by blake m allan submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for...

177
Refining technology to examine animal movement in modified landscapes by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Deakin University December, 2016

Upload: others

Post on 03-May-2020

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

Refining technology to examine animal movement in modified landscapes

by

Blake M Allan

Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of

Doctor of Philosophy

Deakin University

December, 2016

Page 2: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with
parisr
Redacted stamp
Page 3: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with
parisr
Redacted stamp
Page 4: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

i

Acknowledgements

The many years of a PhD leave you with many people to thank. I have done my best

to remember you all here.

Firstly, Euan G. Ritchie, my primary supervisor. Your positivity and optimism have

known no bounds, whether it was designing and re-designing chapters, or running

around in the dark teaching me how to catch possums. Your belief that my thesis would

get finished has been a major support through the roller-coaster that is a PhD.

Also, my three co-supervisors, Dale Nimmo, John Arnould, and Jenny Martin. Each

of you has played very different roles throughout my PhD, and it wouldn’t have come

together without you. Thank you Dale for putting up with my poor understanding of

statistical analysis, pseudo-replication, and auto-correlation. Thank you John for

getting me involved with GPS and accelerometer devices, and your no-nonsense

approach to tasks. Finally thank you Jenny for your extensive knowledge of possums;

their movements, handling, and processing, not to mention the work you undertook on

them which paved the way for my PhD.

Several other people also provided major contributions to this PhD. In particular, my

thanks go to Dr. Kath Handasyde for letting me use her getaway in the Strathbogies as

a base, and knowledge of the possums in the area, to Dr. April Gloury for her help

when I first started, and making sure that field work didn’t overwhelm me. Thank you

Bertram Lobart for your knowledge of the landowners, cups of tea, and a warm

fireplace on cold days. Thank you to Mike Cleeland and Debbie Hynes for your

original spotting of bobucks in Grantville, and your knowledge of where I might find

Page 5: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

ii

more. Also, thank you Dr. Andrew Hoskins, for the codes you had written for

processing accelerometer data in R.

To my (now wife) Lisa. Thank you for putting up with me for the many years of my

PhD. Thank you for being my “volunteer” for numerous pieces of field work, your

knowledge of plants, reading countless drafts, and for providing much love, support

and useful feedback. Thank you for always being there and supporting all my crazy

endeavours. I also thank my mum and dad, for reading through my thesis and hounding

me more than my supervisor about completing it, and to my brother Jordan for just

letting me do my thing.

Much appreciation goes to all the landowners who let me work on their properties:

Chris Coles, Bertram Lobart, Helga Leunig, Ben Brook, Barb Stewart, and the

Grantville Lodge. Thank you to all my volunteers over my years of field work. There

are too many of you to name here, but I am very grateful for your hard work and lack

of sleep while trudging through the dark, cold and (sometimes) wet, only to find no

possums in the traps. I could not have done my field work without you

I thank the many Parks Victoria and the Department of Environment, Land, Water,

and Planning (DELWP) staff who provided permission, access, and support for my

project. Finally, I thank the generous funding groups who made this work possible:

The Centre for Integrative Ecology at Deakin University, and the Holsworth Wildlife

Research Endowment.

Page 6: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

iii

Preface – List of Publications

This thesis comprises four papers that present my PhD research (Chapters 2 - 5). The

General Introduction (Chapter 1) provides a brief review of the relevant literature and

outlines the key themes underlying the research, while the Synthesis (Chapter 6)

describes links between the papers and places the research in a broader context.

The data chapters have been prepared as stand-alone papers for publication in

collaboration with co-authors. Therefore, there is some overlap in chapter content,

particularly with regards to descriptions of the study area, and the pronoun ‘we’ is used

instead of ‘I’ in recognition of the co-authors’ contributions.

The papers, my contributions, and the contributions of the co-authors are as follows:

Thesis Section Manuscript

Chapter 2 Allan, B. M., Arnould, J. P. Y., Martin, J. K., and Ritchie, E. G. 2013. A cost-effective and informative method of GPS tracking wildlife. Wildlife Research 40:345-348.

Contributions: BMA and EGR conceived the study. JPYA assisted with the technology. BMA conducted the fieldwork and analysed the data. BMA and EGR wrote the manuscript with editorial guidance from JPYA and JKM

My contribution: 70% Chapter 3 Allan, B. M., Nimmo, D. G., Arnould, J. P. Y., Martin, J. K., and

Ritchie, E. G. (in prep.) The secret life of possums: data-loggers reveal the movement ecology of an arboreal mammal in a fragmented landscape

Contributions: BMA, DGN, and EGR conceived the study. BMA conducted the fieldwork. DGN and BMA analysed the data. BMA, EGR and DGN wrote the manuscript with editorial guidance from JPYA and JKM

My contribution: 65%

Page 7: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

iv

Chapter 4 Allan, B. M., Ritchie, E. G., Nimmo, D. G., Speakman, J. R., and Arnould, J. P. Y. (in prep.) Measuring energy expenditure using animal-borne movement data loggers in a free-ranging scansorial mammal

Contributions: BMA and JPYA conceived the study. BMA conducted the fieldwork with guidence from JPYA (and assistance from Dr Rod Collins). JRS analysed the blood samples for DLW. DGN, BMA, and JPYA analysed the data. BMA, JYPA, and EGR wrote the manuscript with editorial guidance from DGN.

My contribution: 60% Chapter 5 Allan, B. M., Nimmo, D. G., Martin, J. K., Arnould, J. P. Y., and

Ritchie, E. G. (in prep.) Contrasting congener behaviour in relation to environmental variation

Contributions: BMA, DGN, and EGR conceived the study. BMA conducted the fieldwork. DGN and BMA analysed the data. BMA, EGR and JKM wrote the manuscript with editorial guidance from JPYA and DGN

My contribution: 70%

This work was conducted under Ethics Approval from Deakin University A45-2011,

and B33-2012.

I also contributed to the following publications during my PhD:

Champion, B. T., Joordens, M. A., and Allan, B. M. 2015. Tracking animals to

determine swarm behavior. Pages 105-110 in System of Systems Engineering

Conference (SoSE), 2015 10th.

Murfitt, S. L., Ierodiaconou, D., Bellgrove, A., Allan, B. M., Rattray, A., and

Young, M. A. (submitted to Methods in Ecology and Evolution) Applications

of unmanned aerial vehicles in intertidal reef monitoring

Page 8: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

v

Finally, I prepared and published a manuscript relating to separate research during my

PhD, and have another in preparation:

Allan, B. M., Ierodiaconou, D., Nimmo, D. G., Herbert, M., and Ritchie, E. G.

2015. Free as a drone: ecologists can add UAVs to their toolbox. Frontiers in

Ecology and the Environment 13:354-355. – See Appendix

Allan, B. M., Nimmo, D. G., Ierodiaconou, D., VanDerWal, J., Schimel, A. C.

G., Pin Koh, L., and Ritchie, E. G. (in prep.) Future casting ecological research:

the rise of technoecology

Page 9: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

vi

Table of Contents

Acknowledgements ...................................................................................................... i 

Preface – List of Publications ................................................................................... iii 

Table of Contents ....................................................................................................... vi 

List of Tables .............................................................................................................. ix 

List of Figures .......................................................................................................... xiii 

List of Appendices ................................................................................................... xvi 

Chapter 1: General introduction .............................................................................. 1 

1.1 Species habitat use .............................................................................................. 2 

1.2 Technology for measuring animal behaviour ..................................................... 3 

1.3 Combining technology to advance ecological understanding ............................ 6 

1.4 Study area and species ........................................................................................ 6 

1.5 Research aims and thesis structure ..................................................................... 8 

Chapter 2: A cost-effective and informative method of GPS tracking wildlife .. 13 

2.1 Abstract ............................................................................................................. 15 

2.2 Introduction ....................................................................................................... 15 

2.3 GPS Modification ............................................................................................. 16 

2.4 Field Trials ........................................................................................................ 18 

2.5 GPS Performance .............................................................................................. 20 

Chapter 3: The secret life of possums: data-loggers reveal the movement ecology

of an arboreal mammal in a fragmented landscape .............................................. 23 

3.1 Abstract ............................................................................................................. 23 

3.2 Introduction ....................................................................................................... 24 

3.3 Methods ............................................................................................................ 28 

Page 10: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

vii

3.3.1 Study area and experimental design ......................................................... 28 

3.3.2 Data collection .......................................................................................... 30 

3.3.3 Data processing ......................................................................................... 30 

3.3.4 Statistical analysis ..................................................................................... 35 

3.4 Results ............................................................................................................... 38 

3.5 Discussion ......................................................................................................... 45 

Chapter 4: Measuring energy expenditure using animal-borne movement data

loggers in a free-ranging scansorial mammal ........................................................ 53 

4.1 Abstract ............................................................................................................. 53 

4.2 Introduction ....................................................................................................... 54 

4.3 Methods ............................................................................................................ 57 

4.3.1 Study area, animal handling, and instrumentation ................................... 57 

4.3.2 Sample analysis and data processing ........................................................ 61 

4.3.3 Statistical analyses .................................................................................... 63 

4.4 Results ............................................................................................................... 63 

4.5 Discussion ......................................................................................................... 71 

4.5.1 DLW and energy expenditure .................................................................... 72 

4.5.2 Distance and energy expenditure .............................................................. 73 

4.5.3 Accelerometry as a surrogate for recording movement ............................ 74 

4.5.4 Other influences on energy expenditure .................................................... 76 

Chapter 5: Contrasting congener behaviour in relation to environmental

variation .................................................................................................................... 79 

5.1 Abstract ............................................................................................................. 79 

5.2 Introduction ....................................................................................................... 80 

5.3 Methods ............................................................................................................ 85 

5.3.1 Study area and design ............................................................................... 85 

5.3.2 Data collection .......................................................................................... 87 

5.3.3 Data processing ......................................................................................... 88 

5.3.4 Statistical analysis ..................................................................................... 91 

5.4 Results ............................................................................................................... 92 

5.4.1 Nightly Distance Travelled ........................................................................ 93 

5.4.2 24-hour Activity ......................................................................................... 95 

Page 11: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

viii

5.4.3 Behavioural State Analysis ........................................................................ 97 

5.4.4 Interval Comparison ................................................................................. 99 

5.4.5 Energy Expenditure ................................................................................. 101 

5.5 Discussion ....................................................................................................... 103 

5.5.1 Comparison of bobuck behaviour between habitat types ........................ 103 

5.5.2 Comparison of common brushtail possums across sites ......................... 106 

5.5.3 Comparison of congener behaviour ........................................................ 107 

5.6 Conclusion ...................................................................................................... 108 

Chapter 6: Synthesis .............................................................................................. 110 

6.1 Key insights .................................................................................................... 111 

6.1.1 Technological development ..................................................................... 111 

6.1.2 Effects of habitat modification on animal movement .............................. 112 

6.1.3 The influence of geographic location on morphology and behaviour .... 114 

6.2 Knowledge gaps and further research ............................................................. 115 

6.3 Implications for management ......................................................................... 116 

References ............................................................................................................... 118 

Appendix ................................................................................................................. 141 

Page 12: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

ix

List of Tables

Table 2.1: Examples of commercially available GPS devices. .................................. 18 

Table 3.1: Outline of the measures of animal (Trichosurus cunninghami) movement

.................................................................................................................................... 35 

Table 4.1: Summary statistics for the 17 (7 M, 10 F) bobucks (Trichosurus

cunninghami) examined in this study in south-eastern Australia. .............................. 64 

Table 4.5: Model selection results for the generalised mixed models comparing energy

expenditure to behavioural states for bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) in south-

eastern Australia. ........................................................................................................ 69 

Table 4.6: Parameter estimates from the best Time-Activity model comparing the

overall energy expenditure (kJ) of bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) to the time

spent (seconds) in each of four behavioural states (Denning, Resting at Night, Moving

and Foraging, and High Activity) and mass. .............................................................. 71 

Table 5.1: Outline of the measures of animal (bobuck, Trichosurus cunninghami, and

common brushtail possum, T. vulpecula) movement. ................................................ 90 

Appendix Table 3.2: Comparison of the average nightly distance travelled by bobucks

(Trichosurus cunninghami) in south-eastern Australia categorized by sex and habitat

type. .......................................................................................................................... 141 

Page 13: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

x

Appendix Table 3.3: Comparison of the average VeDBA (g) measured over 24 hours

travelled by bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) in south-eastern Australia

categorized by sex and habitat type. ......................................................................... 142 

Appendix Table 3.4a: Comparison of the average time spent denning within a 24-hour

period, measured in seconds, by bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) in south-eastern

Australia categorized by sex and habitat type. ......................................................... 143 

Appendix Table 3.4b: Comparison of the average time spent resting at night within a

24-hour period, measured in seconds, by bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) in south-

eastern Australia categorized by sex and habitat type. ............................................. 144 

Appendix Table 3.4c: Comparison of the average time spent moving within a 24-hour

period, measured in seconds, by bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) in south-eastern

Australia categorized by sex and habitat type. ......................................................... 145 

Appendix Table 3.4d: Comparison of the average time spent in high activity within a

24-hour period, measured in seconds, by bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) in south-

eastern Australia categorized by sex and habitat type. ............................................. 146 

Appendix Table 3.5: Interval comparison of the average distance travelled (m) versus

the average VeDBA (g), measured in 10 minute intervals for bobucks (Trichosurus

cunninghami) in south-eastern Australia categorized by sex and habitat type. ....... 147 

Appendix Table 4.2: Model selection results for the generalised mixed models

comparing energy expenditure to distance travelled for bobucks (Trichosurus

cunninghami) in south-eastern Australia. ................................................................. 148 

Page 14: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

xi

Appendix Table 4.3: Model selection results for the generalised mixed models

comparing energy expenditure to VeDBA for bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) in

south-eastern Australia. ............................................................................................ 149 

Appendix Table 4.4: Model selection results for the generalised mixed models

comparing daily energy expenditure to average VeDBA for bobucks (Trichosurus

cunninghami) in south-eastern Australia. ................................................................. 150 

Appendix Table 5.2: Comparison of the average nightly distance travelled by bobucks

(Trichosurus cunninghami) and common brushtail possums (T. vulpecula) in their

respective locations: the Strathbogie Ranges (foothill forest) and Grantville (heavily

cleared coastal and lowland forest). ......................................................................... 151 

Appendix Table 5.3: Comparison of the average VeDBA (g) measured over 24 hours

travelled by bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) and common brushtail possums (T.

vulpecula) in their respective locations; the Strathbogie Ranges (foothill forest) and

Grantville (heavily cleared coastal and lowland forest). .......................................... 152 

Appendix Table 5.4: Comparison of the average time spent in the four behavioural

states (denning, resting at night, moving, and high activity) within a 24-hour period,

measured in seconds, by bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) and common brushtail

possums (T. vulpecula) in their respective locations; the Strathbogie Ranges (foothill

forest) and Grantville (heavily cleared coastal and lowland forest). ........................ 153 

Appendix Table 5.5: Interval comparison of the average distance travelled (m) versus

the average VeDBA (g), measured in 10-minute intervals for bobucks (Trichosurus

cunninghami) and common brushtail possums (T. vulpecula) in their respective

Page 15: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

xii

locations: the Strathbogie Ranges (foothill forest) and Grantville (heavily cleared

coastal and lowland forest). ...................................................................................... 155 

Appendix Table 5.6: Comparison of the estimated daily energy expenditure (kJ/d)

calculated from the behavioural states using the equation in Chapter 4, for bobucks

(Trichosurus cunninghami) and common brushtail possums (T. vulpecula) in their

respective locations: the Strathbogie Ranges (foothill forest) and Grantville (heavily

cleared coastal and lowland forest). ......................................................................... 156 

Page 16: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

xiii

List of Figures

Figure 2.1: GPS locations of three male bobucks (M1 = yellow, M2 = blue, M3 = red)

in the vegetation (green) along a roadside (in the Strathbogie Ranges, north-eastern

Victoria, Australia. ..................................................................................................... 21 

Figure 2.2: GPS location of one male bobuck (M1) in vegetation (green) along a

roadside in the Strathbogie Ranges, north-eastern Victoria, Australia, for 11

consecutive nights. ..................................................................................................... 22 

Figure 3.1: Study sites in the Strathbogie Ranges, Victoria, Australia. ..................... 29 

Figure 3.2: The average distance travelled (m) over 24 hours by bobucks (Trichosurus

cunninghami) in south-eastern Australia, by sex and habitat type. ............................ 39 

Figure 3.3: The average VeDBA (g) measured over 24 hours for bobucks (Trichosurus

cunninghami) in south-eastern Australia, by sex and habitat type. ............................ 40 

Figure 3.4: A visual representation of the movement and activity of a bobuck

(Trichosurus cunninghami) in 10-minute intervals for a single night in south-eastern

Australia. .................................................................................................................... 41 

Figure 3.5: The average time spent by bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) in each

behavioural state over a 24-hour period (midnight to midnight GMT) in south-eastern

Australia, by sex and habitat type. ............................................................................. 42 

Figure 3.6: The relationship between activity and the distance travelled per ten-minute

interval for bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) in south-eastern Australia. ............ 44 

Page 17: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

xiv

Figure 4.1: Study sites in the Strathbogie Ranges, Victoria, Australia. ..................... 58 

Figure 4.2: The relationship between total energy expenditure (kJ) and total VeDBA

(g) for bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) in south-eastern Australia. .................... 65 

Figure 4.3: The relationship between daily energy expenditure (kJ) and average

VeDBA (g) for bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) in south-eastern Australia. ..... 66 

Figure 4.4: A depiction of the summed VeDBA (25 Hz) per second, the behavioural

states (1 = denning, 2 = resting at night, 3 = moving, and 4 = high activity), and the

distance travelled (m) for a single bobuck (Trichosurus cunninghami) over 24 hours.

.................................................................................................................................... 68 

Figure 4.5: The relationship between total energy expenditure (kJ) as measured by

DLW, and the best predictor of bobuck energy expenditure, including the four

behavioural states and mass, explaining 70.8 % (R2 = 0.708) of the variation in total

energy expenditure (EE) of the bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) in south-eastern

Australia. .................................................................................................................... 70 

Figure 5.1: The study areas: the Strathbogie Ranges (foothill forest > 500 m a.s.l.) in

north-eastern Victoria, and Grantville (heavily cleared coastal and lowland forest with

an average elevation < 100 m a.s.l.) in southern Victoria, Australia. ........................ 86 

Figure 5.2: The average distance travelled (m) over 24 hours by bobucks (Trichosurus

cunninghami) and common brushtail possums (T. vulpecula) in their respective

locations: the Strathbogie Ranges (foothill forest) and Grantville (heavily cleared

coastal and lowland forest). ........................................................................................ 94 

Page 18: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

xv

Figure 5.3: The average VeDBA (g) measured over 24 hours for bobucks (Trichosurus

cunninghami) and common brushtail possums (T. vulpecula) in their respective

locations: the Strathbogie Ranges (foothill forest) and Grantville (heavily cleared

coastal and lowland forest). ........................................................................................ 96 

Figure 5.4: The average time spent by bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) and

common brushtail possums (T. vulpecula) in each behavioural state over a 24-hour

period (midnight to midnight GMT) in their respective locations: the Strathbogie

Ranges (foothill forest) and Grantville (heavily cleared coastal and lowland forest).

.................................................................................................................................... 98 

Figure 5.5: The relationship between activity and the distance travelled per ten-minute

interval for bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) and common brushtail possums (T.

vulpecula) in in their respective locations: the Strathbogie Ranges (foothill forest) and

Grantville (heavily cleared coastal and lowland forest). .......................................... 100 

Figure 5.6: The average Daily Energy Expenditure (DEE) by bobucks (Trichosurus

cunninghami) and common brushtail possums (T. vulpecula) in their respective

locations: the Strathbogie Ranges (foothill forest) and Grantville (heavily cleared

coastal and lowland forest). ...................................................................................... 102 

Page 19: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

xvi

List of Appendices

Chapter 3 Appendix .................................................................................................. 141 

Chapter 4 Appendix .................................................................................................. 148 

Chapter 5 Appendix .................................................................................................. 151 

Published Article - Free as a drone: ecologists can add UAVs to their toolbox ...... 157 

Page 20: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

1

Chapter 1: General introduction

The environment is being modified at an unprecedented post-historical rate and

geographic extent due to human activities such as agriculture and urbanisation (Fischer

& Lindenmayer 2007). Through changing resource distribution and availability, and

other altered environmental conditions, ecosystem-, community-, and population-

composition is being affected (Brawn et al. 2001). Species’ ranges are shifting,

contracting, expanding, or becoming fragmented (Fletcher et al. 2012), and in turn

many species are declining, and some have become extinct (Sih et al. 2011, Lowry et

al. 2013, Haddad et al. 2015). From a conservation and management perspective

therefore, it is important to understand how species respond and might survive in the

face of such environmental upheaval. Such information can guide proactive

biodiversity conservation measures, such as establishing protected areas and

ecosystem and habitat restoration.

Resources for conservation are severely limited, it is essential therefore that investment

is guided by appropriate ecological information about species of interest (Bottrill et al.

2008, McDonald-Madden et al. 2008, Ochoa-Ochoa et al. 2011). To do so we must

find reliable, and ideally quantitative methods of understanding how species respond

to anthropogenic environmental change. Such methods will lead to a more

comprehensive understanding of how a species distribution and abundance are

affected, including predicting which taxa might be most vulnerable to habitat loss and

modification, as well as the possible over-arching consequences for community

composition and ecosystem function (Kremen et al. 1993, Rosenblatt et al. 1999,

Hughes et al. 2000).

Page 21: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

2

The following sections provide a global overview of the key elements in assessing

species habitat use, before focussing on new technological methods to record

information regarding animal movement, and finally the application and advantages

of combining different technologies to answer related ecological questions.

1.1 Species habitat use

There are three main questions ecologists want to answer regarding how species use

their environment: 1) Where do species occur (i.e. their physical location)? 2) How do

species use resources where they occur? and 3) How do different environmental

features affect species (e.g. behavioural and physiological consequences)? Detailed

information relating to these three questions offers the potential to understand spatial

population processes as the ultimate consequence of individual behaviour,

physiological constraints, and fine-scale environmental influences (Patterson et al.

2008), yet we have insufficient practical information on how to measure all these

aspects related to habitat use (Poisot et al. 2012).

Behaviour, including movement, is a key factor governing the ability of species to

respond to environmental variation and change (Frair et al. 2005, Fahrig 2007, van

Moorter et al. 2013). Behavioural responses to anthropogenic environmental change

fall into several main categories; avoiding or coping with novel enemies (e.g.

predators, parasites, diseases), adopting and using novel resources or habitats (e.g.

artificial structures and cities), avoiding or coping with novel abiotic stressors (e.g.

pollutants, noise), and adjusting to changing spatiotemporal conditions (e.g. habitat

fragmentation, climate change) (Sih et al. 2011). These behavioural responses can lead

to changes in species interactions, population dynamics and evolutionary processes,

Page 22: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

3

which ultimately shape population persistence and speciation (Tuomainen & Candolin

2011). Therefore, the accurate measurement of animal behaviour in different

environmental contexts could greatly further our ecological knowledge.

The importance of the links between environmental change, behaviour, population

dynamics, and evolutionary processes have repeatedly been stressed, however research

in this area has faced obstacles common to movement ecology in general (Harris et al.

1990, Mårell et al. 2002, Ganskopp & Johnson 2007, Dujon et al. 2014). Movement

of wildlife can be recorded via direct observation by humans, however difficulty arises

in measuring the movement and behaviour of free-living animals in situ, as observer

presence potentially interferes with natural behaviours, ethical concerns, and other

issues (e.g. observer safety in the presence of dangerous animals). For example, the

presence of observers can cause animals to increase their vigilance or frequency of

alarm calls, potentially confounding study results and ecological inference (Patterson

et al. 2008).

1.2 Technology for measuring animal behaviour

Ecologists often rely on technology to quantify ecological phenomena, and

technological advancements have often been the catalyst for enhanced understanding

of ecosystems and their parts (Keller et al. 2008). Recent technological developments

provide new ways of collecting behavioural data, including movement. Technologies,

both current and emerging, have the capacity to generate ‘next generation’ ecological

data that, if harnessed effectively, will transform our understanding of the ecological

world. In order to understand where animals persist, spatial movement recording

technology such as VHF tracking, and more recently GPS, have been used to provide

Page 23: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

4

insights into animal space-use in modified landscapes (Fryxell et al. 2008, Gautestad

et al. 2013, Vasudev & Fletcher 2015). GPS technology uses the triangulation of

satellites to determine high quality location data (i.e., errors ≤10 m), 24-hours per day

and under all weather conditions (Rodgers et al. 1996). GPS data can be used to

measure changes in spatial location, and the movement between those points can be

extrapolated to create time-activity models and activity estimates (Lachica & Aguilera

2005). This method has shown potential in long-term studies on animals with large

home ranges and clearly defined active and resting periods (Hulbert et al. 1998,

Johnson et al. 2002, Lachica & Aguilera 2005, Ungar et al. 2005).

Examining how animals use the environment and obtain resources has been

undertaken using technology and techniques such as spool and line tracking (Boonstra

& Craine 1986), camera traps (Brook et al. 2012), and GPS tracking (Allan et al.

2013), but these approaches have been limited in their capacity to capture certain

information. Spool and line techniques provide a very limited timeframe, camera traps

can only capture what happens in restricted locations, and GPS technology only

records individual spatial locations. However, recently, fine scale spatial and temporal

animal movement has been quantified using accelerometers (Fryxell et al. 2004,

Halsey et al. 2011, Rothwell et al. 2011, Elliott et al. 2013). Accelerometers are small,

reusable, externally mounted electronic devices which record movement as a change

in acceleration at a very fine scale (0.001 g) and rate (>100 Hz) across all three

dimensions of movement (x, y, z), providing finer temporal resolution than many other

methods (Halsey & White 2010, Bidder et al. 2012). By examining the fine-scale

movement data recorded by the accelerometer, researchers can infer and quantify

specific animal behaviours such as grooming, fighting, and mating in cryptic species

Page 24: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

5

such as pumas (Puma concolor) (Williams et al. 2014), and cheetahs (Acinonyx

jubatus) (Grunewalder et al. 2012). This allows researchers to gather substantial data

on animal behaviour without the need to directly observe the animal, providing

unprecedented insight into the behaviour of free-living animals (Naito et al. 2010,

Soltis et al. 2012, Awkerman et al. 2014).

Animals are known to use different behavioural strategies to survive in changing

environments, yet there has been little research on how this links to, and affects, overall

energy expenditure. The doubly-labelled water (DLW) technique is one of the most

widely used methods of calculating energy expenditure, however, it is expensive,

invasive, single-use, and provides only a single estimate of energy expenditure over

the study period (Bevan et al. 1994, Butler et al. 2004, Halsey et al. 2008, Qasem et

al. 2012). Due to these limitations, some have used fine scale movement as a proxy

for energy expenditure (Green et al. 2009, Gleiss et al. 2011, Fossette et al. 2012).

Movement converts metabolic energy into mechanical work and, therefore, accurate

quantification of movement has the potential to correlate with the energy expended to

produce it (Cavagna & Kaneko 1977, Fancy & White 1985, Speakman 2000, Yoda et

al. 2001, Gleiss et al. 2011). However, there have been few studies examining the

accuracy with which movement can predict energy expenditure under field conditions,

incorporating a range of ‘active’ and 'resting' natural behaviours or phases (night time

vs. daytime) (Halsey & White 2010, Elliott et al. 2013, Miwa et al. 2015). This is

necessary as laboratory studies suggest that, as activity decreases, the predictive ability

of movement models also decreases (Wilson et al. 2006, Green et al. 2009, Halsey et

al. 2009a, Gleiss et al. 2011). Consequently, there is a need for additional studies,

Page 25: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

6

incorporating a range of animal behaviour, in order to fully assess the ability of

accelerometry to accurately predict energy expenditure in free-living animals.

1.3 Combining technology to advance ecological understanding

The full potential of technology in ecology can be realised by using different

technologies in combination. For instance, GPS technology only measures one broad

aspect of animal movement: the change in the location of the individual over time

(Nathan et al. 2008). Conversely, accelerometers record fine-scale activity irrespective

of any change in spatial location, but without spatial reference. By combining these

technologies we not only record elements of a species’ space-use, but also movements

that don’t result in spatial displacement, or occur in a fraction of a second, such as a

toad flicking its tongue to capture prey (Dean 1980, Duckworth 1998). These types of

behaviours are too often overlooked due to the coarse temporal resolution of most

spatial recording technologies (Wilson et al. 2006). Furthermore, the incorporation of

both technologies may also provide information pertaining to how different behaviours

affect energy use at an individual scale.

1.4 Study area and species

In Australia, many landscapes have been cleared or heavily modified for agriculture,

forestry, urbanisation and grazing since European settlement (Radford et al. 2005,

Selwood et al. 2009). The resulting matrix of forest fragments and linear roadside

strips is surrounded by varying types of agriculture, timber production, and buildings.

The extent and pattern of landscape disturbance varies spatially. For instance, coastal

regions such as south-west Gippsland in Victoria are moderately populated and have

been extensively modified through the channelling of swamps and extensive clearing

Page 26: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

7

to create agricultural land. There are now very few remnant patches > 100 years old,

limiting the landscape’s use for wildlife, particularly hollow-dependent species

(Gibbons & Lindenmayer 2002, Harper et al. 2005). Conversely, Strathbogie Ranges

in north-eastern Victoria, less than 200 km away, is much less populated. While much

of the area has still been cleared, it has primarily been for silviculture, and there are

still remnant patches that have only been selectively logged, resulting in trees in excess

of 150 years old. This variation in anthropogenic disturbance makes Australia a model

system to analyse and assess the movement of animals in modified landscapes using

emerging technology.

Australia has in excess of 400 hollow dependent vertebrate species, most of which are

vulnerable to forest modification (Bennett et al. 1991, Lindenmayer et al. 1997, Banks

et al. 2011). Two such sympatric and congeneric species are the common brushtail

possum (Trichosurus vulpecula) and the mountain brushtail possum, or bobuck,

(Trichosurus cunninghami). Both species are medium-sized (2 – 4 kg), nocturnal,

semi-arboreal marsupials. Common brushtail possums are mainly folivores, but also

consume a wide variety of fruits, and are known to eat birds' eggs, insects, and small

mammals such as rats (Shipley et al. 2009). While populations have declined

extensively in the wild, common brushtail possums have adapted well to urban

landscapes, and are one of Australia’s most widely distributed marsupials. Bobucks,

however, are patchily distributed throughout wet sclerophyll forests and sub-tropical

forests of eastern Australia, and are not known to have adapted to urban landscapes.

