concept maps and vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to...

25
Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to facilitate meaningful learning Author(s): JOSEPH D. NOVAK Source: Instructional Science, Vol. 19, No. 1 (1990), pp. 29-52 Published by: Springer Stable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/23369903 . Accessed: 14/10/2014 16:27 Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at . http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp . JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range of content in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new forms of scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected]. . Springer is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Instructional Science. http://www.jstor.org This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PM All use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Upload: others

Post on 16-Mar-2020

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to …cmap.systemsci.org/rid=1RNRXZP8D-L0213D-10X/BNU Novak 1990 Concept M… · Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic

Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to facilitate meaningful learningAuthor(s): JOSEPH D. NOVAKSource: Instructional Science, Vol. 19, No. 1 (1990), pp. 29-52Published by: SpringerStable URL: http://www.jstor.org/stable/23369903 .

Accessed: 14/10/2014 16:27

Your use of the JSTOR archive indicates your acceptance of the Terms & Conditions of Use, available at .http://www.jstor.org/page/info/about/policies/terms.jsp

.JSTOR is a not-for-profit service that helps scholars, researchers, and students discover, use, and build upon a wide range ofcontent in a trusted digital archive. We use information technology and tools to increase productivity and facilitate new formsof scholarship. For more information about JSTOR, please contact [email protected].

.

Springer is collaborating with JSTOR to digitize, preserve and extend access to Instructional Science.

http://www.jstor.org

This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 2: Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to …cmap.systemsci.org/rid=1RNRXZP8D-L0213D-10X/BNU Novak 1990 Concept M… · Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic

Instructional Science 19: 29-52 (1990) 29 © Kluwer Academic Publishers, Dordrecht — Printed in the Netherlands

Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to facilitate meaningful learning

JOSEPH D. NOVAK Department of Education, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853, USA.

Abstract. This paper describes two metacognitive tools, concept mapping and Vee diagramming, and

reports on research utilizing these tools from grades one through university instruction. The psycho logical and epistemological foundations underlying these tools is presented briefly. The issues of the

dominantly rote-mode nature of much school learning and the resistance of students (and teachers) to move to meaningful learning strategies fostered by concept mapping and Vee diagramming are dis cussed. The data available to date from a variety of qualitative and quantitative research studies

strongly support the value of these metacognitive tools both for cognitive and affective gains.

Introduction

In the past decade, there has been a rapid increase in instruction that helps students

"learn how to learn". This activity derived in part from advances in cognitive

learning psychology (see Mayer, 1981) and the increase in cognitive learning research in school settings. Flavell (1985) defines metacognition as "cognition about cognition" (p. 104). Metacognitive learning occurs whenever a person

acquires some general strategy that facilitates learning or understanding of know

ledge. Weinstein (1987) has described strategies that can be used for reading comprehension. Ideally, the most powerful metacognitive learning would be

acquisition of strategies that apply at any grade level and to any subject matter. The intelligent construction and use of concept maps and Vee diagrams are two

widely applicable metacognitive strategies we have developed at Cornell

University, and world-wide use of these strategies is being reported increasingly in the literature since publication of Learning how to learn, (Novak and Gowin, 1984; 1988).

Concept maps as we have have developed them are a representation of mean

ing or ideational frameworks specific to a domain of knowledge, for a given con

text of meaning. We define concept as a perceived regularity in events or objects, or records of events or objects, designated by a label. Most of the labels we use

are words, but signs such as +, -, X and so forth may also be used. Two or more

concepts can be linked together with words to form propositions and we see prop ositions as the units of psychological meaning. The meaning of any concept for a

person would be represented by all of the prepositional linkages the person could

construct that include that concept. Since individuals have unique sequences of

experiences leading to unique total sets of propositions, all concept meanings are

This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 3: Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to …cmap.systemsci.org/rid=1RNRXZP8D-L0213D-10X/BNU Novak 1990 Concept M… · Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic

30

Figure 1. A concept map showing key concepts and propositions involved in concept mapping. Linking words together with concepts forms propositions and these are shown in a hierarchical

structure.

This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 4: Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to …cmap.systemsci.org/rid=1RNRXZP8D-L0213D-10X/BNU Novak 1990 Concept M… · Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic

31

to some extent idiosyncratic. However, in a given culture, there is sufficient

commonality in experience that persons in that culture share sufficient common

meanings for their concepts that they can communicate ideas to one another using

language or other symbols. For most of us, we only have to look at a blackboard

filled with mathematical relationships expressed in symbols to realize that this

represents a "different world", a world for which we have no meanings. Figure 1

shows a concept map on concept maps. Over the past dozen years, our graduate students and other colleagues have

found that all domains of knowledge can be represented by concept maps (con

cept/propositional structures). Figure 2 shows representations of knowledge struc

tures in basketball. There is no domain of knowledge (or "skills") for which

concept maps cannot be used as a representational tool, in our experience. Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic tool developed by my colleague,

Bob Gowin, to represent the structure of knowledge and the epistemological ele

ments that are involved in new knowledge construction. Epistemology is that

branch of philosophy that deals with the nature and structure of knowledge.

Epistemological elements are those units that together from the structure of some

segment of knowledge and are required to construct a new piece of knowledge. The Vee heuristic is based on a constructivist epistemology, as contrasted to the

empiricist or positivist epistemology that has characterized popular views of

"knowledge discovery" in most elementary textbooks of science and social sci

ences. Kuhn (1962), Toulmin (1972), Brown (1979), Popper (1982) and others

have shown the inadequacies of positivistic (truth-falsity proving) epistemologies and most leading contemporary philosophers concerned with the nature of knowl

edge and knowledge construction are agreed upon some form of constructivist

epistemology. Constructivist epistemology sees production of new knowledge as

a human construction, with all the power and weaknesses associated with the

ideational frameworks, instrumentation used, and emotional vagaries of human

beings. The Vee heuristic represents a constructivist view of knowledge and illus

trates the dozen or so epistemological elements that interact in the process of new

knowledge construction. The Vee heuristic can also be used to dissect an existing domain of knowledge and to see its structural elements. Figure 4 shows a repre sentation of this for one area of biology.

