central auditory system and central auditory processing disorders

128
251 Management of Auditory Processing Disorders; Editor in Chief, Catherine V. Palmer, Ph.D.; Guest Editor, Gail D. Chermak, Ph.D. Seminars in Hearing, volume 23, number 4, 2002. Address for correspondence and reprint requests: Dennis P. Phillips, Ph.D., Professor, Hearing Research Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Dalhousie University, Halifax, NS, Canada B3H 4J1. E-mail: [email protected] a . 1 Hearing Research Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada. Copyright © 2002 by Thieme Medical Publishers, Inc., 333 Seventh Avenue, New York, NY 10001, USA.Tel: +1(212) 584-4662. 00734-0451,p;2002,23,04,251,262,ftx,en;sih00219x. Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders: Some Conceptual Issues Dennis P. Phillips, Ph.D. 1 ABSTRACT The central auditory system has both parallel and hierarchical af- ferent architectures. In the frequency domain, it is tonotopically con- strained, and in the spatial domain, it is dominated by a representation of the contralateral acoustic hemifield. The functions supported by the affer- ent pathways can be somewhat overlapping, and the connectivity among the pathways is to some degree plastic. Partial deafferentation (in the form of high-frequency hearing loss) and behavioral experience are capable of causing alterations in tonotopic maps in the more rostral auditory system, even in adult animals. Central auditory processing is often frequency- specific. The temporal processes needed for normal auditory function are diverse, which is to be expected given the heterogeneous ways in which au- ditory events are disposed in time and encoded neurally. Central auditory pathologies need not respect structural or functional boundaries in the brain, and so should be expected to have idiosyncratic presentations. Man- agement strategies based on auditory training may exploit basic neuroplas- ticity, but more evidence is needed to substantiate any hypothesis of their differential efficacy in remediation of central auditory processing disorders or language and reading problems. KEYWORDS: Auditory neuroscience, neural representation, neural plasticity, temporal processing, perceptual architecture, auditory training Learning Outcomes: Upon completion of this article, the reader will (1) understand the basic design principles of the central auditory pathway, factors that can trigger plastic responses of the central auditory system, and the forms that those responses take; (2) comprehend the diverse range of functions embraced by the term central auditory processing, and the many levels at which specifically temporal processes contribute to hear- ing; and (3) appreciate some of the issues surrounding auditory training paradigms and their effectiveness.

Upload: others

Post on 03-Feb-2022

12 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

251

Management of Auditory Processing Disorders; Editor in Chief, Catherine V. Palmer, Ph.D.; Guest Editor, Gail D.Chermak, Ph.D. Seminars in Hearing, volume 23, number 4, 2002. Address for correspondence and reprint requests:Dennis P. Phillips, Ph.D., Professor, Hearing Research Laboratory, Department of Psychology, Dalhousie University,Halifax, NS, Canada B3H 4J1. E-mail: [email protected]. 1Hearing Research Laboratory, Department of Psychology,Dalhousie University, Halifax, Nova Scotia, Canada. Copyright © 2002 by Thieme Medical Publishers, Inc., 333 SeventhAvenue, New York, NY 10001, USA. Tel: +1(212) 584-4662. 00734-0451,p;2002,23,04,251,262,ftx,en;sih00219x.

Central Auditory System and CentralAuditory Processing Disorders:Some Conceptual IssuesDennis P. Phillips, Ph.D.1

ABSTRACT

The central auditory system has both parallel and hierarchical af-ferent architectures. In the frequency domain, it is tonotopically con-strained, and in the spatial domain, it is dominated by a representation ofthe contralateral acoustic hemifield. The functions supported by the affer-ent pathways can be somewhat overlapping, and the connectivity amongthe pathways is to some degree plastic. Partial deafferentation (in the formof high-frequency hearing loss) and behavioral experience are capable ofcausing alterations in tonotopic maps in the more rostral auditory system,even in adult animals. Central auditory processing is often frequency-specific. The temporal processes needed for normal auditory function arediverse, which is to be expected given the heterogeneous ways in which au-ditory events are disposed in time and encoded neurally. Central auditorypathologies need not respect structural or functional boundaries in thebrain, and so should be expected to have idiosyncratic presentations. Man-agement strategies based on auditory training may exploit basic neuroplas-ticity, but more evidence is needed to substantiate any hypothesis of theirdifferential efficacy in remediation of central auditory processing disordersor language and reading problems.

KEYWORDS: Auditory neuroscience, neural representation, neuralplasticity, temporal processing, perceptual architecture, auditory training

Learning Outcomes: Upon completion of this article, the reader will (1) understand the basic design principlesof the central auditory pathway, factors that can trigger plastic responses of the central auditory system, andthe forms that those responses take; (2) comprehend the diverse range of functions embraced by the termcentral auditory processing, and the many levels at which specifically temporal processes contribute to hear-ing; and (3) appreciate some of the issues surrounding auditory training paradigms and their effectiveness.

Page 2: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

252 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

That information is then disseminated withinthree divisions of the cochlear nuclear com-plex, and from there to a host of caudal audi-tory brainstem nuclei. Almost all of those pro-jections are strictly topographic, that is, theypreserve the general tonotopy (i.e., the spatialarrangement of neurons according to the fre-quency to which they are most sensitive) devel-oped in the cochlea, and connections withinmore central nuclei are usually sufficiently localto preserve (or enhance) the fine frequencytuning of individual neurons.

The divergent projection of the cochlearnerve on the cochlear nucleus begins a patternof parallel processing, in the sense that afferentinformation from the same cochlear sources isreceived and processed, largely independently,by three nuclei at the same time. Those nucleiparticipate in separable and identifiable neuralcircuits. Some of them are involved in binauralspatial hearing. Thus, some nuclei of the su-perior olivary complex receive tonotopicallyconstrained inputs from both ventral cochlearnuclei and, through those routes, can serve ascoincidence detectors for the timing and am-plitudes of stimuli at the two ears. Those inter-aural parameters in turn are cues to soundsource location in the horizontal plane. In thisway, we see the development of hierarchicalprocessing, because the convergence of input re-sults in a serial growth in response complex-ity—from a code for the timing and amplitudeof events at a given ear, to a cross-correlation ofthose encoded events at the two ears.

The cells of the olivary nuclei do not justinherit the properties of their inputs, but exe-cute operations on those inputs so that a newrepresentation emerges. In this instance, the rep-resentation is of the interaural cues for sourcelocation, and because this is done within tono-topically constrained architecture, it followsthat the processing is done on a frequency-by-frequency basis. There is an important behav-ioral outcome of this. As it happens, the magni-tude of the interaural disparities in time andintensity vary not only with the eccentricity ofthe sound source, but also with frequency. Soundsources that are wideband in spectrum thus pro-vide a rich supply of information about sourcelocation and, because the interaural disparityinformation is encoded tonotopically, the fre-

An important key in coming to an ana-lytic understanding of central auditory pro-cessing (CAP) and its disorders (CAPDs) isthe complexity and dynamism of the centralauditory nervous system (CANS) itself. Thebrain, of which the CANS is a relatively smallbut highly integrated part, is even more com-plicated and dynamic. This architectural com-plexity is what supports the diversity of centralauditory function and the brain’s capacity foradaptation and learning. Consider the fol-lowing: the brain must support the detection,discrimination and localization of sound, thesegregation of auditory figure from ground, per-ceptual learning within new or familiar audi-tory dimensions, recognition and identificationof the source, introspection about the perceivedsound, and so on.

The detail of our neurophysiologic de-scriptions of CANS function and behavioraldescriptions of CAP is growing, as is our com-prehension of the links between those two lev-els of description. With these advances arecoming new insights into CAPDs in terms ofboth their genesis and their phenomenologyand, in turn, there is an emergence of new waysof thinking about management and remedia-tion. Because much of this information is new,and because it barely scrapes the surface of thesystem’s structural and functional architecture,much of our understanding is still at a concep-tual level. The purpose of this article is tosketch some of these advances, to illustrate theirstrengths and importance, and to make cau-tionary remarks about some of them. What fol-lows is, of necessity, a somewhat personal viewthat is not intended to cover all aspects of theseissues; it does assume a basic knowledge of theCANS and CAP.

THE CENTRAL AUDITORY NERVOUS SYSTEM

The afferent sensory input to the CANS is thearray of cochlear nerve cell axons from each ofthe two ears. The cochlear transduction pro-cess confers on each afferent fiber narrow fre-quency tuning, and it is the role of each fiber toencode the presence, amplitude and timing ofstimulus energy within its frequency passband.1,2

Page 3: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

CAS AND CAPD: CONCEPTUAL ISSUES/PHILLIPS 253

quency-specificity of the information is pre-served in the brain. This provides a rich neuralrepresentation from which to derive a percep-tual judgement, and it is for these reasons thatbehavioral sound localization acuity is far moreacute for wideband sources than for tonal onesthat provide only a single time and/or intensitydisparity at the ears.

The outputs of the olivary nuclei and theprojections of some cell groups within thecochlear nuclei are sent to the nuclei of the lat-eral lemniscus and/or to the inferior colliculus(auditory midbrain). Some of these projectionsalso are parallel in organization. For example,the lateral superior olive projects contralater-ally upon both the dorsal nucleus of the laterallemniscus and the inferior colliculus (with theformer then projecting on to the latter). Thereis also a hierarchical arrangement in this levelof the projections, because some individualneurons of the inferior colliculus receive, forexample, input from both the dorsal cochlearnucleus and the lateral superior olive, and thusinherit the properties of both.3 The inferiorcolliculus ultimately receives input from thecochlear nuclei, olivary nuclei, and nuclei ofthe lateral lemniscus. These inputs are oftenbilateral, but the excitatory or inhibitory natureof the connectivity is of a kind that preservesand/or refines the sensitivity to interaural cuesfor sound location.2 The inferior colliculus pro-jects on the thalamic medial geniculate body,which then supplies the afferent input to theauditory cortex. There is also a well-developeddescending auditory pathway.4 This includesdense cortico-thalamic and cortico-collicularconnections, and other pathways descendingfrom the brainstem ultimately to the cochlearouter hair cells.

There are two further principles of organi-zation to be revealed here. One is that the con-vergence of input onto the inferior colliculususually continues to respect tonotopy and isnot undifferentiated: there are local territoriesdominated (anatomically and physiologically)by afferents from only a subset of those poten-tially available. These patchy territories are pre-served (or renewed) in the auditory thalamusand cortex, with the result that the primaryauditory cortex has been conceptualized as asingle tonotopic array on which is imposed a

patchy mosaic of smaller territories dedicatedto other facets of stimulus representation (lo-cation, amplitude, bandwidth, periodicity, andso on). Second, many of the circuits in the af-ferent pathway have overlapping functions.Thus, binaural convergence that initially occursin the superior olivary nuclei also may occur denovo in the auditory midbrain (and as far ros-tral as the cortex5). These patterns of connec-tivity make it difficult to assign a unique locusto any particular central auditory function. It isprobably because of the multiple layers of bin-aural interaction that lesion of the olivary nu-clei can leave the patterns of binaural input tocortical neurons nearly normal.6 Whether suchinputs can support normal neurophysiologiccoding of interaural disparity cues in the cortexcurrently is unknown.

CENTRAL REPRESENTATIONS,PLASTICITY, AND BEHAVIORALPERFORMANCE

There is some degree of plasticity in the pat-terns of auditory afferent connectivity. Perhapsthe most dramatic expressions of this have comefrom studies in animals with neonatal cochlearablations in whom there can be marked re-arrangement of connections from the survivingear.7 The striking complexity of the brainstempathways offers numerous opportunities for localshuffling of connections following neonatalablations. Thus, in the absence of input fromone ear to the medial superior olive, the intactear may come to innervate both sides of thenucleus. Projections of this sort appear to beable to support physiological responses morerostrally.

The internal organization of tonotopicmaps in the adult CANS also can be influencedby partial (i.e., frequency specific) cochlea dam-age. Most often, this damage has been to basalcochlear sectors, and changes to central tono-topy have taken the form of a loss of neuralspace devoted to the lesioned cochlear sector,and a nearly equivalent expansion of the terri-tory allocated to peri-lesional cochlear places(i.e., cochlear sites adjacent to the damaged sec-tor).8 This form of plasticity is probably differ-ent to that following wholesale neonatal cochlear

Page 4: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

254 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

ablations, because it is present in adulthood,and because it is expressed within nuclei ratherthan a rearrangement of connectivity betweenpathways. The changes to the affected parts ofthe tonotopic maps have at least two stages.The earlier stage is a loss of sensitivity to stim-ulation of the affected cochlear sector, andwhat residual responsiveness remains is of thekind expected from a pathological endorgan(high thresholds, broad frequency tuning). Thelater stage is the development of near-normalsensitivity to acoustic stimulation, but from theperi-lesional cochlear places (with near-normalfrequency tuning for innervation from thatcochlear site). Note that this re-innervation doesnot support a recovery of behavioral sensitivityto stimulation at the frequencies deprived ofcortical representation (discussed later in thisarticle).

Plasticity in the adult thalamocortical au-ditory system can be driven by peripheral eventsfar less dramatic than ablations. Thus, the be-havioral training on a frequency discriminationtask using tonal stimuli can exert plastic changeson cortical tonotopic maps, notably by expand-ing the territories devoted to the frequenciesinvolved in the training.9 Such changes likelydepend on cholinergic influences from the nu-cleus basalis,10,11 perhaps by influencing activ-ity in the neural loop formed by afferents flow-ing from the midbrain through the thalamus tothe cortex on the one side, and efferents fromthe cortex to the thalamus and colliculus on theother. Through this feedback loop, there maybe a way for the thalamocortical auditory sys-tem to “select” afferent inputs12 of most behav-ioral significance and provide them the mostelaborate representation. In this regard, it istypically an expansion of the cortical territoryallocated to the relevant sector of the cochleathat accompanies behavioral training of func-tion at that cochlear sector and, of the neuro-physiologic (representational) changes that occur,it is the size of the representation that is moststrongly correlated with behavioral acuity.9The further implication of this line of work isthat if the details of cortical (or other) auditoryrepresentations are somewhat experience-de-pendent, then it follows that individual differ-ences in experience will be reflected in individ-

ual differences in the details of auditory repre-sentations and in behavioral performance.

These kinds of observations tend to prompta “bigger is better” view of brain function (i.e.,that a greater volume of neural tissue devotedto a particular function necessarily enhancesperformance of that function). We already haveseen evidence compatible with this view (seethe previous discussion). On the other hand,there are also counter-examples. Monocular de-privation of animals during the critical periodresults in the deprived eye receiving little or nocortical representation (and blindness throughthat eye), while the nondeprived eye presum-ably receives twice the cortical postsynapticspace that it normally would. Despite this, thereis little evidence for behavioral superiority inspatial acuity through the nondeprived eye overthat seen in normal animals. Likewise, we sawabove that in animals, restricted high-frequencyhearing loss can result in an expanded repre-sentation of peri-lesional frequencies. We haveevery reason to believe that this plasticity ex-tends to persons with high-frequency hearingloss. Nevertheless, in the face of any such plas-ticity, while it is no surprise that auditory acuityfor sounds activating the damaged cochlear sec-tor may be poorer than normal, there is littleevidence to support the existence of greater-than-normal frequency acuity for peri-lesionalfrequencies.13

The foregoing observations raise fascinat-ing questions about the nature and extent ofplastic mechanisms in the CANS. The twocases described in the previous paragraph areinstances in which the provision of greater-than-usual neural substrate failed to enhancebehavioral performance above normal. It canbe argued that the naturally-occurring visualspatial acuity in the normal animal is alreadyevolutionarily optimized to be as great as isneurologically supportable, and that throughyears of exposure to speech (and music, etc.)frequency discrimination in man has become ahighly over learned task. The latter may not betrue of animals, and it may be for this reasonthat we see the demonstrable effects of audi-tory frequency discrimination training in ani-mals. From the basic science standpoint then,it would be valuable to ascertain whether train-

Page 5: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

CAS AND CAPD: CONCEPTUAL ISSUES/PHILLIPS 255

ing in a non-over-learned task (e.g., temporalinterval discrimination) in a frequency rangewith an exaggerated central representation (asin peri-lesional frequencies in the case of re-stricted high-frequency hearing loss) would sup-port better acuity than the same training in afrequency range with a normal central repre-sentation. Perhaps a more interesting clinicalquestion concerns the prospects for training inindividuals who at the outset have an impover-ished neurological substrate of some cochlearsector or auditory function. In this general re-gard, developmental dyslexics often have—inaddition to pathological left superior temporalplana—larger-than-normal right plana,14 sug-gesting that an excess of language substrate(whether occurring independently or as a com-pensatory response to the left-sided abnormal-ity) has still left the patients with poor readingperformance.

CENTRAL AUDITORY PROCESSING

As alluded to at the beginning of the article,CAP is an umbrella term for all of the opera-tions executed on peripheral auditory inputs,and which are required for the successful andtimely generation of auditory percepts, theirresolution, differentiation, and identification.Many of the low-level analyses of sound arebased on frequency-specific processing; theseanalyses include sound localization (discussedpreviously; also see Jenkins and Merzenich15),temporal gap detection,16 temporal modulationdetection,17 and loudness perception.18 It alsois clear that more high-level processes (e.g., se-lective auditory attention) can operate locallyin frequency space,19 and that others are con-strained by frequency (e.g., auditory streamsegregation20). The tonotopic architecture ofthe CANS receives behavioral expression infrequency-specificity of much central auditoryprocessing. Sensory information processing isexecuted on a frequency-by-frequency basis(see the previous discussion), and the alloca-tion of sensory signals to perceptual streams(perceptual grouping) has frequency as a prin-cipal guiding force.

Two related features of specifically spatialcoding have emerged in the last two decades.One is that, for centers rostral to the olivarynuclei, neurons that have restricted spatial re-ceptive fields tend to have those receptive fieldslocated in the contralateral auditory hemifield.21

The medial borders of these receptive fields areusually within about 30 degrees of the midline,and are probably determined by binaural inter-actions. Neural response rate for locations withinthe receptive field are shaped more heavily bythe directionality of the pinna within thathemifield. Second, it has become clear that eachside of the CANS is independently capable oflocalizing sound sources in the contralateralauditory hemifield. Each cerebral hemispherecontains the spatial information required forlocalizing sources in the contralateral hemi-field, and unilateral forebrain lesions usuallyresult in deficits in sound localization behavioronly for sources in the auditory hemifield con-tralateral to the lesion.21,22 Psychophysical evi-dence suggests that the human auditory systemprobably has spatial perceptual channels with ahemifield architecture.23 The mechanisms inwhich these hemifield-tuned perceptual chan-nels have their genesis are still being workedout. The revelation of the hemifield architec-ture may offer new insights into a host of audi-tory spatial phenomena, including cocktail partyeffects, spatial masking, and so on. Note thatthe two-channel (left and right) hemifield ar-chitecture of perceptual channels for space isquite different to that for the frequency di-mension—the latter is subject to a multiple-channel (critical band) processing architecture.

The concept of channels is important; itincludes the complete neural representation ofthe stimulus and the perceptual structure sup-ported by that representation.16,24 Auditoryprocessing that is within-channel can often bethought of as low-level or automated and withrelatively little cognitive overhead. Thus, thedetection of temporal gaps between identicalmarkers is done effortlessly and with great acu-ity. In contrast, the detection of temporal gapsbetween dissimilar markers (between-channelgap detection) is more labor-intensive, subjectto a longer learning curve, and has poorer acu-ity.16,23 This difference may exist because the

Page 6: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

256 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

existence of channel architecture normallyobliges the allocation of attentional resourcesselectively to one channel at a time.16,20,23 Bythe same reasoning, perceptual operations ex-plicitly requiring a comparison of activity indifferent channels (e.g., relative timing opera-tions) have poor acuity.

A vast number of central auditory pro-cesses rely on a specifically temporal analysis ofthe incoming signal. The temporal coincidenceof events that facilitates their perceptual group-ing as an auditory object, the segregation of au-ditory figure and ground by temporal streamingprocesses, the discrimination of phonemes dif-fering only in some temporal aspect (e.g., dura-tion or voice onset time), rhythm perception,temporal order judgments and sequencing, andperiodicity pitch perception are all examples thatmay have their mechanisms rooted deeply incentral temporal representations of sounds. It isimportant to understand that these temporalphenomena are not reflections of some singleprocess. Thus, the fidelity with which the audi-tory forebrain represents the timing of onsettransients and temporal periodicities are deter-mined relatively independently.25 Multiple lev-els of temporal processing might be involved inan apparently single perceptual operation. Ahigh-level process might exploit the output of alow-level one. One example of this is the segre-gation of two sound sources with different peri-odicity pitches on the basis of their differentrhythms.20 A second is the erroneous groupingof two spatially-separated sounds as a singleone on the basis of a contiguous periodicities, asin auditory saltation.26,27 In this paradigm, di-chotic clicks are presented in trains of 6 or 8elements at perfectly regular intervals; the firsthalf of the clicks favor one ear (or side) on thebasis of an interaural time difference, while thelast half of the elements favor the other ear (orside). For inter-click intervals less than about120 ms, the resulting percept is of clicks ema-nating not only from the sites of stimulation,but also from points between them, as if a sin-gle source were moving along a trajectory be-tween the click train’s anchor points. One ac-count of the mechanism is thus that the anchorpoints of the train have locations defined by in-teraural time differences; the auditory system

groups the clicks as a single object on the basisof the perfectly regular inter-click intervals, andhaving made that grouping process, assigns tothe middle clicks of the train spatial locationsthat are intermediate between those of the an-chor point clicks. One level of temporal analy-sis (interaural time disparity coding) perceptu-ally anchors the endpoints of the click train,while a second level of analysis (grouping byinter-click interval) ultimately results in the per-ceptual illusion.

Temporal processes in audition are not re-stricted to the accurate encoding of the stimulustime structure, and the generation of veridicalpercepts from that encoding. The generation ofthe percept itself takes time, and there is proba-bly a host of operations performed on sensoryinputs that take place during that time, and towhich we have little or no direct perceptual ac-cess.26–28 It is the distribution in time of auditorysensory and subsequent linguistic and cognitiveprocesses that makes possible the use of event-related evoked potentials to track the time courseof that processing. Because of the diversity oftemporal processes in audition, identifying aprocess (or a disorder) simply as temporal maynot be helpful.

CENTRAL AUDITORY PROCESSINGDISORDERS AND THEIRMANAGEMENT

CAPDs are probably as idiosyncratic as the in-dividuals they affect. Most of the CAPDs seenin the clinic will not have their origins localiz-able at a structural level. The exceptions areusually rare accidental cases in which precisedelineation of the structural and functional pa-thologies have been possible and tremendouslyinformative. For many of the auditory percep-tual dimensions we can imagine, there have beensingle-case studies revealing isolated functionalloss following focal cerebral lesion. These in-clude deficits in sound localization,29 auditorymotion perception,30 frequency discrimination,31

melody perception,32 and various componentsof auditory, phonological or other levels of analy-sis of spoken language.33

Page 7: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

CAS AND CAPD: CONCEPTUAL ISSUES/PHILLIPS 257

It is probably much more common forCAPDs to be highly idiosyncratic, and there isa host of reasons for this to be true. One is thatcentral nervous system pathology, whether de-velopmental or acquired, need not respect func-tional neurological boundaries. A subtle orga-nizational abnormality in the development ofneural connections, or a transient metabolicproblem during a critical period for the incor-poration of certain membrane channel proteins,could have diverse presentations, precisely be-cause the deficit may be relatively scattered inneural space. Second, even in the case of focallesions, we need to remember that brains arethemselves somewhat individual. In both ani-mals and man, the primary auditory cortex islocated in tissue that varies in fissural patternbetween individuals.34,35 There is some likeli-hood that functional territories will not respectfissural boundaries in any case.34 It is muchmore likely that functional territories will betied to cytoarchitectonic ones. Third, for a givenfunctional territory, there may be individualdifferences in the fine detail of structure andorganization, and different propensities foradaptation and recovery; so similarly locatedabnormalities might have different behavioralsequelae in different individuals. Because somuch of auditory perceptual experience is basedon the time structure of sounds and becausethere are many levels of sound time structureto be processed, some sort of temporal process-ing problem will be common in CAPD pre-sentation.

From a practical standpoint, specificationof a subtle neurological abnormality that un-derlies some processing deficit may not be asimmediately helpful as a detailed mapping ofthe functional disability itself. The tremendousrange of functions ascribed to the CANS, andindividual differences in their relative develop-ment, means that we require probes sensitiveto each function. Probing of auditory functionshould be done in the context of a much moregeneral interrogation of sensory, linguistic, andcognitive capacities. This is for a number of rea-sons. First, more high-level cognitive impair-ments might impair performance on auditoryprocessing probes without necessarily being in-dicative of impairments in the latter. Second,

one needs to know whether any auditory prob-lem is a component of a supramodal processingproblem.36 Third, temporal processing problemsmight be markers for, or causally related to, lan-guage impairments.28 In the classical connec-tionist view of language function, poor auditoryreceptive language function was an unremark-able consequence of an acquired impoverishedinput to the language processor. In a develop-mental scenario (following the general line ofargument provided by Tallal et al37 and Fitch etal38), a poor perceptual processor might affectnot only the acquisition of normal auditory re-ceptive language, but through that mechanism,provide poor targets to guide the developmentof speech production and phonology. This isnot to imply that all developmental languagedisorders are cascade error sequelae to low-levelperceptual problems. One view is that manycases of developmental language delay resultfrom various forms of synergism between manyfactors, of which an auditory processing prob-lem is only one.39

Comprehensive evaluation of persons withsuspected CAPDs is not a small task. Giventhe wealth of auditory functions ascribed tothe brain, it should come as no surprise that awealth of assessment is needed to assay thosefunctions. It also raises the prospect that theevaluation itself might require tools that arenot currently available. In this regard, there areincreasing numbers of reports using auditoryassessments that are not part of the standardaudiologic battery (e.g., various forms of tem-poral gap detection, backward masking,39,40 au-ditory stream segregation,41 auditory saltation42).Currently, this may appear a source of frustra-tion because the nonstandardized testing makesit hard to compare studies, either for the relia-bility with which separate studies have probedthe same mechanism, or because different stud-ies may, by design, not be probing the sametemporal mechanisms. In practice however, thisis often the state of a young science and anystandardization of test materials that is appro-priate will emerge as the scientific issues sortthemselves out over time.

There is growing interest in the manage-ment of developmental language delay throughintensive training strategies designed to reme-

Page 8: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

258 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

diate central auditory processing disorders thatmight underlie the language delay.43,44 Perhapsthe most well-known of these is Fast ForWord.45

The training is based on the premise that brief,closely-spaced acoustic events (of the temporalgrain required to distinguish some phonemes33)are perceptually differentiated relatively poorlyin language-learning impaired children and thatthis results in poor phonology. The hope is,therefore, that provided with a more appropri-ately differentiated (i.e., trained) auditory input,an otherwise intact language processor (or onewhich itself adapts to the new inputs) will havean improved output. Fast ForWord43–45 usesstimuli in which target elements (e.g., stopconsonants in speech stimuli) are selectivelylengthened in time and amplified, and in whichthe sounds to be discriminated are presented atprogrammed inter-stimulus intervals (ISIs). Thetraining is adaptive, in the sense that as thechild learns to perform auditory discriminationswithin stimulus pairs, the extent of the tempo-ral stretching, the amplification, and the ISI allcan be reduced. The training also includes com-ponents above the level of acoustics, notablyexercises that target word recognition, auditorymemory and language comprehension.

Evaluating Fast ForWord or other trainingprograms is necessarily a complex issue.46 First,if a training program targets both auditory andlinguistic levels of analysis, but measures out-come using language performance, then it isnot immediately clear which component(s) ofthe training are responsible for the outcome.Second, if a training program is predicated onan account of language-learning disability thatis true for only a subset of affected listeners, thenparticipant selection in outcome studies is im-portant—because negative outcomes in non-relevant participants have little bearing on thequestion. Third, the differential effectivenessof a training program or other intervention de-pends critically on appropriate control for par-ticipants’ attention, motivation, duration oftreatment, and so on across the independenttreatment variable. The issue of Fast ForWord’seffectiveness has become theoretically impor-tant because of the hypothesized origins of de-velopmental language delay in specifically audi-

tory levels of analysis. The development of FastForWord, and discussion of its effectiveness,thus stands in contrast to other interventionsthat more explicitly acknowledge that they cap-italize on the contributions of language compo-nents of the training.47–49

Initial reports of the effectiveness of FastForWord training on auditory temporal process-ing43 showed great advantage over equivalentlytime-intensive classical language training andnonadaptive auditory intervention. Languageoutcome44 was significantly superior for a groupreceiving adaptive training with the modifiedspeech than for a control group receiving expo-sure to natural stimuli in a nonadaptive fashion.

More recently, independent studies haveproduced mixed findings. One group using aFast ForWord-like program has confirmed thatperceptual training with speech materials canimprove perceptual performance with thosematerials50 and may improve phonologic aware-ness.51 The former study50 did not employ acontrol group, so conclusions about the speci-ficity of any benefit for that training programcannot be drawn. Another study group reportedthat it was unclear whether Fast ForWord train-ing had any effects on language outcome abovethose seen with other, specifically language-based training programs.52 However, the par-ticipant population in that study had been se-lected on the basis of reading difficulties, ratherthan on the basis of the existence of auditoryprocessing difficulties. This is important, be-cause dyslexia itself is something of an um-brella category, and it is not clear that all formsof dyslexia (or language impairments) are nec-essarily associated with CAPD.28,39,53,54

Recently, there have been a series of casereports published on the outcome of Fast For-Word training on language55,56 and auditory per-ceptual57,58 performance. Each study was basedon small numbers of participants and so mustbe regarded as preliminary in its findings.With that caveat, the reports had some poten-tially important conclusions beyond that FastForWord training may improve auditory pro-cessing or language performance in some par-ticipants. One is that language outcome fol-lowing participation in a training program may

Page 9: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

CAS AND CAPD: CONCEPTUAL ISSUES/PHILLIPS 259

be independent of the level of language pro-cessing targeted by the training.56 It followsfrom this that the effectiveness of Fast For-Word training may not necessarily reside in thelink between auditory processing and languageperformance. Second, Marler et al58 demon-strated that the psychoacoustic performance(relation between backward and simultaneousmasking thresholds) of children improved acrosstraining, whether or not those children had nor-mally developing language, and roughly equiv-alently for Fast ForWord and another trainingprogram that did not contain acoustically mod-ified speech. This casts some doubt on the dif-ferential effectiveness of Fast ForWord training.

It is clear that the empirical question ofthe effectiveness of training regimes for CAPDand/or developmental language/reading impair-ments will be difficult to answer—though thisshould not discourage attempts to do so. Be-cause all learning involves plastic changes inthe brain, the newer training strategies are notspecial in this regard. What may prove them tobe special is the effectiveness with which theytarget any impaired process, and the nature ofthe link between resolution of that impairmentand performance benefits in higher-level lan-guage and cognitive domains. That link may beas individual as the listeners themselves.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTSSome of the work described in this article wassupported by grants from NSERC of Canadato the author. Thanks are due to Drs. GailChermak, Guinevere Eden, and Richard Olsonfor their very helpful guidance. Thanks also aredue to Susan E. Boehnke, Susan E. Hall, andDrs. Raymond Klein and Christine Sloan forcountless discussions that shaped the content ofthis article.

ABBREVIATIONS

CANS central auditory nervous systemCAP central auditory processingCAPD central auditory processing disorderISI interstimulus intervals

REFERENCES

1. Ruggero MA. Physiology and coding of sound inthe auditory nerve. In: Popper AN, Fay RR, eds.The Mammalian Auditory Pathway: Neurophysi-ology. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1992:34–93

2. Phillips DP. Introduction to the central auditorynervous system. In: Jahn AF, Santos-Sacchi JR,eds. Physiology of the Ear, 2nd ed. San Diego, CA:Singular; 2001:613–638

3. Semple MN, Aitkin LM. Physiology of pathwayfrom dorsal cochlear nucleus to inferior colliculusrevealed by electrical and auditory stimulation. ExpBrain Res 1980;41:19–28

4. Spangler KM, Warr WB. The descending auditorysystem. In: Altschuler RA, Bobbin RP, CloptonBM, Hoffman DW, eds. Neurobiology of Hearing:The Central Auditory System. New York: RavenPress; 1991:27–46

5. Kitzes LM, Doherty D. Influence of callosal ac-tivity on units in the auditory cortex of ferret(Mustela putorius). J Neurophysiol 1994;71:1740–1751

6. Sally SL, Kelly JB. Effects of superior olivary com-plex lesions on binaural responses in rat inferiorcolliculus. Brain Res 1992;572:5–18

7. Kitzes LM, Kageyama GH, Semple MN, Kil J.Development of ectopic projections from the ven-tral cochlear nucleus to the superior olivary com-plex induced by neonatal ablation of the contralat-eral cochlea. J Comp Neurol 1995;353:341–363

8. Rajan R, Irvine DRF, Wise LZ, Heil P. Effectof unilateral partial cochlear lesions in adult catson the representation of lesioned and unlesionedcochleas in primary auditory cortex. J Comp Neu-rol 1993;338:17–49

9. Recanzone GH, Schreiner CE, Merzenich MM.Plasticity in the frequency representation of primaryauditory cortex following discrimination training inadult owl monkeys. J Neurosci 1993;13:87–103

10. Weinberger NM. Learning-induced receptive fieldplasticity in the primary auditory cortex. SeminNeurosci 1997;9:59–67

11. Kilgard MP, Merzenich MM. Cortical map reor-ganization enabled by nucleus basalis activity. Sci-ence 1998;279:1714–1718

12. Suga N, Yan J, Zhang Y. Cortical maps for hearingand egocentric selection for self-organization.TrendsCogn Sci 1997;1:13–20

13. Irvine DRF, Rajan R, McDermott HJ. Injury-induced reorganization in adult auditory cortex andits perceptual consequences. Hearing Res 2000;147:188–199

14. Galaburda AM, Sherman GF, Rosen GD, ArboitisF, Geschwind N. Developmental dyslexia: four

Page 10: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

260 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

consecutive patients with cortical anomalies. AnnNeurol 1985;18:222–233

15. Jenkins WM, Merzenich MM. Role of cat primaryauditory cortex for sound localization behavior. JNeurophysiol 1984;52:819–847

16. Phillips DP, Taylor TL, Hall SE, Carr MM,Mossop JE. Detection of silent intervals betweennoises activating different perceptual channels: Someproperties of “central” auditory gap detection. JAcoust Soc Am 1997;101:3694–3705

17. Viemeister NF. Temporal modulation transferfunctions based upon modulation thresholds. JAcoust Soc Am 1979;66:1364–1380

18. Viemeister NF. Auditory intensity discriminationat high frequencies in the presence of noise. Sci-ence 1983;221:1206–1208

19. Scharf B, Quigley S, Aoki C, Peachey N, Reeves A.Focused auditory attention and frequency selectiv-ity. Percept Psychophys 1987;42:215–223

20. Bregman AS. Auditory Scene Analysis: The Per-ceptual Organization of Sound. Cambridge, MA:MIT Press; 1990

21. Phillips DP, Brugge JF. Progress in neurophysiol-ogy of sound localization. Ann Rev Psychol 1985;36:245–274

22. Jenkins WM, Masterton RB. Sound localization:effects of unilateral lesions in central auditorypathways. J Neurophysiol 1982;47:987–1016

23. Boehnke SE, Phillips DP. Azimuthal tuning ofhuman perceptual channels for sound location. JAcoust Soc Am 1999;106:1948–1955

24. Johnson KO. Sensory discrimination: neural pro-cesses preceding discrimination decision. J Neuro-physiol 1980;43:1793–1815

25. Phillips DP, Hall SE. Response timing constraintson the cortical representation of sound time struc-ture. J Acoust Soc Am 1990;88:1403–1411

26. Phillips DP, Hall SE. Spatial and temporal factorsin auditory saltation. J Acoust Soc Am 2001;110:1539–1547

27. Phillips DP, Hall SE, Boehnke SE, RutherfordLED. Spatial stimulus cue information supplyingauditory saltation. Perception 2002;31:875–885

28. Farmer ME, Klein R. The evidence for a temporalprocessing deficit linked to dyslexia: a review.Psych Bull Rev 1995;2:460–493

29. Sanchez-Longo LP, Forster FM. Clinical signifi-cance of impairment of sound localization. Neurol1958;8:119–125

30. Griffiths TD, Rees A, Witton C, et al. Evidencefor a sound movement area in the human cerebralcortex. Nature 1996;383:425–427

31. Tramo MJ, Shah GD, Braida LD. Functionalrole of auditory cortex in frequency processingand pitch perception. J Neurophysiol 2002;87:122–39

32. Peretz I, Kolinsky R, Tramo M, et al. Functionaldissociations following bilateral lesions of auditorycortex. Brain 1994;117:1283–1301

33. Phillips DP. Central auditory processing: a view fromauditory neuroscience. Am J Otol 1995;16:338–352

34. Merzenich MM, Knight PL, Roth GL. Represen-tation of cochlea within primary auditory cortex inthe cat. J Neurophysiol 1975;38:231–249

35. Musiek FE, Reeves AG. Asymmetries of the audi-tory areas of the cerebrum. J Am Acad Audiol1990;1:240–245

36. Cacace AT, McFarland DJ. Central auditory pro-cessing disorder in school-aged children: a criticalreview. J Speech Lang Hear Res 1998;41:355–373

37. Tallal P, Miller S, Fitch RH. Neurobiological basisof speech: a case for the preeminence of temporalprocessing. Ann NY Acad Sci 1993;682:27–47

38. Fitch RH, Read HL, Benasich AA. Neurophysiol-ogy of speech perception in normal and impairedsystems. In: Jahn AF, Santos-Sacchi JR, eds. Phys-iology of the Ear, 2nd ed. San Diego, CA: Singular;2001:651–672

39. Bishop DVM, Carlyon RP, Deeks JM, Bishop SJ.Auditory temporal processing impairment: neithernecessary nor sufficient for causing language im-pairment in children. J Speech Lang Hear Res1999;42:1295–1310

40. Wright BA, Lombardino LJ, King WM, et al.Deficits in auditory temporal and spectral resolu-tion in language-impaired children. Nature 1997;387:176–178

41. Helenius P, Uutela K, Hari R. Auditory stream seg-regation in dyslexic adults. Brain 1999;122:907–913

42. Hari R, Kiesilä P. Deficit of temporal auditory pro-cessing in dyslexic adults. Neurosci Lett 1996;205:138–140

43. Merzenich MM, Jenkins WM, Johnston P, et al.Temporal processing deficits of language-learningimpaired children ameliorated by training. Science1996;271:77–81

44. Tallal P, Miller SL, Bedi G, et al. Language com-prehension in language-learning impaired childrenimproved with acoustically modified speech. Sci-ence 1996;271:81–84

45. Scientific Learning Corporation. Fast ForWord™[Computer Software]. Berkeley, CA: Author; 1996

46. Gillam RB, Frome Loeb D, Friel-Patti S. Lookingback: a summary of five exploratory studies of FastForWord. Am J Speech-Lang Pathol 2001;10:269–273

47. Chermak GD. Managing central auditory process-ing disorders. Metalinguistic and metacognitiveapproaches. Semin Hear 1998;19:379–392

48. Sloan C. Management of auditory processing diffi-culties. A perspective from speech-language pa-thology. Semin Hear 1998;19:367–378

Page 11: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

CAS AND CAPD: CONCEPTUAL ISSUES/PHILLIPS 261

49. Musiek FE, Schochat E. Auditory training andcentral auditory processing disorders. A case study.Semin Hear 1998;19:357–366

50. Louis M, Espesser R, Rey V, et al. Intensive train-ing of phonological skills in progressive aphasia: amodel of brain plasticity in neurodegenerative dis-ease. Brain Cogn 2001;46:197–201

51. Habib M, Espesser R, Rey V, et al. Training dyslex-ics with acoustically modified speech: evidence ofimproved phonological performance. Brain Cogn1999;40:143–146

52. Hook PE, Macaruso P, Jones S. Efficacy of Fast For-Word training on facilitating acquisition of readingskills by children with reading difficulties—a longi-tudinal study. Ann Dyslexia 2001;51:75–96

53. Tallal P, Stark RE. Perceptual/motor profiles ofreading impaired children with or without concom-itant oral language deficits. Ann Dyslexia 1982;32:163–176

54. Van der Lely HKJ, Rosen S, McClelland A. Evi-dence for a grammar-specific deficit in children.Curr Biol 1998;8:1253–1258

55. Friel-Patti S, DesBarres K, Thibodeau L. Case stud-ies of children using Fast ForWord. Am J SpeechLang Pathol 2001;10:203–215

56. Gillam RB, Crofford JA, Gale MA, Hoffman LM.Language change following computer-assisted lan-guage instruction with Fast ForWord or LaureateLearning Systems software. Am J Speech LangPathol 2001;10:231–247

57. Marler JA, Champlin CA, Gillam RB. Backwardand simultaneous masking measured in childrenwith language-learning impairments who receivedintervention with Fast ForWord or Laureate Learn-ing Systems software. Am J Speech Lang Pathol2001;10:258–268

58. Thibodeau LM, Friel-Patti S, Britt, L. Psycho-acoustic performance in children completing FastForWord training. Am J Speech Lang Pathol 2001;10:248–257

Page 12: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders
Page 13: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

263

Management of Auditory Processing Disorders; Editor in Chief, Catherine V. Palmer, Ph.D.; Guest Editor, Gail D.Chermak, Ph.D. Seminars in Hearing, volume 23, number 4, 2002. Address for correspondence and reprint requests:Frank E. Musiek, Ph.D., Department of Communication Sciences and Otolaryngology, School of Medicine, Universityof Connecticut, Storrs, CT 06269. E-mail: [email protected]. 1Department of Communication Sciences andOtolaryngology, School of Medicine, University of Connecticut, Storrs, Connecticut; 2Dartmouth-Hitchcock MedicalCenter, Lebanon, New Hampshire. Copyright © 2002 by Thieme Medical Publishers, Inc., 333 Seventh Avenue, NewYork, NY 10001, USA. Tel: +1(212) 584-4662. 00734-0451,p;2002,23,04,263,276,ftx,en;sih00220x.

Plasticity, Auditory Training, and AuditoryProcessing DisordersFrank E. Musiek, Ph.D.,1 Jennifer Shinn, M.S.,2 and Christine Hare, M.A.1

ABSTRACT

Auditory training (AT) for the treatment of auditory processing dis-orders (APD) has generated considerable interest recently. There is emergingevidence that well conceived AT programs can improve higher auditory func-tion. The plasticity of the brain underlies the success of AT. This article re-views brain plasticity and the role of plasticity in AT outcomes, and highlightskey studies that provide insight into the clinical use of AT for APD.

KEYWORDS: Auditory processing disorder, auditory training, plasticity

Learning Outcomes: Upon completion of this article, the reader will be able to (1) understand the rationalebehind auditory plasticity as it applies to APD, (2) understand new therapy techniques for APD, and (3) formu-late therapy approaches using plasticity.

auditory nervous system (CANS). Improve-ment in higher auditory function is related tothe capacity of the central nervous system tochange. This change is tightly linked to, and isa result of, plasticity. This article reviews plas-ticity and AT within the framework of APD.

AUDITORY PLASTICITY DEFINED

There is a significant body of evidence fromboth animals and humans that supports im-provement in auditory tasks through the use ofAT. As we begin to gain new insight into the

Recently, there has been great interest inthe use of auditory training (AT) for the treat-ment of auditory processing disorders (APD).The use of AT for peripheral auditory prob-lems is not a new concept, in that it has an ex-tensive history.1 The use of AT for treatmentof APD is new and its application is differentfrom the classic use of AT. Most important tothis difference is that AT applied to APD istargeting the brain as the main site of media-tion, and the brain, unlike the auditory periph-ery, is plastic. Though peripheral sensorineuralloss cannot be improved upon with AT, thismay not be the case for deficits of the central

Page 14: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

264 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

fundamental mechanisms involved in improvedperformance on specific auditory tasks, we musttake into consideration the concept of auditoryplasticity. The scientific literature on the sub-ject of plasticity is immense. For the purposesof our discussion, we will concentrate specifi-cally on auditory plasticity and overview its re-lationship to AT.

Neural plasticity is defined by Lund2 as“neural form and connections that take on apredictable pattern (probably genetically deter-mined) often referred to as neural specificity;exceptions to this predictable pattern, in cer-tain circumstances, represent neural plasticity.”Auditory (specific) plasticity can be defined asthe alteration of nerve cells to better conformto immediate environmental influences, withthis alteration often associated with behavioralchange.1 It is necessary for both clinicians andresearchers to have a strong foundation in thefundamentals of plasticity and its relationshipto AT, as it is key to management of patientswith APD.

ANIMAL RESEARCH

Perhaps the greatest body of evidence that sug-gests plasticity in the auditory system relatedto training lies in the extensive number of ani-mal studies. Though the scope of this articledoes not allow extensive coverage of this topic,we will highlight a few studies that hopefullyprovide some principles of interest regardingplasticity. Some of the early research on plas-ticity and training via animal models focusedon sub-cortical levels and the brainstem; how-ever, the auditory cortex has recently gainedconsiderable attention. The evidence of corti-cal plasticity is shown impressively by func-tional reorganization, that has attracted muchattention from scientists and clinicians in a va-riety of disciplines.

Studies on rodents, monkeys, cats, and theavian species have shown reorganization withinthe auditory cortex.3–7 There are two main con-ditions that can result in reorganization of theauditory cortex, sub cortex, and brainstem. Oneof these is deprivation that, experimentally, isinduced by creating hearing loss in some man-

ner. This can be achieved by creating a conduc-tive loss, which is often done by plugging theear and/or removing or damaging middle earstructures. Sensorineural hearing loss is experi-mentally created by exposing the ear to ototoxicdrugs, high intensity noise, or mechanicallydamaging the cochlea. These hearing losses willdeprive the higher auditory system of stimula-tion and will result in a reorganization of theauditory cortex.3,8–10 Raising animals in acous-tically attenuated chambers is another methodof creating deprivation effects. High stimulationor tasking of the auditory system is the othercondition that can create plastic changes in theCANS.4,11

The barn owl is one of the most studiedanimal models for demonstrating plasticity.One of the reasons for studying the barn owl isthat its optic tectum (or midbrain), which issimilar in nature to the superior colliculus (SC)of the mammal, contains neural substrate withrespect to both auditory and visual maps. TheSC is one of the most highly studied corticalstructures because of its ability to reorganize.The SC is a useful structure to investigate, as itis particularly sensitive to experience-dependentplasticity and alteration of auditory maps. Thebarn owl provides an extensive amount of mod-eling information to base on the derivation ofits auditory map.12

Evidence of auditory plasticity has beengained through both auditory deprivation andauditory training with the barn owl. Previousdeprivation studies, in which barn owls wereraised with monaural occlusion, provided phys-iologic evidence of inter-aural level differencesoccurring in the posterior division of the ven-tral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus within thebrainstem.13 Through the use of acoustic filter-ing devices, Gold and Knudsen14 investigatedthe effect of altered auditory experience on inter-aural timing differences of barn owls. Relativeto owls raised under normal auditory conditions,owls raised with unilateral auditory deprivationshowed neuronal changes of the inferior col-liculus (in the optic tectum) and changes in be-havioral responses, demonstrating that plasticitywithin the inferior colliculus plays a significantrole in adjustments in the inter-aural timingdifference and frequency tuning. These plas-

Page 15: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

AUDITORY TRAINING PROGRAMS FOR APD/MUSIEK ET AL 265

tic changes in auditory experience as a resultof deprivation and accommodation are notedat both the subcortical12 and higher-level sys-tems.15

A substantial body of research suggeststhat reorganization takes place in all mammals.These findings have strong implications forauditory plasticity in humans. The general find-ings show that sensory maps have training de-pendent reorganization capabilities and sug-gest that a majority, if not all of the brain, maybe plastic.10 There also are indications that inmammals plasticity is lateralized. That is, cor-tical reorganization seems to take place in thehemisphere contralateral to the ear deprived ofacoustic input or for which there is peripheraldamage, which reduces peripheral input.10

MECHANISMS UNDERLYINGCORTICAL PLASTICITY ANDREORGANIZATION

Before auditory plasticity and its relationshipto AT can be fully appreciated, there must be acomprehensive understanding of the mecha-nisms that underlie this critical process. Nu-merous theories have been proposed and themore widely accepted will be briefly discussedfor the purpose of this review.