They require silver wattle as a food source, and hollow-bearing trees for den sites (Ims

1987, Lindenmayer et al. 1990, Downes et al. 1997, Martin & Martin 2007), and until

Page 27: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

8

recently (Hynes & Cleeland 2005) were believed to be restricted to higher elevation

areas and cooler climates.

1.5 Research aims and thesis structure

The aim of my thesis was to investigate how emerging technology could be used to

characterise, quantify, and compare animal movement and other behaviour in relation

to variation in habitat attributes associated with fragmented and modified ecosystems.

Common brushtail and bobucks were the focal species for my research. Both species

were examined across two geographically and structurally distinct habitats in Victoria,

Australia; the mountainous Strathbogie Ranges, and coastal south-west Gippsland.

Individuals from habitats subject to different degrees of anthropogenic influence

(heavily modified linear-strip habitat, and remnant forest fragments) were tracked with

custom-made collars containing both GPS, and accelerometer data loggers. These data

was combined with morphological and demographic data to create behavioural

profiles of how these species respond to changing environments.

My chapters and their specific objectives were as follows:

Chapter 2: A cost-effective and informative method of GPS tracking wildlife

Aim: The development and modification of a relatively new and inexpensive off-the-

shelf GPS device to provide high spatial and temporal resolution information on the

movement patterns of individuals (bobucks). This included examining how movement

varied through time, and how individuals interacted with each other.

Specific aims:

1) To modify and adapt consumer GPS devices for use in wildlife research.

Page 28: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

9

2) To collect data equivalent to that captured by commercial wildlife GPS

tracking systems, in order to allow for the examination and analysis of

individual movement, habitat preference, and intraspecific interactions.

This chapter has been published in Wildlife Research.

Allan, B. M., Arnould, J. P. Y., Martin, J. K., and Ritchie, E. G. 2013. A cost-effective

and informative method of GPS tracking wildlife. Wildlife Research 40:345-348.

http://dx.doi.org/10.1071/WR13069

A demonstration video was produced and posted to YouTube:

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=UaSvS0grVjw

Chapter 3: The secret life of possums: data-loggers reveal the movement ecology of an arboreal mammal in a fragmented landscape

Aim: To compare and contrast the different movement patterns of male and female

bobucks using information derived from using GPS and accelerometer data loggers,

and the information that can be obtained by combining technologies. Both male and

female bobucks were tracked in a highly fragmented forest ecosystem consisting of

forest fragments and linear roadside habitat.

Key predictions:

1) Males were expected to travel further, have higher levels of overall activity,

and have a higher activity level per metre travelled than females due to actions

such as territory defence and competition (Schmidt et al. 2003, Zschille et al.

2012, Haan & Halbrook 2015).

Page 29: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

10

2) Based on home range studies (Martin & Handasyde 2007), and the composition

of key resources such as silver wattle and tree hollows (Martin 2006, Martin et

al. 2007) bobucks in forest fragments were expected to travel further, have

higher levels of overall activity, and have a higher activity level per metre

travelled than bobucks in linear-strip environments.

This chapter has been written for submission to Ecosphere, and is currently being

prepared for submission.

Chapter 4: Validation of accelerometry to measure energy expenditure in a free-living, scansorial mammal

Aim: To investigate the efficacy of accelerometry and GPS spatial movement to

measure energy expenditure in a scansorial mammal, the bobuck (Trichosurus

cunninghami), in south-eastern Australia. Movement was derived from accelerometers

to calculate vectorial dynamic body acceleration (VeDBA) and behavioural states, and

GPS to calculate distance travelled. These were compared to estimates of field

metabolic rate derived from Doubly Labelled Water (DLW).

Key predictions:

1) GPS data will be a poor predictor of energy expenditure as the recording

interval is too coarse.

2) VeDBA will only be a moderate predictor of energy expenditure, for while it

is recorded at a high temporal rate, a direct comparison between VeDBA and

energy expenditure assumes that they scale evenly.

Page 30: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

11

3) Time-activity analysis based on behavioural states will be the best predictor of

energy expenditure, as it allows for the calculation of different rates of energy

expenditure for different activities.

4) The inclusion of morphological and demographic traits will improve the fit of

the models.

This chapter has been written for submission to Scientific Reports, and is currently

being prepared for submission.

Chapter 5: Contrasting congener behaviour in relation to environmental variation

Aim: To compare and contrast the habitat use, movement, and energy expenditure of

two sympatric and congeneric species across two geographically and structually

distinct habitats. Male bobucks and common brushtail possums were tracked in linear

strip habitat in two distinct regions; the mountainous Strathbogie Ranges, and coastal

south-west Gippsland.

Key predictions:

1) Bobucks in south-west Gippsland are believed to be at the limit (edge) of their

geographic range, and in habitat less suitable than the Strathbogie Ranges.

Therefore, Bobucks in south-west Gippsland were expected to find it more

difficult to obtain key resources such as nesting hollows and food such as silver

wattle (Lawton 1993). We believe this difficulty to obtain resources will be

exhibited in higher nightly distances travelled, higher activity levels, more time

in high-activity behavioural states, and energy expenditure.

Page 31: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

12

2) Bobucks are physically larger than common brushtail possums, and have

larger home ranges, and were therefore expected to have higher movement and

energy expenditure.

3) Common brushtail possums in the Strathbogies were expected to be inferior

competitors compared with bobucks. Thus, they are expected to be more

resource limited and have higher movement values. This may result in

potentially higher energy expenditure, than common brushtail possums in

south-west Gippsland.

This chapter has been written for publication, however the journal for submission is

still in discussion.

Chapter 6: Synthesis

To synthesise the main findings of my thesis, identify knowledge gaps for future

research, and discuss broader implications for ecological theory, conservation and

management.

Page 32: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

13

Chapter 2: A cost-effective and informative method of

GPS tracking wildlife

Page 33: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

14

Page 34: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

15

2.1 Abstract

In wildlife research, our ability to GPS track sufficient numbers of individuals is

always limited by cost, which restricts inference of species-habitat relationships. Here

we describe the modification and use of a relatively new and inexpensive off-the-shelf

GPS device, to provide detailed and accurate information on the movement patterns of

individuals (mountain brushtail possums, or bobucks; Trichosurus cunninghami),

including how movement varies through time, and how individuals interact with each

other. Our results demonstrate that this technology has enormous potential to

contribute to an improved understanding of the movement patterns and habitat

preferences of wildlife at a fraction of the cost of traditional GPS technology.

2.2 Introduction

There are several commercial suppliers specialising in various types of wildlife GPS

tracking devices. However, GPS tracking remains a relatively expensive method of

collecting location data compared with traditional methods such as spool-and-line

tracking or VHF tracking, with wildlife GPS devices costing thousands to tens of

thousands of dollars (Matthews et al. 2013). This high expense typically reduces the

number of units that can be purchased by researchers and, as a result, the number of

individuals able to be tracked simultaneously. This can create statistical problems (e.g.

low power) and limits the inferences which can be made about populations, such as

habitat use and interactions between individuals. Furthermore, the purchase of

commercially available wildlife GPS tracking devices does not always guarantee

successful deployment, as many fail due to construction error or damage sustained

Page 35: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

16

from the animal (Gau et al. 2004, Soutullo et al. 2007, Strauss et al. 2008, Zucco &

Mourao 2009, Uno et al. 2010, Cumming & Ndlovu 2011, Ruykys et al. 2011).

A cheaper alternative is to purchase a GPS data logger or data transmitter built for

purposes such as travel, sport, domestic pet tracking, or personal tracking (see Table

2.1). These pre-assembled GPS devices usually comprise a GPS antenna, processor,

storage device, power supply, and often, timer chips. Stripping these units of excess

packaging, and attaching them to an animal is the easiest means of creating a cheap

and effective tracking device. However, challenges arise in the repackaging of devices

in ways which reduce size, while creating sufficient protection against both the

environmental conditions and the species to which they are attached.

We describe a novel technique for customising and modifying cheap GPS devices to

increase aspects such as durability, orientation, and battery life. These cost-effective

GPS wildlife tracking devices can be constructed either as data loggers or as data

transmitters, and can be constructed for as little as $USD 50.

2.3 GPS Modification

We designed our GPS devices to study the ecology and movement patterns of an

arboreal mammal, the bobuck (Trichosurus cunninghami) (Martin & Martin 2007).

From our data, we aimed to examine individual possum movements within the habitat,

and examine interactions between possums in the same habitat. Collaring all of the

possums resident in a habitat patch simultaneously could potentially require a large

number of collars, so we required a cost-effective method.

Page 36: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

17

We modified and collar-mounted the Mobile Action i-gotU GT-120 (Table 2.1) as it

was the cheapest available including postage, and is small (44.5 x 28.5 x 13 mm) and

lightweight (20 g, 15 g without casing, 7 g chipboard only). A GPS antenna is most

accurate and most efficient when it is pointed towards the sky with nothing obscuring

its view (Belant 2009, Williams et al. 2012), so we moved the position of the battery

on the collar to counterweight the GPS antenna. We also increased the size of the

battery, as larger batteries will allow the device to record for longer periods of time

(data storage permitting), and larger batteries are also heavier, creating a better

counterbalance. We re-encased the device using epoxy resin, as this is relatively

lightweight, very strong and does not cause skin irritation to animals.

The total cost was $USD 300 per collar including a VHF transmitter ($USD 220). The

GPS in the collars is also rechargeable, making subsequent deployments effectively

free. The VHF is not re-chargeable, however the battery lasts for 15 months. Therefore,

the finished collar can be deployed and re-deployed for 15 months, at which time it

can be re-furbished for approximately $USD 230 by removing and re-mounting a new

VHF transmitter. A full demonstration video on how collars were constructed is

available at (https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=UaSvS0grVjw).

Page 37: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

18

Table 2.1: Examples of commercially available GPS devices.

Prices accurate as of February 2013

Device Name Type Supplier Intended Purpose Retail Price (USD)

GPS CatTrack 1 Data Logger Catnip Technologies Ltd.

Domestic Pet Logger $49

i-gotU GT-120 Data Logger Mobile Action Technology Inc.

Travel and Sports Logger

$49.95

DG-200 Data Logger USGlobalStat Inc. Personal GPS Logger $49.99

GPS CatTrack Live 3

Data Transmitter (GSM Network)

Catnip Technologies Ltd.

Domestic Pet Tracker $110

Spark Nano 3.0 Data Transmitter (GSM Network)

BrickHouse Security

Personal GPS Tracker 169.95

i-gotU GT-1800 Data Transmitter (GSM Network)

Mobile Action Technology Inc.

Personal GPS Tracker $199.95

Dog Tracking Collar

Data Transmitter (GSM Network)

My Pet Tracker Domestic Pet Tracker $249.00

Trackstick Mini Data Logger Telespial Systems Personal GPS Tracker $289.95

2.4 Field Trials

All trappable male bobucks were collared simultaneously along a roadside. We

decided to focus solely on males for the field trial because their home ranges are over

twice that of females (Martin et al. 2007). Using only males also limited the number

of possums required for testing the collars. The collars were set to record from 19:00

- 06:00 h (local time) every day, to coincide with sunset and sunrise, and were set to

record in 5 min intervals until the battery died. For accuracy purposes, only data points

with an Expected Horizontal Position Error (EHPE) of <10 m were deemed accurate

enough for our fine-scale analysis (Frair et al. 2010).

Page 38: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

19

Three individual male bobucks were captured (Identification codes M1, M2, and M3).

Individual M1's collar recorded for 110 hours (10 nights) with 1375 attempted fixes.

Of these, 1003 fixes were successful, 76% of which had an EHPE <10 m. Individual

M2's collar recorded for 44 hours (4 nights) with 465 attempted fixes. Of these, 378

fixes were successful, 70% of which had an EHPE <10m. Individual M3's collar

recorded for 94 hours (9 nights) with 912 attempted fixes. Of these, 610 fixes were

successful, 65% of which had an EHPE <10m. The accuracy and frequency of the GPS

fixes is adequate to clearly divide the roadside into three home ranges with very little

overlap (Figure 2.1). From this, we can surmise that male bobucks occupy exclusive

home ranges along roadsides.

The battery life varied greatly among the 3 collars due to inferior batteries not meeting

their listed milliamp hour (mAh) capacity specifications. We have subsequently

rectified this variation using a different brand of batteries.

Due to the variation in recording time, we chose the individual with the most fixes

(M1) to analyse activity per night (Figure 2.2). The GPS recorded an average of 100.3

successful fixes per night (SD = 23.63), of which an average of 76.4 (SD = 24.83) had

an EHPE <10 m. Fixes with an EHPE >10 m tended to be in groups of 3 to 4

consecutive fixes at a time. The average time between fixes was 6 min 35 seconds,

however, each night tended to have blocks of unsuccessful fixes at the beginning and

the end of the recording time. Possums do not forage for the entire night (J. Martin

pers. obs.), and subsequent spotlighting has led us to believe that the blocks of

unsuccessful fixes represent the time the possum is in the tree hollow. The average

length of time that fixes were recorded was 8.5 hours per night (SD = 1.63 hours). If

we remove the blocks of unsuccessful fixes from the beginning and end of each night,

Page 39: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

20

only 18 fixes over the entire 10 nights were unsuccessful. The average time between

fixes also drops from 6 min 35 seconds to 4 min 49 seconds, slightly faster than the 5-

minute interval the collars were set for.

2.5 GPS Performance

The GPS tracking collars used for this study cost $USD 300 including the VHF

transmitter. While this is only a fraction of the cost of a commercial wildlife GPS

tracking collar, the GPS devices used were still able to record the majority of their

fixes with an Expected Horizontal Position Error (EHPE) of <10 m, which we deemed

accurate enough for fine-scaled analysis of the animal's movement (Frair et al. 2010).

Furthermore, the collars were all retrieved with no damage sustained, and could be re-

deployed after recharging the batteries.

Building your own GPS devices has a considerably higher investment of time to obtain

successfully recording re-useable devices, and the modification of the components

used obviously voids all warranties. However, there is also greater flexibility to

customise the design to specifically suit a particular species. The scope of cost-

effective GPS tracking of wildlife is vast. Furthermore, once built, these devices can

be re-charged and re-deployed as often as required, until the battery in the VHF

transmitter runs out. The construction of a re-chargeable VHF transmitter could greatly

increase the life-span of these collars.

The increased commercial availability of relatively inexpensive GPS devices for

purposes such as sport or domestic pet tracking, has made it possible to obtain high-

quality GPS data cheaply. Such advancements allow for many more individuals to be

Page 40: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

21

tracked at once, providing a better picture of population dynamics and individual

interactions.

Figure 2.1: GPS locations of three male bobucks (M1 = yellow, M2 = blue, M3 =

red) in the vegetation (green) along a roadside (in the Strathbogie Ranges, north-

eastern Victoria, Australia.

Each point encompasses the estimated location of the animal for each fix. The larger

the point, the less accurate the location fix.

Page 41: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

22

Figure 2.2: GPS location of one male bobuck (M1) in vegetation (green) along a

roadside in the Strathbogie Ranges, north-eastern Victoria, Australia, for 11

consecutive nights.

Different colours represent different nights. Each point encompasses the estimated

location of the animal for each fix. The larger the point, the less accurate the location

fix.

Page 42: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

23

Chapter 3: The secret life of possums: data-loggers

reveal the movement ecology of an arboreal mammal

in a fragmented landscape

3.1 Abstract

Understanding animal movement patterns is fundamental to ecology, as it allows

inference about species’ habitat preferences and their niches. Such knowledge

therefore also underpins our ability to predict how animals may respond to

environmental change, including habitat loss and modification. Data logging devices

such as GPSs and accelerometers are rapidly becoming cheaper and smaller, allowing

them to be applied more often and on a broader range of animal species. By combining

animal movement technologies and the different information they can record, it may

be possible to expand our understanding of animal behavior in response to habitat

variation and modification. We examined the different insights derived from using

GPS and accelerometer data loggers in isolation, and together, to examine arboreal

mammal (bobuck, Trichosurus cunninghami) movement patterns, in a highly

fragmented forest ecosystem (consisting of forest fragments and linear roadside

habitat). GPS data were used to examine the distance travelled by bobucks, while

accelerometry was used to create behavioral states and measure activity. Used in

isolation, the accelerometer and GPS data found different movement patterns in

relation to sex and habitat type: the GPS data showed males in linear strips travelled a

greater distance than females in linear strips, but no difference in the distance travelled

by males and females in forest fragments. The Accelerometer data showed higher

Page 43: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

24

activity in males and females in linear strip environments compared to forest

fragments. By coupling GPS and accelerometer data we uncovered three important

ecological insights: 1) bobucks in linear strips had higher activity when moving the

same distance compared with bobucks in forest fragments; 2) male bobucks had higher

activity levels than female bobucks for a given distance travelled; and 3) that

differences in movement patterns between sexes and habitat types were most apparent

when the distance travelled was greater. Our findings suggest habitat fragmentation

changes the amount and type of activity bobucks perform while moving. Combining

information from different movement logging devices has great potential to improve

our understanding of habitat use by individuals and species in ‘energetically-

challenging’ scenarios such as linear forest strips, by managing the spatial patterning

of key resources.

3.2 Introduction

Anthropogenic landscape modification is the greatest threat to biodiversity globally

(Fahrig 2001, Laurance et al. 2008, Selwood et al. 2009). The modification and loss

of habitat affects the spatial configuration and the amount of resources available to

species (Fahrig 2001, Rothermel & Semlitsch 2002, Foley et al. 2011), which in turn

impacts their growth, reproduction and survival. Understanding how biodiversity

responds to and in some cases persists in modified landscapes is therefore critical for

species conservation (Rhodes et al. 2005, Tscharntke et al. 2005, Cattarino et al.

2015).

Examining patterns of animal movement allows examination of how individuals

respond to habitat variation and change. Movement of an organism can be defined in

Page 44: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

25

two ways: a change in the spatial location of the whole individual in time (Nathan et

al. 2008), or changes in behavioural states irrespective of physical location (Nams

2014). These two aspects of animal movement are interwoven, with behavioural states

often leading to changes in spatial location, and are key to assessing the ability of

species to persist in modified environments (Frair et al. 2005, Fahrig 2007, van

Moorter et al. 2013). However, research in this area has faced obstacles common to

movement ecology in general (Ganskopp & Johnson 2007, Dujon et al. 2014).

Spatial recording technology such as VHF tracking, and more recently GPS, has been

used to provide insights into animal space-use in modified landscapes (Fryxell et al.

2008, Gautestad et al. 2013, Vasudev & Fletcher 2015). However, these technologies

measure only one coarse aspect of animal movement: the change in the location of the

entire individual over time (Nathan et al. 2008). While these data are often of

ecological interest as they detail elements of an individual’s space-use (Frair et al.

2005, Patterson et al. 2008, Cagnacci et al. 2010), other important aspects of animal

movement are overlooked (Belant 2009, Bidder et al. 2012, Williams et al. 2012).

Some movements pivotal to the fitness of individuals and the growth of populations

do not result in spatial displacement: for instance, intense localized foraging or mating

(Duckworth 1998). Similarly, important movements may occur in a fraction of a

second, such as a toad flicking its tongue to capture prey (Dean 1980). These too are

often overlooked due to the coarse temporal resolution of most spatial recording

technologies (Wilson et al. 2006).

Recently, accelerometer loggers have been used to quantify animal movement at fine

spatial and temporal scales (Fryxell et al. 2004, Halsey et al. 2011, Rothwell et al.

2011, Elliott et al. 2013). Accelerometers are small, reusable, externally-mounted, and

Page 45: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

26

relatively inexpensive electronic devices which record movement as a change in

acceleration at a very fine scale (0.001 g) and rate (>100 Hz) across all three

dimensions of movement. Furthermore, the data recorded can provide finer temporal

resolution than many other methods (Halsey & White 2010, Bidder et al. 2012). By

examining the fine-scale movement data recorded by accelerometers, researchers can

discern movement irrespective of any change in spatial location, and can even infer

specific animal behaviours (Halsey & White 2010, Grunewalder et al. 2012, Krop-

Benesch et al. 2013). This allows researchers to gather substantial information about

animal behaviour without the need to directly observe the animal, providing

unprecedented insight into the ‘private lives’ of wild animals (Naito et al. 2010, Soltis

et al. 2012, Awkerman et al. 2014). Accelerometers have already been used to examine

movement and prey capture in cryptic species such as pumas (Puma concolor),

revealing new information in energy expenditure and individual variations (Williams

et al. 2014). However, accelerometers by themselves also have shortcomings, namely

that they do not provide spatial data. Thus, when combined, GPS loggers and

accelerometers have the potential to provide complementary insights into the

movement ecology of individuals, yet to date the collection of this type of data has

been rare (but see Nams (2014)).

Here, we use GPS and accelerometer data loggers to examine the movement ecology

of an arboreal mammal in forest fragments embedded within an agricultural landscape

in south-eastern Australia. The study species is the bobuck (Trichosurus

cunninghami), a medium-sized (2.6–4.2 kg), arboreal, nocturnal, hollow-nesting

marsupial (How 1981, Lindenmayer et al. 2002). Habitat availability for the bobuck

has reduced since European settlement, primarily by changing the abundance of the

Page 46: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

27

two key resources required: large hollow-bearing trees for den sites and the leaves of

the silver wattle (Acacia dealbata) for food (Ims 1987, Lindenmayer et al. 1990,

Downes et al. 1997, Martin & Martin 2007). Once a continuous forest, the study

landscape is now a matrix of agricultural land, and two types of remnant forest: forest

fragments and linear roadside strips. The bobuck persists within both types of

remnants, and the effect remnant type has on bobuck population structure, social and

mating system has been examined (Martin & Handasyde 2007, Martin et al. 2007), yet

little is known of how the modified landscape affects individual movement at finer

temporal and spatial resolutions.

Our aim was to use GPS and accelerometer data to examine how habitat modification

and fragmentation affect the movement behaviour of bobucks in the two types of forest

remnants. First, we used GPS data to quantify the nightly distances travelled by

bobucks, and accelerometer data to quantify nightly activity and identify key

behavioural states. Next, we examined whether the nightly distance travelled, nightly

activity or the time spent in particular behavioural states differed between individuals

from forest fragments and linear strips and between sexes. Finally, we highlight the

complementarity of GPS and accelerometer data by examining how the relationship

between distance travelled and activity differs between individuals in forest fragments

and linear strips and between sexes.

Page 47: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

28

3.3 Methods

3.3.1 Study area and experimental design

Our study area was in the Strathbogie Ranges, north-eastern Victoria. The study area

ranged from 550 to 750 m above sea level, and has an annual rainfall of ~ 900 mm.

The vegetation consists of open sclerophyll forest dominated by manna gum

(Eucalyptus viminalis), Victorian blue gum (Eucalyptus globulus bicostata), narrow-

leaf peppermint (Eucalyptus radiata) and broad-leafed peppermint (Eucalyptus dives).

Key middle and understory species include silver wattle (Acacia dealbata), dogwood

(Cassinia aculeata), cherry ballart (Exocarpos cupressiformis), blackwood (Acacia

melanoxylon), and austral bracken (Pteridium esculentum). Approximately 70% of the

native vegetation has been cleared for agriculture and softwood (Pinus radiata)

plantation (Downes et al. 1997).

We trapped bobucks in three contiguous forest fragments (> 160 ha and an average

vegetation width > 750 m, termed 'forest fragments' throughout) and three linear

roadside strips (> 1 km length and an average vegetation width < 40 m, termed 'linear

strips' throughout) (Figure 3.1). Sites (fragments and linear strips) were confined to

areas of habitat that consisted primarily of native vegetation and containing trees large

enough to provide suitable hollows (DBH > 50 cm) (Smith & Lindenmayer 1988,

Pausas et al. 1995). Each site was separated by at least 500 m in order to reduce the

chances of the same individual being caught at two locations, based on home range

data from Martin & Handasyde (2007).

Page 48: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

29

Figure 3.1: Study sites in the Strathbogie Ranges, Victoria, Australia.

Blue shaded ovals indicate forest fragments and yellow shaded ovals indicate linear

strips.

We conducted three nights of trapping in each of the six study sites. Each site was

comprised of two rows of eight large (30 x 30 x 90 cm) wire cage traps, placed on the

ground and baited with peanut butter and apple. We transferred trapped individuals

from the traps to hessian bags. Traps were approximately 50 m apart. On linear strips,

the two rows were placed within the roadside vegetation, and were separated by a

minimum of 200 m. In forest fragments, one row was placed along the edge of the

fragment, and the other placed within the fragment, starting a minimum of 100 m from

the edge, in order to sample both the core and edges of the fragment. We set the traps

Page 49: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

30

before dusk and checked them before sunrise. Traps were partially covered with plastic

to protect animals from weather.

3.3.2 Data collection

Trapping sessions of three nights at each trap site were undertaken over eight

consecutive seasons from spring 2011 to winter 2013 inclusive. For processing, we

anaesthetized individuals with an intramuscular injection of tiletamine/zolazepam (6

mg kg –1; Zoletil ®; Virbac Australia, Peakhurst, NSW, Australia) to facilitate

handling and minimize distress for the animals. We injected individuals with Passive

Integrated Transponder (PIT) tags (Trovan, Ltd.) for subsequent identification. We

then fitted individuals with self-made data-logging collars (80 g) comprised of a VHF

transmitter, GPS data logger and 3-axis accelerometer modified from the design in

Allan et al. (2013). We set the collars to start recording data as soon as they were

attached to the animal with GPS data set to record at 10-minute intervals, and

accelerometers at 25 Hz. We only used adult individuals for this study (classed as

tooth-wear ≥ 3 (Winter 1980)). In order to allow individuals to recover completely

from handling and sedation and to avoid releasing bobucks (which are strictly

nocturnal) in daylight, we held the animals after processing until dusk on that day. We

released the bobucks within 10 m of the site where they were captured. We recaptured

individuals 3-7 days later and removed the data-logging collars.

3.3.3 Data processing

Home range analysis of bobucks has shown have shown they use resources, and the

environment, differently based on both sex and habitat type (McDonald-Madden et al.

2004, Martin 2006, Martin & Handasyde 2007, Martin et al. 2007), however, it is not

Page 50: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

31

known how this habitat use is reflected daily. Therefore, we split the data into groups

based on sex (male vs. female) and habitat type (forest fragment or linear strip) for

analysis, creating four groups (male forest fragment, female forest fragment, male

linear strip, and female linear strip) in order to reduce confounding variation in the

results.

GPS - GPS devices acquire positional fixes from orbiting satellites to triangulate

location. However, if the GPS device is obscured by factors such as dense canopy

cover or due to its orientation on the animal, it can take longer to acquire a fix, or fail

to do so (Williams et al. 2012). As such, although the GPS devices were set to record

at 10 minute intervals, the interval between fixes was almost never exactly 10 minutes:

some fixes failed to be recorded, and no fixes were recorded while bobucks were

denning inside tree hollows. Furthermore, the error of each location fix varies

depending on the accuracy with which the location can be triangulated. In order to

standardize the data, we used a continuous-time correlated random walk model to

convert the GPS location data into nightly interpolated movement tracks through the

environment using the CRAWL package in R statistical environment (Johnson 2012).

Correlated movements imply the animal's location at a given time is dependent on all

previous locations, not just the last one. Continuous-time formulation allows data that

have been non-uniformly collected over time to be modelled without sub-sampling,

interpolation, or aggregation to obtain a set of locations uniformly spaced in time

(Johnson et al. 2008).

Accelerometer - The total acceleration recorded by the accelerometers is made up of

two components: static acceleration and dynamic acceleration (Shepard et al. 2008).

Static acceleration refers to the gravitational field of the earth. An accelerometer not

Page 51: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

32

moving is expected to record a cumulative total of 1 g across its three axes due to the

gravitational force of the earth. Dynamic acceleration represents the change in velocity

as a result of body motion, and has shown great potential as a predictor of energy

expenditure (Wilson et al. 2006, Gleiss et al. 2011). To obtain a measure of dynamic

acceleration, we first smoothed each accelerometer channel to derive the static

acceleration using a running mean over three seconds, and then subtracted this static

acceleration from the raw data, leaving only dynamic acceleration (Gleiss et al. 2011).

We then converted the values for dynamic acceleration to positive values, and

calculated the vector of the dynamic body acceleration to provide a single value for

partial vectorial dynamic body acceleration (VeDBA) at 25 Hz (Qasem et al. 2012,

Elliott et al. 2013). These values were then summed in 1-second intervals to create

partial VeDBA. The accelerometers we used were capable of measuring ± 8 g,

therefore any value outside of these range was deemed an outlier and removed

(<0.001%).

We undertook behavioural state analysis on the partial VeDBA values, measured at 25

Hz, in Igor Pro version 6.36 (Wavemetrics, Portland, OR, USA) using the program

Ethographer (Version 2.02). Ethographer is an automated program for visualizing, and

analysing catalogues of discrete behaviours typically employed by a species, known

as ethograms, from acceleration data (Sakamoto et al. 2009). We converted the time

series accelerometer data into a spectrum via continuous wavelet transformation, then

categorized each second of the spectrum into one of 10 specified behaviour groups by

unsupervised cluster analysis, using k-means methods. Due to the possibility for the

axes to shift in relation to the orientation of the body, fine-scale behavioural states

could not be calculated, instead we used the 10 identified groups to create four broad

Page 52: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

33

behavioural states measured at 1-second intervals, characterized by the periodicities of

body acceleration and the time of acceleration:

1. Denning – Measurements taken during the day (mean VeDBA = 0.028 ± 0.001

m/s2)

2. Resting at Night - very low VeDBA values, but while the bobucks were out of

the den (mean VeDBA = 0.108 ± 0.001 m/s2)

3. Moving - moderate VeDBA values and during the night, including normal

foraging behaviour (mean VeDBA = 0.180 ± 0.001 m/s2)

4. High Activity - Activity values much higher than all the others, accounting for

behavioural states such as mating, fighting, intense foraging, predator

avoidance, etc. (mean VeDBA = 0.260 ± 0.002 m/s2)

We examined the behavioural states over a 24-hour period (midnight to midnight

GMT), including when the bobucks were denning and removed any recordings which

did not span the full 24-hour time period. Given animals could potentially display

different movement patterns after release, we removed the nights of release and

capture from both movement measures.