My work and the work of my students has been based upon Ausubel's assimi

lation theory (1963, 1968) of cognitive learning for the past quarter century. In

his epigraph to his 1968 book, Ausubel asserts: "If I had to reduce all of educa

tional psychology to just one principle, I would say this: The most important sin

gle factor influencing learning is what the learner already knows. Ascertain this

and teach him accordingly." It is this fundamental principle that led our research

group to search for better ways to represent "what the learner already knows" and

to develop the tool of concept mapping in 1972. Although we developed this for

This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 5: Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to …cmap.systemsci.org/rid=1RNRXZP8D-L0213D-10X/BNU Novak 1990 Concept M… · Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic

32

research purposes to represent student's knowledge structures before and after

instruction, we soon learned that concept maps could be a useful tool to help stu

dents move from learning by rote to learning meaningfully. We now see meaning

ful learning as the fundamental process that underlies useful knowledge

acquisition and also new knowledge construction. I have argued that meaningful

learning is the foundation for human constructivism which is both a psychological and an epistemological phenomenon. Figure 5 shows a concept map representa tion of this union of psychological and epistemological meaning making (from Novak, 1987). This representation of meaning making draws upon and incorpo rates ideas from many contemporary psychologists and philosophers including the work of Ausubel et al. (1978); Donaldson (1978); Flavell (1985); Gowin

(1981); Johnson-Laird (1983); Kelley (1955); Kuhn (1962); Mathews (1980);

Mayer (1981, 1983); MacNamara (1982); Piaget (numerous writings); Popper (1982); Steinberg (1985); Toulmin (1972); Vygotsky (1962) and many others.

into/

(Help-sidle.

Below uweN^ j Wrrjui/| K*. otc- A I jshf bttjl W3/ /

(C£^Pu/w<?

2f1 (into H\ihQ»ftto/A IrWf hl6

tnfoiu Iine\v° o»e.s+ea \mi

tUMUj'

\?ro»\

"JV '

"Tfc^i 'MlUl wi

1fiO»t ddiZMSZ

Figure 2. Two concept maps prepared by a basketball player, one (above) early in training and the

other (right) late in the season. Note the increased complexity and integration of concepts of team defense and communication, emphasized in coaching, which was accompanied by much-improved

player performance. (From Novak and Gowin, 1984, p. 44)

This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 6: Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to …cmap.systemsci.org/rid=1RNRXZP8D-L0213D-10X/BNU Novak 1990 Concept M… · Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic

33

lin**

o icrar« -Teuro .

Qms-^ioe TECH- /

VÏSf-i

ßAU-il O/W. 14

PR 1(1 M/fr LlNG „

U>H JCMfO

pîiîé)

/öw É/iAAp (öer< ecAr

\CHA*üeJ

/AVWI-\ wue l

'erOsriH-,

Figure 2. (continued)

This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 7: Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to …cmap.systemsci.org/rid=1RNRXZP8D-L0213D-10X/BNU Novak 1990 Concept M… · Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic

34

CONCEPTUAL/THEORETICAL METHODOLOGICAL

(THINKING) (DOING)

FOCUS QUESTION(S)

Questions that serve to

focus the inquiry about events and/or objects studied.

Value claims: Statements based on

knowledge claims that declare fie worth or

value of the inquiry.

Concepts: Perceived regularity in events or

objects (or records of events or

objects) designated by a label.

World view:

The general belief system motivating and guiding the inquiry.

Philosophy: The beliefs about the nature

of knowledge and knowing

guiding the inquiry.

Theory: The general principles guiding the inquiry that

explain why events or objects exhbit what is observed.

Knowledge claims:

Statements that answer the focus questions and

are reasonable

interpretations of the

records (or data) obtained.

Principles: Statements of relationships between concepts that explain how events or objects can be

expected to appear or behave.

Transformations:

Tables, graphs, concept maps, statistics, or other forms of

organization of records made.

Records The observations made and

recorded from the events/objects studied.

Events and/or objects:

Description of the event(s) and/or object(s) to be studied in order to answer the focus questions.

Figure 3. Gowin's Vee heurestic invented to illustrate the conceptual and methodological elements

that interact in the process of knowledge construction or in the analysis of lectures or documents

presenting knowledge.

This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 8: Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to …cmap.systemsci.org/rid=1RNRXZP8D-L0213D-10X/BNU Novak 1990 Concept M… · Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic

35

FOCUS QUESTION: Can we design an experiment to study some aspect of orientation behavior in an organism-its response to a particular stimuli, the classification of that response, and the possble adaptive benefit of that behavior?

Theory: Orientation behavior- stimulus-response. Adaptation and evolution of behavior.

Principles: 1). Orientation behavior is the act of turning or moving in a predictable way with respect to an external stimulus. 2). Orient, behavior can be classified as a kinesis (the speed or turning rale changes with no orientation of the body with respect to the stimulus) or a taxis (the body is directly orientated toward, away from, or at an angle to the stimulus). 3). Orient, behavior can be named based on three things: a. positive or negative (attracted or repulsed by the stimulus), b. stimulus type (chemo-, photo-, thermo-, etc), c. response of body - kinesis (turning is klino-, speed is ortho-), or taxis (receptors used, at fixed angle to, memory based, etc). 4). Experiments to study orientation behavior should control for variables, include replicates, and provide quantifiable data.

Concepts Orientation behavior, taxis, kinesis, stimulus, response, receptor, adaptation, orthokinesis, klinokinesis, humidity, schooling behavior, bilateral sense receptor, circus movements.

I I

I Value claims: I 1). Designing and conducting a study gives I students experience with scientific knowledge I construction. 2). This lab is valuable for providing | experience with orientation behaviors and their

| adaptive significance.

J Knowledge claims: j 1). Isopods exhfoit a positive hygrokinesis and a | negative phototaxis. 2). Blowfly larvae usually | show a negative photoklinotaxis. 3). Daphnia

usually have a negative phototaxis and a positive geotaxis. 4). Fish species vary in the strength of their schooling behavior. Most tend to spend more time with conspecifics and with the larger group of

conspecifics. This behavior is considered to be a telotaxis.

Transformation*: Total, average and graph data. Compare observed turning or movement rates and time spent with various stimuli to definition of kinesis and taxis in order to decide on classification of observed behavior.

Records: Measurement of times spent near various stimuli. Counts of turns or numbers of organisms at specific time intervals. Measurement of distance traveled and time evolved.