It is well established that plastic changes area result of neuronal responses to both externalstimuli (i.e., sounds in our environment) andinternal stimuli (e.g., thinking about certainsounds when we listen to music or read). Wetend to think of our brain as a highly stableorgan, when in fact it is its instability that we relyon most for development and auditory learning.There are generally three types of plasticity inthe auditory domain: (1) developmental plastic-ity, (2) compensatory plasticity resulting from alesion occurring somewhere within the auditorysystem, and (3)learning-related plasticity.6

Simple auditory tasks, such as detecting thepresence of a pure tone, require relatively littleneural substrate in the CANS. In contrast, com-plex tasks—such as discriminating small differ-ences among various acoustic stimuli in noise—require considerable neural activity. The simpleauditory task may not be improved signifi-

cantly with AT; however, complex tasks aremore likely to benefit as they necessitate morebrain function (and neural substrate) and, there-fore, are more likely to benefit from brain plas-ticity.1

Generally there are two ways in which thebrain is thought to reorganize. Although bothrequire neuronal changes, their mechanismsare quite different. Reorganization may involvethe activation of neurons and neural connec-tions that were previously in a state of rest.Thesereserve neurons replace the nonfunctional neu-rons. That is, the reserve neurons would re-place connections that are no longer active dueto damage or lack of stimulation.The other pro-posed mechanism underlying brain reorganiza-tion requires that new connections be formed.Both mechanisms involve changes that may beslow and require an extensive amount of timeand training, or the changes can be extremelyrapidly occurring through development andmaturation without intervention.10

It should be noted that neural reorganiza-tion might not be considerable and obvious.According to Kaas,10 these structural changesmay be rather subtle, resulting from modifica-tions in the extent of synaptic connections thatbecome more effective, or from changes inneural location, resulting in a more effectivetargeting or spreading of axonal and dendriticarbors, that in turn results in not only more butbetter located synaptic connections. If newdendrites are either being recruited or devel-oped, their connections must be appropriate toensure that they evolve meaningful function.

CLINICAL CORRELATES OF AUDITORY PLASTICITY IN HUMANS

Clinical experience with hearing aids andcochlear implants provide important insightand evidence of the plasticity phenomenon andraise several key questions as well. As mentionedpreviously, auditory reorganization can resultfrom lack of input to central systems, usuallyafter damage to the periphery. It is known thatthe auditory cortex will reorganize tonotopi-cally if input is markedly reduced at certain

Page 16: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

266 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

frequencies secondary to damage to the corre-sponding part of the cochlea. The cortical re-gion that previously responded to the frequen-cies that have been lost will shift to adjacentfrequencies that are active and viable.5 Thispermits the formerly deprived region of theauditory cortex to become physiologically ac-tive and retain its functional status. This neuralreorganization is a result of the dynamic andactive process of plasticity rather than passiveconsequences of the lesion at the cochlear level.16

The observations provided by clinical investi-gators of patients with bilateral sensorineuralhearing loss who wear hearing aids in only oneear provide information potentially related tocortical reorganzation.17 These patients, overa period of time, will demonstrate decreasedspeech recognition in the ear not amplified,while the amplified ear’s speech recognition re-mained stable or, in some cases, improved. Onemight theorize that these clinical results indi-cate possible reorganization in the cortex.

The success of patients with cochlear im-plants also provides some insights into neuralplasticity. Cochlear implant patients have pro-found hearing loss that in turn results in a formof auditory deprivation to the CANS, whichcould result in reorganization of the auditorycortex. The implant allows stimulation of thecentral system, such that it may reconstitutethe more normal organization and activity ofauditory cortical neurons that have assumed adifferent role because of lack of auditory input.Manrique et al18 demonstrated that a group ofpre-lingually deafened children implanted priorto 3 years of age surpassed the speech percep-tion performance of post-lingually implantedpeers as early as the third year of follow-up.They suggested that the critical period of de-velopmental plasticity extended up to 6 yearsof age; beyond 6 years of age, peripheral stimu-lation was not sufficient to overcome the de-privation sustained by the CANS. Their find-ings reaffirm the expectation that the youngerthe brain, the greater the capacity for plasticity.

A number of studies have focused ondemonstrating that the younger the age at thetime of implantation, and the sooner the im-plantation following deafness, the better the

speech perception abilities.19 Although thesefindings often were used as reasons against theimplantation of deafened individuals outsidethis ideal group, evidence continues to mountfor the brain’s ability to adapt in response toAT later in life and following sustained periodsof deprivation. Labadie and coworkers20 foundsignificant increases in post-operative perfor-mance on word and sentence identification fortwo groups of implant patients with mean agesof 46.9 years and 71.5 years. No significant dif-ferences were reported relative to the medicalprocedure itself, and it was concluded thatneural plasticity continues to exist throughoutlife and that age alone should not be a criterionfor implantation. Geier et al21 demonstratedthat those implanted at a younger age and witha shorter term of deafness (defined as less than60% of life with deafness) showed the bestspeech recognition abilities 3 months post-im-plantation; however, even adults who had beendeaf for over 60% of their lives demonstratedsignificant speech recognition improvements.Although the rate of improvement was slowerfor this latter group, speech perception abilitiescontinued to improve with implant experiencefollowing re-assessment at 6 months after im-plantation. Moreover, deaf subjects who usedsign language to communicate demonstratedsuccessful cochlear implantation.22 While si-multaneously listening and watching sign lan-guage, positron emission topography (PET)scans showed a shift from no activity in the au-ditory cortex after short-term experience withthe implant to much higher levels of activityfollowing long-term experience.22 If the audi-tory areas of the brain previously deprived ofauditory input are able to be stimulated, thereis a potential for success, and the degree ofsuccess will largely depend on the length andquality of follow-up and the habilitation pro-cess. Imaging techniques, such as functionalmagnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), positronemission topography (PET) scans, and corticalevoked potentials are gaining in popularity fortheir potential role in demonstrating neuralplasticity of previously deprived systems in re-sponse to stimulation via cochlear implants andthrough AT.

Page 17: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

AUDITORY TRAINING PROGRAMS FOR APD/MUSIEK ET AL 267

AUDITORY TRAINING WITH HUMANS

Diagnostic central test procedures can guidethe clinician to the types of AT that are re-quired by the patient. A key component of anyAT program is to measure AT training effectsusing various test procedures. Traditionally, be-havioral speech perception tests have been usedas measures of auditory processing abilities,and as measures of treatment outcomes and ef-ficacy. Although these tests are crucial in theauditory processing (AP) assessment battery,performance on these measures is affected byfactors such as attention, motivation, and learn-ing. Recently, attention has focused on audi-tory evoked potentials as noninvasive and moreobjective tools to measure auditory processingand plasticity.23–31 Functional imaging tech-niques also are being used to examine physio-logic changes of the CANS subsequent to AT.These tools provide an objective means to dif-ferentiate subjects with known APD fromnormal subjects. In addition, these objectivemeasures are increasingly demonstrating phys-iological changes as a result of behavioral ATin normal subjects and those with particulardeficits.

Auditory evoked potentials are used to as-sess auditory function from the periphery tothe cortex through the use of electrophysio-logic responses. There are primarily two typesof cortical evoked potentials—obligatory anddiscriminative.24 Obligatory potentials are typ-ically elicited by clicks, tone bursts, and speechphonemes that may vary in duration. Thesecortical potentials (e.g., auditory brainstem re-sponse [ABR] and middle latency response[MLR]) are used clinically in auditory process-ing assessment. Discriminative evoked poten-tials use an oddball paradigm and are elicitedpassively (e.g., mismatch negativity [MMN]),or are elicited with an active paradigm (e.g.,P300).

Using the MMN, Kraus et al23 showedthat physiologic changes in central auditoryfunction and auditory discrimination could beimproved with training. Tremblay et al28 dem-onstrated that AT generalizes to listening situ-

ations beyond those used in the training para-digm, both behaviorally and physiologically (asrecorded by the MMN). Tremblay et al30 con-cluded that behavioral changes are likely to fol-low neurophysiologic changes resulting fromAT and that these neurophysiological mea-sures would serve to determine the efficacy ofAT. Jirsa32 showed an increase in the amplitudeof the P300 and decrease in its latency in chil-dren who underwent a general AT program.These changes in the P300 were not noted in acontrol group of children. Based on the MMN,musicians were found to have superior audi-tory processing relative to nonmusicians whilelistening to perfect versus impure chords33 andto have a more precise and longer temporalwindow of integration.34 These studies provideevidence that improvement in higher auditoryfunction can result from AT, this improvementcan be measured with current assessment tools,and evoked potentials can be used to differ-entiate a range of auditory abilities in normallisteners.

FORMAL AUDITORY TRAINING

Typically, the process of diagnosing and man-aging APD begins with parental or schoolconcerns that result in a child being referred toan audiologist for AP testing. Often the con-cerns involve language, learning, reading, andauditory memory rather than, or in addition to,APD. Due to the complexity and interdepen-dence of these processes, it is essential thatchildren referred for AP testing also be evalu-ated by a speech-language pathologist and ed-ucational psychologist, and that the informa-tion from these professionals be incorporatedin the management of these children. A recentconsensus report provides a good reference foridentification and differential diagnosis of APD,and recommends a minimal test battery for thispurpose.35

Formal AT requires the use of special in-strumentation to manipulate auditory stimuliand would typically be conducted by an audiol-ogist in the clinic or laboratory setting.36 For-mal AT usually incorporates an intensive train-

Page 18: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

268 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

ing schedule and targets specific stimuli usingadaptive techniques to foster improved process-ing in a graded manner.The use of commerciallyavailable computer software can complementformal training at school or at home when reg-ular visits to the lab or clinic are not feasible.

Because plasticity in response to AT ishighly correlated with behavioral contexts suchas motivation,37 it is particularly important withchildren to use feedback and reinforcement tomaintain a high level of motivation during for-mal AT. These considerations have been incor-porated in commercial software supportingAT, but they need to be addressed in any ATprogram, especially in training in the clinic orlab. Modifying stimuli to maintain a fair de-gree of success while challenging the listeneralso needs to be an integral part of formal AT.Various types of training have shown that ifthe task is too easy or too difficult, optimalchanges will not result.1 It is important, there-fore, that task difficulty be carefully selectedif adaptive approaches are not employed (andsometimes even when adaptive approaches areused!).

A variety of formal AT techniques areoutlined below. The reader is referred to Cher-mak and Musiek (see pages 297–308, this issue)for an additional discussion of formal and in-formal AT tasks, as well as general principlesunderlying AT.

Temporal Processing

There is a significant body of evidence thatsuggests that temporal processing abilities arethe foundation of auditory processing, specifi-cally with respect to speech perception. Muchdebate, however, concerns the role of temporalauditory processing in children’s language andreading problems.38,39 Considerable researchhas concluded that poor temporal processingskills, measured behaviorally, are found in manycases of dyslexia.38,39 Unresolved were ques-tions regarding the role of temporal processingfor speech versus nonspeech signals and theprimacy of auditory versus visual processingdeficits for language and reading problems.Recently, studies incorporating brain imaging

and electrophysiologic tests have concludedthat temporal processing deficits underlie poorreading skills.40 Using fMRI, Temple et al41

demonstrated a lack of differential activity inthe left prefrontal region of the cortex forrapidly and slowly changing acoustic stimuli inadults with dyslexia. Improvement in differen-tial sensitivity was identified in this area bothfor normal adult subjects and dyslexic subjectsfollowing intensive training. Also using fMRI,Temple et al42 demonstrated physiologic deficitsfor both phonologic and orthographic processesin children with dyslexia.

New training techniques have developedin parallel with the increased study of electro-physiologic measures sensitive to temporal pro-cessing deficits. Merzenich et al43 demonstratedthe efficacy of computer games in training tem-poral processing thresholds of both speech andnonspeech stimuli for children 5 to 10 years ofage. This study included two-tone ordering andsequencing of consonant-vowel syllables (CVs)modified by duration and intensity of conso-nants and duration of interstimulus intervals.Training was intensive (20 days), tasks incorpo-rated various forms of feedback, and reinforce-ment was used as the basis for the developmentof currently available commercial software (i.e.,Fast ForWord [FFW]; Scientific Learning Cor-poration, 1997). Tallal et al44 reported that for-mal AT using these specially designed computergames incorporating modified temporal speechcues and enhanced speech transitions improvedspeech discrimination and language comprehen-sion among language-impaired children follow-ing an intensive 4-week therapy program.

Several recent studies have examined theefficacy of FFW.45–48 In general, these studiesdemonstrate some advantages to the FFW pro-gram, but overall, they question its efficacy.There is concern about the types of problemsand types of populations that may be best suitedfor FFW. It is interesting that the more recentstudies do not demonstrate the same overallvalue of FFW that was shown in the originalstudies.44 However, earlier and more recentFFW efficacy studies did not use the same eval-uative procedures. It also is curious that none ofthe studies employed commonly used centralauditory tests to measure the effects of FFW.

Page 19: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

AUDITORY TRAINING PROGRAMS FOR APD/MUSIEK ET AL 269

Kujala et al49 demonstrated—through bothbehavioral and physiologic (MMN) measures—that a more general temporal processing deficit,rather than a primarily linguistic deficit, is seenin dyslexic adults. Dyslexic subjects had diffi-culty discriminating temporal sound featuresof tonal stimuli.49 Kujala et al50 also publishedthe results of a unique, auditory-visual discrim-ination training procedure for first-graders iden-tified as having dyslexia. A computer game wasdeveloped to graphically represent the frequency,duration, and intensity of sound as representedby the relative height, length, and thickness ofrectangles. One task was to identify which ofthe two displayed visual patterns matched thesubsequent sound pattern. The second task wasto initially look at a visual pattern while listen-ing to the sound pattern and identify when thelast element was heard. Correct responses wererewarded and incorrect responses were fol-lowed by a repeated presentation of the pat-tern. Rectangles changed colors as they werepresented in order to help the child track thepattern. Following auditory-visual discrimina-tion training, children showed improved read-ing skills, as evidenced through correct identi-fication of words and reading speed, as well asphysiologic changes (as measured by the MMN).They also reported a high correlation betweenthe behavioral and physiologic measures. It isimportant to point out that the Kujala et al50

study used sound and visual patterns for train-ing that required temporal sequencing and cross-modality correlations.

Several studies of commercially availableprograms suggest that through the use of intenseAT, temporal integration abilities improve sig-nificantly.51 Several informal AT techniques—asthose reviewed in Musiek et al36 and Musiek25—also are beneficial for strengthening temporalprocessing skills. Identifying or mimicking pat-terns with differing durations or frequencies, andattention to stress and prosody while reading orlistening to poetry are examples of informal ATtasks that can be used to strengthen temporalprocessing. Similarly, sequencing tones with theuse of the commercial game Simon™ strength-ens frequency discrimination as well as sequenc-ing. The Simon game also provides a visual cor-relate to the acoustic patterns used in the game.

In light of the work by Kujala et al,50 the bi-modal element of the Simon game could proveto be quite beneficial.

Auditory Discrimination

Auditory discrimination is one of the most fun-damental auditory processes. Discrimination offrequency, intensity, and duration of tonal stim-uli can be critical to the discrimination of morecomplex acoustic stimuli such as speech seg-ments and or phonemes. Studies of adult owlmonkeys showed improved frequency discrimi-nation after systematic training, and physiologicand anatomic changes that correlated to the fre-quencies trained, versus those that were not.11

More specifically, this study showed an increasein neural substrate (expanding into adjacentneural areas) related to the frequencies involvedin the AT. Improved performance on both be-havioral tasks and increased amplitude of bothN1 and MMN have been reported53 after train-ing on auditory discrimination tasks. Tremblayet al30 demonstrated that as voice-onset-timediscrimination improved, the amplitude of theN1-P2 late potential waveform also increased.They attributed these changes to an increasednumber of neural connections and neural syn-chrony as a result of AT. Naatanen et al54

demonstrated a gradual sharpening or improveddiscrimination following AT in normal adultsubjects that involved simply repeated presen-tations for comparisons of various complexsounds. The comparison of complex sounds wasset up in an oddball type paradigm to generatean MMN response. Although not initially pres-ent, the MMN was elicited later in the training.Kraus et al23 demonstrated significant physio-logical changes, measured by MMN, in normaladult subjects following speech discriminationtraining to differentiate the CV /da/ with alter-ation of the onset frequencies of the second andthird formant transitions.

Formal auditory discrimination trainingstrategies include improving difference limens(DLs) for nonspeech stimuli. This type of ATbecomes most important when abnormally largeDLs are noted for frequency, intensity, or dura-tion in children with APD. Sloan55 provides

Page 20: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

270 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

practical ideas for training the discriminationof speech stimuli necessary for good vowel andconsonant identification, particularly in youngchildren. It is well accepted that vowel and con-sonant identification and discrimination are crit-ical to reading and language skills.

Earobics (available from Cognitive Con-cepts, Inc.) is a relatively inexpensive computerprogram that has versions available for homeand school situations, and is easily integratedinto most AT programs. This program trainsphonemic segmentation and auditory discrimi-nation of vowels, consonants, and blends. It alsoaddresses such skills as auditory memory, at-tention, and listening in low redundancy situa-tions. Like FFW, Earobics uses adaptive tech-niques, but unlike FFW, Earobics does notemphasize temporal processing skills. In an in-depth study of the effects of 4 weeks of Earobicstraining for children with APD, Wheadon56

reported a significant training effect on the fre-quency pattern test and the Test of AuditoryComprehension of Language (TACL); therewere no changes seen on dichotic digits, dura-tion patterns, or MLR. Despite the absence ofimprovement on some central auditory tests,the author concluded that Earobics trainingwas of benefit to children with APD enrolledin this study.

Auditory Closure

Children identified with poor auditory closureskills, with language and learning disabilities,and those with poor speech perception can po-tentially benefit from a vocabulary buildingtechnique based on Miller and Gildea’s researchof how children learn words.57 In this proce-dure, the child is exposed to unknown wordsthrough reading, listening, and pronouncingthe new words. The new words are then placedin context and the child is required to use thiscontext to deduce the meaning of the word(i.e., contextual derivation). It is important thatthe context has a sufficient number of contex-tual cues so the child can successfully derivethe meaning of the unknown word. Improveduse of contextual cues and a larger vocabularybase can serve as a useful compensatory strat-

egy when confronting an unfamiliar or misper-ceived word. As a pre-learning technique, tar-get words can be chosen by school personnel orparents who can identify new words for up-coming lessons. This allows the child to focusattention on the lesson itself and may fosterself-confidence, which may improve the likeli-hood of class participation. This procedure in-corporates multiple processes, such as auditory,visual, and cognitive skills for word learning,rather than mere memorization of vocabularylists.

A recent article outlines specific parametersfor conducting this contextual approach to vo-cabulary building and includes results of a smallsurvey of school personnel following use of thecontextual word derivation approach.37 Elevenof 12 respondents favored the use of this vocab-ulary building program and noted improvementin several broad areas, such as word knowledge,reading, and academics in general.37

Binaural Integration and

Binaural Separation

Binaural integration and binaural separationtasks are warranted when deficits are identifiedduring dichotic evaluations. A common find-ing in children with APD is a left ear deficit ondichotic speech tasks. Musiek and Shochatdemonstrated significant improvement in bin-aural listening when dichotic training taskswere incorporated in AT.58 Before training, thelistener demonstrated a unilateral deficit on di-chotic digits tests and moderate, bilateral deficitson compressed speech with reverberation tests.Training involved directing the stimuli to thestronger ear at a reduced level, while maintain-ing the higher intensity level to the weaker ear.This paradigm maintains good performancelevels in the weaker ear as the intensity level ofthe stronger ear is gradually raised over a pe-riod of time. This procedure can also be modi-fied by using temporal offsets that lag in thepoorer ear, which improves the poorer ear’s per-formance. By using adaptive techniques, theoffset differentials are reduced over multiplepractice sessions. This allows the improved per-

Page 21: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

AUDITORY TRAINING PROGRAMS FOR APD/MUSIEK ET AL 271

formance of the good ear to stabilize back tonormal and maintain the improvement of theweak ear at a higher level of performance.

Dichotic listening training can supportboth binaural integration and binaural separa-tion. The integration task requires the patientto respond to the stimuli in both ears. In theseparation task, the patient is usually asked toignore one ear and respond to stimuli presentedin the other. Dichotic training should employ awide variety of dichotic stimuli such as words,CVs, numbers, phrases, sentences, and com-plex acoustic stimuli. Other modifications ofbinaural integration and separation procedurescan enhance training.59 In a study on the train-ing of dichotic listening similar to that justmentioned,60 improvements were shown in di-chotic listening abilities for a group of childrenwith learning disabilities and dichotic deficitscompared to a control group.

Auditory Vigilance

Auditory vigilance is essentially the ability fora listener to remain attentive to auditory stim-ulation over a sustained period of time. Audi-tory vigilance underlies listening and learningin the classroom, influences APD diagnostictest outcomes, and indeed influences AT itself,as all require the listener to actively focus onauditory stimuli for extended periods of time.It is hypothesized that sustained, increasedawareness to acoustic stimuli will improve au-ditory vigilance. Informal AT techniques in-clude the use of complex or multi-step audi-tory directives requiring that the child firstlisten to instructions in their entirety beforeperforming a task.36 Providing target words,sounds, categories or ideas randomly scatteredthroughout increasingly longer passages thatare read to the child, provides a simple meansof training this ability.

Auditory Memory Enhancement (AME)

As indicated by frequent concerns from par-ents, school personnel, and educational psy-chologists, children with APD often display

poor auditory memory. Auditory memory isinvolved in a number of tasks employed inAP testing, including frequency and durationpatterns tests, dichotic digits, and competingword tests (e.g., the Staggered Spondaic WordTest [SSW]).62 For this reason, AME has beenrecommended when deficits are found acrossthe AP test battery, across multiple academicsubjects, or whenever auditory memory is aconcern.

Based on psychologic research for enhanc-ing recall, AME has been modified for usewith the APD population.52 The procedure in-volves having the child read aloud or havingthe adult read to the child, a paragraph or longerpassage that constitutes a segment. A segmentis defined as the context that conveys a mainidea. The child’s tasks following the reading ofa segment are to distill the segment into themain idea and to sketch the main idea on paper.The process is repeated for each segment untilthe end of the passage. When the child hascompleted all the sketches for each of the de-fined segments, he/she is required to review allthe sketches and verbally relate the conceptsunderlying the segments. The goal of AME isto develop the child’s ability to listen for gen-eral concepts rather than trying to hear, pro-cess, and recall large amounts of detail. A timelimit of one minute for each sketch is recom-mended. This forces the child to reduce infor-mation by transferring it for Gestalt process-ing. This helps the child formulate conceptsand readies the information for easy memoryintegration. Sketching assists memory throughvisual, spatial and motor (multisensory) repre-sentations, that also increase the number ofneural circuits available to memory areas.52

Though based on well-founded concepts re-lated to memory, the AME still awaits furtherevaluation.

SUMMARY AND CLOSINGCOMMENTS

The link between successful AT and plasticityis a critical one. AT for individuals with APDwill benefit from increased understanding ofneural plasticity. As discussed in this article,

Page 22: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

272 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

there is evidence that the brain can reorganizerelatively quickly when it is deprived of acous-tic input. This reorganization is an importantform of neural plasticity. Questions remain asto whether this plasticity evolves so that theneural substrate can remain physiologically vi-able or to accommodate changing demands onthe system. Neural plasticity is demonstratedwhen the auditory system is trained and im-proves in its function. Whether plasticity evolvesbecause there are neurons held in reserve forjust these purposes or because synaptic endingsare growing and making new connections alsoremains to be clarified. These are some criticalquestions that science is on the verge of an-swering. Clinicians involved in treating andmanaging APD should continue to follow ad-vances in brain plasticity in order to best selectand modify training programs for their patients.

A number of AT techniques are emerging,especially in the area of temporal processing.Probably the best known is the FFW program.Reports of the efficacy of FFW are mixed. Theinitial research report by Tallal et al44 was im-pressive; however, more recent reports haveraised questions about the outcomes.45–48 Thediscrepancies in outcomes may be related tothe selection of patients. The current patientbase may be broader than the base in earlierstudies. Some patients may be better suitedthan others to use and benefit from FFW. Ad-ditional research is needed to determine whichauditory processing deficits are best trainedthrough this procedure. Clinicians also shouldconsider the variety of temporal processingtraining procedures that can be employed in amore informal or formal manner to treat par-ticular deficits. In addition, clinicians shouldconsider the use of other commercial programsthat focus on auditory-language (e.g., phone-mic awareness training) (e.g., Earobics™, Cog-nitive Concepts, Inc., Evanston, IL). Theseprograms may be more applicable to a broaderrange of auditory-language deficits. Thoughsome studies indicate Earobics to have thera-peutic value,56 more research needs to be done.

One of the therapies that is rather intrigu-ing and for which some important mechanismsare known is dichotic listening therapy. Thisadaptive technique of training binaural listen-

ing attacks a common dichotic listening deficitobserved in children with APD—asymmetricalear performance. The auditory memory en-hancement procedure (AME) is a simple ther-apy that incorporates some complex processesthat should benefit memory and, therefore, sup-port auditory processing. The AME procedureinvolves multi-modality representations, orga-nizational perspective and concept formulation;therefore, it also should have a positive influ-ence on academic performance.

In closing, we would like to mention twoaspects of AT that have only been alluded to inthis article but are critical to success—attentionand motivation. Though attention and motiva-tion are not considered part of mainstream au-ditory processing or AT, they are factors thatinfluence outcomes and must, therefore, be ad-dressed by clinicians. Therapies will only workif the patient is willing to put forth effort. Ef-fort requires motivation and attention, and ef-fort is key to triggering plasticity. Though manycomputer programs have attractive animationsto keep the child captivated and working, noth-ing will replace the many advantages of a car-ing, supportive and innovative therapist in cre-ating high motivation and sustained attentionduring therapy. In the days when new instru-mentation and computer software are rapidlybecoming a dominant part of our habilitativeapproach, we must understand that the thera-pist remains the critical link to success.

ABBREVIATIONS

ABR auditory brainstem responseAME auditory memory enhancementAP auditory processingAPD auditory processing disordersAT auditory trainingCANS central auditory nervous systemCVs consonant-vowel syllablesDL difference limenFFW Fast ForWordfMRI functional magnetic resonance imagingMLR middle latency responseMMN mismatch negativity potentialPET positron emission topographySC superior colliculus

Page 23: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

AUDITORY TRAINING PROGRAMS FOR APD/MUSIEK ET AL 273

SSW Staggered Spondaic Word TestTACL Test of Auditory Comprehension of

Language

REFERENCES

1. Musiek FE, Berge B. A neuroscience view of audi-tory training/stimulation and central auditory pro-cessing disorders. In: Katz J, Masters MG, SteckerN, eds. Central Auditory Processing Disorders:Mostly Management. Needham Heights, MA:Allyn & Bacon; 1998:15–32

2. Lund R. Development and Plasticity of the Brain:An Introduction. New York: Oxford UniversityPress; 1978

3. Robertson D, Irvine DRF. Plasticity of frequencyorganization auditory cortex of guinea pigs withpartial unilateral deafness. J Comp Neurol 1989;282:456–471

4. Hassamannova J, Myslivecek J, Novakova V. Ef-fects of early auditory stimulation on cortical areas.In: Syka J, Aitkin K, eds. Neuronal Mechanisms ofHearing. New York: Plenum Press; 1981

5. Schwaber M, Garraghty P, Morel A, Kaas J. Neu-roplasticity of the adult primate auditory cortexfollowing cochlear hearing loss. Am J Otol 1993;14:252–258

6. Scheich H. Auditory cortex: comparative aspects ofmaps and plasticity. Current Opinion in Neurobi-ology 1991;1:236–247

7. Chermak G, Musiek F. Central Auditory Disorders:New Perspectives. San Diego, CA: Singular; 1997

8. Willot J. Effects of aging, hearing loss, and ana-tomical location on thresholds of inferior colliculusneurons in C57BL/6 and CBA mice. J Neurophys-iol 1986;56:391–408

9. Rajan R, Irvine D, Wise L, Heil P. Effect of unilateralpartial cochlear lesions in adult cats on the representa-tion of lesioned and unlesioned cochleas in primaryauditory cortex. J Comp Neurol 1993;338:17–49

10. Kaas J. Plasticity of sensory representations in theauditory and other systems of adult mammals. In:Salvi R, Henderson D, Fiorino F, Colletti V, eds.Auditory System Plasticity and Regeneration. NewYork: Thieme; 1996:213–223

11. Recanzone G, Schreiner C, Merzenich M. Plas-ticity in the frequency representation of primaryauditory cortex following discrimination train-ing in adult owl monkeys. J Neurosci 1993;13:97–103

12. Brainard M, Knudsen E. Experience-dependentplasticity in the inferior colliculus: a site for visualcalibration in the neural representation of auditoryspace in the barn owl. J Neurosci 1993;13:4589–4608

13. Miller G, Knudsen E. Early auditory experienceinduces frequency specific adaptive plasticity in theforebrain gaze field of the barn owl. J Neurophysiol2001;85:2154–2194

14. Gold J, Knudsen E. Abnormal auditory experienceinduces frequency-specific adjustments in unit tun-ing for binaural localization cues in the optic tectumof juvenile owls. J Neurosci 2000;20:862–77

15. Korte D, Rauschecker J. Auditory spatial tuning ofcortical neurons is sharpened in cats with earlyblindness. J Neurophysiol 1993;70:1717–1721

16. Irvine D, Rajan R, Brown M. Injury- and use-related plasticity in adult auditory cortex. Audiol-ogy & Neuro-Otology 2001;6:192–195

17. Silman S, Silverman C, Emmer A, Gelfand S.Adult onset auditory deprivation. J Am Acad Au-diol 1992;3:390–396

18. Manrique M, Cervera-Paz F, Huarte A, et al.Cerebral auditory plasticity and cochlear implants.Int J Pediatr Otorhinolarygol 1999;49(Suppl 1):S193–S197

19. Allum-Mecklenburg D, Babighian G. Cochlearimplant performance as an indicator of auditoryplasticity in humans. In: Salvi R, Henderson D,eds. Auditory System Plasticity and Regeneration.New York: Thieme;1996:395–404

20. Labadie R, Carrasco V, Gilmer C, Pillsbury, H.Cochlear implant performance in senior citizens.Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 2000;123:419–424

21. Geier L, Barker M, Fisher L, Opie J. The effect oflong-term deafness on speech recognition in post-lingually deafened adult CLARION cochlearimplant users. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol 1999;177(Suppl):80–83

22. Nishimura H, Hashikawa K, Doi K. Sign lan-guage ‘heard’ in the auditory cortex. Nature 1999;397:116

23. Kraus N, McGee T, Carrell T, et al. Central auditorysystem plasticity associated with speech discrimina-tion training. J Cogn Neurosci 1195;7:25–32

24. Kraus N. Speech sound perception, neurophysiol-ogy, and plasticity. Int J Pediatric Otorhinolaryngol1999; 47:123–129

25. Ponton C. Possible application of functional imag-ing of the human auditory system in the study of ac-climatization and late onset deprivation. Ear Hear1996;17(Suppl):78S–86S

26. Ponton C, Moore J, Eggermont J. Prolonged deaf-ness limits auditory system developmental plastic-ity: evidence from an evoked potentials study inchildren with cochlear implants. Scand Audiol1999;51(Suppl):13–22

27. Ponton C, Eggermont J, Kwong B, Don M. Matu-ration of human central auditory system activity:evidence from multi-channel evoked potentials.Clin Neurophysiol 2000;111: 220–236

Page 24: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

274 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

28. Tremblay K, Kraus N, Carrell T, McGee T. Centralauditory system plasticity: generalization to novelstimuli following listening training. J Acoust SocAm 1997;102:3762–3773

29. Tremblay K, Kraus N, McGee T. The time courseof auditory perceptual learning: neurophysiologicalchanges during speech-sound training. Neurore-port 1998;9:3557–3560

30. Tremblay K, Kraus N, Mcgee T, Ponton C, Otis B.Central auditory plasticity: changes in the N1-P2complex after speech sound-training. Ear Hear2001;22:79–90

31. Purdy S, Kelly A, Thorne P. Auditory evoked po-tentials as measures of plasticity in humans. Audi-ology & Neuro-Otology 2001;6:211–215

32. Jirsa R. The clinical utility of the P3 AERP in chil-dren with auditory processing disorders. J SpeechHear Res 1993;35:903–912

33. Koelsch S, Shroger E, Tervaniemi M. Superiorpre-attentive auditory processing in musicians.Neuroreport 1999;10:1309–1313

34. Russeler J, Altenmuller E, Nager W, Kohlmetz C,Munte T. Event-related brain potentials to soundomissions differ in musicians and non-musicians.Neurosci Lett 2001;308:33–36

35. Jerger J, Musiek FE. Report of the consensus con-ference on the diagnosis of auditory processing dis-orders in school-aged children. J Am Acad Audiol2000;11:467–474

36. Musiek FE, Baran J, Schochat E. Selected man-agement approaches to central auditory process-ing disorders. Scand Audiol 1999;28(Suppl 51):63–76

37. Ahissar E, Vaadia E, Ahissar M, et al. Dependenceof cortical plasticity on correlated activity of singleneurons and on behavioral context. Science 1992;257:1412–1415

38. Farmer M, Klein R. The evidence for a temporalprocessing deficit linked to dyslexia: a review. PschonBull & Rev 1995;2:460–493

39. Bornstein S, Musiek F. Implication of temporal pro-cessing for children with learning and languageproblems. In: Beasley D, ed. Contemporary Issues inAudition. San Diego, CA: College Hill; 1984:25–54

40. Nagarajan S, Mahncke H, Salz T, et al. Cortical au-ditory signal processing in poor readers. Proc NatlAcad Sci USA 1999;96:6483–6488

41. Temple E, Poldrack R, Propopapas A, et al. Dis-ruption of the neural response to rapid acousticstimuli in dyslexia: evidence from functional MRI.Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2000;97:13907–13912

42. Temple E, Poldrack R, Salidis J, et al. Disruptedneural responses to phonological and orthographi-cal processing in dyslexic children: an fMRI study.Neuroreport 2001;12:299–307

43. Merzenich M, Jenkins W, Johnston P, et al. Tem-poral processing deficits of language-learning im-

paired children ameliorated by training. Science1996;271:77–81

44. Tallal P, Miller S, Bedi G, et al. Language compre-hension in language-learning impaired childrenimproved with acoustically modified speech. Sci-ence 1996;271:81–84

45. Friel-Patti S, Frome-Loeb D, Gillam R. Lookingahead: an introduction to five exploratory studiesof Fast ForWord. Am J Speech Lang Pathol 2001;10:195–2002

46. Frome-Loeb D, Stoke C, Fey M. Languagechanges associated with Fast ForWord language:evidence from case studies. Am J Speech LangPathol 2001;10:216–230

47. Thibodeau L, Friel-Patti S, Britt L. Psychoacousticperformance in children completing Fast ForWordtraining. Am J Speech Lang Pathol 2001;10:248–257

48. Gillam R, Frome-Loeb D, Friel-Patti S. Lookingback: a summary of five exploratory studies of FastForWord. Am J Speech Lang Pathol 2001;10:269–273

49. Kujala T, Myllyviita K, Tervaniemi M, et al. Basicauditory dysfunction in dyslexia as demonstratedby brain activity measurements. Psychophysiology2000;37:262–266

50. Kujala T, Karma K, Ceponiene R, et al. Plasticneural changes and reading improvement causedby audiovisual training in reading-impaired chil-dren. PNAS 2001;98:10509–10514

51. Tallal P, Merzenich M, Miller S, Jenkins W. Lan-guage learning impairments: integrating basic sci-ence, technology, and remediation. Exp Brain Res1998;123:210–219

52. Musiek FE. Habilitation and management of audi-tory processing disorders: overview of selected pro-cedures. J Am Acad Audiol 1999;10:329–342

53. Menning H, Roberts LE, Pantev C. Plastic changesin the auditory cortex induced by intensive fre-quency discrimination training. Neuroreport 2000;11:817–822

54. Naatanen R, Schroger E, Karakas S, TervaniemeiM, Paavilainen P. Development of a memory tracefor a complex sound in the human brain. Neurore-port 1993;4:503–506

55. Sloan C. Treating Auditory Processing Difficultiesin Children. San Diego, CA: College Hill; 1986

56. Wheadon L. The influence of auditory training onbehavioral and electrophysiologic test measures inchildren with central auditory processing disorders(Dissertation). Auckland, New Zealand: Universityof Auckland; 1999

57. Miller G, Gildea P. How children learn words. Sci-entific American 1987;257:94–99

58. Musiek FE, Shochat E. Auditory training and cen-tral auditory processing disorders: a case study.Semin Hear 1998;19:357–366

Page 25: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

AUDITORY TRAINING PROGRAMS FOR APD/MUSIEK ET AL 275

59. Musiek FE, Pinheiro M. Dichotic speech tests in thedetection of central auditory dysfunction. In: Pin-heiro M, Musiek FE, eds. Assessment of CentralAuditory Dysfunction: Foundations in Clinical Cor-relates. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins; 1985

60. Katz J, Chertoff M, Sawusch R. Dichotic training,J Audit Res 1984;24:251–264

61. Zattore R, Halpern A, Perry D, Meyer E, Evans A.Hearing in the mind’s ear: a PET investigation of

musical imagery and perception. J Cognitive Neu-rosci 1996;8:29–46

62. Bornstein S, Musiek FE. Implication of temporalprocessing for children with learning and languageproblems. In: Beasley D, ed. Audition in Child-hood: Methods of Study. San Diego, CA: CollegeHill Press; 1984

Page 26: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders
Page 27: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

263

Management of Auditory Processing Disorders; Editor in Chief, Catherine V. Palmer, Ph.D.; Guest Editor, Gail D.Chermak, Ph.D. Seminars in Hearing, volume 23, number 4, 2002. Address for correspondence and reprint requests:Frank E. Musiek, Ph.D., Department of Communication Sciences and Otolaryngology, School of Medicine, Universityof Connecticut, Storrs, CT 06269. E-mail: [email protected]. 1Department of Communication Sciences andOtolaryngology, School of Medicine, University of Connecticut, Storrs, Connecticut; 2Dartmouth-Hitchcock MedicalCenter, Lebanon, New Hampshire. Copyright © 2002 by Thieme Medical Publishers, Inc., 333 Seventh Avenue, NewYork, NY 10001, USA. Tel: +1(212) 584-4662. 00734-0451,p;2002,23,04,263,276,ftx,en;sih00220x.

Plasticity, Auditory Training, and AuditoryProcessing DisordersFrank E. Musiek, Ph.D.,1 Jennifer Shinn, M.S.,2 and Christine Hare, M.A.1

ABSTRACT

Auditory training (AT) for the treatment of auditory processing dis-orders (APD) has generated considerable interest recently. There is emergingevidence that well conceived AT programs can improve higher auditory func-tion. The plasticity of the brain underlies the success of AT. This article re-views brain plasticity and the role of plasticity in AT outcomes, and highlightskey studies that provide insight into the clinical use of AT for APD.

KEYWORDS: Auditory processing disorder, auditory training, plasticity

Learning Outcomes: Upon completion of this article, the reader will be able to (1) understand the rationalebehind auditory plasticity as it applies to APD, (2) understand new therapy techniques for APD, and (3) formu-late therapy approaches using plasticity.

auditory nervous system (CANS). Improve-ment in higher auditory function is related tothe capacity of the central nervous system tochange. This change is tightly linked to, and isa result of, plasticity. This article reviews plas-ticity and AT within the framework of APD.

AUDITORY PLASTICITY DEFINED

There is a significant body of evidence fromboth animals and humans that supports im-provement in auditory tasks through the use ofAT. As we begin to gain new insight into the

Recently, there has been great interest inthe use of auditory training (AT) for the treat-ment of auditory processing disorders (APD).The use of AT for peripheral auditory prob-lems is not a new concept, in that it has an ex-tensive history.1 The use of AT for treatmentof APD is new and its application is differentfrom the classic use of AT. Most important tothis difference is that AT applied to APD istargeting the brain as the main site of media-tion, and the brain, unlike the auditory periph-ery, is plastic. Though peripheral sensorineuralloss cannot be improved upon with AT, thismay not be the case for deficits of the central

Page 28: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

264 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

fundamental mechanisms involved in improvedperformance on specific auditory tasks, we musttake into consideration the concept of auditoryplasticity. The scientific literature on the sub-ject of plasticity is immense. For the purposesof our discussion, we will concentrate specifi-cally on auditory plasticity and overview its re-lationship to AT.

Neural plasticity is defined by Lund2 as“neural form and connections that take on apredictable pattern (probably genetically deter-mined) often referred to as neural specificity;exceptions to this predictable pattern, in cer-tain circumstances, represent neural plasticity.”Auditory (specific) plasticity can be defined asthe alteration of nerve cells to better conformto immediate environmental influences, withthis alteration often associated with behavioralchange.1 It is necessary for both clinicians andresearchers to have a strong foundation in thefundamentals of plasticity and its relationshipto AT, as it is key to management of patientswith APD.

ANIMAL RESEARCH

Perhaps the greatest body of evidence that sug-gests plasticity in the auditory system relatedto training lies in the extensive number of ani-mal studies. Though the scope of this articledoes not allow extensive coverage of this topic,we will highlight a few studies that hopefullyprovide some principles of interest regardingplasticity. Some of the early research on plas-ticity and training via animal models focusedon sub-cortical levels and the brainstem; how-ever, the auditory cortex has recently gainedconsiderable attention. The evidence of corti-cal plasticity is shown impressively by func-tional reorganization, that has attracted muchattention from scientists and clinicians in a va-riety of disciplines.

Studies on rodents, monkeys, cats, and theavian species have shown reorganization withinthe auditory cortex.3–7 There are two main con-ditions that can result in reorganization of theauditory cortex, sub cortex, and brainstem. Oneof these is deprivation that, experimentally, isinduced by creating hearing loss in some man-

ner. This can be achieved by creating a conduc-tive loss, which is often done by plugging theear and/or removing or damaging middle earstructures. Sensorineural hearing loss is experi-mentally created by exposing the ear to ototoxicdrugs, high intensity noise, or mechanicallydamaging the cochlea. These hearing losses willdeprive the higher auditory system of stimula-tion and will result in a reorganization of theauditory cortex.3,8–10 Raising animals in acous-tically attenuated chambers is another methodof creating deprivation effects. High stimulationor tasking of the auditory system is the othercondition that can create plastic changes in theCANS.4,11

The barn owl is one of the most studiedanimal models for demonstrating plasticity.One of the reasons for studying the barn owl isthat its optic tectum (or midbrain), which issimilar in nature to the superior colliculus (SC)of the mammal, contains neural substrate withrespect to both auditory and visual maps. TheSC is one of the most highly studied corticalstructures because of its ability to reorganize.The SC is a useful structure to investigate, as itis particularly sensitive to experience-dependentplasticity and alteration of auditory maps. Thebarn owl provides an extensive amount of mod-eling information to base on the derivation ofits auditory map.12

Evidence of auditory plasticity has beengained through both auditory deprivation andauditory training with the barn owl. Previousdeprivation studies, in which barn owls wereraised with monaural occlusion, provided phys-iologic evidence of inter-aural level differencesoccurring in the posterior division of the ven-tral nucleus of the lateral lemniscus within thebrainstem.13 Through the use of acoustic filter-ing devices, Gold and Knudsen14 investigatedthe effect of altered auditory experience on inter-aural timing differences of barn owls. Relativeto owls raised under normal auditory conditions,owls raised with unilateral auditory deprivationshowed neuronal changes of the inferior col-liculus (in the optic tectum) and changes in be-havioral responses, demonstrating that plasticitywithin the inferior colliculus plays a significantrole in adjustments in the inter-aural timingdifference and frequency tuning. These plas-

Page 29: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

AUDITORY TRAINING PROGRAMS FOR APD/MUSIEK ET AL 265

tic changes in auditory experience as a resultof deprivation and accommodation are notedat both the subcortical12 and higher-level sys-tems.15

A substantial body of research suggeststhat reorganization takes place in all mammals.These findings have strong implications forauditory plasticity in humans. The general find-ings show that sensory maps have training de-pendent reorganization capabilities and sug-gest that a majority, if not all of the brain, maybe plastic.10 There also are indications that inmammals plasticity is lateralized. That is, cor-tical reorganization seems to take place in thehemisphere contralateral to the ear deprived ofacoustic input or for which there is peripheraldamage, which reduces peripheral input.10

MECHANISMS UNDERLYINGCORTICAL PLASTICITY ANDREORGANIZATION

Before auditory plasticity and its relationshipto AT can be fully appreciated, there must be acomprehensive understanding of the mecha-nisms that underlie this critical process. Nu-merous theories have been proposed and themore widely accepted will be briefly discussedfor the purpose of this review.

It is well established that plastic changes area result of neuronal responses to both externalstimuli (i.e., sounds in our environment) andinternal stimuli (e.g., thinking about certainsounds when we listen to music or read). Wetend to think of our brain as a highly stableorgan, when in fact it is its instability that we relyon most for development and auditory learning.There are generally three types of plasticity inthe auditory domain: (1) developmental plastic-ity, (2) compensatory plasticity resulting from alesion occurring somewhere within the auditorysystem, and (3)learning-related plasticity.6

Simple auditory tasks, such as detecting thepresence of a pure tone, require relatively littleneural substrate in the CANS. In contrast, com-plex tasks—such as discriminating small differ-ences among various acoustic stimuli in noise—require considerable neural activity. The simpleauditory task may not be improved signifi-

cantly with AT; however, complex tasks aremore likely to benefit as they necessitate morebrain function (and neural substrate) and, there-fore, are more likely to benefit from brain plas-ticity.1

Generally there are two ways in which thebrain is thought to reorganize. Although bothrequire neuronal changes, their mechanismsare quite different. Reorganization may involvethe activation of neurons and neural connec-tions that were previously in a state of rest.Thesereserve neurons replace the nonfunctional neu-rons. That is, the reserve neurons would re-place connections that are no longer active dueto damage or lack of stimulation.The other pro-posed mechanism underlying brain reorganiza-tion requires that new connections be formed.Both mechanisms involve changes that may beslow and require an extensive amount of timeand training, or the changes can be extremelyrapidly occurring through development andmaturation without intervention.10

It should be noted that neural reorganiza-tion might not be considerable and obvious.According to Kaas,10 these structural changesmay be rather subtle, resulting from modifica-tions in the extent of synaptic connections thatbecome more effective, or from changes inneural location, resulting in a more effectivetargeting or spreading of axonal and dendriticarbors, that in turn results in not only more butbetter located synaptic connections. If newdendrites are either being recruited or devel-oped, their connections must be appropriate toensure that they evolve meaningful function.

CLINICAL CORRELATES OF AUDITORY PLASTICITY IN HUMANS

Clinical experience with hearing aids andcochlear implants provide important insightand evidence of the plasticity phenomenon andraise several key questions as well. As mentionedpreviously, auditory reorganization can resultfrom lack of input to central systems, usuallyafter damage to the periphery. It is known thatthe auditory cortex will reorganize tonotopi-cally if input is markedly reduced at certain

Page 30: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

266 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

frequencies secondary to damage to the corre-sponding part of the cochlea. The cortical re-gion that previously responded to the frequen-cies that have been lost will shift to adjacentfrequencies that are active and viable.5 Thispermits the formerly deprived region of theauditory cortex to become physiologically ac-tive and retain its functional status. This neuralreorganization is a result of the dynamic andactive process of plasticity rather than passiveconsequences of the lesion at the cochlear level.16

The observations provided by clinical investi-gators of patients with bilateral sensorineuralhearing loss who wear hearing aids in only oneear provide information potentially related tocortical reorganzation.17 These patients, overa period of time, will demonstrate decreasedspeech recognition in the ear not amplified,while the amplified ear’s speech recognition re-mained stable or, in some cases, improved. Onemight theorize that these clinical results indi-cate possible reorganization in the cortex.

The success of patients with cochlear im-plants also provides some insights into neuralplasticity. Cochlear implant patients have pro-found hearing loss that in turn results in a formof auditory deprivation to the CANS, whichcould result in reorganization of the auditorycortex. The implant allows stimulation of thecentral system, such that it may reconstitutethe more normal organization and activity ofauditory cortical neurons that have assumed adifferent role because of lack of auditory input.Manrique et al18 demonstrated that a group ofpre-lingually deafened children implanted priorto 3 years of age surpassed the speech percep-tion performance of post-lingually implantedpeers as early as the third year of follow-up.They suggested that the critical period of de-velopmental plasticity extended up to 6 yearsof age; beyond 6 years of age, peripheral stimu-lation was not sufficient to overcome the de-privation sustained by the CANS. Their find-ings reaffirm the expectation that the youngerthe brain, the greater the capacity for plasticity.