Response variables – We tracked thirty-two individual bobucks for 2-7 nights at a

time, totalling 149 nights of data across the eight consecutive seasons (males in forest

fragments = 10 individuals and 36 nights; females in forest fragments = 16 individuals

and 48 nights; males in linear strips = 6 individuals and 21 nights; and females in linear

strips = 12 individuals and 44 nights). Animals classed as inhabiting linear strip

environments spent > 80 % of their nightly movement in linear strips, and 9 of the

individuals strictly inhabited linear strip environments. Animals classed as inhabiting

Page 53: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

34

forest fragments ranged more widely, but had > 60 % of GPS points recorded within

the forest fragments, and made use of the forest fragments on a nightly basis. The

average time between GPS fixes across the study was 14 minutes 59 seconds (range 2

minutes 04 seconds to 63 minutes 29 seconds). We considered data points with an

error > 40 m (6.65% of data) too inaccurate and removed them, resulting in an average

Estimated Horizontal Position Error (EHPE) of 10 m (range 2.72 to 39.84 m).

GPS data - We created two variables from the GPS data; the total distance travelled in

a 24-hour period (midnight to midnight GMT), termed ‘nightly distance travelled’

measured in metres, and distance travelled in 10-minute intervals, termed ‘interval

distance travelled’, also measured in metres. Using midnight in GMT meant that the

break between one day and the next fell during daylight hours in Australia, which is

when the bobucks are denning, meaning no GPS fixes, and a clear break as to when

one GPS track finished, and the next began. For nightly distance travelled, we

measured the distance of the interpolated movement tracks for each full night of

movement. For interval distance travelled, we determined locations at 10- minute

intervals along the interpolated movement tracks, and measured the distance between

these locations.

Accelerometer data - The accelerometer data created two continuous datasets: VeDBA

values, and the time spent in the four behavioural states. As stated previously, VeDBA

is the velocity of the body motion, or activity, of the animal, and this activity has shown

great potential as a predictor of energy expenditure (Wilson et al. 2006, Gleiss et al.

2011). We created three variables from the Accelerometer data: 1) ’24-hour activity’

which is the sum of the partial VeDBA values measured at 25 Hz over 24 hours,

commensurate to ‘nightly distance travelled’; 2) ‘interval activity’ which is the sum of

Page 54: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

35

the partial VeDBA values measured at 25 Hz in 10 minute intervals matching ‘interval

distance travelled’; and 3) ‘behavioural state’ which is the amount of time animals

spent in each of four behavioural states, measured in 1-second intervals, over the same

24-hour period as ‘nightly distance travelled’ ‘24 hour activity’. A list of the measures

of animal movement is provided in Table 3.1.

Table 3.1: Outline of the measures of animal (Trichosurus cunninghami)

movement

Device Measurement Unit of

Measurement Timeframe of measurement

Method of measurement

GPS Nightly Distance Travelled

Metres (m) 24 hours Summed distance based on interpolated movement track from GPS points over 24 hours

Interval Distance Travelled

Metres (m) 10 minute intervals

Nightly interpolated movement track broken into 10 minute intervals

Accelerometer 24 Hour Activity Gravitational Force (g)

24 hours Summed activity based on accelerometer values measured at 25 Hz over 24 hours

Interval Activity Gravitational Force (g)

10 minute intervals

Summed VeDBA values, measured at 25 Hz, into 10 minute intervals

Behavioural State Categorical 1 second intervals over 24 hours

VeDBA values, measured at 25 Hz, used in Ethographer to create four behavioural states measured in 1-second intervals

Predictor variables - All response variables were related to two predictor variables

and their interaction. First, to examine the impact of patch type on bobuck movement,

we included a two-level categorical variable indicating the patch type within which the

animal was captured: either forest fragment or linear strip. Second, as the sex of

animals can affect movement behaviours (Beck et al. 2003), a second two-level

categorical variable was included indicating the sex of the animal: male or female.

3.3.4 Statistical analysis

Page 55: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

36

We used Generalized Linear Mixed Models (GLMMs) to analyse the effects of the

predictor variables on measures of animal movement derived from GPS and

accelerometer data. Mixed models were required as they can consider both fixed and

random effects, the latter of which can account for correlated data (Zuur et al. 2009)

including repeated sampling of the same individuals over time (Zuur et al. 2009). Thus,

the identity of individual animals was specified as a random effect in all models to

account for repeated sampling of individuals over several days. We included the site

and season within which data was collected as additional random effects to account

for season-to-season variance.

We generated GLMMs of the relationship between the movement variables measured

over 24 hours (nightly distance travelled, nightly activity and the time spent in each of

the four behavioural states) and the two predictor variables. We fitted a global model

between each response variable and the two categorical variables (patch type and sex)

and their interaction. We ran four separate models to examine the time bobucks spent

in different behavioural states over a 24-hour period, corresponding to each of the four

behavioural states: denning, resting at night, moving, high activity. We ran the models

with each group (males in linear strips, males in forest fragments, females in linear

strips, and females in forest fragments) as the reference category in order to determine

the differences between all four groups. We applied a Bonferroni correction to the p

values to infer significant differences between groups after accounting for multiple

comparisons. To evaluate the ‘significance’ of differences we observed, we report

effect sizes, confidence intervals, and p values, with alpha set at p < 0.1. We chose this

value for alpha because we didn’t have many observations (nights) for some levels of

tests, and hence a more conservative p value (0.05) might have made it harder to detect

Page 56: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

37

‘significant differences’, as per results and guidance from (Nakagawa & Cuthill 2007)

and (Jennions & Møller 2003). While p < 0.1 creates a higher risk of false positives in

the analysis, we have included information regarding effect sizes and confidence

intervals to assist in indicating the biological importance of the results (Nakagawa &

Cuthill 2007). We assessed model fit using r-squared calculated using the

r.squaredGLMM function in the MuMIn package.

We examined activity in relation to distance travelled as a proxy for the energetic cost

of bobuck spatial movement in each of the patch types and for both sexes. We analysed

these data at a finer temporal scale as variation in activity often occurs over short time

periods (Wilson et al. 2006): we used a 10-minute time interval, the shortest period we

could with the current GPS technology. To understand the additional insights from

coupling these technologies, we modelled partial VeDBA (response variable) as a

function of distance travelled and a four-level categorical variable that concatenated

the patch type and sex predictor variables (i.e. male linear strip, male forest fragment,

female linear strip, and female forest fragment). We specified an interaction between

these two predictor variables to allow a separate curve for the relationship between

VeDBA and distance travelled for each patch type and sex combinations. Differences

in the slopes of VeDBA with distance travelled would be indicative of animals

investing different amounts of activity per unit of distance travelled, depending on

their patch type and sex. We used the lstrends function in the lsmeans package (Lenth

2016) to test for differences in the slopes between the patch type and sex combinations.

We again used a Bonferroni correction to account for multiple comparisons, but set

alpha to 0.05 given the larger number of samples at the finer temporal resolution. As

movements at the scale of 10-minute intervals are likely to be temporally auto-

Page 57: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

38

correlated, we fit an autoregressive models of order one (AR1) to account for residual

temporal correlation in the data. The AR1 model residuals at a given time t as a

function of the residuals at previous times t-1 allowing residuals to be correlated in

time, with residuals further away in time less correlated than those closer in time (Zuur

et al. 2009). GLMMs were generated using the nlme package in the R statistical

environment (R Core Development Team 2014).

3.4 Results

The nightly distance travelled by bobucks was significantly higher for males in linear

strips than all other groups, and was higher for males in forest fragments than females

in linear strips, but there was no significant difference between males and females in

forest fragments (Figure 3.2 and Table 3.2 in Chapter 3 appendix). The activity of

bobucks over 24 hours, as measured by VeDBA, was also significantly higher for

males in linear strips than all other groups (Figure 3.3 and Table 3.3 in Chapter 3

appendix). An example of a bobuck’s movement patterns relative to activity for a

single night has been depicted in Figure 3.4.

Page 58: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

39

Figure 3.2: The average distance travelled (m) over 24 hours by bobucks

(Trichosurus cunninghami) in south-eastern Australia, by sex and habitat type.

The coloured points indicate the raw data. The error bars indicate Standard Error.

Categories sharing a letter did not show significantly different relationships between

the coefficients.

Page 59: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

40

Figure 3.3: The average VeDBA (g) measured over 24 hours for bobucks

(Trichosurus cunninghami) in south-eastern Australia, by sex and habitat type.

The coloured points indicate the raw data. The error bars indicate Standard Error.

Categories sharing a letter did not show significantly different relationships between

the coefficients.

Page 60: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

41

Figure 3.4: A visual representation of the movement and activity of a bobuck

(Trichosurus cunninghami) in 10-minute intervals for a single night in south-

eastern Australia.

Each section of line represents a 10-minte interval. The length of the line represents

the distance travelled, while the colour ramp (red-to-blue corresponding to high-to-

low activity respectively) represents the level of activity. The white crosses indicate

the GPS points.

All groups spent a similar amount of time denning, resting at night, and moving (Figure

3.5 and Tables 3.4a, 3.4b, and 3.4c in Chapter 3 appendix). However, differences were

observed for time spent in the high activity behavioural states. Males in linear strips

spent significantly more time in high activity than all other groups, while males in

Page 61: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

42

fragments spent similar time in high activity as the females in linear strips, and

significantly more than females in fragments. Females in fragments and females in

linear strips spent similar time in high activity (Figure 3.5 and Table 3.4d in Chapter

3 appendix).

Figure 3.5: The average time spent by bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) in

each behavioural state over a 24-hour period (midnight to midnight GMT) in

south-eastern Australia, by sex and habitat type.

The coloured points indicate the raw data. The error bars indicate Standard Error.

Categories sharing a letter did not show significantly different relationships between

the coefficients.

Page 62: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

43

There was a significant interaction between the ‘group’ an individual belonged to (i.e.

male in linear strip, female in linear strip, male in forest fragment, female in forest

fragment) and distance moved over-ten minute intervals when explaining activity

levels (measured as 10 minutes summed VeDBA). Model predictions show (1) that

bobucks in linear strips have higher summed VeDBA values when moving the same

distance compared to bobucks in forest fragments; (2) that males had higher summed

VeDBA values than females for a given distance travelled; and (3) that differences

between the four groups (i.e. males in fragments, female in fragments, males in linear

strips, females in linear strips) were most apparent when the distance travelled was

greater (Figure 3.6 and Table 3.5 in Chapter 3 appendix).

Page 63: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

44

Figure 3.6: The relationship between activity and the distance travelled per ten-

minute interval for bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) in south-eastern

Australia.

Lines and shaded area represent the fitted relationship and 95% confidence intervals

from generalized linear mixed models, respectively. Solid line, yellow shading =

female bobuck in forest fragments; dotted line, blue shading = male bobucks forest

fragments; dashed broken line, green shading = female bobucks in linear strip; dotted

and dashed broken line, grey shading = male bobucks in linear strips. Categories

sharing a letter did not show significantly different relationships between the

coefficients.

Page 64: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

45

3.5 Discussion

Understanding variation in patterns of animal movement is critical to both theoretical

and applied ecology (Ponchon et al. 2013). Despite this, finding techniques that can

simply and reliably quantify an individual’s movement is challenging. Here, we show

the unique insights that can be gained by combining the use of accelerometers and

GPS for terrestrial species. While GPS data showed male bobucks in linear strips were

moving furthest as compared to other groupings over a 24-hour period, accelerometer

data showed that these animals also more frequently engaged in bouts of very high

activity. These bursts of high activity help explain why male bobucks in linear strips

display higher levels of activity overall (and probably exert more energy) per unit of

distance travelled.

We found male bobucks in linear strips travelled significantly further than all other

groups. Males travelling further than females of the same species is consistent with

studies of the movement of a host of mammal species, including rodents (Maroli et al.

2015), minks (Neovison vison) (Haan & Halbrook 2015), wolves (Canis lupus)

(Jedrzejewski et al. 2001), Iriomote cats (Prionailurus bengalensis iriomotensis)

(Schmidt et al. 2003), and arctic foxes (Alopex lagopus) (Anthony 1997), as well as

reptiles such as lizards (Rose 1981, Thompson et al. 1999) and turtles (Aresco 2005).

In these instances, males travelled greater distances due to larger body sizes and higher

energy demands, to maximize home range overlap with females for breeding, and for

mate guarding (Schmidt et al. 2003, Zschille et al. 2012, Haan & Halbrook 2015). On

the other hand, the daily movement of females is presumed to be only as far as

Page 65: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

46

necessary for efficient foraging (Schmidt et al. 2003), and restricted due to caring for

young (Jedrzejewski et al. 2001). Males in forest fragments did not travel greater

distances than females in forest fragments, and females in linear strips travelled the

least distance, so we do not believe the increased movement by male bobucks is best

explained by foraging requirements as related to food availability. Rather, one

possibility, as suggested in Schmidt et al. (2003), is that males in linear strips are

travelling greater distances to guard both mates and territory. Martin & Martin (2007)

reported much greater population densities of both male and female bobucks in linear

strips within the Strathbogies than fragments. The higher population density may result

in greater vigilance being required to guard mates.

Female bobucks in linear strips travelled similar distances to females in forest

fragments, and had similar activity patterns over 24 hours, yet they had a higher cost

of movement per unit distance travelled. This disparity may be due to how arboreal

species traverse their environment. An arboreal mammal can move considerable

distances climbing up and down trees vertically, without resulting in a change to its

two-dimensional position measured by the GPS. Accelerometers are able to capture

this three-dimensional activity because they are recording on all three axes of animal

movement, providing a much more comprehensive picture of the animal’s true

movement. Ryan et al. (2013) showed a similar pattern when studying koalas, finding

males moved farther and defended territory more, but had the same amount of activity

as females. In our case, we propose that females in linear strip environments may

undertake more intense foraging, ascending and descending trees, and predator

vigilance, all of which can occur within a few metres of horizontal spatial movement,

which is within the error of the GPS devices we used.

Page 66: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

47

Male bobucks in linear strips also had the highest activity over 24 hours, showing a

similar pattern to distance travelled. The behavioural state analysis again showed

bobucks in linear strips spend more time in high activity behavioural states,

commensurate to more activity than the other groups. Males in fragments also spent

more time in the high activity behavioural state compared with females in fragments.

This may be due to the additional activities males undertake while moving, such as

defending their territories from other males (Martin et al. 2007). These types of

activities are important sources of energy expenditure for a range of species (Shamoun-

Baranes et al. 2012, Christiansen et al. 2013), but are obscured when using GPS

technology alone. However, females in linear strips travelled the least distance, so

other factors must also be influencing the more active behaviour in linear strips.

Female bobucks in fragments were the least active group. We also found no difference

in any of the behavioural states for male bobucks in fragments and females in linear

strips.

The sex of an individual clearly affected their movement, such that males have higher

activity for a given distance travelled as compared with females. We also found that

the differences between sexes were amplified in particular habitats—the difference

between males and females was far greater in linear strips than forest fragments, as

males in linear strips moved greater distances than females in linear strips. We

expected females in linear strips to travel the least distance, as a previous study showed

linear strips have greater access to their key food source (silver wattle), and greater

access to their key shelter (tree hollows) (Martin & Martin 2007). Male movement is

more influenced by defending their territories, and the greater number of bobucks in

linear strips may result in more movement for defence (Martin et al. 2007).

Page 67: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

48

Linear strip environments have significantly altered the behaviour of bobucks. Male

bobucks are travelling further distances each night, with many more bursts of high

activity, while females have maintained similar distances travelled, however, their

activity levels in so doing have increased. Linear strip environments often differ

greatly in vegetation composition and density when compared with forest fragments,

and thus the distribution and availability of resources can vary widely (Sabino-

Marques & Mira 2011), yet in the environment in question, linear strip environments

have higher abundance of both silver wattle and tree hollows, the key resources for

bobucks, than the surrounding remnant vegetation (Martin & Martin 2007). Based on

resource variables alone, bobucks would be expected to preferentially select for linear

strips over other environments to reduce movement (van der Ree 2002, Pereira &

Rodriguez 2010). Martin et al. (2007) did hypothesize that males would exert more

energy to maintain a home range of equivalent size in the linear strips owing to the

much greater distances between the ranges' extremities. However, the same hypothesis

does not explain the activity of females in linear strips, who travelled similar distances

to females in forest fragments, yet exhibited higher activity. A comparative analysis

showed that both males and females on linear strips are investing more energy per

metre they move than bobucks in forest fragments. Based on this information, there is

an additional cost associated with living in linear strip environments compared with

their counterparts in forest fragments which is not identified by distance travelled

alone.

Linear strip environments have been shown to affect species by changing movement

patterns (Bonnot et al. 2013), increasing predation (Estrada et al. 2002) and disease

risk (Brearley et al. 2013), creating barriers to movement (Asari et al. 2010), and

Page 68: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

49

changing the shape of home ranges (van der Ree & Bennett 2003). The altered forest

structure in linear strip environments can create an environment less permeable to

movement due to reduced connectivity, especially in the composition and abundance

of groundcover and mid-story vegetation (McLean et al. 2016). Conversely,

individuals in remnant forest fragments may have greater flexibility to choose

movements via “least-cost” pathways by either reducing vertical distance travelled, or

travelling more direct routes (Hopkins 2011). Mapping the bobuck’s activity-to-

movement ratio in relation to forest structure could aid in understanding the

connectivity implications of different patterns of forest structure.

Linear strips have also been shown to increase animal stress levels (Van Meter et al.

2009, Arroyo-Rodriguez & Dias 2010, Brearley et al. 2012, Johnstone et al. 2012).

Detrimental effects such as more active behavioural states for bobucks in linear strip

environments were directly identified, but the drivers are most likely linked to

pressures not able to be examined as part of this study. For instance, predators of

bobucks such as powerful owls (Ninox strenua) have been found to prey more heavily

on the closely related common brushtail possum (T. vulpecula) in highly disturbed

landscapes (Cooke et al. 2006), while introduced predators such as foxes (Vulpes

vulpes) and cats (Felis catus) also take advantage of the more disturbed environments

for hunting (Claridge 1998). The added predation pressure would be expected to

increase both stress and vigilance for bobucks in linear strip environments. Linear

strips also have denser populations of both bobucks (Martin et al. 2007) and other

arboreal species as they use these corridors to traverse the environment (Beier & Noss

1998), so encounters resulting in either fighting or fleeing would also be expected to

be higher, potentially explaining the greater bursts of high activity seen in both males

Page 69: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

50

and females in linear strips. The impact of these detrimental effects on growth and

survival rates requires further investigation.

Both types of loggers can be used to measure animal movement and predict behaviour,

but they measure different aspects of animal movement. By coupling data from the

movement loggers, we were able to create a more comprehensive picture of animal

behaviour. We identified that both environmental context (habitat fragment type and

shape) and individual traits (sex) strongly affect the amount of activity individuals

undertake in order to move a given distance. Notably, there is important variation

within this pattern: both male and female bobucks in linear strips had

disproportionately higher activity levels per unit distance compared with bobucks in

forest fragments. It is only through this combined data that we can show the increase

in activity is not accounted for by greater distances travelled, and must therefore be

caused by higher activity at small spatial scales. This supports the theory that linear

strip environments detrimentally affect animal behaviour irrespective of smaller home

ranges, as higher activity levels will, in turn, result in higher energetic demands,

potentially impacting growth and survivorship (Wilson et al. 2006, Elliott et al. 2013,

Schell et al. 2013, Almasi et al. 2015).

The relationship between activity and distance travelled can help infer behaviours that

would be missed if either technology was used in isolation. For example, a large

activity value from an accelerometer over a given time interval could be due to an

animal moving between areas or defending their territory. As accelerometers do not

contain spatial data, it is only possible to say that an animal undertook a given amount

of dynamic acceleration. Similarly, GPS data would overlook any movement that does

not involve an animal travelling from one point to another. For instance, when

Page 70: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

51

examining the distance travelled by bobucks, we can identify components of each

night's movement where animals stayed in the same place for an extended period of

time, and assume the animal is resting (Heurich et al. 2012). However, if we include

the summed VeDBA information, we identify that only some of these periods have the

low activity levels expected while resting, while others have very high activity levels.

This can be the difference between resting and behaviours such as intense foraging,

fighting, or mating. By using both data loggers together, we can more accurately

characterise animal movement and potential explanations.

We argue that GPS data loggers are able to provide broader scale spatial and temporal

information within the environment, while accelerometer data loggers are able to

record finer scale activity and behavioural states. By combining these movement

loggers, we were able to build a detailed picture of an individual’s spatio-temporal

activity budgets as a whole, and specific behavioural states at a very fine scale. Our

approach allows animal movement to be examined and quantified at a scale rarely

achieved for free-ranging animals, and will be simple to replicate for many other

species in different systems. In addition to the fine temporal resolution of data from

accelerometers, the data also had fewer missed recordings. This is due to the

accelerometer using its own inertial measurement sensor to record information, which

is not affected by forest cover like the GPS. As such, the accelerometers can provide

information to estimate animal activity during poor GPS reception. This is particularly

important for arboreal species, which spend long periods in dense forest cover.

Our study highlights the difference between the spatial and temporal movement

information collected by GPS and accelerometer data loggers, and the significant

advantage of coupling these two movement recording technologies to better describe

Page 71: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

52

and quantify the spatio-temporal activity budgets of animals as influenced by habitat

variation. By combining the information from these loggers, we achieved a greater

understanding of how individuals interact with their environment. Such information

could be used in practical conservation applications, such as the design of vegetation

corridors or placement of nest boxes relative to food resources within the environment.

Individual activity and behaviour across different habitat compositions could also be

used to predict responses to environmental change (habitat loss and modification).

Page 72: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

53

Chapter 4: Measuring energy expenditure using

animal-borne movement data loggers in a free-

ranging scansorial mammal

4.1 Abstract

While the ability to measure the energy expenditure of free-living animals in their

natural habitat is integral to understanding their physiology and ecology, obtaining

accurate data is logistically and ethically difficult using conventional laboratory

techniques. Consequently, various alternative methods have been developed to derive

estimates of energy expenditure in free-living animals using data logging technology,

with accelerometry recently being proposed as an ideal method. We examined the

efficacy of accelerometry and GPS spatial movement to measure energy expenditure

in a scansorial mammal, the bobuck (Trichosurus cunninghami), in south-eastern

Australia. Using accelerometers to derive vectorial dynamic body acceleration

(VeDBA) and behavioural states, and GPS to derive distance travelled, we compared

these indices of energy expenditure with doubly labelled water (DLW) derived

estimates of field metabolic rate. The GPS distance travelled only explained 0.8 % of

the variation in energy expenditure values derived from DLW, while VeDBA

measurements explained 41.8 %. However, a multivariate model incorporating

accelerometry-derived behavioural states was able to explain 70.8 % of the variation

in total energy expenditure of the bobucks, confirming that accelerometry has high

potential for estimating energy expenditure in scansorial animals.

Page 73: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

54

4.2 Introduction

Measurement of energy expenditure can be used in ecology to attribute a physiological

‘cost’ to different animal activities which, in turn, can be used to assess how animals

behave and optimise their energy use (Ricklefs & Wikelski 2002, Shepard et al. 2009,

Wright et al. 2014). The ability of species to occupy environments is determined by

their energetic requirements, and the availability and distribution of resources

(Humphries et al. 2002). Therefore, knowledge of the energy expenditure of free-

living animals in relation to their habitat is integral to understanding animal physiology

and ecology which, in turn, can provide insights into how species might respond to

environmental variability and change (Nagy 1987, Withers et al. 2006, Laich et al.

2011).

An animal’s metabolic rate can be determined most accurately in the laboratory by

open-circuit respirometry (± 3-5 % (Withers 2001)). However, this method requires

the subject to be enclosed in a chamber which constrains natural behavioural

movement, and the results obtained may not be representative of the full range of

normal activity related to energy expenditure in free-ranging animals (Halsey & White

2010, Gleiss et al. 2011). Consequently, ecologists have explored methods to

accurately measure the energy expenditure of free living animals in the wild

(Speakman 2000, Butler et al. 2004, Wilson et al. 2006).

The doubly-labelled water (DLW) technique was one of the first methods used to

measure energy expenditure in free-living animals (Bennett & Nagy 1977, Bryant et

al. 1985). It has been previously validated by comparison with direct calorimetry in a

range of small mammals, and provides an accurate measure of energy expenditure over

Page 74: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

55

several days, with an estimated accuracy of ± 8 - 10% (Nagy 1989, Speakman et al.

1994, Speakman 1997). While this method has been used widely for many taxa (Corp

et al. 1999, Halsey & White 2010, Wright et al. 2014), it is expensive, and provides

only a single estimate of energy expenditure averaged over the study period (Butler et

al. 2004, Halsey et al. 2008, Qasem et al. 2012). Consequently, it is not possible to

assign energy expenditures to specific behaviours, limiting the ability to identify the

variation between individuals relative to their actions and habitat features.

Movement has been examined as a proxy for measuring energy expenditure (Green et

al. 2009, Gleiss et al. 2011, Fossette et al. 2012). Movement converts metabolic energy

into mechanical work and, therefore, accurate quantification of movement has the

potential to correlate with the energy expended to produce it (Speakman 2000, Yoda

et al. 2001, Gleiss et al. 2011). Measurement of movements can be recorded from

direct observation (D'Amico & Hemery 2007). However, the effectiveness of direct

observation is limited in free-living animals as often insufficient replicates can be

acquired, direct human observation may modify animal behaviour, and/or some

animals and terrains are non-conducive to direct observations. As such, technology is

employed to detect and quantify the movement of animals (Rutz & Hays 2009).

Location information derived from GPS data loggers has been used extensively to

track the spatial movements of a wide variety of animals (Weimerskirch et al. 2002,

Hebblewhite & Haydon 2010, Matthews et al. 2013) and extrapolated to create time-

activity models and activity estimates (Lachica & Aguilera 2005). This method has

shown potential in long-term studies on animals with large home ranges and clearly

defined active and resting periods (Hulbert et al. 1998, Johnson et al. 2002, Lachica &

Aguilera 2005). However, GPS derived movement models require a spatial change,

Page 75: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

56

meaning behaviours with little or no change in location such as grooming, fighting,

and mating, cannot be determined accurately. While there have been some

investigations of GPS derived distance and behaviour relative to energy expenditure,

the resulting relationships have been poor (Ganskopp & Johnson 2007, Munn et al.

2013, Marteinson et al. 2015).

Accelerometer data loggers have been proposed as a new method to record animal

activity. Accelerometers are reusable, and can record acceleration data at a very high

resolution (0.001 g) and rate (>100 Hz) across all three dimensions of movement. This

equates to a device which can record the vibrations created from the hum of a computer

hard-drive, and measure that vibration 100 times a second, giving a finer temporal

resolution than many other methods (Halsey & White 2010, Bidder et al. 2012).

Accelerometry has shown great potential as a predictor of energy expenditure in

laboratory conditions (R2 of 0.81 to 0.94) (Wilson et al. 2006, Fahlman et al. 2008,

Halsey et al. 2009b, Gleiss et al. 2011) and it has been used to predict behavioural

states in free-living animals (e.g. (Papailiou et al. 2008, Shepard et al. 2008, Elliott et

al. 2013)). However, there have been relatively few studies examining the accuracy

with which accelerometers can predict energy expenditure under field conditions,

covering few taxa, transport modes, and habitats (Elliott et al. 2013, Jeanniard‐du‐Dot

et al. 2016, Stothart et al. 2016). In addition, even fewer studies have incorporated the

whole range of 'active' and 'resting' natural behaviours. This is necessary as laboratory

studies suggest that, as activity decreases, the predictive ability of accelerometry

models also decreases (Wilson et al. 2006, Green et al. 2009, Gleiss et al. 2011).

Consequently, it is integral to determining how the relationships between behavioural

states and energy expenditure vary in relation to different environmental conditions.

Page 76: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

57

The bobuck (Trichosurus cunninghami) is a medium-sized (2.6–4.2 kg), hollow-

nesting, scansorial marsupial (How 1981, Lindenmayer et al. 2002). It occupies a

three-dimensional environment within wooded areas, incorporating both vertical and

horizontal movement within the canopy and across the ground, and is strictly nocturnal

with clearly defined active periods at night and resting periods during the day (Smith

et al. 1984). Understanding the energetic requirements of bobucks in relation to

behaviour and habitat is necessary in determining how these animals use, and persist

within, the current landscape as well as how they may respond to environmental

change. However, there is currently no information on the behavioural energetics of

the species. Unlike in birds (flight or walking (Elliott et al. 2013)) and fur seals (diving,

surface travel or walking on land (Jeanniard‐du‐Dot et al. 2016)), where various

behaviours involving movement are easily detectable/quantifiable with accelerometry

and incur very different transport costs, little is known of the three-dimensional

movement patterns of mammals like the bobuck and their energetic costs (Franz et al.

2005, Nams 2014).

The aims of this study, therefore, were to examine in bobucks: 1) the relationships

from GPS and accelerometry derived movement information and energy expenditure;

and 2) assess the factors which may influence their accuracy.

4.3 Methods

4.3.1 Study area, animal handling, and instrumentation

This study was conducted in March 2013 in the Strathbogie Ranges, north-eastern

Victoria. Bobucks were captured in large wire cage traps (30 x 30 x 90 cm) placed on

the ground and baited with peanut butter and apple. Trapping was undertaken in three

Page 77: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

58

contiguous forest fragments and three linear roadside strips (Figure 4.1). Trapping

sessions comprised one trap night in each location to avoid recaptures. The traps,

partially covered with plastic to protect animals from adverse weather conditions, were

set before dusk and checked before sunrise. Captured individuals were transferred

from the traps to hessian bags. Only adult individuals were used for this study (classed

as tooth-wear ≥ 3 (Winter 1980)).

Figure 4.1: Study sites in the Strathbogie Ranges, Victoria, Australia.

Blue shaded ovals indicate forest fragments and yellow shaded ovals indicate linear

strips.

Page 78: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

59

In order to reduce handling stress, animals were anaesthetised during processing using

isoflurane delivered via a facemask attached to a portable gas vaporizer (StingerTM,

Advanced Anaesthesia Specialists, Gladesville, NSW, Australia (Gales & Mattlin

1998)). Individuals were weighed (± 0.05 kg) in the Hessian bag using a suspension

scale (Pesola Macro-Line 80005). All individuals were fitted with data-logging collars

(80 g, < 3 % of body mass) comprised of a VHF transmitter, GPS data logger and 3-

axis accelerometer modified from the design in Allan et al. (2013). The collars were

set to start recording data as soon as they were attached to the animal with GPS data

set to record at 10 minute intervals, and accelerometers at 25 Hz.