Objects: Isopods, petri dish experimental apparatus, desiccant, paper towels, black paper, lights, marking pens, map measurer, blowfly larvae, Daphnia, graduated cylinders, ring stands, black plastic, several species of fish, test fish tank, jars. Events: Design a study using one of the experimental organisms to determine the type of orientation behavior used in response to a particular stimulus. Gather data, transform data, and present results. Consider adaptive significance of the observed behavior.

Figure 4. A Vee diagram produced by a student for laboratory work in a study by Robertson-Taylor

(1985). A PC software program was used for the construction.

This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 9: Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to …cmap.systemsci.org/rid=1RNRXZP8D-L0213D-10X/BNU Novak 1990 Concept M… · Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic

36

Figure 5. A concept map showing key concepts and propositions involved in "human constructivism",

uniting meanings from psychology and epistemology.

Classroom research using concept maps and vee diagrams

As noted above, the development of concept mapping derived from our research

program wherein we sought to represent science concept meanings possessed by students before and after instruction. We were engaged in a twelve-year study of

concept development and needed a tool to show simply but also explicitly the

concept meanings a student possessed as indicated in modified Piagetian clinical

interviews (see Pines et al, 1978). Figures 6 and 7 shows a representation of the

concept/propositional framework held by a student in grade two and later in grade twelve. These maps were drawn from clinical interviews (see Novak and Gowin, 1984 and notedly Chapter 7) with the student They show growth in the number

and relationships of concept meanings for this student over the ten-year span of

schooling.

This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 10: Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to …cmap.systemsci.org/rid=1RNRXZP8D-L0213D-10X/BNU Novak 1990 Concept M… · Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic

37

Our first use of concept maps to help students learn subject matter meaning

fully were in the areas of mathematics and science at the college level.

Cardemone (1975) found that preparation of a "master" concept map for the topic of "ratio and proportion" helped him to plan instruction on this topic. Copies of

his map were distributed to students but only a minority of students reported in

their questionnaire responses that Cardemone's concept maps were helpful to

them for learning of this topic. Similarly, Bogden (1977) found that concept maps

prepared by him and a professor for each lecture in a genetics course were

reported to be of value in learning genetics by a small minority of students. Some

students indicated they were confused by concept maps prepared for them.

Concept maps proved useful, however, in designing and interpreting answers for

course examinations. The concept maps used by Cardemone and Bogden did not

have words on the linking lines between concepts. From the Cardemone and Bogden studies, we learned that the primary benefit

of concept maps accrues to the person who constructs the maps. It was of little

value to distribute teacher-prepared concept maps to students when the latter were

not involved in constructing their own concept maps. In more recent work, we

have found concept maps prepared by a teacher to be helpful to students, but only

after they had practice in constructing their own concept maps. Also, we find that

it is very important to have "linking words" on the lines connecting concepts in

order to form explicit propositional statements. As concept maps begin to appear more widely in textbooks, it will be unfortunate if students are not instructed in

preparation of concept maps and required to prepare some of their own maps. It

will be even more unfortunate if teachers require students to memorize and dupli cate exactly concept maps prepared by others.

During the period 1975 to 1977,1 tried teaching upper elementary and secon

dary school children concept mapping in classroom settings. In general, students

from grades four onward were successful in constructing concept maps and both

students and teachers were enthusiastic about the value of concept mapping. In

1980, David Symington (in Melbourne, Australia) and I began using concept

maps with children in grades one through six, again with success and enthusiasm

expressed by students and their teachers. These early efforts were directed toward

tryout of concept mapping strategies with various classes and in various subject matter areas, primarily to assess student and teacher reactions to the technique and to work out techniques for introducing concept mapping in a variety a class

room settings. As strategies to introduce concept mapping to students were

refined, most students at all grade levels demonstrated success in constructing

concept maps. These strategies were later described in Learning how to learn

(Novak and Gowin, 1984, chapter 2). These early efforts with concept mapping in

school settings made no attempt to evaluate the influence of concept mapping on

student achievement

This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 11: Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to …cmap.systemsci.org/rid=1RNRXZP8D-L0213D-10X/BNU Novak 1990 Concept M… · Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic

38

CD W

if w -E

- co E

Cvj

1X1 Q < CC CD _i ID < CL

,g " _ CO

to !*

£l !

— -C o

k J

/ >

C ® - os g> o w

V ^

'ca

^ V

b? —52 — ffl ®

> -£

) V J

Figure 6. A concept map prepared from a clinical interview with Paul, a second grade student, repre

senting his knowledge of the "particulate structure of matter".

This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 12: Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to …cmap.systemsci.org/rid=1RNRXZP8D-L0213D-10X/BNU Novak 1990 Concept M… · Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic

39

Figure 7. A concept map prepared from a clinical interview with Paul in twelfth grade. Note the extent

of additional new concepts and propositional meanings compared with his knowledge in grade two

(Figure 6).

This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 13: Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to …cmap.systemsci.org/rid=1RNRXZP8D-L0213D-10X/BNU Novak 1990 Concept M… · Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic

40

A similar tryout of the Vee heuristic occurred during 1975 to 1977, although we did not attempt to introduce this tool below grade four. Later work has shown

that the Vee can be used successfully with primary grade children.

Moreira (1977) used concept maps with university students in physics.

Concept maps were used together with topical reorganization in "experimental" classes and traditional subject matter organization (Halliday and Resnick, 1966) was used in control classes and no concept maps were used. Moreira found that

students in the experimental classes performed significantly better on tests requir

ing graphical structuring of physics concepts; this difference increased over the

semester course of study (Table 1). No significant differences between the groups was found on traditional course exams or on word association tests. Although the

use of concept maps was confounded with content sequence reorganization, stu

dents were increasingly positive about the value of concept maps as the semester

progressed; this may have been a significant contribution to gains in mean scores.

Another finding illustrated in Moreira's data is a pattern of achievement that

we have subsequently seen repeated often when instructional strategies are used

that require meaningful learning. For two to four weeks we generally see an aver

age decline in performance on standard course exams and then score averages

move up, usually finishing significantly higher for students using tools like

concept mapping and learning more meaningfully. This is shown in data from

Table 1. Average concept map test scores and I values for experimental and control groups on three

tests given during the semester, on the basis of three criteria (general, specific and intermediate

concepts indicated).

