaide, Adelaide, Australia, March 2014. Address reprint requests to Ann Meulders, PhD, Department of Psychol- ogy, University of Leuven, Tiensestraat 102, box 3726, 3000 Leuven, Belgium. E-mail: [email protected] 1526-5900/$36.00 ª 2014 by the American Pain Society http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2014.07.005 1046 The Journal of Pain, Vol 15, No 10 (October), 2014: pp 1046-1056 Available online at www.jpain.org and www.sciencedirect.com

Upload: g-lorimer

Post on 16-Feb-2017

213 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Contingency Learning Deficits and Generalization in Chronic Unilateral Hand Pain Patients

The Journal of Pain, Vol 15, No 10 (October), 2014: pp 1046-1056Available online at www.jpain.org and www.sciencedirect.com

Contingency Learning Deficits and Generalization in Chronic

Unilateral Hand Pain Patients

Ann Meulders,*,y Daniel S. Harvie,z Jane K. Bowering,z Suzanne Caragianis,x

Johan W. S. Vlaeyen,*,y,jj and G. Lorimer Moseleyz

*Research Group on Health Psychology, University of Leuven, Leuven, Belgium.yCenter for Excellence on Generalization in Health and Psychopathology, University of Leuven, Leuven, Belgium.zSansom Institute for Health Research, University of South Australia, Adelaide, Australia.xSA Hand Therapy, Daw Park/Mawson Lakes, South Australia, Australia.jjDepartment of Clinical Psychological Science, Maastricht University, Maastricht, the Netherlands.

ReceivedA.M. is a(FWO-Vlaby the OProgramBelgium,to A.M. GCouncil oHealth anThe authThe dataopments

1046

Abstract: Contingency learning, in particular the formation of danger beliefs, underpins conditioned

fear and avoidance behavior, yet equally important is the formation of safety beliefs. That is, when

threat beliefs and accompanying fear/avoidance spread to technically safe cues, it might cause

disability. Indeed, such overgeneralization has been advanced as a transdiagnostic pathologic marker,

but it has not been investigated in chronic pain. Using a novel hand pain scenario contingency learning

task, we tested the hypotheses that chronic hand pain patients demonstrate less differential pain ex-

pectancy judgments because of poor safety learning and demonstrate broader generalization gradi-

ents than healthy controls. Participants viewed digitized 3-dimensional hands in different postures

presented in random order (conditioned stimulus [CS]) and rated the likelihood that a fictive patient

would feel pain when moving the hand into that posture. Subsequently, the outcome (pain/no

pain) was presented on the screen. One hand posture was followed by pain (CS1), another was not

(CS�). Generalization was tested using novel hand postures (generalization stimuli) that varied in

how similar they were to the original conditioned stimuli. Patients, but not healthy controls, demon-

strated a contingency learning deficit determined by impaired safety learning, but not by exaggerated

pain expectancy toward the CS1. Patients showed flatter, asymmetric generalization gradients than

the healthy controls did, with higher pain expectancy for novel postures that were more similar to

the original CS�. The results clearly uphold our hypotheses and suggest that contingency learning def-

icits might be important in the development and maintenance of the chronic pain–related disability.

Perspective: Chronic hand pain patients demonstrate 1) reduced differential contingency learning

determined by a lack of safety belief formation, but not by exaggerated threat belief formation,

and 2) flatter, asymmetric generalization gradients than the healthy controls.

ª 2014 by the American Pain Society

Key words: Generalization, hand pain, chronic pain, unconditioned stimulus–expectancy, contingency

learning.

Contingency learning is adaptive—the ability toidentify cues in our environment that signal threator negative outcomes promotes survival by initi-

ating avoidance (and other defensive responses); the

December 21, 2013; Revised July 3, 2014; Accepted July 8, 2014.postdoctoral researcher of the Research Foundation Flandersanderen), Belgium (12E33714N). The study was also supporteddysseus Grant ‘‘The Psychology of Pain and Disability Research’’ fundedby theResearch FoundationFlanders (FWO-Vlaanderen),to J.W.S.V. (G090208N) andbyanEFIC-Gr€unenthal ResearchGrant.L.M. is supported by the National Health and Medical ResearchfAustralia (ID 1061279). Thisworkwas also supportedbyNationald Medical Research Council grant (ID 1047317).ors report no conflict of interest.in this study were presented at the symposium on ‘‘Recent Devel-in Understanding and Treating Chronic Pain: From Fundamental

ability to identify cues that signal reward or positive out-comes promotes approach behavior. Basic contingencylearning, and more specifically the formation of dangerexpectancy beliefs, is indeed known to play a causal

Research toNovel Targets for Treatment’’ at the 43rd EuropeanAssociationfor Behavioural and Cognitive Therapies (EABCT), Marrakech, Morocco,September 2013, and at PainAdelaide, Adelaide, Australia, March 2014.Address reprint requests to Ann Meulders, PhD, Department of Psychol-ogy, University of Leuven, Tiensestraat 102, box 3726, 3000 Leuven,Belgium. E-mail: [email protected]

1526-5900/$36.00

ª 2014 by the American Pain Society

http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2014.07.005

Page 2: Contingency Learning Deficits and Generalization in Chronic Unilateral Hand Pain Patients