The energy expenditure of individuals was calculated using the doubly labelled water

(DLW) technique (Lifson & McClintock 1966). This method has been previously

validated by comparison with indirect calorimetry in a range of marsupials, and

provides an accurate measure of energy expenditure over periods of several days

(Berteaux et al. 1996, Speakman & Krol 2005). Individuals were injected with DLW

at the same time collars were fitted.

Following instrumentation, a pre-dose blood sample (2 mL) was obtained by

venipuncture of a forearm vein for determination of the background isotope

enrichments of 2H and 18O (method C of (Speakman & Racey 1987)). A known mass

(± 0.001 g) of DLW (approximately 1 ml/kg; 30% atom 18O and 15% atom 2H) was

then administered intravenously through a catheter. The individuals were placed back

in their Hessian bags and left undisturbed for a 90 - 120 min period to allow the

isotopes to equilibrate in the body water pool (Krol & Speakman 1999) before a second

blood sample (2 mL) was obtained to determine initial isotopic enrichment. Blood

samples were kept cool (4 ºC) for several hours before being centrifuged and the

Page 79: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

60

plasma fraction separated. Eight plasma aliquots for each individual were then stored

in flame sealed 75-μl glass micro-capillary tubes at room temperature until analysis in

the laboratory.

In order to allow individuals to recover completely from handling and sedation, and to

avoid release of a strictly nocturnal animal in daylight, they were kept in hessian bags

until being released <10 m from the capture site at sunset (approximately 6-8 h

following post-dose blood sampling). Individuals were recaptured 5-7 days after their

release to remove the data-logging collars and take a final blood sample (2 mL) to

determine isotope elimination rates.

In the laboratory, the plasma samples were vacuum distilled (Nagy 1983) and water

from the resulting distillate was used to produce CO2 and H2. The isotope ratios 18O :

16O and 2H : 1H were analysed using gas-source isotope ratio mass spectrometry

(Isoprime IRMS and Isochrom μG; Micromass, Manchester, United Kingdom).

Background isotope levels were determined from the pre-dose blood sample. The

water obtained was used for isotope ratio mass spectrometric analysis of 2H and 18O.

Isotope enrichments were converted to values of daily energy expenditure using a

single pool model (Speakman 1993). Equation 7.17 of Speakman (1997) was used as

it has been established to minimize error in a range of conditions (Visser &

Schekkerman 1999). Daily energy expenditure was converted into overall energy

expenditure for the time the animal was injected with DLW.

Page 80: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

61

4.3.2 Sample analysis and data processing

The GPS spatial data points with an error > 40 m (5.63% of data) were deemed too

inaccurate and removed, resulting in an average Estimated Horizontal Position Error

(EHPE) of 9.41 ± 0.13 m. The spatial data was then converted into continuous-time

correlated random walk models, using the CRAWL package in R (Johnson 2012), to

re-create the animal's track through the environment. Correlated movements imply the

animal's location at a given time is dependent on all previous locations, not just the

last one, and continuous-time formulation allows data that have been non-uniformly

collected over time to be modelled without sub-sampling, interpolation, or aggregation

to obtain a set of locations uniformly spaced in time (Johnson et al. 2008). Nightly

movement tracks were derived for the movements of each animal based on the spatial

information, and from these tracks calculated the distance travelled by each individual

for the duration of the study.

The accelerometer was mounted to a collar around the animal's neck and, while it was

weighted in order to assist in orientation, there was the possibility for the collar to

rotate around the animal’s neck and shift the accelerometer’s axes in relation to the

orientation of the body. Any shift in the accelerometer has the potential to either

misidentify a behaviour, causing more behavioural states than actually exist to be

categorised, or identifying different behavioural states as the same. As such, Vectorial

Dynamic Body Acceleration (VeDBA) was used across all three axes as it is less

sensitive to changes in device orientation (Gleiss et al. 2011, Halsey et al. 2011, Qasem

et al. 2012). VeDBA was calculated at 25 Hz using the equation:

VeDBA

Page 81: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

62

The static acceleration (S), measured in gravitational force (g), was separated from the

total acceleration (A) for each of the three axes using a running mean of three seconds

(Gleiss et al. 2011). The resulting VeDBA values were summed to create a total

acceleration (VeDBATotal) for the duration of the DLW study period.

Behavioural state analysis was undertaken in Igor Pro version 6.36 (Wavemetrics,

Portland, OR, USA) using the Ethographer (Version 2.02) module (Sakamoto et al.

2009). The time series VeDBA data was converted into a spectrum by continuous

wavelet transformation. Then, each second of the spectrum was categorized into one

of 10 behavioural states by unsupervised cluster analysis, using k-means methods, and

measured at 1-second intervals.

The time spent in each behavioural state was summed for each animal. Energy

expenditure per behavioural state was calculated by fitting the following model:

. . .

where EE is the total energy expenditure (in KJ), Ci are the estimates (which

correspond to the rate of energy expenditure, in KJ per second) for the parameters Ti,

the time (seconds) spent per behavioural state listed above (i), following the

methodology of Elliot et al. (2013). The intercept was forced through 0 because no

energy is spent if no time passes (Jeanniard‐du‐Dot et al. 2016). As R2 values get

overinflated in models without intercepts, R2 was computed as 1 - (RSS / ESS), where

RSS is the regression sum of squares and ESS the error sum of squares of the models,

as an additional means of comparison of the variance between models.

Page 82: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

63

4.3.3 Statistical analyses

Generalised Linear Mixed Models (package MuMIn in R statistical environment (R

Core Development Team 2014)) were used to investigate the relationships between

DLW-derived estimates of energy expenditure for each individual and predictor

variables: VeDBA and distance travelled. Energy expenditure is often standardised by

mass (EEmass) or mass0.75 (EEmass 0.75) (Nagy et al. 1999, Nagy 2005) and, therefore,

both were also explored relative to VeDBA. Similarly, other studies have also

compared an average VeDBA (VeDBAAverage) to Daily Energy Expenditure (DEE)

(Elliott et al. 2013) so these were also explored. Multivariate models were used to

include other variables as random effects, such as sex, mass, and age. Unless otherwise

stated, data are presented as Means ± SE.

4.4 Results

Matched GPS, accelerometry, and energy expenditure data were obtained from 17

individuals (7 M, 10 F). Age (approximately 4 years, estimated from average tooth

wear 4.35 ± 0.29), and body mass (3.20 ± 0.08 kg) did not differ significantly between

the sexes. The duration of the DLW measurement period across all animals was 5.58

± 0.13 days, with a total energy expenditure of 3651.44 ± 172.19 KJ and daily energy

expenditure (DEE) of 651.37 ± 20.65 KJ/d. The total distance travelled by bobucks

during the study was 4976.92 ± 404.28 m. The total VeDBA (recorded at 25 Hz) was

973594.96 ± 30282.44 g with an average of 0.081 ± 0.002 g (Table 4.1).

Page 83: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

64

Table 4.1: Summary statistics for the 17 (7 M, 10 F) bobucks (Trichosurus

cunninghami) examined in this study in south-eastern Australia.

Variable Mean Standard Error Tooth Wear 4.35 0.29 Mass (kg) 3.2 0.08 Measurement Period (days) 5.58 0.13 Total Energy Expenditure (kJ) 3651.44 172.19 Daily Energy Expenditure (kJ/d) 651.37 20.65 Distance Travelled (m) 4976.92 404.28 Total VeDBA (g) 973594.96 30282.44 Average VeDBA (g) 0.081 0.002

The distance travelled by bobucks was a poor predictor for energy expenditure,

whether comparing total distance travelled (DistanceTotal) or daily distance travelled

(DistanceDaily), explaining < 1 % of the variation in both cases (Table 4.2 in Chapter 4

Appendix).

VeDBATotal was a moderate predictor of total energy expenditure (EE), explaining

41.8% of the variation in total energy expenditure. This was improved marginally to

46.6% with the inclusion of distance travelled. Neither energy expenditure

standardised by mass (EEmass), and mass0.75 (EEmass 0.75) proved to be a better predictor

of total energy expenditure, with total acceleration (VeDBATotal) only explaining 24.6

% and 33.1 % of the variation in the standardised energy expenditures respectively

(Figure 4.2, Table 4.3 in Chapter 4 Appendix).

Page 84: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

65

Figure 4.2: The relationship between total energy expenditure (kJ) and total

VeDBA (g) for bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) in south-eastern Australia.

Lines and shaded area represent the fitted relationship and 95% confidence intervals

from generalised linear mixed models, respectively. Solid line, yellow shading =

overall energy expenditure (EE); dotted line, blue shading = mass standardised overall

energy expenditure (EEmass); dashed broken line, green shading = mass0.75 standardised

total energy expenditure (EEmass0.75).

Average acceleration (VeDBAAverage) was compared to daily energy expenditure

(DEE), but was found to be a poor predictor, only explaining 11.8 % of the variation

in DEE. While DEE standardised by mass (EEmass), and mass0.75 (EEmass 0.75)

marginally improved the relationship with VeDBAAverage, explaining 28.5% and 27.5%

respectively, neither proved to be a good predictor of daily energy expenditure (Figure

4.3, Table 4.4 in Chapter 4 Appendix).

Page 85: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

66

Figure 4.3: The relationship between daily energy expenditure (kJ) and average

VeDBA (g) for bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) in south-eastern Australia.

Lines and shaded area represent the fitted relationship and 95% confidence intervals

from generalised linear mixed models, respectively. Solid line, yellow shading = daily

energy expenditure (DEE); dotted line, blue shading = mass standardised daily energy

expenditure (DEEmass); dashed broken line, green shading = mass0.75 standardised daily

energy expenditure (DEEmass0.75).

As specific behavioural states could not be validated visually, the 10 identified

behaviours categorised by unsupervised cluster analysis using k-means methods were

analysed in a frequency distribution taking into account time of day, and the

behavioural states both preceding and following each value (Figure 4.4). From this,

four broad behavioural categories were identified.

Page 86: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

67

1. Denning – Measurements taken during the day, and validated via GPS data

(mean VeDBA = 0.028 ± < 0.001 m/s2)

2. Resting at Night - very low VeDBA values, but while the bobucks were out of

the den (mean VeDBA = 0.108 ± < 0.001 m/s2)

3. Moving - moderate VeDBA values and during the night, including normal

foraging behaviour (mean VeDBA = 0.180 ± < 0.001 m/s2)

4. High Activity - Activity values much higher than all the others, accounting for

behavioural states such as mating, fighting, intense foraging, predator

avoidance, etc. (mean VeDBA = 0.260 ± 0.002 m/s2)

Page 87: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

68

Figure 4.4: A depiction of the summed VeDBA (25 Hz) per second, the

behavioural states (1 = denning, 2 = resting at night, 3 = moving, and 4 = high

activity), and the distance travelled (m) for a single bobuck (Trichosurus

cunninghami) over 24 hours.

The two inserts are exploded views of the data over 10 minutes.

Page 88: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

69

The average proportion of time animals time spent ‘denning’ was 52.7 ± 1.2%, ‘resting

at night’ was 26.4 ± 0.8 %, ‘moving’ was 20.5 ± 0.9 %, and ‘high activity’ was 0.4 ±

<0.1 %. Using the time spent in each behavioural state, multiplied by the rate of energy

expenditure for that state explained 60.6 % (R2 = 0.606) of the variation in total energy

expenditure (EE) of the bobucks (Table 4.5).

Table 4.5: Model selection results for the generalised mixed models comparing

energy expenditure to behavioural states for bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami)

in south-eastern Australia.

Overall energy expenditure (kJ) was compared to the time spent (seconds) in each of

four behavioural states (Denning, Resting at Night, Moving and Foraging, and High

Activity), with all models including morphological variables. AICc values, differences

in AICc values (∆i) and Akaike weight (wi) are shown for models with ∆i < 10.

Denning Resting at Night

Moving High

Activity Distance Travelled

Sex Tooth Wear

Mass R2 df AICc Δi wi

0.010309 0.000495 0.022378 0.134078

-0.463 0.708 6 124.746 269.89 0

0.008275 -0.00236 0.017135 0.096801

0.606 5 127.289 270.03 0.13

0.008264 -0.00116 0.016624 0.133869 -0.034

0.610 6 127.202 274.80 4.91

0.013765 0.001251 0.024565 0.092517 +

0.715 7 124.534 275.51 5.62

0.010298 0.001685 0.021867 0.171096 -0.033

-0.463 0.712 7 124.62 275.70 5.81

The best predictors of total bobuck energy expenditure were the behavioural states as

well as mass, and explained 70.8 % (R2 = 0.708) of the variation in total energy

expenditure (EE) of the bobucks (Figure 4.5, Table 4.5). The rate of energy

expenditure for each behaviour was; denning: 0.0103 ± 0.003 kJ/s, resting at night:

0.0005 ± 0.006 kJ/s, moving: 0.0224 ± 0.008 kJ/s, and high activity: 0.1341 ± 0.212

Page 89: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

70

kJ/s. The two significant variables for predicting energy expenditure in bobucks were

the behavioural states ‘denning’ and ‘moving’ (Table 4.6).

Figure 4.5: The relationship between total energy expenditure (kJ) as measured

by DLW, and the best predictor of bobuck energy expenditure, including the four

behavioural states and mass, explaining 70.8 % (R2 = 0.708) of the variation in

total energy expenditure (EE) of the bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) in

south-eastern Australia.

Lines and shaded area represent the fitted relationship and 95% confidence intervals

from generalised linear mixed models.

Page 90: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

71

Table 4.6: Parameter estimates from the best Time-Activity model comparing the

overall energy expenditure (kJ) of bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) to the

time spent (seconds) in each of four behavioural states (Denning, Resting at Night,

Moving and Foraging, and High Activity) and mass.

The model was forced through the intercept as no energy is spent if no time passes.

Variable Coefficient Standard Error p-value Denning 0.0103 kJ/s 0.003 0.002 Resting at Night 0.0005 kJ/s 0.006 0.930 Moving 0.0224 kJ/s 0.008 0.019 High Activity 0.1341 kJ/s 0.212 0.539 Mass (g) -0.4626 0.226 0.063

4.5 Discussion

The ability to quantify the behavioural and energetic information for free-living

animals across a broad range of animal movement allows researchers to determine the

physiological costs associated with particular behaviours of animals in the field

(Wilson et al. 2006, Halsey & White 2010, Elliott et al. 2013). This knowledge is

crucial in understanding an animal’s physiology and ecology and, in turn, species’

limitations (distribution and abundance) with respect to the influence of resource

availability and distribution, and predicting the effects of environmental change

(Halsey & White 2010). This study found that estimates of energy expenditure both

from GPS data and raw accelerometer data were poor. However, accelerometer-

derived behavioural states have the potential to accurately estimate energy expenditure

in a free-living, scansorial mammal, across both active and resting behavioural states.

The behavioural state estimates of free-living bobucks in this study had a predictive

Page 91: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

72

accuracy similar to studies conducted on other species under laboratory conditions

(Wilson et al. 2006, Halsey et al. 2009b).

4.5.1 DLW and energy expenditure

Bobucks were tracked for 5-7 days in order to provide adequate isotope elimination

rates for the DLW (Speakman 1997). The average energy expenditure recorded for this

time was comparatively low compared to other studies on similarly sized mammals

(1810 kJ/d for a 3.20 kg mammal) (Nagy 2005). However, marsupials such as the

bobuck are known to have a metabolic rate approximately 30% lower than that of

eutherian mammals (Dawson & Hulbert 1970, Nagy et al. 1999), and arboreal

frugivorous and folivorous species also tend to have lower basal metabolic rates than

other mammals (McNab 1995). There was also high variability in the daily activity

both between and within individuals, meaning that there was potentially high variation

in the daily energy expenditure as well. Unfortunately, DLW provides only a single

value for the study period, so this cannot be explored further.

Measures of energy expenditure for free-living bobucks have not been undertaken

before, so direct comparisons with other studies and study areas are not possible.

However, Nagy & Bradshaw (2000) found the general DEE of marsupials to be 10.1

x M0.59 kJ/day, where M is body mass in grams. Using this equation, the average daily

energy expenditure of a 3.20 kg bobuck would be 1181 kJ/d, which is almost double

the 651 ± 22 kJ/d recorded in our study. Other studies on free-living marsupials also

found higher energy expenditures (e.g. spectacled hare wallaby (Lagorchestes

conspicillatus), body mass 2529g, DEE 680 kJ/day (Nagy & Bradshaw 2000), and the

female tammar wallaby (Macropus eugenii), body mass 3328 g, DEE 964 kJ/day

Page 92: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

73

(Nagy et al. 1990). A laboratory-based study on the closely related common brushtail

possum also found higher energy expenditure (T. vulpecula, body mass 2478 g, DEE

833 kJ/day (Williams & Turnbull 1983)).

4.5.2 Distance and energy expenditure

Distance travelled, derived from GPS data, was a poor predictor of energy expenditure

in bobucks. This is most likely due to the manner in which a GPS records data. A GPS

requires the change in spatial location to be greater than the error associated in the

location recording. In this study, the average GPS error was 9.41 m, meaning activities

which occur within a small spatial area, such as intense foraging or fighting, may not

have been recorded. The relatively coarse recording rate (10 minutes) may also affect

the distance recorded, as travel is calculated as a straight line between points, whereas

animals may often travel a more circuitous route (Munn et al. 2013). Furthermore,

when the bobuck was stationary, the error between location fixes can create false

recordings of movement. In a study on cattle, this false movement was found to

increase the daily distance an animal travelled by as much as 15 % (Ganskopp &

Johnson 2007). On average, bobucks spent over half their time at night in low-

movement behaviour, which could lead to a much greater measure of false movement.

A GPS records changes in spatial location, however, it only records changes in

horizontal location, and not changes in vertical location. Measures of horizontal

movement may be appropriate for non-climbing terrestrial species, but spool-and-line

tracking of several scansorial species has shown they can move considerable distances

vertically, climbing up and down trees (Cunha & Vieira 2002, Wells et al. 2006). The

inability of the GPS to record these movements can lead to underestimating the

Page 93: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

74

distance travelled by scansorial species. Conversely, accelerometers are able to capture

this three-dimensional activity, record movement irrespective of a change in spatial

location, and derive different energy expenditure for different activities. As such,

estimates of energy expenditure derived from accelerometry provide a much more

comprehensive picture of animal movement.

4.5.3 Accelerometry as a surrogate for recording movement

Accelerometry provides a method to record long-term movement data over days, or

even months, for species which are otherwise difficult to observe directly, such as

nocturnal and cryptic species, and/or species which live in challenging environments

such as underwater, underground, or within tree canopies. The total acceleration

(VeDBATotal) only proved to be a moderate predictor of energy expenditure. However,

accelerometry is capable of providing more than just a direct measure to compare to

energy expenditure, it was also able expand on the knowledge of bobuck ecology.

During this study, the accelerometry identified a clear circadian rhythm of nocturnal

activity and diurnal inactivity. This is well documented for possum species, which are

considered a strictly nocturnal species (Smith et al. 1984), and is also shown in the

GPS data. However, unlike spatially recording devices such as GPS, the accelerometer

is recording multiple times a second, and irrespective of external data connection. For

example, while bobucks only spent about 50% of their time denning, the accelerometry

data was able to identify cycles of resting during the night, bringing the total time spent

in low activity states to approximately 80% of their day. Given the large amount of

time spent in behavioural states with low accelerometry values, accurate field

measurements of energy expenditure necessitate the prediction of all behaviours

contributing to energy expenditure (Gleiss et al. 2011).

Page 94: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

75

The high temporal resolution recording provides information which can, in turn, be

used to provide information relating to behavioural states. By calculating energy

budget ethograms to derive behavioural states we were able to create time-activity

budgeting for each individual. This allowed for the calculation of different rates of

energy expenditure for different activities. As shown in studies by Elliott et al. (2013)

and Jeanniard-du-Dot et al. (2016), the rate of energy expenditure for behavioural

states is different from the rate of acceleration. For instance, the behavioural state

‘moving’ had an average VeDBA almost 6.5 times higher than ‘denning’, but a rate of

energy expenditure only approximately twice as high. The ability to assign different

rates of energy expenditure to different behavioural states ultimately led to behavioural

states explaining the most variation in DLW derived energy expenditure.

Accelerometry has the potential to identify a greater number of specific behavioural

states. However, the ability to estimate energy expenditure from behavioural states

relies on the precision of determining the different activities and behavioural states

from the accelerometer data, and the consistency of energy expenditure for that

behaviour (Corp et al. 1999). For example, the denning behaviour category consists of

occasional short temporal-scale spikes of higher activity, as seen in Figure 4.2. These

specific activities, which could not be identified in the present study, presumably

include actions such as grooming, mating, and other social interactions. These other

activities are believed to drive the higher rate of energy expenditure during denning

compared to resting at night. The use of broad behavioural states means that the energy

coefficient for some states will have a greater margin of error than others, and

ultimately some identified behavioural states will estimate energy expenditure better

than others. A validation study on the identification of fine-scale behavioural signals

Page 95: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

76

in acceleration data could potentially provide a wider range of identified behavioural

states, greater accuracy in prediction of energy expenditure, and a greater

understanding of how individuals use the environment.

Accelerometry also has the potential to be paired with other movement recording

technology to better identify and refine measures of both behavioural states and energy

expenditure relative to spatial environmental data. GPS devices were used in

combination with accelerometry in Chapter 3 to compare between spatial movement

and activity. Similarly, GPS devices have been used in combination with

accelerometry to identify behavioural states (Nams 2014). In respect to arboreal and

aerial species, barometers could also be incorporated to measure both the elevation,

and relative changes in altitude in animal movement, better identifying vertical

movement (Hunter et al. 2007).

4.5.4 Other influences on energy expenditure

Mass is well established as being one of the dominant factors influencing the variation

in basal metabolic rate (BMR) and field metabolic rate (FMR) between different

species (Dawson & Hulbert 1970, Harris et al. 1985, Speakman 2000). Mass

influences energy expenditure via thermal regulation, food requirement, and the cost

of moving, as mechanical power output for an action is relative to the mass required

to move (Nagy 2005, Speakman 2005). Directly scaling energy expenditure by mass

did not improve the relationships with accelerometry or GPS data, but including mass

as a variable did improve the relationship in analyses such as the behavioural states.

This may be due to the study focussing on multiple individuals of the same species.

While general equations for energy expenditure based on mass between different

Page 96: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

77

species are common (Nagy & Bradshaw 2000), the influence of mass on energy

expenditure between individuals of the same species is less well known.

Sex and age are also known to have the potential to influence metabolic rate (Nagy

1987, Gillooly et al. 2001, White & Seymour 2003). Sex can influence the rate of

energy expenditure due to the fundamental physiological differences related to size,

hormones, body composition, and breeding, which can cause variation in the rate of

EE independent of movement (Beck et al. 2003, Cryan & Wolf 2003, Careau et al.

2013). Similarly, age can influence energetic requirements based on caloric intake and

activity (Kirkwood & Holliday 1979, Ramsey et al. 2000). Therefore, sex and age

(measured by tooth wear) were also included as potential influences to energy

expenditure. However, neither influenced the relationship between distance travelled,

VeDBA, or accelerometer derived behavioural states and energy expenditure. There

was no significant difference between the mass of male and female bobucks, and the

study was conducted during the non-breeding season, which may account for why sex

did not influence the relationship. Similarly, juveniles were not included in the study,

reducing the influence of age.

In summary, the results of the present study indicate that accelerometry can be used to

accurately predict total energy expenditure in free-living, scansorial, medium-sized

mammals, such as bobucks, and is most informative when the accelerometry data are

also used to derive behavioural information. The incorporation of behavioural

information allows for the allocation of different rates of energy expenditure relative

to the activity level of the animal. This information will greatly improve energetics

studies conducted under field conditions, which include both active and resting

behavioural states. Unfortunately, the information collected via GPS was a poor

Page 97: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

78

predictor of energy expenditure, meaning that retrospectively examining energy

expenditure for GPS tracked animals is not possible. However, given the ability of

accelerometry to predict energy expenditure in free-living animals, future research into

how behaviour is shaped by habitat variation and environmental change could help

advance ecological theory (e.g. species’ niches) and guide conservation actions (e.g.

reserve design and habitat restoration).

Page 98: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

79

Chapter 5: Contrasting congener behaviour in

relation to environmental variation

5.1 Abstract

Habitat loss and modification are the greatest threats to biodiversity globally. To

predict how species might respond to future environmental change, we must first

understand how the environment limits species’ current distribution. Advances in GPS

and accelerometer technology provide new methods to accurately collect species’

movement and behavioural data, providing insight into how habitat variation and

modification affect animals. Using GPS and accelerometry data, we examined

movement and other behaviours of two congeners, the common brushtail possum

(Trichosurus vulpecula) and bobuck (T. cunninghami), in two spatially and

structurally distinct sites, the Strathbogie Ranges (foothill forest > 500 m a.s.l.) and

Grantville in Gippsland (heavily cleared coastal and lowland forest with an average

elevation < 100 m a.s.l.) in Victoria, Australia. Bobucks in Grantville spent more time

resting at night, and had reduced abundance, body mass, and a restricted distribution

in Grantville. Despite this, individual bobucks at both locations had similar daily

energy expenditure. These behavioural patterns and differences in morphology may

relate to constraints in resource availability. Our low bobuck capture success in

Grantville suggests there is limited suitable habitat in the region, reducing bobuck

movement and activity, and constraining them to a limited set of resources. Grantville

bobucks may be close to the limits of their niche space. Common brushtail possums

had much greater body mass in the Grantville region compared with the Strathbogies,

counter. Common brushtail possums in the Strathbogies may be experiencing

Page 99: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

80

ecological character displacement; only persisting in more modified environments,

denning for longer, spending less time in high activity behavioural states, and having

lower body mass and daily energy expenditure compared to bobucks, in order to

occupy similar habitat. These findings suggest that species’ responses to different

environments can change greatly both intra- and inter-specifically. As such, effective

conservation and management requires ecological understanding of species across

their distributions in order to understand the possible range of responses to

environmental change.

5.2 Introduction

A fundamental goal of ecology is to describe and understand which factors affect

patterns of species distribution and abundance. Resource availability, which affects an

individual’s growth, survival, and reproduction (Johnson 1980, Hoffmann et al. 2012,

Fordyce et al. 2015), is important in this context. Under ideal conditions, individuals

select habitats promoting their vital rates, freely and without cost (Fretwell & Lucas

1969), however there are typically constraints that limit an animal’s habitat use, such

as dispersal ability, habitat connectivity, predation risk, and competition (Potter et al.

2012). The environment in which a species can survive given resource availability and

other constraints is often referred to as its niche. Niche statistics are also used to predict

a species’ future distribution in response to environmental change (Hirzel & Le Lay

2008), and hence, can be used to establish conservation priority areas and select

reserve designs (Kearney & Porter 2009).

Habitat loss and modification are the largest threats to biodiversity globally, greatly

impacting species’ niches (Fahrig 2001, Laurance et al. 2008, Selwood et al. 2009).

Page 100: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

81

Anthropogenic modification such as urbanisation, roads, agriculture, and timber

production, reduces the amount of unmodified habitat available to species (Tuomainen

& Candolin 2011). While habitat loss is the immediate effect, the long-term effects

can include disruptions to ecosystem, community, and population structure by creating

a landscape mosaic of natural fragments surrounded by less hospitable, modified

environments, which in turn leads to changes in species distribution and abundance

(McCall et al. 2010, Pike et al. 2011, Lowry et al. 2013).

To understand how species might respond to habitat change and loss, we must first

understand how the environment limits their current distribution. Anthropogenic

environmental changes currently occurring in ecosystems are often detrimental for the

survival of species (Lawton 1993, Pounds et al. 1999, Harvell et al. 2002). For

instance, climate warming is drastically reducing sea ice in the arctic, which is the

primary habitat for polar bears (Ursus maritimus), which in turn is causing their

population decline and has resulted in them becoming vulnerable to extinction (Stirling

& Derocher 2012, IUCN 2016). However, for other species, such as the black rat

(Rattus rattus), environmental change and urbanisation has increased both available

habitat, and rat population numbers, often at the expense of other species (Feng &

Himsworth 2014). Species such as black rats are capable of adjusting to a range of

environmental conditions and novel ecosystems (Morris 1996, Devictor et al. 2008),

and can become invasive (Tuomainen & Candolin 2011). Studying fragmented

ecosystems enables us to examine the consequences of habitat loss and modification,

how this might impact habitat processes, and allow predictions regarding the

likelihood of species persistence and survival in the face of future environmental

change.

Page 101: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

82

In the midst of extensive habitat loss and modification, it is essential to accurately

classify species’ habitat requirements and what factors influence these (Barnosky et

al. 2011, Ceballos et al. 2015). Presence-absence models have traditionally been used

to identify habitat suitability (Dorazio 2012, Haby et al. 2012, Matthews & Whittaker

2015). While data on species presence can describe habitat associations, it does not

provide any information on how the resources are exploited (Shipley et al. 2009,

Eycott et al. 2012, Vasudev & Fletcher 2015). Given the relative plasticity some

species exhibit in response to varying resource availability, models not taking resource

exploitation into account, and its effects on animal movement and other behaviour,

have limited scope for predicting a species reaction to a changing environment (Jeffree

& Jeffree 1994, Tilman 1994, Ritchie et al. 2008, Loxdale et al. 2011). Therefore, we

require new methods focussing on animal movement and behaviour to quantify how

animals use their environment.

Animal movement governs the ability of species to persist in, and respond to, changing

environments, as resource selection is strongly influenced by movement decisions

(Frair et al. 2005, Fahrig 2007, Fordyce et al. 2015). Animals move to acquire

resources (Bell 2012), avoid predators and competition (Creel & Christianson 2008,

Ritchie & Johnson 2009), and to be near conspecifics for mating and other social

interactions (Fahrig 2007). Furthermore, the initial response of animals to habitat

disturbance is often behavioural (Tuomainen & Candolin 2011). These behavioural

responses can result in changes to species interactions, population dynamics and

evolutionary processes, which ultimately determine population persistence and

speciation (Tuomainen & Candolin 2011). Therefore, the accurate measurement of

Page 102: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

83

movement and behaviour could greatly further our understanding of how animals use

the environment.