Test One Test Two Test Three

N Mean t N Mean 1 N Mean (

Identification of General Concepts

37 1.11 35 2.11

35 1.23 -1.23 34 1.53 4.41"

35 2.46

33 1.48 7.15**

Identification of Intermediate Concepts

37 1.03 35 1.60

35 1.06 -.63 34 1.18 3.67**

35 1.89

33 1.30 4.18**

Identification of Most Specific Concepts

37 1.11 - 35 1.83

35 1.20 -.95 34 1.44 2.53*

35 1.97

33 1.52 2.81**

+ (Score range was 0 to 3) (From Moreira, 1977, pp. 110-112) * P<.05 **P<.01

This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 14: Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to …cmap.systemsci.org/rid=1RNRXZP8D-L0213D-10X/BNU Novak 1990 Concept M… · Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic

41

Moreira's study in Table 1. Student attitudes also tend to shift from generally neg ative toward the meaningful learning required to positive and sometimes highly

positive attitudes both toward the instructional approach and toward the subject studied. These patterns account for the commonly observed significantly lower

average performance among "experimental" students exposed to a new strategy for only two to four weeks and "no significant differences" in mean scores

achieved over 10 or 12 weeks when compared over-all with students in "control" or traditional instruction that usually rewards rote-mode learning. In addition, of

course, a study using metacognitive or other strategies to encourage meaningful

learning is not likely to show significant advantages over "control" methods when

the evaluation instruments do not require meaningful learning and transfer of

knowledge to novel contexts, and rote learning can suffice to achieve high scores.

Our first comprehensive study utilizing concept maps and Vee diagrams was

conducted with junior high school students (Novak et al., 1983). Our principal focus in that study was also on methodological issues related to implementation of these strategies in school settings, including the issue of whether or not seventh

and eighth grade students, taught by their teachers, would be successful in acquir

ing skill in use of the strategies. This study led to the following major findings:

1. Concept mapping and Vee diagramming skills continued to improve over the

span of a school year.

2. Seventh grade students outperformed eighth grade students in use of the

strategies, probably as a result of more practice and experience.

3. There was little correlation between success in acquisition of skill in using

concept mapping and ability or achievement test scores (see Table 2).

4. Students using the strategies outperformed their counterparts on a test of novel problem solving by a wide margin (see Novak et al., 1983, p. 643).

The study led to the following conclusions:

1. Classroom teachers motivated to use new metacognitive learning strategies can be successful in employing concept mapping and Vee diagramming tools with junior high school science students.

2. Skill in use of these tools takes time, perhaps one to two years if used only in

a single course.

3. Conventional measures of student ability/achievement are poor indicators of

success with use of these strategies.

4. Novel problem solving success is significantly correlated with success in

concept mapping scores.

5. Junior high school students have become adapted to primarily rote-mode

learning and it is not easy to move them to meaningful learning strategies. This is an inference arising out of the data, but it has been corroborated by other studies we have done.

This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 15: Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to …cmap.systemsci.org/rid=1RNRXZP8D-L0213D-10X/BNU Novak 1990 Concept M… · Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic

42

Concept mapping when used in conjunction with other educational strategies has led to superior achievement Basconas and Novak (1985) found that mean

scores on problem solving test in high school physics were two to three standard deviations higher for students preparing concept maps and following a revised

sequence of topics to enhance building new learning on prior learning, when com

pared with students following a traditional physics program and not using concept

maps. The concept mapping group excelled at all ability levels (based on Raven's

Progressive Matrices test) and there was no significant interaction between treat

ment and ability (See Table 3).

By 1981, when the above study was nearing completion, we were keenly aware of the difficulties of moving both teachers and learners away from rote

mode learning and toward meaningful mode learning. Published textbook/ worksheet/test packages used in most science classes push teachers in the direc tion of "covering the material". These materials take essentially no account of the

conceptual difficulty of the topics presented and treat all information similarly, presenting key vocabulary words (concept labels) or problem solving algorithms

Table 2. Correlation matrix for variables in a study with junior high school science students. Top number is the correlation coefficient; the middle number represents the significance level; the bottom

number (in parentheses) represents the size of the sample for analysis. "Winebottle" problem was a

novel transfer of knowledge open response question.

Identifying,

defining and

examples on

the Vee

Total correct

relationships -

"Winebottle"

SAT reading

percentile

SAT math

percentile

Final course

examination

grade

Total score

on concept

map

Identifying,

defining and

examples of

the Vee

example

Total number SAT - SAT -

of correct reading math

relationships - percentile percentile "Winebottle"

0.36

0.0001

(149)

0.40

0.001

(43)

-0.02

0.815

(146)

0.02

0.784

(146)

0.02

0.796

(156)

0.09

0.572

(46)

0.30

0.0003

(147)

0.30

0.0001

(147)

0.24

0.002

(158)

0.43

0.0001

(80)

0.39

0.0003

(80)

0.31

0.035

(46)

0.77

0.0001

(200)

0.74

0.0001

(155)

0.70

0.0001

(155)

This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 16: Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to …cmap.systemsci.org/rid=1RNRXZP8D-L0213D-10X/BNU Novak 1990 Concept M… · Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic

43

at about the same rate with little regard for the pupil's understanding. Students memorize word definitions or formulas usually with little or no understanding of

the concept meanings represented by the words or symbols and with litüe or no

relationship to prior learning, or anticipation of future, related conceptual learn

ing. Topical sequence is of little importance since new conceptual understanding is not being built on prior conceptual learning. Extended absences, or new stu

dents moving into the school are a minimum difficulty when rote learning of new

topics is the predominant practice. On the other hand, when instruction is focused

on meaningful learning, prior course woik, absences, and/or the sequence of topics in a course becomes very important. We had grossly underestimated the logistic, curricular and pedagogical practices that needed to be modified if maximum ben

efit was to accrue from the use of metacognitive tools and a focus on meaningful

learning. In about half of the cases, teachers reverted to traditional instructional

practices within weeks, or a year or two after introduction of concept mapping and/or use of the Vee heuristic. Only those teachers who refused to accept pre

dominantly rote learning and associated satisfactory short-term test performance as standard practice have persisted in the use of these tools. This has been true, in

our experience, at all educational levels from primary grades through adult educa tion programs.