Meulders et al The Journal of Pain 1047

role in shaping conditioned physiological responses6

and avoidance behavior24 (for overviews, see2,25). Forexample, anomalies in expectancy learning during aclassical fear conditioning procedure—in which aneutral conditioned stimulus (CS) after repeatedpairing with an aversive unconditioned stimulus (US)begins to elicit fear—have been proposed to inducesustained anxiety5,13,28,41 and may in turn augmentpain in susceptible individuals.27,36 Meta-analysis ofexperimental fear conditioning studies,21 together withmore recent empirical evidence,11,14,15,17,23 revealedthat for pathologic anxiety, the failure to inhibit fear inthe presence of safety cues is more characteristic thanexcessive fear to danger cues is.Stimulus generalization, in which individuals extrapo-

late knowledge from one situation to another withoutactually having to experience the new situation, is alsohighly adaptive. However, when threat beliefs, and theaccompanying persistent fear and avoidance, spread toa wide range of novel neutral or technically safe cues,generalization may become overprotective. This processis considered maladaptive and is implicated in patho-logic anxiety-related disability.20,22

Nowadays, it is widely accepted that the conditions forestablishing a causal relationship between 2 neutralevents closely resemble those that foster Pavlovian con-ditioning (with an intrinsically significant US).9,37 Onthe basis of the well-established tradition of human con-tingency learning7 and the presumed involvement ofcontingency learning deficits in chronic pain,16 we devel-oped a contingency learning task based around a clinicalhand pain scenario. By assessing pain expectancy judg-ments as an index of contingency learning, we wereable to evaluate contingency learning and generaliza-tion without relying on experimentally induced pain oraggravating the patients’ clinical symptoms. We hypoth-esized that people with chronic hand pain woulddemonstrate 1) less differential pain expectancy judg-ments (ie, contingency learning deficit) because ofpoor safety learning and 2) broader generalization gra-dients (ie, expecting pain due to a wider range of novelstimuli) than healthy controls.

Methods

ParticipantsThe study used a convenience sample of 48 subjects

including 2 gender and age group-matched diagnosticgroups: 24 hand pain patients (14 females;mean 6 standard deviation [range] age = 48 6 14 [19–71] years) and 24 healthy controls (14 females;mean 6 standard deviation [range] age = 47 6 14 [16–67] years). (We did not use absolute age-matchedgroups, but used 5-year age ranges to match the healthycontrols to the hand pain patient group.We do not thinkthat the capacity to make verbal pain expectancy judg-ments would be significantly different within the pro-posed age ranges.) The most important inclusioncriterion for the hand pain group was to have unilateralpain at some part of the hand for at least 3 months.

Patients were diagnosed with unilateral and chronichand pain by a certified hand therapist before beingnotified of the study. Prior to data collection, currentpain and average pain over the last 2 days were assessedusing a 101-point numerical rating scale (NRS), from 0 =‘‘no pain at all’’ to 100 = ‘‘the worst pain you can ima-gine.’’ The inclusion criterion for the healthy controlgroup was to not have any hand pain. Exclusion criteriafor both groups were as follows: other pain conditions,diagnosed dyslexia, and cognitive impairments or anycondition that might influence the ability to make judg-ments or give verbal ratings (ie, stroke, brain injury, diag-nosed mental health condition). Hand pain patientswere recruited from hand therapy clinics inmetropolitanAdelaide, South Australia, and healthy controls were re-cruited via flyers, social media, and word of mouth, alsofrom metropolitan Adelaide. All participants providedwritten informed consent, and the experimental proto-col was approved by the institutional Human ResearchEthics Committee. After data collection, participantscompleted the Fear of Pain Questionnaire–III,26 thePain Catastrophizing Scale,40 and the Quick Disabilitiesof theArm, Shoulder andHandOutcomeMeasure (QuickDASH).1 More detailed demographic and clinical charac-teristics of both groups can be found in Table 1.

Stimulus MaterialCues or conditioned stimuli (CS1/�), distractor stimuli

(D1–4), and generalization stimuli (GSs1–6) were handpictures created with Poser (Smith Micro Software,Productivity and Graphics Division, Watsonville, CA), a3-dimensional animation program (see Fig 1). Hand pic-tures were presented in 4 different angles (2 medialand 2 lateral orientations) in order to facilitatemotor im-agery based on mental rotation of one’s own hand.33 Allstimuli were presented on a computer monitor, on awhite rectangle with a black background screen. Theoutcome (US) was the text ‘‘pain’’ or ‘‘no pain’’ presentedon the computer screen. Stimulus presentation wascontrolled by a program that was created with Affect4.0, a Windows-based experimental software package.38

ProcedureThe experimental session lasted approximately 40min-

utes. Patients were tested at the hand therapy clinic.Healthy controls were tested either at the University ofSouth Australia or in their own home with a portableset-up. Data were collected by 1 of 4 experimenters(A.M., J.K.B., and 2 others) evenly spread across both pa-tients and controls. In all cases, every effort was made toensure a quiet, softly lit, and comfortable data collectionenvironment. Participants received written informationconcerning the computerized task. The experimenterstarted the computer program, and standardized in-structions appeared on the screen (see Appendix A forverbatim instructions). When the participant read the in-structions, the experimenter asked if there were anyquestions or uncertainties regarding the task. The exper-imenter answered any possible questions and the partic-ipant proceeded with the task. During the task, the

Page 3: Contingency Learning Deficits and Generalization in Chronic Unilateral Hand Pain Patients

Table 1. Demographic and Clinical Characteristics for the Hand Pain Patient Group and HealthyControl Group Separately

CHRONIC HAND PAIN PATIENT GROUP (N = 24) HEALTHY CONTROL GROUP (N = 24)

