brotherhoods atelines

Upload: edna-beltran

Post on 03-Jun-2018

220 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 8/11/2019 Brotherhoods Atelines

    1/17

    BROTHERHOODS AMONG ATELINS:KINSHIP,AFFILIATION, AND COMPETITION

    by

    KAREN B. STRIER1)(Department of Anthropology, University of Wisconsin-Madison, Madison, WI 53706,

    U.S.A.)

    (With 3 Figures)

    (Acc. 20-VII-1994)

    Summary

    This paper examines differences in male affiliative associations and dominance relation-ships in atelin primates. Despite the occurrence of male philopatry in all three genera, malesocial relations varied considerably. The strength of male affiliations was attributed todifferences in between group competition for access to groups of females, while the type ofmale dominance relationships was attributed to differences in within group competition foraccess to individual females. Female grouping patterns and the socionomic sex ratio

    appeared to be more important than group size in predicting the strength of between groupcompetition and male affiliations. Male dominance over females appeared to be moreimportant than estrus synchrony in predicting the type of within group competition and thepresence of hierarchical or egalitarian relationships among males.

    Introduction

    Affiliative relationships among same-sexed primates have long been asso-

    ciated with kinship and kin selection theory (HAMILTON, 1964). Because

    extended matrilinies characterize the societies of many of the best-studied

    1) A version of this paper was presented at the XIVth Congress of the InternationalPrimatological Society. I thank Dr. David HILLand Dr. Jan VANHOOFFfor inviting me toparticipate in their symposium, and for their valuable comments on earlier drafts of thismanuscript. Long-term field research on Brachyteleswas possible thanks to permission fromthe Brazilian government and CNPq, sponsorship by Dr. Celio VALLEand Dr. Gustavo DEFONSECA,and funds from NSF grants BNS-8305322, BNS-8619442, and BNS/DBS-8958298, theFulbright Foundation, grant no. 213 from the Joseph Henry Fund ofNAS,

    the L.S.B. Leakey Foundation, WWF, the Graduate School of the University of Wisconsin-Madison, the National Geographic Society, the Chicago Zoological Society, and the LizClaiborne and Art Ortenberg Foundation. F. MENDES,J.RMOLl,A. RMOLl,P. COUTINHO,F.NERI,C. NOGUEIRA,L. OLIVEIRA,and A. CARVALHOcontributed to the long-term demo-graphic data described here.

  • 8/11/2019 Brotherhoods Atelines

    2/17

    152

    Fig. 1.Dispersal patterns in anthropoid primates. Genera are added to each taxonomic groupin a cumulative way, such that the percentage plotted for cercopithecinesincludes fivegenera(listed below the x-axis), that plotted for colobines includes four colobine genera plus theoriginal five cercopithecine genera, and so on. Sources from Moore (1984, 1992);Pusey &

    Packer (1987);Strier (1990).

    primates, studies of affiliative relationships have tended to focus on

    females (e.g. GouzouLES & GouzouLES, 1987). However, a growing bodyof data on a number of other species indicate that female philopatry and

    male-biased dispersal are not as widespread among anthropoids as was

    previously thought (MOORE, 1984, 1992). Indeed, female philopatry

    appears to be a pattern only among the cercopithecine monkeys; when

    other taxonomic groups are considered, dispersal and residency are evi-

    dently quite variable (Fig. 1). Dispersal by both sexes occurs in manycolobines and apes, while female-biased dispersal coupled with male

    philopatry are prevalent among the polygamous New World monkeys.The three genera that comprise the Atelin subfamily, Lagothrix, Ateles,

    and Brachyteles, are particularly interesting subjects for examining affilia-

  • 8/11/2019 Brotherhoods Atelines

    3/17

    153

    tive relationships among males: each lives in variably sized, extended

    patrilineal groups resulting from female-biased dispersal and male phi-

    lopatry (ROSENBERGER & STRIER, 1989; NISHIMURA, 1990a). Yet, the

    nature of relationships among males differs strikingly in these closely-related genera. Evaluating the correlates of male affiliations and relation-

    ships in the atelins may therefore provide useful insights into the bases of

    male social behavior in other nonfemale-bonded primates.