Recent advances in technology provide new methods to accurately collect movement

and behavioural data, particularly the combination of GPS and accelerometry. GPS

has been used to provide insights into animal space-use in modified landscapes

(Fryxell et al. 2008, Gautestad et al. 2013, Vasudev & Fletcher 2015). However, these

technologies measure only one coarse aspect of animal movement: the change in the

location of the individual over time (Nathan et al. 2008). Conversely, accelerometers

record movement as a change in acceleration at a very fine scale (0.001 g) and rate

(>100 Hz) across all three dimensions of movement. Using accelerometry, several

studies have shown that an animal’s free-ranging movement at a fine temporal scale is

correlated to energy expenditure ((Wilson et al. 2006, Elliott et al. 2013) Chapter 4).

Therefore, by combining these two technologies we are able to measure the movement

and behaviour of animals. Such methods lead to a more comprehensive understanding

of ecological interactions, species’ niches, and allow us to predict not only which taxa

might be vulnerable to habitat loss, but also some of the consequences for community

composition and ecosystem function (Kremen et al. 1993, Rosenblatt et al. 1999,

Hughes et al. 2000).

Arboreal and scansorial animals are especially vulnerable to habitat modification, as

they require habitat connectivity among the canopy to traverse the environment, or

else must come to ground and face potential increased predation risk (Louys et al.

2011, Mendonca et al. 2015, Vasudev & Fletcher 2015). Closely-related congeneric

species provide good opportunities to compare and contrast how different behavioural

responses might affect species and individuals in changing environments. Two

Page 103: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

84

sympatric arboreal and scansorial species capable of living in heterogeneous

landscapes are the common brushtail possum (Trichosurus vulpecula) and the

mountain brushtail possum, or bobuck, (T. cunninghami). Both species are medium-

sized (2 – 4 kg), nocturnal, semi-arboreal marsupials. While populations of the

common brushtail possum have declined extensively in the wild, they have adapted

well to urban landscapes, and are one of Australia’s most widely distributed

marsupials. Bobucks, however, have a patchy distribution throughout wet sclerophyll

forests and sub-tropical forests of eastern Australia, and are not found in urban

environments. Unlike common brushtails, bobucks are considered more specialised

and dependent on a few key resources (Martin & Martin 2007), particularly a food tree

(silver wattle, Acacia dealbata). This may mean bobucks are more limited in their

ability to respond to changes such as urbanisation, habitat loss and modification.

Therefore, while these species are sometimes sympatric, and exploit some of the same

resources, they are not always found together.

Our aim was to examine the movement and behavioural strategies employed by

common brushtails and bobucks in two spatially and structurally distinct sites, the

Strathbogie Ranges (foothill forest > 500 m a.s.l.) and Grantville in Gippsland (heavily

cleared coastal and lowland forest with an average elevation < 100 m a.s.l.). Examining

distance travelled and activity levels on a nightly basis, we expected little difference

in bobucks across the two regions as we expected them to be less able to adapt

behaviourally to change, and to therefore be more limited in their habitat choices in

the Grantville region (Bonier et al. 2007, Jiguet et al. 2007). Conversely, we expected

to observe greater variation in the behaviour and habitat use of common brushtail

possums between the two regions, as they adapted their behaviour to exploit different

Page 104: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

85

resources available between the two locations. Finally, we examined how the

movement and behavioural strategies of each species affected energy expenditure. By

examining these different aspects of behaviour, we aimed to better understand how

these two species use different environments, and their ability to respond to and

survive future environmental change.

5.3 Methods

5.3.1 Study area and design

The study areas were the Strathbogie Ranges in north-eastern Victoria, and Grantville

in southern Victoria (Figure 5.1). The straight-line distance between the two study

areas is approximately 165 km. The vegetation in the Strathbogie Ranges mainly

consists of herb-rich foothill forest and grassy dry forest dominated by manna gum

(Eucalyptus viminalis), Victorian blue gum (Eucalyptus globulus bicostata), narrow-

leaf peppermint (Eucalyptus radiata) and broad-leafed peppermint (Eucalyptus dives).

Key middle and understorey species include silver wattle (Acacia dealbata), dogwood

(Cassinia aculeata), cherry ballart (Exocarpos cupressiformis), blackwood (Acacia

melanoxylon) and austral bracken (Pteridium esculentum) (Martin & Handasyde

2007). The Grantville vegetation is more variable with a mix of lowland forest, heathy

woodland, swampy woodland, riparian scrub, and even mangrove shrubland. The

vegetation is predominantly eucalypts such as messmate (Eucalyptus obliqua) and

narrow-leaf peppermint. Key understory species include blackwood, prickly tea-tree

(Leptospermum continentale), she-oak (Casuarina spp.), sweet pittosporum

(Pittosporum undulatum), mangroves (Avicennia marina), and various sedges,

bracken, and grasses (Hynes & Cleeland 2005).

Page 105: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

86

Figure 5.1: The study areas: the Strathbogie Ranges (foothill forest > 500 m a.s.l.)

in north-eastern Victoria, and Grantville (heavily cleared coastal and lowland

forest with an average elevation < 100 m a.s.l.) in southern Victoria, Australia.

The habitat configuration in Grantville greatly differs from the Strathbogie ranges.

First and foremost is the history of anthropogenic influence. Grantville has been more

extensively cleared since European Settlement, leaving little remnant old-growth

vegetation > 100 years old. Furthermore, extensive fires in 1898 reduced much of the

old-growth forest, the result of which is stands of trees which are a uniform age of

approximately 120 years, not considered old enough to have hollows for large arboreal

species (> 150 years) (Lindenmayer et al. 1991). The region is also coastal, with a

Page 106: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

87

dense understory of prickly tea-tree, a species not used by bobucks (Gloury &

Handasyde 2016), and stunted tree growth due to strong coastal winds.

We undertook trapping sessions over eight consecutive seasons from spring 2011 to

winter 2013 inclusive. Within each study site, we trapped for male bobucks and

common brushtail possums. We targeted male possums because they do not have

factors that complicate the measurement of mass and movement (i.e. pouch young,

back young, and milk production during different times of the year). We trapped

possums in linear strip environments (> 1 km length and an average vegetation width

of ~ 40 m, termed 'linear strips' throughout). Our sites were confined to areas of habitat

consisting primarily of native vegetation and containing trees large enough to provide

suitable hollows (DBH > 50 cm) (Smith & Lindenmayer 1988, Pausas et al. 1995).

Each site was separated by at least 500 m to reduce the chances of the same individual

being caught at two locations, based on home range data from Martin & Handasyde

(2007). After four consecutive trapping sessions in Grantville we had few possum

captures. Based on local knowledge we moved three trap lines which had zero captures

from their placement on linear strips to target gully lines in the forest fragments.

Each site had two rows of eight traps (50m apart), with each row a minimum of 200 m

apart. Trapping sessions comprised three trap nights in each of the trap locations. We

captured individuals in large (30 x 30 x 90 cm) wire cage traps placed on the ground

and baited with peanut butter and apple. We set the traps before dusk and checked

them before sunrise. Traps were partially covered with plastic to protect animals from

weather.

5.3.2 Data collection

Page 107: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

88

For processing, we sedated individuals with an intramuscular injection of

tiletamine/zolazepam (6 mg kg –1; Zoletil ®; Virbac Australia, Peakhurst, NSW,

Australia) to facilitate handling and minimise distress for the animals. We weighed

individuals (± 0.05 kg) in hessian bags using a suspension scale (Pesola Macro-Line

80005). We injected individuals with Passive Integrated Transponder (PIT) tags

(Trovan, Ltd.) for subsequent identification. We then fitted individuals with self-made

data-logging collars (80 g) comprised of a VHF transmitter, GPS data logger and 3-

axis accelerometer modified from the design in Allan et al. (2013). We set the collars

to start recording data as soon as they were attached to the animal with GPS data set

to record at 10-minute intervals, and accelerometers at 25 Hz. We only used adult

males for this study (classed as tooth-wear ≥ 3 (Winter 1980)). In order to allow

individuals to recover completely from handling and sedation, and to avoid releasing

possums (which are strictly nocturnal) in daylight, we held the animals after processing

until dusk on that day. We released the possums within 10 m of the site where they

were captured. We recaptured individuals 3-7 days later and removed the data-logging

collars.

5.3.3 Data processing

We expected the two possum species to use resources and the environment differently

based on both species and study site location. Therefore, we split the data into species

(bobucks (T. cunninghami) and common brushtails (T. vulpecula)) and location

(Strathbogies and Grantville) for analysis, creating four groups (bobucks Strathbogies,

bobucks Grantville, common brushtails Strathbogies, and common brushtails

Grantville) in order to reduce confounding variation in the results.

Page 108: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

89

GPS data – We converted the raw GPS data into two variables; the total distance

travelled each night, termed ‘nightly distance travelled’ measured in metres, and

distance travelled in 10 minute intervals, termed ‘interval distance travelled’ also

measured in metres, following the methods described in Chapters 3 and 4. The nights

of release and capture were removed from both, as we cannot be sure a complete night

of data was collected. We examined the nightly distance travelled because both species

den in tree hollows during daylight hours. It also created a clear break as to when one

GPS track finished, and the next began. For interval distance travelled, we determined

locations at 10-minute intervals along the interpolated movement tracks, and measured

the distance between these locations.

Accelerometer data – We converted the raw accelerometer data into 1-second intervals

of partial VeDBA following the methods described in Chapters 3 and 4. We then used

partial VeDBA to create 3 different variables: 1) ’24-hour activity’ which is the sum

of the partial VeDBA values measured at 25 Hz over 24 hours, commensurate to

‘nightly distance travelled’; 2) ‘interval activity’ which is the sum of the partial

VeDBA values measured at 25 Hz in 10 minute intervals matching ‘interval distance

travelled’; and 3) ‘behavioural state’ which is the amount of time animals spent in each

of four behavioural states, measured in 1-second intervals, over the same 24-hour

period as ‘nightly distance travelled’ ‘24 hour activity’, again following the methods

described in Chapters 3 and 4.

We derived the energy expenditure of each individual using the most accurate method

found in Chapter 4, which included the accelerometer-derived behavioural states and

mass, using the equation:

Page 109: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

90

Denning 0.0103 RestingatNight 0.0005 Moving 0.0224 HighActivity 0.1341

0.4626

This equation explained 70.8 % (R2 = 0.708) of the variation in total energy

expenditure (EE) of the bobucks in the Strathbogies (See Chapter 4). The energy

demands of individuals may vary moving through different forest structures. However,

the classification of movement into four broad behavioural states instead of energy

allocation for specific movements should help account for the variation of forest

structure. We calculated the total energy expenditure (kJ) for each individual, and then

converted to daily energy expenditure (DEE kJ/day) for comparison, as the number of

days each individual was active varied A list of the measures of animal movement is

provided in Table 5.1.

Table 5.1: Outline of the measures of animal (bobuck, Trichosurus cunninghami,

and common brushtail possum, T. vulpecula) movement.

Device Measurement Unit of

Measurement Timeframe of measurement

Method of measurement

GPS Nightly Distance Travelled

Metres (m) 24 hours Summed distance based on interpolated movement track from GPS points over 24 hours

Interval Distance Travelled

Metres (m) 10 minute intervals

Nightly interpolated movement track broken into 10 minute intervals

Accelerometer 24 Hour Activity Gravitational Force (g)

24 hours Summed activity based on accelerometer values measured at 25 Hz over 24 hours

Interval Activity Gravitational Force (g)

10 minute intervals

Summed VeDBA values, measured at 25 Hz, into 10 minute intervals

Behavioural State Categorical 1 second intervals over 24 hours

VeDBA values, measured at 25 Hz, used in Ethographer to create four behavioural states measured in 1-second intervals

Daily Energy Expenditure

Kilojoules (kJ)

24 hours Daily energy expenditure (kJ/d) calculated from the behavioural state information, using the equation from Chapter 4

Predictor variables - All response variables were related to two predictor variables

and their interaction. First, to examine the impact of species on movement and energy

expenditure, we included a two-level categorical variable indicating the species

Page 110: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

91

examined: either bobuck (T. cunninghami) or common brushtail (T. vulpecula). A

second two-level categorical variable was included indicating the individual possums’

locations, either Strathbogies or Grantville.

5.3.4 Statistical analysis

We used Generalized Linear Mixed Models (GLMMs) to analyse the effects of the

predictor variables on measures of animal movement derived from GPS and

accelerometer data, following the analysis conducted in Chapter 3. We examined the

movement variables measured over 24 hours (nightly distance travelled, nightly

activity and the time spent in each of the four behavioural states), and activity in

relation to distance travelled and two predictor variables (individuals, species and site).

GLMMs were generated using the nlme package in R (R Core Development Team

2014). We derived total energy expenditure for each individual by following the

equation from Chapter 4, and divided it by the time each individual was collared to

calculate DEE. While this model was developed for bobucks, common brushtail

possums are ecologically, morphologically and physiologically similar, and

evolutionarily closely-related (Smith et al. 1984, Kerle & Saunders 2001). We

generated GLMMs of the relationship between the DEE and the predictor variables

and determined the r-squared value for the resulting model using the r.squaredGLMM

function in the MuMIn package. Second, we ran models with each discrete group as a

predictor variable in order to determine the mean DEE and the significant differences

between the three groups.

We applied a Bonferroni correction to the p-values to infer significant differences

between groups after accounting for multiple comparisons. To evaluate the

Page 111: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

92

‘significance’ of differences we observed, we report effect sizes, confidence intervals,

and p-values, with alpha set at p < 0.1 for all multiple comparisons except the interval

comparison. We chose p < 0.1 for alpha because we didn’t have many observations

(nights) for some levels of tests, and hence a more conservative p-value (0.05) might

have made it harder to detect ‘significant differences’, as per results and guidance from

(Nakagawa & Cuthill 2007) and (Jennions & Møller 2003). The interval comparison

had a larger number of samples at a finer temporal resolution, so we set alpha to 0.05

for this comparison.

5.4 Results

We trapped bobucks and common brushtail possums in both the Strathbogie Ranges

and Grantville. Male bobucks were found in both forest fragments and linear strips in

the Strathbogie Ranges, but were only in found in linear-strip environments in

Grantville, with all but two individuals trapped in gullies. We collared a total of 8 male

bobucks in the Strathbogie Ranges (SB) (average mass: 3.29 kg, range: 2.50 – 3.55

kg), and 5 male bobucks in Grantville (GB) (average mass: 2.86 kg, range: 2.65 – 3.10

kg). In the Strathbogie Ranges, we only trapped common brushtail possums in linear

strip environments, while common brushtail possums in Grantville were trapped in

forest fragments, gullies, and linear roadside strips. We collared a total of 6 male

common brushtail possums in the Strathbogie Ranges (SC) (average mass: 2.72 kg,

range: 2.60 – 2.85 kg). While we did trap 6 male common brushtail possums in

Grantville (average mass: 3.58 kg, range: 3.45 – 3.65 kg), only 3 were successfully

collared, therefore their movement data was not formally analysed. In total, nineteen

individuals were successfully collared and analysed across the eight consecutive

seasons, totalling 99 nights of behavioural data. Due to collar malfunctions, not all the

Page 112: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

93

individuals could be used to address each of the aims. The average time between GPS

fixes for individuals across the study was 14 minutes 17 seconds (range 4 minutes 09

seconds to 119 minutes 31 seconds). Data points with an error > 40 m were removed

(11.11% of data) resulting in an average Estimated Horizontal Position Error (EHPE)

of 10.71 m (range 2.88 to 39.68 m).

5.4.1 Nightly Distance Travelled

Eighteen individuals (GB = 5, SB = 7, SC = 6) were used to compare the nightly

distance travelled by each species. The nightly distance travelled by bobucks in the

Strathbogie Ranges was 45% further than common brushtail possums in the

Strathbogie Ranges. There was no significant difference in distance travelled between

bobucks in Grantville and bobucks in the Strathbogie Ranges, and bobucks in

Grantville and common brushtail possums in the Strathbogie Ranges (Figure 5.2 and

Table5.2 in Chapter 5 appendix).

Page 113: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

94

Figure 5.2: The average distance travelled (m) over 24 hours by bobucks

(Trichosurus cunninghami) and common brushtail possums (T. vulpecula) in their

respective locations: the Strathbogie Ranges (foothill forest) and Grantville

(heavily cleared coastal and lowland forest).

The coloured points indicate the raw data. The error bars indicate Standard Error.

Categories sharing a letter did not show significantly different relationships between

the coefficients.

Page 114: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

95

5.4.2 24-hour Activity

Sixteen individuals (GB = 4, SB = 7, SC = 5) were used to compare the activity of

individuals over a 24-hour period. The 24-hour activity of common brushtail possums

was approximately 25% less than bobucks in Grantville and bobucks in the Strathbogie

Ranges. There was no significant difference between the 24-hour activity of bobucks

in the Strathbogie Ranges and bobucks in Grantville (Figure 5.3 and Table 5.3 in

Chapter 5 appendix).

Page 115: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

96

Figure 5.3: The average VeDBA (g) measured over 24 hours for bobucks

(Trichosurus cunninghami) and common brushtail possums (T. vulpecula) in their

respective locations: the Strathbogie Ranges (foothill forest) and Grantville

(heavily cleared coastal and lowland forest).

The coloured points indicate the raw data. The error bars indicate Standard Error.

Categories sharing a letter did not show significantly different relationships between

the coefficients.

Page 116: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

97

5.4.3 Behavioural State Analysis

Sixteen individuals (GB = 4, SB = 7, SC = 5) were included in behavioural state

analysis. Four separate models were run to examine the time spent in different

behavioural states over a 24-hour period, corresponding to each of the four behavioural

states; Denning, Resting at Night, Moving & Foraging, and High Activity. Common

brushtail possums in the Strathbogie Ranges spent over 13% more time, or 1 hour and

45 minutes, denning than bobucks, while bobucks in Grantville spent 11% more time,

or approximately 35 minutes, ‘Resting at Night’ compared to both species in the

Strathbogie Ranges. Common brushtail possums in the Strathbogie Ranges spent 30%

less time, or 1 hour and 10 minutes, ‘Moving and Foraging’ than bobucks in the

Strathbogie Ranges, but no significant difference compared to bobucks in Grantville.

There was no difference between sites for bobucks ‘Moving and Foraging’ and in

‘High Activity’. Common brushtail possums in the Strathbogie Ranges spent 300%

less time, or 16 minutes, in ‘High Activity’ than bobucks in both locations (Figure 5.4

and Table 5.4 in Chapter 5 appendix).

Page 117: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

98

Figure 5.4: The average time spent by bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) and

common brushtail possums (T. vulpecula) in each behavioural state over a 24-

hour period (midnight to midnight GMT) in their respective locations: the

Strathbogie Ranges (foothill forest) and Grantville (heavily cleared coastal and

lowland forest).

The coloured points indicate the raw data. The error bars indicate Standard Error.

Categories sharing a letter did not show significantly different relationships between

the coefficients.

Page 118: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

99

5.4.4 Interval Comparison

Fourteen individuals (GB = 4, SB = 5, SC = 5) were included in interval comparison.

Model predictions were created by plotting the model fit for the three discrete predictor

variables; bobucks in Grantville, bobucks in the Strathbogie Ranges, and common

brushtail possums in the Strathbogie Ranges (Figure 5.5 and Table 5.5 in Chapter 5

appendix). These results showed: (1) that common brushtail possums in the

Strathbogie Ranges have a much lower level of activity compared with their distance

travelled; (2) that bobucks in Grantville have high activity levels for short distances

travelled; and (3) that bobucks in the Strathbogies had high activity levels when

travelling greater than 200 m in 10 minutes.

Page 119: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

100

Figure 5.5: The relationship between activity and the distance travelled per ten-

minute interval for bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) and common brushtail

possums (T. vulpecula) in in their respective locations: the Strathbogie Ranges

(foothill forest) and Grantville (heavily cleared coastal and lowland forest).

Lines and shaded area represent the fitted relationship and 95% confidence intervals

from generalised linear mixed models. Dashed broken line, pink shading = bobucks in

Grantville; dotted line, green shading = Bobucks in the Strathbogie Ranges; solid line,

blue shading = common brushtail possums in the Strathbogie Ranges. Categories

sharing a letter did not show significantly different relationships between the

coefficients.

Page 120: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

101

5.4.5 Energy Expenditure

Sixteen individuals (GB = 4, SB = 7, SC = 5) were included in Daily Energy

Expenditure analysis. The DEE of common brushtail possums in the Strathbogie

Ranges was 10 % (74.9 kJ) less than DEE of bobucks in Grantville and approximately

15% less (119.66 kJ) than bobucks in the Strathbogie Ranges. There was no significant

difference in DEE between bobucks in Grantville and the Strathbogie Ranges (Figure

5.6 and Table 5.6 in Chapter 5 appendix).

Page 121: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

102

Figure 5.6: The average Daily Energy Expenditure (DEE) by bobucks

(Trichosurus cunninghami) and common brushtail possums (T. vulpecula) in their

respective locations: the Strathbogie Ranges (foothill forest) and Grantville

(heavily cleared coastal and lowland forest).

The coloured points indicate the raw data. The error bars indicate Standard Error.

Categories sharing a letter did not show significantly different relationships between

the coefficients.

Page 122: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

103

5.5 Discussion

Animals can alter their behaviour in response to changing environmental conditions

(Gilliam & Fraser 1987, Fortin et al. 2005, Schuttler et al. 2015), and by so doing,

adapt to shifting resource availability and habitat modification (Badyaev 2005,

Tornroos et al. 2015). However, for most species we have insufficient information

about the impacts such changes have, including behavioural and physiological

consequences, and ultimately, species persistence. Here, we have described and

quantified the ways arboreal mammals respond to variation in resource composition

and distribution. Bobucks showed changes in their movement patterns between the

Strathbogies and Grantville, resting more and moving less distance in Grantville, but

this did not significantly alter their energy expenditure, despite the difference in habitat

composition and body mass of the individuals. Common brushtail possums employed

a different behavioural strategy from bobucks within the same environment, moving

less and resting more, ultimately resulting in lower energy expenditure.

5.5.1 Comparison of bobuck behaviour between habitat types

Bobucks exhibited different behavioural patterns across two spatially and structurally

distinct habitat types: foothill forest > 500 m a.s.l. (Strathbogies) and heavily cleared

coastal and lowland forest with an average elevation < 100 m a.s.l. (Grantville). When

habitat structure is considered (Martin & Martin 2007, Martin & Handasyde 2007,

Banks et al. 2008), it was not surprising bobucks in Grantville were found almost

exclusively in riparian zone gullies, where large, hollow-bearing trees remain, and the

cooler microclimate associated with permanent water provides more reliable access to

food resources (e.g. fungi and silver wattle) year round. The restricted distribution of

Page 123: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

104

this habitat raises questions regarding bobuck population genetics and their ongoing

survival in the region. Continued land clearing may reduce connectedness between

these already naturally patchily-distributed habitats, with the potential to isolate

bobuck populations and reduce genetic flow, ultimately leading to population collapse,

as seen for other species in similar situations (Bender et al. 1998). Genetic analysis of

the remaining bobuck populations would assist in understanding their likelihood of

survival, and what conservation actions might be required (e.g. habitat corridors and

stepping stones (Soanes et al. 2013)).

Bobucks in the Strathbogies exist in the core of their range (Martin 2006, Martin &

Handasyde 2007), and were found in both habitat types surveyed (habitat fragments

and linear strip environments). The behaviour of bobucks in the forest fragments is

believed to be more representative of behaviour in undisturbed habitat, with lower

abundance, pair bonding, and lower activity patterns ((Martin & Handasyde 2007), see

Chapter 3). Common brushtails were not found in forest fragments, so this study

focussed on bobucks in the more modified linear strip environments to allow for

comparison. Linear strips have been shown to alter bobuck behaviour; support higher

abundance and a polygynous mating system (Martin et al. 2007), and males have

increased movement and activity, ultimately resulting in an environment more

energetically costly to the individuals (see Chapter 3). A comparison of the movements

of bobucks in forest fragments with common brushtails in linear strips may identify

greater similarity between the two species, and more difference between bobucks in

the Strathbogies and bobucks in Grantville.

Bobucks in Grantville spent more time resting at night and travelled significantly

shorter distances on a nightly basis, than bobucks in the Strathbogies. Bobucks in

Page 124: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

105

Grantville also had a much lower body mass than bobucks in the Strathbogies, and

were in fact more similar in mass to the common brushtail possums in Strathbogies.

These behavioural patterns and differences in morphology may relate to the ways in

which animals obtain the resources required for their survival, encompassing both

habitat resources and energetic requirements (Shamoun-Baranes et al. 2012, Nams

2014). Bobucks in the Strathbogies must cope with much colder temperatures in

winter, so a larger body size and higher energy expenditure may be required for

homeostasis and thermoregulation (Speakman 2005). However, while mass and

behavioural patterns varied, bobucks in Grantville had similar daily energy

expenditure to bobucks in the Strathbogies. This suggests the overall energy

requirements of individual bobucks did not differ between these regions even though

bobucks in Grantville were almost 15% lighter, or, conversely, that their energy

expenditure relative to mass is higher in Grantville than the Strathbogies. Smaller

animals are known to work harder than larger animals to meet their energetic

requirements (Taylor et al. 1982), however, bobucks in Grantville would still be

expected to have a lower daily energy expenditure. High stress environments can cause

higher energy expenditure through behaviours such as increased vigilance or reduced

sleep (Careau et al. 2008, Tuomainen & Candolin 2011), which is worth further

examination in the more modified Grantville site.

Previous studies have described how species’ home ranges and resource requirements

shift between habitat types and locations (Angert et al. 2011, Clapp & Beck 2015,

Merkle et al. 2015). Measuring movement at a fine spatial (< 10 m) and temporal (<

1 s) scale allows for the quantification of changes in behaviour and energy use based

on habitat use, informing us about species-specific costs associated with habitat

Page 125: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

106

change. Our limited capture success of bobucks across the Grantville region suggests

there is limited suitable habitat for bobucks in the Grantville region, reducing their

movement and activity, and constraining them to a limited set of resources. This may

mean that bobucks living in Grantville are close to the limits of their niche space.

Understanding the limits of a species niche provides information regarding their ability

to respond to change, and persist in the face of future habitat modification. Bobucks in

Grantville would not be expected to persist if the vegetation in gullies was cleared.

5.5.2 Comparison of common brushtail possums across sites

The average mass of common brushtail possums in the Strathbogies was only

approximately 75% of those in Grantville. Common brushtail possums have an

extensive distribution, and their mass is known to vary greatly within it (1200 g to

4500 g (Kerle & Saunders 2001)). According to Bergmann’s Rule, temperature is often

thought to be a main driver of morphological plasticity in species’ mass, with larger

individuals in cooler climates (Ashton et al. 2000, Briscoe et al. 2015). This

phenomenon has been observed for common brushtail possums across large latitudinal

clines (Yom‐Tov et al. 1986). However, the Strathbogie Ranges is on average cooler

(average maximum temperature of 18.6⁰C, and an average minimum of 6.2⁰C) than

Grantville (average maximum temperature of 18.7⁰C and an average minimum of

11.7⁰C) (Bureau of Meteorology 2016), therefore, this contradicts the prediction of

Bergmann’s Rule. The number of individual common brushtail possums caught at each

site was similar in this study, but Grantville had far fewer bobucks. Common brushtail

possums may be out-competed by bobucks for resources in the Strathbogies, leading

to smaller body size (McNab 2010), but this may not be the case in Grantville where

bobuck abundance is lower and more spatially restricted. A comparison of the

Page 126: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

107

morphology and behaviour of common brushtail possums throughout their

distribution, relative to the abundance of other species, could examine competition and

its potential effect on body size in relation to environmental and climatic gradients.

5.5.3 Comparison of congener behaviour

Common brushtail possums in the Strathbogies were found in the same habitat as

bobucks, but displayed different behavioural patterns. Common brushtail possums

were physically smaller, only travelled approximately 70% of the distance of bobucks

each night, denned longer, spent less time foraging, and ultimately had less daily

energy expenditure than bobucks. Common brushtail possums were also only found in

linear strip environments within the Strathbogies, while bobucks are known to exist in

both remnant vegetation and linear strip environments (Chapter 3). Linear strip

environments are modified and disturbed, altering the availability and distribution of

resources (Andren 1994, Ewers & Didham 2006). In the case of the Strathbogie

Ranges, linear strip environments actually have a higher abundance of suitable tree

hollows (large, old remnant trees), and a greater density of food trees (e.g. silver

wattle), than forest fragments (Martin et al. 2007). Therefore, the small size of

common brushtails in Grantville may also be a reaction to the resources available

(Berumen et al. 2005, Pinaud et al. 2005), and only linear strip environments have

enough resources to support both common brushtail and bobuck possums.

The difference in movement, other behaviours, and energy expenditure between

common brushtail possums and bobucks may also potentially be an adaptation by

common brushtail possums to use a different environmental niche than the more

abundant bobucks, also known as ecological character displacement (Schoener 1965).

Page 127: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

108

Closely-related sympatric species often must develop different behavioural strategies

or change body size to minimise competition and co-exist in a habitat of limited

resources (Dayan & Simberloff 2005, Tobias et al. 2014). For example, competition

within a population of three-spine sticklebacks (Gasterosteus aculeatus) has been

shown to cause niche variation in diet (Svanback & Bolnick 2007), while different

mustelids in Great Britain have shown changes in skull size and tooth diameter in order

to specialise on different prey species (Dayan & Simberloff 1994). Both bobucks and

common brushtail possums use tree hollows as nest sites, so being physically smaller

may allow common brushtail possums to use smaller hollows not available to bobucks

(Saunders et al. 1982, Lindenmayer et al. 1991). This could be examined by installing

nest boxes with different entrance sizes and assessing their relative use by bobucks

and common brushtail possums in different habitats.

5.6 Conclusion

Environmental change, including habitat loss, modification, and climate change, are

affecting the behaviour, distribution and survival of species. To understand how such

processes will affect species both now and in the future, we need individual- and

population-level behavioural data within species’ core distributions and range limits.

This would allow an assessment of species’ responses and adaptability to change, as

well as determine which species are most at risk. Using a combination of

accelerometry and GPS, we can gauge the movement and energy requirements of

animals, compare their movements across different regions, and assess their ability to

adapt to change. This information could aid in the design and selection of reserves to

protect at-risk species, and identify key restoration projects.