M (SD) RANGE M (SD) RANGE

Age (y) 48 (14) 19–71 47 (14) 16–67

Current pain report (0–100) 32.88 (23.35) 0–90

Ongoing pain report (0–100) 38.44 (23.76) 0–80

Duration of complaints (mo) 30.71 (62.98) 4–312

Duration of therapy (wk) 58.25 (86.43) 1–364

Questionnaire scores

FPQ 83.96 (17.89) 52–108 77.88 (14.02) 53–108

FPQ—Medical pain 24.92 (8.43) 11–39 25.33 (5.63) 17–37

FPQ—Minor pain* 22.08 (6.17) 12–34 18.42 (4.62) 11–28

FPQ—Severe pain 36.96 (6.59) 22–46 34.13 (7.99) 18–47

PCS 18.33 (7.60) 6–33 14.85 (10.00) 2–30

PCS—Magnification 3.75 (1.48) 1–7 3.13 (2.17) 0–8

PCS—Helplessness 8.08 (4.01) 2–16 5.90 (3.93) .5–12

PCS—Rumination 6.50 (3.20) 1–14 5.83 (4.79) 0–15

QDASH—Disability/symptom score (n = 23) 39.43 (20.58) 6.82–84.09

QDASH—Work (n = 18) 41.67 (30.47) 0–100

QDASH—Sport (n = 5) 58.75 (39.43) 0–100

Abbreviations: M, mean; SD, standard deviation; FPQ, Fear of Pain Questionnaire–III; PCS, Pain Catastrophizing Scale.

NOTE. One patient failed to fill out the QDASH and was excluded from the statistical analysis. The optional modules of the QDASH can only be scored when patients are

still employed or engaged in sports/cultural activities. The sample included 5 patients (21%) with wrist pain, 6 (25%) with thumb pain, 2 (8%) with pain in another

specific finger and, 11 (46%) with more general hand pain; 13 of these patients had left hand pain and 11 patients had right hand pain. 7 patients (29%) used medi-

cation for pain relief.

*P < .05.

1048 The Journal of Pain Generalization and Chronic Pain

experimenter was present in the test room, but out ofview of the participant.

Hand Pain Scenario Contingency LearningTask

We adapted the food allergy task,7 a widely used sce-nario contingency learning task, to make it relevant forhand pain patients. During the task, participants werepresented with a set of hand pictures and they re-sponded to each picture by predicting whether a fictivehand pain patientwould feel painwhen she or hemovedthe hand into the posture displayed in the picture. Uponevery hand picture presentation, after 2 seconds, thequestion ‘‘How much do you expect the patient to feelpain?,’’ and an 11-point NRS from 0 = ‘‘totally not’’ to10 = ‘‘very much’’ appeared on the screen (see Fig 2). Par-ticipants used the left and right arrows of the keyboardto move a red dot on the rating scale, and then clickedthe computer mouse to confirm their pain expectancyjudgment. After they made a judgment, the outcome‘‘PAIN’’ or ‘‘NO PAIN’’ appeared in the middle of thescreen for 1.5 seconds. Then, the background of the com-puter screen cleared for 1 second, the following handpicture was presented, and the procedure was repeated.The pain rating for each picture was the primaryoutcome variable on which contingency learning andgeneralization gradients were evaluated.The experiment consisted of 3 experimental phases: an

acquisition phase, a generalization phase, and a cross-lateral generalization phase (see design in Table 2).The acquisition phase was divided into 2 acquisition

blocks (ACQ), each consisting of 16 trials (pictures), pre-sented in a semirandomized order with the restrictionthat no more than 2 consecutive trials could be of thesame type. Each acquisition block comprised 4CS1 presentations, 4 CS� presentations, and 2 presenta-tions each of the 4 distractor stimuli. The CS1 trials werefollowed by the ‘‘pain’’ outcome, and the CS� and thedistractor trials were followed by the ‘‘no pain’’outcome. Which hand picture served as the CS1 andthe CS� was counterbalanced across participants, andhalf of the participants received acquisition trainingwith left hand pictures whereas the other half receivedacquisition training with right hand pictures. Duringthe acquisition phase, all conditioned stimuli (CSs)were presented twice in each orientation (Fig 1), andthe distractor stimuli were presented once in each orien-tation. The generalization phase included 32 trials, pre-sented in a semirandomized order with the restrictionthat no more than 2 consecutive trials could be of thesame type. This phase consisted of 4 CS1 presentations,4 CS� presentations, and 4 presentations each of the 6generalization stimuli (GSs). The CS1 trials were againfollowed by the ‘‘pain’’ outcome, but the CS� and theGS trials were always followed by the ‘‘no pain’’outcome. During this phase, all CSs and GSs were pre-sented once in each orientation, and GSs were of thesame laterality as the CSs. The cross-generalization phasewas identical to the previous phase, except that 1) noneof the trial types were followed by the ‘‘pain’’ outcome,and 2) mirrored hand pictures were used as GSs (ie, handpictures showing the opposite hand in postures identicalto those used for the previous phases).

Page 4: Contingency Learning Deficits and Generalization in Chronic Unilateral Hand Pain Patients

Figure 1. Hand pictures used as CSs, distractors, and GSs.

Meulders et al The Journal of Pain 1049

Data Analysis OverviewThe data were analyzed using a series of repeated

measures analyses of variance (RM ANOVAs) to examinethe differences in pain expectancy judgments betweenthe hand pain patients and the healthy controls. To testour first main hypothesis, Do chronic hand pain patientsshow less differential pain expectancy judgments(ie, contingency learning deficit) due to poor safety

Table 2. Experimental Design (N = 48)

ACQUISITION GENERALIZATION

CROSS-LATERALGENERALIZATION*

Hand pain

patient group

Healthy control

group

8 � CS1 4 � CS1 4 � mirror CS1

8 � CS� 4 � CS� 4 � mirror CS�4 � D1–4 4 � GSs1–6 4 � mirror GSs1–6

NOTE. CS1 and CS� are 2 hand positions (each presented in 4 orientations).