A number of studies have focused ondemonstrating that the younger the age at thetime of implantation, and the sooner the im-plantation following deafness, the better the

speech perception abilities.19 Although thesefindings often were used as reasons against theimplantation of deafened individuals outsidethis ideal group, evidence continues to mountfor the brain’s ability to adapt in response toAT later in life and following sustained periodsof deprivation. Labadie and coworkers20 foundsignificant increases in post-operative perfor-mance on word and sentence identification fortwo groups of implant patients with mean agesof 46.9 years and 71.5 years. No significant dif-ferences were reported relative to the medicalprocedure itself, and it was concluded thatneural plasticity continues to exist throughoutlife and that age alone should not be a criterionfor implantation. Geier et al21 demonstratedthat those implanted at a younger age and witha shorter term of deafness (defined as less than60% of life with deafness) showed the bestspeech recognition abilities 3 months post-im-plantation; however, even adults who had beendeaf for over 60% of their lives demonstratedsignificant speech recognition improvements.Although the rate of improvement was slowerfor this latter group, speech perception abilitiescontinued to improve with implant experiencefollowing re-assessment at 6 months after im-plantation. Moreover, deaf subjects who usedsign language to communicate demonstratedsuccessful cochlear implantation.22 While si-multaneously listening and watching sign lan-guage, positron emission topography (PET)scans showed a shift from no activity in the au-ditory cortex after short-term experience withthe implant to much higher levels of activityfollowing long-term experience.22 If the audi-tory areas of the brain previously deprived ofauditory input are able to be stimulated, thereis a potential for success, and the degree ofsuccess will largely depend on the length andquality of follow-up and the habilitation pro-cess. Imaging techniques, such as functionalmagnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), positronemission topography (PET) scans, and corticalevoked potentials are gaining in popularity fortheir potential role in demonstrating neuralplasticity of previously deprived systems in re-sponse to stimulation via cochlear implants andthrough AT.

Page 31: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

AUDITORY TRAINING PROGRAMS FOR APD/MUSIEK ET AL 267

AUDITORY TRAINING WITH HUMANS

Diagnostic central test procedures can guidethe clinician to the types of AT that are re-quired by the patient. A key component of anyAT program is to measure AT training effectsusing various test procedures. Traditionally, be-havioral speech perception tests have been usedas measures of auditory processing abilities,and as measures of treatment outcomes and ef-ficacy. Although these tests are crucial in theauditory processing (AP) assessment battery,performance on these measures is affected byfactors such as attention, motivation, and learn-ing. Recently, attention has focused on audi-tory evoked potentials as noninvasive and moreobjective tools to measure auditory processingand plasticity.23–31 Functional imaging tech-niques also are being used to examine physio-logic changes of the CANS subsequent to AT.These tools provide an objective means to dif-ferentiate subjects with known APD fromnormal subjects. In addition, these objectivemeasures are increasingly demonstrating phys-iological changes as a result of behavioral ATin normal subjects and those with particulardeficits.

Auditory evoked potentials are used to as-sess auditory function from the periphery tothe cortex through the use of electrophysio-logic responses. There are primarily two typesof cortical evoked potentials—obligatory anddiscriminative.24 Obligatory potentials are typ-ically elicited by clicks, tone bursts, and speechphonemes that may vary in duration. Thesecortical potentials (e.g., auditory brainstem re-sponse [ABR] and middle latency response[MLR]) are used clinically in auditory process-ing assessment. Discriminative evoked poten-tials use an oddball paradigm and are elicitedpassively (e.g., mismatch negativity [MMN]),or are elicited with an active paradigm (e.g.,P300).

Using the MMN, Kraus et al23 showedthat physiologic changes in central auditoryfunction and auditory discrimination could beimproved with training. Tremblay et al28 dem-onstrated that AT generalizes to listening situ-

ations beyond those used in the training para-digm, both behaviorally and physiologically (asrecorded by the MMN). Tremblay et al30 con-cluded that behavioral changes are likely to fol-low neurophysiologic changes resulting fromAT and that these neurophysiological mea-sures would serve to determine the efficacy ofAT. Jirsa32 showed an increase in the amplitudeof the P300 and decrease in its latency in chil-dren who underwent a general AT program.These changes in the P300 were not noted in acontrol group of children. Based on the MMN,musicians were found to have superior audi-tory processing relative to nonmusicians whilelistening to perfect versus impure chords33 andto have a more precise and longer temporalwindow of integration.34 These studies provideevidence that improvement in higher auditoryfunction can result from AT, this improvementcan be measured with current assessment tools,and evoked potentials can be used to differ-entiate a range of auditory abilities in normallisteners.

FORMAL AUDITORY TRAINING

Typically, the process of diagnosing and man-aging APD begins with parental or schoolconcerns that result in a child being referred toan audiologist for AP testing. Often the con-cerns involve language, learning, reading, andauditory memory rather than, or in addition to,APD. Due to the complexity and interdepen-dence of these processes, it is essential thatchildren referred for AP testing also be evalu-ated by a speech-language pathologist and ed-ucational psychologist, and that the informa-tion from these professionals be incorporatedin the management of these children. A recentconsensus report provides a good reference foridentification and differential diagnosis of APD,and recommends a minimal test battery for thispurpose.35

Formal AT requires the use of special in-strumentation to manipulate auditory stimuliand would typically be conducted by an audiol-ogist in the clinic or laboratory setting.36 For-mal AT usually incorporates an intensive train-

Page 32: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

268 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

ing schedule and targets specific stimuli usingadaptive techniques to foster improved process-ing in a graded manner.The use of commerciallyavailable computer software can complementformal training at school or at home when reg-ular visits to the lab or clinic are not feasible.

Because plasticity in response to AT ishighly correlated with behavioral contexts suchas motivation,37 it is particularly important withchildren to use feedback and reinforcement tomaintain a high level of motivation during for-mal AT. These considerations have been incor-porated in commercial software supportingAT, but they need to be addressed in any ATprogram, especially in training in the clinic orlab. Modifying stimuli to maintain a fair de-gree of success while challenging the listeneralso needs to be an integral part of formal AT.Various types of training have shown that ifthe task is too easy or too difficult, optimalchanges will not result.1 It is important, there-fore, that task difficulty be carefully selectedif adaptive approaches are not employed (andsometimes even when adaptive approaches areused!).

A variety of formal AT techniques areoutlined below. The reader is referred to Cher-mak and Musiek (see pages 297–308, this issue)for an additional discussion of formal and in-formal AT tasks, as well as general principlesunderlying AT.

Temporal Processing

There is a significant body of evidence thatsuggests that temporal processing abilities arethe foundation of auditory processing, specifi-cally with respect to speech perception. Muchdebate, however, concerns the role of temporalauditory processing in children’s language andreading problems.38,39 Considerable researchhas concluded that poor temporal processingskills, measured behaviorally, are found in manycases of dyslexia.38,39 Unresolved were ques-tions regarding the role of temporal processingfor speech versus nonspeech signals and theprimacy of auditory versus visual processingdeficits for language and reading problems.Recently, studies incorporating brain imaging

and electrophysiologic tests have concludedthat temporal processing deficits underlie poorreading skills.40 Using fMRI, Temple et al41

demonstrated a lack of differential activity inthe left prefrontal region of the cortex forrapidly and slowly changing acoustic stimuli inadults with dyslexia. Improvement in differen-tial sensitivity was identified in this area bothfor normal adult subjects and dyslexic subjectsfollowing intensive training. Also using fMRI,Temple et al42 demonstrated physiologic deficitsfor both phonologic and orthographic processesin children with dyslexia.

New training techniques have developedin parallel with the increased study of electro-physiologic measures sensitive to temporal pro-cessing deficits. Merzenich et al43 demonstratedthe efficacy of computer games in training tem-poral processing thresholds of both speech andnonspeech stimuli for children 5 to 10 years ofage. This study included two-tone ordering andsequencing of consonant-vowel syllables (CVs)modified by duration and intensity of conso-nants and duration of interstimulus intervals.Training was intensive (20 days), tasks incorpo-rated various forms of feedback, and reinforce-ment was used as the basis for the developmentof currently available commercial software (i.e.,Fast ForWord [FFW]; Scientific Learning Cor-poration, 1997). Tallal et al44 reported that for-mal AT using these specially designed computergames incorporating modified temporal speechcues and enhanced speech transitions improvedspeech discrimination and language comprehen-sion among language-impaired children follow-ing an intensive 4-week therapy program.

Several recent studies have examined theefficacy of FFW.45–48 In general, these studiesdemonstrate some advantages to the FFW pro-gram, but overall, they question its efficacy.There is concern about the types of problemsand types of populations that may be best suitedfor FFW. It is interesting that the more recentstudies do not demonstrate the same overallvalue of FFW that was shown in the originalstudies.44 However, earlier and more recentFFW efficacy studies did not use the same eval-uative procedures. It also is curious that none ofthe studies employed commonly used centralauditory tests to measure the effects of FFW.

Page 33: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

AUDITORY TRAINING PROGRAMS FOR APD/MUSIEK ET AL 269

Kujala et al49 demonstrated—through bothbehavioral and physiologic (MMN) measures—that a more general temporal processing deficit,rather than a primarily linguistic deficit, is seenin dyslexic adults. Dyslexic subjects had diffi-culty discriminating temporal sound featuresof tonal stimuli.49 Kujala et al50 also publishedthe results of a unique, auditory-visual discrim-ination training procedure for first-graders iden-tified as having dyslexia. A computer game wasdeveloped to graphically represent the frequency,duration, and intensity of sound as representedby the relative height, length, and thickness ofrectangles. One task was to identify which ofthe two displayed visual patterns matched thesubsequent sound pattern. The second task wasto initially look at a visual pattern while listen-ing to the sound pattern and identify when thelast element was heard. Correct responses wererewarded and incorrect responses were fol-lowed by a repeated presentation of the pat-tern. Rectangles changed colors as they werepresented in order to help the child track thepattern. Following auditory-visual discrimina-tion training, children showed improved read-ing skills, as evidenced through correct identi-fication of words and reading speed, as well asphysiologic changes (as measured by the MMN).They also reported a high correlation betweenthe behavioral and physiologic measures. It isimportant to point out that the Kujala et al50

study used sound and visual patterns for train-ing that required temporal sequencing and cross-modality correlations.

Several studies of commercially availableprograms suggest that through the use of intenseAT, temporal integration abilities improve sig-nificantly.51 Several informal AT techniques—asthose reviewed in Musiek et al36 and Musiek25—also are beneficial for strengthening temporalprocessing skills. Identifying or mimicking pat-terns with differing durations or frequencies, andattention to stress and prosody while reading orlistening to poetry are examples of informal ATtasks that can be used to strengthen temporalprocessing. Similarly, sequencing tones with theuse of the commercial game Simon™ strength-ens frequency discrimination as well as sequenc-ing. The Simon game also provides a visual cor-relate to the acoustic patterns used in the game.

In light of the work by Kujala et al,50 the bi-modal element of the Simon game could proveto be quite beneficial.

Auditory Discrimination

Auditory discrimination is one of the most fun-damental auditory processes. Discrimination offrequency, intensity, and duration of tonal stim-uli can be critical to the discrimination of morecomplex acoustic stimuli such as speech seg-ments and or phonemes. Studies of adult owlmonkeys showed improved frequency discrimi-nation after systematic training, and physiologicand anatomic changes that correlated to the fre-quencies trained, versus those that were not.11

More specifically, this study showed an increasein neural substrate (expanding into adjacentneural areas) related to the frequencies involvedin the AT. Improved performance on both be-havioral tasks and increased amplitude of bothN1 and MMN have been reported53 after train-ing on auditory discrimination tasks. Tremblayet al30 demonstrated that as voice-onset-timediscrimination improved, the amplitude of theN1-P2 late potential waveform also increased.They attributed these changes to an increasednumber of neural connections and neural syn-chrony as a result of AT. Naatanen et al54

demonstrated a gradual sharpening or improveddiscrimination following AT in normal adultsubjects that involved simply repeated presen-tations for comparisons of various complexsounds. The comparison of complex sounds wasset up in an oddball type paradigm to generatean MMN response. Although not initially pres-ent, the MMN was elicited later in the training.Kraus et al23 demonstrated significant physio-logical changes, measured by MMN, in normaladult subjects following speech discriminationtraining to differentiate the CV /da/ with alter-ation of the onset frequencies of the second andthird formant transitions.

Formal auditory discrimination trainingstrategies include improving difference limens(DLs) for nonspeech stimuli. This type of ATbecomes most important when abnormally largeDLs are noted for frequency, intensity, or dura-tion in children with APD. Sloan55 provides

Page 34: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

270 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

practical ideas for training the discriminationof speech stimuli necessary for good vowel andconsonant identification, particularly in youngchildren. It is well accepted that vowel and con-sonant identification and discrimination are crit-ical to reading and language skills.

Earobics (available from Cognitive Con-cepts, Inc.) is a relatively inexpensive computerprogram that has versions available for homeand school situations, and is easily integratedinto most AT programs. This program trainsphonemic segmentation and auditory discrimi-nation of vowels, consonants, and blends. It alsoaddresses such skills as auditory memory, at-tention, and listening in low redundancy situa-tions. Like FFW, Earobics uses adaptive tech-niques, but unlike FFW, Earobics does notemphasize temporal processing skills. In an in-depth study of the effects of 4 weeks of Earobicstraining for children with APD, Wheadon56

reported a significant training effect on the fre-quency pattern test and the Test of AuditoryComprehension of Language (TACL); therewere no changes seen on dichotic digits, dura-tion patterns, or MLR. Despite the absence ofimprovement on some central auditory tests,the author concluded that Earobics trainingwas of benefit to children with APD enrolledin this study.

Auditory Closure

Children identified with poor auditory closureskills, with language and learning disabilities,and those with poor speech perception can po-tentially benefit from a vocabulary buildingtechnique based on Miller and Gildea’s researchof how children learn words.57 In this proce-dure, the child is exposed to unknown wordsthrough reading, listening, and pronouncingthe new words. The new words are then placedin context and the child is required to use thiscontext to deduce the meaning of the word(i.e., contextual derivation). It is important thatthe context has a sufficient number of contex-tual cues so the child can successfully derivethe meaning of the unknown word. Improveduse of contextual cues and a larger vocabularybase can serve as a useful compensatory strat-

egy when confronting an unfamiliar or misper-ceived word. As a pre-learning technique, tar-get words can be chosen by school personnel orparents who can identify new words for up-coming lessons. This allows the child to focusattention on the lesson itself and may fosterself-confidence, which may improve the likeli-hood of class participation. This procedure in-corporates multiple processes, such as auditory,visual, and cognitive skills for word learning,rather than mere memorization of vocabularylists.

A recent article outlines specific parametersfor conducting this contextual approach to vo-cabulary building and includes results of a smallsurvey of school personnel following use of thecontextual word derivation approach.37 Elevenof 12 respondents favored the use of this vocab-ulary building program and noted improvementin several broad areas, such as word knowledge,reading, and academics in general.37

Binaural Integration and

Binaural Separation

Binaural integration and binaural separationtasks are warranted when deficits are identifiedduring dichotic evaluations. A common find-ing in children with APD is a left ear deficit ondichotic speech tasks. Musiek and Shochatdemonstrated significant improvement in bin-aural listening when dichotic training taskswere incorporated in AT.58 Before training, thelistener demonstrated a unilateral deficit on di-chotic digits tests and moderate, bilateral deficitson compressed speech with reverberation tests.Training involved directing the stimuli to thestronger ear at a reduced level, while maintain-ing the higher intensity level to the weaker ear.This paradigm maintains good performancelevels in the weaker ear as the intensity level ofthe stronger ear is gradually raised over a pe-riod of time. This procedure can also be modi-fied by using temporal offsets that lag in thepoorer ear, which improves the poorer ear’s per-formance. By using adaptive techniques, theoffset differentials are reduced over multiplepractice sessions. This allows the improved per-

Page 35: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

AUDITORY TRAINING PROGRAMS FOR APD/MUSIEK ET AL 271

formance of the good ear to stabilize back tonormal and maintain the improvement of theweak ear at a higher level of performance.

Dichotic listening training can supportboth binaural integration and binaural separa-tion. The integration task requires the patientto respond to the stimuli in both ears. In theseparation task, the patient is usually asked toignore one ear and respond to stimuli presentedin the other. Dichotic training should employ awide variety of dichotic stimuli such as words,CVs, numbers, phrases, sentences, and com-plex acoustic stimuli. Other modifications ofbinaural integration and separation procedurescan enhance training.59 In a study on the train-ing of dichotic listening similar to that justmentioned,60 improvements were shown in di-chotic listening abilities for a group of childrenwith learning disabilities and dichotic deficitscompared to a control group.

Auditory Vigilance

Auditory vigilance is essentially the ability fora listener to remain attentive to auditory stim-ulation over a sustained period of time. Audi-tory vigilance underlies listening and learningin the classroom, influences APD diagnostictest outcomes, and indeed influences AT itself,as all require the listener to actively focus onauditory stimuli for extended periods of time.It is hypothesized that sustained, increasedawareness to acoustic stimuli will improve au-ditory vigilance. Informal AT techniques in-clude the use of complex or multi-step audi-tory directives requiring that the child firstlisten to instructions in their entirety beforeperforming a task.36 Providing target words,sounds, categories or ideas randomly scatteredthroughout increasingly longer passages thatare read to the child, provides a simple meansof training this ability.

Auditory Memory Enhancement (AME)

As indicated by frequent concerns from par-ents, school personnel, and educational psy-chologists, children with APD often display

poor auditory memory. Auditory memory isinvolved in a number of tasks employed inAP testing, including frequency and durationpatterns tests, dichotic digits, and competingword tests (e.g., the Staggered Spondaic WordTest [SSW]).62 For this reason, AME has beenrecommended when deficits are found acrossthe AP test battery, across multiple academicsubjects, or whenever auditory memory is aconcern.

Based on psychologic research for enhanc-ing recall, AME has been modified for usewith the APD population.52 The procedure in-volves having the child read aloud or havingthe adult read to the child, a paragraph or longerpassage that constitutes a segment. A segmentis defined as the context that conveys a mainidea. The child’s tasks following the reading ofa segment are to distill the segment into themain idea and to sketch the main idea on paper.The process is repeated for each segment untilthe end of the passage. When the child hascompleted all the sketches for each of the de-fined segments, he/she is required to review allthe sketches and verbally relate the conceptsunderlying the segments. The goal of AME isto develop the child’s ability to listen for gen-eral concepts rather than trying to hear, pro-cess, and recall large amounts of detail. A timelimit of one minute for each sketch is recom-mended. This forces the child to reduce infor-mation by transferring it for Gestalt process-ing. This helps the child formulate conceptsand readies the information for easy memoryintegration. Sketching assists memory throughvisual, spatial and motor (multisensory) repre-sentations, that also increase the number ofneural circuits available to memory areas.52

Though based on well-founded concepts re-lated to memory, the AME still awaits furtherevaluation.

SUMMARY AND CLOSINGCOMMENTS

The link between successful AT and plasticityis a critical one. AT for individuals with APDwill benefit from increased understanding ofneural plasticity. As discussed in this article,

Page 36: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

272 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

there is evidence that the brain can reorganizerelatively quickly when it is deprived of acous-tic input. This reorganization is an importantform of neural plasticity. Questions remain asto whether this plasticity evolves so that theneural substrate can remain physiologically vi-able or to accommodate changing demands onthe system. Neural plasticity is demonstratedwhen the auditory system is trained and im-proves in its function. Whether plasticity evolvesbecause there are neurons held in reserve forjust these purposes or because synaptic endingsare growing and making new connections alsoremains to be clarified. These are some criticalquestions that science is on the verge of an-swering. Clinicians involved in treating andmanaging APD should continue to follow ad-vances in brain plasticity in order to best selectand modify training programs for their patients.

A number of AT techniques are emerging,especially in the area of temporal processing.Probably the best known is the FFW program.Reports of the efficacy of FFW are mixed. Theinitial research report by Tallal et al44 was im-pressive; however, more recent reports haveraised questions about the outcomes.45–48 Thediscrepancies in outcomes may be related tothe selection of patients. The current patientbase may be broader than the base in earlierstudies. Some patients may be better suitedthan others to use and benefit from FFW. Ad-ditional research is needed to determine whichauditory processing deficits are best trainedthrough this procedure. Clinicians also shouldconsider the variety of temporal processingtraining procedures that can be employed in amore informal or formal manner to treat par-ticular deficits. In addition, clinicians shouldconsider the use of other commercial programsthat focus on auditory-language (e.g., phone-mic awareness training) (e.g., Earobics™, Cog-nitive Concepts, Inc., Evanston, IL). Theseprograms may be more applicable to a broaderrange of auditory-language deficits. Thoughsome studies indicate Earobics to have thera-peutic value,56 more research needs to be done.

One of the therapies that is rather intrigu-ing and for which some important mechanismsare known is dichotic listening therapy. Thisadaptive technique of training binaural listen-

ing attacks a common dichotic listening deficitobserved in children with APD—asymmetricalear performance. The auditory memory en-hancement procedure (AME) is a simple ther-apy that incorporates some complex processesthat should benefit memory and, therefore, sup-port auditory processing. The AME procedureinvolves multi-modality representations, orga-nizational perspective and concept formulation;therefore, it also should have a positive influ-ence on academic performance.

In closing, we would like to mention twoaspects of AT that have only been alluded to inthis article but are critical to success—attentionand motivation. Though attention and motiva-tion are not considered part of mainstream au-ditory processing or AT, they are factors thatinfluence outcomes and must, therefore, be ad-dressed by clinicians. Therapies will only workif the patient is willing to put forth effort. Ef-fort requires motivation and attention, and ef-fort is key to triggering plasticity. Though manycomputer programs have attractive animationsto keep the child captivated and working, noth-ing will replace the many advantages of a car-ing, supportive and innovative therapist in cre-ating high motivation and sustained attentionduring therapy. In the days when new instru-mentation and computer software are rapidlybecoming a dominant part of our habilitativeapproach, we must understand that the thera-pist remains the critical link to success.

ABBREVIATIONS

ABR auditory brainstem responseAME auditory memory enhancementAP auditory processingAPD auditory processing disordersAT auditory trainingCANS central auditory nervous systemCVs consonant-vowel syllablesDL difference limenFFW Fast ForWordfMRI functional magnetic resonance imagingMLR middle latency responseMMN mismatch negativity potentialPET positron emission topographySC superior colliculus

Page 37: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

AUDITORY TRAINING PROGRAMS FOR APD/MUSIEK ET AL 273

SSW Staggered Spondaic Word TestTACL Test of Auditory Comprehension of

Language

REFERENCES

1. Musiek FE, Berge B. A neuroscience view of audi-tory training/stimulation and central auditory pro-cessing disorders. In: Katz J, Masters MG, SteckerN, eds. Central Auditory Processing Disorders:Mostly Management. Needham Heights, MA:Allyn & Bacon; 1998:15–32

2. Lund R. Development and Plasticity of the Brain:An Introduction. New York: Oxford UniversityPress; 1978

3. Robertson D, Irvine DRF. Plasticity of frequencyorganization auditory cortex of guinea pigs withpartial unilateral deafness. J Comp Neurol 1989;282:456–471

4. Hassamannova J, Myslivecek J, Novakova V. Ef-fects of early auditory stimulation on cortical areas.In: Syka J, Aitkin K, eds. Neuronal Mechanisms ofHearing. New York: Plenum Press; 1981

5. Schwaber M, Garraghty P, Morel A, Kaas J. Neu-roplasticity of the adult primate auditory cortexfollowing cochlear hearing loss. Am J Otol 1993;14:252–258

6. Scheich H. Auditory cortex: comparative aspects ofmaps and plasticity. Current Opinion in Neurobi-ology 1991;1:236–247

7. Chermak G, Musiek F. Central Auditory Disorders:New Perspectives. San Diego, CA: Singular; 1997

8. Willot J. Effects of aging, hearing loss, and ana-tomical location on thresholds of inferior colliculusneurons in C57BL/6 and CBA mice. J Neurophys-iol 1986;56:391–408

9. Rajan R, Irvine D, Wise L, Heil P. Effect of unilateralpartial cochlear lesions in adult cats on the representa-tion of lesioned and unlesioned cochleas in primaryauditory cortex. J Comp Neurol 1993;338:17–49

10. Kaas J. Plasticity of sensory representations in theauditory and other systems of adult mammals. In:Salvi R, Henderson D, Fiorino F, Colletti V, eds.Auditory System Plasticity and Regeneration. NewYork: Thieme; 1996:213–223

11. Recanzone G, Schreiner C, Merzenich M. Plas-ticity in the frequency representation of primaryauditory cortex following discrimination train-ing in adult owl monkeys. J Neurosci 1993;13:97–103

12. Brainard M, Knudsen E. Experience-dependentplasticity in the inferior colliculus: a site for visualcalibration in the neural representation of auditoryspace in the barn owl. J Neurosci 1993;13:4589–4608

13. Miller G, Knudsen E. Early auditory experienceinduces frequency specific adaptive plasticity in theforebrain gaze field of the barn owl. J Neurophysiol2001;85:2154–2194

14. Gold J, Knudsen E. Abnormal auditory experienceinduces frequency-specific adjustments in unit tun-ing for binaural localization cues in the optic tectumof juvenile owls. J Neurosci 2000;20:862–77

15. Korte D, Rauschecker J. Auditory spatial tuning ofcortical neurons is sharpened in cats with earlyblindness. J Neurophysiol 1993;70:1717–1721

16. Irvine D, Rajan R, Brown M. Injury- and use-related plasticity in adult auditory cortex. Audiol-ogy & Neuro-Otology 2001;6:192–195

17. Silman S, Silverman C, Emmer A, Gelfand S.Adult onset auditory deprivation. J Am Acad Au-diol 1992;3:390–396

18. Manrique M, Cervera-Paz F, Huarte A, et al.Cerebral auditory plasticity and cochlear implants.Int J Pediatr Otorhinolarygol 1999;49(Suppl 1):S193–S197

19. Allum-Mecklenburg D, Babighian G. Cochlearimplant performance as an indicator of auditoryplasticity in humans. In: Salvi R, Henderson D,eds. Auditory System Plasticity and Regeneration.New York: Thieme;1996:395–404

20. Labadie R, Carrasco V, Gilmer C, Pillsbury, H.Cochlear implant performance in senior citizens.Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg 2000;123:419–424

21. Geier L, Barker M, Fisher L, Opie J. The effect oflong-term deafness on speech recognition in post-lingually deafened adult CLARION cochlearimplant users. Ann Otol Rhinol Laryngol 1999;177(Suppl):80–83

22. Nishimura H, Hashikawa K, Doi K. Sign lan-guage ‘heard’ in the auditory cortex. Nature 1999;397:116

23. Kraus N, McGee T, Carrell T, et al. Central auditorysystem plasticity associated with speech discrimina-tion training. J Cogn Neurosci 1195;7:25–32

24. Kraus N. Speech sound perception, neurophysiol-ogy, and plasticity. Int J Pediatric Otorhinolaryngol1999; 47:123–129

25. Ponton C. Possible application of functional imag-ing of the human auditory system in the study of ac-climatization and late onset deprivation. Ear Hear1996;17(Suppl):78S–86S

26. Ponton C, Moore J, Eggermont J. Prolonged deaf-ness limits auditory system developmental plastic-ity: evidence from an evoked potentials study inchildren with cochlear implants. Scand Audiol1999;51(Suppl):13–22

27. Ponton C, Eggermont J, Kwong B, Don M. Matu-ration of human central auditory system activity:evidence from multi-channel evoked potentials.Clin Neurophysiol 2000;111: 220–236

Page 38: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

274 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

28. Tremblay K, Kraus N, Carrell T, McGee T. Centralauditory system plasticity: generalization to novelstimuli following listening training. J Acoust SocAm 1997;102:3762–3773

29. Tremblay K, Kraus N, McGee T. The time courseof auditory perceptual learning: neurophysiologicalchanges during speech-sound training. Neurore-port 1998;9:3557–3560

30. Tremblay K, Kraus N, Mcgee T, Ponton C, Otis B.Central auditory plasticity: changes in the N1-P2complex after speech sound-training. Ear Hear2001;22:79–90

31. Purdy S, Kelly A, Thorne P. Auditory evoked po-tentials as measures of plasticity in humans. Audi-ology & Neuro-Otology 2001;6:211–215

32. Jirsa R. The clinical utility of the P3 AERP in chil-dren with auditory processing disorders. J SpeechHear Res 1993;35:903–912

33. Koelsch S, Shroger E, Tervaniemi M. Superiorpre-attentive auditory processing in musicians.Neuroreport 1999;10:1309–1313

34. Russeler J, Altenmuller E, Nager W, Kohlmetz C,Munte T. Event-related brain potentials to soundomissions differ in musicians and non-musicians.Neurosci Lett 2001;308:33–36

35. Jerger J, Musiek FE. Report of the consensus con-ference on the diagnosis of auditory processing dis-orders in school-aged children. J Am Acad Audiol2000;11:467–474

36. Musiek FE, Baran J, Schochat E. Selected man-agement approaches to central auditory process-ing disorders. Scand Audiol 1999;28(Suppl 51):63–76

37. Ahissar E, Vaadia E, Ahissar M, et al. Dependenceof cortical plasticity on correlated activity of singleneurons and on behavioral context. Science 1992;257:1412–1415

38. Farmer M, Klein R. The evidence for a temporalprocessing deficit linked to dyslexia: a review. PschonBull & Rev 1995;2:460–493

39. Bornstein S, Musiek F. Implication of temporal pro-cessing for children with learning and languageproblems. In: Beasley D, ed. Contemporary Issues inAudition. San Diego, CA: College Hill; 1984:25–54

40. Nagarajan S, Mahncke H, Salz T, et al. Cortical au-ditory signal processing in poor readers. Proc NatlAcad Sci USA 1999;96:6483–6488

41. Temple E, Poldrack R, Propopapas A, et al. Dis-ruption of the neural response to rapid acousticstimuli in dyslexia: evidence from functional MRI.Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2000;97:13907–13912

42. Temple E, Poldrack R, Salidis J, et al. Disruptedneural responses to phonological and orthographi-cal processing in dyslexic children: an fMRI study.Neuroreport 2001;12:299–307

43. Merzenich M, Jenkins W, Johnston P, et al. Tem-poral processing deficits of language-learning im-

paired children ameliorated by training. Science1996;271:77–81

44. Tallal P, Miller S, Bedi G, et al. Language compre-hension in language-learning impaired childrenimproved with acoustically modified speech. Sci-ence 1996;271:81–84

45. Friel-Patti S, Frome-Loeb D, Gillam R. Lookingahead: an introduction to five exploratory studiesof Fast ForWord. Am J Speech Lang Pathol 2001;10:195–2002

46. Frome-Loeb D, Stoke C, Fey M. Languagechanges associated with Fast ForWord language:evidence from case studies. Am J Speech LangPathol 2001;10:216–230

47. Thibodeau L, Friel-Patti S, Britt L. Psychoacousticperformance in children completing Fast ForWordtraining. Am J Speech Lang Pathol 2001;10:248–257

48. Gillam R, Frome-Loeb D, Friel-Patti S. Lookingback: a summary of five exploratory studies of FastForWord. Am J Speech Lang Pathol 2001;10:269–273

49. Kujala T, Myllyviita K, Tervaniemi M, et al. Basicauditory dysfunction in dyslexia as demonstratedby brain activity measurements. Psychophysiology2000;37:262–266

50. Kujala T, Karma K, Ceponiene R, et al. Plasticneural changes and reading improvement causedby audiovisual training in reading-impaired chil-dren. PNAS 2001;98:10509–10514

51. Tallal P, Merzenich M, Miller S, Jenkins W. Lan-guage learning impairments: integrating basic sci-ence, technology, and remediation. Exp Brain Res1998;123:210–219

52. Musiek FE. Habilitation and management of audi-tory processing disorders: overview of selected pro-cedures. J Am Acad Audiol 1999;10:329–342

53. Menning H, Roberts LE, Pantev C. Plastic changesin the auditory cortex induced by intensive fre-quency discrimination training. Neuroreport 2000;11:817–822

54. Naatanen R, Schroger E, Karakas S, TervaniemeiM, Paavilainen P. Development of a memory tracefor a complex sound in the human brain. Neurore-port 1993;4:503–506

55. Sloan C. Treating Auditory Processing Difficultiesin Children. San Diego, CA: College Hill; 1986

56. Wheadon L. The influence of auditory training onbehavioral and electrophysiologic test measures inchildren with central auditory processing disorders(Dissertation). Auckland, New Zealand: Universityof Auckland; 1999

57. Miller G, Gildea P. How children learn words. Sci-entific American 1987;257:94–99

58. Musiek FE, Shochat E. Auditory training and cen-tral auditory processing disorders: a case study.Semin Hear 1998;19:357–366

Page 39: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

AUDITORY TRAINING PROGRAMS FOR APD/MUSIEK ET AL 275

59. Musiek FE, Pinheiro M. Dichotic speech tests in thedetection of central auditory dysfunction. In: Pin-heiro M, Musiek FE, eds. Assessment of CentralAuditory Dysfunction: Foundations in Clinical Cor-relates. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins; 1985

60. Katz J, Chertoff M, Sawusch R. Dichotic training,J Audit Res 1984;24:251–264

61. Zattore R, Halpern A, Perry D, Meyer E, Evans A.Hearing in the mind’s ear: a PET investigation of

musical imagery and perception. J Cognitive Neu-rosci 1996;8:29–46

62. Bornstein S, Musiek FE. Implication of temporalprocessing for children with learning and languageproblems. In: Beasley D, ed. Audition in Child-hood: Methods of Study. San Diego, CA: CollegeHill Press; 1984

Page 40: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders
Page 41: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

277

Management of Auditory Processing Disorders; Editor in Chief, Catherine V. Palmer, Ph.D.; Guest Editor, Gail D.Chermak, Ph.D. Seminars in Hearing, volume 23, number 4, 2002. Address for correspondence and reprint requests: JamesW. Hall III, Ph.D., Department of Communicative Disorders, P.O. Box 100174, 1600 Archer Road D2–13, University ofFlorida, Gainesville, FL 32610–0174. Email. [email protected]. 1Department of Communicative Disorders, College ofHealth Professions, University of Florida, Gainesville, Florida. Copyright © 2002 by Thieme Medical Publishers, Inc., 333Seventh Avenue, New York, NY 10001, USA. Tel: +1(212) 584-4662. 00734-0451,p;2002,23,04,277,286,ftx,en;sih00221x.

Auditory Processing Disorders: ManagementApproaches Past to PresentDiane Wertz, M.S.,1 James W. Hall III, Ph.D.,1 and Wes Davis II, Au.D.1

ABSTRACT

The ultimate goal of screening and diagnostic assessment for audi-tory processing disorder (APD) is to determine an effective managementstrategy. The effectiveness and precision of treatment for APD is directly re-lated to the specificity and accuracy of the diagnosis. Historically, confusionand vagueness in the definition and diagnosis of APD was associated withscattered management approaches with unproven or, at best, questionabletherapeutic value. Although embraced by some audiologists and speech-language pathologists, the outcome of these often broad-based and rathergeneric management methods was not supported by any evidence. In thisarticle, we review the evolution of APD management from the two distinctprofessional perspectives of audiology and speech-language pathology.Linkages between current treatment options and earlier management tech-niques are emphasized. The goal of this brief review is to provide the readerwith a better understanding of the precursors to diverse conventional andcomputer-mediated therapeutic techniques that are now available.

KEYWORDS: Auditory processing disorder, history, management

Learning Outcomes: After reading this article, the reader will be able to (1) discuss the origins of audiologistinvolvement in the assessment and management of APD; (2) differentiate the approaches to APD assessmentand management taken by audiologists versus speech pathologists; and (3) identify, based on a historical per-spective, the major advantages of deficit specific treatment approaches for APD.

(Fig. 1) stressed the importance of clinicallyevaluating central auditory function, especiallyin children suspected of communicative disor-ders.1 Dr. Myklebust developed an internationalreputation in special education and learning

Interest in the assessment and manage-ment of APD can be traced to the very begin-ning of audiology as a profession. In his text-book Auditory Disorders in Children: A Manualfor Differential Diagnosis, Helmer Myklebust

Page 42: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

278 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

Figure 1 Helmer Myklebust, Ph.D. A professor inthe learning disabilities program at Northwestern Uni-versity for many years, Dr. Myklebust was instrumen-tal in raising the awareness of central auditory func-tion among audiologists and speech pathologists.

disabilities. He earned an M.A. in the psychol-ogy of deafness from Gallaudet University in1935, another M.A. in clinical psychology fromTemple University in 1942, and then an Ed.D.in psychology and counseling from RutgersUniversity in 1945. His first position, in 1931,was as an instructor at the School for the Deafin Knoxville, Tennessee. From 1948 to 1969,Myklebust was a professor of psychology, neu-rology, and psychiatry, and Director of theInstitute for Language Disorders at North-western University where he was a colleague ofRaymond Carhart (the Father of Audiology)and where he also influenced a promising youngauditory scholar—James Jerger. In his writings,Myklebust clearly delineated the connectionbetween the auditory modality and language.In addition, he admonished the clinician (i.e.,audiologist) to go beyond an evaluation of pe-ripheral auditory function in the clinical as-sessment of hearing in children. In the erawhen the audiogram reigned supreme, Mykle-bust promoted true measurement of hearing(i.e., of higher level auditory processes).

Even with the emergence in the 1970s oftest batteries for differential diagnosis of centralauditory function,2,3 management of APD wasnot within the scope of practice for the typicalaudiologist. Support for this statement can be

found in the audiology textbooks of the day. Forexample, in his popular book, Audiology (4th edi-tion), Hayes Newby4 provided a very modestdiscussion of central auditory disorders, empha-sizing neuropathologies such as brain tumorsand abcesses, vascular insults, traumatic brain in-jury, infections (e.g., meningitis and encephali-tis), degenerative diseases (e.g., Parkinson’s dis-ease or multiple sclerosis), and pediatric centralnervous system disorders (e.g., erythroblastosisfetalis, kernicterus). In an apparent reference towhat we would now refer to as APD, Newbyused the term auditory imperception and notedthat “children of normal intelligence and normalhearing sensitivity who do not develop languageabilities at the usual time are frequently des-ignated aphasic.” Following a brief review oforganic brain disorders, Newby concluded witha somewhat curious and contradictory passage:“Because it is a neurological disorder, a centralproblem falls within the domain of the neurolo-gist, neurosurgeon, and psychiatrist, rather thanthat of the otologist. The audiologist, however, isinterested in central auditory disorders becausepart of the audiologist’s responsibility is to dif-ferentiate peripheral from central impairments”(page 101). Hayes proffers no other commentsregarding management. In another popular text-book of the time—Hearing and Deafness (3rd edi-tion), Davis and Silverman5 provide a similar re-view of organic etiologies for central dysacusisand also do not venture into the topic of man-agement by the audiologist.

Given the paucity of formal education onthe principles of APD assessment and man-agement, and of clinical training with APDpatient populations available to audiologistsduring this time period, it is not surprising thattheir management of patients with APD, ifconsidered at all, tended to be rather unin-spired and generic. In fact, the responsibilityfor treatment of children with “auditory imper-ception” or “central dysacusis” was more com-monly assumed by the speech-language pathol-ogist rather than the audiologist.

As early as the 1970s, one audiologist—Robert W. Keith (Fig. 2)—tackled the clinicalchallenge of central auditory processing disor-ders head on. Dr. Keith has for many years beenon the faculty at the University of CincinnatiCollege of Medicine. Beginning with his edited

Page 43: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

MANAGEMENT APPROACHES TO APT/WERTZ ET AL 279

Figure 2 Robert W. Keith, Ph.D. Dr. Keith has madevaried contributions to our understanding of auditoryprocessing disorders. He has also developed stan-dardized measures of auditory processing for chil-dren and adults.

textbook Central Auditory Dysfunction,”6 Keithexerted a significant influence on the audiologicapproach to assessment of this important clini-cal population. With over 50 book chapters andjournal articles, and hundreds of workshops andconvention presentations on APD, he single-handedly altered clinical standard-of-care formany practicing audiologists. He also served asa mentor for dozens of graduate students.Among audiologists and speech pathologists,Keith is well known for his published tests. Hiscommercially-available clinical procedures in-clude the SCAN-C: A Screening Test for Audi-tory Processing Disorders in Children (Psycho-logical Corporation, 1986), SCAN-A: A Testfor Auditory Processing Disorders in Adoles-cence and Adults (Psychological Corporation,1994), the Auditory Continuous PerformanceTest (Psychological Corporation, 1994) for dif-ferentiation of attention deficit disorder, theAuditory Fusion Test Revised (Auditec of St.Louis, 1996), the Random Gap Detection Test(Auditec of St. Louis, 2000), and the TimeCompressed Sentence Test (Auditec of St.Louis, 2002). We are indebted to Dr. Keith forhis innumerable contributions to the professionof audiology and, in particular, to our under-standing of auditory processing disorders.

In the following discussions, we reviewbriefly the roles assumed by each professionalin the management of children with APD.

THE SPEECH-LANGUAGEPATHOLOGY PERSPECTIVE

Historically, the management of APD hasbeen as diverse as the definitions and theoriesthat define the disorder. Professionals who ap-proach APD can be broadly divided into twogroups: those who ascribe to a language-basedorigin, in which deficits are viewed in terms oftheir linguistic dependency, and those whomaintain that disorders are auditory-percep-tual in nature, and occur due to a breakdown inthe central auditory nervous system. Cognitiveneuroscience correlates these two views as top-down processing or bottom-up processing. In-tervention strategies have varied throughoutthe years depending on the processing modelused. We review the history of APD manage-ment strategies that have used either or a com-bination of these processing models.

Myklebust1 was one of the first researchersto state that an auditory deficit could explainlanguage disorders in children. This auditorydeficit was labeled as the “auditory perceptualdeficit” or “auditory language learning disorder,”and was considered a list of symptoms withouta disease.7 The symptoms included an inabilityto use spoken or linguistic auditory input ef-fectively. If this model were endorsed, thenpresumably the symptoms caused the languagedysfunction and isolating the symptoms andtreating them would cure the language prob-lem. Rees,8 and Bloom and Lahey,9 among oth-ers, disagreed with this bottom-up languageprocessing view. Instead, these researchers as-serted that the auditory problem was due to alanguage disorder, a top-down language pro-cessing view. Therefore, management was con-centrated on training language skills, ratherthan isolated auditory functions. The goals ofthis language intervention were formulated interms of language use: the sounds, words, sen-tences, and communicative function that thechild needed to employ to function more suc-cessfully in his or her environment. Speech-language pathologists made the distinction

Page 44: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

280 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

between auditory perception and auditory com-prehension, and asked the question: are thechildren misperceiving what they hear or fail-ing to understand what they perceive?

Top-Down Management Approaches

In 1971, Patricia and Phyllis Lindamood10 de-veloped the multisensory Auditory Discrimi-nation in Depth (ADD) Program (now calledLIPS) based on research that stressed the im-portance of auditory perception and compar-ing phonemes in spoken syllables (or, as it wastermed by the Lindamoods, auditory concep-tual function), and its relationship to speech,reading, and spelling. Samuel Orton,11 a neu-rologist, first described this important rela-tionship in 1937. The Lindamoods explainedauditory processing as five general processes:sensory input, perception, conceptualization,storage, and retrieval. According to the Lin-damoods, if sensory input is incomplete, thenany other process can encounter difficulty. TheLIPS program is based on a multisensory ex-perience. The speech sounds are taught basedon their individual characteristics that can beheard, seen, and felt through oral-motor activi-ties. A questioning process is used that helpsthe student discover the sensory feedback fromear, eye, and mouth involved in identifying,classifying, and labeling the speech sounds.

Orton and Anna Gillingham also devel-oped a multisensory reading instruction programthat was language based. The Orton-Gilling-ham program is a structured, sequential, andcumulative program, similar to LIPS. How-ever, the kinesthetic portion of the remediationwas tactile, having the child write letters andwords with their finger rather than having thesource of sensory information come from theoral-motor activity that produces them, as inthe LIPS program.

Bottom-Up Management Approaches

A bottom-up, auditory perceptual approachto management was developed by ChristineSloan.12 Sloan stated that auditory perceptionwas the outcome of auditory processing. She,

along with other researchers, suggested that anauditory processing deficit underlies certainspeech and language disorders in children.That is, difficulties in auditory processing ledto difficulties in auditory perception, and sub-sequently in impaired language development.Sloan12 defined auditory processing difficultyas the difficulty in processing the acoustic sig-nal that interferes with accurate and efficientperception of speech. She developed a treat-ment program that was based on an integrationof theory, research, and clinical experience. TheSloan program was designed to facilitate moreaccurate and efficient speech perception bytraining speech discrimination. The child learnsto discriminate speech-sound contrasts in in-creasingly more difficult phonetic sequences.In noting that her program was not intendedto address all aspects of auditory processingdifficulties, Sloan suggested additional therapyto develop better use of contextual cues in au-ditory perception and comprehension.12

Combined Top-Down/Bottom-Up

Management Approach

In 1992, the American Speech-LanguageHearing Association13 issued a definition ofcentral auditory processing disorders (CAPD):“CAPD refers to limitations in the ongoingtransmission, analysis, organization, transfor-mation, elaboration, storage, retrieval, and useof information contained in audible signals.”Expanding on this approach, Dorothy Kelly14

described central auditory processing as a phe-nomenon involving a range of behaviors fromawareness of the presence of sound to higher-order analysis of linguistic information. Kellydeveloped management strategies that blendedboth bottom-up and top-down processing,concentrating on auditory memory, auditorydiscrimination, auditory figure-ground, audi-tory cohesion, and auditory attention.14 In1996, the ASHA Task Force on Central Audi-tory Processing developed a much more concisedefinition of CAPD, due to improved diagnos-tic testing. CAPD was defined as a deficiencyin one or more of the central auditory pro-cesses. A year later, ASHA published guide-lines for APD management.15

Page 45: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

MANAGEMENT APPROACHES TO APT/WERTZ ET AL 281

THE AUDIOLOGY PERSPECTIVE

Even in the 1990s, most audiologists did not in-corporate assessment and management of APDinto their clinical practice. Based on the contentof popular audiology textbooks, the relativelyfew who performed screening and audiologic as-sessment of children with suspected APD wereencouraged to employ general and one-size-fits-all strategies for the management of chil-dren. The two most common management ap-proaches, and the strategies that remain the mostreadily embraced by the audiologist, are modifi-cations of the classroom and the recommenda-tion for some type of assistive listening device.

For children with APD, classroom envi-ronmental modifications are crucial. The accu-rate perception of speech is perhaps the mostvital factor in a child’s academic success or fail-ure in the classroom.16 Therefore, classroomacoustics are a key variable in academic achieve-ment. Children with APD often experiencecompromised ability to perform auditory clo-sure tasks (such as speech-in-noise tasks) andmay therefore benefit from an improved signal-to-noise ratio (SNR).17 For some children withAPD, classroom acoustic modification is nodoubt an element in ensuring academic success.

One important aspect of the classroomenvironment is reverberation. Reverberation isthe prolongation of sound waves from a soundsource as they reflect off of hard surfaces in aroom. Reverberation can detrimentally affect achild’s ability to understand a spoken messageand consequently can hinder his/her academicprogress.16 Generally, the larger the classroom,the greater the reverberation problems. Addi-tionally, classrooms with hard surfaces andbare walls, floors, and ceilings tend to exhibithigher reverberation times (i.e., poorer acous-tic properties). Reverberation time can be de-creased by using smaller classrooms and byusing absorptive surfaces (e.g., carpeting, drap-ery, acoustic ceiling tiles).

Another major problem many childrenface in today’s educational setting is generalclassroom noise. Noise may be defined as anyundesired auditory disturbance that interfereswith what the listener intends to hear.18 Noisecan compromise speech perception abilities inchildren with normal hearing as well as in chil-

dren with hearing loss.16,19 Certainly, one canassume that children with APD also may expe-rience difficulties with noise. Examples ofsources of noise in a typical classroom may in-clude other children talking, shuffling papers,chair/desk legs sliding on a floor, air condition-ing units, and so on. Children may experiencethe aforementioned detrimental effects of noiseon speech perception in a variety of ways, in-cluding upward spread of masking, attentionproblems, and/or concentration.16

Assessment of the child’s individual edu-cational situation may reveal specific opportu-nities for noise reduction and SNR improve-ment, such as preferential seating away fromnoise generators (e.g., air conditioners) or plac-ing tennis balls on the bottoms of metal chairlegs, etc. However, excessive reverberation andnoise in many classrooms may make acousticmodification of the room (that meets acousticstandards) prohibitively expensive. In such cases,frequency-modulated (FM) systems may bethe best consideration.19 FM systems attemptto reconcile two separate classroom acousticsproblems. FM systems enhance the SNR byincreasing the teacher’s voice intensity com-pared to the rest of the classroom. Addition-ally, FM systems effectively decrease speaker-to-listener distance, which can improve speechperception. These improvements can affectthe student’s ability to adequately understandthe teacher and thus improve academic perfor-mance. FM systems are available as personalFM systems (with headphone or earbud use, asmall desktop speaker, or via direct input into ahearing aid), and sound-field systems. Histori-cally, clinicians believed that FM systems wouldbenefit all children diagnosed with APD. How-ever, as diagnostic assessment has improved,FM systems are not recommended for everychild with APD,20 but rather for those childrenthat have difficulty understanding speech inthe presence of competing noise. The reader isreferred to the articles by Ferre, and by Rosen-berg (this issue, pages 319–326, and pages309–318, respectively) for a further discussionof this topic.