    A model for affiliations among male kin

    Two parameters, or indices, of relationships among male kin can bereadily compared between taxa and studies. The first is the strength of

    male affiliative associations, which can be assessed based on spatial

    relationships, such as proximity and nearest neighbor preferences, and on

    the frequency of affiliative interactions, including grooming partner and

    embrace partner preferences (see FURUICHI & IHOBE, this volume, for

    other examples). The second parameter is the type of relationships among

    males, which can be characterized as hierarchical, based on agonistically-

    determined dominance relations, or egalitarian, if agonistic competition

    among males is rare or absent altogether (VAN SCHAIK, 1989).

    1. Affiliative associations.

    Variation in the strength of affiliative associations and in the type of

    relationships among same-sexed primates has been attributed to variation

    in the relative

    importance

    of betweengroup competition

    and within

    group competition, respectively (e.g. VANSCHAIK, 1989; VAN HOOFF & VAN

    SCHAIK, 1992). Between group competition may lead to strong affiliative

    associations that facilitate cooperation among males if such cooperationincreases males' access to groups of females. Whether such cooperation is

    beneficial to males depends on behavioral and demographic variables

    (HILL, this volume; VANHOOFF & VANSCHAIK, this volume). VAN HOOFF &

    VAN SCHAIK (1992) postulate that fluid female grouping patterns and large

    group size may make groups of females difficult for males to monopolizewithout cooperative efforts. In extreme cases, male cooperation in

    between group competition may even be effective in herding females into

    cohesive groups (WRANGHAM, 1980; see below). High socionomic sex

  • 8/11/2019 Brotherhoods Atelines

    4/17

    154

    ratios, or the ratio of reproductive-aged females to males, might also

    make it difficult for males to keep track of females, particularly among

    large groupsof arboreal

    primateswhere members

    are frequently out ofvisual contact despite cohesive and coordinated movements. The socio-

    nomic sex ratio may thus be an additional influence on male affiliative

    associations and the benefits of cooperation in between group

    competition.

    2. Male relationships.

    Strong male affiliations and cooperation in monopolizing groups offemales do not necessarily lead to tolerant, egalitarian relationships

    among males within groups (VEHRENCAMP, 1983). Rather, male relation-

    ships are expected to be hierarchical when within group competition is of

    the contest type, such that males benefit by competing for access to

    individual females. Egalitarian relationships among males, by contrast,should only occur when within group competition is of the scramble type,and aggressive competition among males does not improve their access to

    individual females (VAN HOOFF & VAN SCHAIK, 1992). Thus, while maleaffiliative associations should reflect the degree to which groups of

    females can be monopolized in between group competition, male domi-

    nance relationships should reflect the degree to which individual females

    can be monopolized by individual males in within group competition.There are at least two factors that determine whether contest competi-

    tion among males increases a male's access to females. First, if female

    estrus isasynchronous,

    a dominant malemay

    be able tomonopolizesexually-receptive females either directly, by preventing other males from

    copulating, or indirectly, by restricting female choice (NEWTON, 1988;COWLISHAW & DUNBAR, 1991; VAN HOOFF & VAN SCHAIK, 1992). How-

    ever, a male's ability to monopolize a female is also dependent on

    whether males are individually dominant over females, and thus, on the

    degree to which female mate choice can be expressed. If females are

    dominant over, or codominant with, males, as in lemurs (YOUNG et al.,

    1990) and Saimiri (BOINSKI, 1987), female choice may override maleefforts to monopolize copulations, and there may be few advantages to

    overt competition among males (STRIER, 1990, 1992a). In this case,

    egalitarian, rather than hierarchical, relationships should occur.