Page 128: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

109

The ability to remotely measure movement data at a temporal scale capable of

capturing animal behaviour is still relatively new (Wilson et al. 2006, Green et al.

2009, Halsey & White 2010), and its uses have not been fully explored. Accelerometry

is providing the ability to record animal movement at a fraction of a second, in three-

dimensions, and irrespective of external influence. When coupled with GPS, we can

place the fine-scale movement of individuals within environmental contexts to better

understand the behaviour of animals in respect to habitat variation and modification.

This technology has the potential to identify the vertical use of environments, which

is lacking from most terrestrial analyses and hence our ecological understanding (but

see (Tracey et al. 2014)). For arboreal and scansorial animals, the vertical structure

and composition of the environment is just as important as resources in the horizontal

plane. Habitat modification can cause changes in the vertical structure of the habitat

(e.g. forestry can remove shelter such as hollows and decrease the availability of food

items (Root et al. 2003, Attum et al. 2006, Nepstad et al. 2008)). Furthermore, many

species rarely or reluctantly come to ground, and will only cover short distances when

they do so. By examining three-dimensional habitat use, we can improve our

understanding of how arboreal and scansorial animals use the environment, leading to

better-informed conservation of suitable habitat and management of reserves, and

greater chances of protecting the habitat elements important to these species in the face

of environmental change.

Page 129: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

110

Chapter 6: Synthesis

Anthropogenic habitat modification and climate change are major threats to

environments and biodiversity globally, contributing to population decline and species

extinctions (Thomas et al. 2004, Cahill et al. 2013). In order to tackle this conservation

challenge, and protect at-risk species into the future, we need reliable methods for

describing and quantifying resource use by animals. Animal behaviour governs how

species use and react to their environment, and therefore offers great potential for

understanding how species respond to environmental change. Examining animal

behaviour will allow predictions regarding how species might survive in the face of

novel and rapidly changing environments, and what management actions (e.g. habitat

protection or restoration) might assist them.

Emerging technology provides a means of recording and quantifying animal

movement, and thus a method of describing and understanding how animals use

environments. The movement recording technology I used was GPS and accelerometer

data loggers. GPS data loggers have been used to measure the position and movement

of numerous species (Obbard et al. 1998, Gautestad et al. 2013, Matthews et al. 2013),

while accelerometers are showing potential in measuring the behaviour and activity of

animals (Elliott et al. 2013, Wright et al. 2014, Jeanniard‐du‐Dot et al. 2016). Both

types of technology have different advantages and shortfalls (Chapter 3). GPS data

provides spatial information, but recording intervals and errors in fixes mean it cannot

reliably record fine-scale (< 3 m change in position) movement. Conversely,

accelerometers record fine-scale movement, but without location information. By

Page 130: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

111

using these technologies in combination, we can gather more comprehensive datasets

regarding animal movement and behaviour (Chapter 5).

While technology provides a means of recording animal movement, new methods of

data analysis are also required in order to understand and interpret the raw data. My

thesis investigated how emerging technology can be used in ecology, including the

means and methods of integrating different technologies to compare and quantify how

animals (common brushtail possums and bobucks) use and respond to changing

habitats across two geographically separate environments. In this chapter, I outline the

key ecological insights from my thesis, provide recommendations for further research,

and summarise applications of animal movement recording technology for examining

the effects of environmental change on species and their management.

6.1 Key insights

6.1.1 Technological development

The GPS and accelerometer devices used in my thesis were consumer-grade devices

which already existed in their most basic form; separate data logging devices.

However, these devices weren’t previously integrated, and were not designed for field-

based ecology. I was able to find versions of both devices small enough to fit to animals

weighing less than 2 kg, logged raw time-stamped data for over seven days, and could

be integrated to run simultaneously from a single battery. These devices were designed

to be re-charged and re-deployed as often as required, and all collars were used

numerous times throughout my study. Finally, by assembling the technology myself,

I had much greater flexibility in design features, and could better fit the devices to my

study species (possums).

Page 131: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

112

I found that GPS data loggers were able to provide broad scale spatial information

within the environment, while accelerometer data loggers recorded fine scale activity

and behavioural states. By combining the data, I was able to examine and quantify

animal behaviour at a finer spatial- and temporal-scale than achieved for most free-

ranging animals, and attain a greater understanding of how individuals interact with

their environment. I also identified a strong relationship between behavioural

information, and energy expenditure as measured by doubly labelled water.

Behavioural states were calculated every second, and allowed for different rates of

energy expenditure to be estimated for different movement types, unlike Doubly

Labelled Water, which only provides a single value for the study period (Qasem et al.

2012). Using accelerometry to estimate energy expenditure has great potential for

understanding the energetic cost different environments impose on animals through

effects on their behaviour, and how this varies between individuals and between

genders.

6.1.2 Effects of habitat modification on animal movement

Habitat modification in the Strathbogie Ranges appeared to influence the movement

of possum congeners, and the two species responded differently. Common brushtail

possums were exclusively found in the more modified linear strip environment. Linear

strips represented corridors linking the remnant vegetation, and would therefore be

expected to have high throughput of other arboreal species moving between patches,

leading to potentially higher rates of competition and confrontation (Beier & Noss

1998). The linear strip environment is also more exposed to predators and weather,

ultimately leading to habitat which is believed to be less favourable for many native

species (Forman & Alexander 1998, McKinney 2002). The occupation of linear strips

Page 132: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

113

by common brushtails may indicate their ability to adapt to changing environments,

similar to their success in living in urban environments (Statham & Statham 1997).

Notably, where sympatric, common brushtails were significantly lighter than bobucks

(Chapter 5), so the move to modified environments may also potentially be

competition related, pushing common brushtails into less desirable environments. A

study whereby bobucks could be excluded from certain areas which common

brushtails could potentially occupy could test this hypothesis. This could assess

whether common brushtails occupy forest fragments in the absence of competition

with bobucks, and whether brushtail body size differs in these different situations.

Bobucks were found in both the forest fragments and linear roadside vegetation, but

their movement patterns differed between habitat types. In the linear strip

environments, both males and females had increased activity levels for the distances

they travelled, and males were also travelling further distances each night, with many

more bursts of high activity (Chapter 3). This is despite the home ranges of bobucks

in linear strip environments being smaller, as more silver wattle and tree hollows, the

key resources for bobucks (Martin & Martin 2007, Martin & Handasyde 2007), are

available. Based on the link between activity and energy expenditure identified in

Chapter 4, the increased activity in linear strips suggests there is an additional energetic

cost to bobucks associated with living in this anthropogenically-modified habitat. This

could be due to influences such as competition, modified distribution of resources, or

stressors such as increased predator vigilance and noise (Careau et al. 2008,

Tuomainen & Candolin 2011). Field-based experiments examining energy

expenditure relative to potential stressors, competition, and resources could explore

this further.

Page 133: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

114

6.1.3 The influence of geographic location on morphology and behaviour

Bobucks and common brushtail possums were trapped in both the Strathbogie Ranges

and Grantville, but their abundance and capture locations varied within and between

environments. Bobucks were found throughout the Strathbogie Ranges, but were only

found in linear-strip environments in Grantville, and in much lower population

numbers (13 individuals in Grantville versus 101 in the Strathbogies). Meanwhile,

common brushtails in Grantville were caught in both forest fragments and linear strip

environments, and in similar numbers to the Strathbogies (20 versus 19 respectively).

There was also a difference in the average mass of individuals between the Strathbogie

Ranges and Grantville, with bobucks in Grantville approximately 15% lighter than

bobucks in the Strathbogies, while common brushtails in the Strathbogies were

approximately 25% lighter than common brushtails in Grantville. When examined

together, this data suggests a potential interaction between the two species that may

change in relation to body-size differences between the two sites. In the Strathbogies,

bobucks were over 20% heavier than common brushtails, and the more abundant and

widely distributed species, while in Grantville common brushtails were approximately

10% heavier and the more abundant and widely distributed species. An examination

of trends in possum body size and abundance relationships across a greater number of

sites could potentially shed more light on this observation, and the possible underlying

mechanisms.

The movement and behaviour of individuals between the sites also differed. Bobucks

in Grantville spent more time resting at night and travelled significantly shorter

distances on a nightly basis than bobucks in the Strathbogies. However, while the

behavioural patterns varied, bobucks in Grantville had similar daily energy

Page 134: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

115

expenditure to bobucks in the Strathbogies. This suggests that the overall energy

requirements of individual bobucks does not differ with region even though bobucks

in Grantville are almost 15% lighter, or, conversely, that their energy expenditure

relative to mass is higher in Grantville than the Strathbogies. Given the limited capture

success of bobucks across the Grantville region, and the higher levels of movement

recorded, it suggests that bobucks living in Grantville might be approaching their

physiological and environmental limits, reducing their movement and activity, and

constraining their resource use.

6.2 Knowledge gaps and further research

My research identified how combining emerging technology can be used to more

accurately identify and quantify animal behaviour than any one technology alone.

However, improvements to the technology used in this thesis, and its application,

remain. Initially, I had issues with battery life in the collars, with some devices only

recording for half the desired time. This was rectified early in my research by using

higher quality components, which increased the recording time and life-span of the

collars. Similarly, the construction of re-chargeable VHF transmitters could greatly

improve the collar design, increase their life-span, and hence ability to collect data and

information on animal behaviour.

Improvements could also be made regarding accelerometer mounting. For this

research, the accelerometer was mounted around the possum’s neck on a collar. This

meant there was potential for the orientation of the accelerometer to shift during its

deployment. While I was still able to use the accelerometry as a measure of activity, I

was not able to identify nuanced differences in behavioural state, such as moving

Page 135: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

116

vertically versus moving horizontally. A backpack harness would potentially improve

the accuracy of the accelerometry collected by restricting any shift in orientation in the

accelerometer. It would also be beneficial to compare the accelerometry data to visual

recordings of animal movement, in order to match data patterns to specific behaviours.

Mounting a small camera to animals could assist with this and would be superior to

direct observation by researchers, which might affect animal behaviour.

My research focussed on GPS and accelerometry technology in order to understand

animal behaviour. However, since the commencement of this research, there has been

a surge in cost-effective low-powered consumer technology, specifically around

Inertial Measurement Units, or IMUs. IMUs are small devices integrating

accelerometers, magnetometers, gyroscopes, and barometers to measure force, angular

rates, magnetic fields, etc. When coupled with a GPS device, IMUs have the ability to

record 3D movement, and in conjunction with 3D habitat maps, reconstruct real-time

models of animal habitat use in the field. These reconstructed movement models are

already being undertaken as a diagnostic and maintenance tool in technology such as

Unmanned Aerial Vehicles (UAVs), so the methodology to process the raw data

already exists (da Silva & da Cruz 2016, Wang et al. 2016).

6.3 Implications for management

Insights into animal movement can help improve species conservation and

management. Using emerging technology, we can quantify changes in the movement

and behaviour of individuals.

Page 136: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

117

In terms of bobucks, I identified that individuals in modified habitats have higher

activity than those in forest fragments, even though their home ranges are smaller than

individuals living in forest fragments. The higher levels of activity mean there is

ultimately a higher energetic cost to bobucks living in modified habitats. This

information provides a means of quantifying the impact different environments pose

on individuals and populations. This information could be used to understand the

possible implications of land clearing on other species, including those of conservation

concern.

By comparing between congeners, I was able to show how each species responded to

altered habitats, employed varied behavioural strategies, and exhibited morphological

plasticity. My findings suggest that responses to change can differ greatly both intra-

and inter-specifically in relation to environmental context. Therefore, it is fraught to

broadly generalise about species’ responses to environmental and habitat change, let

alone generalise about responses of closely-related species, and different populations

of the same species. Effective, well-informed species conservation and management

will therefore require ecological understanding of species across their distributions, in

order to predict the possible range of responses to environmental change.

Page 137: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

118

References

Allan, B. M., Arnould, J. P. Y., Martin, J. K., and Ritchie, E. G. 2013. A cost-effective and informative method of GPS tracking wildlife. Wildlife Research 40:345-348.

Almasi, B., Beziers, P., Roulin, A., and Jenni, L. 2015. Agricultural land use and human presence around breeding sites increase stress-hormone levels and decrease body mass in barn owl nestlings. Oecologia 179:89-101.

Andren, H. 1994. Effects of habitat fragmentation on birds and mammals in landscapes with different proportions of suitable habitat - a review. Oikos 71:355-366.

Angert, A. L., Crozier, L. G., Rissler, L. J., Gilman, S. E., Tewksbury, J. J., and Chunco, A. J. 2011. Do species' traits predict recent shifts at expanding range edges? Ecology Letters 14:677-689.

Anthony, R. M. 1997. Home ranges and movements of arctic fox (Alopex lagopus) in Western Alaska. Arctic 50:147-157.

Aresco, M. J. 2005. The effect of sex-specific terrestrial movements and roads on the sex ratio of freshwater turtles. Biological Conservation 123:37-44.

Arroyo-Rodriguez, V., and Dias, P. A. D. 2010. Effects of Habitat Fragmentation and Disturbance on Howler Monkeys: A Review. American Journal of Primatology 72:1-16.

Asari, Y., Johnson, C. N., Parsons, M., and Larson, J. 2010. Gap-crossing in fragmented habitats by mahogany gliders (Petaurus gracilis). Do they cross roads and powerline corridors? Australian Mammalogy 32:10-15.

Ashton, K. G., Tracy, M. C., and de Queiroz, A. 2000. Is Bergmann's rule valid for mammals? American Naturalist 156:390-415.

Attum, O., Eason, P., Cobbs, G., and El Din, S. M. B. 2006. Response of a desert lizard community to habitat degradation: Do ideas about habitat specialists/generalists hold? Biological Conservation 133:52-62.

Awkerman, J. A., Cruz, S., Proano, C., Huyvaert, K. P., Uzcategui, G. J., Baquero, A., Wikelski, M., and Anderson, D. J. 2014. Small range and distinct distribution in a satellite breeding colony of the critically endangered Waved Albatross. Journal of Ornithology 155:367-378.

Badyaev, A. V. 2005. Stress-induced variation in evolution: from behavioural plasticity to genetic assimilation. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 272:877-886.

Page 138: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

119

Banks, S. C., Knight, E. J., Dubach, J. E., and Lindenmayer, D. B. 2008. Microhabitat heterogeneity influences offspring sex allocation and spatial kin structure in possums. Journal of Animal Ecology 77:1250-1256.

Banks, S. C., Lindenmayer, D. B., McBurney, L., Blair, D., Knight, E. J., and Blyton, M. D. J. 2011. Kin selection in den sharing develops under limited availability of tree hollows for a forest marsupial. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 278:2768-2776.

Barnosky, A. D., Matzke, N., Tomiya, S., Wogan, G. O., Swartz, B., Quental, T. B., Marshall, C., McGuire, J. L., Lindsey, E. L., and Maguire, K. C. 2011. Has the Earth/'s sixth mass extinction already arrived? Nature 471:51-57.

Beck, C. A., Bowen, W. D., and Iverson, S. J. 2003. Sex differences in the seasonal patterns of energy storage and expenditure in a phocid seal. Journal of Animal Ecology 72:280-291.

Beier, P., and Noss, R. F. 1998. Do habitat corridors provide connectivity? Conservation Biology 12:1241-1252.

Belant, J. L. 2009. Effects of Antenna Orientation and Vegetation on Global Positioning System Telemetry Collar Performance. Northeastern Naturalist 16:577-584.

Bell, W. J. 2012. Searching behaviour: the behavioural ecology of finding resources. Springer Science & Business Media.

Bender, D. J., Contreras, T. A., and Fahrig, L. 1998. Habitat loss and population decline: a meta‐analysis of the patch size effect. Ecology 79:517-533.

Bennett, A. F., Lumsden, L. F., Alexander, J. S. A., Duncan, P. E., Johnson, P. G., Robertson, P., and Silveira, C. E. 1991. Habitat use by arboreal mammals along an environmental gradient in north-eastern Victoria. Wildlife Research 18:125-146.

Bennett, A. F., and Nagy, K. A. 1977. Energy-expenditure in free-ranging lizards. Ecology 58:697-700.

Berteaux, D., Thomas, D. W., Bergeron, J. M., and Lapierre, H. 1996. Repeatability of daily field metabolic rate in female Meadow Voles (Microtus pennsylvanicus). Functional Ecology 10:751-759.

Berumen, M. L., Pratchett, M. S., and McCormick, M. I. 2005. Within-reef differences in diet and body condition of coral-feeding butterflyfishes (Chaetodontidae). Marine Ecology Progress Series 287:217-227.

Bevan, R. M., Woakes, A. J., Butler, P. J., and Boyd, I. L. 1994. The use of heart-rate to estimate oxygen-consumption of free-ranging black-browed albatrosses diomedea melanophrys. Journal of Experimental Biology 193:119-137.

Page 139: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

120

Bidder, O. R., Soresina, M., Shepard, E. L., Halsey, L. G., Quintana, F., Gomez-Laich, A., and Wilson, R. P. 2012. The need for speed: testing acceleration for estimating animal travel rates in terrestrial dead-reckoning systems. Zoology 115:58-64.

Bonier, F., Martin, P. R., and Wingfield, J. C. 2007. Urban birds have broader environmental tolerance. Biology Letters 3:670-673.

Bonnot, N., Morellet, N., Verheyden, H., Cargnelutti, B., Lourtet, B., Klein, F., and Hewison, A. J. M. 2013. Habitat use under predation risk: hunting, roads and human dwellings influence the spatial behaviour of roe deer. European Journal of Wildlife Research 59:185-193.

Boonstra, R., and Craine, I. T. M. 1986. Natal nest location and small mammal tracking with a spool and line technique. Canadian Journal of Zoology-Revue Canadienne De Zoologie 64:1034-1036.

Bottrill, M. C., Joseph, L. N., Carwardine, J., Bode, M., Cook, C. N., Game, E. T., Grantham, H., Kark, S., Linke, S., McDonald-Madden, E., Pressey, R. L., Walker, S., Wilson, K. A., and Possingham, H. P. 2008. Is conservation triage just smart decision making? Trends in Ecology & Evolution 23:649-654.

Brawn, J. D., Robinson, S. K., and Thompson, F. R. 2001. The role of disturbance in the ecology and conservation of birds. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 32:251-276.

Brearley, G., McAlpine, C., Bell, S., and Bradley, A. 2012. Influence of urban edges on stress in an arboreal mammal: a case study of squirrel gliders in southeast Queensland, Australia. Landscape Ecology 27:1407-1419.

Brearley, G., Rhodes, J., Bradley, A., Baxter, G., Seabrook, L., Lunney, D., Liu, Y., and McAlpine, C. 2013. Wildlife disease prevalence in human-modified landscapes. Biological Reviews 88:427-442.

Briscoe, N. J., Krockenberger, A., Handasyde, K. A., and Kearney, M. R. 2015. Bergmann meets Scholander: geographical variation in body size and insulation in the koala is related to climate. Journal of Biogeography 42:791-802.

Brook, L. A., Johnson, C. N., and Ritchie, E. G. 2012. Effects of predator control on behaviour of an apex predator and indirect consequences for mesopredator suppression. Journal of Applied Ecology 49:1278-1286.

Bryant, D. M., Hails, C. J., and Prysjones, R. 1985. Energy-expenditure by free-living dippers (cinclus-cinclus) in winter. Condor 87:177-186.

Bureau of Meteorology. 2016. Australian daily temperature data.in B. o. M. Australian Government, editor.

Page 140: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

121

Butler, P. J., Green, J. A., Boyd, I. L., and Speakman, J. R. 2004. Measuring metabolic rate in the field: the pros and cons of the doubly labelled water and heart rate methods. Functional Ecology 18:168-183.

Cagnacci, F., Boitani, L., Powell, R. A., and Boyce, M. S. 2010. Animal ecology meets GPS-based radiotelemetry: a perfect storm of opportunities and challenges. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 365:2157-2162.

Cahill, A. E., Aiello-Lammens, M. E., Fisher-Reid, M. C., Hua, X., Karanewsky, C. J., Ryu, H. Y., Sbeglia, G. C., Spagnolo, F., Waldron, J. B., Warsi, O., and Wiens, J. J. 2013. How does climate change cause extinction? Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 280:9.

Careau, V., Réale, D., Garant, D., Pelletier, F., Speakman, J. R., and Humphries, M. M. 2013. Context-dependent correlation between resting metabolic rate and daily energy expenditure in wild chipmunks. Journal of Experimental Biology 216:418-426.

Careau, V., Thomas, D., Humphries, M. M., and Réale, D. 2008. Energy metabolism and animal personality. Oikos 117:641-653.

Cattarino, L., McAlpine, C. A., and Rhodes, J. R. 2015. Spatial scale and movement behaviour traits control the impacts of habitat fragmentation on individual fitness. Journal of Animal Ecology:n/a-n/a.

Cavagna, G. A., and Kaneko, M. 1977. Mechanical work and efficiency in level walking and running. Journal of Physiology-London 268:467-481.

Ceballos, G., Ehrlich, P. R., Barnosky, A. D., García, A., Pringle, R. M., and Palmer, T. M. 2015. Accelerated modern human–induced species losses: Entering the sixth mass extinction. Science Advances 1:e1400253.

Christiansen, F., Rasmussen, M. H., and Lusseau, D. 2013. Inferring activity budgets in wild animals to estimate the consequences of disturbances. Behavioral Ecology 24:1415-1425.

Clapp, J. G., and Beck, J. L. 2015. Evaluating distributional shifts in home range estimates. Ecology and Evolution 5:3869-3878.

Claridge, A. W. 1998. Use of tracks and trials by introduced predators: An important consideration in the study of native ground-dwelling mammals. Victorian Naturalist (Blackburn) 115:88-93.

Cooke, R., Wallis, R., Hogan, F., White, J., and Webster, A. 2006. The diet of powerful owls (Ninox strenua) and prey availability in a continuum of habitats from disturbed urban fringe to protected forest environments in south-eastern Australia. Wildlife Research 33:199-206.

Page 141: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

122

Corp, N., Gorman, M. L., and Speakman, J. R. 1999. Daily energy expenditure of free-living male Wood Mice in different habitats and seasons. Functional Ecology 13:585-593.

Creel, S., and Christianson, D. 2008. Relationships between direct predation and risk effects. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 23:194-201.

Cryan, P. M., and Wolf, B. O. 2003. Sex differences in the thermoregulation and evaporative water loss of a heterothermic bat, Lasiurus cinereus, during its spring migration. Journal of Experimental Biology 206:3381-3390.

Cumming, G. S., and Ndlovu, M. 2011. Satellite telemetry of Afrotropical ducks: methodological details and assessment of success rates. African Zoology 46:425-434.

Cunha, A. A., and Vieira, M. V. 2002. Support diameter, incline, and vertical movements of four didelphid marsupials in the Atlantic forest of Brazil. Journal of Zoology 258:419-426.

D'Amico, F., and Hemery, G. 2007. Time-activity budgets and energetics of Dipper Cinclus cinclus are dictated by temporal variability of river flow. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology a-Molecular & Integrative Physiology 148:811-820.

da Silva, A. L., and da Cruz, J. J. 2016. Fuzzy adaptive extended Kalman filter for UAV INS/GPS data fusion. Journal of the Brazilian Society of Mechanical Sciences and Engineering 38:1671-1688.

Dawson, T. J., and Hulbert, A. J. 1970. Standard metabolism, body temperature, and surface areas of australian marsupials. American Journal of Physiology 218:1233-&.

Dayan, T., and Simberloff, D. 1994. Character displacement, sexual dimprphism, and morphological variation among British and Irish mustelids. Ecology 75:1063-1073.

Dayan, T., and Simberloff, D. 2005. Ecological and community‐wide character displacement: the next generation. Ecology Letters 8:875-894.

Dean, J. 1980. Encounters between bombardier beetles and two species of toads (Bufo americanus, B. marinus): Speed of prey-capture does not determine success. Journal of Comparative Physiology 135:41-50.

Devictor, V., Julliard, R., and Jiguet, F. 2008. Distribution of specialist and generalist species along spatial gradients of habitat disturbance and fragmentation. Oikos 117:507-514.

Dorazio, R. M. 2012. Predicting the Geographic Distribution of a Species from Presence‐Only Data Subject to Detection Errors. Biometrics 68:1303-1312.

Page 142: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

123

Downes, S. J., Handasyde, K. A., and Elgar, M. A. 1997. The use of corridors by Mammals in fragmented Australian eucalypt forests. Conservation Biology 11:718-726.

Duckworth, J. W. 1998. The difficulty of estimating population densities of nocturnal forest mammals from transect counts of animals. Journal of Zoology 246:443-486.

Dujon, A. M., Lindstrom, R. T., and Hays, G. C. 2014. The accuracy of Fastloc-GPS locations and implications for animal tracking. Methods in Ecology and Evolution:n/a-n/a.

Elliott, K. H., Le Vaillant, M., Kato, A., Speakman, J. R., and Ropert-Coudert, Y. 2013. Accelerometry predicts daily energy expenditure in a bird with high activity levels. Biology Letters 9.

Estrada, A., Rivera, A., and Coates-Estrada, R. 2002. Predation of artificial nests in a fragmented landscape in the tropical region of Los Tuxtlas, Mexico. Biological Conservation 106:199-209.

Ewers, R. M., and Didham, R. K. 2006. Confounding factors in the detection of species responses to habitat fragmentation. Biological Reviews 81:117-142.

Eycott, A., Stewart, G., Buyung-Ali, L., Bowler, D., Watts, K., and Pullin, A. 2012. A meta-analysis on the impact of different matrix structures on species movement rates. Landscape Ecology 27:1263-1278.

Fahlman, A., Wilson, R., Svard, C., Rosen, D. A. S., and Trites, A. W. 2008. Activity and diving metabolism correlate in Steller sea lion Eumetopias jubatus. Aquatic Biology 2:75-84.

Fahrig, L. 2001. How much habitat is enough? Biological Conservation 100:65-74.

Fahrig, L. 2007. Non-optimal animal movement in human-altered landscapes. Functional Ecology 21:1003-1015.

Fancy, S. G., and White, R. G. 1985. Energy expenditures by caribou while cratering in snow. The Journal of wildlife management:987-993.

Feng, A. Y. T., and Himsworth, C. G. 2014. The secret life of the city rat: a review of the ecology of urban Norway and black rats (Rattus norvegicus and Rattus rattus). Urban Ecosystems 17:149-162.

Fischer, J., and Lindenmayer, D. B. 2007. Landscape modification and habitat fragmentation: a synthesis. Global Ecology and Biogeography 16:265-280.

Fletcher, R. J., Orrock, J. L., and Robertson, B. A. 2012. How the type of anthropogenic change alters the consequences of ecological traps. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 279:2546-2552.

Page 143: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

124

Foley, J. A., Ramankutty, N., Brauman, K. A., Cassidy, E. S., Gerber, J. S., Johnston, M., Mueller, N. D., O/'Connell, C., Ray, D. K., West, P. C., Balzer, C., Bennett, E. M., Carpenter, S. R., Hill, J., Monfreda, C., Polasky, S., Rockstrom, J., Sheehan, J., Siebert, S., Tilman, D., and Zaks, D. P. M. 2011. Solutions for a cultivated planet. Nature 478:337-342.

Fordyce, A., Hradsky, B. A., Ritchie, E. G., and Di Stefano, J. 2015. Fire affects microhabitat selection, movement patterns, and body condition of an Australian rodent (Rattus fuscipes). Journal of Mammalogy.

Forman, R. T., and Alexander, L. E. 1998. Roads and their major ecological effects. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics:207-C202.

Fortin, D., Beyer, H. L., Boyce, M. S., Smith, D. W., Duchesne, T., and Mao, J. S. 2005. Wolves influence elk movements: Behavior shapes a trophic cascade in Yellowstone National Park. Ecology 86:1320-1330.

Fossette, S., Schofield, G., Lilley, M. K. S., Gleiss, A. C., and Hays, G. C. 2012. Acceleration data reveal the energy management strategy of a marine ectotherm during reproduction. Functional Ecology 26:324-333.

Frair, J. L., Fieberg, J., Hebblewhite, M., Cagnacci, F., DeCesare, N. J., and Pedrotti, L. 2010. Resolving issues of imprecise and habitat-biased locations in ecological analyses using GPS telemetry data. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 365:2187-2200.

Frair, J. L., Merrill, E. H., Visscher, D. R., Fortin, D., Beyer, H. L., and Morales, J. M. 2005. Scales of movement by elk (Cervus elaphus) in response to heterogeneity in forage resources and predation risk. Landscape Ecology 20:273-287.

Franz, T. M., Demes, B., and Carlson, K. J. 2005. Gait mechanics of lemurid primates on terrestrial and arboreal substrates. Journal of Human Evolution 48:199-217.

Fretwell, S. D., and Lucas, H. L. J. 1969. On territorial behavior and other factors influencing habitat distribution in birds part 1 theoretical development. Acta Biotheoretica 19:16-36.

Fryxell, J. M., Hazell, M., Borger, L., Dalziel, B. D., Haydon, D. T., Morales, J. M., McIntosh, T., and Rosatte, R. C. 2008. Multiple movement modes by large herbivores at multiple spatiotemporal scales. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 105:19114-19119.

Fryxell, J. M., Wilmshurst, J. F., and Sinclair, A. R. E. 2004. Predictive models of movement by Serengeti grazers. Ecology 85:2429-2435.

Gales, N. J., and Mattlin, R. H. 1998. Fast, safe, field-portable gas anesthesia for otariids. Marine Mammal Science 14:355-361.

Page 144: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

125

Ganskopp, D. C., and Johnson, D. D. 2007. GPS error in studies addressing animal movements and activities. Rangeland Ecology & Management 60:350-358.

Gau, R. J., Mulders, R., Ciarniello, L. J., Heard, D. C., Chetkiewicz, C. L. B., Boyce, M., Munro, R., Stenhouse, G., Chruszcz, B., Gibeau, M. L., Milakovic, B., and Parker, K. L. 2004. Uncontrolled field performance of Televilt GPS-Simplex (TM) collars on grizzly bears in western and northern Canada. Wildlife Society Bulletin 32:693-701.

Gautestad, A. O., Loe, L. E., and Mysterud, A. 2013. Inferring spatial memory and spatiotemporal scaling from GPS data: comparing red deer Cervus elaphus movements with simulation models. Journal of Animal Ecology 82:572-586.

Gibbons, P., and Lindenmayer, D. 2002. Tree hollows and wildlife conservation in Australia. CSIRO publishing.