Classroom acoustic modification, whetherby environment modification, preferential seat-ing, or FM system use, remains an importantconsideration for APD treatment in the aca-

Page 46: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

282 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

demic setting. FM systems are a cost effectiveway to effectively increase the SNR of theteacher’s voice while decreasing speaker-to-listener distance.19 As previously discussed, audi-ologists have viewed classroom amplification asa simple, relatively cost effective strategy for en-hancing the chances of academic success, espe-cially for children with APD. For these reasons,classroom acoustics modification deservedly hascontinued to be a viable option for the audiolo-gist in managing the child with APD.

Jack Katz, widely acknowledged as an ex-pert on APD, summarized the foregoing genericmanagement approach for children identifiedwith APD 21. He identified four specific compo-nents for management of APD in the school:

• classroom instruction adaptations (e.g., writ-ten information to supplement verbal infor-mation),

• classroom acoustic modifications to reducenoise,

• assistive listening devices (classroom and in-dividual FM),

• unilateral earplugs (to minimize the deleteri-ous effect of the weak ear in dichotic tasks)or bilateral earplugs (to minimize the dis-traction of background noise).

Suggesting a shift in the traditional audio-logic management approach, Katz went on tonote that “recommendations should be basedon the individual’s needs and the problem situ-ations faced, rather than simply a generic ap-proach.”21 He then cited specific examples ofindividual and direct therapy techniques to im-prove phonemic concepts and skills, desensiti-zation to background noise, and developmentof auditory memory and sequencing abilities.Recently, the trend in APD management hasbeen toward more individualized, prescriptive,and evidence-based therapy.

RECENT DEVELOPMENTS

With time, management of APD has taken ona multidisciplinary approach. The managementteam now must include an audiologist, a speech-language pathologist, teachers, and parents, and

often a neuropsychologist, occupational thera-pist, and other health care professionals.22,23

The importance of an individualized manage-ment plan for each child is now stressed. Indi-vidualized plans that include modifications(i.e., changes in what a child is expected to learnand demonstrate) and accommodations (i.e.,provisions made in how a student accesses anddemonstrates learning) for the child with APDhave become easier to obtain in the school set-ting with the passing of the Individuals withDisabilities Education Act (IDEA), the Ameri-cans with Disabilities Act (ADA) and the Sec-tion 504 of the Rehabilitation Act. Childrenwith APD who are not eligible under IDEA,may still be eligible for modifications and ac-commodations under Section 504 and ADA.Examples of accommodations that have beenrecommended for children with APD includethe use of a note-taker, visual supplements, studyguides and pre-teaching, peer partners, ad-justed pace of instruction, repetition of ideas,and reduced language level. Examples of mod-ifications that have been recommended forchildren with APD include reducing the diffi-culty of the material, shortening assignments,giving alternative assignments, and using analternative grading system. See the article byFerre (this issue, pages 319–326) for a discus-sion of this topic.

In 1997, Chermak and Musiek24 devel-oped a comprehensive management approachto APD to address the range of listening andlearning deficits experienced by children withAPD (Table 1). The intervention was a combi-nation of auditory training, and metalinguisticand metacognitive strategies designed to in-crease the scope and use of auditory and cen-tral resources. The auditory training portionfocused on detection, discrimination, vigilance,binaural listening, and interhemispheric trans-fer.The metalinguistic strategies focused on dis-course cohesion devices, vocabulary building,segmentation and auditory discrimination, andprosody. The metacognitive strategies focusedon understanding task demands, appropriatelyallocating attention, identifying important partsof a message, self-monitoring, self-question-ing, and deployment of debugging strategies.

The proliferation of the affordable personalcomputer during the last two decades has per-

Page 47: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

MANAGEMENT APPROACHES TO APT/WERTZ ET AL 283

Table 1 Strategies and Techniques for Management of Auditory Processing Disorders

Functional Deficit Strategies Techniques

Distractibility/inattention Increase signal-to-noise ALD/FM system; acoustic modifications;ratio preferential seating

Poor memory Metalanguage Chunking, verbal chaining, mnemonics,rehearsal, paraphrasing, summarizing

Right hemisphere activation Imagery, drawingExternal aids Notebooks, calendars

Restricted vocabulary Improve closure Contextual derivation of word meaningCognitive inflexibility Diversify cognitive style Top-down (deductive) and bottom-up

(predominantly analytic (inductive) processing, inferential reasoning,or predominantly questioning, critical thinkingconceptual)

Poor listening Induce formal schema to Recognize and explain connectivescomprehension aid organization, integration, (additives; causal; adversative; temporal)

and prediction and patterns of parallelism and correlative pairs (not only/but also; neither/nor)

Maximize visual and Substitutions for notetakingauditory summation

Reading, spelling, and Enhance multisensory Phonemic analysis and segmentationlistening problems integration

Maladaptive behaviors Assertiveness and cognitive Self-control, self-monitoring, self-evaluation,(passive, hyperactive, behavior modification self-instruction, problem solvingimpulsive)

Poor motivation Attribution retraining, internal Failure confrontation, attributionlocus of control to factors under control

Adapted from Chermak and Musiek,30 with permission.

mitted a new strategy for APD management.Computer software programs are now availableand no doubt more will be developed that offera variety of APD-specific therapies. Whilemany habilitation techniques developed prior tothe widespread availability of computers haveinvolved frequent and often intensive patient-therapist interaction, computer programs offerthe audiologist a different treatment option. Be-cause many families own computers, softwareprograms can be used in the home setting with-out the need for direct specialist supervision. In-deed, most software programs currently avail-able offer products directly to the consumer.Earobics™ (Cognitive Concepts, Evanston,IL25) and Fast ForWord™ (Scientific Learning,Berkley, CA26) are two such computer-basedsoftware patient-interaction programs. FastForWord provides a variety of software pack-ages for different patient populations, addressingseveral auditory-related components including

pitch pattern, temporal processing, workingmemory, as well as reading and language skills.Similarly, Earobics provides several softwareoptions for patients of different skill levels andages. Earobics programs engage the child in avariety of activities from phonologic awarenessto language comprehension. The two programsmentioned are by no means the only softwarepackages available, but at the time of this publi-cation they do garner a great deal of market at-tention. See also the articles by Musiek, Shinnand Hare, and by Phillips (this issue, pages263–276, and pages 251–263, respectively).

CONCLUSIONS

In reviewing the history of the management ofAPD in children, the paucity of empirical dataregarding efficacy becomes obvious. Demon-

Page 48: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

284 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

strating treatment efficacy requires document-ing that an important change occurred in achild’s performance due to the treatment ratherthan from maturation or some uncontrolled orunknown factor. Efficacy of treatment in APDshould be a high priority for future research.Indeed, the report of the 2000 ConsensusConference on APDs in Children27 identifiedamong future research needs:

• the relationship between APD test outcomesand management strategies,

• outcomes of early intervention for APD,• the relative efficacy of intervention approaches

at various ages (p. 472).

Some have questioned the 2000 Con-sensus Conference’s emphasis on differentialdiagnosis of APD. Katz et al28 stated: “the audi-ologist’s most valuable role is guiding the man-agement of the child with APD” and “For chil-dren with APD, our goal is to address theirlearning and communication difficulties ratherthan to determine if pathological/physiologicalauditory variations are present” (page 17). Whilewe wholeheartedly agree that audiologists mustplay an important role in the management ofAPD, we also concur with Jerger and Musiek,29

who emphasized that “appropriate interventionmust derive from accurate diagnosis” (p. 19).Indeed, the increased specificity in manage-ment strategies within recent years appears tobe a direct outgrowth of more precise methodsfor description, definition, and diagnosis ofspecific auditory processes. (See the articles byFerre and by Bellis, this issue, pages 319–326,and pages 287–296, respectively).

ABBREVIATIONS

ADA Americans with Disabilities ActAPD auditory processing disorderCAPD central auditory processing disorderFM frequency-modulatedIDEA Individuals with Disabilities Education

ActSNR signal-to-noise-ratio

REFERENCES

1. Myklebust HR. Auditory Disorders in Children: AManual for Differential Diagnosis. New York:Grune & Stratton; 1954

2. Jerger J, Jerger S. Clinical validity of central audi-tory tests. Scand Audiol 1975;4:147–163

3. Williford JA, Bilger JM. Auditory perception withlearning disabilities. In: Katz J, ed. Handbook ofClinical Audiology, 2nd ed. Baltimore, MD:Williams & Wilkins; 1978:410–425

4. Newby HA. Audiology, 4th ed. Englewood Cliffs,NJ: Prentice-Hall; 1979

5. Davis H, Silverman SR. Hearing and Deafness, 3rded. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston; 1970

6. Keith RW, ed. Central Auditory Dysfunction.New York: Grune and Stratton; 1977

7. Northern J, Downs M. Hearing in Children. Balti-more, MD: Williams & Wilkins; 1991

8. Rees NS. Auditory processing factors in languagedisorders: the view from Procruste’s bed. J SpeechHear Disord 1973;38:304–315

9. Bloom L, Lahey M. Language Development andLanguage Disorders. New York: Wiley; 1978

10. Lindamood C, Lindamood P. The Lindamood Au-ditory Test of Conceptualization (LAC). Boston,MA: Teaching Resources Corp.; 1971

11. Orton ST. Reading, Writing, and Speech Problemsin Children. New York: W.W. Norton; 1937

12. Sloan C. Treating Auditory Processing Difficultiesin Children. San Diego, CA: Singular; 1991

13. American Speech-Language-Hearing Associa-tion. Issues in Central Auditory Processing Disor-ders: A Report from the ASHA Ad Hoc Commit-tee on Central Auditory Processing. Rockville,MD: American Speech-Language-Hearing Asso-ciation; 1992

14. Kelly DA. Central Auditory Processing Disorder:Strategies for Use with Children and Adoles-cents. San Antonio, TX: Communication SkillBuilders; 1995

15. American Speech-Language-Hearing Associa-tion. Management Guidelines for Central Au-ditory Processing Disorders. Rockville, MD:American Speech-Language-Hearing Associa-tion; 1997

16. Crandell C, Smaldino J. The importance of roomacoustics. In: Tyler RS, Schum DJ, eds. AssistiveDevices for Persons with Hearing Impairment.Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon; 1995

17. Bellas T. Assessment and Management of CentralAuditory Processing Disorders in the EducationalSetting. From Science to Practice. San Diego, CA:Singular; 1996:128

18. Finitzo-Heiber T. Classroom acoustics. In: RoeserR, Downs, eds. Auditory Disorders in SchoolChildren, 2nd ed. 1988:221–233

Page 49: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

MANAGEMENT APPROACHES TO APT/WERTZ ET AL 285

19. Crandell C. Page ten: utilizing sound field FM am-plification in the educational setting. Hear J 1998;51:10–19

20. Ferre J. CAP tips. Ed Audiol Rev 1999;16:28–3221. Katz J, Wilde L. Auditory processing disorders. In:

Katz J, ed. Handbook of Clinical Audiology, 4th ed.Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins; 1994:490–502

22. Hall JW III. CAPD in Y2K: an introduction to au-diologic assessment and management. Hear J 1996;52:35–42

23. Hall JW III, Mueller HG III. Audiologists’ DeskReference, vol. I. San Diego, CA: Singular

24. Chermak G, Musiek F. Central Auditory Process-ing Disorders: New Perspectives. San Diego, CA:Singular; 1997

25. Cognitive Concepts. www.Earobics.com 1998

26. Scientific Learning. www.Scientificlearning.com1998

27. Jerger J, Musiek FE. Report of consensus confer-ence on the diagnosis of auditory processing disor-ders in school-aged children. J Am Acad Audiol2000;11:467–474

28. Katz J, Johnson CD, Brandner S, et al. Clinical andresearch concerns regarding the 2000 APD con-sensus report and recommendations. AudiologyToday 2002;11:14–17

29. Jerger J, Musiek FE. On the diagnosis of audi-tory processing disorder. Audiology Today 2002;11:19–21

30. Chermak GD, Musief FE. Managing central audi-tory processing disorders in children and youth.Am J Audiol 1992;1:61–65

Page 50: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders
Page 51: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

287

Management of Auditory Processing Disorders; Editor in Chief, Catherine V. Palmer, Ph.D.; Guest Editor, Gail D.Chermak, Ph.D. Seminars in Hearing, volume 23, number 4, 2002. Address for correspondence and reprint requests: TeriJames Bellis, Ph.D., Department of Communication Disorders, University of South Dakota, 414 E. Clark Street,Vermillion, SD 57069. E-mail: [email protected]. 1Department of Communication Disorders, University of South Dakota,Vermillion, South Dakota. Copyright © 2002 by Thieme Medical Publishers, Inc., 333 Seventh Avenue, New York, NY10001, USA. Tel: +1(212) 584-4662. 00734-0451,p;2002,23,04,287,296,ftx,en;sih00222x.

Developing Deficit-Specific InterventionPlans for Individuals with AuditoryProcessing DisordersTeri James Bellis, Ph.D.1

ABSTRACT

Intervention for auditory processing disorders (APD) should ariselogically from the nature of the individual’s auditory deficit and how thatdeficit relates to functional difficulties and behavioral sequelae. Althoughmany management and remediation techniques have been proposed fortreating APD, the relative efficacy of any given approach will depend on itsappropriateness to the specific APD in question. This article will addressmethods of customizing APD intervention based on diagnostic findingsand functional deficit profiles.

KEYWORDS: Auditory processing disorders, intervention,management, environmental modifications, compensatory strategies,auditory training

Learning Outcomes: After reading this article, readers will be able to: (1) Discuss the rationale underlyingdeficit-specific intervention for APD, (2) Identify methods of customizing APD intervention based on functionaldeficit profiles, and (3) Delineate needed areas of research in deficit-specific intervention for APD.

Auditory Processing Consensus Development,1these fundamental skills or processes includesound localization and lateralization, auditorydiscrimination, auditory pattern recognition,temporal aspects of audition, and auditory per-formance with degraded and/or competingacoustic signals. As such, APD is best viewedas a sensory-based input deficit in one or more

The utility of deficit-specific interventionfor auditory processing disorders (APD) is basedon three primary assumptions. First is the as-sumption that certain basic auditory skills orprocesses underlie more complex listening,learning, and communication abilities. Accord-ing to the American Speech-Language-HearingAssociation (ASHA) Task Force on Central

Page 52: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

288 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

of these processes that then can lead to diffi-culty in higher-order, more complex behaviorssuch as listening, language use, and learning.

A second assumption underlying the util-ity of deficit-specific intervention for APD isthat the capability exists for identifying thoseauditory processes that are dysfunctional in agiven individual through the use of diagnostictests of central auditory function. Toward thisend, Domitz and colleagues2–4 conducted a se-ries of studies to investigate the relationship be-tween several commonly used central auditorytests and the fundamental auditory processesidentified in the 1996 ASHA consensus state-ment.1 Their results indicated that no singletest was sufficient to diagnose APD. Rather,the use of a combination of tests that assessmultiple auditory processes was indicated.

A final assumption important to the utilityof deficit-specific intervention for APD is that,once identified, remediation of the underlying,deficient auditory process or processes will facili-tate improvement in those higher-order, morecomplex functional ability areas with which agiven individual is experiencing difficulties.Thus,from a theoretical perspective, deficit-specific in-tervention for APD follows a fundamental med-ical model in which the primary underlyingcause of the disease or disorder is identified andthen treated in a bottom-up fashion. At the sametime, the symptoms of the disorder—or the lan-guage, learning, and communication sequelae,including secondary metacognitive or metalin-guistic deficits—are addressed through top-downintervention techniques. Appropriate interven-tion for APD is dependent on the accurate iden-tification and diagnosis of the nature of theunderlying deficit(s) as well as on the determina-tion of how the deficit(s) affects the individual inhis or her daily life.

Readers should be cautioned that, how-ever logical this approach to APD interventionseems, there is a paucity of empirical evidenceto support these assumptions. The causal link-age between auditory processing deficits andlanguage, learning, or communication disordershas been called into question by several re-searchers.5–7 The sensitivity and specificity ofcentral auditory tests in current clinical use havebeen criticized, in particular, for the lack of agold standard for validation of these tests and

the influence of nonauditory factors, such asattention, memory, and language, on test per-formance.6–9 Finally, data to support the effi-cacy of specific treatment approaches for APDare in large part lacking at the present time.

Notwithstanding these caveats, logic dic-tates that the most efficacious approach to APDintervention would be one that arises logicallyfrom the specific auditory deficit area(s) identi-fied and the functional, behavioral symptomsexhibited by the individual. Outcomes of diag-nostic testing, combined with information frommultidisciplinary evaluations of language, learn-ing, and cognitive function, provide a frame-work for guiding the intervention effort.8,10–14

In this manner, a management program can bedeveloped that is appropriate for the individual,and the inevitable wasted time that is often theresult of using a shotgun approach to treatmentcan be avoided.

COMPONENTS OF APDINTERVENTION

Comprehensive intervention for APD shouldseek to remediate the underlying auditory defi-cit(s) as well as improve the individual’s abilityto function in real-world communication andlearning environments. Any management planfor APD should include elements of each of thefollowing three primary components.10–12,14

Environmental Modifications to

Improve Acoustic Clarity and

Enhance Learning/Listening

Acoustic-based environmental modificationscan include the use of assistive listening—either personal or soundfield—devices, archi-tectural interventions to reduce reverberationand improve signal-to-noise ratio, and reduc-tion or removal of mechanical or other com-peting noise sources from within and outsidethe room.15 It is universally accepted that alllisteners perform better in an environment thatfosters acoustic clarity and desirable signal-to-noise ratios. However, for some individualswith auditory deficits, particularly those thatreduce the intrinsic redundancy and clarity of

Page 53: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

DEFICIT-SPECIFIC INTERVENTION/BELLIS 289

the acoustic signal, extrinsic redundancy andacoustic clarity of the incoming message willbe far more critical than it will for others.

Other environmental modifications mayinclude teacher- or speaker-based interventions.However, these modifications are not equallybeneficial to all individuals with APD. There-fore, each environmental modification recom-mended as part of a comprehensive APD in-tervention plan should be examined carefullyto ensure that it is appropriate for the individ-ual in question. For additional information re-garding environmental modifications, readersare referred to the articles by Ferre and byRosenberg (this issue, pages 319–326 andpages 309–318, respectively).

Compensatory Strategies to

Strengthen Higher-Order Top-

Down Processing Skills

The presence of APD often is associated withsecondary deficits in attention, memory, learn-ing, and other higher-order cognitive and re-lated areas.1,8–9,13 Deficits or weaknesses in thesenonmodality-specific, global domains can exac-erbate the impact of an APD on daily function.Any comprehensive intervention plan for APDshould address metacognitive and metalinguis-tic factors in a top-down fashion. Compensatorystrategies training is not designed to remediatethe underlying disorder, but rather to strengthenhigher-order skills that can impact auditoryfunctioning and to teach the individual to be-come an active participant in his or her own lis-tening and comprehension success.8,13–14,16

Metalinguistic strategies to benefit spokenlanguage comprehension can consist of trainingin the rules of language, formal and informalschema induction, using context to build vocabu-lary, and a variety of other activities.These activi-ties may be most appropriate for those individu-als with auditory deficits that affect auditoryclosure, or who exhibit language-based disordersthat either co-exist with or arise from APD.Metacognitive and metamemory strategies aredesigned to enhance problem-solving skills, im-prove the memory trace, and encourage the lis-tener to take responsibility for his or her own lis-tening success. These strategies may be most

effective for those individuals who exhibit audi-tory memory difficulties, or who have difficultyindependently analyzing and solving compre-hension problems in real-world settings. Theyalso are useful for children and adults with APDwho, over time, have become passive listeners andwho feel powerless to effect any change in theirlistening success. For an in-depth description ofcompensatory strategies, readers are referred toChermak,13 and Chermak and Musiek.8

Direct Remediation Techniques

The purpose of direct remediation activities is tomaximize neuroplasticity and improve auditoryperformance by changing the way the brain pro-cesses auditory information.8,10–14,16 Dependingon the specific deficit present, direct therapy maybe targeted toward phoneme discrimination ac-tivities (and concomitant speech-to-print skills);dichotic listening training in which the inten-sity levels for each ear are gradually adjustedto improve the listener’s performance in theweaker (usually left) ear; localization/lateraliza-tion training; enhancing perception of stress,rhythm, and intonational aspects of speech; ac-tivities requiring temporal resolution or integra-tion; perception of acoustic patterns; or multi-modality interhemispheric stimulation activities,among others. For an update and review ofauditory training techniques and neuroplasticity,readers are referred to the articles by Musiek etal, and by Chermak and Musiek (this issue, pages263–276 and pages 297–308, respectively).

CUSTOMIZING APDINTERVENTION PLANS

When designing an individualized, deficit-specific intervention plan for APD, severalsteps are required.

Step One: Identifying the

Auditory Deficit(s)

Programming deficit-specific intervention plansfor APD requires the identification of the spe-cific auditory process(es) that is dysfunctional

Page 54: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

290 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

using diagnostic central auditory tests. Accord-ing to the diagnostic construct suggested bySchow and colleagues,2–4 measurable behavioralauditory processes include:

• Binaural separation (BS): This is the abilityto attend to one ear while, at the same time,ignoring a competing signal in the oppositeear. BS can be assessed through the use of di-chotic measures that require directed reportof one ear, such as the Competing SentencesTest.17

• Binaural integration (BI): BI also reflects au-ditory performance with competing acousticsignals, and can be assessed through dichotictests that require integration and report ofstimuli directed to both ears, such as the Di-chotic Digits Test.18

• Auditory pattern/temporal ordering (APTO):APTO requires auditory discrimination offrequency and/or duration differences, order-ing or sequencing of stimuli, and specific tem-poral processes. In addition, depending on thetask, APTO may require interhemispheric in-tegration if the subjects are required to verballylabel nonverbal tonal patterns.Tests that assessAPTO include Frequency Patterns19 and Du-ration Patterns.20

• Monaural separation/closure (MSC): MSCunderlies performance with degraded signals,such as filtered or time-compressed speech,as well as monaural performance with ipsi-lateral competing acoustic signals.

• Auditory discrimination: Auditory discrimi-nation, or the ability to tell when two stimuliare different, may be thought of as a com-plex auditory behavioral skill that is fun-damental to all of the previously discussedauditory phenomena. It can be assessed in-dependently through the use of differencelimens for frequency, intensity, duration, orspeech stimuli, as well as via speech-soundand word discrimination tests, many of whichare key components of the speech-languagepathologist’s and/or audiologist’s diagnosticarmamentarium.

• Sound localization and lateralization: Theability to localize a sound source and similarskills also are complex behavioral processesthat underlie many of the previously men-tioned auditory abilities. One measure that

taps into lateralization at the brainstem levelis the Masking Level Difference. Additionalmeasures of brainstem-level binaural inter-action have been designed; however, theirsensitivity and clinical utility is question-able.10 Localization of sound sources relieson far more than just binaural interaction inthe brainstem. Additional research is neededto determine how best to measure localiza-tion, lateralization, and binaural interaction.

• Other temporal aspects of audition: Tests oftemporal processing (other than temporal or-dering mentioned previously) may includemeasures of temporal resolution (e.g., gapdetection, difference threshold for duration),temporal masking and temporal integrationor summation. Temporal processes are criti-cal for a wide variety of auditory behaviors andabilities, including discrimination of speechand nonspeech signals, perception of prosodicelements of speech, and localization/lateral-ization. Therefore, temporal processing maybe thought of as underlying all of the previ-ously mentioned behavioral auditory pro-cesses. However, with the exception of theRandom Gap Detection Test,21 commerciallyavailable tests of temporal processing arelargely lacking at present. For a discussion oftemporal aspects of audition, readers are re-ferred to Phillips.22

The auditory processes discussed in thissection represent all six of the auditory phe-nomena delineated in the ASHA 1996 state-ment of central auditory processes.1 As can beseen, some can be directly assessed at the pres-ent time, whereas others represent more com-plex abilities requiring further investigation toisolate and measure. We are able currently toidentify many underlying dysfunctional pro-cesses for the purpose of programming deficit-specific intervention using existing central au-ditory tests.

Step Two: Examining the Individual’s

Functional Difficulties and Sequelae

The second step in customizing APD inter-vention is to determine how the underlyingauditory deficit relates to (or co-exists with)

Page 55: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

DEFICIT-SPECIFIC INTERVENTION/BELLIS 291

functional learning, language, and communica-tion sequelae in the individual. To this end,Bellis and Ferre10–12,14 developed a model ofAPD in which information from central audi-tory and multidisciplinary evaluations is usedto create functional deficit profiles for both di-agnostic and intervention purposes. Each ofthese profiles yields specific, neurophysiologi-cally tenable patterns of findings on diagnostictests of central auditory function as well as onmeasures of cognition, communication, andlearning. Briefly, the three primary profiles ofthe Bellis/Ferre model are:

• Auditory Decoding Deficit: Characterizedprimarily by a deficit in MSC, Auditory De-coding Deficit represents dysfunction in theprimary (usually left) cerebral hemisphere.Additional auditory deficits often are seen inauditory discrimination of minimally contrast-ing speech sounds and fundamental temporalprocesses, including gap detection. Diagnos-tically, Auditory Decoding Deficit results inpoor performance on monaural tests of de-graded speech and binaural or right-ear defi-cits on dichotic speech tests. Secondary deficitsmay be seen in the areas of BI and/or BS, asdichotic tests inherently reduce the extrinsicredundancy of the speech signal throughcompetition. Secondary sequelae associatedwith Auditory Decoding Deficit include sig-nificant difficulty hearing in noise, readingand spelling difficulties usually confined tophonological decoding (or word attack) skills,poor vocabulary and other language-basedconcerns, auditory fatigue, and better perfor-mance with visual or multimodality augmen-tation.

• Prosodic Deficit: Also referred to as right-hemisphere APD, Prosodic Deficit is char-acterized by a deficit in APTO, particularlyas related to perception and recognition oftonal patterns. Frequency, duration, and in-tensity difference limens also may be ele-vated. Central auditory diagnostic findingsinclude poor performance on duration and/orfrequency pattern testing in both hummingand labeling report conditions combinedwith left-ear deficit on dichotic speech tests.Although this latter finding may suggest thatindividuals with Prosodic Deficit also exhibit

BS/BI dysfunction, typically hearing in noiseand other binaural abilities are good becauseof preserved auditory closure, discrimination,performance with degraded acoustic signals,and related abilities. Secondary sequelaeoften include difficulties in perceiving stress,rhythm, and intonational patterns of speechleading to poor comprehension of commu-nicative intent and frequent misunderstand-ings or miscommunications; poor pragmat-ics, or social communication skills; and poormusic or singing skills. Associated sequelaecan include reading and spelling difficultiesconfined to right-hemisphere-based, Gestaltpatterning, sight-word abilities; difficultieswith sequencing information; and deficits inother abilities attributed to the right hemi-sphere, including mathematics calculation andvisual-spatial tasks.

• Integration Deficit: The result of inefficientcommunication between the two cerebralhemispheres, Integration Deficit is primarilycharacterized by deficits in BI and BS. Cen-tral auditory testing yields a pattern of left-ear deficit on dichotic speech tasks, combinedwith poor performance on tests of temporalpatterning in the verbal report condition only.Sound localization abilities also may be poor.Associated sequelae may include difficultyacross modalities with any task requiringinterhemispheric integration. Separating asound source from a competing backgroundof noise may be problematic, as may associat-ing a speech sound with the orthographicsymbol on the page during reading or spell-ing, linking the prosodic and linguistic ele-ments of speech to arrive at an understand-ing of the entire message (especially whenforms such as sarcasm are employed), and ac-tivities requiring bimanual or bipedal coordi-nation. The addition of visual or multimodal-ity cues often confuses, rather than clarifies,the message for individuals with IntegrationDeficit.

Readers are cautioned that these three pri-mary profiles are not the only manner in whichAPD can manifest itself. Secondary profilesthat affect language, executive function, andplanning more overtly also are possible.10–12,14

Isolated deficits that impact performance on

Page 56: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

292 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

just one diagnostic test or in just one auditoryarea also can exist. These profiles can occursingularly or in combination; however, oneprofile is typically primary and the combina-tions usually arise from anatomically adjacentbrain areas. If a child or adult exhibits deficitsin all auditory processes assessed, or a combi-nation of all three profiles, one should look tomore global, higher-order cognitive, memory,or related dysfunction rather than APD as aprimary condition affecting listening and re-lated abilities.

Step Three: Selecting the Appropriate

Management Strategies and

Remediation Techniques

Once the underlying auditory deficit(s) andthe secondary or associated functional difficul-ties are determined, appropriate environmentalmodifications, compensatory strategies, anddirect treatment options can be selected thatwill address the individual’s APD in a deficit-specific manner. Intervention for APD shouldarise logically from the nature of the auditorydeficit(s) and the behavioral sequelae the indi-vidual is experiencing. Table 1 outlines someappropriate management and intervention tech-niques for each of the above-mentioned per-formance deficit profiles. It should be empha-sized that the information contained in thistable is not intended to provide a cookbook ap-proach to APD intervention, but rather toserve as a general guide to the types of activi-ties and modifications that may be indicatedfor these specific profiles.

Step Four: Monitoring

Intervention Efficacy

Although a discussion of documentation oftreatment outcomes is not within the scope ofthis paper, it is critical that the efficacy of allintervention techniques be monitored on anongoing basis and alterations to the individual-ized program made as needed. For additionalinformation regarding monitoring treatmentefficacy, readers are referred to the articles by

Putter-Katz et al, and by Jirsa (this issue, pages357–364 and pages 349–356, respectively).

SUMMARY AND DIRECTIONS FORFURTHER RESEARCH

This article has described the rationale under-lying deficit-specific intervention for APD andmethods of developing individualized manage-ment and intervention plans. The approachsuggested here is largely theoretical in nature,with intervention recommendations arising log-ically from the specific auditory deficit(s) andfunctional behavioral sequelae present in a givenindividual. However, readers should be awarethat a great deal of further research is neededto support empirically the approach and activi-ties described in this article.

Although it generally is accepted that tomaximize neuroplasticity, direct remediationactivities for APD should be frequent, intense,and challenging, the minimum amount of timeand effort needed to effect a maximum changein central auditory function has yet to be iden-tified. Some therapy programs recommend sev-eral weeks of intense, daily activity. However,Tremblay and colleagues23 have shown thatsignificant changes in behavioral perceptualabilities and neural representation of speechcan occur after just five days of directed train-ing, and can generalize to other, untrained stim-uli. Because therapy is costly in terms of bothmoney and time, research is needed to deter-mine the frequency and intensity of therapythat is necessary but sufficient to achieve de-sired results.

Moreover, we may find that deficit-specifictraining is not as crucial to, nor as predictableof, functional outcomes as might be anticipated.Gillam and colleagues24–25 assessed languagechanges in children with language impairmentusing two completely different, computer-assisted language intervention programs thatused different types of auditory stimuli, em-ployed different language units, targeted differ-ent levels of language, and were designed to im-prove different aspects of language. Despitethese differences, they found astonishingly sim-ilar improvements following treatment. Because

Page 57: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

DEFICIT-SPECIFIC INTERVENTION/BELLIS 293

Ta

ble

1S

ele

cte

d M

an

ag

em

en

t a

nd

In

terv

en

tio

n T

ech

niq

ue

s f

or

Sp

ecif

ic A

ud

ito

ry D

efi

cit

Pro

file

s a

nd

Dy

sfu

ncti

on

al P

roce

sse

s

De

ficit

Pro

ce

ss(e

s)

En

vir

on

me

nta

lC

om

pe

nsa

tory

Dir

ect

Re

me

dia

tio

n

Pro

file

Aff

ecte

dM

od

ific

ati

on

sS

tra

teg

ies

Acti

vit

ies

Aud

itory

dec

odin

gM

SC

*Pr

efer

entia

l sea

ting,

ass

istiv

e A

udito

ry c

losu

re, v

ocab

ular

y P

hone

me

disc

rimin

atio

n,de

ficit

Spe

ech-

soun

d lis

teni

ng d

evic

e, p

rete

ach

build

ing,

pro

blem

-sol

ving

,te

mpo

ral r

esol

utio

n an

d/or

disc

rimin

atio

n†ne

w in

form

atio

n, r

epet

ition

,ac

tive

liste

ning

, sch

ema

indu

ctio

nin

tegr

atio

n, p

hono

logi

cal

Tem

pora

l pro

cess

es†

visu

al a

ugm

enta

tion

awar

enes

s, w

ord

atta

ckB

S/B

I†(s

peec

h-to

-prin

t) s

kills

Pros

odic

def

icit

AP

TO*

Pla

cem

ent

with

“an

imat

ed”

Mem

ory

enha

ncem

ent,

sch

ema

Perc

eptio

n of

pro

sody

,N

onsp

eech

-sou

ndte

ache

r, ac

oust

ic c

larit

y le

ssin

duct

ion,

use

of

pros

ody

and

tem

pora

l pat

tern

ing,

disc

rimin

atio

n†cr

itica

lso

cial

judg

men

tpr

agm

atic

sIn

tegr

atio

n de

ficit

BS

/BI*

Aco

ustic

enh

ance

men

ts, a

void

use

M

etal

ingu

istic

and

met

acog

nitiv

eIn

terh

emis

pher

ic t

rans

fer,

Loca

lizat

ion†

of m

ulti-

mod

ality

cue

sst

rate

gies

as

need

edbi

naur

al s

kills

usi

ng d

icho

tic

stim

uli,

soun

d lo

caliz

atio

n

*Prim

ary

defic

it ar

ea; †

seco

ndar

y or

pos

sibl

e de

ficit

area

.M

SC

, mon

aura

l sep

arat

ion/

clos

ure;

BS,

bin

aura

l sep

arat

ion;

BI,

bina

ural

inte

grat

ion;

AP

TO, a

udito

ry p

atte

rn/t

empo

ral o

rder

ing.

Page 58: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

294 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

the efficacy of intervention for APD should beassessed on the basis of functional abilities ratherthan improved auditory skills or processes, it ispossible that intensity of treatment, repetitiveauditory stimulation, and engagement and mo-tivation of the subject are more essential to effi-cacy and outcome than is the specific auditoryskill trained.8,24–25 Only further investigationwill help to elucidate this issue. See also the ar-ticles by Phillips, and Chermak and Musiek(this issue, pages 251–262 and pages 297–308,respectively).

Finally, as discussed earlier, more data areneeded to fully support the basic assumptionsupon which the rationale for deficit-specificAPD intervention is based. Data remain scarceto validate the conceptualization of APD as anauditory input disorder that leads to higher-order language, learning, and communicationdifficulties; that can be identified through di-agnostic central auditory tests; and that can betreated so as to foster improvements in audi-tory and related behavior. Until such time asthese data are obtained, the clinical utility ofthe APD diagnosis will remain limited and thebenefits of deficit-specific intervention will re-main uncertain.

ABBREVIATIONS

APD auditory processing disorderAPTO auditory pattern temporal orderingASHA American Speech-Language-Hearing

AssociationBI binaural integrationBS binaural separationMSC monaural separation/closure

REFERENCES

1. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.Central auditory processing: current status of re-search and implications for clinical practice. Am JAudiol 1996;5:41–54

2. Schow R, Chermak GD. Implications from factoranalysis for central auditory processing disorders.Am J Audiol 1999;8:137–142

3. Domitz DM, Schow RL. A new CAPD battery—multiple auditory processing assessment (MAPA):

factor analysis and comparisons with SCAN. Am JAudiol 2000;9:101–111

4. Schow RL, Seikel JA, Chermak GD, Berent M.Central auditory processes and test measures:ASHA 1996 revisited. Am J Audiol 2000;9:1–6

5. Rees N. Auditory processing factors in languagedisorders: the view from Procrustes’ bed. J SpeechHear Disord 1973;38:304–315

6. McFarland DJ, Cacace AT. Modality specificity asa criterion for diagnosing central auditory process-ing disorder. Am J Audiol 1995;4:36–48

7. Cacace AT, McFarland DJ. Central auditory pro-cessing disorder in school-aged children: a criticalreview. J Speech Lang Hear Res 1998;41:355–373

8. Chermak GD, Musiek FE. Central Auditory Pro-cessing Disorders: New Perspectives. San Diego,CA: Singular; 1997

9. Jerger J, Musiek F. Report of the consensus confer-ence on the diagnosis of auditory processing disor-ders in school-aged children. J Am Acad Audiol2000;11:467–474

10. Bellis TJ. Assessment and Management of CentralAuditory Processing Disorders in the EducationalSetting: From Science to Practice. San Diego, CA:Singular; 1996

11. Bellis TJ, Ferre JM. Multidimensional approach tothe differential diagnosis of central auditory pro-cessing disorders in children. J Am Acad Audiol1999;10:319–328

12. Bellis TJ. Subprofiles of central auditory processingdisorders. Ed Audiol Rev 1999;2:9–14

13. Chermak GD. Managing central auditory process-ing disorders: megalinguistic and metacognitiveapproaches. Semin Hear 1998;19:379–392

14. Bellis TJ. When the Brain Can’t Hear: Unravelingthe Mystery of Auditory Processing Disorder. NewYork: Pocket Books; 2002

15. Technical Committee on Architectural Acoustics.Classroom Acoustics: A Resource for CreatingLearning Environments with Desirable ListeningConditions. Melville, NY: Acoustical Society ofAmerica; 2000

16. Musiek FM. Habilitation and management of au-ditory processing disorders: overview of selectedprocedures. J Am Acad Audiol 1999;10:329–342

17. Willeford JA, Burleigh JM. Sentence procedures incentral testing. In: J. Katz, ed. Handbook of Clini-cal Audiology, 4th ed. Baltimore, MD: Williams &Wilkins; 1994:256–268

18. Musiek FE. Assessment of central auditory asym-metry: the dichotic digit test revisited. Ear Hearing1983;4:79–83

19. Ptacek PH, Pinheiro ML. Pattern reversal in theperception of noise and tone patterns. J Acoust SocAm 1971;49:493–498

20. Pinheiro ML, Musiek FE. Sequencing and tempo-ral ordering in the auditory system. In: Pinheiro

Page 59: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

DEFICIT-SPECIFIC INTERVENTION/BELLIS 295

ML, Musiek FE, eds. Assessment of Central Au-ditory Dysfunction: Foundations and ClinicalCorrelates. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins;1985:219–238

21. Keith RW. Random Gap Detection Test. St Louis,MO: Auditec; 2000

22. Phillips DP. Auditory gap detection, perceptualchannels, and temporal resolution in speech per-ception. J Am Acad Audiol 1999;10:343–354

23. Tremblay K, Kraus N, Carrell TD, McGee T. Cen-tral auditory system plasticity: generalization to

novel stimuli following listening training. J AcoustSoc Am 1997;102:3762–3773

24. Gillam RB, Crofford JA, Gale MA, Hoffman LM.Language change following computer-assisted lan-guage instruction with Fast ForWord or LaureateLearning Systems software. Am J Speech LangPathol 2001;10:231–247

25. Gillam RB, Loeb DF, Friel-Patti S. Looking back:a summary of five exploratory studies of Fast For-Word. Am J Speech Lang Pathol 2001;10:269–273

Page 60: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders
Page 61: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

297

Management of Auditory Processing Disorders; Editor in Chief, Catherine V. Palmer, Ph.D.; Guest Editor, Gail D.Chermak, Ph.D. Seminars in Hearing, volume 23, number 4, 2002. Address for correspondence and reprint requests: GailD. Chermak, Ph.D., Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, Washington State University, Pullman, WA99164–2420. E-mail: [email protected]. 1Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, Washington State University,Pullman, Washington; 2Department of Communication Sciences and Otolaryngology, University of Connecticut, Storrs,Connecticut. Copyright © 2002 by Thieme Medical Publishers, Inc., 333 Seventh Avenue, New York, NY 10001, USA.Tel: +1(212) 584-4662. 00734-0451,p;2002,23,04,297,308,ftx,en;sih00223x.

Auditory Training: Principles andApproaches for Remediating and ManagingAuditory Processing DisordersGail D. Chermak, Ph.D.,1 and Frank E. Musiek, Ph.D.2

ABSTRACT

Recent reports suggest that auditory training (AT) can serve asa valuable intervention tool, particularly for individuals with languageimpairment and auditory processing disorder (APD). This article sug-gests a continuum of AT approaches, including those that do not requiremajor instrumentation and can be implemented by speech-languagepathologists and audiologists through their clinical practices. AT ap-proaches are categorized as formal and informal. Formal AT is con-ducted by the professional in a controlled setting. Informal AT can beconducted as part of a home or school management program for APD.Formal AT employs acoustically controlled, bottom-up tasks using tonesand speech elements, as well as language-based or top-down tasks. Infor-mal AT is designed to improve auditory perceptual skills through lan-guage-based, predominantly top-down tasks. Coupling formal with in-formal AT should maximize treatment efficacy as skills are practicedtoward mastery and automatism in real world settings that establishfunctional significance and provide repeated opportunities for general-ization of skills.

KEYWORDS: Auditory processing, auditory processing disorder,auditory training

Learning Outcomes: Upon completion of this article, the reader will be able to (1) differentiate informal fromformal auditory training approaches, (2) identify at least three informal auditory training exercises and the audi-tory process(es) targeted, and (3) identify at least three formal auditory training exercises and the auditory pro-cess(es) targeted.

Page 62: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

298 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

and exercises we suggest reflect our opinion,clinical experience, and our interpretation ofthe literature reviewed in this article.

AUDITORY PROCESSINGDISORDER

APD is a complex and heterogeneous group ofdisorders usually associated with a range of lis-tening and learning deficits despite normalhearing sensitivity.18–21 Jerger and Musiek statedthat APD “may be broadly defined as a deficitin the processing of information that is specificto the auditory modality.”21 More specifically,Chermak and Musiek described APD as “adeficit observed in one or more of the centralauditory processes responsible for generatingthe auditory evoked potentials and the follow-ing behaviors: sound localization and lateral-ization; auditory discrimination; auditory pat-tern recognition; temporal aspects of auditionincluding, temporal resolution, temporal mask-ing, temporal integration, and temporal order-ing; auditory performance with competingacoustic signals; and auditory performance withdegraded acoustic signals.”19 Given the rangeof listening and learning deficits associatedwith APD, AT must be seen as only one com-ponent of a comprehensive management ap-proach to improving auditory processing. Thereader is referred to companion articles in thisissue, as well as the authors’ other work on thistopic for a thorough discussion of the range ofintervention approaches useful in managingAPD, including linguistic, metalinguistic andmetacognitive strategies, acoustic signal en-hancement, and environmental modifications.19

PRINCIPLES OF AUDITORYTRAINING

AT can be employed to improve auditory pro-cessing abilities in a number of clinical popula-tions presenting auditory deficits due to con-firmed CANS pathology (e.g., traumatic braininjury, aphasia, progressive neurodegenerativedisorders), as well as in cases of suspected CANS

Auditory training (AT) is designed toimprove the function of the auditory system inresolving acoustic signals. There has been re-newed interest in AT recently driven by thesubstantial body of literature demonstratingthe plasticity of the auditory system1–11 andfollowing reports that confirm the value of ATas an intervention tool, particularly for individ-uals with language impairment and auditoryprocessing disorder (APD).12–16 For an over-view of the neurobiology of the central audi-tory nervous system (CANS) and discussion ofthe role of plasticity in AT, see the articles byPhillips and Museik et al (this issue, pages251–262, and pages 263–276, respectively).

The purpose of this article is to describe acontinuum of AT approaches, including thosethat do not require major instrumentation, andcan be implemented by speech-language pathol-ogists and audiologists through their clinicalpractices. These approaches are designed pri-marily for children diagnosed with APD; how-ever, many exercises can be adapted for usewith adults and for clients with other disor-ders, including peripheral hearing loss (see thesection on Principles of Auditory Training, pre-sented later in this article). Given the scarcityof published information about AT, coupledwith the growing body of literature suggestingthe potential benefit of some types of AT forremediation and management of APD,5,8,12,14–17

it is crucial that audiologists and speech-languagepathologists undertake AT to ascertain the rela-tive efficacy of the various approaches and ex-ercises, including those outlined in this article.The approaches and tasks described in this ar-ticle follow from the authors’ theoretical frame-work18,19 and professional consensus20 and arein use in the authors’ clinics. Preliminary datareported by Musiek revealed improvements incentral auditory test scores, as well as improvedacademic and communicative performanceamong 15 children between the ages of 8 and13 years who underwent formal AT as describedhere.14 It must be emphasized, however, thatthe approaches we suggest are among a num-ber of possible approaches that ultimately maybe deemed efficacious for the treatment of APD.The principles we delineate and the approaches

Page 63: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

AUDITORY TRAINING FOR AUDITORY PROCESSING DISORDERS/CHERMAK, MUSIEK 299

pathology (e.g., learning disabilities, languageimpairment, attention deficit disorders). A com-prehensive audiologic evaluation, supplementedas needed with language and psychoeducationalevaluations, as well as feedback from parentsand teachers, must be conducted prior to im-plementing any AT program. These measuresshould be re-assessed periodically to ascertainprogress, redirect therapy if necessary, and ter-minate therapy if the client has attained themaximum benefit that can be expected. Gener-ally, younger subjects can be expected to bene-fit from AT to a greater degree due to neuralplasticity.22 Children whose auditory deficit isassociated with a neuromaturational lag can beexpected to benefit from intense AT as stimu-lation extends plasticity and the purported sen-sitive periods for learning, thereby maximizingthe potential for successful rehabilitative ef-forts.3,23,24 Similarly, individuals with auditorydeficits secondary to brain damage can profitfrom AT that stimulates and challenges dam-aged neural networks and induces neural reor-ganization.24 In either case, the degree of neu-ronal change and maturation is dependent onthe quality and consistency of stimulation.

Auditory maturation must be consideredin designing auditory tasks and establishingcriterion performance. Some of the AT tasksreviewed in this article are appropriate forpreschool-aged children (e.g., auditory dis-crimination, auditory directives, and readingaloud); other AT tasks demand greater cogni-tive sophistication and would be more appro-priately used with older children and adults(e.g., intensity and frequency training).

A variety of stimuli and tasks should beused to train auditory processes. If the clientpresents an auditory deficit restricted to a spe-cific auditory region or auditory process, a morenarrowly designed AT program might be ap-propriate. For example, subjects with both tem-poral processing deficits and auditory perfor-mance decrements for degraded acoustic signalsmight benefit from exercises requiring recogni-tion of time-compressed speech. In contrast,gap detection might be a more appropriate ex-ercise to specifically train temporal processingfor subjects presenting temporal processing

deficits in the absence of auditory performancedeficits for degraded signals. Tasks should bepresented systematically and graduated in diffi-culty to be challenging and hence motivating,but not overwhelming. We find that targetingaccuracy between 30 to 70% allows for suffi-cient practice and challenge while maintainingthe client’s motivation and on-task persistence.Nonetheless, the clinician should determine thecriterion appropriate for each client. Detectionand discrimination tasks should precede moredemanding psychoacoustic tasks requiring iden-tification, recognition, and production. We rec-ommend that clients attain a minimum of 70%accuracy before proceeding to the more de-manding tasks. To the extent possible, ATshould be conducted in an intensive manner,scheduling multiple sessions, perhaps 5 to 7 ses-sions weekly.14 Generally, exercises should bepresented at the client’s comfortable listeninglevel, although slightly louder presentations mayproduce more clear speech and ease the listeningtask.25

The ultimate goal of AT, and therefore thetrue measure of the efficacy of an AT program,is measured in functional abilities (i.e., improvedlistening comprehension, spoken language pro-cessing, and educational achievement).26,27 Dataconfirming treatment efficacy are derived frommultiple measures that are sufficiently broad todocument the client’s progress in deploying thenewly acquired or enhanced processing strate-gies and skills in a variety of contexts.19 Repeatadministration of auditory processing tests (e.g.,dichotic digits, pitch patterns, gap detection) areless useful in establishing treatment efficacy andmay instead reveal a learning effect.28 Measuresof speech recognition in competition and recog-nition of time-compressed speech, for example,provide more functional indices of the status ofbinaural and temporal auditory processes thando dichotic digits and pitch pattern tests, re-spectively. Performance scales and inventoriesalso may serve as useful outcome measures.29–31

Musiek’s report of improved communicationfunction and academic performance in 9- to 11-year-old children following AT, as outlined inthis article, illustrates the types of measuresneeded to confirm efficacy of AT.14

Page 64: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

300 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

AUDITORY TRAINING APPROACHES

The authors categorize AT approaches as for-mal and informal. Formal AT is conducted bythe professional in a controlled setting (i.e., aclinic or laboratory). Informal AT can be con-ducted as part of a home or school manage-ment program for APD. Coupling formal withinformal AT should maximize treatment effi-cacy as skills are practiced toward mastery andautomatism in real world settings that establishfunctional significance and provide repeatedopportunities for generalization of skills.