CS1 and CS� pictures were counterbalanced across participants; D1–4 are

distractors and GSs1–6 are generalization stimuli. Stimuli are presented in a

semirandomized order with the restriction that no more than 2 consecutive pic-

tures could be of the same stimulus type.

*Mirror hand positions of the CS1 are not reinforced andGSs are never followed

by the ‘‘pain outcome.’’

learning?, we conducted an RM ANOVA with 1between-subjects factor (Group; 2 levels: Patient/Con-trol) and 2 within-subjects factors (Stimulus Type; 2levels: CS1/CS� and Block; 2 levels: ACQ1–2). Becausewe had clear a priori hypotheses, follow-up between-group (Patient vs Control) and within-group (Patientand Control separately) planned comparisons wereused to compare differential pain expectancy judgments(CS1 vs CS�) at the end of acquisition (ACQ2). Further,we calculated a between-group (Patient vs Control)planned contrast to evaluate safety learning and dangerexpectancy learning (CS1 and CS� separately) duringthe acquisition phase (ACQ1–2).To test our second main hypothesis, Do chronic hand

pain patients show flatter generalization gradients ofpain expectancy judgments?, we used an RM ANOVAwith between-factor Group (2 levels: Patient/Control)and within-factor Stimulus Type (8 levels: CS1/CS�/GSs1–6). Again, because we had clear a priori hypothe-ses, within-group (Patient and Control separately) andbetween-group (Patient vs Control) linear and quadratictrend analyses were used to further test the differencesin generalization.

Page 5: Contingency Learning Deficits and Generalization in Chronic Unilateral Hand Pain Patients

Figure 2. Flow chart of the experimental task.

1050 The Journal of Pain Generalization and Chronic Pain

Furthermore, 2 exploratory hypotheses were evalu-ated with secondary analyses. One analysis testedwhether pain expectancy judgments were affected bythe congruence between their own painful hand andthe hand picture used as the CS1 in the experimentaltask. In particular, we expected that patients mightshow stronger differential pain expectancy judgmentswhen the fictive patients’ pain in the experimental taskmatched the side on which they themselves reportedclinical pain. This post hoc analysis was possible becauseapproximately half of the participants had left hand pain(n = 13) and the other half had right hand pain (n = 11),and the design was balanced so that the hand pain sce-nario contingency learning task included either left orright hand pictures as CSs during the acquisition training.Thus, half of the patients received congruent acquisitiontraining and the other half received incongruent acquisi-tion training (congruent: left hand pain, left handpicture as CS1 or right hand pain, right hand picture asCS1; incongruent: left hand pain, right hand picture asCS1 or right hand pain, left hand picture as CS1).We car-ried out an RMANOVAwith factors Congruence (2 levels:congruent/incongruent), Stimulus Type (2 levels: CS1/CS�), and Block (2 levels: ACQ1–2) on the data of the pa-tient group alone (n = 24). Post hoc Scheffewithin-group(congruent and incongruent separately) comparisonswere used to compare differential pain expectancy judg-ments (CS1 vs CS�) at the end of acquisition (ACQ2).Another secondary analysis was run to test cross-lateralgeneralization (test with right hand pictures, if acquisi-tion and generalization tests were carried out with lefthand pictures, and vice versa). We expected that hand

pain patients might generalize their pain expectancymore to the opposite hand pictures than healthycontrols. We conducted an RM ANOVA with factorsGroup (2 levels: Patient/Control) and Stimulus Type(8 levels: CS1/CS�/GSs1–6). Statistical analyses were runwith Statistica 11 software (StatSoft Inc, Tulsa, OK).

Results

AcquisitionAs can be seen in Fig 3, pain expectancies were higher

for the CS1 pictures than for the CS� pictures (maineffect of Stimulus Type F[1, 46] = 65.88, P < .001), andas predicted, this difference became greater in the sec-ond acquisition block than in the first (StimulusType � Block interaction, F[1, 46] = 33.44, P < .0001).Moreover, this difference in pain expectancy betweenboth CSs was smaller in the patient group than inthe healthy controls (Stimulus Type � Group interaction,F[1, 46] = 10.65, P < .01) and developed differently duringacquisition for the patients, as compared to thehealthy controls (Stimulus Type� Group� Block interac-tion, F[1, 46] = 7.03, P < .05).Planned comparisons further confirmed that both

the patients (within-group contrast: F[1, 46] = 11.77,P < .01) and the healthy controls (within-group contrast:F[1, 46] = 64.76, P < .0001) successfully acquired differen-tial pain expectancy judgments but that differentiallearning was more substantial in the healthy controlgroup than it was in the hand pain patient group(between-group contrast: F[1, 46] = 13.63, P < .001).

Page 6: Contingency Learning Deficits and Generalization in Chronic Unilateral Hand Pain Patients

Figure 3. Meanpain expectancy judgments for the CS1 and the CS� for the handpain patient group (n = 24) and the healthy controlgroup (n = 24) separately during both acquisition blocks (ACQ1–2). Vertical bars denote 95% confidence intervals.