Gilliam, J. F., and Fraser, D. F. 1987. Habitat selection under predation hazard - test of a model with foraging minnows. Ecology 68:1856-1862.

Gillooly, J. F., Brown, J. H., West, G. B., Savage, V. M., and Charnov, E. L. 2001. Effects of size and temperature on metabolic rate. Science 293:2248-2251.

Gleiss, A. C., Wilson, R. P., and Shepard, E. L. C. 2011. Making overall dynamic body acceleration work: on the theory of acceleration as a proxy for energy expenditure. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 2:23-33.

Gloury, A. M., and Handasyde, K. A. 2016. Comparative dietary ecology of two congeneric marsupial folivores. Austral Ecology 41:355-366.

Green, J. A., Halsey, L. G., Wilson, R. P., and Frappell, P. B. 2009. Estimating energy expenditure of animals using the accelerometry technique: activity, inactivity and comparison with the heart-rate technique. Journal of Experimental Biology 212:471-482.

Grunewalder, S., Broekhuis, F., Macdonald, D. W., Wilson, A. M., McNutt, J. W., Shawe-Taylor, J., and Hailes, S. 2012. Movement Activity Based Classification of Animal Behaviour with an Application to Data from Cheetah (Acinonyx jubatus). Plos One 7.

Haan, D. M., and Halbrook, R. S. 2015. Home Ranges and Movement Characteristics of Minks in East-central New York. American Midland Naturalist 174:302-309.

Haby, N. A., Delean, S., and Brook, B. W. 2012. Specialist resources are key to improving small mammal distribution models. Austral Ecology 37:216-226.

Haddad, N. M., Brudvig, L. A., Clobert, J., Davies, K. F., Gonzalez, A., Holt, R. D., Lovejoy, T. E., Sexton, J. O., Austin, M. P., and Collins, C. D. 2015. Habitat

Page 145: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

126

fragmentation and its lasting impact on Earth’s ecosystems. Science Advances 1:e1500052.

Halsey, L. G., Green, J. A., Wilson, R. P., and Frappell, P. B. 2009a. Accelerometry to Estimate Energy Expenditure during Activity: Best Practice with Data Loggers. Physiological and Biochemical Zoology 82:396-404.

Halsey, L. G., Shepard, E., Quintana, F., Gomez Laich, A., Green, J., and Wilson, R. 2009b. The relationship between oxygen consumption and body acceleration in a range of species. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology Part A: Molecular & Integrative Physiology 152:197-202.

Halsey, L. G., Shepard, E. L. C., Hulston, C. J., Venables, M. C., White, C. R., Jeukendrup, A. E., and Wilson, R. P. 2008. Acceleration versus heart rate for estimating energy expenditure and speed during locomotion in animals: Tests with an easy model species, Homo sapiens. Zoology 111:231-241.

Halsey, L. G., Shepard, E. L. C., and Wilson, R. P. 2011. Assessing the development and application of the accelerometry technique for estimating energy expenditure. Comparative Biochemistry and Physiology a-Molecular & Integrative Physiology 158:305-314.

Halsey, L. G., and White, C. R. 2010. Measuring Energetics and Behaviour Using Accelerometry in Cane Toads Bufo marinus. Plos One 5.

Harper, M. J., McCarthy, M. A., and van der Ree, R. 2005. The abundance of hollow-bearing trees in urban dry sclerophyll forest and the effect of wind on hollow development. Biological Conservation 122:181-192.

Harris, P. M., Dellow, D. W., and Broadhurst, R. B. 1985. Protein and energy-requirements and deposition in the growing brushtail possum and rex rabbit. Australian Journal of Zoology 33:425-436.

Harris, S., Cresswell, W. J., Forde, P. G., Trewhella, W. J., Woollard, T., and Wray, S. 1990. Home-range analysis using radio-tracking data - a review of problems and techniques particularly as applied to the study of mammals. Mammal Review 20:97-123.

Harvell, C. D., Mitchell, C. E., Ward, J. R., Altizer, S., Dobson, A. P., Ostfeld, R. S., and Samuel, M. D. 2002. Ecology - Climate warming and disease risks for terrestrial and marine biota. Science 296:2158-2162.

Hebblewhite, M., and Haydon, D. T. 2010. Distinguishing technology from biology: a critical review of the use of GPS telemetry data in ecology. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 365:2303-2312.

Heurich, M., Traube, M., Stache, A., and Lottker, P. 2012. Calibration of remotely collected acceleration data with behavioral observations of roe deer (Capreolus capreolus L.). Acta Theriologica 57:251-255.

Page 146: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

127

Hirzel, A. H., and Le Lay, G. 2008. Habitat suitability modelling and niche theory. Journal of Applied Ecology 45:1372-1381.

Hoffmann, W. A., Geiger, E. L., Gotsch, S. G., Rossatto, D. R., Silva, L. C., Lau, O. L., Haridasan, M., and Franco, A. C. 2012. Ecological thresholds at the savanna‐forest boundary: how plant traits, resources and fire govern the distribution of tropical biomes. Ecology Letters 15:759-768.

Hopkins, M. E. 2011. Mantled Howler (Alouatta palliata) Arboreal Pathway Networks: Relative Impacts of Resource Availability and Forest Structure. International Journal of Primatology 32:238-258.

How, R. A. 1981. Population parameters of 2 congeneric possums, trichosurus spp, in northeastern New South Wales. Australian Journal of Zoology 29:205-215.

Hughes, J. B., Daily, G. C., and Ehrlich, P. R. 2000. Conservation of insect diversity: a habitat approach. Conservation Biology 14:1788-1797.

Hulbert, I. A., Wyllie, J. T., Waterhouse, A., French, J., and McNulty, D. 1998. A note on the circadian rhythm and feeding behaviour of sheep fitted with a lightweight GPS collar. Applied Animal Behaviour Science 60:359-364.

Humphries, M. M., Thomas, D. W., and Speakman, J. R. 2002. Climate-mediated energetic constraints on the distribution of hibernating mammals. Nature 418:313-316.

Hunter, A. J. S., El-Sheimy, N., Stenhouse, G. B., and Wright, D. B. 2007. Animal tracking system. Google Patents.

Hynes, D., and Cleeland, M. 2005. Presence of Bobucks Trichosurus caninus in The Gurdies on Westernport, Victoria. The Victorian Naturalist 122:141-145.

Ims, R. A. 1987. Responses in spatial-organization and behavior to manipulations of the food resource in the vole Clethrionomys rufocanus. Journal of Animal Ecology 56:585-596.

IUCN. 2016. The IUCN Red List of Threatened Species. Version 2016-2.

Jeanniard‐du‐Dot, T., Guinet, C., Arnould, J. P., Speakman, J. R., and Trites, A. W. 2016. Accelerometers can measure total and activity‐specific energy expenditures in free‐ranging marine mammals only if linked to time‐activity budgets. Functional Ecology.

Jedrzejewski, W., Schmidt, K., Theuerkauf, J., Jedrzejewska, B., and Okarma, H. 2001. Daily movements and territory use by radio-collared wolves (Canis lupus) in Bialowieza Primeval Forest in Poland. Canadian Journal of Zoology-Revue Canadienne De Zoologie 79:1993-2004.

Page 147: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

128

Jeffree, E. P., and Jeffree, C. E. 1994. Temperature and the biogeographical distributions of species. Functional Ecology 8:640-650.

Jennions, M. D., and Møller, A. P. 2003. A survey of the statistical power of research in behavioral ecology and animal behavior. Behavioral Ecology 14:438-445.

Jiguet, F., Gadot, A. S., Julliard, R., Newson, S. E., and Couvet, D. 2007. Climate envelope, life history traits and the resilience of birds facing global change. Global Change Biology 13:1672-1684.

Johnson, C. J., Parker, K. L., Heard, D. C., and Gillingham, M. P. 2002. A multiscale behavioral approach to understanding the movements of woodland caribou. Ecological Applications 12:1840-1860.

Johnson, D. H. 1980. The comparison of usage and availability measurements for evaluating resource preference. Ecology 61:65-71.

Johnson, D. S. 2012. crawl: Fit continuous-time correlated random walk models to animal movement data. R package version 1.3-4. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=crawl.

Johnson, D. S., London, J. M., Lea, M. A., and Durban, J. W. 2008. Continuous-time correlated random walk model for animal telemetry data. Ecology 89:1208-1215.

Johnstone, C. P., Lill, A., and Reina, R. D. 2012. Does habitat fragmentation cause stress in the agile antechinus? A haematological approach. Journal of Comparative Physiology B-Biochemical Systemic and Environmental Physiology 182:139-155.

Kearney, M., and Porter, W. 2009. Mechanistic niche modelling: combining physiological and spatial data to predict species' ranges. Ecology Letters 12:334-350.

Keller, M., Schimel, D. S., Hargrove, W. W., and Hoffman, F. M. 2008. A continental strategy for the National Ecological Observatory Network. Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 6:282-284.

Kerle, J. A., and Saunders, V. 2001. Possums : the brushtails, ringtails and greater glider. UNSW Press, Sydney.

Kirkwood, T. B. L., and Holliday, R. 1979. Evolution of aging and longevity. Proceedings of the Royal Society Series B-Biological Sciences 205:531-546.

Kremen, C., Colwell, R. K., Erwin, T. L., Murphy, D. D., Noss, R. F., and Sanjayan, M. A. 1993. Terrestrial arthropod assemblages - Their use in conservation planning. Conservation Biology 7:796-808.

Page 148: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

129

Krol, E., and Speakman, J. R. 1999. Isotope dilution spaces of mice injected simultaneously with deuterium, tritium and oxygen-18. Journal of Experimental Biology 202:2839-2849.

Krop-Benesch, A., Berger, A., Hofer, H., and Heurich, M. 2013. Long-term measurement of roe deer (Capreolus capreolus) (Mammalia: Cervidae) activity using two-axis accelerometers in GPS-collars. Italian Journal of Zoology 80:69-81.

Lachica, M., and Aguilera, J. 2005. Energy expenditure of walk in grassland for small ruminants. Small Ruminant Research 59:105-121.

Laich, A. G., Wilson, R. P., Gleiss, A. C., Shepard, E. L. C., and Quintana, F. 2011. Use of overall dynamic body acceleration for estimating energy expenditure in cormorants Does locomotion in different media affect relationships? Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 399:151-155.

Laurance, W. F., Laurance, S. G., and Hilbert, D. W. 2008. Long-Term Dynamics of a Fragmented Rainforest Mammal Assemblage. Conservation Biology 22:1154-1164.

Lawton, J. H. 1993. Range, population abundance and conservation. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 8:409-413.

Lenth, R. V. 2016. Least-squares means: the R Package lsmeans. J Stat Softw 69:1-33.

Lifson, N., and McClintock. 1966. Theory of use of turnover rates of body water for measuring energy and material balance. Journal of Theoretical Biology 12:46-&.

Lindenmayer, D. B., Cunningham, R. B., and Donnelly, C. F. 1997. Decay and collapse of trees with hollows in eastern Australian forests: Impacts on arboreal marsupials. Ecological Applications 7:625-641.

Lindenmayer, D. B., Cunningham, R. B., Tanton, M. T., Smith, A. P., and Nix, H. A. 1990. Habitat requirements of the mountain brushtail possum and the greater glider in the montane ash-type eucalypt forests of the Central Highlands of Victoria. Australian Wildlife Research 17:467-478.

Lindenmayer, D. B., Cunningham, R. B., Tanton, M. T., Smith, A. P., and Nix, H. A. 1991. Characteristics of hollow-bearing trees occupied by arboreal marsupials in the montane ash forests of the central highlands of victoria, south-east Australia. Forest Ecology and Management 40:289-308.

Lindenmayer, D. B., Dubach, J., and Viggers, K. L. 2002. Geographic dimorphism in the mountain brushtail possum (Trichosurus caninus): the case for a new species. Australian Journal of Zoology 50:369-393.

Page 149: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

130

Louys, J., Meloro, C., Elton, S., Ditchfield, P., and Bishop, L. C. 2011. Mammal community structure correlates with arboreal heterogeneity in faunally and geographically diverse habitats: implications for community convergence. Global Ecology and Biogeography 20:717-729.

Lowry, H., Lill, A., and Wong, B. B. M. 2013. Behavioural responses of wildlife to urban environments. Biological Reviews 88:537-549.

Loxdale, H. D., Lushai, G., and Harvey, J. A. 2011. The evolutionary improbability of 'generalism' in nature, with special reference to insects. Biological Journal of the Linnean Society 103:1-18.

Mårell, A., Ball, J. P., and Hofgaard, A. 2002. Foraging and movement paths of female reindeer: insights from fractal analysis, correlated random walks, and Lévy flights. Canadian Journal of Zoology 80:854-865.

Maroli, M., Vadell, M. V., Iglesias, A., Padula, P. J., and Villafane, I. E. G. 2015. Daily Movements and Microhabitat Selection of Hantavirus Reservoirs and Other Sigmodontinae Rodent Species that Inhabit a Protected Natural Area of Argentina. Ecohealth 12:421-431.

Marteinson, S. C., Giroux, J. F., Helie, J. F., Gentes, M. L., and Verreault, J. 2015. Field Metabolic Rate Is Dependent on Time-Activity Budget in Ring-Billed Gulls (Larus delawarensis) Breeding in an Anthropogenic Environment. Plos One 10:17.

Martin, J., and Martin, A. 2007. Resource distribution influences mating system in the bobuck (Trichosurus cunninghami: Marsupialia). Oecologia 154:227-236.

Martin, J. K. 2006. Den-use and home-range characteristics of bobucks, Trichosurus cunninghami, resident in a forest patch. Australian Journal of Zoology 54:225-234.

Martin, J. K., and Handasyde, K. A. 2007. Comparison of bobuck (Trichosurus cunninghami) demography in two habitat types in the Strathbogie Ranges, Australia. Journal of Zoology 271:375-385.

Martin, J. K., Handasyde, K. A., and Taylor, A. C. 2007. Linear roadside remnants: Their influence on den-use, home range and mating system in bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami). Austral Ecology 32:686-696.

Matthews, A., Ruykys, L., Ellis, B., FitzGibbon, S., Lunney, D., Crowther, M. S., Glen, A. S., Purcell, B., Moseby, K., Stott, J., Fletcher, D., Wimpenny, C., Allen, B. L., Van Bommel, L., Roberts, M., Davies, N., Green, K., Newsome, T., Ballard, G., Fleming, P., Dickman, C. R., Eberhart, A., Troy, S., McMahon, C., and Wiggins, N. 2013. The success of GPS collar deployments on mammals in Australia. Australian Mammalogy 35:65-83.

Page 150: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

131

Matthews, T. J., and Whittaker, R. J. 2015. Review: On the species abundance distribution in applied ecology and biodiversity management. Journal of Applied Ecology 52:443-454.

McCall, S. C., McCarthy, M. A., van der Ree, R., Harper, M. J., Cesarini, S., and Soanes, K. 2010. Evidence that a Highway Reduces Apparent Survival Rates of Squirrel Gliders. Ecology and Society 15.

McDonald-Madden, E., Baxter, P. W. J., and Possingham, H. P. 2008. Making robust decisions for conservation with restricted money and knowledge. Journal of Applied Ecology 45:1630-1638.

McDonald-Madden, E., Elgar, M. A., and Handasyde, K. A. 2004. Responses to threat by female bobucks, Trichosurus caninus, during different stages of offspring development. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 56:322-327.

McKinney, M. L. 2002. Urbanization, biodiversity, and conservation the impacts of urbanization on native species are poorly studied, but educating a highly urbanized human population about these impacts can greatly improve species conservation in all ecosystems. Bioscience 52:883-890.

McLean, K. A., Trainor, A. M., Asner, G. P., Crofoot, M. C., Hopkins, M. E., Campbell, C. J., Martin, R. E., Knapp, D. E., and Jansen, P. A. 2016. Movement patterns of three arboreal primates in a Neotropical moist forest explained by LiDAR-estimated canopy structure. Landscape Ecology 31:1849-1862.

McNab, B. K. 1995. Energy-expenditure and conservation in frugivorous and mixed-diet carnivorans. Journal of Mammalogy 76:206-222.

McNab, B. K. 2010. Geographic and temporal correlations of mammalian size reconsidered: a resource rule. Oecologia 164:13-23.

Mendonca, A. F., Armond, T., Camargo, A. C. L., Camargo, N. F., Ribeiro, J. F., Zangrandi, P. L., and Vieira, E. M. 2015. Effects of an extensive fire on arboreal small mammal populations in a neotropical savanna woodland. Journal of Mammalogy 96:368-379.

Merkle, J. A., Cherry, S. G., and Fortin, D. 2015. Bison distribution under conflicting foraging strategies: site fidelity vs. energy maximization. Ecology 96:1793-1801.

Miwa, M., Oishi, K., Nakagawa, Y., Maeno, H., Anzai, H., Kumagai, H., Okano, K., Tobioka, H., and Hirooka, H. 2015. Application of Overall Dynamic Body Acceleration as a Proxy for Estimating the Energy Expenditure of Grazing Farm Animals: Relationship with Heart Rate. Plos One 10:e0128042.

Morris, D. W. 1996. Coexistence of specialist and generalist rodents via habitat selection. Ecology 77:2352-2364.

Page 151: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

132

Munn, A. J., Dawson, T. J., McLeod, S. R., Dennis, T., and Maloney, S. K. 2013. Energy, water and space use by free-living red kangaroos Macropus rufus and domestic sheep Ovis aries in an Australian rangeland. Journal of Comparative Physiology B-Biochemical Systemic and Environmental Physiology 183:843-858.

Nagy, K. A. 1987. Field metabolic-rate and food requirement scaling in mammals and birds. Ecological Monographs 57:111-128.

Nagy, K. A. 1989. Field bioenergetics - accuracy of models and methods. Physiological Zoology 62:237-252.

Nagy, K. A. 2005. Field metabolic rate and body size. Journal of Experimental Biology 208:1621-1625.

Nagy, K. A., Bradley, A. J., and Morris, K. D. 1990. Field metabolic rates, water fluxes, and feeding rates of quokkas, setonix-brachyurus, and tammars, macropus-eugenii, in western-australia. Australian Journal of Zoology 37:553-560.

Nagy, K. A., and Bradshaw, S. D. 2000. Scaling of energy and water fluxes in free-living arid-zone Australian marsupials. Journal of Mammalogy 81:962-970.

Nagy, K. A., Girard, I. A., and Brown, T. K. 1999. Energetics of free-ranging mammals, reptiles, and birds. Annual Review of Nutrition 19:247-277.

Naito, Y., Bornemann, H., Takahashi, A., McIntyre, T., and Plötz, J. 2010. Fine-scale feeding behavior of Weddell seals revealed by a mandible accelerometer. Polar Science 4:309-316.

Nakagawa, S., and Cuthill, I. C. 2007. Effect size, confidence interval and statistical significance: a practical guide for biologists. Biological Reviews 82:591-605.

Nams, V. O. 2014. Combining animal movements and behavioural data to detect behavioural states. Ecology Letters.

Nathan, R., Getz, W. M., Revilla, E., Holyoak, M., Kadmon, R., Saltz, D., and Smouse, P. E. 2008. A movement ecology paradigm for unifying organismal movement research. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 105:19052-19059.

Nepstad, D. C., Stickler, C. M., Soares, B., and Merry, F. 2008. Interactions among Amazon land use, forests and climate: prospects for a near-term forest tipping point. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 363:1737-1746.

Obbard, M. E., Pond, B. A., and Perera, A. 1998. Preliminary evaluation of GPS collars for analysis of habitat use and activity patterns of black bears. International Association Bearresearch & Management, West Glacier.

Page 152: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

133

Ochoa-Ochoa, L. M., Bezaury-Creel, J. E., Vazquez, L. B., and Flores-Villela, O. 2011. Choosing the survivors? A GIS-based triage support tool for micro-endemics: Application to data for Mexican amphibians. Biological Conservation 144:2710-2718.

Papailiou, A., Sullivan, E., and Cameron, J. L. 2008. Behaviors in rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) associated with activity counts measured by accelerometer. American Journal of Primatology 70:185-190.

Patterson, T. A., Thomas, L., Wilcox, C., Ovaskainen, O., and Matthiopoulos, J. 2008. State-space models of individual animal movement. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 23:87-94.

Pausas, J. G., Braithwaite, L. W., and Austin, M. P. 1995. Modeling habitat quality for arboreal marsupials in the south coastal forests of New South Wales, Australia. Forest Ecology and Management 78:39-49.

Pereira, M., and Rodriguez, A. 2010. Conservation value of linear woody remnants for two forest carnivores in a Mediterranean agricultural landscape. Journal of Applied Ecology 47:611-620.

Pike, D. A., Webb, J. K., and Shine, R. 2011. Removing forest canopy cover restores a reptile assemblage. Ecological Applications 21:274-280.

Pinaud, D., Cherel, Y., and Weimerskirch, H. 2005. Effect of environmental variability on habitat selection, diet, provisioning behaviour and chick growth in yellow-nosed albatrosses. Marine Ecology Progress Series 298:295-304.

Poisot, T., Canard, E., Mouquet, N., and Hochberg, M. E. 2012. A comparative study of ecological specialization estimators. Methods in Ecology and Evolution:no-no.

Ponchon, A., Gremillet, D., Doligez, B., Chambert, T., Tveraa, T., Gonzalez-Solis, J., and Boulinier, T. 2013. Tracking prospecting movements involved in breeding habitat selection: insights, pitfalls and perspectives. Methods in Ecology and Evolution 4:143-150.

Potter, S., Eldridge, M. D. B., Cooper, S. J. B., Paplinska, J. Z., and Taggart, D. A. 2012. Habitat connectivity, more than species' biology, influences genetic differentiation in a habitat specialist, the short-eared rock-wallaby (Petrogale brachyotis). Conservation Genetics 13:937-952.

Pounds, J. A., Fogden, M. P. L., and Campbell, J. H. 1999. Biological response to climate change on a tropical mountain. Nature 398:611-615.

Qasem, L., Cardew, A., Wilson, A., Griffiths, I., Halsey, L. G., Shepard, E. L. C., Gleiss, A. C., and Wilson, R. 2012. Tri-Axial Dynamic Acceleration as a Proxy for Animal Energy Expenditure; Should We Be Summing Values or Calculating the Vector? Plos One 7.

Page 153: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

134

R Core Development Team. 2014. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria.

Radford, J. Q., Bennett, A. F., and Cheers, G. J. 2005. Landscape-level thresholds of habitat cover for woodland-dependent birds. Biological Conservation 124:317-337.

Ramsey, J. J., Colman, R. J., Binkley, N. C., Christensen, J. D., Gresl, T. A., Kemnitz, J. W., and Weindruch, R. 2000. Dietary restriction and aging in rhesus monkeys: the University of Wisconsin study. Experimental Gerontology 35:1131-1149.

Rhodes, J. R., McAlpine, C. A., Lunney, D., and Possingham, H. P. 2005. A spatially explicit habitat selection model incorporating home range behavior. Ecology 86:1199-1205.

Ricklefs, R. E., and Wikelski, M. 2002. The physiology/life-history nexus. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 17:462-468.

Ritchie, E. G., and Johnson, C. N. 2009. Predator interactions, mesopredator release and biodiversity conservation. Ecology Letters 12:982-998.

Ritchie, E. G., Martin, J. K., Krockenberger, A. K., Garnett, S., and Johnson, C. N. 2008. Large-herbivore distribution and abundance: Intra-and interspecific niche variation in the tropics. Ecological Monographs 78:105-122.

Rodgers, A. R., Rempel, R. S., and Abraham, K. F. 1996. A GPS-based telemetry system. Wildlife Society Bulletin:559-566.

Root, T. L., Price, J. T., Hall, K. R., Schneider, S. H., Rosenzweig, C., and Pounds, J. A. 2003. Fingerprints of global warming on wild animals and plants. Nature 421:57-60.

Rose, B. 1981. Factors affecting activity in sceloporus-virgatus. Ecology 62:706-716.

Rosenblatt, D. L., Heske, E. J., Nelson, S. L., Barber, D. H., Miller, M. A., and MacAllister, B. 1999. Forest fragments in east-central Illinois: Islands or habitat patches for mammals? American Midland Naturalist 141:115-123.

Rothermel, B. B., and Semlitsch, R. D. 2002. An experimental investigation of landscape resistance of forest versus old-field habitats to emigrating juvenile amphibians. Conservation Biology 16:1324-1332.

Rothwell, E. S., Bercovitch, F. B., Andrews, J. R. M., and Anderson, M. J. 2011. Estimating Daily Walking Distance of Captive African Elephants Using an Accelerometer. Zoo Biology 30:579-591.

Rutz, C., and Hays, G. C. 2009. New frontiers in biologging science. Biology Letters 5:289-292.

Page 154: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

135

Ruykys, L., Ward, M. J., Taggart, D. A., and Breed, W. G. 2011. Preliminary spatial behaviour of warru (Petrogale lateralis MacDonnell Ranges race) in the Anangu Pitjantjatjara Yankunytjatjara Lands, South Australia. Australian Mammalogy 33:181-188.

Ryan, M. A., Whisson, D. A., Holland, G. J., and Arnould, J. P. Y. 2013. Activity Patterns of Free-Ranging Koalas (Phascolarctos cinereus) Revealed by Accelerometry. Plos One 8:10.

Sabino-Marques, H., and Mira, A. 2011. Living on the verge: are roads a more suitable refuge for small mammals than streams in Mediterranean pastureland? Ecological Research 26:277-287.

Sakamoto, K. Q., Sato, K., Ishizuka, M., Watanuki, Y., Takahashi, A., Daunt, F., and Wanless, S. 2009. Can Ethograms Be Automatically Generated Using Body Acceleration Data from Free-Ranging Birds? Plos One 4:12.

Saunders, D., Smith, G. T., and Rowley, I. 1982. The availability and dimensions of tree hollows that provide nest sites for cockatoos (Psittaciformes) in Western Australia. Wildlife Research 9:541-556.

Schell, C. J., Young, J. K., Lonsdorf, E. V., and Santymire, R. M. 2013. Anthropogenic and physiologically induced stress responses in captive coyotes. Journal of Mammalogy 94:1131-1140.

Schmidt, K., Nakanishi, N., Okamura, M., Doi, T., and Izawa, M. 2003. Movements and use of home range in the Iriomote cat (Prionailurus bengalensis iriomotensis). Journal of Zoology 261:273-283.

Schoener, T. W. 1965. The evolution of bill size differences among sympatric congeneric species of birds. Evolution 19:189-213.

Schuttler, S. G., Ruiz-Lopez, M. J., Monello, R., Wehtje, M., Eggert, L. S., and Gompper, M. E. 2015. The interplay between clumped resources, social aggregation, and genetic relatedness in the raccoon. Mammal Research 60:365-373.

Selwood, K., Mac Nally, R., and Thomson, J. R. 2009. Native bird breeding in a chronosequence of revegetated sites. Oecologia 159:435-446.

Shamoun-Baranes, J., Bom, R., van Loon, E. E., Ens, B. J., Oosterbeek, K., and Bouten, W. 2012. From Sensor Data to Animal Behaviour: An Oystercatcher Example. Plos One 7.

Shepard, E. L. C., Wilson, R. P., Quintana, F., Laich, A. G., and Forman, D. W. 2009. Pushed for time or saving on fuel: fine-scale energy budgets shed light on currencies in a diving bird. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 276:3149-3155.

Page 155: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

136

Shepard, E. L. C., Wilson, R. P., Quintana, F., Laich, A. G., Liebsch, N., Albareda, D. A., Halsey, L. G., Gleiss, A., Morgan, D. T., Myers, A. E., Newman, C., and Macdonald, D. W. 2008. Identification of animal movement patterns using tri-axial accelerometry. Endangered Species Research 10:47-60.

Shipley, L. A., Forbey, J. S., and Moore, B. D. 2009. Revisiting the dietary niche: When is a mammalian herbivore a specialist ? Integrative and Comparative Biology 49:274-290.

Sih, A., Ferrari, M. C. O., and Harris, D. J. 2011. Evolution and behavioural responses to human-induced rapid environmental change. Evolutionary Applications 4:367-387.

Smith, A., Hume, I. D., and Australian Mammal Society. 1984. Possums and gliders. Surrey Beatty & Sons in association with the Australian Mammal Society, Chipping Norton, N.S.W.

Smith, A., and Lindenmayer, D. 1988. Tree hollow requirements of Leadbeater's possum and other possums and gliders in timber production ash forests of the Victorian Central Highlands. Wildlife Research 15:347-362.

Soanes, K., Lobo, M. C., Vesk, P. A., McCarthy, M. A., Moore, J. L., and Van Der Ree, R. 2013. Movement re-established but not restored: inferring the effectiveness of road-crossing mitigation for a gliding mammal by monitoring use. Biological Conservation 159:434-441.

Soltis, J., Wilson, R. P., Douglas-Hamilton, I., Vollrath, F., King, L. E., and Savage, A. 2012. Accelerometers in collars identify behavioral states in captive African elephants Loxodonta africana. Endangered Species Research 18:255-263.

Soutullo, A., Cadahia, L., Urios, V., Ferrer, M., and Negro, J. J. 2007. Accuracy of lightweight satellite telemetry: a case study in the Iberian Peninsula. Journal of Wildlife Management 71:1010-1015.

Speakman, J. R. 1993. How should we calculate CO2 production in doubly labeled water studies of animals. Functional Ecology 7:746-750.

Speakman, J. R. 1997. Doubly labelled water : theory and practice. Chapman & Hall, London ; New York.

Speakman, J. R. 2000. The cost of living: Field metabolic rates of small mammals. Pages 177-297 in A. H. Fitter and D. G. Raffaelli, editors. Advances in Ecological Research, Vol 30. Elsevier Academic Press Inc, San Diego.

Speakman, J. R. 2005. Body size, energy metabolism and lifespan. Journal of Experimental Biology 208:1717-1730.

Page 156: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

137

Speakman, J. R., and Krol, B. 2005. Comparison of different approaches for the calculation of energy expenditure using doubly labeled water in a small mammal. Physiological and Biochemical Zoology 78:650-667.

Speakman, J. R., and Racey, P. A. 1987. The equilibrium concentration of o-18 in body-water - implications for the accuracy of the doubly-labeled water technique and a potential new method of measuring rq in free-living animals. Journal of Theoretical Biology 127:79-95.