We were not alone on our failure to recognize the pervasive, pernicious charac

ter of rote learning and associated pedagogical strategies; however, most reports on the use of metacognitive strategies make no mention of this as a potential or real problem. We now see this as a major (if not the major) concern to be addressed when moving to include metacognitive strategies into real school set

tings. Of course, for short-term, isolated efforts with metacognitive tools, Hawthorn and other novelty effects may continue to show positive results for

almost any reasonable metacognitive approach.

Table 3. Analysis of variance in problem-solving lest scores for concept mapping and traditional

groups.

Mean Mean

Source df Square F Probability

Method 1 6836.19 480.49 0.00

Ability group** 2 36.97 2.60 0.08

Method x Ability 3 15.11 1.06 0.35

Error 70 14.23

** Based on Raven test scores

From Bascones and Novak (1985)

This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 17: Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to …cmap.systemsci.org/rid=1RNRXZP8D-L0213D-10X/BNU Novak 1990 Concept M… · Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic

44

Recognizing the problem of student adherence to rote-mode learning patterns,

we turned some of our attention to research on how students think they learn.

Edmondson (1985) attempted to instruct Cornell University freshman in strate

gies to "learn how to learn". Seventy-two freshman who were selected from a

new program providing financial aid to promising students were invited by letters

to participate in the program. Twenty-one attended an orientation/introduction

meeting, but only one remained in the program through a series of tutorial ses

sions. Most of the others said they "didn't have time" for the program, and some

recognized that strategies for meaningful learning were not consistent with the

way they study nor what they believed was required to pass courses. Piqued by this rejection of free tutorial help to "leam how to learn", Edmondson interviewed

fifteen students in a college psychology course and found that only three (20%) were clearly committed to learning for meaning, and this in general has been a

pattern observed in our studies on how school/university students learn.

From 1975 to date, I have taught a course at Cornell University on "Learning to Learn". Although this course (Education 312) carried a number that suggests

sophomore, junior and senior enrolment, in fact the course enrols almost exclu

sively juniors and seniors. Occasional freshmen who have enrolled (with my

blessing) have dropped out early in the semester. It seems that the sophomore

year is often a year of reckoning for students at Cornell University when many find that their straight "A" averages in high school have dropped to B's, C's, or

worse as they enter more advanced courses. Students begin to recognize that

either they are just poorly endowed with intelligence, or they must be doing

something wrong in the way in which they study. Some tum to "how to study" courses to get new ideas on "time management" or "confidence building" etc., while others recognize that something more basic is wrong with their learning/

study approaches. Some of the latter enrol in "Learning to Learn", and the over

whelming majority of these discover that indeed they are not stupid; they simply have been using weak learning strategies; they have been engaged in rote rather

than meaningful learning. Some of these students comment that they never knew

there was another way to learn other than to memorize definitions or "facts" and

review answers to old exam questions. They also recognize that, except for an

occasional course or studies related to their hobbies, they have rarely been mean

ingful learners. It should be noted that Cornell University students are on average the "best" of our high school graduates with SAT scores averaging in the 600's or

better for most fields of study.

My experiences with Ed 312, Edmondson's work, and research we were doing on metacognitive learning all led to the same conclusion; the predominantly rote

mode learning practices encouraged (or required) in so much of school/university

learning had put "braces on the brains" of many students, especially female stu

dents who tend to play the "school game" more conscientiously than males, (see

This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 18: Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to …cmap.systemsci.org/rid=1RNRXZP8D-L0213D-10X/BNU Novak 1990 Concept M… · Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic

45

Best, 1983; Belenky et al., 1985), accounting perhaps for some of the underrepre sentation of women in science, mathematics and engineering (Ridley and Novak,

1984). Could these patterns of learning be reversed? Experiences with Ed 312 stu

dents, math students, graduate students in our programs, anecdotal comments

from students in our junior high school study, and other "success stories" sug

gested the answer was clearly yes. In a study with college students in introductory

biology, Robertson-Taylor (1985) utilized concept maps and Vee diagrams in a

study to see if students could be helped to acquire a meaningful understanding of

biology laboratory work. Her earlier research (Taylor, 1984) showed that the

large majority of students had little or no understanding of how or why the results

they observed in laboratory work could be explained. Most students moved

through laboratory work "procedurally", doing the things prescribed in the labora

tory guide, but with little or no understanding of why they were doing what they were doing, or the meaning of the data or observations they were recording.

Surely, most of the readers of this paper will recollect science laboratory experi ences where this was also true. Taylor sought to make biology laboratory

experience meaningful, rewarding, and emotionally satisfying. She taught two

laboratory sections (N=30) where students were instructed briefly in concept

mapping and Vee diagramming techniques, and Taylor required each student to

prepare either concept maps or Vee diagrams prior to each laboratory session.

She had no control over and no influence upon the lectures given in the course by the professor. These were conducted in the usual "here are the facts you need to

learn" manner and course examinations were the usual machine-scored multiple choice, "What is the right answer" type.

What Taylor found was that on the objective course examinations, her students

scored somewhat better, as was also true in previous semesters. Where the truly

significant differences occurred were in the students' feelings and attitudes

regarding biology and biology laboratory studies. Typical of the student com

ments were these given by students before their first lab and after their last ses

sion (from Taylor, 1985, p. 108).

PRE -1 really dislike biology lab and wish I didn't. I guess I don't see the pur

poses behind the labs or the studies do not really interest me.

POST - Better this semester than last; concept maps a definite help! I like it

more because I'm understanding more. Much more organized than last

semester.

PRE - Unfortunately, after coming out of a terrible lab last semester, I am very

wary of biology lab and its actual meaning, even though it was one of my favorite courses throughout my secondary schools.

POST - My attitude toward bio lab is really positive now, especially compared to last semester. I prepare for class, try to be more attentive, and like it a lot

This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 19: Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to …cmap.systemsci.org/rid=1RNRXZP8D-L0213D-10X/BNU Novak 1990 Concept M… · Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic

46

PRE - Last semester it was a chore and I dreaded coming because it was usu

ally boring.

POST - This semester my attitude toward lab has greatly improved - I even

look forward to going to lab. It seems much more worthwhile and I enjoy it

most of the time.