  • 8/11/2019 Brotherhoods Atelines

    5/17

  • 8/11/2019 Brotherhoods Atelines

    6/17

    156

  • 8/11/2019 Brotherhoods Atelines

    7/17

    157

    TABLE 1. Distribution of copulations across individual male Brachyteles.

    Initials represent all adult and adolescent males in the group during each period, includingmales which were born and subsequently matured in the group. Numbers in parenthesesindicate the proportion of copulations by each male observed during the respective studyperiods. Only copulations in which the male participant was positively identified areincluded here. a STRIER,1987. b MENDES,1 990.present study, which includes continuousdata from March 1990-November 1992. d Disappearance and presumed death prior tosubsequent study period (see STRIER,1991, 1992c). " Adolescent male in 1983-1984, butfully adult by 1986-1987.

    Agonistic interactions among group members were extremely rare, and

    there was no evidence of any dominance hierarchy based on aggression,either among males, among females, or between males and females

    (STRIER, 1986, 1990, 1992a,c). Yet, despite the absence of overt competi-

    tion, there were pronounced differences in male mating success (Table 1).These differences were evident across males during the same period, and

    for particular males across successive years. For example, the most sexu-

    ally-active male between 1983-1984 and 1986-1987, IV, accounted for38.5% and 50.0% of observed copulations, respectively. By the

    1990-1992 period he accounted for only 15.4% of observed copulationswhile a younger male, DA, born in 1985, accounted for 20.4%. DA is the

    only sexually-active male positively known to have an older maternal

    brother, DI, in the group, and it is tempting to infer that DI aided his

    younger brother in securing mates. However, we found no evidence for

    coalitions between these males.

    Copulations routinely occurred in full view of other group members(STRIER, 1992c), and nearly all adult and subadult males were observed to

    copulate across the three study periods analyzed here (Table 2). The

    proportion of copulating males approached 100% as both the number of

  • 8/11/2019 Brotherhoods Atelines

    8/17

    158

    TABLE 2. Changes in the distribution of group male versus extra-group

    male copulations

    All data refer to study group males except "% males identified", which includes both studygroup and extra-group males, and "% copulations by ja6 malcs", or males from aneighboring group and thus, extra-group males. Sources as in Table 1.

    observed copulations and the number of males which could be positivelyidentified during these copulations increased. Although we currently lack

    information on paternity, and therefore male reproductive success, it was

    evident that males did not compete aggressively to monopolize estrous

    females. The fact that the distribution of copulations was nonethelessstrongly skewed across males suggests that female mate choice may be

    responsible for differential mating success among male Brachyteles.Females demonstrated their ability to assert mate choice by actively

    initiating sexual inspections and copulations with particular males, and

    by avoiding unwanted attention from others by simply walking away

    (STRIER, 1992a, c). Some females exhibited strong preferences for particu-lar males, but it is not evident what male attributes these preferences were

    based on (STRIER, 1992a, in prep.). Copulations tended to be seasonal

    (STRIER, 1991), but females did not exhibit tight estrous synchrony.

    Rather, the inability of males to dominate females appeared to be respon-sible for the high degree of tolerance males exhibited toward one

    another's mating activities, and for the apparent nonaggressive relation-

    ships among them (STRIER, in press).While male Brachyteles did not compete overtly with other males in their

    group for sexual access to females (STRIER, 1992a), relationships betweenmales from different groups were hostile. Male group members joinedforces in prolonged vocal displays and chases when males from a neigh-

    boring group at this site were encountered, and often they succeeded in

  • 8/11/2019 Brotherhoods Atelines

    9/17

    159

    leading their female associates away from male intruders (STRIER, 1986,

    1992c). As group size, female subgrouping, and sex ratio increased,

    however, even such cooperative efforts became increasingly less effective.During the first six years that these Brachyteles were observed, the adult

    females in the group remained together as a cohesive unit (STRIER, 1991)and the six adult males were successful at preventing incursions by extra-

    group males. Since 1988, however, males from a neighboring group, Jao,

    began to account for a substantial proportion of copulations with groupfemales (Table 2).