Formal Auditory Training Tasks

Formal AT employs rigorous, acoustically con-trolled training paradigms using bottom-up (i.e.,analytic) tasks with nonverbal signals (e.g.,tones) and simple speech elements (e.g., dis-criminating paired consonant-vowel [CV] syl-lables) to target specific auditory processes.Formal AT to improve temporal processingmight target gap detection and ordering of twoor three rapidly presented acoustic elements,varying the interstimulus interval and/or stim-ulus duration to alter task difficulty. FormalAT may involve language-based or top-downtasks (e.g., resolving semantic distinctions sig-naled by temporal cues; using a phonemicanalysis task to exercise auditory discrimination;resolving prosodic distinctions to challengetemporal resolution).

Formal AT programs have many advan-tages, including the ability to specify and pre-cisely alter acoustic stimuli. The acoustic com-position of the stimuli, as well as their rate ofpresentation, can be controlled using computersand other instrumentation. Presentation of con-trolled stimuli through adaptive computer pro-grams engages the listener and ensures the taskis maintained at the appropriate difficulty level.In addition, computer software can simplify themonitoring of performance. Generalization ofauditory perceptual skills from clinic or labora-tory to real-world settings (e.g., school, work-place, playground) may not occur readily; how-ever, unless formal AT is extended throughcollaboration with the speech-language pathol-

ogist or educational audiologist, families, andteachers who employ informal AT approachesin the home or classroom setting.

A variety of habilitation techniques can beemployed to improve auditory processes, as illus-trated in Table 1. Clinical decisions to target oneor more auditory processes are based on the re-sults of specific auditory tests and procedures, aswell as reports of functional, real-world deficits.(The reader should note that interhemispherictransfer, while not a specific auditory perceptualskill, is included in Table 1 because it underliesbinaural processing required for auditory separa-tion and auditory integration.)

Some AT tasks target more than one audi-tory process. For example, intensity trainingmay benefit localization and lateralization, au-ditory vigilance, and auditory discrimination.32

Similarly, training dichotic listening mayimprove binaural separation and binaural inte-gration. Perhaps most important, formal ATshould benefit spoken language processing byimproving the auditory processes essential func-tion in resolving auditory signals. For example,improved temporal processing should strengthenresolution of prosodic detail underlying mean-ingful distinctions between heteronyms, as wellas distinctions of temporal cueing (e.g., “Theysaw the car go on the ferry” vs. “They saw thecargo on the ferry.”). Targeting recognition ofexpanded or compressed words or speech seg-ments should improve auditory closure.

Examples of commercially available for-mal AT programs include Fast ForWord (avail-able from Scientific Learning Corp., Berkley,CA) and Earobics™ (available from CognitiveConcepts, Inc., Evanston, IL). Both programsinvolve computer-mediated presentations ofacoustically modified stimuli.The Earobics pro-gram, as well as Away We Go (also availablefrom Scientific Learning Corporation), can beemployed on the home computer (in support ofinformal AT), as well as in the professional clin-ical setting. See the articles by Musiek et al, andby Phillips (this issue, pages 263–276, and pages251–262, respectively), for a discussion of soft-ware for AT. While these commercially availabletools for formal AT require instrumentation,other formal AT activities can be administeredin a clinical setting with minimal if any instru-

Page 65: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

AUDITORY TRAINING FOR AUDITORY PROCESSING DISORDERS/CHERMAK, MUSIEK 301

Ta

ble

1S

ele

cte

d F

orm

al a

nd

In

form

al A

ud

ito

ry T

rain

ing

Te

ch

niq

ue

s T

arg

eti

ng

Sp

ecif

ic A

ud

ito

ry P

roce

sse

s

Pro

ce

ss

Asse

ssm

en

t Te

st/

Pro

ce

du

re(R

e)H

ab

ilit

ati

on

Aud

itory

dis

crim

inat

ion

Diff

eren

ce li

men

s fo

r in

tens

ity, f

requ

ency

, and

Aud

itory

vig

ilanc

e; P

aire

d co

mpa

rison

s of

pho

nem

es,

dura

tion;

dis

tort

ed s

peec

h re

cogn

ition

sylla

bles

, ton

es, o

r w

ords

; dis

crim

inat

e in

tens

ity(e

.g.,

filte

red

spee

ch; c

ompr

esse

d sp

eech

)in

crem

ents

; dis

crim

inat

e fr

eque

ncy

incr

emen

ts/t

rans

ition

s; d

iscr

imin

ate

dura

tion

incr

emen

ts/g

aps/

diffe

renc

es fo

r to

nes

or C

Vs;

phon

emic

ana

lysi

s, p

hone

mic

syn

thes

isA

udito

ry c

losu

reD

isto

rted

spe

ech

reco

gniti

on (e

.g. f

ilter

ed s

peec

h;M

iller

-Gild

ea v

ocab

ular

y bu

ildin

g; r

ecog

nize

wor

dsco

mpr

esse

d sp

eech

)an

d ac

oust

ical

ly d

egra

ded

wor

ds in

sen

tenc

e co

ntex

tTe

mpo

ral a

naly

sis/

synt

hesi

sTw

o el

emen

t or

derin

g; p

atte

rn p

erce

ptio

n; c

lick

Gap

det

ectio

n; d

urat

ion

disc

rimin

atio

n; s

eque

ncin

gfu

sion

or

gap

dete

ctio

nta

sks;

pro

sody

tra

inin

g; h

eter

onym

diff

eren

tiatio

n;id

entif

y w

ord

boun

darie

s vi

a te

mpo

ral c

uein

g; r

eadi

ngpo

etry

; fol

low

aud

itory

dire

ctiv

esB

inau

ral s

epar

atio

nD

irect

ed d

icho

tic s

peec

h re

cogn

ition

; dio

ticIn

tens

ity a

ltere

d di

chot

ic li

sten

ing;

tem

pora

lly a

ltere

dsp

eech

rec

ogni

tion

in c

ompe

titio

ndi

chot

ic li

sten

ing;

spe

ech

reco

gniti

on in

com

petit

ion;

liste

n to

inte

nsity

imba

lanc

ed s

tere

o m

usic

; aud

itory

vigi

lanc

eB

inau

ral i

nteg

ratio

n/su

mm

atio

nN

ondi

rect

ed d

icho

tic s

peec

h re

cogn

ition

Inte

nsity

alte

red

dich

otic

list

enin

g; t

empo

rally

alte

red

dich

otic

list

enin

g; s

peec

h re

cogn

ition

in c

ompe

titio

n;lis

ten

to in

tens

ity im

bala

nced

ste

reo

mus

ic; a

udito

ryvi

gila

nce

Loca

lizat

ion,

late

raliz

atio

nD

icho

tic r

hym

e; lo

caliz

atio

n/la

tera

lizat

ion

Sig

nal d

etec

tion

in s

ound

-fie

ld w

ith c

hang

ing

targ

et(b

inau

ral i

nter

actio

n)po

sitio

nIn

terh

emis

pher

ic t

rans

fer

Patt

ern

perc

eptio

nVe

rbal

iden

tific

atio

n th

roug

h ta

ctile

cue

s of

obj

ects

held

in le

ft h

and;

des

crib

e pi

ctur

e w

hile

dra

win

g;le

ft-s

ided

mot

or r

espo

nse

to v

erba

l com

man

d

Page 66: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

302 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

mentation required. Auditory vigilance trainingcan be accomplished by presenting various stim-uli (live voice or recorded), varying the timebetween presentations. Similarly, temporal or-dering can be targeted by asking clients to de-termine the order in which tones or syllables arepresented. The tasks outlined here are typicallypresented in the sound field at a most comfort-able listening level.

Intensity Training

Intensity training may be indicated on thebasis of reduced difference limens for intensity.Intensity discrimination training requires theclient to discern an intensity increment thatis superimposed on a continuous tone of thesame frequency. We have found that an inten-sity increment varied from 1 to 5 dB, approxi-mately 300 msec in duration, and with a 50msec rise/fall time provides a success-to-failureratio needed to maintain challenge as well asmotivation.

Frequency Training

Training on frequency discrimination and tran-sitions may be indicated by poor performanceon the Pitch Pattern Test.33 A frequency detec-tion task requires the client to detect frequencymodulations (i.e., pitch variations) of continu-ous tones (approximately 5 sec in duration).The degree and rate of frequency modulationare adjusted to attain the desired success-to-failure ratio. The frequency of the modulationtone can be varied from session to session tospan across low, mid, and high frequencies.

Discriminating and identifying differencesin modulation rates offers a more challengingtask involving both frequency and temporalprocessing abilities. The client is asked to com-pare the modulation rates of two successivetones, each approximately 1 sec in duration witha 1 sec interstimulus interval. The degree offrequency modulation is held constant at a fre-quency difference easily recognized by the client.The client’s task is to first determine whether

the modulation rates are the same or differentand, if different, to identify which of the twotones is modulated at a faster rate.

Another frequency training task involvesascending and descending frequency sweeps ofvarying glide rates. The sweep range is usuallyless than 10% of the frequency of the base tone.The client’s task is to determine whether achange has occurred and if so in which direc-tion (i.e., low to high or high to low frequency).

Temporal Training

Temporal training tasks are indicated by poorperformance on the Pitch Pattern Test andDuration Pattern Test.34 Cognate pairs (e.g.,/pa/ vs. /ba/) that are distinguished by theirvoice-onset times (VOT) provide stimulus ma-terial for temporal training. Clients can betrained to discriminate and identify CV pairsas different (/ba/, /da/) or the same (/ba/, /ba/),varying the interstimulus interval to adjust dif-ficulty level.16 Gap detection offers another ap-proach to temporal training. Stimuli consist of10 sec periods of broadband noise with two orthree interruptions (gaps). The gaps or silentintervals are systematically varied to permit theappropriate range of performance. Ordering oftwo or three rapidly presented acoustic elementsalso targets temporal processing. The clinicianmay alter the interstimulus interval and/or du-ration of the stimuli to adjust task difficulty.

Speech Recognition Training

Speech recognition training in various compet-ing conditions builds on a number of auditoryprocesses, including auditory discriminationand temporal processing. Poor performance ondichotic listening or compressed/reverberantspeech recognition tests suggests a need forspeech recognition training.

The speech recognition task can be con-ducted to train binaural separation and binau-ral integration. In the first variation, the clientis required to direct attention to the loudspeakerdelivering the less intense stimuli, while ignor-

Page 67: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

AUDITORY TRAINING FOR AUDITORY PROCESSING DISORDERS/CHERMAK, MUSIEK 303

ing the stimuli presented through the secondloudspeaker (i.e., binaural separation). In thesecond variation, the client is asked to attendto both loudspeakers (i.e., binaural integra-tion). The intensity of the stimuli in both con-ditions varies in 10-dB steps, with 5 to 10 trialsat each intensity. Words, sentences and conso-nant-vowel-consonant syllables (CVC) may beused as stimuli. Stimulus intensity should bevaried to achieve the desired success-failurerate. Competing speech may be added to thebinaural speech recognition task to increasedifficulty. Stimuli should be presented at 50 dBHL, or comfortable listening level, and speech-to-competition ratios should be adjusted to en-sure challenge and maintain motivation.

Informal Auditory Training

Employing verbal stimuli and emphasizing theuse of linguistic context to benefit auditory func-tion, informal AT offers a complementary andmore synthetic approach to AT. Informal AT isdesigned to improve auditory perceptual skillsthrough language-based, predominantly top-down tasks. Because required instrumentationis minimal, AT can be conducted in the homeor school, as well as by the speech-languagepathologist or the educational audiologist in theclinic. The speech-language pathologist can useinformal AT to train the same auditory pro-cesses targeted by the audiologist, who, becauseof access to particular instrumentation, wouldmore likely administer formal AT techniques.For example, binaural separation (i.e., selectivelistening) and binaural integration (i.e., dividedattention) can be trained through formal ATusing intensity altered and temporally altereddichotic tasks. These same skills can be trainedinformally with a two-channel stereo system,using the balance control to adjust the inten-sity of the two channels. Similarly, informaltasks requiring the following of sequenced, au-ditory directives can be paired with formal tasksrequiring ordering of rapidly presented acous-tic elements of varying interstimulus intervaland/or duration to target temporal processing.

Informal AT programs tap multiple pro-cesses concurrently, which can be helpful in

training integrative functions. Moreover, infor-mal AT offers potential for broader impact, asa number of informal techniques improve lan-guage skills as well as auditory skills. For exam-ple, discrimination and recognition of degradedspeech stimuli (e.g., time compressed or filteredspeech) can be used to improve auditory clo-sure. These same speech stimuli presented insentences can be used to build vocabulary (i.e.,context-derived vocabulary building).19 Simi-larly, prosody training with heteronyms targettemporal processing while also aiding vocabu-lary development.19

Informal AT offers substantial opportunityfor generalization of newly learned skills andstrategies. Informal AT provides a wide variety ofAT stimuli, contexts, and tasks, all of which fostergeneralization of skills. Because these programscan be undertaken at home and in school, infor-mal AT allows for additional practice that pro-motes mastery and generalization of skills andmaximizes the efficacy of the treatment program.

In addition to the specific methods below,the professional may adapt many tests of cen-tral auditory processing for use as therapy ma-terials. For example, temporal gap detectionmay be trained using the Auditory FusionTest-R.35 (or the revised form, the RandomGap Detection Test). Temporal sequencing maybe trained using the Duration Patterns Test.34

Auditory directives may be trained using theToken Test for Children.36 Auditory vigilancemay be targeted using the Auditory Continu-ous Performance Test.37 The speech-languagepathologist should make the decision to use testmaterials for therapy in collaboration with theaudiologist who may plan to re-administer thesetests as one measure of progress in therapy.Using the same test for assessment and therapycould lead to a situation where the subject’sperformance on repeat testing reflects familiar-ity with the test (i.e., learning effect) rather thana true measure of auditory function.

The following informal AT methods relyon equipment readily available to most speech-language pathologists and educational audiol-ogists. Preliminary data support the potentialof these methods to improve the communica-tion and academic performance of childrenwith APD.14

Page 68: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

304 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

Auditory Discrimination Training

Auditory discrimination is perhaps the mostfundamental auditory processing skill underly-ing spoken language comprehension.19 Theability to perceive acoustic similarities and dif-ferences between sounds is essential (but notsufficient) for segmentation skills and phone-mic analysis and synthesis.

Children with APD often experience diffi-culty discriminating and identifying vowels, per-haps reflecting an interaction between classroomacoustics and auditory perception.14 Because thebackground or ambient classroom noise has aspectrum that is often similar to many of the lowfrequency vowels,38,39 many distinctive acousticelements that are key to distinguishing and iden-tifying vowels are masked. Auditory discrimina-tion training for vowels should be conducted in aquiet environment, without visual cues, althoughit may be necessary to initially provide visual in-formation that is then removed as the clientachieves some success.

The clinician must ascertain that the childunderstands metalinguistic concepts (e.g., themeaning of long versus short vowels, soundposition in words, phoneme-grapheme corre-spondence rules, etc.) before proceeding. In-formal vowel training progresses through ahierarchy of steps. First the child is asked toidentify the sound corresponding to each ofthe vowel graphemes, as these graphemes arewritten for the child. Next, the clinician pre-sents the vowel sounds auditorily, asking thechild to point to or state the correspondinggrapheme, denoting long or short. Vowels maythen be presented for identification in contextusing CV and CVC stimuli and words.

Once the child’s understanding of pho-neme-grapheme correspondence has been es-tablished, auditory discrimination training com-mences with paired (same-different judgments)comparisons of vowels. The task can be gradu-ated in difficulty by presenting vowels with in-creasingly more similar acoustic structure (i.e.,those closer together in the vowel quadralat-eral). The child should be directed to listen forthe subtle acoustic changes that mark vowelscoarticulated in syllabic and word (rhyming) con-texts. Auditory discrimination training can befocused on single consonant and consonant

blends. Consonant pairs sharing few acousticfeatures will be easiest to discriminate. Trainingcan be extended to sentence contexts, providinga transition to therapy directed to auditory clo-sure and vocabulary building.19 Construction ofa confusion matrix of vowel, consonant, and worddiscrimination errors can help direct trainingtoward more frequently occurring errors. Asthe child masters these errors, these sounds areremoved from the high-error pool and atten-tion is directed to discriminating items that re-main challenging to the child.

As noted previously, auditory discrimi-nation exercises also tax and therefore train au-ditory attention. Similarly, auditory discrimi-nation tasks can be used to train temporalprocessing by requiring the child to distinguishheteronyms and identify vowels in proper se-quence. Sloan’s four-part AT program devel-ops auditory discrimination, sound analysis,and phoneme-grapheme association skills andapplies these skills to reading and spelling ofwords.40

Auditory Discrimination Training

for Young Children.

For most preschool children, discriminatingdifferences among sounds is a challenging, yetengaging, task. Environmental sounds differ-ing in intensity, frequency, duration, and qual-ity can be used to develop auditory discrimina-tion. The child might be asked to state whichof three bells of different pitch has the highest,middle, and lowest pitch or to identify differ-ent, familiar voices. To increase task difficulty,speakers can alter their voices, speak quickly,say short words or CV combinations, or use acombination of these modifications.41 Playinggames with toy animals (“The cow says moo,the sheep says baaa.”) and picture or stickerpointing games (“Show me the cat.”) also trainauditory discrimination and identification.42

Prosody Training

Prosody guides attention to the most informa-tive parts of the message, provides informationabout lexical, semantic, and syntactic aspects of

Page 69: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

AUDITORY TRAINING FOR AUDITORY PROCESSING DISORDERS/CHERMAK, MUSIEK 305

the spoken message, and also helps establishauditory memory patterns important to lan-guage.43–47 Also known as acoustic contourrecognition, discrimination and identificationof prosodic features (e.g., intonation, rhythm,and acoustic stress) require the perception ofrather subtle and rapid temporal, frequency,and intensity changes.47 Because individualswith APD often do not have good frequency,temporal or intensity discrimination,48,49 prosodyand intonation cannot be appreciated fully.

Informal auditory training for recognitionof rhythm, prosody, and intonation can be ac-complished in a variety of ways. One approachsimilar to the temporal tasks employed by Tal-lal and colleagues16 is to require clients to audi-torily define word boundaries and intervals be-tween words. The client is given words withone segment stressed and asked to identify thestressed segment and repeat the word withstress on the appropriate syllable. For example,a city in Ohio can be said in three differentways with stress on three different syllables, asfollows: Ashtabula, Ashtabula, Ashtabula.

Sentences with varying prosodic cues canbe used as therapy material to demonstrate howprosodic changes alter meaning. For example,the meaning of the following phrase changesas emphasis is placed on different words: Look!out the window; Look out! the window; Lookout the window. The pitch, intensity, and dura-tion variations that signal proper segmentationand change meaning should be discussed.

Sentences in which temporal cues signalword boundaries and change meaning also pro-vide opportunity to train auditory closure andbuild vocabulary. Subtle timing cues mark dis-tinctive differences in meaning as illustrated inthe following sentences, taken from Cole andJakimik.50 Resolution of prosodic detail is neces-sary to properly segment and derive the correctmeaning of ambiguous words in these sentences.

“The doctor said that nose drops will helpthe cold.”

“The doctor said that he knows drops willhelp the cold.”

“He just hated forgetting the right number.”“He was noted for getting the right

number.”

Reading poetry is another useful activityto improve temporal processing of speechrhythms.14 Having the child mimic a cadenceof simple finger tapping or sequence of noteson a piano also can be used as a form of tempo-ral training.

Auditory Directives

The ability to follow auditory directions is anessential life skill and is key to academic success.Following auditory directives can be trainedwithin naturally occurring (i.e., authentic) con-texts in which the individual must follow se-quenced directives to successfully complete atask, as well as through the use of games thatrequire the child to follow directions presentedauditorily.41

The child should be asked to repeat the di-rective before executing the motor task to en-hance reauditorization and auditory memory.Requesting motor activity rather than simplyrequiring repetition of the directive ensures thatthe client integrated the auditory input with rel-evant knowledge and motor commands in com-pleting the required task. Moreover, the motortask will channel excessive motor activity pre-sented by children whose APD co-exists withattention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD).The impulsivity of children with ADHD shouldbe counteracted by instructing them not toperform the motor task until the directive(s)has been completed.

Auditory commands should progress indifficulty level from simple to complex, involv-ing one or multiple sequenced actions.14,41

Group actions requiring cooperation amongchildren may be used, and children can be giventhe chance to act as clinicians (i.e., reciprocalteaching) by generating directives for others tofollow. Reciprocal teaching expands practiceopportunities while building confidence andself-esteem.19

Directives can be presented in the contextof a game format. The Token Test for Childrencan be used as material for auditory directives.36

Incorporating this type of therapy outside theclinical setting (e.g., in the classroom, at home inthe context of chores, or in recreational settings)should maximize generalization. An example of

Page 70: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

306 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

the latter would be in teaching a child how toswing a tennis racket or a golf club while alsoteaching the game of tennis or golf, respectively.

Auditory Vigilance Training

Auditory vigilance involves sustaining a highlevel of auditory attention for necessary peri-ods of time in anticipation of a target stimulus.Auditory vigilance is usually trained by askinga client to listen for a target stimulus presentedat random or at pseudo-random intervals withina series of other auditory events. One might,for example, read a story to a child (or group ofchildren), designating a target item (e.g., allwords that end in the “d’ sound or any wordthat has to do with food). The more randomthe target presentation and the greater the in-terval between targets, the more challengingthe task. This type of activity is appropriatefor children in preschool or early elementarygrades and can be adapted for use with olderclients as well. For example, the target mightbe subtle prosodic elements, thereby trainingprosody in the context of an auditory vigilanceexercise. The option to use a wide variety oftarget stimuli gives the auditory vigilance para-digm flexibility to train other auditory pro-cesses while accommodating clients of varyingage and ability.

CONCLUSIONS

Given the range of listening and learning deficitsassociated with APD, AT must be seen as onlyone component of a comprehensive manage-ment approach to improving auditory process-ing. Coupling formal with informal AT shouldmaximize treatment efficacy. Collaborationamong professionals administering these ther-apies is crucial to the success of AT. The in-volvement of families in home practice is cru-cial because many children with APD do notqualify for special services in many school dis-tricts. Those who do qualify may not receivesufficient school-based therapy to achieve theirpotential. Although there is little empiricaldata relating to the amount of practice consid-

ered optimal for remediating APD, it is nowrecognized that regular and consistent practice,perhaps even for 10 to 15 minutes daily for anumber of weeks, provides the intensity of focusand training needed to maximize success.16 Al-though economic, time, and motivational fac-tors certainly influence the length of time ther-apy can be provided, optimally, progress towardmeeting therapy objectives should be the pri-mary guide to decisions regarding schedulingand terminating therapy.

ABBREVIATIONS

ADHD attention deficit hyperactivity disorderAPD auditory processing disorderAT auditory trainingCANS central auditory nervous systemCV consonant-vowel syllablesCVC consonant-vowel-consonant syllables

REFERENCES

1. Edeline JM, Weinberger NM. Thalamic short-termplasticity in the auditory system: associative retun-ing of receptive fields in the ventral medial genicu-late body. Behav Neurosci 1991;105:618–639

2. Elbert T, Pantev C, Wienbruch C, Rockstroh B,Taub E. Increased cortical representation of thefingers of the left hand in string players. Science1995;270:305–306

3. Hassamannova J, Myslivecek J, Novakova V. Ef-fects of early auditory stimulation on cortical areas.In: Syka J, Aitkin L, eds. Neuronal Mechanisms ofHearing. New York: Plenum Press; 1981:355–359

4. Irvine DRF, Rajan R, McDermott HJ. Injury-induced reorganization in adult auditory cortx andits perceptual consequences. Hearing Res 2000;147:188–199

5. Knudsen EI. Experience shapes sound localizationand auditory unit properties during developmentin the barn owl. In: Edlman G, Gall W, Kowan W,eds. Auditory Function: Neurobiological Basis ofHearing. New York: John Wiley; 1988:137–152

6. Merzenich M, Jenkins W. Cortical plasticity,learning and learning dysfunction. In: Julesz B,Kovacs I, eds. Maturational Windows and AdultCortical Plasticity: SFI Studies in the Sciences ofComplexity, vol. XXIII. Reading, PA: Addison-Wesley; 1995: 247–272

Page 71: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

AUDITORY TRAINING FOR AUDITORY PROCESSING DISORDERS/CHERMAK, MUSIEK 307

7. Moore DR. Plasticity of binaural hearing and somepossible mechanisms following late-onset depriva-tion. J Am Acad Audiol 1993;4:227–283

8. Recanzone GH, Schreiner CE, Hradek G, et al.Functional reorganization of the primary auditorycortex I adult owl monkeys parallel improvements inperformance in an auditory frequency discrimina-tion task. Soc Neurosci 1991;17(213.2):534 (Abst)

9. Recanzone GH, Schreiner CE, Merzenich MM.Plasticity in the frequency representation of primaryauditory cortex following discrimination training inadult owl monkeys. J Neurosci 1993;13:87–103

10. Weinberger NM, Diamond DM. Physiologicalplasticity in auditory cortex: rapid induction bylearning. Prog Neurobiol 1987;29:1–55

11. Willott JF, Aitken LM, McFadden SL. Plasticityof auditory cortex associated with sensorineuralhearing loss in adult mice. J Comp Neurol 1993;329:402–411

12. Jirsa RE.The clinical utility of the P3 AERP in chil-dren with auditory processing disorders in school-aged children. J Speech Hear Res 1992;35:903–912

13. Merzenich MM, Jenkins WM, Johnston P, et al.Temporal processing deficits of language-learningimpaired children ameliorated by training. Science1996;271:77–80

14. Musiek FE. Habilitation and management of audi-tory processing disorders: overview of selected pro-cedures. J Am Acad Audiol 1999;10:329–342

15. Musiek FE, Schochat E. Auditory training andcentral auditory processing disorders: a case study.Semin Hear 1998;19:357–366

16. Tallal P, Miller S, Bedi G, et al. Language compre-hension in language-learning impaired childrenimproved with acoustically modified speech. Sci-ence 1996;271:81–84

17. Katz J, Chertoff M, Sawusch J. Dichotic training. JAudiol Res 1984;24:251–264

18. Chermak GD, Musiek FE. Managing central au-ditory processing disorders in children and youth.Am J Audiol 1992;1:61–65

19. Chermak GD, Musiek FE. Central Auditory Pro-cessing Disorders: New Perspectives. San Diego,CA: Singular; 1997

20. American Speech-Language-Hearing AssociationTask Force on Central Auditory Processing Devel-opment. Central auditory processing: current statusof research and implications for clinical practice.Am J Audiol 1996;5(2):41–54

21. Jerger J, Musiek F. Report of the consensus confer-ence on the diagnosis of auditory processing disor-ders in school-aged children. J Am Acad Audiol2000;11:467–474

22. Kolb B. Brain Plasticity and Behavior. Mahwah,NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1995

23. Kalil R. Synapse formation in the developingbrain. Sci Am 1989;261:76–87

24. Black JE. How a child builds its brain: some lesionsfrom animal studies of neural plasticity. Prev Med1998;27:168–171

25. Picheny M, Durlach N, Braida L. Speaking clearlyfor the hard of hearing: intelligibility differencesbetween clear and conversational speech. J SpeechHear Res 1985;28:96–103

26. Bain B, Dollaghan C. Treatment efficacy: the no-tion of clinically significant change. Lang SpeechHear Serv School 1991;22:264–270

27. Olswang LB, Bain B. Data collection: monitoringchildren’s treatment progress. Am J Speech LangPathol 1994;3(3):55–66

28. Yencer KA. The effects of auditory integrationtraining for children with central auditory process-ing disorders. Am J Audiol 1998;7:32–44

29. Anderson KL. S.I.F.T.E.R.: Screening Instrumentfor Targeting Educational Risk. Austin, TX: ProEd, 1989

30. Anderson KL, Smaldino J. Listening Inventoriesfor Education (L.I.F.E.). Tampa, Fl: EducationalAudiology Assoc; 1998

31. Smoski WJ, Brunt MA, Tannahill JC. Listeningcharacteristic of children with central auditory pro-cessing disorders. Lang Speech Hear Serv School1992;23:145–152

32. Schlanger B. Effects of listening training on audi-tory thresholds of mentally retarded children.ASHA 1962;4:273–274

33. Pinheiro ML. Auditory pattern reversal in audi-tory perception in patients with left and righthemisphere lesions. Ohio J Speech Hear 1977;12:9–20

34. Musiek FE, Baran JA, Pinheiro ML. Durationpattern recognition in normal subjects and in pa-tients with cerebral and cochlear lesions. Audiol-ogy 1990;29:304–313

35. McCrosky RL, Keith RW. The auditory fusiontest-revised. St. Louis, MO: Auditec; 1996

36. DiSimoni FG. The Token Test for Children.Allen, TX: University Park Press; 1978

37. Keith RW. The Auditory Continuous Perfor-mance Test. San Antonio, TX: The PsychologicalCorp; 1994

38. Sanders DA. Aural Rehabilitation. EnglewoodCliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall; 1971

39. Hetu R, Gagnon-Ttuchon C, Bilideau S. Problemsof noise in school settings: a review of literatureand the results of an exploratory study. J SpeechPathol Audiol 1990;14:31–39

40. Sloan C.Treating Auditory Processing Difficulties inChildren. San Diego, CA: College Hill Press; 1986

41. Musiek FE, Chermak GD. Three commonly askedquestions about central auditory processing disor-ders: management. Am J Audiol 1995;4:15–18

42. Tye-Murray N. Foundation of Aural Rehabilita-tion. San Diego, CA: Singular; 1998

Page 72: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

308 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

43. Goldinger SD, Pisoni DB, Luce PA. Speech per-ception and spoken word recognition: research andtheory. In: Lass NJ, ed. Principles of ExperimentalPhonetics. St. Louis, MO: Mosby; 1996:277–327

44. Cutler A, Foss DJ. On the role of sentence stress insentence processing. Lang Speech 1977;20:1–10

45. Studdert-Kennedy M. Speech perception. LangSpeech 1980;23:46–66

46. Valett RE. Developing linguistic auditory memorypatterns. J Lear Disabil 1983;16:462–466

47. Handel S. Listening: An Introduction to the Per-ception of Auditory Events. Cambridge, MA:MIT Press; 1989

48. Bornstein SP, Musiek FE. Implication of temporalprocessing for children with learning and languageproblems. In: Beasley D, ed. Contemporary Issuesin Audition. San Diego, CA: College-Hill Press;1984:25–65

49. Thompson M, Abel S. Indices of hearing in pa-tients with central auditory pathology. II: Choiceresponse time. Scand Audiol 1996;21:17–22

50. Cole R, Jakimik J. A model of speech perception.In: Cole R, ed. Perception and Prediction of FluentSpeech. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum; 1980:133–160

Page 73: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

309

Management of Auditory Processing Disorders; Editor in Chief, Catherine V. Palmer, Ph.D.; Guest Editor, Gail D.Chermak, Ph.D. Seminars in Hearing, volume 23, number 4, 2002. Address for correspondence and reprint requests: GailGegg Rosenberg, M.S., Director, Special Needs Education, Interactive Solutions, Inc., 6448 Parkland Drive, Sarasota, FL34243. E-mail: [email protected]. 1Vice President, Interactive Solutions Incorporated, Sarasota, Florida.Copyright © 2002 by Thieme Medical Publishers, Inc., 333 Seventh Avenue, New York, NY 10001, USA. Tel: +1(212)584-4662. 00734-0451,p;2002,23,04,309,318,ftx,en;sih00224x.

Classroom Acoustics and Personal FMTechnology in Management of AuditoryProcessing DisorderGail Gegg Rosenberg, M.S.1

ABSTRACT

Acoustical conditions in the classroom, home, and social environ-ments are often barriers to listening and learning for students with auditoryprocessing disorder (APD). The effective management of APD requirescareful attention to classroom acoustics and the use of personal frequencymodulated (FM) systems as strategies to improve the quality of the listen-ing environment and the student’s access to acoustic information. As mem-bers of the multidisciplinary team responsible for both auditory assessmentand management, audiologists have the responsibility to guide the evalua-tion of the listening environment and make recommendations for modifi-cations, as well as the selection, fitting, and monitoring of personal FMtechnologies. Demonstrating efficacy is an essential part of the manage-ment process. Audiologists are challenged to contribute to the researchbase by collecting data to support the use of personal FM systems as an ef-ficacious management strategy.

KEYWORDS: Auditory processing disorder, profiles, personal FM,classroom management

Learning Outcomes: After reading this article, the reader will be able to (1) discuss the quantitative and quali-tative indicators identified through the assessment process that support the use of a personal FM system asan APD management tool, (2) identify the APD profile characteristics that support the use of FM technology asan APD management tool, and (3) organize and implement training and efficacy measures related to the use ofFM technology as an APD management tool.

Page 74: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

310 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

needs; and (4) ensuring that the student andteacher receive inservice training and that effi-cacy measures are taken.

Step 1: Determining the APD Profile

and Indicators

Whether the audiologist uses the Bellis-Ferre2,6

or the Buffalo Model7 profiles to describe thestudent’s auditory processing strengths andweaknesses and identify a profile type, it is im-portant to not only consider the test results,but also the case history, observational, andqualitative test behavior information. The au-thor finds Auditory Decoding, Organization/Output, and Tolerance-Fading Memory (TFM)profiles to offer the strongest indications that apersonal FM system would be an appropriatemanagement strategy. Abnormal results onmonaural low redundancy speech and dichotictests, that is, tasks involving degraded signals,figure-ground, or speech in competition, stronglysuggest that a personal FM system would be anappropriate strategy to modify the effects ofthe classroom listening environment.1,8

Table 1 provides a summary of quantita-tive and qualitative indicators for personal FMsystem use based on: (1) case history, observa-tion checklist, and qualitative test behaviors;and (2) monaural low redundancy speech anddichotic test findings and auditory processesand skills tested. Table 2 summarizes APDprofiles that typically support the use of an FMsystem as a management strategy. It is impor-tant to note the contraindications for recom-mending a personal FM system as a manage-ment tool when reviewing the APD profile.For instance, FM is seldom recommended forthe student with a Prosodic deficit since theprimary difficulty is not related to the clarity ofthe acoustic signal.6 Generally, an FM systemwould not be recommended for the studentwith an Integration deficit stemming from dif-ficulties synthesizing auditory information, al-though it may be deemed appropriate insofaras it benefits the speech-in-noise deficits thatalso characterize this APD subtype.6,9 The stu-dent with an Auditory Association deficit pro-file does not understand the auditory message

Acoustical conditions in the classroom,home, and social environments can present se-rious challenges for students with auditory pro-cessing disorder (APD). These students, by thevery nature of their disorder, are at risk forlearning.The challenge of listening to degraded,reduced, or distorted speech in acoustically poorenvironments may result in fatigue and audi-tory overload that causes the student to tuneout or alternatively become distracted and ex-hibit annoying behaviors. When a student isdiagnosed with APD, all too often the primaryor only recommendation offered is to fit a per-sonal FM system.1,2 While this may be an ap-propriate management tool for some studentswho exhibit an auditory input disorder, it shouldnot be regarded as a routine recommendation,for there are other critical factors to be consid-ered. In fact, the recommendation of a personalFM system must be considered as a part of themanagement process and not as the sole or pri-mary management strategy. Unfortunately, whilethe benefits of sound field amplification for allchildren, and particularly those at risk for lis-tening and learning are well documented,3,4

little data have been published that demon-strate the efficacy of personal FM amplifica-tion as a management strategy for studentswith APD.1,5

MANAGEMENT OF APD

The management of APD involves three pri-mary areas—the listening environment, com-pensatory strategies, and direct therapy for re-mediation. Managing classroom acoustics andthe use of a personal FM system, both a part ofmanaging the listening environment, should beapproached in a systematic manner. A suggestedfour-step management process includes: (1)evaluating the student’s auditory processingstrengths and weaknesses, determining the pri-mary APD profile, and identifying APD pro-file indicators that support or contraindicatethe use of a personal FM system; (2) evaluatingthe acoustical classroom environment and rec-ommending appropriate modifications; (3) se-lecting and fitting the personal FM systemthat most appropriately meets the student’s

Page 75: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

CLASSROOM ACOUSTICS AND PERSONAL FM TECHNOLOGY/ROSENBERG 311

Ta

ble

1Q

ua

nti

tati

ve

an

d Q

ua

lita

tive

In

dic

ato

rs S

ug

ge

sti

ng

FM

Te

ch

no

log

y a

s a

n A

PD

Ma

na

ge

me

nt

Str

ate

gy

Ca

se

His

tory

,O

bse

rva

tio

n C

he

ck

list,

an

d Q

ua

lita

tive

Te

st

Be

hav

ior

Ind

ica

tors

Com

plai

nt o

f di

ffic

ulty

hea

ring/

liste

ning

in n

oise

(Huh

? I d

idn’

t he

ar y

ou.)

App

ears

to

mis

hear

and

sub

stitu

tes

sim

ilar

soun

ding

wor

ds fo

r th

e au

dito

ry t

arge

tR

equi

res

repe

titio

n of

info

rmat

ion

Poor

dis

crim

inat

ion

of f

ine

acou

stic

diff

eren

ces

in c

onve

rsat

iona

l spe

ech

Exc

essi

ve a

udito

ry fa

tigue

; lis

teni

ng b

ehav

iors

det

erio

rate

as

audi

tory

ove

rload

incr

ease

sD

iffic

ulty

list

enin

g in

situ

atio

ns w

ith r

educ

ed r

edun

danc

y (e

.g.,

exce

ssiv

e no

ise/

reve

rber

atio

n, a

udito

ry c

haos

,gr

oup

sett

ings

)D

iffic

ulty

follo

win

g di

rect

ions

Diff

icul

ty k

eepi

ng u

p w

ith t

he f

low

of

com

mun

icat

ion

Diff

icul

ty le

arni

ng fo

reig

n la

ngua

ges

Del

ayed

res

pons

es

AP

D T

est

Ind

ica

tors

Te

st

Au

dit

ory

Pro

ce

sse

s o

r S

kil

ls T

este

d

Mon

aura

l Low

Red

unda

ncy

Spe

ech-

in-n

oise

Mon

aura

l aud

itory

sep

arat

ion/

audi

tory

fig

ure-

grou

ndS

peec

h Te

sts

(abn

orm

al s

core

/sig

nific

ant

SC

AN

: Aud

itory

Fig

ure

Gro

und

Mon

aura

l aud

itory

sep

arat

ion/

audi

tory

fig

ure-

grou

ndea

r di

ffere

nce)

SC

AN

: Filt

ered

Wor

ds a

nd o

ther

low

-pas

s fil

tere

d A

udito

ry p

erfo

rman

ce w

ith d

egra

ded

audi

tory

spee

ch m

easu

res

sign

als/

audi

tory

clo

sure

Tim

e-co

mpr

esse

d sp

eech

(bot

h al

one

and

Tem

pora

l pro

cess

ing/

audi

tory

per

form

ance

with

with

rev

erbe

ratio

n)de

grad

ed a

udito

ry s

igna

lsSy

nthe

tic S

ente

nce

Iden

tific

atio

n w

ith ip

sila

tera

l M

onau

ral a

udito

ry s

epar

atio

n/au

dito

ry f

igur

e-gr

ound

com

petin

g m

essa

ge (S

SI-I

CM

)D

icho

tic Te

sts

(sig

nific

ant

ear

diffe

renc

e;

Com

petin

g S

ente

nces

Bin

aura

l sep

arat

ion/

bina

ural

com

petit

ion

com

plai

nt a

bout

the

com

petit

ion;

att

empt

C

ompe

ting

Wor

ds*

Bin

aura

l int

egra

tion/

bina

ural

com

petit

ion

to m

ove

the

earp

hone

with

the

com

petit

ion)

Dic

hotic

Dig

its*

Bin

aura

l int

egra

tion/

bina

ural

com

petit

ion

Synt

hetic

Sen

tenc

e Id

entif

icat

ion

with

con

tral

ater

al

Bin

aura

l sep

arat

ion/

bina

ural

com

petit

ion

com

petin

g m

essa

ge (S

SI-C

CM

)S

tagg

ered

Spo

ndai

c W

ord

Test

(SS

W)—

peak

in a

B

inau

ral i

nteg

ratio

n/bi

naur

al c

ompe

titio

nco

mpe

ting

liste

ning

con

ditio

n(s)

;si

gnifi

cant

res

pons

e bi

ases

*Com

petin

g W

ords

and

Dic

hotic

Dig

its c

an b

e co

nsid

ered

as

test

s of

bin

aura

l sep

arat

ion

if th

e re

spon

se t

ask

is a

dire

cted

ear

repo

rt, w

here

as t

hey

are

bina

ural

inte

grat

ion

test

sw

hen

the

free

rep

ort

resp

onse

form

at is

use

d.FM

, fre

quen

cy m

odul

ated

; AP

D, a

udito

ry p

roce

ssin

g di

sord

er; S

CA

N, S

cree

ning

Test

for A

udito

ry P

roce

ssin

g D

isor

ders

; SS

I, Sy

nthe

tic S

ente

nce

Inde

x; IC

M, I

psila

tera

l Com

pet-

ing

Mes

sage

; CC

M, C

ontr

alat

eral

Com

petin

g M

essa

ge; S

SW

, Sta

gger

ed S

pond

aic

Wor

d Te

st.

Page 76: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

312 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

Table 2 Summary of APD Profile Indicators Suggesting FM Technology

as a Management Strategy

Profile Indicator APD Test Findings Selected Management Strategies

Auditory Poor auditory discrimination Assistive listening technology to improve S/Ndecoding Bilateral deficit on monaural low Bottom-up processing approach (auralprofile redundancy speech tests rehabilitation therapy model)

Bilateral deficit on dichotic speech tests Environmental modifications to improve signal clarity and redundancy

Evidence of associated sequelae noted Activities to enhance auditory closureon case history, observation forms, Self-advocacy (listening strategies,and qualitative test behaviors self-management)

Output/ Deficit on any task requiring report of Assistive listening technology to improve S/Norganization more than two critical elements Environmental modifications to enhance S/Nprofile (e.g., DDT, SSW, PPST, DPT) Break information into smaller units; provide

May have poor speech-in-noise skills multi-step directions one at a timeMay exhibit reversals and ordering Strategies training for study skills, note taking,

problems test taking, and use of external organizersMay omit target or substitute (e.g., checklists, agenda book, calendar)

previously heard word Organizational and metacognitive strategiesEvidence of associated sequelae noted training to strengthen the memory trace

on case history, observation forms, Highly structured, rules-based environmentand qualitative test behaviors with consistent routines

Tolerance- SSW—LC peak, high/low order effect, Assistive listening device to improve the S/Nfading low/high ear effect Improve classroom acousticsmemory Phonemic synthesis—quick, impulsive Noise desensitization practiceprofile responses Flexible preferential seating

Speech in noise—significant Quiet work/study areadiscrepancy from score in quiet Direct instruction to improve outlining and

MLD—may be reduced notetaking skillsCase history may reveal hyperacusis, Use of metalinguistic and metacognitive

weak short-term memory, oral and strategies (e.g., rehearsal, chunking,written discourse errors, poor imagery, mnemonics)reading comprehension, phonemic Rule out attention deficit disorderbreakdown

FM, frequency modulated; APD, auditory processing disorder; DDT, Dichotic Digits Test; SSW, Staggered SpondaicWord Test; PPST, Pitch Pattern Sequence Test; DPT, Duration Pattern Test; S/N, signal-to-noise ratio; MLD, maskinglevel difference.

as a result of a linguistic comprehension prob-lem and as such a recommendation for a per-sonal FM system would not be indicated.There is some question as to whether the Au-ditory Associative profile should actually beconsidered an APD subtype or perhaps moreappropriately classified as a language process-ing impairment.10 Students who exhibit a trueTolerance-Fading Memory (TFM) profile, thatis, significant difficulty understanding speechunder adverse listening conditions and short-

term memory weaknesses, may benefit from apersonal FM system as a management strategy,but caution must be exercised in this case. Theother important TFM management plan com-ponents, such as noise desensitization practiceand compensatory strategies for auditory mem-ory deficits must be fully implemented, ratherthan relying exclusively on the improved sig-nal-to-noise ratio offered by the FM system toreduce the other symptoms of this specific APDprofile.

Page 77: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

CLASSROOM ACOUSTICS AND PERSONAL FM TECHNOLOGY/ROSENBERG 313

Step 2: Classroom Acoustics Evaluation

and Modification

The synergistic effects of noise and reverbera-tion and the resulting degradation of speechperception, such as the masking of key elementsof speech, the smearing of temporal cues, andthe loss of speech energy over distance, are wellknown.11–14 These conditions place all studentsat risk in their listening environments, espe-cially those with APD who have documenta-tion to support their difficulty processing de-graded or competing auditory stimuli. It requiresconsiderable and focused effort for these stu-dents to attend, listen, and process acoustic in-formation under optimum listening conditions.Compromises in the quality of the acousticalenvironment will undoubtedly impact the qual-ity of the student’s educational opportunities.The acoustical environment is but one of theparameters that affect a student’s auditory pro-cessing capabilities, although it is indeed apowerful factor in that it can modify acousticsignals and produce reduced communicationopportunities in these primarily auditory-verbal environments. Unfortunately, noise andreverberation levels in classrooms have remainedrelatively unchanged over the past 30 years.3,9,12

The Classroom Acoustics Working Grouphas developed a working draft of a standard foracoustics in school classrooms and other learn-ing spaces that may be used as a guide to createenhanced listening and learning environmentswhere students can receive more consistent au-ditory signals.15 A variety of materials in theschool and classroom environments can absorbsounds and decrease the effects of noise and re-verberation on speech perception. A summaryof acoustical modifications for the classroomare shown in Table 3. Sound and noise controlin the classroom and school environment isboth a science and an art, and information onspecific acoustical modifications may be foundin comprehensive resources.9,12–14 Audiologistshave the knowledge and skills to measure class-room noise and reverberation levels using asystematic method and the appropriate instru-mentation.12 Use of an observational checklistmay be beneficial and it could serve as the pre-liminary measure for studying the effectiveness

of FM technology as a management strat-egy.9 Ultimately, the findings of the classroomacoustics appraisal could result in specific mod-ifications or accommodations being includedin the student’s Individual Educational Plan(IEP) or 504 plan. In addition to modifyingclassroom acoustics, effective teaching strate-gies also should be suggested.9,12

Evaluation and treatment of classroomacoustics always should be done first. Onlyafter this is accomplished should the audiolo-gist move forward with fitting the personalFM system. There will be times during theschool day when the student may not use theFM system, and at some point the student willneed to transition from dependency on the as-sistive device as he/she learns to cope with theenvironmental listening challenges by usingstrategies implemented as part of the manage-ment plan. For these reasons, it is imperativethat classroom acoustics be assessed and im-proved to the extent possible before or at leastconcurrent with the fitting of the personal FMsystem. It is advisable to consider the acousti-cal environment in the home, and offer man-agement recommendations for that environ-ment so that the student will be able to benefitfrom a similar management plan in both lis-tening and learning environments.

Step 3: Personal FM Selection

and Fitting

An audiologist must evaluate students withAPD for whom a personal FM system has beenrecommended.9,20,21 When a personal FM sys-tem has been recommended as a managementstrategy for a student with APD, members ofthe Individual Education Plan (IEP) team arerequired under the Individuals with DisabilitiesEducation (IDEA) to consider assistive tech-nology, which it defines as any item, piece ofequipment, or product system that is used toincrease, maintain, or improve the functionalcapabilities of a child with a disability. This re-quirement uses stronger language than that in-cluded prior to the 1997 IDEA amendments.Local education agencies (LEA) that do notprovide audiological services, either directly or

Page 78: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

314 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

Table 3 Summary of Management Strategies to Improve Classroom Acoustics

Classroom Environment Management Strategy

Ceilings Optimal ceiling height for listening environment: 9–12 feetSuspended acoustical tile ceiling or installation of other absorbent material to

control reverberation if the ceiling height exceeds 12 feetSound absorption material in the plenum space if suspended ceiling is shared

by another classroom Acoustical ceiling tile with sound absorption coefficient sufficient to achieve

desired noise and/or reverberation reduction Suspended banners, student work, and hanging plants

Floors Carpeting installed over pad of adequate thickness to reduce noise and reverberation

Extend carpeting onto the bottom portion of wall surfaces Alternatives: rubberized or resilient tile with an acoustical underlayment

and/or strategically placed area rugs or carpeting Regular carpet maintenance to enhance indoor air quality and diminish

allergic effectsWindows Double-pane windows

Cloth draperies without rubberized backing Closed windows

Walls and Doors Acoustical panels placed on non-parallel, partially down the wallCork bulletin boards, felt or flannel boards, and acoustical or fabric-covered

wall surfacesSolid core-door Well-fitting doors with noise lock seal, gasket, or felt treatment Cover vents in doors or insert a filter

Seating and Furniture Staggered desk and table arrangementArrangement Felt or rubber caps, or tennis balls on chair and table legs

Main instruction area away from noise sourcesFurniture arrangement to accommodate the teacher’s instructional styleAvoid open plan rooms

Heating, Ventilation, Acoustical duct lining for supply and return ducts and Air Conditioning Regular maintenance of HVAC systems (HVAC) Systems Noise control devices in HVAC systems (e.g., duct silencers, adequate duct

length, vibration isolators, adequate duct and diffuser sizes)Lighting Change fluorescent light ballasts on regular basis to avoid hum

House lighting above acoustical tile ceiling Special Purpose Areas Location away from high noise sources

Mobile bulletin boards and bookcases placed at angles to walls to decrease reverberation

Study carrels lined with acoustic tiles or rubber pads

contractually, place themselves at risk if theyfail to engage the services of an audiologist inthe FM evaluation, selection, and fitting pro-cess. In addition to the fact that a mild gainamplification device is being recommended fora student with normal hearing, there are stu-

dent-specific factors to be considered in the se-lection and fitting process that must be the re-sponsibility of an audiologist.