In our more recent research, we have been interviewing Cornell University stu

dents to ascertain their learning approaches. Overwhelmingly, we have found that

our students are engaged in essentially rote-mode learning. They recognize that

"memorizing notes" has little long-term value, but they also find that this is the

most expedient way to earn high grades. Many of them also recognize that this

kind of learning has little lasting value and is one of the reasons why they have

negative feelings about the subject matter. Typical of quotes from these students

are those cited in Edmondson (1985, p. 72-73), in response to her question, "Can

you describe a meaningful learning experience you have had?"

Male Student: Everyone was developing questions... we participated. You

know, what you put into it is what you get out of it. And then, it wasn't like the

student talking to the professor, it wasn't like one on one, it was like everyone was talking among themselves, and I don't believe the professor - it was like

the professor was participating, but he wasn't trying to keep discussion going

by asking questions; a lot of the students would do that themselves, by "Well, if that's this, then what happens when it does that? You know? Things like

that...

Investigator: Is that different from the other classes that you have had here?

Male Student: Yeah. Well, see, that happened only once. It wasn't a structured

class. It stands out because usually you're just listening. You're just watching and listening.

Female Student: When I felt like I was really learning... Um, I'm trying to

think. I can think of so many examples when I wasn't learning... (laughs) It's

far rarer when you find that where you do.

Working with college chemistry students, Feldsine (1987) found a resistance on

the part of the majority of his students to prepare concept maps for topics (text

chapters) studied. However, as the course progressed and "reluctant mappers"

began to see that difficult topics became conceptually clear using concept maps, all members of his class moved toward preparation of good to excellent maps. Moreover, Feldsine found in case after case in his qualitative analysis that

important misconceptions regarding chemistry were recognized by students and

subsequently altered, with stable, valid conceptions resulting. Given the wide

recognition (see Helm and Novak, 1983; Novak, 1987) of the intractability of

student misconceptions to conventional instruction, Feldsine's findings have

important implications for improved pedagogy.

This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 20: Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to …cmap.systemsci.org/rid=1RNRXZP8D-L0213D-10X/BNU Novak 1990 Concept M… · Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic

47

Most of the studies done by my students at Cornell University have focused in

recent years on qualitative, rather than quantitative, analysis of cognitive develop ment In part, this qualitative emphasis has been necessary to search for better

understanding of the interplay of cognitive and affective factors in facilitation of

meaningful learning and pedagogical strategies effective in encouraging students to move toward meaningful learning practices. Unfortunately, qualitative studies

producing qualitative data are not easily reported in the form of short journal arti

cles. Fortunately, other researchers have begun to report quantitative studies on

the effect of concept mapping and Vee diagramming strategies. Sherris and Kahle (1984) used concept maps with 282 high school biology stu

dents in a five-week unit of instruction, comparing achievement with 259 students

receiving instruction in the same content but without utilization of concept maps.

They also administered the Norvicki-Strickland locus of control scale. Using three

forms of a 25-item multiple choice test and 5 short answer items requiring appli cation of concepts and principles, they found no significant differences on post test scores, nor on 6-week retention test scores, between the two treatment groups. However, there was a difference (Pc.Ol) favoring internal locus of control students

and a significant interaction effect (P<.03) on the retention test between treatment

and locus of control, with "external" students benefiting more from the concept

map aided instruction. Their study shows a five-week study unit may have only minimal effect on student learning patterns and achievement scores, although there may be some benefit on affective dimensions (e.g., locus of control).

Lehman, Carter and Kahle (1985) used concept maps and Vee diagrams with

119 inner-city black students and text outlining with 124 students in an 8-week

biology unit They found no significant differences in achievement scores between the groups. The fact that text outlining was a familiar strategy to these

students and that the concept mapping/Vee diagramming group had to learn new

strategies was suggested by the authors as a reason why only small, non

significantly higher mean scores were observed for the mapping group. Moreover, teachers were previously unfamiliar with concept mapping and Vee

diagramming strategies and experienced some difficulties using these strategies. In a similar study with 103 ninth grade general science students, Pankratius

and Keith (1987) compared concept mapping and text outlining and found 10 per cent higher mean scores for the concept mapping group over an 18 week period. No tests of significance was reported for this group. For a group of 139 twelfth

grade physics students, Pankratius and Keith reported significantly higher

(P <.05) mean achievement scores for students who prepared concept maps prior to and after a unit of study, as compared with a group who prepared maps only after they studied the unit Both groups had practice with concept mapping for

four preceding physics topics. The authors also noted that test items requiring

"higher order thinking" showed wider differences, with 19 out of 23 correct

answers to one item given by the concept mappers.

This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 21: Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to …cmap.systemsci.org/rid=1RNRXZP8D-L0213D-10X/BNU Novak 1990 Concept M… · Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic

48

Working with college students in a course on human anatomy and physiology, Cliburn (1987) found that students using concept maps during a three-week unit

on the skeletal systems showed significantly higher (P <.03) performance on a

retention posttest but not on an immediate posttest when other covariates were

held constant. He also found that students using concept maps gave few verbatim

textbook answers to essay questions in another unit of study. Alvarez and Risko (1987) used Vee diagrams with 28 third grade students

studying seed germination. They found all students were successful in construct

ing Vee diagrams. Scoring the Vee diagrams (interrater reliability r =.96) showed

no significant mean differences between groups with high Stanford Achievement

Test (SAT) scores and those with low scores, which raises the question of the

SAT as an indicator of ability. In another study with 25 first grade students, Alvarez and Risko (1987) found that all were successful in preparing concept

maps and Vee diagrams. No statistical comparisons were made, but anecdotal

reports showed that students at all stanine levels on the SAT performed well in

construction of maps and diagrams and in classroom discussions on four different

topics. In a recent study by Okebukola (in press, a), concept maps were used with 138

high school biology students in Lagos, Nigeria. He found that the concept map

ping group (N = 63) scored significantly higher than a non-mapping group (N =

75) on a genetics unit. Mean scores were 28.12 for the mapping group and 17.23 for the non-mapping group, yielding a t value of 16.01 (P c.001). For a study unit

on ecology, mean scores were 25.98 for the concept mapping group and 19.11 for

the non-mappers, with t = 12.27 (P c.001). In a related study, Okebukola (in press, b) administered the Fraser, Nash and

Fisher (1983) Science Anxiety Scale to the same groups of students noted above.