Speakman, J. R., Racey, P. A., Haim, A., Webb, P. I., Ellison, G. T. H., and Skinner, J. D. 1994. Interindividual and intraindividual variation in daily energy-expenditure of the pouched mouse (saccostomus-campestris). Functional Ecology 8:336-342.

Statham, M., and Statham, H. L. 1997. Movements and habits of brushtail possums (Trichosurus vulpecula Kerr) in an urban area. Wildlife Research 24:715-726.

Stirling, I., and Derocher, A. E. 2012. Effects of climate warming on polar bears: a review of the evidence. Global Change Biology 18:2694-2706.

Stothart, M. R., Elliott, K. H., Wood, T., Hatch, S. A., and Speakman, J. R. 2016. Counting calories in cormorants: dynamic body acceleration predicts daily energy expenditure measured in pelagic cormorants. Journal of Experimental Biology 219:2192-2200.

Strauss, M., Botha, H., and van Hoven, W. 2008. Nile crocodile Crocodylus niloticus telemetry: observations on transmitter attachment and longevity. South African Journal of Wildlife Research 38:189-192.

Svanback, R., and Bolnick, D. I. 2007. Intraspecific competition drives increased resource use diversity within a natural population. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 274:839-844.

Taylor, C. R., Heglund, N. C., and Maloiy, G. 1982. Energetics and mechanics of terrestrial locomotion. I. Metabolic energy consumption as a function of speed and body size in birds and mammals. Journal of Experimental Biology 97:1-21.

Thomas, C. D., Cameron, A., Green, R. E., Bakkenes, M., Beaumont, L. J., Collingham, Y. C., Erasmus, B. F. N., de Siqueira, M. F., Grainger, A., Hannah, L., Hughes, L., Huntley, B., van Jaarsveld, A. S., Midgley, G. F., Miles, L., Ortega-Huerta, M. A., Peterson, A. T., Phillips, O. L., and Williams, S. E. 2004. Extinction risk from climate change. Nature 427:145-148.

Thompson, G. G., De Boer, M., and Pianka, E. R. 1999. Activity areas and daily movements of an arboreal monitor lizard, Varanus tristis (Squamata : Varanidae) during the breeding season. Australian Journal of Ecology 24:117-122.

Page 157: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

138

Tilman, D. 1994. Competition and biodiversity in spatially structured habitats. Ecology 75:2-16.

Tobias, J. A., Cornwallis, C. K., Derryberry, E. P., Claramunt, S., Brumfield, R. T., and Seddon, N. 2014. Species coexistence and the dynamics of phenotypic evolution in adaptive radiation. Nature 506:359-+.

Tornroos, A., Nordstrom, M. C., Aarnio, K., and Bonsdorff, E. 2015. Environmental context and trophic trait plasticity in a key species, the tellinid clam Macoma balthica L. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 472:32-40.

Tracey, J. A., Sheppard, J., Zhu, J., Wei, F. W., Swaisgood, R. R., and Fisher, R. N. 2014. Movement-Based Estimation and Visualization of Space Use in 3D for Wildlife Ecology and Conservation. Plos One 9:15.

Tscharntke, T., Klein, A. M., Kruess, A., Steffan-Dewenter, I., and Thies, C. 2005. Landscape perspectives on agricultural intensification and biodiversity - ecosystem service management. Ecology Letters 8:857-874.

Tuomainen, U., and Candolin, U. 2011. Behavioural responses to human-induced environmental change. Biological Reviews 86:640-657.

Ungar, E. D., Henkin, Z., Gutman, M., Dolev, A., Genizi, A., and Ganskopp, D. 2005. Inference of animal activity from GPS collar data on free-ranging cattle. Rangeland Ecology & Management 58:256-266.

Uno, H., Suzuki, T., Tachiki, Y., Akamatsu, R., and Hirakawa, H. 2010. Performance of GPS collars deployed on free-ranging sika deer in eastern Hokkaido, Japan. Mammal Study 35:111-118.

van der Ree, R. 2002. The population ecology of the squirrel glider (Petaurus norfolcensis) within a network of remnant linear habitats. Wildlife Research 29:329-340.

van der Ree, R., and Bennett, A. E. 2003. Home range of the squirrel glider (Petaurus norfolcensis) in a network of remnant linear habitats. Journal of Zoology 259:327-336.

Van Meter, P. E., French, J. A., Dloniak, S. M., Watts, H. E., Kolowski, J. M., and Holekamp, K. E. 2009. Fecal glucocorticoids reflect socio-ecological and anthropogenic stressors in the lives of wild spotted hyenas. Hormones and Behavior 55:329-337.

van Moorter, B., Bunnefeld, N., Panzacchi, M., Rolandsen, C. M., Solberg, E. J., and Saether, B. E. 2013. Understanding scales of movement: animals ride waves and ripples of environmental change. Journal of Animal Ecology 82:770-780.

Page 158: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

139

Vasudev, D., and Fletcher, R. J. 2015. Incorporating movement behavior into conservation prioritization in fragmented landscapes: An example of western hoolock gibbons in Garo Hills, India. Biological Conservation 181:124-132.

Visser, G. H., and Schekkerman, H. 1999. Validation of the doubly labeled water method in growing precocial birds: The importance of assumptions concerning evaporative water loss. Physiological and Biochemical Zoology 72:740-749.

Wang, D. J., Chen, L., and Wu, J. 2016. Novel In-flight Coarse Alignment of Low-cost Strapdown Inertial Navigation System for Unmanned Aerial Vehicle Applications. Transactions of the Japan Society for Aeronautical and Space Sciences 59:10-17.

Weimerskirch, H., Bonadonna, F., Bailleul, F., Mabille, G., Dell'Omo, G., and Lipp, H. P. 2002. GPS tracking of foraging albatrosses. Science 295:1259-1259.

Wells, K., Pfeiffer, M., Lakim, M. B., and Kalko, E. K. 2006. Movement trajectories and habitat partitioning of small mammals in logged and unlogged rain forests on Borneo. Journal of Animal Ecology 75:1212-1223.

White, C. R., and Seymour, R. S. 2003. Mammalian basal metabolic rate is proportional to body mass(2/3). Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America 100:4046-4049.

Williams, C. K., and Turnbull, H. L. 1983. Variations in seasonal nutrition, thermoregulation and water-balance in 2 new-zealand populations of the common brushtail possum, trichosurus-vulpecula (phalangeridae). Australian Journal of Zoology 31:333-343.

Williams, D. M., Quinn, A. D., and Porter, W. F. 2012. Impact of Habitat-Specific GPS Positional Error on Detection of Movement Scales by First-Passage Time Analysis. Plos One 7.

Williams, T. M., Wolfe, L., Davis, T., Kendall, T., Richter, B., Wang, Y., Bryce, C., Elkaim, G. H., and Wilmers, C. C. 2014. Instantaneous energetics of puma kills reveal advantage of felid sneak attacks. Science 346:81-85.

Wilson, R. P., White, C. R., Quintana, F., Halsey, L. G., Liebsch, N., Martin, G. R., and Butler, P. J. 2006. Moving towards acceleration for estimates of activity-specific metabolic rate in free-living animals: the case of the cormorant. Journal of Animal Ecology 75:1081-1090.

Winter, J. W. 1980. Tooth wear as an age index in a population of the brush-tailed possum, Trichosurus vulpecula (Kerr). Australian Wildlife Research 7:359-363.

Withers, P. C. 2001. Design, calibration and calculation for flow-through respirometry systems. Australian Journal of Zoology 49:445-461.

Page 159: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

140

Withers, P. C., Cooper, C. E., and Larcombe, A. N. 2006. Environmental correlates of physiological variables in marsupials. Physiological and Biochemical Zoology 79:437-453.

Wright, S., Metcalfe, J. D., Hetherington, S., and Wilson, R. 2014. Estimating activity-specific energy expenditure in a teleost fish, using accelerometer loggers. Marine Ecology Progress Series 496:19-32.

Yoda, K., Naito, Y., Sato, K., Takahashi, A., Nishikawa, J., Ropert-Coudert, Y., Kurita, M., and Le Maho, Y. 2001. A new technique for monitoring the behaviour of free-ranging Adelie penguins. Journal of Experimental Biology 204:685-690.

Yom‐Tov, Y., Green, W., and Coleman, J. 1986. Morphological trends in the common brushtail possum, Trichosurus vulpecula, in New Zealand. Journal of Zoology 208:583-593.

Zschille, J., Stier, N., Roth, M., and Berger, U. 2012. Dynamics in space use of American mink (Neovison vison) in a fishpond area in Northern Germany. European Journal of Wildlife Research 58:955-968.

Zucco, C. A., and Mourao, G. 2009. Low-Cost Global Positioning System Harness for Pampas Deer. Journal of Wildlife Management 73:452-457.

Zuur, A. F., Ieno, E. N., Walker, N. J., Saveliev, A. A., Smith, G. M., Zuur, A. F., Ieno, E. N., Walker, N. J., Saveliev, A. A., and Smith, G. M. 2009. Mixed Effects Models and Extensions in Ecology with R. Mixed Effects Models and Extensions in Ecology with R. Springer, 233 Spring Street, New York, Ny 10013, United States.

Page 160: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

141

Appendix

Chapter 3 Appendix

Appendix Table 3.2: Comparison of the average nightly distance travelled by

bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) in south-eastern Australia categorized by sex

and habitat type.

A positive estimate indicates a relatively higher average distance travelled during the

night, while a negative estimate indicates a relatively lower distance travelled. The p-

values displayed are corrected for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni correction.

Predictor Response Estimate SE t-value p-value

male forest fragment

Female forest fragment -280.30 130.77 2.14 0.258

Female linear strip -371.60 130.73 2.84 0.054

Male linear strip 797.96 215.62 3.70 0.006

female forest fragment

Female linear strip -91.31 131.39 -0.70 0.999

Male forest fragment 280.30 130.77 2.14 0.258

Male linear strip 1078.26 216.37 4.98 p<0.001

male linear strip

Female forest fragment -1078.26 216.37 4.98 p<0.001

Female linear strip -1169.56 214.34 5.46 p<0.001

Male forest fragment -797.96 215.62 3.70 0.006

female linear strip

Female forest fragment 91.31 131.39 0.70 0.999

Male forest fragment 371.60 130.73 2.84 0.054

Male linear strip 1169.56 214.34 5.46 p<0.001

Page 161: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

142

Appendix Table 3.3: Comparison of the average VeDBA (g) measured over 24

hours travelled by bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) in south-eastern Australia

categorized by sex and habitat type.

A positive estimate indicates a relatively higher average distance travelled during the

night, while a negative estimate indicates a relatively lower distance travelled. The p-

values displayed are corrected for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni correction.

Predictor Response Estimate SE t-value p-value

male forest fragment

Female forest fragment -10770.00 6101.26 1.77 0.516

Female linear strip -3738.00 6320.56 0.59 0.999

Male linear strip 27791.00 8905.00 3.12 0.024

female forest fragment

Female linear strip 7032.69 6243.47 1.13 0.999

Male forest fragment 10770.26 6101.26 1.77 0.516

Male linear strip 38561.24 8884.69 4.34 p<0.001

Male linear strip

Female forest fragment -38561.24 8884.69 4.34 p<0.001

Female linear strip -31528.55 8904.14 3.54 0.006

Male forest fragment -27791.00 8905.00 3.12 0.024

Female linear strip

Female forest fragment -7032.69 6243.47 1.13 0.999

Male forest fragment 3737.56 6320.56 0.59 0.999

Male linear strip 31528.55 8904.14 3.54 0.006

Page 162: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

143

Appendix Table 3.4a: Comparison of the average time spent denning within a 24-

hour period, measured in seconds, by bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) in

south-eastern Australia categorized by sex and habitat type.

A positive estimate indicates a relatively higher average time spent denning, while a

negative estimate indicates a relatively lower time spent denning. The p-values

displayed are corrected for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni correction.

Behaviour Predictor Response Estimate SE t-value p-value

denning

Male forest fragment

Female forest fragment 2491.20 1637.30 1.52 0.822

Female linear strip -835.30 1708.50 0.49 0.999

Male linear strip -3005.10 2376.50 1.27 0.999

Female forest fragment

Female linear strip -3326.51 1656.69 2.01 0.312

Male forest fragment -2491.20 1637.30 1.52 0.822

Male linear strip -5496.28 2339.53 2.35 0.144

Male linear strip

Female forest fragment 5496.28 2339.53 2.35 0.144

Female linear strip 2169.77 2389.88 0.91 0.999

Male forest fragment 3005.11 2376.46 1.27 0.999

Female linear strip

Female forest fragment 3326.51 1656.69 2.01 0.312

Male forest fragment 835.34 1708.45 0.49 0.999

Male linear strip -2169.77 2389.88 0.91 0.999

Page 163: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

144

Appendix Table 3.4b: Comparison of the average time spent resting at night

within a 24-hour period, measured in seconds, by bobucks (Trichosurus

cunninghami) in south-eastern Australia categorized by sex and habitat type.

A positive estimate indicates a relatively higher average time spent resting at night,

while a negative estimate indicates a relatively lower time spent resting at night. The

p-values displayed are corrected for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni

correction.

Behaviour Predictor Response Estimate SE t-value p-value

resting at night

Male forest fragment

Female forest fragment -1080.40 1118.10 0.97 0.999

Female linear strip 712.50 1160.20 0.61 0.999

Male linear strip 123.90 1651.30 0.08 0.999

Female forest fragment

Female linear strip 1792.85 1150.83 1.56 0.774

Male forest fragment 1080.39 1118.13 0.97 0.999

Male linear strip 1204.33 1653.41 0.73 0.999

Male linear strip

Female forest fragment -1204.33 1653.41 0.73 0.999

Female linear strip 588.53 1650.76 0.36 0.999

Male forest fragment -123.93 1651.33 0.08 0.999

Female linear strip

Female forest fragment -1792.85 1150.83 1.56 0.774

Male forest fragment -712.46 1160.22 0.61 0.999

Male linear strip -588.53 1650.76 0.36 0.999

Page 164: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

145

Appendix Table 3.4c: Comparison of the average time spent moving within a 24-

hour period, measured in seconds, by bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) in

south-eastern Australia categorized by sex and habitat type.

A positive estimate indicates a relatively higher average time spent moving, while a

negative estimate indicates a relatively lower time spent moving. The p-values

displayed are corrected for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni correction.

Behaviour Predictor Response Estimate SE t-value p-value

moving

Male forest fragment

Female forest fragment -626.17 923.40 0.68 0.999

Female linear strip 131.46 957.53 0.14 0.999

Male linear strip 2464.53 1343.15 1.84 0.450

Female forest fragment

Female linear strip 757.63 941.81 0.80 0.999

Male forest fragment 626.17 923.40 0.68 0.999

Male linear strip 3090.69 1336.60 2.31 0.156

Male linear strip

Female forest fragment -3090.69 1336.60 2.31 0.156

Female linear strip -2333.06 1343.02 1.74 0.546

Male forest fragment -2464.53 1343.15 1.84 0.450

Female linear strip

Female forest fragment -757.63 941.81 0.80 0.999

Male forest fragment -131.46 957.53 0.14 0.999

Male linear strip 2333.06 1343.02 1.74 0.546

Page 165: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

146

Appendix Table 3.4d: Comparison of the average time spent in high activity

within a 24-hour period, measured in seconds, by bobucks (Trichosurus

cunninghami) in south-eastern Australia categorized by sex and habitat type.

A positive estimate indicates a relatively higher average time spent in high activity,

while a negative estimate indicates a relatively lower time spent in high activity. The

p-values displayed are corrected for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni

correction.

Behaviour Predictor Response Estimate SE t-value p-value

high activity

Male forest fragment

Female forest fragment -373.50 96.75 3.86 0.006

Female linear strip -158.89 100.75 1.58 0.750

Male linear strip 758.72 140.72 5.39 p<0.001

Female forest fragment

Female linear strip 214.62 98.13 2.19 0.216

Male forest fragment 373.50 96.75 3.86 0.006

Male linear strip 1132.22 138.85 8.15 p<0.001

Male linear strip

Female forest fragment -1132.22 138.85 8.15 p<0.001

Female linear strip -917.61 141.67 6.48 p<0.001

Male forest fragment -758.72 140.72 5.39 p<0.001

Female linear strip

Female forest fragment -214.62 98.13 2.19 0.216

Male forest fragment 158.89 100.75 1.58 0.750

Male linear strip 917.61 141.67 6.48 p<0.001

Page 166: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

147

Appendix Table 3.5: Interval comparison of the average distance travelled (m)

versus the average VeDBA (g), measured in 10 minute intervals for bobucks

(Trichosurus cunninghami) in south-eastern Australia categorized by sex and

habitat type.

A positive estimate indicates a relatively higher average values, while a negative

estimate indicates a relatively lower average values. The p-values displayed are

corrected for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni correction.

Predictor Response Estimate SE t-value p-value

Male forest fragment

Female forest fragment 2.02 1.13 1.79 0.4419

Female linear strip -2.47 1.17 2.12 0.2082

Male linear strip -8.48 1.16 7.29 p<0.001

Female forest fragment

Female linear strip -4.49 1.29 3.48 0.003

Male forest fragment -2.02 1.13 1.79 0.4419

Male linear strip -10.50 1.29 8.17 p<0.001

Male linear strip

Female forest fragment 10.50 1.29 8.17 p<0.001

Female linear strip 6.00 1.32 4.54 p<0.001

Male forest fragment 8.48 1.16 7.29 p<0.001

Female linear strip

Female forest fragment 4.49 1.29 3.48 0.003

Male forest fragment 2.47 1.17 2.12 0.2082

   Male linear strip -6.00 1.32 4.54 p<0.001

Page 167: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

148

Chapter 4 Appendix

Appendix Table 4.2: Model selection results for the generalised mixed models

comparing energy expenditure to distance travelled for bobucks (Trichosurus

cunninghami) in south-eastern Australia.

Overall energy expenditure (EE, kJ) was compared to Total Distance (DistanceTotal,

m), and Daily Energy Expenditure (DEE, kJ/day) was compared to Daily Distance

(DistanceDaily, m/day), with both models including variables. AICc values,

differences in AICc values (∆i) and Akaike weight (wi) are shown for models with ∆i

< 10.

DistanceTotal Sex Tooth Wear Mass R2 df AICc Δi wi

0.000 2 275.29 0.00 0.46

-0.599 0.081 3 276.84 1.56 0.21

+ 0.009 3 278.12 2.83 0.11

0.037 0.008 3 278.15 2.86 0.11

0.036 -0.598 0.088 4 280.20 4.91 0.04

+ -0.590 0.087 4 280.21 4.92 0.04

0.057 + 0.025 4 281.33 6.05 0.02

0.054 + -0.583 0.101 5 284.07 8.78 0.01

DistanceDaily Sex Tooth Wear Mass R2 df AICc Δi wi

0.000 2 205.18 0.00 0.53

-0.044 0.027 3 207.70 2.52 0.15

0.016

0.003 3 208.12 2.94 0.12

+ 0.000 3 208.17 2.99 0.12

0.021 -0.046 0.032 4 211.10 5.91 0.03

+ -0.044 0.027 4 211.19 6.00 0.03

0.018 + 0.003 4 211.60 6.42 0.02

Page 168: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

149

Appendix Table 4.3: Model selection results for the generalised mixed models

comparing energy expenditure to VeDBA for bobucks (Trichosurus

cunninghami) in south-eastern Australia.

Overall energy expenditure (EE, kJ), mass standardised overall energy expenditure

(EEmass, kJ), and mass0.75 standardised overall energy expenditure (EEmass0.75, kJ)

were compared to total VeDBA, with all models including variables and the total

distance travelled (m). AICc values, differences in AICc values (∆i) and Akaike

weight (wi) are shown for models with ∆i < 10.

VeDBATotal Distance Travelled

Sex Tooth Wear

Mass R2 df AICc Δi wi

EE

0.003749 0.418 2 266.08 0.00 0.48

0.004279 -0.103 0.466 3 267.61 1.53 0.22

0.004507 -0.234 0.449 3 268.16 2.08 0.17

0.005113 -0.108 -0.250 0.501 4 269.96 3.88 0.07

0.003830 + 0.454 4 271.48 5.40 0.03

0.003825 + -0.586 0.531 5 273.02 6.94 0.01

0.004277 -0.084 + 0.482 5 274.69 8.61 0.01

EEmass

0.001190 NA 0.246 2 239.76 0.00 0.69

0.001392 -0.039 NA 0.288 3 241.77 2.01 0.25

0.001181 + NA 0.260 4 245.90 6.14 0.03

+ NA 0.002 3 247.50 7.74 0.01

0.001371 -0.036 + NA 0.292 5 249.29 9.53 0.01

EEmass0.75

0.001584 NA 0.286 2 246.19 0.00 0.68

0.001841 -0.050 NA 0.331 3 248.08 1.89 0.27

0.001587 + NA 0.305 4 252.21 6.02 0.03

+ NA 0.004 3 254.85 8.67 0.01

0.001823 -0.044 + NA 0.337 5 255.54 9.36 0.01

Page 169: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

150

Appendix Table 4.4: Model selection results for the generalised mixed models

comparing daily energy expenditure to average VeDBA for bobucks (Trichosurus

cunninghami) in south-eastern Australia.

Daily energy expenditure (DEE, kJ/day), mass standardised daily energy expenditure

(DEE mass, kJ/day), and mass0.75 standardised daily energy expenditure (DEE mass0.75,

kJ/day) were compared to the average VeDBA (VeDBAAverage), with all models

including variables and daily distance travelled (m/day). AICc values, differences in

AICc values (∆i) and Akaike weight (wi) are shown for models with ∆i < 10.

VeDBAAverage Distance Travelled Sex Tooth Wear Mass R2 df AICc Δi wi

DEE

7979.236 -0.122 2 207.13 0.00 0.33

+ 0.000 3 208.17 1.04 0.20

3765.747 + 0.149 4 208.92 1.79 0.14

6873.345 0.028 -0.103 3 209.83 2.70 0.09

8340.448 -0.006 -0.111 3 209.97 2.83 0.08

+ -0.044 0.027 4 211.19 4.05 0.04

4514.820 + -0.078 0.226 5 211.42 4.29 0.04

0.003 + 0.002 4 211.62 4.49 0.04

3968.422 -0.004 + 0.153 5 212.95 5.81 0.02

7258.202 -0.005 0.027 -0.095 4 213.20 6.07 0.02

0.002 + -0.044 0.029 5 215.27 8.14 0.01

0.201 -0.818 2 215.35 8.21 0.01

4845.768 -0.006 + -0.081 0.236 6 216.14 9.01 0.00

DEEmass

+ NA 0.000 3 179.28 0.00 0.50

2515.294 NA -0.289 2 180.61 1.34 0.25

407.437 + NA 0.010 4 182.60 3.32 0.09

0.000 + NA 0.000 4 182.76 3.49 0.09

2591.796 -0.001 NA -0.287 3 183.57 4.29 0.06

459.629 -0.001 + NA 0.011 5 186.69 7.42 0.01

DEEmass0.75

+ NA 0.000 3 185.87 0.00 0.48

3353.777 NA -0.279 2 187.08 1.20 0.26

812.920 + NA 0.026 4 188.92 3.04 0.10

0.000 + NA 0.000 4 189.36 3.49 0.08

3467.666 -0.002 NA -0.276 3 190.01 4.14 0.06

885.428 -0.001 + NA 0.028 5 193.00 7.13 0.01

Page 170: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

151

Chapter 5 Appendix

Appendix Table 5.2: Comparison of the average nightly distance travelled by

bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) and common brushtail possums (T.

vulpecula) in their respective locations: the Strathbogie Ranges (foothill forest)

and Grantville (heavily cleared coastal and lowland forest).

A positive estimate indicates a relatively higher average distance travelled during the

night, while a negative estimate indicates a relatively lower distance travelled. The p-

values displayed are corrected for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni correction.

Predictor Response Estimate SE t-value p-value

Grantville T. cunninghami

Strathbogies T. cunninghami 424.4 202.8 2.093 0.117

Strathbogies T. vulpecula -97.6 232.0 0.421 0.999

Strathbogies T. cunninghami

Grantville T. cunninghami -424.4 202.8 2.093 0.117

Strathbogies T. vulpecula -522.0 203.1 2.570 0.049

Strathbogies T. vulpecula

Grantville T. cunninghami 97.6 232.0 0.421 0.999

Strathbogies T. cunninghami 522.0 203.1 2.570 0.049

Page 171: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

152

Appendix Table 5.3: Comparison of the average VeDBA (g) measured over 24

hours travelled by bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) and common brushtail

possums (T. vulpecula) in their respective locations; the Strathbogie Ranges

(foothill forest) and Grantville (heavily cleared coastal and lowland forest).

A positive estimate indicates a relatively higher average activity, while a negative

estimate indicates a relatively lower average activity. The p-values displayed are

corrected for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni correction.

Predictor Response Estimate SE t-value p-value

Grantville T. cunninghami

Strathbogies T. cunninghami -9035 13918 0.649 0.999

Strathbogies T. vulpecula -55387 15714 3.525 0.009

Strathbogies T. cunninghami

Grantville T. cunninghami 9035 13918 0.649 0.999

Strathbogies T. vulpecula -46353 14815 3.129 0.020

Strathbogies T. vulpecula

Grantville T. cunninghami 55387 15714 3.525 0.009

Strathbogies T. cunninghami 46353 14815 3.129 0.020

Page 172: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

153

Appendix Table 5.4: Comparison of the average time spent in the four

behavioural states (denning, resting at night, moving, and high activity) within a

24-hour period, measured in seconds, by bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) and

common brushtail possums (T. vulpecula) in their respective locations; the

Strathbogie Ranges (foothill forest) and Grantville (heavily cleared coastal and

lowland forest).

A positive estimate indicates a relatively higher average time spent in a behavioural

state, while a negative estimate indicates a relatively lower time spent in a behavioural

state. The p-values displayed are corrected for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni

correction.

Behaviour Predictor Response Estimate SE t-value p-value

Denning

Gippsland T. cunninghami

Strathbogies T. cunninghami 788 1728 0.456 0.999

Strathbogies T. vulpecula 7206 1867 3.860 0.001

Strathbogies T. cunninghami

Gippsland T. cunninghami -788 1728 0.456 0.999

Strathbogies T. vulpecula 6417 1831 3.504 0.007

Strathbogies T. vulpecula

Gippsland T. cunninghami -7206 1867 3.860 0.001

Strathbogies T. cunninghami -6417 1831 3.504 0.007

Resting at Night

Gippsland T. cunninghami

Strathbogies T. cunninghami -2160 804 2.688 0.033

Strathbogies T. vulpecula -3329 857 3.886 0.002

Strathbogies T. cunninghami

Gippsland T. cunninghami 2160 804 2.688 0.033

Strathbogies T. vulpecula -1169 890 1.314 0.999

Strathbogies T. vulpecula

Gippsland T. cunninghami 3329 857 3.886 0.002

Strathbogies T. cunninghami 1169 890 1.314 0.999

Moving

Gippsland T. cunninghami

Strathbogies T. cunninghami 1296 1445 0.897 0.999

Strathbogies T. vulpecula -2948 1582 1.863 0.260

Page 173: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

154

Strathbogies T. cunninghami

Gippsland T. cunninghami -1296 1445 0.897 0.999

Strathbogies T. vulpecula -4244 1486 2.855 0.037

Strathbogies T. vulpecula

Gippsland T. cunninghami 2948 1582 1.863 0.260

Strathbogies T. cunninghami 4244 1486 2.855 0.037

High Activity

Gippsland T. cunninghami

Strathbogies T. cunninghami 157 237 0.661 0.999

Strathbogies T. vulpecula -965 270 3.573 0.012

Strathbogies T. cunninghami

Gippsland T. cunninghami -157 237 0.661 0.999

Strathbogies T. vulpecula -1122 247 4.541 0.002

Strathbogies T. vulpecula

Gippsland T. cunninghami 965 270 3.573 0.012

Strathbogies T. cunninghami 1122 247 4.541 0.002

Page 174: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

155

Appendix Table 5.5: Interval comparison of the average distance travelled (m)

versus the average VeDBA (g), measured in 10-minute intervals for bobucks

(Trichosurus cunninghami) and common brushtail possums (T. vulpecula) in their

respective locations: the Strathbogie Ranges (foothill forest) and Grantville

(heavily cleared coastal and lowland forest).

A positive estimate indicates a relatively higher VeDBA value, while a negative

estimate indicates a relatively lower VeDBA value. The p-values displayed are

corrected for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni correction.

Predictor Response Estimate SE t-value p-value

Grantville T. cunninghami

Strathbogies T. cunninghami -4.85 1.34 3.615 0.001

Strathbogies T. vulpecula 8.91 1.66 5.381 p<0.001

Strathbogies T. cunninghami

Grantville T. cunninghami 4.85 1.34 3.615 0.001

Strathbogies T. vulpecula 13.76 1.48 9.327 p<0.001

Strathbogies T. vulpecula

Grantville T. cunninghami -8.91 1.66 5.381 p<0.001

Strathbogies T. cunninghami -13.76 1.48 9.327 p<0.001

Page 175: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

156

Appendix Table 5.6: Comparison of the estimated daily energy expenditure

(kJ/d) calculated from the behavioural states using the equation in Chapter 4, for

bobucks (Trichosurus cunninghami) and common brushtail possums (T.

vulpecula) in their respective locations: the Strathbogie Ranges (foothill forest)

and Grantville (heavily cleared coastal and lowland forest).

A positive estimate indicates a relatively higher daily energy expenditure, while a

negative estimate indicates a relatively lower daily energy expenditure. The p-values

displayed are corrected for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni correction.

Predictor Response Estimate SE t-value p-value

Grantville T. cunninghami

Strathbogies T. cunninghami 44.76 26.64 1.680 0.350

Strathbogies T. vulpecula -74.90 29.18 2.567 0.073

Strathbogies T. cunninghami

Grantville T. cunninghami -44.76 26.64 1.680 0.350

Strathbogies T. vulpecula -119.66 27.77 4.309 0.002

Strathbogies T. vulpecula

Grantville T. cunninghami 74.90 29.18 2.567 0.073

Strathbogies T. cunninghami 119.66 27.77 4.309 0.002

Page 176: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

157

Published Article - Free as a drone: ecologists can add UAVs to their toolbox

Page 177: by Blake M Allan Submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for …dro.deakin.edu.au/eserv/DU:30103230/allan-refining... · i Acknowledgements The many years of a PhD leave you with

158