When recommending a personal FM sys-tem, the audiologist and the multidisciplinaryteam should consider the unique benefits of

Page 79: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

CLASSROOM ACOUSTICS AND PERSONAL FM TECHNOLOGY/ROSENBERG 315

Table 4 Considerations in Selecting and Fitting Personal FM Systems for APD Management

Personal FM Components Consideratio

Receiver options Body wornSelf-contained ear levelPortable desktop

Transmitter/microphone Microphone qualityDirectional microphoneNoise canceling microphoneMicrophone style (boom, lapel, collar)Team teaching optionsMute switch

Coupling options Headset (earphone, walkman, stethoclip)Earbud (monaural or binaural)Special applications (open earmold, retention devices)Attenuation capabilities (headset or ear level receiver)

Small and large group applications Team teaching or pass microphone optionDesktop application

this technology to improve the signal-to-noiseratio and decrease the effects of noise, rever-beration, and distance from the speaker. Table4 lists the factors to be considered in the selec-tion and fitting of a student with APD with apersonal FM system. The available options arethe wired body-worn systems, wireless self-contained receivers, and the portable desktopreceiver. Special attention should be given tomicrophone options to ensure that criticalspeech components are delivered to the listenerwhile maintaining the highest possible acous-tic signal quality. The audiologist, again as partof the multidisciplinary management team,should consider the age of the student, theclassroom or particular applications and settingswhere the device will be used, the student’smotivation to use the technology, the responsi-bility level of the student, and the ease of useand maintenance of the various systems inmaking the recommendation for a specific typeof device. Careful attention to each of the se-lection and fitting options should allow for thepersonal FM system to be used as a tool to en-hance the effectiveness of other managementstrategies. The personal FM system should notbe considered as the device that is capable ofsolving all of the student’s educational and com-municative problems.1,8,9,16 The personal FMsystem should enable the student to assume

greater responsibility as the manager of his/herlistening environment, although the studentmay require specific training in self-advocacystrategies and assertiveness training to reachthis level of sophistication.20

Step 4: Inservice Training and

Efficacy Measures

A structured trial evaluation period will allowthe multidisciplinary team to determine if thepersonal FM system is an effective managementstrategy for a specific student. Florida’s TechnicalAssistance Report on Auditory Processing Dis-order proposes a model for the trial evaluationperiod and includes a detailed framework for theinservice training component.7 Important con-siderations in the trial evaluation period are:

• assessment of the classroom (or other) listen-ing environment, perhaps using a tool suchas the Listening Environment Profile9,22 or aworksheet or computerized program avail-able from some FM manufacturers;

• completion of a pre-trial observation form, suchas the Screening Instrument for Targeting Edu-cational Risk (SIFTER),23 the Fisher’s Audi-tory Problems Checklist,24 or the Children’sAuditory Performance Scale (CHAPS)25;

Page 80: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

316 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

• inservice training for the classroom teacher,student, and parent or other professionals asappropriate1,9;

• completion of a post-trial observation formby the classroom teacher and the parent9;

• completion of post-trial evaluation form bythe classroom teacher, parent, and student9;

• plan for monitoring use and maintenance ofthe FM system9; and

• periodic audiologic reevaluation to monitorhearing sensitivity and assess functional lis-tening skills.1,26

The evaluation trial period should extendfor at least six weeks in order to adequately de-termine if this management strategy producespositive behavioral, communicative, and edu-cational changes. The student, as well as theparent and the teacher, should provide inputduring the post-evaluation trial measurements.If the recommendation is to continue use ofthe personal FM system, careful, ongoing doc-umentation of efficacy is encouraged. Eventu-ally the goal should be to train the student todepend less upon the FM system and to imple-ment the other strategies he/she has masteredin order to attend, listen, learn, and self-manageauditory processing challenges in various lis-tening environments. Audiologists may wish tocompile pre- and post-trial evaluation data inorder to support the efficacy of this manage-ment strategy.1,8–10

CONCLUSIONS

Assisting in the management of the acousticalclassroom environment and the appropriaterecommendation, fitting, and monitoring of per-sonal FM technologies for students with APDare among the responsibilities of the audiolo-gist as a member of the multidisciplinary team.Careful attention to classroom acoustics mustbe the first consideration, followed by recom-mendation of the personal assistive listeningdevice only if it is indicated by the student’sprofile of auditory processing strengths andweaknesses. Systematic fitting and monitoringof the personal FM system is indicated inorder to determine the efficacy of this recom-

mendation as an effective management strat-egy. Due to the limited research supporting theefficacy of personal FM for students with APD,audiologists remain challenged to contributeto the research base to support the continuedrecommendation of this assistive hearing tech-nology.

ABBREVIATIONS

APD auditory processing disorderCHAPS Children’s Auditory Perception of

SpeechFM frequency modulatedSIFTER Screening Instrument for Targeting

Educational RiskTFM Tolerance-Fading Memory

REFERENCES

1. Bellis T. Central Auditory Processing Disorders inthe Educational Setting: From Science to Practice.San Diego, CA: Singular; 1996

2. Stein R. Application of FM technology to themanagement of central auditory processing disor-ders. In: Masters M, Stecker N, Katz J, eds. CentralAuditory Processing Disorders: Mostly Manage-ment. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon;1998:89–102

3. Rosenberg G, Blake-Rahter P, Heavner J, et al. Im-proving classroom acoustics (ICA): a three-yearFM sound field classroom amplification study. J EdAudiol 1999;7:8–28

4. Rosenberg G. FM sound field research identifiesbenefits for students and teachers. Ed Audiol Rev1998;15 (3):6–8

5. Stach B, Loiselle L, Jerger J, Mintz S, Taylor C.Clinical experience with personal FM assisting lis-tening devices. Hear J 1987;5:1–6

6. Bellis T, Ferre J. Multidimensional approach to thedifferential diagnosis of central auditory processingdisorders in children. J Am Acad Audiol 1999;10:319–328

7. Florida Department of Education. Central audi-tory processing disorders: technical assistance paper.Tallahassee, FL: Author; 2001

8. Stecker N. Overview and update of CAPD. In:Masters M, Stecker N, Katz J, eds. Central Audi-tory Processing Disorders: Mostly Management.Needham Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon; 1998:1–14

9. Hall J, Mueller H. Audiologists Desk Reference,vol. 1. San Diego, CA: Singular; 1997

Page 81: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

CLASSROOM ACOUSTICS AND PERSONAL FM TECHNOLOGY/ROSENBERG 317

10. Bellis T. Subprofiles of central auditory processingdisorders. Ed Audiol Rev 1999;16(2):4–9

11. Smaldino J, Crandell C. Speech perception in theclassroom. In: Crandell C, Smaldino J, eds. Class-room Acoustics: Understanding Barriers to Learn-ing. The Volta Rev 2001; 101:15–21

12. Crandell C, Smaldino J. Room acoustics for listenerswith normal-hearing and hearing impairment. In:Valente M, Hosford-Dunn H, Roeser R, eds. Audi-ology:Treatment. New York:Thieme; 2000:601–623

13. Finitzo-Hieber T, Tillman T. Room acoustic ef-fects on monosyllabic word discrimination abilityfor normal hearing and hearing impaired children.J Speech Hear Res 1978;21:440–458

14. Nelson, P, Soli, S. Acoustical barriers to learning:children at risk in every classroom. Lang SpeechHear Serv School 2000;31:356–361

15. Classroom Acoustics Working Group. Improvingacoustics in American schools: working draft ofstandard. Lang Speech Hear Serv School 2000;31:391–393

16. Crandell C, Smaldino J. Acoustical modificationsfor the classroom. In: Crandell C, Smaldino J, eds.Classroom Acoustics: Understanding Barriers toLearning. The Volta Rev 2001;101:33–46

17. Sieben G, Gold M, Sieben G, Ermann, M. Tenways to provide a high-quality acoustical environ-

ment in schools. Lang Speech Hear Serv School2000;31:376–384

18. Improving Classroom Acoustics: Inservice Train-ing Manual. Tallahassee, FL: Florida Departmentof Education; 1995

19. Educational Audiology Association. FAQs aboutclassroom acoustics. Ed Audiol Review 2000;17:8–9

20. Chermak GD, Musiek FE. Central Auditory Pro-cessing Disorders: New Perspectives. San Diego,CA: Singular; 1997

21. ASHA. Guidelines for fitting and monitoring FMsystems. ASHA Desk Reference. Rockville, MD:ASHA; 2000

22. Cockburn J. Listening Environment Profile. Peta-luma, CA: Phonic Ear, Inc.; 1995

23. Anderson K. Screening Instrument for TargetingEducational Risk (S.I.F.T.E.R.). Tampa, FL: Edu-cational Audiology Association; 1989

24. Fisher L. Fisher’s Auditory Problems Checklist.Tampa, FL: Ed Aud Assn; 1976

25. Smoski W, Brunt M, Tannahill J. Children’s Audi-tory Performance Scale. Tampa, FL: Ed Aud Assn;1998

26. Johnson C, Von Almen P. The functional listeningevaluation. In: Johnson C, Benson P, Seaton J, eds.Educational Audiology Handbook. San Diego.CA: Singular; 1996:336–339

Page 82: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders
Page 83: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

319

Management of Auditory Processing Disorders; Editor in Chief, Catherine V. Palmer, Ph.D.; Guest Editor, Gail D.Chermak, Ph.D. Seminars in Hearing, volume 23, number 4, 2002. Address for correspondence and reprint requests:Jeanane M. Ferre, Ph.D., Private Practice, 1010 Lake Street, Suite 111, Oak Park, IL 60301. E-mail: [email protected] Practice, Oak Park, Illinois. Copyright © 2002 by Thieme Medical Publishers, Inc., 333 Seventh Avenue, NewYork, NY 10001, USA. Tel: +1(212) 584-4662. 00734-0451,p;2002,23,04,319,326,ftx,en;sih00225x.

Managing Children’s Central AuditoryProcessing Deficits in the Real World: WhatTeachers and Parents Want to KnowJeanane M. Ferre, Ph.D.1

ABSTRACT

A diagnosis of (central) auditory processing disorder has beenmade. What do you tell the child’s teacher(s) regarding management of thedisorder’s effects in the classroom? How do you get the parents to under-stand and become involved in the management process? This article de-scribes environmental modifications and curriculum-based compensationsfor specific types of central auditory processing deficits (CAPD) as well ascommon games and everyday activities that can improve auditory and au-ditory-related skills to create long-term benefit for children with CAPD.

KEYWORDS: Central auditory processing, management,environmental modifications

Learning Outcomes: Upon reading this article, the reader will be able to (1) list at least five specific managementstrategies appropriate for various types of CAPD, (2) list at least five common games or activities that have reme-dial benefit for various types of CAPD, and (3) describe the use of assistive technology for students with CAPD.

of any kind is to allow appropriate manage-ment to be devised. This is true with respect tochildren’s central auditory processing deficits(CAPD). As Myklebust1 eloquently pointedout, “unless a differential diagnosis is made,[children’s] potentialities are lost because dif-ferent types of problems will result in differentlanguage-learning-listening needs.”

The converse is equally true. That is, chil-dren presenting with similar language-learning-

The practical importance of making a correctdiagnosis is that children having different types ofproblems vary significantly in their needs and un-less a differential diagnosis is made, their poten-tialities are lost.

H. Myklebust, 1954

These words, written nearly 50 years ago,are still true today. It generally is accepted thata key reason for conducting diagnostic testing

Page 84: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

320 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

listening needs may do so for different reasons.Two children may each be poor at following di-rections in the classroom and both may presentwith diagnosed (central) auditory processing dis-order. However, we need to manage the poordirection following of each one differently, de-pending upon the type of central auditory deficitidentified through diagnostic testing. As moreremediation programs appear on the market, allpromising to improve an array of processingand related skills, it is obvious that specifyingthe nature of CAPD is important in choosingthe right remediation.

Direct therapeutic remediation is one thirdof the management equation. Like a tripod thatcannot stand without all three legs, effectivemanagement of children’s CAPD requires theinclusion of appropriate environmental modi-fications and compensatory strategies. It is nolonger acceptable to make blanket recommen-dations such as “reduce extraneous noise, sit nearthe front of the room, or repeat and rephrase toimprove signal quality” for each and every childpresenting with CAPD. Just as we choose atherapy program that appears to be a best fit,these seemingly straightforward recommenda-tions for everyday management must be turnedon their ear and made more specific based onthe diagnostic information obtained through thecentral auditory evaluation. Parents and teach-ers need to know how to provide daily inter-vention. This kind of intervention does notrefer to the individualized pull-out sessions withthe speech-language pathologist or educationalspecialist. Rather, it is intervention that com-plements the efforts of specialists and is on-going throughout the day, at school, and athome. These are strategies that parents andteachers can use to modify the way they talk toand with our children, that help them choosethe games they play, and/or set up classroomsto enable all children to meet their potential.

This article focuses on common environ-mental modifications and curriculum-basedcompensations for specific types of CAPD.Common games and everyday activities usefulfor improving auditory and auditory-relatedprocessing skills are suggested. The reader isreferred to other publications,2,3 as well as toChermak and Musiek, and to Musiek, Shinn,and Hare (this issue, pages 297–308 and pages

263–276, respectively) for a discussion of directremediation approaches, and to Baran (pages327–336) for a discussion of management ap-proaches with older children and adults.

SPECIFIC CENTRAL AUDITORYPROCESSING DEFICITS

The Bellis-Ferre model4 delineates five types ofCAPD based on key central auditory test find-ings, and describes typical behavioral manifes-tations associated with each deficit. The modeldescribes three primary central auditory deficitscharacterized by presumed underlying site ofdysfunction. Two secondary deficit types are in-cluded that, while yielding unique patterns ofresults on central auditory tests, may be appro-priately described as manifestations of moresupramodal or cognitive-linguistic disorder. Fora detailed discussion of these deficits, the readeris referred to the article by Bellis (this issue,pages 287–296). For the purposes of this dis-cussion, the deficits are described briefly here.

Auditory decoding deficit is characterizedby poor discrimination of fine acoustic dif-ferences in speech with behavioral characteris-tics similar to those observed among childrenwith peripheral hearing loss. Auditory decod-ing deficit can create secondary difficulties incommunication (e.g., vocabulary, syntax, seman-tics, and/or second language acquisition) and/oracademic skills (e.g., reading decoding, spell-ing, notetaking, and/or direction following).

Integration deficit likely is due to ineffi-cient interhemispheric communication and ischaracterized by deficiency in the ability toperform tasks that require intersensory and/ormultisensory communication. The child doesnot synthesize information well, may complainthat there is too much information, and hasdifficulty intuiting task demands, starting com-plex tasks, transitioning from task to task, orcompleting tasks in a timely fashion. Impact oncommunication is variable, and academic diffi-culties in reading, spelling, writing, and otherintegrative tasks are typically observed.

Prosodic deficit is characterized by defi-ciency in using prosodic features of a target, apredominantly right hemisphere function. Thechild displays difficulty in auditory pattern

Page 85: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

MANAGING CHILDREN’S CAPD/FERRE 321

recognition, which is important for perceivingrunning speech, and may have difficulty recog-nizing and using other sensory patterns (e.g.,visual, tactile). Difficulties are observed withpragmatic language (e.g., reading facial expres-sions, body language, gestures; recognizing orusing sarcasm or heteronyms), rhythm percep-tion, music, and nonverbal learning.

Auditory associative deficit, a secondaryCAPD subtype, is characterized by significantauditory-language processing difficulties. Chil-dren with this deficit do not intuit the rules oflanguage as well as theirs peers. While normalprocessors too often do not think outside the box,these children rarely are in the box; the box beingthose rules of language, both explicit and im-plicit, that we use to get the message. They mayexhibit specific language impairments in syntax,vocabulary, semantics, verbal and/or written ex-pression, pragmatics, or social communication.More importantly, though, they exhibit func-tional communication deficits even in the ab-sence of specific language impairment. A keybehavioral characteristic is a finding of adequateacademic performance in early elementarygrades with increasing difficulty as linguistic de-mands increase in upper grades. This child maypresent with sub-average to sub-normal intel-lectual potential when assessed using standard(language-biased) intelligence tests.

Output-organization deficit is anothersecondary subtype showing a unique pattern oncentral auditory processing tests. This deficit ischaracterized by difficulty in performing tasksrequiring efficient motor path transmission/motor planning and may be a behavioral mani-festation of impaired efferent function or plan-ning/executive function deficit. Behaviorally,the child may be disorganized, impulsive, and apoor planner/problem solver. Difficulties inexpressive language, articulation, and syntacticskills may be observed as well as educationalproblems in direction following, note-taking,and remembering assignments.

CAPD MANAGEMENT ISSUES

As noted previously, effective management ofCAPD requires environmental modifications,use of compensatory strategies, and direct re-

mediation. Direct remediation seeks to improvedeficient skills and to teach additional compen-satory/ coping strategies. Environmental modi-fications and use of compensatory strategiesminimize the impact of the deficit on everydayfunctions. The environmental modifications andcompensatory strategies discussed here focuson the impact of noise, changes in oral messagepresentation (e.g., repetition and rephrasing),classroom seating and use of visual cues, andmodifications to the curriculum including pre-viewing and scheduling.

Impact of Extraneous Noise

A listener’s ability to function in backgroundnoise depends on: (1) the type of noise present,(2) the loudness of the noise relative to the tar-get, and (3) task demands.5 Any listener willbenefit from reduction of extraneous noise inthe listening environment. The specific impactof noise and management of a noisy environ-ment depends on the central auditory deficitidentified.

Children with auditory decoding deficittend to perform more poorly as noise increasesrelative to the target and when the backgroundnoise is more similar to the target. The gymna-sium, lunchroom, and playgrounds are areasof particular difficulty. For this child, it is im-perative that excessively loud and/or highly re-verberant noise be minimized either throughdirect signal enhancement or noise abatementtechniques.

Direct signal enhancement can be achievedthrough the use of assistive listening devices(ALD) in which the signal-to-noise ratio (SNR)reaching the child’s ear is improved. Unliketraditional amplification where the speech sig-nal and any ambient noise may both be ampli-fied, the physical configuration of the personalALD has the effect of pulling the target signalaway from the noise via very mild gain amplifi-cation, thereby pushing the noise further intothe perceptual background. This results in asignificantly more favorable SNR and, in turn,improved speech reception. Likewise, sound-field FM (i.e., frequency modulated) systemsenhance the target signal for groups of chil-dren. When used with proper diagnosis and

Page 86: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

322 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

monitoring, a personal ALD can improve au-ditory attention, short term memory, auditorydiscrimination and perception6–9 and would beappropriate for a child with auditory decodingdeficit.10,11

Recommended classroom SNR should ex-ceed +15 dB with reverberation time of lessthan 0.4 seconds.12 Classroom noise abatementcan be accomplished by carpeting rooms, usingcurtains, drapes and/or acoustic ceiling tiles,placement of baffles within the listening space,eliminating open classrooms, or damping highlyreflective surfaces. Placing bookcases perpen-dicular to each other or creating a 6–8 inchspace between side-by-side bookcases can cre-ate baffles and minimize noise. Cork bulletinboards and the use of fabric to cover hard sur-faces increases sound absorption and dampensreflective surfaces. Felt pads on the bottoms ofchair and table legs minimize furniture-to-floornoise. For a discussion of classroom acousticsand FM technology, see the articles by Rosen-berg (this issue, pages 309–318), as well otherpublications.13,14

In contrast to the child with auditory de-coding deficit where SNR is central to im-proved performance, children with integrationand prosodic deficits are bothered by noise—not as a function of its level, but as a functionof the task demands. While performing simpletasks with which they are highly familiar, noiseis of little consequence; however, in a test-takingor study situation with its increased multisen-sory demands, even simple random noises suchas coughing, pencil sharpening, or a ringingtelephone may be intrusive and too loud. Be-cause of the nature of the deficit, ALDs areusually not beneficial. Instead, in these situa-tions, the task demands should be altered, thechild should be removed from the environ-ment, or a way found to mask the random every-day noises that intrude. Taking frequent breakswhile studying, extending test or project com-pletion time, and reducing assignment size canlessen potential processing overload, therebyimproving tolerance for noise. Use of separaterooms for test taking, study carrels, and smallerclass size also would reduce the noise that mayundermine the listener’s processing. Consider-ation should be given to having these children

use noise-reducing earplugs while studying toattenuate background school and householdnoise. Conversely, many children with integra-tion deficits benefit from listening to music orother steady-state signals as a way to mask ran-dom, aperiodic noises that may disrupt con-centration.

The child with output-organization deficitgenerally has difficulty filtering out distrac-tions of any kind, not just auditory noise. Envi-ronments that are visually and/or auditorilychaotic are especially difficult for this child.This child may benefit from use of an ALD orfrom noise desensitization activities to improvetolerance and filtering abilities. The reader isreferred to Ferre11 and Garstecki15 for a furtherdiscussion of noise desensitization. For a childwith associative deficit, background noise maybe no more bothersome than for normal proces-sors, provided the child understands the mes-sage and the rules. One recent client could dorepetitive, soccer practice drills without anythought to the background noise; however, hecomplained that it was too noisy when involvedin a real soccer match in the same environ-ment. For him, soccer practice was easy to un-derstand and not very demanding; however, hewas overwhelmed by the constantly changingdemands of a real soccer game and could nottolerate the game’s attendant noise. Like thechild with integration deficit, the noise levelwas not central to his difficulties and directsignal enhancement (e.g., ALD) was not rec-ommended. Instead, intervention focused onimproving his ability to understand the rules ofthe game as opposed to the rules of a practice.Other noise management techniques for thechild with associative deficit involve improvingthe signal’s linguistic clarity rather than itsacoustic clarity. Linguistic clarity is discussedbelow.

Changes In Oral Message Presentation

Altering message presentation typically has in-volved recommendations to speak more slowlyto the child or to repeat or rephrase informa-tion. While all are reasonable, each carries acodicil. It often is not necessary to actually

Page 87: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

MANAGING CHILDREN’S CAPD/FERRE 323

slow down one’s speech to enhance it. Rather,altering the timing, pacing, emphasis and pars-ing of the message can enhance the salience ofthe key parts. These modifications benefit stu-dents with auditory decoding or prosodic deficit.In addition, speaking more slowly will be anatural consequence of greater articulatory pre-cision. Repetition of the signal is most helpfulif the repeated message is enhanced or empha-sized in some fashion, spoken with greater pre-cision, or accompanied by a related visual ortactile cue (e.g., pointing, gesture).

For the child with an associative deficit,repetition rarely produces an improvement inunderstanding. For these students, rephrasing—by offering the listener a more familiar tar-get—is the method of choice when the mes-sage has been misunderstood linguistically. Inaddition, this child requires the use of explicitlanguage when given commands or asked ques-tions. The ambiguous sit still should be replacedwith keep your hands (or feet, or head) still. Forthis child, it is essential that we say what wemean and mean what we say.To gauge the child’sunderstanding, replace “do you understand?”with “tell me what you think I said.” That is, askthe child to paraphrase instructions or state-ments rather than repeating them verbatim.

Target rephrasing is counterproductive ifthe processing deficit is related to issues of in-tegration (i.e., how quickly and efficiently thetarget was synthesized) or output (i.e., how ef-ficiently targets were organized). For these lis-teners, rephrasing adds additional perceptualtargets rather than enhancing signal clarity.Rephrasing to these children is confusing as itgives them too much information to which tolisten and to organize. For these children, wemust remember two simple rules: (1) keep itshort and simple (KISS), and (2) don’t say it;show me.

Use of Visual Cues and Preferential

Classroom Seating

In preferential seating, an effort is made tomaximize both the acoustic and visual aspectsof the target based on the presumption that theaddition of visual cues will enhance the listen-

ing experience. For the child with auditory de-coding deficit, the addition of visual cues isimportant and the child should be encouragedto look and listen and may be taught lipreading/speechreading skills. However, for the childwhose ability to integrate auditory and visualinformation is compromised (integration defi-cit), the addition of visual cues may create fur-ther confusion. For this child, the look and lis-ten rule should be modified to look or listen.10,11

In the classroom, children are asked to copymaterial from a board or screen while simulta-neously listening to related information. Inthese situations, allow the child with integra-tion or output-organization difficulties to lookthen listen (i.e., sequential processing) in orderto minimize potential overload on his or hersynthesis and/or organization skills.10,11 Forother children, classroom seating is of less im-portance than the method of presentation. Thechild with a prosodic deficit requires an ani-mated teacher with a melodic voice who usesmultisensory techniques to make the point. Forthe child with an associative deficit, additionalvisual cues may take the form of notes writtenin margins, highlighted text, diagrams, or othervisual aids that help the child to visualize theconcept or that make the message more ex-plicit and familiar. The child with organiza-tional difficulties may need outlines or check-lists to assist planning and recall skills.

Modifications to the Curriculum

Pre-teaching or previewing material is de-signed to enhance familiarity with the target.For any listener, the more familiar one is withthe target, the easier the processing becomes.Books on tape, copies of teachers’ notes/texts,Cliffs Notes™ (Hungry Minds, Inc.), seeingmovies, and reading aloud to children can en-hance their familiarity with the subject, taskdemands, main ideas, key elements, and vocab-ulary. For children with integration, associa-tive, prosodic, or output-organization deficit,knowledge of the rules, structure, and task de-mands up front will minimize overload. How-ever, it is important to note that for these chil-dren this knowledge is not acquired through

Page 88: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

324 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

mere exposure, but through explicit instruc-tion, repeated practice, and review across a va-riety of contexts and settings.

Many children with CAPD, regardless ofspecific type, report experiencing excessive au-ditory fatigue. Scheduling breaks in the listen-ing day helps to minimize this reported audi-tory overload. For example, do not schedule areading lesson right after an English grammarlesson. Intersperse lecture classes with activi-ties that are more hands-on or less academi-cally challenging (e.g., physical education). Donot schedule homework or therapy immedi-ately after school. Instead schedule some min-utes of downtime following school.

When determining particular courses orcurricular requirements, the presence of a CAPDshould be considered. Students with decodingdeficit should consider a nonverbal alternativefor foreign language requirements. Many statesnot only recognize American Sign Languageas a foreign language, but also accept it forcredit at the high school and collegiate levels.16

Children with associative deficit should begranted a waiver or substitution (e.g., a culturecourse) for a foreign language requirement dueto the nature of their auditory-language diffi-culties. Because children with integration orprosodic deficit are deficient in their ability tointuit multisensory or unisensory patterns, theforeign language chosen should minimize thedemands on these skills. Rather than a nonver-bal language, consider Hebrew or Latin as asecond language for these children. These lan-guages are highly structured and well ordered.In addition, they tend to be read-only languages,at least at the elementary and secondary levels.Finally, regardless of the second language cho-sen for students with output-organization defi-cit, we should consider and minimize any po-tential adverse impact on their expressive speechand language (i.e., verbal and written), recall,and planning skills by allowing dictated re-sponses on written exams, and minimizing orallanguage exams.

Children with integration, associative, pro-sodic, or output-organization deficit shouldavoid timed tests, as these underestimate trueknowledge and skills. In general, tests withclosed-set questions (i.e., multiple choice or fill-

in) rather than those with more open-endedquestions (e.g., essays) are preferred for thesechildren. For children with integration or out-put-organization deficit, it may be necessary toallow them to write in a test booklet rather thantransfer answers to a score sheet, especially forstandardized testing. Because of its impact onlanguage skills, associative deficit may result in achild performing below his or her potential onmany tests, especially standard language-biasedIQ tests. Non-language biased instruments (e.g.,Test of Nonverbal Intelligence or Leiter Perfor-mance Inventory) should be used to gain a morereliable estimate of the child’s potential. Forother tests, it may be necessary to read the ques-tions to the student to ensure understanding.

Management Issues Summary

Children with auditory decoding deficit bene-fit from modifications and compensations thatfocus on improving the quality and access tothe acoustic signal (e.g., repetition, preferentialseating, noise abatement, and use of ALD ortape recorder). Children with integration deficitwho do not synthesize information as quicklyand efficiently as other children benefit frommodifications and compensations that adjustthe quantity and structure of the signal (e.g.,repetition with related cues, short and simpleinstructions, pre-teaching material, untimed andclosed set tests, highly structured second lan-guage). Children with associative deficit do notspeak the same language as their peers and re-quire modifications and compensations thattransform the signal to improve comprehension(e.g., rephrasing, use of unambiguous language,pre-teaching rules and vocabulary, second lan-guage substitution or waiver, non-language testinstruments). Children with prosodic deficithave difficulty recognizing and attaching mean-ing to auditory patterns and benefit from mod-ifications that improve both the structure andmeaning of the signal (e.g., repetition with em-phasis, knowledge of rules, untimed activities,highly structured second language). Finally,students with output-organization deficit havedifficulty organizing information. These chil-dren benefit from modifications and compen-

Page 89: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

MANAGING CHILDREN’S CAPD/FERRE 325

§Clever Endeavor is a registered trademark of MindGames, Inc.; Plexers is a registered trademark of Plexers,Inc.; Password is a registered trademark of Mark Good-son Productions, LLC.; Wheel of Fortune is a registeredtrademark of Merv Griffin Enterprises, LLC.; all othergames listed are registered trademarks of Hasbro, Inc.

*Boggle, Scattergories, Simon, Scrabble, Bopit, Bopit Ex-treme, Catch Phrase, Brain Warp, Taboo, and UpWords areregistered trademarks of Hasbro, Inc.; Rummikub is aregistered trademark of Pressman Toy Corporation.

sations that enhance signal salience (e.g., use ofALDs, note-taking service, pre-teaching rulesand outlines, closed-set tests) and assist outputskills (e.g., oral responses for written exams,extended time tests).

THE GAMES WE PLAY

Like their nonimpaired peers, everyday gamesand activities can enhance the auditory and re-lated processing skills of children with CAPD.For improved auditory decoding and generallistening skills, the child should be involved inactivities that teach or require active listeningor discrimination and analysis for success. Com-mon games and activities include: telephonegame, rhyming games, start-stop games (e.g.,Red Light- Green Light), identification of same-different, or sound recognition games (e.g., thechild says a word that begins with the last soundof your word: caT – Tack, doG – Gum).

For improved integration, prosody, or or-ganizational skills games and activities that re-quire synthesis (e.g., parts-to-whole), patternrecognition, increase in complexity or speed ofresponse, or require verbal mediation for suc-cess should be used. Examples include name thattune, feely bag (e.g., place several common ob-jects in a bag, the child names or describes theobjects through touch only), Scrabble®, Bog-gle®, UpWords®, Rummikub®, Bopit®, BopitExtreme®, Brain Warp®, Simon®*, simon says,card games, chess, karate, dance, singing, dra-matic arts, and gymnastics.

For improved associative or output skills, ac-tivities that encourage out of the box thinking,build vocabulary, enhance use of linguistic rulesor concepts, and improve problem-solving skillsshould be used. Common games in this areainclude Catch Phrase®, Scattergories®, Up-Words®, Taboo®, Clever Endeavor®, alphabetiz-ing games, rebus puzzles, word puzzles (e.g.,Plexers™), Password®, Boggle®, Scrabble®,

Wheel of Fortune®§, anagrams or scrambledwords, change-a-letter games (e.g., turn the wordback into the word soak by changing one letter at atime), or words-in-a-word game (e.g., make tenwords out of the letters in the word cafeteria).

SUMMARY

Children with CAPD have a variety of prob-lems and vary significantly in their educational,communicative, and psychosocial needs. It isimportant to differentially diagnose the prob-lem to understand those particular needs. It alsois essential that we as clinicians differentiallymanage those needs not only in our choices forremediation, but also in the ways we changethe environment to minimize the adverse ef-fects of the deficit and in how we assist thechild in compensating for his or her deficits.By making recommendations more deficit-specific and infusing this deficit-specificity intoeveryday activities throughout the entire day,we can minimize the adverse effects of thesedeficits on the child’s life and ensure that po-tentialities are not lost.

ABBREVIATIONS

ALD assistive listening devicesCAPD central auditory processing deficitsFM frequency modulatedSNR signal-to-noise ratio

REFERENCES

1. Myklebust H. Auditory Disorders in Children.New York: Grune & Stratton; 1954

2. Masters G, Stecker N, Katz J, eds. Central Audi-tory Processing Disorders: Mostly Management.Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon; 1998

Page 90: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

326 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

3. Ferre J. Behavioral therapeutic approaches for cen-tral auditory problems. In: Katz J, ed. Handbook ofClinical Audiology, 5th ed. Philadelphia, PA: Lip-pincott Williams & Wilkins; 2002:525–531

4. Bellis T, Ferre J. Multidimensional approach to dif-ferential diagnosis of central auditory processingdisorders in children. J Am Acad Audiol 1999;10:319–328

5. Crandell C, Smaldino J. Room acoustics and audi-tory rehabilitation technology. In: Katz J, ed. Hand-book of Clinical Audiology, 5th ed. Philadelphia,PA: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins; 2002:607–630

6. Shapiro A, Mistal G. ITE-aid auditory training forreading and spelling-disabled children: clinical casestudies. Hear J 1985;38:14–16

7. Shapiro A, Mistal G. ITE-aid auditory training forreading and spelling-disabled children: a longitu-dinal study of matched groups. Hear J 1986;39:14–16

8. Stach B, Loiselle L, Jerger J, Mintz S, Taylor C.Clinical experience with personal FM assistive lis-tening devices. Hear J 1987;40:24–30

9. Blake R, Field B, Foster C, Plott F, Wertz P. Effectof FM auditory trainers on attending behaviors oflearning-disabled children. Lang Speech Hear ServSchool 1991;22:111–114

10. Bellis T. Assessment and Management of CentralAuditory Processing Disorders in the EducationalSetting. San Diego, CA: Singular; 1996

11. Ferre J. Processing Power: A Guide to CAPD As-sessment and Management. San Antonio, TX: ThePsychological Corp; 1997

12. American Speech Language Hearing Association.Guidelines for acoustics in educational environ-ments. ASHA 1995:37(Suppl 14)

13. Stein R. Application of FM technology to themanagement of central auditory processing disor-ders. In: Masters G, Stecker N, Katz J, eds. CentralAuditory Processing Disorders: Mostly Manage-ment. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon; 1998:89–102

14. Crandell C, Smaldino J. Sound field amplificationin the classroom: applied & theoretical issues. In:Bess F, Gravel J, Tharpe A, eds. Amplification forChildren with Auditory Deficits. Nashville, TN:Bill Wilkerson Center Press; 1996:229–250

15. Garstecki D. Auditory-visual training paradigmfor hearing-impaired adults. J Acad Rehab Audiol;1981;14:223–238

16. National Information Center on Deafness. Statesthat Recognize American Sign Language as a For-eign Language. Washington, DC: Gallaudet Uni-versity, Author; 2001

Page 91: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

327

Management of Auditory Processing Disorders; Editor in Chief, Catherine V. Palmer, Ph.D.; Guest Editor, Gail D.Chermak, Ph.D. Seminars in Hearing, volume 23, number 4, 2002. Address for correspondence and reprint requests: JaneA. Baran, Ph.D., Department of Communication Disorders, University of Massachusetts, 715 North Pleasant Street,Amherst, MA 01003–9304. E-mail: [email protected]. 1Department of Communication Disorders, University ofMassachusetts, Amherst, Massachusetts. Copyright © 2002 by Thieme Medical Publishers, Inc., 333 Seventh Avenue,New York, NY 10001, USA. Tel: +1(212) 584-4662. 00734-0451,p;2002,23,04,327,336,ftx,en;sih00226x.

Managing Auditory Processing Disorders inAdolescents and AdultsJane A. Baran, Ph.D.1

ABSTRACT

This article provides an overview of the nature of auditory pro-cessing disorders (APD) in adolescents and adults and provides a frame-work for professionals working with patients across the lifespan. Threemajor approaches to the management of the patient with APD are dis-cussed, with emphasis placed on those approaches that incorporate bothecological and context-specific perspectives in the management of the ado-lescent and adult with APD. Also discussed are implications of APD forthe fitting of hearing aids and other amplification devices in patients withcomorbid peripheral hearing losses.

KEYWORDS: Auditory processing disorder, adolescents and adults,rehabilitation

Learning Outcomes: Upon completion of this article, the reader will be able to (1) identify the major ap-proaches to the management of auditory processing disorders in adolescents and adults, and (2) discuss theimplications of a comorbid auditory processing disorder when fitting hearing aids and other amplification de-vices to patients with peripheral hearing losses.

processing deficits observed in patients withboth developmental and acquired APD.3–6 Morerecently, efforts have been directed toward thedevelopment of management programs to meetthe needs of these individuals.4,5,7–14

A review of the literature in this area re-veals that most of the efforts directed towardthe development of management proceduresand programs for APD have focused on the re-

The diagnosis of auditory processing dis-orders (APD) has received considerable atten-tion in both the clinical and research arenassince the mid 1950s, when Bocca and his col-leagues reported the results of their landmarkinvestigation of the auditory deficits associatedwith temporal lobe compromise.1,2 Since thattime, considerable attention has been directedtoward defining the nature and the extent of the

Page 92: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

328 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

habilitative needs of children, with consider-ably less attention being paid to the develop-ment of management programs specifically de-signed to meet the needs of the older patientwith APD.7 This situation is presumed to berelated to a number of factors. Most notableamong these factors is the large increase in thenumber of children with language and learningproblems who have been referred for APDassessments, and who subsequently have beendiagnosed as having APD over the past twodecades. With the increase in the number ofchildren diagnosed with APD has come pres-sure to develop efficacious management pro-grams to serve the needs of this population.The more recent desire to turn our attention tothe rehabilitative needs of the adolescent andadult with APD has been motivated by twofactors. First, the school-aged children whowere identified as having APD in the last dec-ade or two are now maturing into adolescentsand adults with APD; and second, with anincreased awareness of APD on the part ofthe public, many previously undiagnosed adultswith APD are seeking evaluation, and as a re-sult, are being diagnosed with this disorder.This growing number of older individuals withconfirmed APD presents new challenges forthose professionals who are responsible for de-signing management programs to meet theirneeds.

PATIENT CHARACTERISTICS

In attempting to serve the needs of adolescentsand adults with APD, the audiologist is likelyto encounter four groups of patients. These in-clude (1) individuals who were identified at anearly age with APD and who received sometype of intervention for these auditory deficitsin the past, (2) individuals who present for thefirst time as adults with significant auditorycomplaints in the absence of other significantfindings—many of these individuals may havehad difficulties in the past that were addressedby teachers and/or parents who were able toprovide the individualized attention and care

needed to deal with these difficulties in the ab-sence of a specific diagnosis, (3) individuals withAPD associated with confirmed compromiseof the central auditory nervous system (CANS),and (4) older individuals who are experiencingnormal degenerative processes associated withaging. Many of these individuals, especiallythose in the first two groups, will have devel-oped compensatory strategies that served themwell when they were younger; however, as theymatured into adulthood, some of these indi-viduals will have found that the strategies thatproved to be useful when they were youngerfailed to meet their needs as adults. As chil-dren, these individuals would have been con-figured within an ecological system as learnersand they would have been defined relative totheir cognitive and learning abilities. As adults,their ecological systems expand and their audi-tory deficits become apparent across multiplesettings (e.g., employment, leisure, family, com-munity life).15 The older individual is no longerdefined primarily on the basis of his or her per-formance in school. Contexts and environ-ments change as do communicative demandsand as a result the adult with APD may facenew challenges that will vary across time andcontext.

COMORBIDITY

Although APD may exist in isolation, it isoften the case that the patient with APD alsoexperiences one or more additional comorbidconditions. These comorbid conditions may in-clude speech and language disorders, learningdisabilities, attention deficit disorders with orwithout hyperactivity, psychological disorders,emotional disorders, frank neurological involve-ment of the CANS, and peripheral hearingloss.To date, there have been numerous attemptsto establish cause and affect relationships be-tween each of these disorders and APD; how-ever, these efforts have failed to elucidate theexact nature of these relationships—which areby nature complex, interrelated, and not neces-sarily unidirectional. Also, a consideration of

Page 93: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

MANAGING APD IN ADOLESCENTS AND ADULTS/BARAN 329

Table 1 Common Presenting

Symptomatology Associated with

Auditory Processing Disorders and Related

Disorders in Adolescents and Adults

Inordinate difficulty hearing in noisy or reverberant environments

Lack of music appreciationDifficulty following conversations on the telephoneDifficulty following multi-step directions/instructionsDifficulty taking notes during lecturesDifficulty following long conversationsDifficulty learning a foreign language Difficulty learning technical or discipline-specific

vocabulary where the language is largely unfamiliar or novel

Difficulty in directing, sustaining, or dividing attention

Auditory memory deficitsSpelling difficultiesReading difficultiesOrganizational problemsBehavioral, psychological and/or social problemsAcademic or vocational difficulties

the presenting symptomatology associated withAPD and many of these comorbid disorders re-veals that a number of behavioral symptomsare shared by several of these disorders (Table 1).

Although the establishment of cause andeffect relationships among these disorders maybe desirable from a differential diagnostic per-spective, what is more critical to the successfulmanagement of the individual with comorbidconditions is the identification of these co-existing deficits and problems. Once these defi-cits are uncovered, a comprehensive manage-ment plan can be developed that will addressall areas of weakness. Moreover, since the be-havioral deficits associated with some of thesedisorders may negatively affect the efficacy of aparticular APD approach for a given patient, itis important that a comprehensive, and prefer-ably interdisciplinary, assessment of the individ-ual’s auditory, linguistic, cognitive, academic andvocational functioning be undertaken beforean APD management plan is developed when-ever deficits or problems are anticipated in anyof these areas.

MANAGEMENT APPROACHES

Management approaches used to remediate oralleviate the auditory deficits associated withAPD can be categorized into three major cate-gories. These approaches are designed to accom-plish the following goals: (1) to improve signalquality, (2) to improve the individual’s auditoryperceptual skills, and (3) to enhance the individ-ual’s language and cognitive skills.16 An alterna-tive classification system divides the proceduresinto two categories based on the nature of themechanisms that underlie the processing require-ments involved. In this classification system, theapproaches would be classified as either bottom-up (i.e., stimulus-driven) or top-down (i.e., con-cept-driven) procedures.5 Bottom-up procedureswould encompass those approaches designed tofacilitate the individual’s ability to receive andprocess the acoustic signal, whereas top-downapproaches would involve those procedures thatare designed to facilitate the interpretation of au-ditory information according to linguistic rulesand conventions and other available sensory in-formation. Although there is not necessarily aone-to-one fit, for the most part approaches thatare designed to improve the signal quality or toimprove the individual’s auditory perceptual skillswould be classified as bottom-up or stimulus-driven procedures, whereas approaches that areused to enhance the individual’s linguistic andcognitive skills would be classified as top-downor concept-driven procedures.

Although many of the management op-tions used with children can and have been usedwith older individuals, adolescents and adultswith APD often present unique challenges andspecial needs. Brain plasticity, that is optimalduring childhood, is less amenable to signifi-cant alternation in the older individual.5,7 Hence,there may be less reliance on formal and infor-mal approaches specifically designed to im-prove the individual’s auditory perceptual skillswhen the individual in need of services is older.In these cases, rehabilitative efforts are morelikely to focus on the other two managementstrategies (i.e., improving the signal quality andenhancing the individual’s cognitive and lin-guistic resources). In addition, academic, voca-

Page 94: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

330 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

Table 2 Examples of Academic Modifications

for Students in Secondary and Postsecondary

Academic Placements

Test administration in alternative environments (e.g., less noise, fewer distractions)

Provision of notetaking servicesProvision of tutoring services (may include peer

tutoring)Reduced course loadsCreative scheduling of classes to ensure that course

requirements and task demands are equally distributed over the course of the academic program

Preferential seating (if seating is assigned)Tape recordings of lectures and presentationsVideo recordings of lectures and presentationsAssignment or selection of course section where

instructor’s teaching philosophy and style are consistent with student’s needs

Provision of outlines, lecture notes, or reading assignments prior to class presentation so that thestudent can review materials in advance

Other accommodations to address comorbid-learning difficulties (e.g., untimed tests, alternativetest formats)

tional, psychological and emotional issues oftensurface and/or become more severe as the indi-vidual with APD matures and as auditory pro-cessing demands increase in new and variedlistening contexts. Individuals who have metwith success as a result of previous interventionefforts may now find that strategies that servedthem well in earlier years fail to provide themwith the assistance needed to face the challengesthey experience as adolescents and young adults.Additional management options, such as aca-demic or vocational modifications (Table 2),psychological counseling, career counseling, andtransition planning must be entertained. Hence,there are a number of additional issues andconsiderations that need to be addressed whenplanning a management program for adoles-cents and adults. Some of these may be appro-priately managed by the audiologist, whereasother services may be more efficiently providedby other professionals in related areas. As wasthe case for assessment, an interdisciplinary or

transdisciplinary approach to management ofAPD is recommended.

Auditory Training

Auditory training (AT) approaches can involveboth formal and informal techniques directedat improving the individual’s auditory percep-tual skills. Selection of the specific type of ATshould be informed by a comprehensive centralauditory assessment through which the spe-cific auditory deficit areas can be identified andthen targeted for remediation. Musiek and hiscolleagues have identified a number of audi-tory processes that can be assessed using anumber of different central auditory tests andthey have outlined specific management proce-dures that can be used to address each deficitarea.10,11 Information on these procedures aswell as a discussion of other issues and consid-erations in the selection of formal and informalauditory training procedures can be found inthese resources. The reader is referred toMusiek, Shinn, and Hare, and to Chermak andMusiek (this issue, pages 263–276, and pages297–308, respectively) for additional discus-sions of AT.

Signal Enhancement Approaches

Signal enhancement procedures involve the useof specialized equipment, environmental modi-fications and speaker training. Approaches in-clude the provision of either a personal FM(frequency-modulated) system or a classroom/group amplification system, reduction of extra-neous or competing environmental noise, pref-erential seating, enhancing the presentation ofthe message though a variety of means (e.g., useof clear speech17–19), and use of specialized tech-nology (e.g., telephone amplifiers). All of theseapproaches can be used at home, at school, or onthe job, with appropriate modifications.4,7,9–14,20

For additional discussions of personal FM sys-tems and classroom modifications, the reader isreferred to the articles by Rosenberg and by

Page 95: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

MANAGING APD IN ADOLESCENTS AND ADULTS/BARAN 331

Ferre, (this issue, pages 309–318, and pages319–326, respectively).

Linguistic and Cognitive Interventions

As the individual matures into adolescence andthen adulthood, interventions are less likely tofocus on specific skill acquisition (e.g., phono-logical awareness training) and more likely tofocus on the development of metalinguisticand metacognitive skills. Many well-conceivedstrategies have been developed and used withindividuals with APD. Specific information onthese procedures can be found in a number ofsources and will not be detailed here.4,7–13 Thereader also is referred to Bellis (this issue, pages287–296) for additional discussions of met-alinguistic and metacognitive approaches.

Although many of the strategies that havebeen recommended in the literature appear tobe obvious and therefore should require littleinstruction other than identification of the strat-egy, this may in fact not be the case. If the strat-egies were obvious to the individual with APD,then it is likely that this individual would haveadopted the use of the strategy without theneed for specific instruction. Moreover, even ifthe individual adopted a strategy that workedwell for a given situation or context in the past,but did not learn how to generalize the strategyto new situations, the individual would con-tinue to meet with communicative failure innew contexts. In light of these comments, a for-malized strategy instruction procedure shouldbe given serious consideration for incorporationinto any management plan developed for ado-lescents and adults with APD.7

ECOLOGICAL PERSPECTIVES

Auditory problems are complex and the man-ner in which they affect daily life are varied.Although individualized management plans areindicated for all patients with APD, they areespecially important for adolescents and adultswith APD. Management procedures selectedfor incorporation into a management plan

should be deficit-specific and tailored to meetthe unique learning and communication cir-cumstances and needs of the patient. Olderstudents and adults need to function in a num-ber of different contexts with varying commu-nicative demands and they typically do notenjoy the same level of support that is providedto young children. There may be no Individu-alized Education Plan (IEP) team, advocate, orparent to inform others of the difficulties thatthe individual is experiencing. Moreover, as thechild moves from an elementary school place-ment into a middle school, junior high, andhigh school placement, he or she is likely to beworking with more adults in more contexts.The student in the upper grades is expected tomove from classroom to classroom to meetwith teachers who have expertise in particularsubject matters. With each different subjectcomes a different teacher with his or her ownteaching style—which may be conducive to thechild’s learning and communication needs, ormay prove to be at odds with the child’s learn-ing style. Not only will the teaching stylechange, so will the environment within whichthe student must function. Depending on theacoustics of the classroom, its location relativeto noise sources both inside and outside of theschool building, and the size and compositionof the class, the student with APD may meetwith more or less favorable listening environ-ments as he or she moves through the day.These issues loom even larger when the indi-vidual moves from a secondary educational pro-gram to a postsecondary or technical schoolenvironment. In these settings, classes are gen-erally much larger and instructors often do nothave the opportunity to get to know each stu-dent’s learning style and needs, at least not in atimely manner. Moreover, the student may nothave an individual who can serve as an advo-cate on his or her behalf. Therefore, it is im-portant that the individual assume responsibil-ity for his or her listening success. He or sheneeds to become a self-advocate and addition-ally needs to become strategic. The simple ap-plication of a strategy that the student learnedas a younger student may not be of assistancein each new context. The older student must beable to evaluate each new listening situation,

Page 96: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

332 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

identify possible strategies to compensate forany processing difficulties that might be en-countered in the novel listening environment,select the strategy that appears to be the bestalternative given the circumstances, and findanother alternative if the selected strategy failsto meet the individual’s needs in this particularcontext. In other words, adolescents and adultswith APD need to not only know a strategy,but they need to become strategic. What workedin one environment may not prove to be usefulin another environment.