This instrument gives a measure of the student's anxiety toward the study of

selected concepts in science. Two versions were administered, one dealing with

genetics concepts and another with ecology concepts. On both of the scales, Okebukola found highly significant differences in mean anxiety scores (P c.001)

favoring the concept mapping students. Both test results and anecdotal comments

showed that when meaningful learning was facilitated using concept maps, stu

dent anxiety levels toward study of the subject decrease and attitudes toward the

study of biology became increasingly positive.

Summary and conclusions

Concept maps and Vee diagrams can be useful heuristics for planning instruction

and textbooks. Our first major effort in curriculum design using concept maps was in the area of waste management where we developed a 21-module program for instruction in land application of wastes (Loehr et al., 1979). We have found

This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 22: Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to …cmap.systemsci.org/rid=1RNRXZP8D-L0213D-10X/BNU Novak 1990 Concept M… · Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic

49

concept mapping and Vee diagrams useful in redesign of laboratory work in

college physics (Chen, 1980; Buchweitz, 1981; Lavendowski, 1981). Other

colleagues are using concept mapping in planning earth science courses (Ault, 1985), microbiology (Barenholz and Tamir, 1987) and other fields. The BSCS

group in Colorado Springs is employing concept mapping to plan new elementary school and high school science programs. Several publishers now include concept mapping as a learning tool. It is likely that the majority of science books published in the future will include concept mapping, and perhaps also Vee diagramming, as metacognitive tools to help teachers and learners. Hoz (1987) and his

colleagues at Beer-Sheva University are using these tools in teacher education

programs and in medical education. We are exploring ways to use these tools in

our new MAT program for science and mathematics teachers at Cornell

University. It is likely that concept mapping, and perhaps Vee diagramming, will become

widely used metacognitive tools for science and mathematics education, as well

as for education in other fields of study. No educational tool is so robust, so

infallible that it cannot be abused. What could be worse than instruction where

students are required to memorize exactly the structure of complex concept maps in their syllabus or textbook? I would not be surprised to learn that this is already

happening somewhere in the world. On balance, however, I believe the growing

body of research evidence will point toward a wider and better utilization of these

metacognitive tools. As Italian, Spanish and Thai versions of Learning how to

learn now in press become available, hopefully world-wide experimentation, research and critical appraisal of these tools will increase substantially.

Acknowledgements

The Novak et al., 1983 study was supported by NSF Grant (SED-78-116761).

References

Alvarez, M. C. and Risco, V. J. (1987a). Vee diagrams: helping students understand the structure of

knowledge. Washington, DC.: Paper presented at meetings of the American Educational Research

Association.

Alvarez, M. C. and Risco, V. J. (1987b). Concept maps and Vee diagrams: a visual representation of

children's thinking. Washington, DC.: Paper presented at AERA meeting.

Ault, C. R., Jr. (1985). Concept mapping as a study strategy in earth science. Journal of College Science Teaching, 15 (Sept/Oct), 38-44.

Ault, C. R., Jr., Novak, J. D. and Gowin, D. B. (1984). Constructing Vee maps for clinical interviews

on molecule concepts. Science Education, 68(4), 441-462.

Ausubel, D. P. (1963). The psychology of meaningful verbal learning. New York: Gnme and Stratton.

Ausubel, D. P. (1968). Educational psychology: a cognitive view. New York: Holt, Rinehart and

Winston.

This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 23: Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to …cmap.systemsci.org/rid=1RNRXZP8D-L0213D-10X/BNU Novak 1990 Concept M… · Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic

50

Ausubel, D. P., Novak, J. D. and Hanesian, H. (1978). Educational psychology: a cognitive view

(Second Edition). New York: Holt, Rinehait, and Winston.

Ausubel, D. P., Novak, J. D. and Hanesian, H. (1986). Educational psychology: a cognitive view

(Second Edition). New York: Warbel and Peck (reprinted). Bar-Lavie, B. (1987). Enhancing meaningful learning in an environmental education program: a case

study of a class empowered through the use of Novak's and Gowin's principles of learning how to

leam, concept mapping, interviewing and educating. Ithaca, NY.: Unpublished Ph. D. Dissertation,

Department of Education, Cornell University. Barenholz, H. and Tamir, P. (1987). The design, implementation and evaluation of a microbiology

course with special reference to misconceptions and concept maps. In J. D. Novak (Ed.),

Proceedings of the second international seminar on misconceptions and educational strategies in science and mathematics (Vol. I: 32-45). Ithaca, NY.: Department of Education, Cornell

University.

Belenky, M. F., Clinchy, B., Goldberger, N. R. and Tarule, J. M. (1986). Woman's ways of knowing. New York: Basic Books.

Best, R. (1983). We all got scares. Bloomington, Indiana: Indiana University Press.

Bogden, C. A. (1977). The use of concept mapping as a possible strategy for instructional design and evaluation in college genetics. Ithaca, NY: Unpublished MS Thesis, Department of Education, Cornell University.

Brown, H. I. (1978). perception, theory and commitment: the new philosophy of science. Chicago:

University of Chicago Press.

Buchweitz, B. (1981). An epistemological analysis of curriculum and an assessment of concept learn

ing in physics laboratory. Ithaca, NY: Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, Department of Education, Cornell University.

Cardemone, P. F. (1975). Concept mapping: a technique of analyzing a discipline and its use in the

curriculum and instruction in a portion of a college level mathematics skills course. Ithaca, NY.:

Unpublished M.S. Thesis, Department of Education, Cornell University. Chen, H. H. (1980). Relevance of Gowin's structure of knowledge and Ausubel's learning theory to a

method for improving physics laboratory instructions. Ithaca, NY.: Unpublished M.S. Thesis,

Department of Education, Cornell University. Clibum, J. W. (1987). Systematic expository science teaching with concept maps. Cincinnati, OH.:

Paper presented at meetings of the National Association of Biology Teachers.

Donaldson, M. (1978). Children's minds. New York: W. W. Norton Publishing. Edmondson, K. M. (1985). College students' conceptions of their responsibilities for learning. Ithaca,

NY.: Unpublished M.S. Thesis, Department of Education, Cornell University. Feldsine, J. E. Jr. (1987). the construction of concept maps facilitates the learning of general college

chemistry: a case study. Ithaca, NY.: Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, Department of Education, Cornell University.

