children really do see things differently

1
30 October 2010 | NewScientist | 19 India’s island past rewritten in amber FIFTY-million-year-old insects preserved in amber are helping to rewrite the story of India’s almighty crash into the Eurasian continent, suggesting that for as long as a few million years before the collision, India was connected to Asia by archipelagos. India spent tens of millions of years as an island before colliding with Asia. Yet the fossil record contains no evidence that unique species evolved on the subcontinent during this time, so India may not have been as isolated as it seemed to be. To study this further, Jes Rust of the University of Bonn, Germany, and colleagues collected some 150 kilograms of amber from the Cambay region of Gujarat, India. The amber dates from just before the Indian-Asian collision and contains more than 700 arthropods, preserved with “lifelike, microscopic fidelity” (Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1007407107). Many of the insects are close relatives of species found in Eurasia at the time, indicating that there may have been island chains linking India and southern Asia by 50 million years ago. Thank snowballs and spikes for complex life THE rise of complex life on Earth may have been given a boost by a spike in the levels phosphate, best known now as a plant fertiliser. Timothy Lyons at the University of California, Riverside, and colleagues have worked out how phosphate levels changed in Earth’s oceans over the last 3 billion years by measuring the relative amounts of phosphorus in 700 samples from various rock formations around the world. More phosphorus in the rocks means there was more of the mineral phosphate in the sea when they were formed. The team found that between 750 and 620 million years ago, phosphate levels were much higher than they were either before or since. That high level roughly corresponds to the period known as “snowball Earth”, when most or possibly all of the planet was iced over. Lyons says that shifting ice sheets would have ground away at the continents, releasing large amounts of rocks and sediment rich in phosphate. And when the ice retreated, the phosphate WE’RE all happier on sunny days, but why? It seems that light taps directly into brain areas that process emotion – good and bad. Although light is used to treat mood disorders, we don’t understand how this works. While rods and cones in the eye process visible light, a third type of photoreceptor, particularly sensitive to blue light, mediates non-visual responses such as sleep cycles and alertness. So light may make us feel better because it helps regulate circadian rhythms. Gilles Vandewalle at the University of Liège, Belgium, and colleagues wondered whether this pathway directly affects our emotional state too. To find out, they scanned the brains of volunteers exposed to green or blue light while a neutral or angry voice recited meaningless words. As expected, brain areas responsible for processing emotion responded more strongly to the angry voice, but this effect was amplified by blue light (Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1010180107). Vandewalle suggests blue light is likely to amplify emotions in both directions. Moody blue light taps into emotions ELEMENTAL STUDIOS/ALAMY would have been dumped into the oceans, creating a huge surplus. Phosphate is a fertiliser, and the surplus would have driven the growth of many more plants, says Lyons. In turn, these would have released extra oxygen into the atmosphere, making it more hospitable for multicellular animals (Nature, DOI: 10.1038/ nature09485). The rise of complex animals has long been linked to a jump in atmospheric oxygen, and Lyons says that a phosphate spike could help explain why that happened. How kids get grown-up vision CHILDREN do not see objects in a fully grown-up way until the age of 13, a new study suggests. When judging whether shaded images are convex or concave, adult brains assume that light comes from above unless there is reason to think otherwise. Young children have to learn this ability. To investigate when this happens, Jim Stone at the University of Sheffield, UK, showed embossed shapes such as squares and shaded images such as footprints to 171 children aged from 4 to 10. Each child was shown 10 images and asked whether they were convex or concave. The “correct” answer assumed an object was lit from above. The children got better with age, with the average score out of 10 improving by 0.43 each year (Perception, DOI: 10.1068/p6725). If children of other ages develop at the same rate, Stone predicts that babies will learn to assume that light comes from above at about 21 months. But this aspect of their visual perception won’t be “fully grown” until the age of 13 or so. “Children really do see the world differently to adults, inasmuch as their perceptions seem to be more variable,” says Stone. “No wonder they can’t look at a cloud without seeing a dog or a bear.” BURAZIN/GETTY For new stories every day, visit www.NewScientist.com/news

Upload: ngodat

Post on 04-Jan-2017

219 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Children really do see things differently

30 October 2010 | NewScientist | 19

India’s island past rewritten in amber

FIFTY-million-year-old insects preserved in amber are helping to rewrite the story of India’s almighty crash into the Eurasian continent, suggesting that for as long as a few million years before the collision, India was connected to Asia by archipelagos.