Career Counseling and

Transition Planning

Career counseling and transition planning isan often overlooked but essential componentof a management plan for the older studentwith APD. Many adolescents and young adultsdo not fully appreciate how their learning andcommunication problems may interact withjob demands. This lack of understanding mayset them up for future frustration, possible fail-ure, and loss of self-esteem as they enter thejob market. Some of this may be avoided if stu-dents are provided guidance in this areathrough the use of appropriate counseling ser-vices. Students who can identify their strengthsand weaknesses and explore the variety of ca-reer options that are open to them will be morelikely to choose a career where they can meetwith reasonable success. Often parental or so-cietal pressures push students into careerchoices for which they are ill equipped, unin-terested, and unmotivated. Counseling mayhelp the student learn how to deal with thesesocietal pressures and how to become moreself-assertive.

Even when a good match is found be-tween an individual’s interests, skills, and careeror educational choices, it is likely that individ-uals with APD will encounter new problems asthey move from one context to another. Explo-rations of the potential difficulties that may beencountered will help individuals with APDplan how they will react to these difficulties ifthey are encountered. Finally, an essential com-ponent of the management plan for individu-

als with an APD or related disorder is self-advocacy training.

Self-Advocacy Training

As individuals mature from childhood to ado-lescence and into adulthood, the likelihood thatthey will have a case manager, advocate, orother professional to advocate for their needsbecomes increasingly smaller. Young studentsoften become dependent on these individualsto advocate on their behalf because these needsare provided for the students through theirIEPs. As a consequence, many young studentsdo not develop the skills and abilities needed totake on this role as the need arises. They maystruggle in new and difficult situations ratherthan ask for assistance and reasonable accom-modations. This is particularly true at the post-secondary level, where many students may feelthat faculty are unapproachable, or that thefaculty will be biased against them if they dis-close their disorders or deficits. Without train-ing in self-advocacy skills, the postsecondarystudent may not be equipped to access the ac-commodations needed to ensure success.

Although problems related to poor devel-opment of self-advocacy skills often are en-countered in postsecondary environments, theymay be equally as problematic for the em-ployee. Employees with APD who are not ableto advocate for their own needs may find thatthey are at risk for job loss or lack of advance-ment or promotion due to perceived employeeineffectiveness. These individuals also may findthat they are not accepted by colleagues in theworkplace, as they are perceived by co-workersas aloof or antisocial.

MANAGEMENT OF THE PATIENTWITH APD AND PERIPHERALHEARING LOSS

The discussion thus far has focused on interven-tion programs and procedures designed for usewith the individual who has APD in the absenceof any involvement of the peripheral hearingsystem. However, many individuals will presentwith an APD that either co-exists with, or is

Page 97: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

MANAGING APD IN ADOLESCENTS AND ADULTS/BARAN 333

secondary to, a peripheral hearing loss. More-over, it is anticipated that the number of patientspresenting with concomitant peripheral and cen-tral auditory system involvement will continueto grow as medical advances increase life ex-pectancy and the baby boomers continue to age.

The need for careful determination of thepresence of a comorbid APD is critical to thesuccessful management of the patient with pe-ripheral hearing loss. Many of the interventionsoutlined above can be used to alleviate the audi-tory problems that these individuals experience.However, this group of individuals presentsunique challenges to the audiologist responsiblefor the selection and fitting of amplification.Current preferred practice patterns require as-sessment of the auditory periphery prior to thefitting of amplification; however, the assessmentof the integrity and function of the CANS in pa-tients with peripheral loss being seen for fittingof amplification has received little attention.23,24

Most, if not all, patients presenting withhearing loss are considered to be viable candi-dates for amplification given the advances thathave occurred in hearing aid technology overthe past several years. In addition, since thebenefits of binaural amplification have been es-tablished, binaural fittings have become thestandard of care even in many cases with asym-metrical hearing loss.25 In spite of the obviousbenefits of binaural fittings for many if not themajority of cases with peripheral hearing loss,there exists a group of individuals for whombinaural amplification may be contraindicated.There are few professionals that would chal-lenge the benefits of binaural amplification inpatients with symmetrical hearing losses. How-ever, as we learn more about the functioning ofthe CANS and the potential for less than opti-mal binaural processing associated with someCANS disorders, it is questionable whether thefinding of a symmetrical peripheral hearing lossis sufficient evidence to justify a binaural fittingin these patients. For instance, Jerger and hiscolleagues provided evidence of binaural inter-ference in four patients with symmetrical hear-ing losses.26 Aided speech recognition scoreswere derived for three of these four patientsunder three test conditions: aided right ear,aided left ear, and binaural fitting. In each case,obvious differences were noted between the

monaural fittings for the two ears in spite of thefinding of symmetrical pure tone hearing sensi-tivity during pure tone assessments. Even moreinteresting, however, was the finding that thebinaural test condition resulted in significantlypoorer performance when this score was com-pared to the aided speech recognition score ofthe better ear.

These same investigators obtained middlelatency responses for three of the original fourpatients under three conditions: monaural right,monaural left, and binaural presentation. Simi-lar patterns of performance were noted in thatthe morphologies of the waveforms derivedunder the binaural presentation condition werenoticeably poorer than the morphologies of thewaveforms derived from the better ear in eachcase.26 For these patients, the presentation ofauditory information to the second or poorerear seemed to interfere with the processing ofinformation presented to the better ear. Al-though the exact mechanisms underlying thisphenomenon are not understood, it is likelythat some type of distortion is being introducedby the peripheral and/or central system. An ob-vious implication of these findings is that thebinaural processes must work appropriately if apatient fitted with hearing aids is to make opti-mal use of binaural amplification.

Other researchers have demonstrated pa-tient profiles that lend additional support to theimportance of considering the status of theCANS when fitting patients with peripheralhearing loss who may be at-risk for CANS in-volvement. Musiek and Baran24 outlined fourdifferent case profiles where binaural amplifi-cation may be contraindicated based on thefindings of a CANS compromise. Binaural am-plification may be contraindicated if: (1) sym-metrical hearing loss is present, but the centralauditory test performance of one ear is markedlypoorer than that of the other ear; (2) an asym-metrical hearing loss is present, and the centralauditory test performance of the better ear is sig-nificantly poorer than that of the poorer ear; (3)a symmetrical hearing loss is present and abnor-mal middle and/or late potentials are noted overone hemisphere versus the other (i.e., a signifi-cant electrode effect); and (4) a symmetricalhearing loss is present and an ear effect is notedon electrophysiological testing.

Page 98: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

334 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

These cases, taken together with thoseidentified by Jerger and his colleagues26 dem-onstrate instances of less-than-optimal binau-ral processing in some patients with peripheralhearing loss. To take advantage of binaural am-plification, all of the binaural processes mustwork appropriately, including those that aremediated in the brainstem and the auditorycortex. Because the function of the binauralprocesses of the CANS are not adequately ad-dressed by routine peripheral assessments, it isimportant that the audiologist consider theneed to assess auditory processing when work-ing with patients with peripheral hearing loss.

Given the profiles outlined above, the in-clusion of an auditory processing assessment inthe hearing aid selection and fitting procedurefor patients with peripheral hearing loss shouldbe given serious consideration, particularlywhen evaluating a patient with known CANSinvolvement (e.g., a stroke, etc.) or one whomay be considered at-risk for an APD even inthe absence of a confirmed neurological factor(i.e., the elderly patient with severe auditorycomprehension difficulties, etc.). Recommendedprocedures include, but are not limited to, di-chotic speech testing (e.g., dichotic digits) anda comparison of aided speech recognition scoresobtained with monaural right, monaural left,and binaural fittings.24,26,27 In many cases, ad-ditional behavioral and electrophysiologic testsmay be warranted.23,24 Decisions regarding theinclusion of any of these procedures should bebased on a consideration of the patient’s pre-senting complaints and the existence of othercomorbid medical conditions.23,24 Alternativeprocedures for incorporating such assessmentsinto the evaluation protocol for patients whoare being seen for hearing aid selection and fit-ting procedures can be found in Musiek andBaran.24

CONCLUDING COMMENTS

Auditory processing disorders (APD) representa complex and heterogeneous group of disor-ders. No single approach will address the needsof each individual who presents with APD.Management programs must be individualized

to meet the unique needs of each individual.Furthermore, these programs need to be closelymonitored and systematically evaluated, and theindividual’s management plan should be modi-fied if the results do not show adequate prog-ress or if the patient’s context changes.

REFERENCES

1. Bocca E, Calearo, C, Cassinari V. A new methodfor testing hearing in temporal lobe tumors. ActaOtolaryngol 1954;44:219–221

2. Bocca E, Calearo C, Cassinari V, Migliavacca F.Testing “cortical” hearing in temporal lobe tumors.Acta Otolaryngol 1955;45:289–304

3. Baran JA, Musiek FE. Behavioral assessment ofthe central auditory nervous system. In: MusiekFE, Rintelmann WF, eds. Contemporary Perspec-tives in Hearing Assessment. Boston, MA: Allyn& Bacon; 1999:375–413

4. Bellis TJ. Assessment and Management of CentralAuditory processing Disorders in the EducationalSetting: From Science to Practice. San Diego, CA:Singular; 1996

5. Chermak GA, Musiek FE. Central Auditory Pro-cessing Disorders: New Perspectives. San Diego,CA: Singular; 1997

6. Musiek FE, Baran JA, Pinheiro ML. Neuroaudiol-ogy: Case Studies. San Diego, CA: Singular; 1994

7. Baran JA. Management of adolescents and adultswith central auditory processing disorders. In: Mas-ters GA, Stecker NA, Katz J, eds. Central AuditoryProcessing Disorders: Mostly Management. Boston,MA: Allyn & Bacon; 1998:195–214

8. Chermak GA. Metacognitive approaches to man-aging central auditory processing disorders. In:Masters GA, Stecker NA, Katz J, eds. Central Au-ditory Processing Disorders: Mostly Management.Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon; 1998:49–62

9. Chermak GA, Musiek FE. Managing central audi-tory processing disorders in children and youth.Am J Acad Audiol 1992;1:62–65

10. Musiek FE. Habilitation and management of audi-tory processing disorders: overview of selected pro-cedures. J Am Acad Audiol 1999;10:329–342

11. Musiek FE, Baran JA, Schochat E. Selected man-agement approaches to central auditory processingdisorders. Scand Audiol 1999;28(Suppl 51):63–76

12. Musiek FE, Schochat E. Auditory training andcentral auditory processing disorders. Semin Hear1998;19:357–365

13. Masters GA, Stecker NA, Katz J, eds. Central Au-ditory Processing Disorders: Mostly Management.Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon; 1998

Page 99: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

MANAGING APD IN ADOLESCENTS AND ADULTS/BARAN 335

14. Stein R. Application of FM technology to man-agement of central auditory processing disorders.In: Masters GA, Stecker NA, Katz J, eds. CentralAuditory Processing Disorders: Mostly Manage-ment. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon;1998;89–92

15. Kleinman SN, Bashir AS. Adults with language-learning disabilities: new challenges and changingperspectives. Semin Hear 1996;17:201–216

16. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.Central auditory processing: current status of re-search and implications for practice. Am J Audiol1996; 5:41–54

17. Picheny MA, Durlach, NI, Briada LD. Speakingclearly for the hard of hearing I. Intelligibility dif-ferences between clear and conversational speech. JSpeech Hear Res 1985;28:96–103

18. Picheny MA, Durlach, NI, Briada LD. Speakingclearly for the hard of hearing II. Acoustic charac-teristics of clear and conversational speech. JSpeech Hear Res 1986;29:434–436

19. Picheny MA, Durlach, NI, Briada LD. Speakingclearly for the hard of hearing III. Attempt to de-termine the contribution of speaking rate to differ-ences in intelligibility between clear and conversa-tional speech. J Speech Hear Res 1989;32:600–603

20. Ray H, Sarff LS, Glassford JE. Sound field ampli-fication: an innovative educational intervention formainstreamed learning disabled students. The Di-rective Teacher 1984; Summer/Fall:18–20

21. Bender WA. Learning Disabilities: Characteristics,Identification, and Teaching Strategies. 2nd ed.Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon; 1995

22. Hallahan DP, Kauffman JM, Lloyd JW. Introduc-tion to Learning Disabilities. Boston, MA: Allyn& Bacon; 1996

23. American Speech-Language-Hearing Association.Preferred Practice Patterns for the Profession ofAudiology. Rockville, MD: Author; 1997

24. Musiek FE, Baran JA. Amplification and the cen-tral auditory nervous system. In: Valente M, ed.Hearing Aids: Standards, Options, and Limita-tions. New York: Thieme; 1996:407–437

25. Dillon H. Hearing Aids. New York: Thieme; 200126. Jerger, J, Silman, S, Lew HL, Chmiel R. Case stud-

ies in binaural interference: converging evidencefrom behavioral and electrophysiologic measures. JAm Acad Audiol 1993;4:122–131

27. Strouse Carter A, Noe CM, Wilson RH. Listenerswho prefer monaural to binaural hearing aids. J AmAcad Audiol 2001;12:261–272

Page 100: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders
Page 101: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

337

Management of Auditory Processing Disorders; Editor in Chief, Catherine V. Palmer, Ph.D.; Guest Editor, Gail D.Chermak, Ph.D. Seminars in Hearing, volume 23, number 4, 2002. Address for correspondence and reprint requests: KimL. Tillery, Ph.D., Assistant Professor, State University of New York College at Fredonia, Department of SpeechPathology and Audiology, Thompson Hall, Fredonia, NY 14063. E-mail: [email protected]. 1Neuropsychologist,Independent Practice, East Amherst Psychology Group, East Amherst, New York; 2Assistant Professor, State Universityof New York College at Fredonia, Department of Speech Pathology and Audiology, Fredonia, New York. Copyright ©2002 by Thieme Medical Publishers, Inc., 333 Seventh Avenue, New York, NY 10001, USA. Tel: +1(212) 584-4662.00734-0451,p;2002,23,04,337,348,ftx,en;sih00227x.

Reliable Differential Diagnosis and EffectiveManagement of Auditory Processing andAttention Deficit Hyperactivity DisordersWarren D. Keller, Ph.D.,1 and Kim L. Tillery, Ph.D.2

ABSTRACT

Children with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD)manifest behaviors strikingly similar to children with Auditory ProcessingDisorders (APD). Recent research suggests that these two disorders areoften co-morbid, but can occur independently. The appropriate manage-ment of ADHD and APD is dependent upon a reliable and valid diagnosisusing multiple sources of information and measures. The most effective ev-idence based approaches for these two disorders are reviewed with an em-phasis on the importance of differential diagnosis.

KEYWORDS: Attention deficit, hyperactivity, auditory processing,central nervous system stimulant medication

Learning Outcomes: Upon completion of this article, the reader will understand (1) the assessment strate-gies and techniques that should be employed in order to accurately differentially diagnose ADHD and APD; (2)the lack of evidenced-based treatment approaches regarding the management of these two disorders; and (3)that ADHD and APD are two separate disorders, necessitating an interdisciplinary evaluation by both the audi-ologist and psychologist.

orders, constitute a heterogeneous group withmany co-morbid conditions, and present withstrikingly similar symptoms of inattention, dis-tractibility, hyperactivity, and poor listeningskills. The appropriate management of a childnecessitates accurate differential diagnosis.

Children behave similarly for a variety ofreasons and it is imperative that we rememberthat all that is hyper is not hyperactivity and allthat is poor listening is not auditory processingdysfunction. Children with attention disorders,as well as children with auditory processing dis-

Page 102: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

338 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

ATTENTION DEFICITHYPERACTIVITY DISORDER

In the early 1900s, the complex of symptoms,now commonly referred to as hyperactivity, wasregarded as being a flaw in moral character. Ter-minology has changed as our knowledge of thedisorder has improved.1 Currently, the Diagnos-tic and Statistical Manual (DSM-IV)2 describesADHD as: 1) hyperactive-impulsive (ADHD-HI), 2) predominantly inattentive (ADHD-PI),and 3) combined (ADHD-C) types.

ADHD is one of the most common child-hood problems referred to child treatment cen-ters, and is believed to comprise approximately40 % of referrals.1 It is characterized by an in-ability to sustain attention, motoric hyperactiv-ity, and impulsivity inappropriate to the child’schronological age.2 There is typically an exac-erbation of symptoms in situations where phys-ical movement is restrained or where sustainedattention is required, such as in classroom situ-ations. Barkley describes the child with ADHDas having difficulty inhibiting and followingrule-governed behaviors.1

The difficulties that children with ADHDexperience are cross situational in nature, oc-curring at home, in school, with peers, and inmost social situations.1 This is a chronic devel-opmental disorder that most often is not out-grown.1 Children with ADHD present with avariety of secondary features that may be help-ful in making an accurate diagnosis and areoften the focus of treatment. A substantial por-tion of children with ADHD will manifest co-morbid learning disorders.1 It is believed that25 to 40 % of those with ADHD will experi-ence specific learning disabilities that will ad-versely impact their academic achievement.1Of the remaining children, 90 % will under-achieve in school given their intellectual poten-tial.1 In addition, children with ADHD presentwith a variety of socioemotional disturbances,and are often disliked by their peers.3

Associated features of individuals withADHD include higher rates of noncompliance,an increased rate of minor physical anomalies,4difficulty mastering bladder and bowel con-trol,5 disruption in sleep/wake cycles,1 and in-

creased chronic otitis media.6 As young adults,those with ADHD are at great risk for the de-velopment of alcoholism, substance abuse, andanti-social behaviors.1

There is increasing evidence that childrenexhibiting ADHD are experiencing an underarousal of the central nervous system (CNS)and hence, many clinicians and researchersrefer to ADHD as an arousal disorder. Lou andcolleagues found a hypoperfusion of blood flowto the frontal area of the brain in children withADHD.7 Zametkin and colleagues found re-ductions in glucose metabolism in the premotorcortex and superior prefrontal cortex in individ-uals diagnosed with ADHD, these areas of thebrain being implicated in the control of atten-tion and motor activity.8 Clinically, we see neu-ropsychological tests of executive functions thatare sensitive to frontal lobe behaviors, such asthe Category Test,9and the Wisconsin CardSort Test,10 being helpful in differentiating in-dividuals with ADHD from other disorders.

ADHD-PI TYPE OR APD?

The diagnostic classification of attention disor-ders in children is divided into two types—ADHD-HI and ADHD-PI. This distinctionhas occurred from the time diagnostic classifica-tion began to emphasize attention as opposedto mere hyperkinetic behavior.11 The child withADHD-PI subtype differs in many ways fromthe child with ADHD-C subtype. ADHD-PIchildren are more likely to experience internal-izing symptoms as opposed to the disruptive be-havior that characterizes hyperactive children. Ifanything, they present with hypoactivity as op-posed to hyperactivity. The gender ratio is lessimbalanced among the ADHD-PI subtype.Girls more frequently fall into the ADHD-PIsubtype than the ADHD-C or ADHD-HI sub-types.12 The differences between the two groupsare such that some have even argued that theyrepresent two separate disorders, and should beconceptualized as distinct and unrelated.12 Theextent to which children with ADHD-PI sub-type may be manifesting APD that is respon-

Page 103: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

DIAGNOSIS AND MANAGEMENT OF APD AND ADHD/KELLER, TILLERY 339

sible for their primarily inattentive symptomsneeds to be investigated.

AUDITORY PROCESSINGDISORDERS

Individuals with APD do not effectively useauditory information, often have difficulty inunderstanding complex and lengthy directions,require repetition with directions, and may ap-pear to have a peripheral hearing loss.13 Themisinterpretation of speech signals may be dueto a variety of APD problems, better defined inspecific subtypes. The reader is referred to otherpublications for the method to diagnose thesespecific subtypes13–16and to Bellis (this issue,pages 287–296).

Decoding

Weak phonemic awareness, known as decod-ing type of APD13or termed Auditory Closure(Decoding),14 is the most common type ofAPD.13–15 Besides discrimination errors, poordecoding ability also is manifested by difficultyin manipulating sounds, resulting in poor abil-ity to blend sounds together. Such errors mayreflect the child’s faulty mental perception ofthe sounds, resulting in poor reading, spellingand word finding abilities and difficulty in ac-curately understanding what is said. The leftposterior temporal area is responsible for audi-tory processing and receptive language func-tions, thus giving rise to the poor receptive lan-guage skills commonly associated with clientswho are poor decoders.13–15,17

Tolerance-Fading Memory

Signs of frontal and anterior temporal involve-ment are consistent with the second most com-mon type of APD, the Tolerance-Fading Mem-ory (TFM) subtype.13 A small region of theparietal lobe (post central gyrus) also may beassociated with TFM, giving rise to short-termmemory limitations and speech-in-noise diffi-

culty that involve anterior cerebral areas.13,18

Weak expressive language and poor handwrit-ing are not listening problems, but are associ-ated anatomically with TFM—the motor plan-ning region of the frontal lobe—which is thesame cortical area that Zametkin implicateswith ADHD.8 Broca’s area (inferior frontal re-gion) is associated with expressive language andboth motor programming of articulatory move-ments and writing functions are regulated bythe pre-motor area of the frontal lobes.13–15

Organization

While Decoding and TFM types of APD areseen in isolation or in combination with eachother, the Organization subtype is not usuallyseen by itself.13,19 Organization type of APD istermed an Output-Organization Deficit14 andis diagnosed when significant corrupted audi-tory sequencing or planning is noted and thechild displays difficulty with sequential infor-mation and is disorganized at home or inschool.13–15 The pre- and post- central gyri,and anterior temporal areas may be responsiblefor these auditory behaviors.13,15,17

Integration

A final subtype of APD is Integration orIntegration Deficit.13–16 Poor inter-hemisphericfunction (via the corpus callosum) may underliethis subtype given the left ear deficit on dichotictests that require language response.14 Charac-teristics include long delays in response to audi-tory stimuli, poor reading, spelling, writing, anduse of language skills.13–15,19

APD SUBTYPES SEEN IN ADHD

Subtyping APD may serve an important role inthe differential diagnosis of APD, ADHD, aswell as specific learning disorders. Althoughsome researchers caution that APD subtypeshave yet to be validated,20,21 our research andclinical work involves an exploration as to how

Page 104: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

340 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

Table 1 Summary of Studies Evaluating Co-Morbidity of ADHD and APD, and Indicating the

Criteria Used to Diagnosis ADHD

Study APD in ADHD Cases ADHD in APD Cases

Gascon et al23** 15 / 19 with APD (79%)Cook et al24** 12 / 15 with APD (80%)Riccio et al.25* 15 / 30 with ADHD (50%)Tillery et al.22*** 36 / 66 with APD (55%)

19 / 66 mild APD (29%)

ADHD was diagnosis by: *teacher and parental ratings on behavioral questionnaires; **physician / DSM III-R; and***comprehensive psychological evaluation / DSM-IV/ Clinical titration of Ritalin®.

the subtypes may be instrumental in differentialdiagnosis, recommending appropriate referralsand providing effective remediation strategies.

A published study investigating APD sub-types among the population of those diag-nosed with APD and ADHD found a highpercentage of children with TFM APD.16 Thisalso was observed in the children who partici-pated in the Tillery et al study,22 where 80% ofthe children presented with TFM APD and55% were found to exhibit significant reversals,thereby showing an Organization (APD) Defi-cit. It is not surprising that TFM is associatedwith ADHD, given its association with ante-rior temporal functions and the proximity tothe frontal area associated with ADHD.8,13

Clinically, it has been noted that reversals maybe more common in ADHD. It has been ourpractice to refer children for psychological eval-uation to rule out ADHD when these APDsubtypes have been found.

CO-MORBIDITY OF ADHD AND APD

Increasingly, research suggests that ADHD andAPD are two separate disorders that can occurindependently or co-morbidly.22 Table 1 sum-marizes the results of the studies that investi-gated the relationship between these two disor-ders.22–25 Varying criteria for the assessment ofADHD make interpretation questionable. Thesemethodologic assessment issues hamper the va-lidity of much of the ADHD literature.

COMPREHENSIVE ASSESSMENT OF ADHD

Comprehensive assessment of children withADHD has been almost as controversial amongmental health clinicians as has the proper assess-ment of children with APD has been among au-diologists. While there are those clinicians whohave argued that ADHD can be effectively andproperly diagnosed using only standardized be-havioral questionnaires and a comprehensive his-tory, clinically elevated scores on these standard-ized behavioral report measures only indicate thepresence of ADHD symptomatology, but tell usnothing about the cause of these behaviors.

Colegrove and colleagues,26 among others,found that the use of behavioral questionnairesalone are not particularly useful in the differen-tial diagnosis of ADHD, and propose that theuse of questionnaires alone may lead to improperdiagnosis. Others have argued that in order toreduce the over identification of childhoodADHD and to make an accurate diagnosis, it isnecessary to approach the assessment in a com-prehensive fashion that includes continuous per-formance tests, standardized normed behavioralmeasures, neuropsychologic and psycho-educa-tional assessments, measures of memory func-tioning, a comprehensive medical and develop-mental history, as well as an assessment ofsocio-emotional and personality factors in orderto rule out disorders such as anxiety which sooften mimics ADHD.26,27 It is our position thatin order to provide effective management, an as-

Page 105: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

DIAGNOSIS AND MANAGEMENT OF APD AND ADHD/KELLER, TILLERY 341

sessment of ADHD must include all of theabove as well as consideration as to whether anAPD itself may be responsible for behavioralsymptomatology suggestive of ADHD.

RELIABLE DIAGNOSIS OF APD INTHE HYPERACTIVE CHILD

Some clinicians speculate that the high comor-bidity of ADHD and APD may be related tothe inherent inattention of the child rather thanpure auditory processing dysfunction.20,28 Oneof the misconceptions about children withADHD is that they are constantly hyperactive,fidgety and restless. Children with ADHD whoare provided frequent reinforcement, novelty,strict control, and highly salient rewards andpunishments can perform behaviorally muchlike their nonhyperactive peers.27 The reader isreferred to published sources for specific rec-ommendations for administration proceduresto ensure that the auditory processing evalua-tions control for inattention.29,30

REVIEW OF CENTRAL NERVOUSSTIMULANT (CNS) MEDICATIONEFFECTS ON APD

To date, there have been four conflicting stud-ies that have investigated the effects of Ritalinon the APD test performance of children witheither ADHD, APD, or both. Three studiesfound a medication effect,23,24,31 while a morewell-controlled study did not find a medica-tion effect.22 The conflict likely stems from thefact that the earlier studies (1) did not controlfor learning effects or peak medication peri-ods,23,31 (2) involved small sample sizes,23,24 (3)used different criteria for selection of childrenwith APD or ADHD,23,24,31 (4) provided de-scriptive rather than statistical analysis,23 and(5) did not clinically titrate medication.23,24

The Tillery et al study used a double-blind,placebo-controlled design while controlling forlearning, maturation, and fatigue effects amonga large group of children clinically diagnosedwith ADHD and APD, and who had been re-

ceiving Ritalin.22 While a significant medica-tion effect was not found in the APD test per-formance, there was a significant medicationeffect found on a vigilance measure, the Audi-tory Continuous Performance Test (ACPT).32

Because Ritalin improves sustained attentionor vigilance and decreases behaviors that mayotherwise interfere with test taking, childrenprescribed stimulant medication for ADHDshould follow their medication regime as usualwhen seen for auditory processing testing.

MANAGEMENT OF ADHD

There is considerable research investigating evi-dence-based treatment approaches for the man-agement of ADHD; however, much less re-search has been done on APD managementapproaches. The criteria for empirically sup-ported psychosocial interventions for childhoodmental disorders have been established by theAmerican Psychological Association.33 Stimu-lant medication, parent training, the use of be-havioral interventions in the classroom, andpsychotherapy, with a focus on behavior man-agement and education, all meet criteria forwell-established treatments for ADHD.34 Ap-proximately 70 to 80% of children respond pos-itively to stimulant medication, with an evengreater number of children responding favor-ably if several stimulant trials are conducted. Ofthe remaining children, the medication either isnot of therapeutic benefit or the adverse side ef-fects outweigh the advantages. Of those chil-dren who respond positively to medication, theirbehavioral functioning is improved, but is farfrom being normalized. Training parents in con-sistency management programs, the use of dailyreport cards that incorporate novelty and imme-diate feedback regarding performance, and be-havioral interventions focusing on social skillstraining and compliance using intensive behav-ioral interventions complement or in some casesprovide the basis for effective management ofthose with ADHD.34

The results of the Multimodal TreatmentStudy of ADHD (MTA Study), sponsored bythe National Institute of Mental Health and

Page 106: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

342 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

Department of Education was conducted at sixdifferent sites throughout the United Statesand one additional site in Canada.35–37 Thestudy compared four groups of children withADHD (N=578). One group received medica-tion alone in a very carefully titrated fashion,which is not particularly characteristic of mostclinical practices. The second group partici-pated in a psychosocial treatment program basedlargely on behavioral interventions, includingparent training and a behavioral treatment pro-gram based primarily on that described by Pel-ham.34,38,39 Another group received a combina-tion of both interventions and the last groupreceived community care and was to functiongenerally as a control group.

The details of the MTA study are providedin several publications.35–37 In general, childrenwho received the two medication treatmentsfared better in having their ADHD sympto-matology reduced. Parents were found to pre-fer nonpharmacologic interventions; however,the combined treatment approaches yieldedbenefits not obtained with either medicationor therapy alone.

The effectiveness of stimulant medicationtherapy in the management of children withADHD is well documented.1 Ritalin and otherstimulant medications can help normalize sus-tained attention and reduce some of the associ-ated features of the disorder in order to im-prove successful management. The range ofstimulant medications available, and the sec-ond line medications, such as Clonidine, Well-butrin, and selective serotonin reuptake in-hibitors (SSRIs), such as Paxil, provide a rangeof pharmacologic options so that medicationmanagement may be individualized dependingon the symptoms any particular child is pre-senting. Because ADHD is a disorder that isco-morbid with other disorders, poly-pharma-cologic treatment is often indicated.40

Medication management remains at theforefront of treatment for children with ADHD.In addition to Ritalin, newer stimulant medica-tions shown to have positive therapeutic valueare being released. Adderall, Adderall XR, Con-certa, Metadate, Ritalin LA, and Focalin are allstimulant medications that provide a longer du-ration effect so that multiple dosing throughout

the day may be avoided, simplifying the phar-macologic management of ADHD. Anothermedication, Atomoxetine, a noradrenergic agent,will likely obtain Food and Drug Administra-tion approval later this year.

MANAGEMENT OF APD / ADHD

Criteria for evidence-based treatment of APDare only beginning to be established.

APD therapeutic and management strate-gies have been detailed in the literature,14,19,30,41–43

with a recognized difference between the man-agement and remediation of APD. Remedia-tion efforts strive to change auditory function;management approaches attempt to alter be-havioral performance due to the application ofcompensatory strategies.44

Reports of the efficacy of various remedia-tion and management approaches for APD arelimited compared to the evidence-based crite-ria establishing treatments for ADHD. Med-ication is effective for ADHD and ineffectivefor APD,22 while therapeutic measures for APDare recommended with the intent to improveauditory function. When the two disordersoccur comorbidly, there are no established evi-dence-based treatments. Thus, the clinician’sonly alternative is to creatively combine man-agement strategies. Table 2 illustrates some ofthe evidence-based management approachesfor ADHD, as well as some suggested manage-ment approaches for APD and combinedADHD/APD. Because there is a paucity of ef-ficacy data regarding effective treatment ofchildren with both disorders, management ofthese children remains speculative. Treatmentsfor ADHD may be contraindicated for chil-dren with APD and vice versa. Therefore, a re-liable diagnosis of both disorders is necessaryfor the beginning of effective management.

EFFECTIVE CASE MANAGEMENT:ADHD, APD, LD

The importance of an accurate differential di-agnosis in the management of children withattention related disorders might be best illus-

Page 107: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

DIAGNOSIS AND MANAGEMENT OF APD AND ADHD/KELLER, TILLERY 343

Table 2 Contrasting Management Strategies for ADHD, ADHD/APD, and APD

Strategy ADHD ADHD/APD APD

Medication X X NoNovelty X ? ?Immediate feedback X ? ?Highly salient rewards and punishments X ? ?Parent training X ? ?Low level background stimulation (e.g., music) X ? ?Psychotherapy X ? ?Daily report cards X ? ?Social skills training X ? ?Decreased work load X X XSelf advocacy X X XEducate parents/families X X XFlexibility in teaching style X X XPre-tutoring X X XPreferential seating X X XRepetition of directions X X XEnvironmental modifications X X XOrganization skills X X XAuditory processing therapy NA ? XMetacognitive approaches ? ? XAuditory trainer ? ? XRedundancy ? ? XHighly structured teachers ? ? XCounseling ? ? X

NA, not applicable; ?, unknown.

trated using two case studies of children whowere assessed for behavioral and school adjust-ment difficulties. Given the overlapping be-haviors that these children present with, diag-nosis has often been contingent upon whetheror not they visit the audiologist or psychologistfirst.27,45,46 Given the co-morbidity of thesedisorders, it becomes necessary to comprehen-sively evaluate children in order to determinewhether they may be exhibiting one or more ofthese often overlapping conditions.

Case One: APD without ADHD

Andrea first presented to the audiologist at 5years of age because of her inability to accu-rately follow directions, and her consistent mis-interpretation of spoken messages. She had ahistory of otitis media and hyperactivity.

AUDIOLOGIC EVALUATION

Andrea was found to exhibit normal peripheralhearing, bilaterally. She failed two of the threeAPD subtests by five standard deviations, show-ing evidence of Tolerance-Fading Memory andDecoding types of APD. The three APD testswere the Staggered Spondaic Word (SSW)Test,47 Phonemic Synthesis (PS) Test,48 and aspeech-in-noise test.49 She received 17 individ-ualized auditory processing therapy sessions thatconsisted of targeting phonemic awareness (au-ditory closure,14,41 rhyming,29,50 synthesis,51 andanalysis52). Because she evidenced extreme motoractivity, she was referred to a psychologist aftercompleting the ninth therapy session.

PSYCHOLOGIC EVALUATION

Andrea was described by her parents as havinghad a “rough five years,” that she was a highlypersistent child bordering on obsessiveness, and

Page 108: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

344 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

was often in a negative mood state. Her atten-tion on a continuous performance test32 was weakfor her age, and on a screening test for APD53

her overall performance fell at the 7th percentile.A neuropsychologic examination yielded over-all average measured intelligence, but with per-formance on immediate memory tasks fallingat the 2nd percentile, raising the possibility thatattention factors might have been suppressingher performance because attention is a prereq-uisite for remembering. She presented with sig-nificant motor activity suggestive of ADHD,but diagnosis was deferred given her young ageand the auditory processing weaknesses thatshe was manifesting.

MANAGEMENT

In Andrea’s case, overall management consistedof addressing her severe auditory processingdeficits. Besides the above-mentioned process-ing therapies, Andrea also benefited from a co-operative school district and supportive parentsemploying many of the strategies listed in Table2. On an intellectual evaluation at 9 years ofage, she manifested a significant disparity be-tween measured verbal intelligence and visualperceptual functioning, with a Verbal IQ of 127and a Performance IQ of 82, and with sup-pressed performance on particular subtests, sug-gesting some form of auditory processing dys-function. Further neuropsychologic evaluationconfirmed a Nonverbal Learning Disability(NLD)54 and her earlier attentional weaknesseshad resolved, presumably due to increased righthemisphere maturation (associated with herNLD). Currently, her phonemic awareness skillsare above grade level, she continues to be an ex-cellent reader, and she is not showing classroomAPD behaviors.

While she presented with symptomatol-ogy suggestive of ADHD, and may have beenimproperly placed on stimulant medicationhad a less thorough assessment been conducted,it was the attentional weaknesses associatedwith NLD syndrome, a particular learning dis-ability subtype that is also associated withweak attention27,54 and symptoms associatedwith APD that made her present as a childwith ADHD. Most striking is that children withAndrea’s neuropsychologic profile typically ex-

perience pronounced delays in reading acquisi-tion. Her reading scores were consistently abovethe 90th percentile, possibly related to the in-dividualized auditory processing therapy ses-sions she received. Andrea’s hyperactivity, whichappeared to be ADHD, was instead a behaviorassociated with her NLD and APD.

Case Two: ADHD without APD

Cindy first presented for psychologic evalua-tion at 6 years of age. She was born 2 monthspremature and showed no evidence of any in-tracranial bleeding on a computerized axial to-mography (CAT) scan. Motor milestones hadbeen delayed and she was subsequently diag-nosed with cerebral palsy.

PSYCHOLOGIC EVALUATION

On evaluation, Cindy obtained a verbal score of107 and a performance score of 73 on theWechsler Scales. She was diagnosed as havingNLD with average verbal memory abilities, butwith visual memory skills at the first percentile.Neuropsychologic evaluation was consistentwith NLD, with compromised performance onmeasures of tactile learning, nonverbal reason-ing skills, and on measures sensitive to the visu-ospatial abilities thought to be subserved by theright hemisphere. It was felt that her inatten-tion was due to the presence of NLD syndromeand possibly APD.

AUDIOLOGIC EVALUATION

Cindy was referred for an auditory processingevaluation due to her poor performance on ascreening measure for APD.53 She was foundto exhibit normal peripheral hearing bilater-ally, did not present any indication of APD,and passed all three APD subtests: SSW, PS,and a speech-in-noise test. However, she dis-played a significant number of reversals on theSSW test.

MANAGEMENT

Given the association between reversals andprimary attention disorders, she was subse-quently prescribed a low dose of stimulant med-ication, to which she responded very well. The

Page 109: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

DIAGNOSIS AND MANAGEMENT OF APD AND ADHD/KELLER, TILLERY 345

medication enhanced her performance on taskssensitive to attention, as well as her behavioralfunctioning both at home and school. In thisinstance, audiologic studies were of assistancein ruling out APD and making the determina-tion to consider a trial of stimulant medicationfor what was ADHD associated with NLDsyndrome.

CONCLUSIONS

These two case studies illustrate that reliabledifferential diagnosis is successful when theaudiologist and psychologist work as a team.Reliable differential diagnosis will direct im-proved evaluation-based treatment approachesfor children with APD, ADHD, and those withcombined APD and ADHD. The use of APDsub-types may assist with reliable differentialdiagnosis and effective management practicesof auditory processing, attention, and learningdisorders.

ABBREVIATIONS

ADHD attention deficit hyperactivity dis-order

ADHD-C ADHD-combinedADHD-HI ADHD hyperactivity-impulsiveADHD-PI ADHD-predominantly inatten-

tionAPD auditory processing disordersCNS central nervous systemCAT computerized axial tomographyNLD nonverbal learning disabilityPS phonemic synthesisSSRIs selective serotonin reuptake in-

hibitorsSSW staggered spondaic wordTFM tolerance fading memory

REFERENCES

1. Barkley RA. Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Dis-order: A Handbook for Diagnosis and Treatment.New York: Guilford Press; 1990

2. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic andStatistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed.Washington, DC: Author; 1994

3. Pelham W, Bender M. Peer relationships and hy-peractive children: description and treatment. In:Gadow K, Bailer I, eds. Advances in Learning andBehavioral Disabilities, vol. 1. Greenwich, CT: JAIPress; 1982

4. Pomeroy J, Sprafkin J, Gadow, K. Minor physicalanomalies as a biologic marker for behavioral dis-orders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatr 1988;27:466–473

5. Ingersoll B. Your Hyperactive Child. New York:Doubleday; 1988

6. Hagerman R, Falkenstein A. An association be-tween recurrent otitis media in infancy and laterhyperactivity. Clin Ped 1987;5:253–257

7. Lou H, Henrickson L, Bruhn P. Focal cerebral hy-poperfusion in children with dysphasia and/or atten-tion deficit disorder. Arch Neurol 1984;41:825–829

8. Zametkin A , Nordahl T, Gross M. Cerebral glu-cose metabolism in adults with hyperactivity ofchildhood onset. New Eng J Med1990;323:1361–1366

9. Reitan R, Wolfson D. The Halstead-Reitan Neu-ropsychological Test Battery: Theory and ClinicalInterpretation. Tucson, AZ: Neuropsychology Press;1985

10. Grant D, Berg E. Wisconsin Card Sort Test. Odessa,FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.; 1981

11. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic andStatistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 2nd ed.Washington DC: Author; 1968

12. Milich R, Balentine A, Lynam D. The Predomi-nantly Inattentive Subtype—not a subtype ofADHD. ADHD Report 2000;10:1

13. Katz J, Smith P. The Staggered Spondaic WordTest: a ten-minute look at the central nervous sys-tem through the ears. Ann NY Acad Sci 1991;620:233–251

14. Bellis T. Assessment and Management of CentralAuditory Processing Disorders in the EducationalSetting: From Science to Practice. San Diego: CA:Singular; 1996

15. Katz J. Classification of auditory processing disor-ders. In: Katz J, Stecker N, Henderson D, eds.Central Auditory Processing: A TransdisciplinaryView. St. Louis, MO: Mosby;1992:81–93

16. Riccio C, Hynd G, Cohen M, Molt L. The Stag-gered Spondaic Word Test: performance of chil-dren with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder.Am J Audiol 1996;5:55–62

17. Luria A. Aspects of aphasia. J Neuro Sci 1965;2:278–287

18. Efron R, Crandall P, Koss P, et al. Central auditoryprocessing. III: the ‘cocktail party effect’ and tem-poral lobectomy. Brain Lang 1983;19:254–263

Page 110: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

346 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

19. Masters G. Speech and language management ofcentral auditory processing disorders. In: MastersG, Stecker N, Katz J, eds. Central Auditory Pro-cessing Disorders: Mostly Management. NeedhamHeights, MA: Allyn & Bacon;1998:117–129

20. Cacace A, McFarland D. Central auditory process-ing disorder in school-aged children: a critical re-view. J Speech Lang Hear Res 1998; 41:355–373

21. Chermak G, Musiek FE. Central Auditory Pro-cessing Disorders: New Perspectives. San Diego,CA: Singular; 1997

22. Tillery K, Katz J, Keller W. Effects of methyl-phenidate (Ritalin™) on auditory performance inchildren with attention and auditory processing dis-orders. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2000;43:893–901

23. Gascon G, Johnson R, Burd L. Central auditoryprocessing and attention deficit disorders. J ChildNeurol 1986;1:27–33

24. Cook J, Mausbach T, Burd L, et al. A preliminarystudy of the relationship between central auditoryprocessing and attention deficit disorder. J Psychia-try Neurosci 1993;18:130–137

25. Riccio C, Hynd G, Cohen M, Hall J, Molt L . Co-morbidity of central auditory processing disorderand attention-defict hyperactivity disorder. J AmAcad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1994;33:849–857

26. Colegrove R, Homayounjan H, Williams J,Hanken J. Reducing the over identification ofchildhood ADHD: a stepwise diagnostic model.ADHD Report 2001;9:4

27. Keller W. The relationship between ADHD,CAPD and specific learning disorders. In: MastersG, Stecker N, Katz J, eds. Central Auditory Pro-cessing Disorders: Mostly Management. NeedhamHeights, MA: Allyn & Bacon; 1998:33–47

28. American Speech-Language and Hearing Associa-tion (ASHA) Task Force on Central Auditory Pro-cessing Consensus Development. Central auditoryprocessing: current status of research and implica-tions for clinical practice. Am J Audiol 1996;5:41–54

29. Tillery K. Central auditory processing assessmentand therapeutic strategies for children with atten-tion deficit hyperactivity disorder. In: Masters G,Stecker N, Katz J, eds. Central Auditory Process-ing Disorders: Mostly Management. NeedhamHeights: MA: Allyn & Bacon; 1998:175–194

30. Chermak G, Hall J, Musiek F. Differential diagno-sis and management of central auditory processingdisorder and attention deficit hyperactivity disor-der. J Acad Audiol 1999;10:289–303

31. Keith R, Engineer P. Effects of methylphenidateon the auditory processing abilities of children withADHD. J Learn Dis1991;24:630–636

32. Keith R. ACPT: Auditory Continuous Perfor-mance Test, Examiner’s Manual. San Antonio, TX:Harcourt Brace; 1994

33. Lonigan C, Elbert J, Johnson S. Empiricallysupported psychosocial interventions for chil-dren: an overview. J Clin Child Psychol 1998;27:138–145

34. Pelham W, Wheeler T, Chronis A. Empiricallysupported psychosocial treatments for attentiondeficit hyperactivity disorder. J Clin Child Psychol1998;27:190–205

35. The MTA Cooperative Group. A 14-month ran-domized clinical trial of treatment strategies for at-tention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Arch GenPsychiatry 1999;56:1073–1086

36. Arnold L, Abikoff H, Cantwell D, et al. NationalInstitute of Mental Heath Collaborative Multi-modal Treatment Study of children with ADHD(the MTA). Arch Gen Psychiatry 1997;54:865–870

37. Greenhill L, Abikoff H, Arnold L, et al. Medica-tion treatment strategies in the MTA Study: rele-vance to clinicians and researchers. J Am AcadChild Adolesc Psychiatry 1996;35:1304–1313

38. Pelham W, Hoza B. Intensive treatment: a summertreatment program for children with ADHD. In:Hibbs E, Jensen P, eds. Psychosocial Treatmentsfor Child and Adolescent Disorders: EmpiricallyBased Strategies for Clinical Practice. New York:APA Press; 1996:311–340

39. Pelham W. The NIMH multimodal treatmentstudy for attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder:just say yes to drugs alone? Can J Psychiatry 1999;44:981–990

40. Biederman J, Faraones S, Lapey K. Comorbidity ofdiagnosis of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder.In:Weiss G, ed. Child and Adolescent Psychologi-cal Clinic. Philadelphia: Saunders; 1992:335–360

41. Chermak G, Musiek FE. Managing central audi-tory processing disorders in children and youth.Am J Audiol 1992;1:61–65

42. Musiek FE. Habilitation and management of audi-tory processing disorders: overview of selected pro-cedures. J Am Acad Audiol 1999;4:15–17

43. Ferre J. Behavioral therapeutic approaches for cen-tral auditory problems. In: Katz J, ed. Handbook ofClinical Audiology, 5th ed. Philadelphia: Lippin-cott Williams & Wilkins; 2002:525–531

44. Keith R. Clinical issues in central auditory process-ing disorders. Lang Speech Hear Serv School1999;30:339–344

45. Keller W. Auditory processing disorder or attentiondeficit disorder? In: Katz J, Stecker N, HendersonD, eds. Central Auditory Processing: A Transdisci-plinary View. St. Louis, MO: Mosby; 1992:107–114

46. Moss W, Sheiffele W. Can we differentially diag-nose an attention deficit disorder without hyperac-tivity from a central auditory processing disorder?Child Psych Human Dev 1994;25:85–96

Page 111: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

DIAGNOSIS AND MANAGEMENT OF APD AND ADHD/KELLER, TILLERY 347

47. Katz J. The use of staggered spondaic words for as-sessing the integrity of the central auditory system.J Auditory Res 1962;2:327–337

48. Katz J, Harmon C. Phonemic synthesis: testingand training. In: Keith R, ed. Central Auditory andLanguage Disorders in Children. Houston, TX:College-Hill Press; 1981

49. Mueller G, Beck G, Sedge R. Comparison of theefficiency of cortical level speech tests. Semin Hear1987;8:279–298

50. Ferre J. The M3 Model for treating central auditoryprocessing disorders. In: Masters G, Stecker N,Katz J, eds. Central Auditory Processing Disor-

ders: Mostly Management. Needham Heights,MA: Allyn & Bacon; 1998:103–115

51. Katz J, Harmon-Fletcher C. Phonemic SynthesisProgram Training. Vancouver, WA: PrecisonAcoustics; 1982

52. Lindamood C, Lindamood P. Auditory Discrimi-nation in Depth. Allen, TX: DLM Teaching; 1975

53. Keith R. SCAN: A Screening Test for AuditoryProcessing Disorders. San Diego, CA: The Psy-chological Corp; 1986

54. Rourke B. Nonverbal Learning Disabilities: TheSyndrome and the Model. New York: GuilfordPress; 1989

Page 112: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders
Page 113: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

349

Management of Auditory Processing Disorders; Editor in Chief, Catherine V. Palmer, Ph.D.; Guest Editor, Gail D.Chermak, Ph.D. Seminars in Hearing, volume 23, number 4, 2002. Address for correspondence and reprint requests:Robert E. Jirsa, Ph.D., Department of Communication Disorders, Davis Hall, Rm 012I, Southern Connecticut StateUniversity, 501 Crescent Street, New Haven, CT 06515. E-mail: [email protected]. 1Professor of Audiology,Department of Communication Disorders, Southern Connecticut State University, New Haven, Connecticut. Copyright© 2002 by Thieme Medical Publishers, Inc., 333 Seventh Avenue, New York, NY 10001, USA. Tel: +1(212) 584-4662.00734-0451,p;2002,23,04,349,356,ftx,en;sih00219x.