Meulders et al The Journal of Pain 1051

Interestingly, patients did not have higher pain expec-tancy ratings for the CS1 (F[1, 46] = 1.17, P = .28) thanthe healthy controls had, but they did show higherpain expectancy ratings for the CS– (F[1, 46] = 5.73,P < .05).The differential (CS1 vs CS�) pain expectancy judg-

ments (see Fig 4) did not seem to develop differently

Figure 4. Mean online pain expectancy judgments for the CS1 andbeing trainedwith hand pictures corresponding to their ownpainfulpictures not corresponding to their ownpainful hand side (incongruedenote 95% confidence intervals.

from the first acquisition block to the second dependingonwhether therewas (in)congruencebetween the clinicalpain and the handpicture (CS1) used to predict the fictivepatient’s pain (Stimulus Type� Congruence� Block inter-action, F[1, 22] =1.38,P= .25). Although the3-way interac-tion was not significant, we further interrogated thisexploratory hypothesis that patients might learn better

the CS� for the hand pain patients (n = 24) separately for thosehand side (congruent; n = 12) and those being trainedwith handnt; n = 12) during both acquisition blocks (ACQ1–2). Vertical bars

Page 7: Contingency Learning Deficits and Generalization in Chronic Unilateral Hand Pain Patients

Figure 5. Mean online pain expectancy judgments for the CS1, CS�, and the 6 generalization stimuli (GSs1–6) for the hand pain pa-tient group (n = 24) and the healthy control group (n = 24) separately during both the generalization phase and the cross-lateralgeneralization phase. Vertical bars denote 95% confidence intervals.

1052 The Journal of Pain Generalization and Chronic Pain

to expect pain in response to the CS1, when this handpicture corresponds to their own painful hand side. Inter-estingly, post hoc Scheffe comparisons confirmed that atthe end of the acquisition phase, patients did showdiffer-ential pain expectancy ratings when they were trainedwith congruent hand pictures (F[1, 22] = 11.85, P < .001),but they did not show differential pain expectancyratings when they were trained with incongruent handpictures (F[1, 22] = 3.20, P = .29).

GeneralizationOverall, patients did not give higher pain expectancy

judgments for the novel hand pictures that varied in sim-ilarity between the original CS1 and CS� (GSs) thanhealthy controls (main effect of Group, F < 1), but painexpectancy judgments did vary across these novelhand pictures in both groups (main effect for StimulusType, F[7, 322] = 42.52, P < .0001). More importantly,pain expectancy judgments for these novel pictures var-ied in a manner that was different between patientsand controls (Stimulus Type � Group interaction, F[7,322] = 2.67, P < .05). That is, pictures that were moresimilar to the CS� elicited higher pain expectancy judg-ments in the patients than they did in the healthy controls(see Fig 5). Planned comparisons revealed that there waslinear decrease in pain expectancy judgment withdecreasing GS similarity to the CS1 for the healthy con-trols (F[1, 46] = 61.68, P < .0001), as well as for the patients(F[1, 46] = 19.29, P < .0001). Interestingly however, thisgradient decreased less steeply for the patients than itdid for the healthy controls (F[1, 46] = 5.99, P < .05). Therewas also a quadratic decrease in the pain expectancyjudgment for the healthy controls (F[1, 46] = 25.55,P < .0001) and the patients (F[1, 46] = 17.69, P < .001),

but this quadratic decrease was not different betweengroups, F < 1.

Cross-Lateral GeneralizationPain expectancy judgments in response to themirrored

GSs, that is, pictures of the opposite hand to that usedduring generalization, varied with higher pain expec-tancy judgments for novel hand pictures that weremore similar to the original CS1 (main effect for StimulusType, F[7, 322] = 36.03, P < .0001). As can be seen in Fig 5,overall, patients had higher pain expectancy ratings dur-ing cross-lateral generalization than did healthy controls(main effect for Group, F[1, 46] = 4.12, P < .05), irrespec-tive of stimulus type (Stimulus Type � Group, F < 1).

DiscussionThe findings clearly support our first hypothesis, that

chronic unilateral hand pain patients show a contin-gency learning deficit in comparison with pain-freeage- and gender-matched controls. We found that in asimple contingency learning task based around a clinicalhand pain scenario, the patients acquired less differen-tial pain expectancy judgments than the healthy con-trols. As expected, this pain expectancy bias did notrelate to the CS1; that is, patients did not expect thepain outcome to occurmore following the hand posturesthat were actually paired with the pain outcome, butthey did expect the pain outcome more with the handpostures that were never paired with the pain outcome(CS�). The study also provided preliminary evidence forour secondary hypothesis: the contingency learningdeficit seemed to be attenuated when the a priori painbeliefs of the patient matched the prearranged

Page 8: Contingency Learning Deficits and Generalization in Chronic Unilateral Hand Pain Patients

Meulders et al The Journal of Pain 1053

experimental contingencies. That is, the likelihood ofpicking up the contingency appeared greater when thehand picture paired with the painful outcome corre-sponded to the patient’s own painful hand than whenpictured hands corresponded to the patient’s nonpainfulhand. Critically, this was an exploratory analysis and our3-way interaction was not significant, which means thatthe hypothesis should be a priori tested in a subsequentstudy before the result is endorsed.Our second hypothesis, that patients show a flatter

generalization gradient than healthy controls, was alsosupported. Particularly, patients showed higher pain ex-pectancies to novel hand postures that were increasinglysimilar to the safe (CS�) posture. We also found supportfor our secondary hypothesis relating to themore explor-atory cross-lateral generalization test:Generalizationgra-dients were similar in both groups, but patients reportedhigher pain expectancy for all themirrored handpictures.Contemporary associative learning theory proposes