India spent tens of millions of years as an island before colliding with Asia. Yet the fossil record contains no evidence that unique species evolved on the subcontinent during this time, so India may not have been as isolated as it seemed to be.

To study this further, Jes Rust of the University of Bonn, Germany, and colleagues collected some 150 kilograms of amber from the Cambay region of Gujarat, India. The amber dates from just before the Indian-Asian collision and contains more than 700 arthropods, preserved with “lifelike, microscopic fidelity” (Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1007407107).

Many of the insects are close relatives of species found in Eurasia at the time, indicating that there may have been island chains linking India and southern Asia by 50 million years ago.

Thank snowballs and spikes for complex lifeTHE rise of complex life on Earth may have been given a boost by a spike in the levels phosphate, best known now as a plant fertiliser.

Timothy Lyons at the University of California, Riverside, and colleagues have worked out how phosphate levels changed in Earth’s oceans over the last 3 billion years by measuring the relative amounts of phosphorus in 700 samples from various rock formations around the world. More phosphorus in the rocks means there was more of the mineral phosphate in the

sea when they were formed.The team found that between

750 and 620 million years ago, phosphate levels were much higher than they were either before or since.

That high level roughly corresponds to the period known as “snowball Earth”, when most or possibly all of the planet was iced over. Lyons says that shifting ice sheets would have ground away at the continents, releasing large amounts of rocks and sediment rich in phosphate. And when the ice retreated, the phosphate

WE’RE all happier on sunny days, but why? It seems that light taps directly into brain areas that process emotion – good and bad.

Although light is used to treat mood disorders, we don’t understand how this works. While rods and cones in the eye process visible light, a third type of photoreceptor, particularly sensitive to blue light, mediates non-visual responses such as sleep cycles and alertness. So light may make us feel better because it helps regulate circadian rhythms.

Gilles Vandewalle at the University of Liège, Belgium, and colleagues

wondered whether this pathway directly affects our emotional state too. To find out, they scanned the brains of volunteers exposed to green or blue light while a neutral or angry voice recited meaningless words. As expected, brain areas responsible for processing emotion responded more strongly to the angry voice, but this effect was amplified by blue light (Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1010180107).

Vandewalle suggests blue light is likely to amplify emotions in both directions.

Moody blue light taps into emotions

ElEm

Enta

l St

ud

ioS/

ala

my

would have been dumped into the oceans, creating a huge surplus.

Phosphate is a fertiliser, and the surplus would have driven the growth of many more plants, says Lyons. In turn, these would have released extra oxygen into the atmosphere, making it more hospitable for multicellular animals (Nature, DOI: 10.1038/nature09485).

The rise of complex animals has long been linked to a jump in atmospheric oxygen, and Lyons says that a phosphate spike could help explain why that happened.

How kids get grown-up vision

CHILDREN do not see objects in a fully grown-up way until the age of 13, a new study suggests.

When judging whether shaded images are convex or concave, adult brains assume that light comes from above unless there is reason to think otherwise. Young children have to learn this ability.

To investigate when this happens, Jim Stone at the University of Sheffield, UK, showed embossed shapes such as squares and shaded images such as footprints to 171 children aged from 4 to 10. Each child was shown 10 images and asked whether they were convex or concave. The “correct” answer assumed an object was lit from above.

The children got better with age, with the average score out of 10 improving by 0.43 each year (Perception, DOI: 10.1068/p6725). If children of other ages develop at the same rate, Stone predicts that babies will learn to assume that light comes from above at about 21 months. But this aspect of their visual perception won’t be “fully grown” until the age of 13 or so.

“Children really do see the world differently to adults, inasmuch as their perceptions seem to be more variable,” says Stone. “No wonder they can’t look at a cloud without seeing a dog or a bear.”

bur

azi

n/g

Ett

y

For new stories every day, visit www.NewScientist.com/news

101030_N_InBrief.indd 19 25/10/10 18:07:44