    Demographic changes in the group may have been responsible, at least

    in part, for what appeared to be a decline in male between group

    competitive ability (Table 2). Between 1982 and 1992, the study groupmore than doubled in size, increasing from its original 22 members to 48

    members. The socionomic sex ratio also jumped dramatically, increasingfrom 1.1 in 1987 to 1.7 in 1988 due to the presumed deaths of two adult

    males and the documented maturation of adolescent females which had

    previously immigrated. This increase in sex ratio coincided with a

    number of otherchanges

    in thegroup, including

    agreater tendency by

    group females to fission into smaller temporary feeding parties (STRIER et

    al., 1993), as well as a greater proportion of copulations by extra-groupmales (Table 2).

    2. Ateles.

    Descriptions of affiliative associations among male Ateles appear to be

    comparable to those among male Brachyteles. A. geoffroyl, studied at Tikal,Guatamela, exhibited significantly higher rates of affinitive interactions,

    including sitting in contact, embracing, touching, and grooming, than did

    females, and males directed 85% of all affinitive interactions toward other

    males (FEDIGAN & BAXTER, 1984). At Manu National Park, Peru, associa-

    tion indices for A. paniscus males were more than twice as high, on

    average, as those of females. These males also groomed one another

    significantly more than

    expectedby their representation in the study

    population (SYMINGTON, 1987, 1990).

    Despite their strong affiliations, however, Ateles male relationships

    appeared to be hierarchical rather than egalitarian, even among male

    subgroups. SYMINGTON(1987) observed that although all males were likely

  • 8/11/2019 Brotherhoods Atelines

    10/17

    160

    TABLE 3. Correlates of male associations

    Data for Lagothrix(5 studies)and Ateles(5 studies)are summarized from NISHIMURA'S(1990a)review; data from Brachytelesare based on the present study of a single group over a 10 yearperiod. BGC, or between group competition, is discussed in the text. Categorical designa-tions are based on cited literature.

    to have copulated during her 12-month study, the dominant male was the

    only male which mated in view of other group members instead of in

    seclusion. Ateles males are dominant over females, and relationshipsbetween the sexes are reportedly tense (FEDIGAN & BAXTER, 1984; SYM-

    INGTON, 1987).Affiliative associations among male Ateles have been attributed to the

    importanceof male coalitions

    during intergroupencounters

    (WRANGHAM,1980; SYMINGTON, 1987, 1990). Not only are female grouping patterns in

    Ateles fluid, the socionomic sex ratio of nearly all groups studied to date is

    higher than that of other atelin groups (NISHIMURA, 1990a). Both of these

    conditions are consistent with predictions that male cooperation is critical

    in between group competition over access to a relatively large number of

    dispersed females (Table 3).The advantages of cooperation in between group competition do not

    appear, however, to override the advantages of within group contestcompetition in Ateles or in Pan, a species which exhibits remarkable

    behavioral parallels with Ateles (SYMINGTON,1990; FURUICHI & IHOBE, this

    volume). Like Brachyteles, Ateles females do not experience tightly syn-chronized estrus, but unlike Brachyteles, Ateles males are dominant over

    females (Table 4). Male dominance in Ateles, but not in Brachyteles, may be

    related to other differences between the genera. Both genera exhibit

    relatively low levels of sexual dimorphism in body size, but canine dimor-

    phism may be more pronounced in Ateles than in Brachyteles (ROSENBERGER& STRIER, 1989). Brachyteles, however, have extraordinarily large testes

    size, suggesting that males may employ more subtle forms of sperm

    competition (MILTON, 1985; STRIER, 1986, 1992a, c) than Ateles, which

  • 8/11/2019 Brotherhoods Atelines

    11/17

    161

    TABLE 4. Correlates of male relationships

    WGC, or within group competition, is discussed in the text. Categorical designations arebased on cited literature, summarized in NISHIMURA(1990a) and the present study.

    have small testes and compete overtly (FEDIGAN & BAXTER, 1984). Fur-

    thermore, male coalitions in chasing Ateles females (FEDIGAN & BAXTER,

    1984; SYMINGTON,1987) have not been observed in Brachyteles, raising the

    intriguing possibility that Ateles males achieve their dominance over

    females through cooperative efforts.