that learning is based on the information value of predic-tive cues and that conditioneddefensive responses suchasfearandanxiety arebasedonexpectancyabout theoccur-rence of aversive events.24,25 Contingency learningdeficits are assumed to be critically involved in chronicpain–related disability. There is preliminary evidencethat contingency learning during fear conditioning isdisturbed in fibromyalgia patients: When a visual cuewas reinforced with a painful heat stimulus in 50% ofthe trials, fibromyalgia patients appeared less likely toidentify the contingency (50%) than rheumatoidarthritis patients (86%), who were in turn less likely toidentify the contingency than healthy controls were(100%).16 Theauthors concluded that the rateofunawarefibromyalgia patients is high because they have fearlearning deficits, although their design does not allowconclusions about mechanisms, for example, changes inthe nociceptive processing system or in the fear responsesystem. The results of the present study, inwhichwe ruledout this possible confound because we did not explicitlyinduce fearorpain inourprotocol, suggest that thedeficitmight be rooted at amore basic associative learning level.We further suggest that suchbasic learningdeficit inducesincreasing generalized anxiety, which might in turn leadto enhanced pain in chronic pain populations. Althoughspeculative, this suggestion is not outrageous and wouldprovide a missing link between associative fear learningand upregulation of the nociceptive/pain system.The contingency learning deficit in the patients was

expressed by increased pain expectancy in response tohand postures that were never paired with the painoutcome (CS�), but patients did not expect the painoutcome to occur more following the hand posturesthat were in fact paired with the pain outcome. Theseresults strongly suggest a lack of safety learning in thesepatients. These findings corroborate results from otherfields—again mostly stemming from fear conditioningexperiments, such as the lack of safety learning in clinicalanxiety disorders17,23 and in healthy individuals withanxiety proneness.3,11

Overgeneralization has been advanced as a transdiag-nostic pathologic marker in diverse psychological disor-

ders, such as depression,19 posttraumatic stress,17 andpanic,22,23 but so far it has not been investigated withregard to chronic pain. In this study, we demonstratedthat chronic hand pain patients showed a flattergeneralization gradient than healthy controls. Inparticular, patients expected the pain outcome morewhen viewing the novel hand postures that wereincreasingly similar to the safe (CS�) posture. Thisasymmetrical generalization gradient is in line with thelack of inhibition to the nonpainful hand postureduring acquisition. Patients did not learn the safety ofthe CS�, and therefore they also expect the painfuloutcome more with the novel stimuli that are similar tothe original CS�. No such asymmetry between healthycontrols and patients was observed for the CS1; that is,patients do not expect the painful outcome more withthe novel stimuli resembling the original CS1.Some interesting findings deserve further attention.

First, it should be noted that the acquisition trainingitself can be interpreted as a type of generalizationlearning, since the CS1 consisted of the same hand posi-tion presented in 4 orientations. This procedure wasapplied to inducemotor imagerybasedonmental rotationof the hand pictures.8,12,34,39 We reasoned that imaginedhand movements would be more relevant and salient forthe hand pain patients than neutral 2-dimensional pic-tures. We predicted that using 3-dimensional pictureswouldpromoteattentiontothepicturesandenhancecon-tingency learning.35 As a consequence, participants had tointegrate this information and generalize the pain expec-tancyacross the4orientationsofeachstimulus type. Itmaybe possible that patients are slower in extracting thisinformation and formulating clear propositions, but theabsence of any group differences for the CS1 suggestsagainst this possibility.Second, cross-lateral generalization gradients were

similar for both groups, but patients reported higherpain expectancy for all the mirrored hand pictures.This pattern of results may be due to lower pain expec-tancy judgments for the mirrored CS1 than for theoriginal CS1 in the healthy controls but not in thehand pain patients, probably because cross-lateralgeneralization was tested under extinction. Moretentatively, this might imply that patients generalizetheir pain expectancy beliefs more to the oppositehand because they are more uncertain about the safetyof these novel but related stimuli, or because they sim-ply have more firm expectancy beliefs that are resistantto extinction.One important methodological strength is that our

experimental task offers the possibility to tap into verybasic associative learning mechanisms and can be easilyadapted to test other pain populations. We contendthat fundamental pain research would benefit fromthe development and implementation of flexible, easilyapplicable, and ethically approved experimental proce-dures to identify possible contingency learning deficitsin different pain populations without the need toactually administer painful/aversive stimuli.Of course, this study also has some limitations. First, in

contrast to the Jenewein study,16 we did not include any

Page 9: Contingency Learning Deficits and Generalization in Chronic Unilateral Hand Pain Patients

1054 The Journal of Pain Generalization and Chronic Pain

other chronic or acute pain condition as a specificitycontrol, but rather used the broad criterion of ‘‘handpain of at least 3 months’ duration.’’ Future researchwith particular conditions might yield idiosyncraticgroup differences, as has been documented, forexample, for the presence of dysynchiria, a sensory pro-cessing disorder, between complex regional pain syn-drome and chronic neuropathic hand pain.18 Second,this study does not allow us to draw conclusions aboutthe causal relationship of this contingency learningdeficit in the development of chronic pain–relateddisability. Prospective studies or follow-up of patientsafter successful exposure treatment might shed morelight on these dynamics. More specifically, are theselearning deficits present before the onset of chronicpain (ie, vulnerability factor), or do they disappear/diminish after successful treatment (ie, epiphenomenonof chronic pain)? Relevant to this is the demonstratedbenefit of repeated left/right judgments of picturedhands (ie, motor imagery) for people with complexregional pain syndrome of the hand4,30,32,42—perhapsthis effect relates to exposure and redifferentiation ofpainful and nonpainful hand postures. That the timetaken to make such judgments relates strongly to thepain that would be predicted on undertaking theshown movement,31 and improves in advance of symp-tomatic relief,29 seems to support this idea. Third,groups differ at least on 2 aspects: Hand pain patientshave 1) a history of hand pain and 2) real-life pain