Fisher, K. M. (1986). Elaboration of cognitive knowledge of biology from childhood to adulthood

(monograph). San Diego: San Diego State University. Flavell, J. H. (1985). Cognitive development (Second Edition). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. General Accounting Office (1984). New directions for federal programs to aid mathematics and

science teaching. Washington, DC.: General Accounting Office (PEMO 84-85). Gowin, D. B. (1981). Educating. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.

Halliday, D. and Resnick, R. (1966). Physics. New York: John Wiley and Sons.

Helm, H. and Novak, J. D. (1983). Proceedings of the international seminar on misconceptions in

science and mathematics conference. Ithaca, NY.: Department of Education, Cornell University. Hoz, R. (1987). Dimensions of teachers' knowledge structures and their identification by concept

mapping. Washington, DC.: Paper presented at the annual meetings of the American Education

Research Association.

Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1983). Mental models. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Kelley, G. A. (1955). The psychology of personal constructs. Vol. 1 and 2. New York: W.W. Norton.

Kitchener, R. F. (1986). Piagel's theory of knowledge. New Haven: Yale University Press.

Kuhn, T. S. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 24: Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to …cmap.systemsci.org/rid=1RNRXZP8D-L0213D-10X/BNU Novak 1990 Concept M… · Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic

51

Lehman, J. D., Carter, C. and Kahle, J. B. (1985). Concept mapping, Vee mapping, and achievement:

results of a field study with black high school students. Journal of Research in Science Teaching,

22(7), 663-673.

Levandowski, C. E. (1981). Epistemology of a physics laboratory on electricity and magnetism. Ithaca, NY.: Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, Department of Education, Cornell University.

Loehr, R. C., Jewell, W. J., Novak, J. D., Clark son, W. W. and Friedman, G. S. (1979). Land applica tion of wastes. Volumes I and II. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold.

Macnamara, J. (1982). Names for things: a study of human learning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

Matthews, G. B. (1980). Philosophy and the young child. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

Mayer, R. E. (1981). The promise of cognitive psychology. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman.

Mayer, R. E. (1983a). What have we learned about increasing the meaningfulness of science prose? Science Education, 67(2), 223-237.

Mayer, R. E. (1983b). Thinking, problem solving, cognition. New York: W.H. Freeman.

Moreira, M. (1977). An Ausubelian approach to physics instruction: an experiment in an introductory

college course in electromagnetism. Ithaca, NY.: Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, Department of

Education, Cornell University.

Novak, J. D. (1977). A theory of education. Ithaca, NY.: Cornell University Press.

Novak, J. D., Gowin, D. B. and Johansen, G. T. (1983). The use of concept mapping and knowledge Vee mapping with junior high school science students. Science Education, 67(5), 625-645.

Novak, I. D. and Gowin, D. B. (1984). Learning how to learn. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Novak, J. D. (1987). Human constructivism: toward a unity of psychological and epistemological

meaning making. In J. D. Novak (Ed.), Proceedings of the second international seminar on mis

conceptions and educational strategies in science and mathematics, Vol. 1, 349-360. Ithaca, NY.:

Department of Education, Cornell University.

Novak, J. D. (Ed.) (1987). Proceedings of the second international seminar on misconceptions and

educational strategies in science and mathematics. Ithaca, NY.: Department of Education, Cornell

University.

Okebukola, P. A. Students' anxiety toward and perception of difficulty of some biological concepts under the concept mapping heuristic. European Journal of Research in Science Teaching (in

press, a).

Okebukola, P. A. Attaining meaningful learning of concepts in genetics and ecology: an examination

of the potency of the concept mapping technique. Journal of Research in Science Teaching (in

press, b).

Pankratius, W. J. and Keith, T.M. (1987). Building an organized knowledge base: concept mapping in

secondary school science. Paper presented at the annual meetings of the National Science

Teachers Association, Washington, DC.

Piaget, J. (1965). The child's concept of number. New York: W.W. Norton.

Piaget, J. (1971). Psychology and epistemology. New York: Viking Press.

Piaget, J. (1974). Understanding causality. New York: W.W. Norton.

Piaget, J. (1976). To understand is to invent. New York: Penguin.

Pines, A. L., Novak, J. D., Posner, G. J. and VanKirk, J. (1978). The clinical interview: a method for

evaluating cognitive structure. Research Report #6, Department of Education, Cornell University.

Popper, K. (1982). Unended quest: an intellectual autobiography. London: Open Court.

Ridley, D. R. and Novak, J. D. (1983). Sex-related differences in high school science and mathematics

enrolments: do they give males a critical headstart toward science- and math-related careers? The

Alberta Journal of Educational Research, 29(4), 308-318.

Robertson, M. (1984). Use of videotape-stimulated recall interviews to study the thoughts and feelings in an introductory biology laboratoiy course. Ithaca, NY.: Unpublished M.S. Thesis, Department of Education, Cornell University.

Robertson-Taylor, M. (1985). Changing the meaning of experience: empowering learners through the

use of concept maps, vee diagrams, and principles of educating in a biology lab course. Ithaca,

NY.: Unpublished Ph.D. Dissertation, Department of Education, Cornell University.

This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions

Page 25: Concept maps and Vee diagrams: two metacognitive tools to …cmap.systemsci.org/rid=1RNRXZP8D-L0213D-10X/BNU Novak 1990 Concept M… · Vee diagrams (see Figure 3) are a heuristic

52

Shenis, J. D. and Kahle, J. B. (1984). The effects of instructional organization and locus of control

orientation on meaningful learning in high school biology students. Journal of Research in

Science Teaching, 27(1), 83-94.

Sternberg, R. J. (1985). Beyond IQ: a triarchic theory of human intelligence. New York: Cambridge

University Press.

Symington, D. and Novak, J. D. (1982). Teaching children how to learn. The Educational Magazine,

39(5), 13-16.

Toulmin, S. (1972). Human understanding. Volume 1: The Collective Use and Evolution of Concepts. Princeton, NJ.: Princeton University Press.

Vygotsky, L. S. (1962). Thought and language. Cambridge, MA.: MIT Press (edited and translated by

Eugenia Hanfmann and Gertrude Vakar).

Weinstein, C. E. (1987) Fostering learning autonomy through the use of learning strategies. Journal of

Reading 20(7), 590-595, (April).

This content downloaded from 149.175.1.33 on Tue, 14 Oct 2014 16:27:30 PMAll use subject to JSTOR Terms and Conditions