Clinical Efficacy of ElectrophysiologicMeasures in APD Management ProgramsRobert E. Jirsa, Ph.D.1

ABSTRACT

Determining the relative efficacy of various intervention pro-grams for auditory processing disorders (APD) is a major goal in the fieldof rehabilitative audiology. Currently, because of their widespread availabil-ity, the most commonly used measuring tools to assess the central auditorysystem have been behaviorally based. Such measures do have notable dis-advantages in that they may be influenced by a number of extraneous vari-ables that may impede, or at least influence, efficacy measures. Electrophys-iologic measures offer unique advantages not available from the behavioralmeasures. A significant amount of research has been completed offeringcompelling evidence relative to the clinical utility of a number of theseelectrophysiologic measures, including the maximum length sequences-auditory brainstem response, the middle latency response, the obligatorylong latency responses, and the MMN and P3 event-related potentials.This article will review the current research related to electrophyiologicmeasures and present a rationale for including them in the managementprogram.

KEYWORDS: Electrophysiology, middle latency response, mismatchnegativity, P3 event-related potential, speech perception, clinicalefficacy

Learning Outcomes: Upon completion of this article, the reader will (1) gain insight into those electrophysio-logic protocols most commonly used in the evaluation of APD, (2) become familiar with the research related tothe clinical application of these electrophysiologic measures as they relate to APD, and (3) gain insight into therationale for the use of electrophysiologic measures in assessing the efficacy of clinical intervention with APD.

Page 114: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

350 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

management program, the monitoring of prog-ress throughout the course of therapy is essen-tial.7 Because they evaluate neurophysiologicresponses, and neurophysiologic change oftenprecedes behavioral change,11,13 electrophysio-logic measures may offer a significant advantageover traditional behavioral measures used to as-sess progress in the therapeutic program.3,7,13

APPLICABLEELECTROPHYSIOLOGIC MEASURES

Investigators in a number of disciplines haveused electrophysiologic measures for years toevaluate aspects of the central nervous system.14

Audiologists have, until rather recently, usedelectrophysiologic measures primarily in the as-sessment of the peripheral auditory system. Thediagnostic usefulness of both the auditory brain-stem response (ABR) and the middle latencyresponse (MLR) has been documented.4,14,15

Within the past few years audiologists and hear-ing scientists have directed their efforts towardusing a variety of electrophysiologic measures tomore fully explore and understand the centralauditory nervous system.5,12 While there is muchyet to be learned, compelling evidence is accu-mulating related to the clinical relevance of anumber of electrophysiological measures includ-ing the maximum length sequences (MLS),12

the middle latency response (MLR),15–18 the longlatency N1 and P2 components,19–22 the mis-match negativity (MMN), 6,11,13,23–32 and the P3event-related-potential (ERP).2,3,33–37

While each of these electrophysiologicprocedures has shown promise of clinical ap-plicability, most clinical interest has focused onthe MLR, MMN, and the P3. These threecomponents will be reviewed in more detail.

Middle Latency Response

MLR is an obligatory potential occurring be-tween 10 and 100 msec from stimulus onset. Itis derived from multiple generator sites thoughtto reside in auditory specific and nonspecificareas ranging from the upper brainstem andreticular formation, to the thalamus and thethalamocortical pathways to the posterior tem-

According to the Consensus Conferenceon Auditory Processing Disorders (APD), acritical need in the management of APDs is de-termining the efficacy of various interventionapproaches and strategies.1 This may be accom-plished by comparing pre- and post-therapy per-formance on selected auditory tests. Behavioraltests have the advantage of being widely avail-able and accessible to clinicians in a variety ofsettings. Behavioral tests have notable disad-vantages in that they may be influenced by anumber of extraneous variables, such as higherorder cognitive and linguistic factors, that mayimpede, or at least influence, efficacy mea-sures.2–6 Electrophysiologic measures on theother hand, are much less influenced by theseextraneous factors and, in addition, offer manyunique advantages over behavioral measures.7Using electrophysiologic measures, all areas ofauditory processing from the auditory nerve8,9

to the auditory cortex can be evaluated withminimal influence from extraneous variablesthat often impact behavioral measures.1,5,10 Be-cause an auditory processing disorder involvesthe interaction of deficits in the auditory modal-ity at both peripheral and central sites, electro-physiologic measures are ideally suited for usein both the diagnosis and management of thisproblem.1,7 Furthermore, in the more general-ized area of speech perception, electrophysio-logic measures may provide the unique oppor-tunity to evaluate perception at all levels: (1) theacoustic level where acoustic information is en-coded neurologically, (2) the phonemic levelwhere basic acoustic contrasts are learned, and(3) the linguistic or language level.11 BecauseAPDs often are associated with deficits in lan-guage development, speech understanding, andcognitive function, electrophysiologic measuresare useful in focusing the management programto the dysfunctional areas.3,6,7,12 By facilitatingthe determination of the primary site of thedisorder, auditory problems may be better de-fined in relation to other nonauditory problemsand appropriate remedial measures initiated.7,11

In addition, in children with pure auditory pro-cessing disorders, electrophysiologic measuresare most useful in determining the extent of in-volvement of the various auditory processes.This is a necessary first step in developing theappropriate management program.3,12 In any

Page 115: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

EFFICACY OF ELECTROPHYSIOLOGIC MEASURES IN APD MANAGEMENT/JIRSA 351

poral lobe representing the primary auditorycortex.7 The MLR is highly maturational andin young children is usually identified by a broadpositive wave, Pa, followed by a negative com-ponent, Nb.38,39 By the end of the first decade,the MLR is usually approaching adult valuesand is identified by a negative trough, Na, fol-lowed by Pa, then Nb and finally Pb occurringat about 50 msec post onset.38,40 The MLR maybe generated by either frequency specific tonepips or the abrupt onset acoustic click.

The clinical efficacy of the MLR in exam-ining children with APDs and other learn-ing disorders has been well documented.40–43

Musiek has advocated its use in monitoringthe maturation of the auditory pathways.7 De-layed maturational patterns are one of the pre-sumed etiological factors in various learningproblems including APDs. While the MLRmay be difficult to correctly interpret in someyoung children, its absence in those over 10years of age may be indicative of high-level au-ditory dysfunction including neurodegenerativeproblems.7 However, when used in conjunc-tion with behavioral results showing, for exam-ple, a significant left ear weakness for speechand/or nonspeech material, as well as a nega-tive neurological diagnosis, the absent, or poorlyformed MLR may be an objective marker formaturational lag.7 In this case, these resultswould support a management program focus-ing on auditory training activities addressingthe maturational issues. The MLR would thenbe an appropriate means to objectively monitorthis aspect of the management program. Morespecific information regarding maturationalstatus may be obtained by recording the MLRnot just from the vertex, but from each tempo-ral lobe as well. Examining for hemisphericand ear asymmetries often provides more de-finitive information regarding the auditory areasaffected than using just absolute latency andamplitude measures.4,43

Mismatch Negativity

The mismatch negativity (MMN) is an event-related potential (ERP), occurring as a nega-tive voltage following N1 between 100 and 500msec that reflects the pre-conscious detection

of stimulus change within an ongoing acousticstream.6,30 Unlike other long latency ERPs, theMMN appears to be present at birth.32,44 Whilethe generator sites are not clearly defined, theMMN is thought to represent primarily theauditory cortex with some input from the hip-pocampus and thalamus.10,13 The waveform iselicited using an oddball paradigm in which astring of standard stimuli are infrequently andrandomly interrupted by a deviant or oddballstimulus. According to Naatanen,27 a neuraltrace, or template, is formed by the standardstimulus and held in short-term memory, andthe negative shift from baseline (MMN) oc-curs when the central auditory system detects amismatch between this neural trace and theneural template formed by the deviant stimu-lus. It should be noted that the MMN is not anobligatory response, but that it is only gener-ated in response to the detection of stimuluschange. Because of this, and because of the factthat it is passively elicited without the activeparticipation of the listener, it has accrued sub-stantial clinical interest.

Much of the clinical research has focusedon the ability of the MMN to detect neurolog-ical change during listening training activities.For example, Kraus and her colleagues13 wantedto determine whether training listeners to iden-tify difficult speech sound contrasts would re-sult in neurophysiologic changes within thecentral auditory system. As expected, throughlistening training, subjects learned to behav-iorally discriminate the contrasts. In addition,the listening training also resulted in neuro-physiologic changes evidenced through an en-hancement in the MMN. Specifically, compar-ing pre-and post-training measures, Kraus etal13 noted significant changes in both the dura-tion and the magnitude of the MMN and con-cluded that listening training activities changedthe neurophysiology of the central auditorysystem and that these changes could be mea-sured and monitored using the MMN.

Tremblay et al28 examined the clinical util-ity of the MMN. Their findings were similar toKraus et al13 and demonstrated that behavioraltraining resulted in neurophysiologic changesas evidenced by an enhancement in the MMNresponse. Additionally, they found that theseneurophysiologic changes were evident in re-

Page 116: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

352 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

sponse to untrained stimuli. The authors feltthat their findings demonstrated the clinicalefficacy of the MMN not only in detecting andmonitoring neurophysiologic change from lis-tening training, but also in establishing whetheror not beneficial changes derived during an au-ditory training program might generalize toother listening activities.

Dalebout and Stack11 further explored theclinical implications of the MMN in a wellconceived investigation. These authors were in-terested in determining whether or not theMMN could be elicited by synthetically gener-ated speech contrasts that were not behav-iorally differentiated by listeners. Subjects wereasked to take part in a behavioral forced choicediscrimination task involving stimulus pairsalong the /da-ga/ continuum that differed onlyon the basis of the starting frequency of the f2and f3 formant transitions. For each listener,MMN data were collected using the samestimuli used in the behavioral task. Comparingboth behavioral and electrophysiologic resultsfor each of the stimulus contrasts, the authorsfound the MMN present not only for contraststhat were differentiated behaviorally, but alsofor those contrasts that could not be differenti-ated behaviorally. The authors noted that theirfindings lend support to the concept that thereare varying levels of processing for speech per-ception. As noted in the introduction, theselevels range from the acoustic level, to thephonemic level, and finally to the higher orderlinguistic level. The MMN appears to reflectthe pre-conscious acoustic level and provides aneurophysiologic way of defining levels of au-ditory processing as well as differentiating thoseindividuals who have primary difficulty at theacoustic level from those with problems at thehigher level of processing.

While the MMN appears to have sub-stantial clinical potential, further research is re-quired before it can become clinically viable.7,11

One issue centers around the detectability ofthe MMN. Because the MMN is not consis-tently identifiable in normal listeners, researchis needed to explore methods of improvingMMN detectability.11 This is related to a sec-ond issue in that there is currently no stan-dardization for the methodology used to iden-

tify, measure, and validate responses.11,29 Fi-nally, the MMN requires a comparatively longtesting time as well as additional time for off-line data analysis.22 Until these issues are ad-dressed, routine clinical use of the MMN isproblematic.

P3 Event-Related Potential

The P3 auditory event-related potential(AERP), which occurs roughly from 300 to 700msec,2,45 is similar to the MMN in that it is anonobligatory waveform elicited using an odd-ball paradigm. It also is referred to as the P3b toseparate it from an earlier occurring nonatten-tive waveform often labeled as P3a.36 Unlike theMMN, however, the P3 is not passively elicited,but requires the active participation of the lis-tener in attending to specific stimuli in an ongo-ing train of standard stimuli.35,46 Also, unlikethe MMN, the P3 matures somewhat later thanthe earlier waves with several studies showing adecrease in latency and an increase in amplitudefrom 5 through 16 years of age, followed by aprogressive decrease in amplitude and an in-crease in latency throughout adulthood.47–49

While there is considerable controversy regard-ing the neural generator sites for the P3, accu-mulating evidence suggests involvement of thethalamus, inferior parietal lobe, temporal lobe,dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, cingulated cortex,amygdala, and the hippocampus.36

Because generation of the P3 does requireactive listener participation, it is widely recog-nized as a physiologic measure of cognitiveprocessing.2 Thus, the P3 reflects processes re-lated to attention, decision-making, and mem-ory updating. P3 latency appears to be a func-tion of stimulus evaluation time including therecognition and categorization of a stimulus,44

the speed of information processing,47 andshort-term working memory processes.37 P3amplitude appears to be related to the subjec-tive probability of the stimulus, stimulus mean-ing, and information processing.50

Because the P3 is related to the active stateof the listener, it has been extensively used toevaluate various aspects of psychophysiology,psychopathology, and aging,48,49,51 In general,

Page 117: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

EFFICACY OF ELECTROPHYSIOLOGIC MEASURES IN APD MANAGEMENT/JIRSA 353

results have shown increases in latency and de-creases in amplitude in the clinical popula-tion.2 In addition, the P3 has been used to in-vestigate various learning and developmentalprocesses in both children and adults, includ-ing hyperactivity,52 and language and motorspeech disorders.40

The P3 has been investigated in childrenwith APDs. Jirsa and Clontz2 compared chil-dren with confirmed APDs to age matchedcontrols and observed significant increases inlatency and decreases in amplitude in the clini-cal group. In addition, comparing performanceon behavioral measures to the P3 results, theauthors observed a relationship between P3 la-tency and amplitude, and deficits in selectiveattention, short-term memory, and auditorydiscrimination ability.

The clinical relevance of the P3 in a man-agement program was investigated by Jirsa.3 Inthis study, two groups of children were evalu-ated behaviorally and electrophysiologicallyand confirmed to have APDs. One group re-ceived 14 weeks of auditory training therapytwice a week and the other group served as thecontrols and received no therapy. Following the14-week program, all children in both groupswere re-evaluated. Children in the clinical groupshowed a significant decrease in P3 latency anda significant increase in P3 amplitude follow-ing the treatment program. No changes werenoted in the control group. Children in the clin-ical group also demonstrated significant im-provements on the behavioral test battery.

Results such as these support the clinicalutility of the P3. Because it does appear to besensitive to behavioral changes resulting from atherapeutic program, the P3 may be most usefulin monitoring therapy progress. Because the P3can be elicited by a variety of stimuli, includingphonemes and speech contrasts, it would be use-ful in directing the focus of therapy in discrimi-nation tasks, or for assessing sensitivity to fre-quency and intensity changes. Also, the P3,when used in conjunction with the MMN, mayprovide an objective means for evaluating thevarious levels of speech perception. Because theMMN has been shown to reflect the pre-atten-tive auditory level of processing,11 and the P3 hasbeen shown to reflect activity at the higher cog-

nitive levels of processing,46 together these neu-rophysiologic measures may provide a means formore precisely identifying the auditory capabili-ties of individuals diagnosed with APDs, and as-sist in establishing the most appropriate therapyprogram. Certainly, individuals whose auditoryproblems stem from deficits in the neurophysio-logic processes underlying speech perceptionwould require a different management programfrom those whose difficulties reside at the higherlevels of processing. Furthermore, a recent inves-tigation by Salamat and McPherson,35 using asustained attention task at various interstimulusintervals to generate the P3, demonstrated theefficacy of using the P3 to quantify attentionalprocesses. The P3 may provide a diagnosticmeans of differentiating those with primarily at-tentional problems from those with auditory-based problems.

SUMMARY

The electrophysiologic measures discussed inthis section hold much promise in the diagno-sis and management of APDs when used inconjunction with their behavioral counterparts.Because a critical component of any manage-ment program is an accurate diagnosis of theproblem, one of the strengths of these mea-sures is the potential to be able to identify anddescribe problems at all levels of auditory pro-cessing. Both the MMN and the N1-P2 com-plex, as well as perhaps the MLS-ABR, holdpromise of providing critical information at thepre-attentive neurophysiologic level of process-ing, while the P3 provides information relativeto the higher levels of processing. In additionto providing diagnostic data to direct therapy,electrophysiological measures provide an excel-lent means of monitoring therapy progress. Byproviding information relative to the underly-ing neurophysiology in conjunction with be-havioral change, these measures can serve avital function in focusing the management pro-gram to achieve the best possible outcome. Asresearch continues in this area, there is everyreason to believe that the clinical efficacy ofelectrophysiologic measures will become evenmore apparent.

Page 118: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

354 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

ABBREVIATIONS

ABR auditory brainstem responseAPD auditory processing disorderERP event-related potentialMLR middle latency responseMLS maximum length sequencesMMN mismatch negativity

REFERENCES

1. Jerger JF, Musiek FE. Report of a consensus con-ference on the diagnosis of auditory processing dis-orders in school-aged children. J Am Acad Audiol2000;11:467–474

2. Jirsa RE, Clontz K. Long latency auditory event-related potentials from children with auditory pro-cessing disorders. Ear Hear 1990;11:222–232

3. Jirsa RE. The clinical utility of the P3 AERP inchildren with auditory processing disorders. JSpeech Hear Res 1992;35:903–912

4. Musiek FE, Baran JA, Pinheiro ML. Neuroaudiol-ogy Case Studies. San Diego, CA: Singular; 1994

5. Jerger JF. Controversial issues in central auditoryprocessing disorders. Semin Hear 1998;19:393–397

6. Kraus N, Koch D, McGee T, Nicol T, CunninghamJ. Speech-sound discrimination in school-age chil-dren: psychophysical and neurophysiologic mea-sures. J Speech Lang Hear Res 1999;42:1042–1060

7. Musiek FE, Berge BE. How electrophysiologictests of central auditory processing influence man-agement. In: Bess F, ed. Children with HearingImpairment. Nashville, TN:Vanderbilt-Bill Wilk-erson Center Press;1998:145–161

8. Delgutte B, Kiang NYS. Speech coding in the au-ditory nerve. I. Vowel-like sounds. J Acoust SocAm 1984;75:866–878

9. Werner LA, Folsom RC, Mancl LR, Syapin CL.Human auditory brainstem response to temporalgaps in noise. J Speech Lang Hear Res 2001;44:737–750

10. Kraus N, McGee T, Carrell T, et al. Discriminationof speech-like contrasts in the auditory thalamusand cortex. J Acoust Soc Am 1994; 96:2758–2768

11. Dalebout SD, Stack JW. Mismatch negativity toacoustical differences not differentiated behav-iorally. J Am Acad Audiol 1999;10:388–399

12. Jirsa RE. Maximum length sequences-auditorybrainstem responses from children with auditoryprocessing disorders. J Am Acad Audiol 2001;12:155–164

13. Kraus N, McGee T, Carrell TD, et al. Central au-ditory system plasticity associated with speech dis-crimination training. J Cog Neurosci 1995;7:25–32

14. Hall JW III. Handbook of Auditory Evoked Re-sponses. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon; 1992

15. Goldstein R, Aldrich WM. Evoked Potential Au-diometry. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon; 1999

16. Fifer R, Sierra-Irizarry B. Clinical applications ofthe auditory middle latency response. Am J Otol1988;9(Suppl 1):47–56

17. Musiek FE, Lenz S, Gollegly K. Neuroaudiologiccorrelates to anatomical changes in the brain. Am JAudiol 1991;1:19–24

18. Cacace AT, McFarland DJ. Middle-latency audi-tory evoked potentials: basic issues and potentialapplications. In: Katz J, ed. Handbook of ClinicalAudiology, 5th ed. Philadelphia, PA: LippincottWilliams & Wilkins; 2002:349–377

19. Sharma A, Kraus N, McGee TJ, Nicol T. Develop-mental changes in P1 and N1 central auditoryresponses elicited by consonant-vowel syllables.Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophys 1997;104:540–545

20. Martin BA, Kurtzberg D, Stapells DR. The effectsof decreased audibility produced by high-passnoise masking on N1 and the mismatch negativityto speech sounds /ba/ and /da/. J Speech LangHear Res 1999;42: 271–286

21. Sharma A, Dorman MF. Cortical auditory evokedpotential correlates of categorical perception ofvoice-onset time. J Acoust Soc Am 1999;106:1078–1083

22. Tremblay K, Kraus N, McGee T, Ponton C, Otis B.Central auditory plasticity: changes in the N1-P2complex after speech-sound training. Ear Hear2001;22:79–90

23. Sams M, Paavilainen P, Alho K, Naatanen, R. Au-ditory frequency discrimination and event-relatedpotentials. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophys1985;62:437–448

24. Kraus N, McGee T, Micco A, et al. Mismatch neg-ativity in school-age children to speech stimuli thatare just perceptibly different. J Clin Neurophys1993;88:123–130

25. Sharma A., Kraus N, McGee T, Carrell T, Nicol T.Acoustic versus phonetic representation of speechas reflected by the mismatch negativity event-related potential. Electroencephalogr Clin Neuro-phys 1993;88:64–71

26. Kraus N, McGee T, Littman T, Nicol T, King C.Nonprimary auditory thalamic representation ofacoustic change. J Neurophys 1994;72:1270–1277

27. Naatanen R. The mismatch negativity: a powerfultool for cognitive neuroscience. Ear Hear 1995;16:6–18

28. Tremblay K, Kraus N, Carrell TD, McGee T. Cen-tral auditory system plasticity: generalization tonovel stimuli following listening training. J AcoustSoc Am 1997;102:3762–3773

Page 119: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

EFFICACY OF ELECTROPHYSIOLOGIC MEASURES IN APD MANAGEMENT/JIRSA 355

29. Dalebout SD, Fox LG. Identification of the mis-match negativity in the responses of individual lis-teners. J Am Acad Audiol 2000;11:12–22

30. Dalebout SD, Fox LG. Reliability of the mismatchnegativity in the responses of individual listeners. JAm Acad Audiol 2001;12:245–253

31. Takegata R, Paavilainen P, Naatanen R, Winkler I.Preattentive processing of spectral, temporal, andstructural characteristics of acoustic regulatrities: amismatch negativity study. Psychophysiology 2001;38:92–98

32. Walker LJ, Carpenter M, Down CR, et al. Possibleneuronal refractory or recovery artifacts associatedwith recording the mismatch negativity response. JAm Acad Audiol 2001;12:348–356

33. Polich J. Task difficulty, probability, and interstim-ulus interval as determinants of P300 from audi-tory stimuli. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophys1987;68:311–320

34. Polich J. P300 clinical utility and control of vari-ability. J Clin Neurophys 1998;15:14–33

35. Salamat M, McPherson D. Interactions amongvariables in the P300 response to a continuous per-formance task. J Am Acad Audiol 1999;10:379–387

36. Kiehl KA, Laurens KR, Duty TL, Forster BB,Liddle PF. Neural sources involved in auditorytarget detection and novelty processing: an event-related fMRI study. Psychophysiology 2001;38:133–142

37. Yordanova J, Kolev V, Polich J. P300 and alphaevent-related desynchronization (ERD). Psycho-physiology 2001;38:143–152

38. Suzki T, Hirabayashi M. Age-related morphologi-cal changes in auditory middle-latency response.Audiology 1987;26:312–320

39. Musiek FE, Verkst SB, Gollegly KM Effects ofneuro-maturation in auditory-evoked potentials.Semin Hear 1988;9:1–13

40. Mason BM, Mellor DH. Brain-stem, middle la-tency, and late cortical evoked potentials in chil-dren with speech and language disorders. Elec-troencephalogr Clin Neurophys 1984;59:297–309

41. Jerger S, Jerger J. Audiologic applications of early,middle, and late evoked potentials. Hear J 1985;38:31–36

42. Jerger J, Oliver T, Chimel R. Auditory middle la-tency response: a perspective. Semin Hear 1988;9:75–86

43. Musiek FE, Charette L, Kelly T, Wee WW, MusiekE. Hit and false-positive rates for the middle la-tency response in patients with central nervoussystem involvement. J Am Acad Audiol 1999;10:124–132

44. Alho K, Sainio K, Reinikainen K, Naatanen R.Electrical brain response of human new borns topitch change of an acoustic stimulus. Electroen-cephalogr Clin Neurophys 1990;77:151–155

45. Squires KC, Hecox KC. Electrophysiological eval-uation of higher level auditory processing. SeminHear 1983;4:415–433

46. McPherson D. Late Potentials of the AuditorySystem. San Diego, CA: Singular; 1996

47. Courchesne E. Neurophysiological correlates ofcognitive development: changes in long-latencyevent-related potentials from childhood to adult-hood. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophys 1978;45:468–482

48. Pfefferbaum A, Ford JM, Roth WT, Kopell BS.Age-related changes in auditory event-related po-tentials. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophys 1980;49:266–276

49. Polich J, Howard L, Starr A. Effects of age on theP300 component of the event-related potentialfrom auditory stimuli: peak definition, variation,and measurement. J Gerontol 1985;40:721–726

50. Johnson R. A triarchic model of P300 amplitude.Psychophysiology 1986;30:367–384

51. Ford JM, White PM, Csernansky JG, et al. ERPsin schizophrenia: effects of antipsychotic medica-tion. Biol Psychol 1994;36:153–170

52. Satterfield JH, Schell AM, Backs RW, Hidaka KC.A cross-sectional and longitudinal study of age ef-fects of electrophysiological measures in hyperactiveand normal children. Biol Psychol 1984;19:973–990

Page 120: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders
Page 121: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

357

Management of Auditory Processing Disorders; Editor in Chief, Catherine V. Palmer, Ph.D.; Guest Editor, Gail D.Chermak, Ph.D. Seminars in Hearing, volume 23, number 4, 2002. Address for correspondence and reprint requests:Minka Hildesheimer, Ph.D., Speech and Hearing Center, Department of Communication Disorders, Sheba MedicalCenter, Tel-Hashomer, 52621 Israel. E-mail: [email protected]. 1Speech and Hearing Center, Chaim ShebaMedical Center, Tel Hashomer, Israel, and Department of Communication Disorders, Tel Aviv University, Tel Aviv,Israel. Copyright © 2002 by Thieme Medical Publishers, Inc., 333 Seventh Avenue, New York, NY 10001, USA. Tel:+1(212) 584-4662. 00734-0451,p;2002,23,04,357,364,ftx,en;sih00229x.

Treatment and Evaluation Indices ofAuditory Processing DisordersHanna Putter-Katz, Ph.D.,1 Limor Adi-Ben Said, M.A.,1 Irit Feldman, M.A.,1Dana Miran, B.A.,1 Dana Kushnir, M.A.,1 Chava Muchnik, Ph.D.,1 andMinka Hildesheimer, Ph.D.1

ABSTRACT

Listening skills were compared before and after a structured inter-vention program for a group of 20 children with auditory processing disor-ders (APD). Comparisons of pre- and post- management measures indi-cated a significant increase in speech recognition performance in degradedlistening conditions (background noise and competing speech). The APDmanagement approach was integrative and included top-down and bot-tom-up strategies. These findings add to a growing body of literature sug-gesting that interactive auditory training can improve communicationskills.

KEYWORDS: Auditory processing disorder, auditory learning,management, degraded listening conditions

Learning Outcomes: Upon completion of this article, the reader will (1) become familiar with one of the mea-sures of auditory processing, the index of performance accuracy, which reflects a global assessment of audi-tory information processing; (2) recognize the value of performance accuracy scores in degraded listening con-ditions in evaluating treatment efficacy for auditory processing disorders; and (3) recognize some of thelimitations of the performance accuracy index and the possible contribution of temporal measures in indicatingauditory processing difficulties and in evaluating treatment efficacy for auditory processing disorders.

ing as a function of age, peripheral hearing sen-sitivity1,2 and neurological status. Furthermore,because APD affects information processingranging from sound reception to discourse un-derstanding, there is considerable opportunityfor various types and degrees of disruption.3 Fi-

The population manifesting auditory pro-cessing disorders (APD) consists of subgroupsbased on diagnostic findings and diverse func-tional deficit profiles. A number of factors con-tribute to the variability of functional deficits.Subjects with APD are heterogeneous, differ-

Page 122: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

358 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

nally, a range of evaluation procedures are usedto diagnose APD, including a variety of audi-tory stimuli (i.e., verbal/non-verbal; synthesized/natural), differing tasks (i.e., auditory/ phono-logical/semantic), and several presentationmodes (i.e., monotic/dichotic/binaural). Giventhe multiplicity of factors underlying the inter-subject variability of deficits involved in APD,4most reports concerning APD managementhave been case studies.5–7 Few group studiesof APD treatment have been reported in theliterature.8–10

Among the demands placed on the audi-tory system during everyday listening is theseparation of speech from background noise.11

Not surprisingly, most individuals with APDdemonstrate particular communication diffi-culties in noise.11–15 Increased susceptibility tomasking, dichotic listening and selective atten-tion deficits, and temporal processing difficul-ties are the most frequently reported deficits inchildren with APD.3 The complex process ofspeech recognition requires central pattern rec-ognition, as well as detailed analysis of sensoryinformation.16 Children are especially vulnera-ble to degraded listening environments, achiev-ing lower speech recognition scores than adultsto spectrally degraded speech.16 Part of thespeech recognition deficit observed in youngerchildren with APD is due to their inability tofully use sensory information; part of thedeficit is due to their incomplete linguistic andcognitive development. Despite general ac-knowledgment that competing noise and com-peting speech excessively tax auditory percep-tion, there are only a few reports on the effectsof management on auditory behaviors in thesedifficult listening situations among the popula-tion with APD.

Management of APD is designed to im-prove listening skills and spoken language com-prehension.14 The intervention techniques gen-erally are classified into two categories: (1)enhancement of the primary signal via manip-ulation of the listening environment, and (2)specific training of auditory skills. The latterfrequently includes the teaching of compensa-tory strategies that allow the child to functionbetter in the classroom and at home.6 The effi-cacy of these programs is not uniform acrosschildren with APD.5 This variability may be

based partly on variable functional deficits andon individual listening challenges. Thus, in thepresent study we evaluated the efficacy of in-tervention in a group of 20 children diagnosedwith APD at the Speech and Hearing Centerof the Sheba Medical Center in Tel-Hashomer,Israel. The present report is restricted to APDin children whose peripheral hearing and intel-ligence were within normal range and withoutany known neurological pathology. Treatmentefficacy was evaluated by measuring behavioralspeech performance accuracy in the presenceof competing noise and competing speech.

METHOD

Participants

Twenty children between 7 years and 11months, and 14 years and 4 months of age(mean age = 9.4; SD = 1.8) participated in theAPD treatment program. The participants (8girls and 12 boys) were native Hebrew speakersand were referred for central auditory assess-ment due to listening difficulties and one ofthe following symptoms: deficient spoken andwritten language comprehension, limited at-tention, or academic underachievement. Theirmost common behavioral complaints were dif-ficulties in understanding verbal stimuli in thepresence of noise or competing speech. All hadhearing sensitivity within normal limits bilat-erally,17 excellent word recognition abilities inquiet, normal middle ear function, normal au-ditory brain stem response (ABR), and normalacoustic reflex thresholds. Their intelligencewas within normal range without psychologi-cal or emotional difficulties or known neuro-logical complications.

PROCEDURE

APD Evaluation

All participants underwent an auditory process-ing evaluation using conventionally recom-mended measures to assess the functional in-tegrity of the auditory system. The auditoryassessment was conducted in three sessions at

Page 123: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

TREATMENT AND EVALUATION INDICES OF APD/PUTTER-KATZ ET AL 359

the Speech and Hearing Clinic of the ShebaMedical Center. In the first session, a routine au-diologic evaluation consisting of pure-tone airand bone conduction, speech reception thresh-old, word recognition, immittance testing, acous-tic reflex testing, ABR and transient evoked oto-acoustic emissions (TEOAE) was conducted.Each child and his or her parents were inter-viewed prior to central auditory testing, andthorough information regarding presenting com-plaints, academic abilities, and developmentaland medical history were obtained in the secondsession.

Central auditory testing was conducted inthe third session, which consisted of the Com-peting Sentences (i.e., binaural separation or se-lective attention) test,19 a speech-in-noise test,20

gap detection, and masking level differences(MLD). Two versions of the competing sen-tences tests were administered. One version con-tained three word sentences (i.e., short compet-ing) and the other version contained five wordsentences (i.e., long competing). The speech-in-noise test20 is a monaural low-redundancy speechtask that presents monosyllabic words in speechnoise. Speech tests were adapted for presentationin Hebrew.

Gap detection and the MLD procedureare nonverbal, temporal auditory tasks. Partici-pants were required to detect gaps in whitenoise bursts presented monaurally and to de-tect the presence of a binaurally presented 500Hz tone in binaural white noise. Rest periodsof 5 to 15 min were provided as needed be-tween test conditions. Participants were diag-nosed with APD on the basis of abnormal per-formance (at least one standard deviation (SD)below the mean)4 in either ear on any one test.This criterion was based on normative data ac-cumulated at our clinic. On the basis of perfor-mance on the pre-treatment central speech teststwo subgroups were observed. Group I in-cluded 11 subjects who presented reduced per-formance on the speech in noise test and nor-mal performance on the dichotic tasks. GroupII included 9 subjects who demonstrated poorperformance on both speech in noise and di-chotic listening tasks. Of the children who wereable to complete the nonverbal tests, most dem-onstrated abnormal MLDs, while gap detec-tion was within normal range. Inclusion crite-

ria for treatment included the APD diagnosisbased on this assessment protocol and a co-existing report of difficulties in auditory per-formance by the parents, teachers, speech andhearing clinicians or by the child.12

APD Management

All participants were scheduled for one 45 mintreatment session per week,8 extending over afour-month period (i.e., 13–15 sessions). Par-ticipants were divided into four groups of 5subjects each for technical clinical constraints.Treatment sessions were conducted by 2 speechand hearing clinicians experienced in auditoryprocessing difficulties in children. The APDtreatment sessions were held both in a stan-dard therapy room and in the sound-treatedroom of the Speech and Hearing Center. Themain goal of the APD management was to en-able the children to better cope with the vari-ous auditory demands placed upon them athome and at school. The APD managementprogram focused on environmental modifica-tions, remediation techniques, and compensa-tory strategies.12,14

ENVIRONMENTAL MODIFICATION AND

TEACHING SUGGESTIONS

The children’s teachers were given a brief ex-planation of the nature of APD and the ways itmight impact school activities. Teachers wereinstructed to try to keep the learning environ-ment highly redundant. Some of the recom-mendations they were given included decreasingbackground noise, using preferential seating,and using a tape-recorder in lectures. The im-portance of the educator’s involvement and co-operation was emphasized.12

REMEDIATION TECHNIQUES

The main goal of this component was to im-prove overall auditory processing abilities, es-pecially in a noisy environment.21,22 Tasks in-cluded: listening and comprehension activitiesin the presence of noise and competing verbalstimuli, and selective and divided attention tasks.

Page 124: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

360 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

Treatment tasks were built hierarchically as afunction of language complexity, and differentlevels of background noise and competingspeech. Each treatment session was specificallydesigned to build progressively upon prior ac-complishments in an effort to maximize prog-ress.8

COMPENSATORY STRATEGIES

The goal of this component of the manage-ment program was to assist the child in learn-ing to cope with auditory problems by using thefollowing strategies: auditory closure, speech-reading, assistive listening (i.e., demonstrationand trial of frequency modulated [FM] de-vices), and metacognitive awareness enhance-ment. Parental involvement was especially inte-gral to this component of the managementprogram. Parents were pivotal to ensuring thechildren applied these compensatory strategiesbeyond the intervention sessions. Moreover,parents were essential to the subjective assess-ment of treatment efficacy.

RESULTS

All test subjects completed the treatment pro-gram. At the end of the intervention program,all subjects were re-administered the centralspeech test battery (i.e., speech in noise andcompeting sentences).

The effects of the APD management onspeech perception performance were analyzedseparately for Group I and Group II and com-pared between subgroups. Each subject servedas his or her own control; auditory performancewas compared pre- and post- treatment withineach subject.

Figure 1 displays the pre-treatment per-formance accuracy (in percent correct) of thepre-treatment speech test battery for bothgroups. A multivariate analysis of variance(MANOVA) with repeated measures revealedno difference between groups in speech innoise scores (F=1.02, df=1,18, p> 0.05 for theright ear; F=0.22, df=1,18, p> 0.05 for the leftear). Significant differences between groupswere found for both ears in the performance onthe short version of competing sentences(F=13.86, df= 1,18, p< 0.005 for the right ear;F=5.62, df= 1, 18, p< 0.05 for the left ear) andfor the right ear results of the long version ofthis test (F=17.73, df=1,18, p< 0.0005). Theseresults confirmed the groups’ differentiationand classification.

Pre- and post-treatment central speech testresults for Group I are shown in Table 1. A sig-nificant improvement was found following treat-ment for the right ear in the speech in noisetest (p< 0.05). No differences were found forthe performance accuracy scores of the left earpre- and post-treatment. No differences wereobserved for the pre- and post-treatment per-formance accuracy scores of Group I in the di-chotic listening tasks.

Figure 1 Mean and standard errors of pre-treatment performance accuracy scores (in percent correct) of thespeech test battery for Group I and Group II. rsn, speech-in-noise of the right ear; lsn, speech-in-noise of theleft ear; res, lcs, right and left competing sentences; short/long, short and long version of cs.

Page 125: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

TREATMENT AND EVALUATION INDICES OF APD/PUTTER-KATZ ET AL 361

Table 1 Group I Mean and Standard Deviations of Performance Accuracy (in Percent Correct)

Pre- and Post-Treatment

Pre-Treatment Post-Treatment

Right Left Right Left

Test X SD X SD X SD X SD

Speech-in-noise 64.63 6.98 62.63 13.45 74.09* 10.42 72.54 8.3Short competing sentences 98.09 3.08 88.09 10.6 98.7 1.7 88.4 11.43Long competing sentences 84.4 19.72 69.2 26.85 88.0 10.88 79.0 17.2

*p<0.05SD, standard deviation; X, mean.

Table 2 shows pre- and post-treatmentcentral speech test results for Group II. Signif-icant differences were found for each test inthe battery following intervention, except theshort competing sentences test for the right ear(p< 0.05)(Table 3).

Figure 2 displays the posttreatment per-formance accuracy (in percent correct) of thespeech test battery of both groups. MANOVAwith repeated measures indicated no differ-ences between groups for speech in noise of theright ear (F= 3.78, df=1,18, p> 0.05), the shortversion of competing sentences (F=3.43, df=1,18, p> 0.05, for the right ear; F= 1.28, df=1,18,p> 0.05 for the left ear), or for the left ear forthe long version of competing sentences (F=1.24, df= 1, 18, p> 0.05). Significant differ-

ences between groups were found for the rightear in the long version of competing sentences(F=8.54, df=1, 18, p< 0.05) and for speech innoise of the left ear (F=5.34, df=1, 18, p< 0.05)

DISCUSSION

The APD management described and evalu-ated in the present study was based on an inte-grative approach predicted on the growing bodyof literature suggesting that central auditory pro-cessing invokes both bottom-up and top-downprocesses3. In normally hearing individuals, au-ditory processing is regulated by passive and ac-tive learning, resulting in experience-dependentenhancements of performance.2 The impor-

Table 2 Group II Mean and Standard Deviations of Performance Accuracy (in Percent Correct)

Pre- and Post-Treatment

Pre-Treatment Post-Treatment

Right Left Right Left

Tests X SD X SD X SD X SD

Speech-in-noise 67.33 5.91 65.66 15.7 81.77* 6.2 81.33* 8.66Short dichotic sentences 66.2 26.78 59.55 34.88 82.33 27.99 79.66* 21.23Long dichotic sentences 31.33 34.07 48.44 29.47 62.11* 24.55 65.88* 29.26

*p<0.05SD, standard deviation; X, mean.

Table 3 T-tests for Pre- and Post-Treatment Measures of Auditory Processing for Group

Auditory Task Right Left

Speech-in-noise t =�6.7 df=8 p =.0002 t =�2.47 df=8 p =.04Short competing sentences t =�1.73 df=8 p > .05 t =�3.32 df=8 p =.01Long competing sentences t =�3.45 df=8 p =.0087 t =�2.39 df=8 p =.043

Page 126: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

362 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

Figure 2 Mean and standard error of post-treatment performance accuracy scores (in percent correct) of thespeech test battery for Group I and Group II. rsn, speech-in-noise of the right ear; lsn, speech-in-noise of theleft ear; res, lcs, right and left competing sentences; short/long, short and long version of cs.

tance of integrative processing of auditory in-formation (i.e., sensory/peripheral and central)is underscored in children16 who are developingcognitive and linguistic abilities. Moreover, forchildren with APD, internal distortions degradethe auditory signal so that top-down process-ing typically predominates in most listeningsituations, particularly those in which complexlinguistic and cognitive demands are coupledwith background noise. Therefore, managementof APD should be based upon an integrativetop-down and bottom-up approach that con-siders both skill-specific deficits and cognitive-linguistic features.23

The focus of the APD integrative man-agement in the present study was on auditorylearning.2 Specifically, the children acquiredauditory experience in hierarchically structuredlistening conditions that are known to be chal-lenging for individuals with APD. This ap-proach is consistent with the ultimate goal ofAPD management in children to maximize theauditory learning abilities so that communica-tion and classroom learning experiences aremore successful.14,23

The auditory function of the children par-ticipating in the present study demonstratedsome improvement following intervention, asindicated by improved performance on speechin noise and competing sentences tasks. Thesetests give a direct assessment of auditory percep-tual and functional deficits.3 In addition, theseimprovements were corroborated by children,parents and teachers who reported improve-

ments in overall listening behaviors and abilitiesat home and in the classroom. These resultssuggest that skills generalized from the clinic toother speech communication situations.24

Although the children were classified intotwo groups based on different patterns of per-formance on the central auditory speech testsprior to intervention, little difference was ob-served between groups following treatment.(Following treatment the two groups differedonly in left ear speech in noise performance andright ear performance on the long competingsentence test.) While Group II demonstratedimprovement following treatment on almostall measures, Group I demonstrated improvedspeech in noise for the right ear only. The dif-ference in response to treatment may reflect thedifference in severity of the APD betweengroups. In addition, the limited improvementof Group I following treatment may be more areflection of the limitations of the outcomemeasures than a true absence of behavioralchange. Behavioral tests that reveal functionalprocessing deficits reflect only the end productand not the whole process. Subjects with APDusually achieve high word recognition scores toundistorted speech materials. However, as re-ported in the literature and on the basis of ourexperience, their auditory behavior differs fromthat of control subjects even when their perfor-mance accuracy scores are high. Recent datafrom our center indicate the potential of time-based measures to complement information ob-tained from speech recognition testing of sub-

Page 127: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

TREATMENT AND EVALUATION INDICES OF APD/PUTTER-KATZ ET AL 363

jects with APD. Preliminary results indicatelonger reaction time (RT) in subjects with APDcompared to controls, despite similar high speechrecognition scores. These results, although pre-liminary, imply that RT may be a useful crite-rion to evaluate speech recognition in degradedlistening conditions and to evaluate manage-ment efficacy.

Studying children who undergo APDmanagement holds the promise of providingimportant insights into perceptual changes as-sociated with auditory learning. It is importantto determine which children might benefit fromintervention and how treatment may alter theirspeech processing in everyday listening. Theauditory processing remediation approach shouldincorporate analysis, synthesis, integration andinterpretation of auditory input for communi-cation, and learning as Chermak had alreadysuggested in 1981.25 Using electrophysiologicmeasures (e.g., event-related potentials: MMN,P300, N400) may complement the behavioraltest battery used in this study, and providemore specific information on the various levelsand mechanisms of auditory processing.8 Withthese tools, we hope it will be possible to assessthe role of sensory, perceptual, cognitive, andlinguistic processes that contribute to the ob-served variation among subjects with APD.

ABBREVIATIONS

ABR auditory brain stem responseAPD auditory processing disordersFM frequency modulatedMANOVA multivariate analysis of varianceMLD masking level differencesRT reaction timeSD standard deviationTEOAE transient evoked otoacoustic

emissions

REFERENCES

1. Jerger J, Musiek FE. Report of the consensus con-ference on the diagnosis of auditory processing dis-orders in school-aged children. J Am Acad Audiol2000;11:467–474

2. Moore D, Hogan SC, Kacelnik O, et al. Auditorylearning as a cause and treatment of central dys-function. Audiol Neurootol 2001;6:216–220

3. Chermak GD. Central testing. In: Gerber SE, ed.The Handbook of Pediatric Audiology. Washing-ton, DC: Gallaudet University Press; 1996:206–253

4. Ferre J, Willber L. Normal and learning disabledchildren’s central auditory processing skills: an ex-perimental test battery. Ear Hear 1986;7:336–343

5. Musiek FE. Habilitation and management of audi-tory processing disorders: overview of selected pro-cedures. J Am Acad Audiol 1999;10:329–342

6. Bellis TJ, Ferre JM. Multidimensional Approach tothe differential diagnosis of central auditory pro-cessing disorders in children. J Am Acad Audiol1999;10:319–328

7. Garstecki D, Hutton CL, Nerbonne MA, NewmanCW, Smoski WJ. Case study examples using self-assessment. Ear Hear 1990;11(Suppl):48S–56S

8. Jirsa RE. The clinical utility of the P3 AERP in chil-dren with auditory processing disorders. J SpeechHear Res 1992;35:903–912

9. Yencer KA. The effects of auditory integrationtraining for children with central auditory process-ing disorders. Am J Audiol 1998;7:32–44

10. Hesse G, Netling M, Mohrmann B, Laubert A,Ptok M. Intensive inpatient therapy of auditoryprocessing and perceptual disorders in childhood.HNO 2001;49:636–641

11. Kraus N. Auditory pathway encoding and neuralplasticity in children with learning problems. Au-diol Neurootol 2001;6:221–227

12. Bellis TJ. Assessment and Management of CentralAuditory Processing Disorders in the EducationalSetting: From Science to Practice. San Diego, CA:Singular; 1996

13. Welsh LW, Welsh JJ, Healy MP. Learning disabili-ties and central auditory dysfunction. Ann OtolRhinol Laryngol 1996;105:117–122

14. Chermak GD, Musiek FE. Central Auditory Pro-cessing Disorders: New Perspectives. San Diego,CA: Singular; 1997

15. Cunningham J, Nicol T, Zecker SG, Kraus N.Neurobiologic responses to speech in noise in chil-dren with learning problems: deficits and strategiesfor improvement. Clin Neurophysiol 2001;112:758–767

16. Eisenberg LS, Shannon RV, Martinez AS,Wygonski J, Boothroyd A. Speech recognitionwith reduced spectral cues as a function of age. JAcoust Soc Am 2000;107:2704–2710

17. ANSI. ANSI s3.6–1996, “Specifications for Au-diometers.” New York: American National Stan-dards Institute; 1996

18. American Speech-Language-Hearing AssociationTask Force on Central Auditory Processing Con-

Page 128: Central Auditory System and Central Auditory Processing Disorders

364 SEMINARS IN HEARING/VOLUME 23, NUMBER 4 2002

sensus Development. Central auditory processing:current status of research and implications for clin-ical practice. Am J Audiol 1996;5:41–54

19. Willeford JA, Burleigh JM. Sentence procedures incentral auditory testing. In: Katz J, ed. Handbookof Clinical Audiology, 4th ed. Baltimore, MD:Williams and Wilkins; 1994:259–268

20. Musiek FE. Central auditory tests. Scand Audiol1999;28(Suppl.51): 33–46

21. Katz J, Stecker N, Henderson D, eds. Central Audi-tory Processing: A Transdisciplinary View. St Louis,MO: Mosby; 1992

22. Schneider, D. Audiologic management of centralauditory processing disorders. In: Katz J, Stecker

N, Henderson D, eds. Central Auditory Process-ing: A Transdisciplinary View. St Louis, MO:Mosby; 1992:161–168

23. Young ML, Protti-Patterson E. Management per-spectives of central auditory problems in children:top-down and bottom-up considerations. SeminHear 1984;5:251–261

24. Pisoni DB, Aslin RN, Perey AJ, Hennessy BL.Some effects of laboratory training on identifica-tion and discrimination of voicing contrasts in stopconsonants. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform1982;8:297–314

25. Chermak GD. Handbook of Audiological Rehabil-itation. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas; 1981