during the experimental task. Alternatively, both as-pects might have influenced the contingency judg-ments. Pain interferes with cognitive tasks viaattentional disruption,10 and contingency learning canbe viewed as such a task. Future research shouldexamine whether contingency learning in healthy par-ticipants is equally impaired during experimental pain.To conclude, we have shown that chronic hand pain

patients demonstrate a pain expectancy bias that is char-acterized by a lack of safety learning, but not by exagger-ated pain expectancy toward the CS1, as compared withthe healthy controls. Furthermore, we showed that thesepatients showed flatter, asymmetric generalizationgradients than the healthy controls, with higher painexpectancy for novel stimuli that were more similar tothe original CS�. We argued that contingency learningdeficits and, more importantly, overgeneralization—asa transdiagnostic pathologic marker—represent a prom-ising and underinvestigated pathologic factor thatmight be relevant for pain disorders. Deficits in selectivethreat appraisalmight cause anomalies in fear condition-ing and, therefore, the maintenance of the chronic pain-related disability.

AcknowledgmentsThe authors thank Jacki B. Eads and Rohan G. Miegel

for their assistance in data collection.

References

1. Beaton DE, Wright JG, Katz JN, Group UEC: Developmentof the QuickDASH: Comparison of three item-reduction ap-proaches. J Bone Joint Surg 87A:1038-1046, 2005

2. Boddez Y, Baeyens F, Luyten L, Vansteenwegen D,Hermans D, Beckers T: Rating data are underrated: Validityof US expectancy in human fear conditioning. J BehavTher Exp Psychiatry 44:201-206, 2013

3. Boddez Y, Vervliet B, Baeyens F, Lauwers S, Hermans D,Beckers T: Expectancy bias in a selective conditioning proce-dure: trait anxiety increases the threat value of a blockedstimulus. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry 43:832-837, 2012

4. Bowering K, O’Connell N, Tabor A, Catley MJ, Leake HB,Moseley GL, Stanton TR: The effects of graded motor imag-ery and its components on chronic pain: a systematic reviewand meta-analysis. J Pain 14:3-13, 2013

5. Craske M, Hermans D, Vansteenwegen D: Fear andLearning. From Basic Processes to Clinical Applications.Washington, DC, American Psychological Association, 2006

6. Davey GCL: An expectancymodel of laboratory prepared-ness effects. J Exp Psychol Gen 121:24-40, 1992

7. De Houwer J, Beckers T: A review of recent developmentsin research and theories on human contingency learning. Q JExp Psychol B 55:289-310, 2002

8. Decety J, Perani D, Jeannerod M, Bettinardi V, Tadary B,Woods R, Mazziotta JC, Fazio F: Mapping motor representa-tions with positron emission tomography. Nature 371:600-602, 1994

9. Dickinson A: Contemporary Animal Learning Theory.Cambridge, Cambridge University Press, 1980

10. Eccleston C, Crombez G: Pain demands attention: Acognitive-affective model of the interruptive function ofpain. Psychol Bull 125:356-366, 1999

11. Gazendam FJ, Kamphuis JH, Kindt M: Deficient safetylearning characterizes high trait anxious individuals. BiolPsychol 92:342-352, 2013

12. Gerardin E, Sirigu A, Lehericy S, Poline JB, Gaymard B,Marsault C, Agid Y, Le Bihan D: Partially overlapping neuralnetworks for real and imagined hand movements. CerebCortex 10:1093-1104, 2000

13. Grillon C: Associative learning deficits increasesymptoms of anxiety in humans. Biol Psychiatry 51:851-858, 2002

14. Grillon C, Falls WA, Ameli R, Davis M: Safety signals andhuman anxiety: A fear-potentiated startle study. Anxiety 1:13-21, 1994

15. Haddad ADM, Pritchett D, Lissek S, Lau JYF: Trait anxietyand fear responses to safety cues: Stimulus generalizationor sensitization? J Psychopathol Behav Assess 34:323-331,2012

16. Jenewein J,Moergeli H, Sprott H,HoneggerD, Brunner L,Ettlin D, Grillon C, Bloch K, Br€ugger M, Schwegler K,Schumacher S, Hasler G: Fear-learning deficits in subjectswith fibromyalgia syndrome? Eur J Pain 17:1374-1384, 2013

17. Jovanovic T, Kazama A, Bachevalier J, Davis M: Impairedsafety signal learning may be a biomarker of PTSD.Neuropharmacology 62:695-704, 2012

Page 10: Contingency Learning Deficits and Generalization in Chronic Unilateral Hand Pain Patients

Meulders et al The Journal of Pain 1055

18. Kramer HH, Seddigh S, Lorimer Moseley G, Birklein F:Dysynchiria is not a common feature of neuropathic pain.Eur J Pain 12:128-131, 2008

19. Lenaert B, Claes S, Raes F, Boddez Y, Joos E, Vervliet B,Hermans D: Generalization of conditioned responding: Ef-fects of autobiographical memory specificity. J Behav TherExp Psychiatry 43:S60-S66, 2012

20. Lissek S, Grillon C: Overgeneralization of conditionedfear in the anxiety disorders. J Psychol 218:146-148, 2010

21. Lissek S, Powers AS, McClure EB, Phelps EA,Woldehawariat G, Grillon C, Pine DS: Classical fear condi-tioning in the anxiety disorders: A meta-analysis. BehavRes Ther 43:1391-1424, 2005

22. Lissek S, Rabin S, Heller RE, Lukenbaugh D, Geraci M,Pine DS, Grillon C: Overgeneralization of conditioned fearas a pathogenic marker of panic disorder. Am J Psychiatry167:47-55, 2010

23. Lissek S, Rabin SJ, McDowell DJ, Dvir S, Bradford DE,Geraci M, Pine DS, Grillon C: Impaired discriminative fear-conditioning resulting from elevated fear responding tolearned safety cues among individuals with panic disorder.Behav Res Ther 47:111-118, 2009

24. Lovibond PF: Fear and avoidance: An integrated expec-tancy model, in Craske MG, Hermans D, Vansteenwegen D(eds): Fear and Learning: Basic Science to Clinical Application.Washington, DC, American Psychological Association, 2006

25. Lovibond PF, Shanks DR: The role of awareness inPavlovian conditioning: Empirical evidence and theoreticalimplications. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 28:3-26, 2002

26. McNeil DW, Rainwater AJ: Development of the Fear ofPain Questionnaire–III. J Behav Med 21:389-410, 1998

27. Meulders A, VansteenwegenD, Vlaeyen JW:Women, butnot men, report increasingly more pain during repeated (un)predictable painful electrocutaneous stimulation: Evidencefor mediation by fear of pain. Pain 153:1030-1041, 2012

28. Mineka S, Zinbarg R: A contemporary learning theoryperspective on the etiology of anxiety disorders: It’s notwhat you thought it was. Am Psychol 61:10-26, 2006

29. Moseley GL, Butler D, Beames T, Giles T: The Graded Mo-tor ImageryHandbook. Adelaide,NOIgrouppublishing, 2012

30. Moseley GL: Gradedmotor imagery is effective for long-standing complex regional pain syndrome: A randomisedcontrolled trial. Pain 108:192-198, 2004

31. Moseley GL: Why do people with complex regional painsyndrome take longer to recognize their affected hand?Neurology 62:2182-2186, 2004

32. Moseley GL: Graded motor imagery for pathologicpain—A randomized controlled trial. Neurology 67:2129-2134, 2006

33. Parsons LM: Imagined spatial transformations of one’shands and feet. Cogn Psychol 19:178-241, 1987

34. Parsons LM, Fox PT, Downs JH, Glass T, Hirsch TB,Martin CC, Jerabek PA, Lancaster JL: Use of implicit motorimagery for visual shape discrimination as revealed by PET.Nature 375:54-58, 1995

35. Rescorla RA, Wagner AR: A theory of Pavlovian condi-tioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcementand nonreinforcement, in Black AH, Prokasy WF (eds): Clas-sical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory. NewYork, Appleton-Century-Crofts, 1972, pp 64-99

36. Rhudy JL, Meagher MW: Fear and anxiety: Divergent ef-fects on human pain thresholds. Pain 84:65-75, 2000

37. Shanks DR, Dickinson A: Associative accounts of causal-ity judgment, in Bower GH (ed): The Psychology of Learningand Motivation. San Diego, CA, Academic Press, 1987, pp229-261

38. Spruyt A, Clarysse J, Vansteenwegen D, Baeyens F,Hermans D: Affect 4.0: A free software package for imple-menting psychological and psychophysiological experi-ments. Exp Psychol 57:36, 2010

39. Stephan KM, Fink GR, Passingham RE, Silbersweig D, Ce-ballos-Baumann AO, Frith CD, Frackowiak RS: Functionalanatomy of the mental representation of upper extremitymovements in healthy subjects. J Neurophysiol 73:373-386,1995

40. Sullivan MJ, Bishop SR, Pivik J: The pain catastrophizingscale: Development and validation. Psychol Assess 7:524-532, 1995

41. Vansteenwegen D, Iberico C, Vervliet B, Marescau V,Hermans D: Contextual fear induced by unpredictability ina human fear conditioning preparation is related to thechronic expectation of a threatening US. Biol Psychol 77:39-46, 2008

42. Walz AD, Usichenko T,Moseley GL, LotzeM: Gradedmo-tor imagery and the impact on pain processing in a case ofCRPS. Clin J Pain 29:276-279, 2013

Page 11: Contingency Learning Deficits and Generalization in Chronic Unilateral Hand Pain Patients

Appendix A

Verbatim Instructions of the Scenario Contingency Learning Task

Dear participant,In this experiment, your task is to predict whether a fictive hand pain patient will have pain when he moves his

hand into a certain position. A series of pictures displaying hand positionswill be presented on the computer screen.Upon every presentation of a hand position, youwill be asked to answer the following question: ‘‘Howmuch do youexpect the patient to feel pain?’’ A rating scale will appear at the bottom of the screen on which you can indicateyour prediction.The rating scale goes from 0 to 10, labeled respectively, ‘‘0’’ = I totally not expect the patient to feel pain, and

‘‘10’’ = I very much expect the patient to feel pain. Use the left and right arrows to move the cursor on the ratingscale, and use a left mouse click to confirm your prediction.After you have made your prediction, we will let you know whether the patient did or did not feel pain, so that

you can evaluate the accuracy of your prediction. Your predictions will be random in the beginning of the experi-ment, but do not worry, gradually you will learn which hand positions cause pain in this patient.Do you have any questions? If yes, please ask the experimenter for more explanation. If no, press <ENTER> to start

the experiment.

1056 The Journal of Pain Generalization and Chronic Pain