    3. Lagothrix.

    Male affiliations inLagothrix appear

    to be weaker thanthose

    inthe otheratelins, but their hierarchical relationships resemble those of Ateles. Male

    L. lagotricha, studied in the upper Colombian Amazon, were rarelyobserved in close proximity to one another (NISHIMURA, 1990a). Males

    were proportionately more interactive with one another than were

    females, due largely to higher frequencies of play among juvenile and

    subadult males, and higher frequencies of agonistic interactions amongmales. NISHIMURA(1990a: 115) concludes that "... so far as only adults are

    concerned, it could not be said that males are more associative (with) oneanother than females."

    Dominance relationships among Lagothrix males were based on the

    outcomes of agonistic interactions, and appeared to be related to age

    (NISHIMURA,in prep.). At La Macarena, Colombia, male rank correlated

    with the frequency of affiliative interactions with females, and the domi-

    nant male mated more frequently (31.7%) than others. (NISHIMURA,

    1990b). Nonetheless, every male accounted for at least 13.7% of observed

    copulations, suggesting that the variance in male mating success was

    lower in Lagothrix than in the more egalitarian Brachyteles.The absence of strong affiliative associations among Lagothrix males

    appears to correspond to an absence of tension between groups.

  • 8/11/2019 Brotherhoods Atelines

    12/17

  • 8/11/2019 Brotherhoods Atelines

    13/17

    163

    Atelin comparisons indicate two possible additions to VAN HOOFF &

    VAN SaHam's (1992) model. First, these comparisons suggest that group

    size may not be as important as previously assumed in predicting cooper-ative affiliations among males in between group competition, at least in

    primates which exhibit flexible grouping patterns. Intraspecific variation

    in group size is similar across all atelins, with reported group sizes rangingfrom lows of 10 and 16 in Lagothrix and Ateles to highs of 45, 42, and 48 in

    Lagothrix, Ateles, and Brachyteles, respectively (Table 3; NISHIMURA, 1990a).All Ateles groups studied to date have fluid grouping patterns regardless of

    group size, but some Lagothrix groups are reportedly more fluid than those

    considered here (see DEFLER, 1987; SOINI, 1987), and Brachyteles groups atother sites have been described as both fluid (MILTON, 1984) and cohesive

    (LEMOS DE Spa, 1988). Data on male social relationships, sex ratios, and

    intergroup encounters are needed from these other Lagothrix and Bra-

    chyteles populations before further intraspecific comparisons can made.

    Demographic changes in the Brachyteles group described here suggestthat increases in both group size and sex ratio were associated with

    increasinglyfluid associations

    among females, implyingthat lower levels

    of within group feeding competition among females and/or male cooper-ative efforts were responsible for the cohesion that characterized this

    group during the first six years of observation (STRIER et al., 1993). The

    greater tendency toward fluid grouping patterns in this study group has

    led to the proposition that Brachyteles groups are facultatively cohesive

    (STRIER, 1989, 1992b), particularly when food patches are large enoughto support all group members (see also STRIER, 1986; LEMOS DE SA &

    STRIER, 1992). While fluid grouping patterns were consistently associatedwith strong male affiliative relations in Brachyteles as well as in Ateles, it is

    not clear whether increased group size (relative to food patch size) or sex

    ratio is responsible for their fission-fusion social organization.

    Second, the exceptional egalitarian relationships among Brachyteles

    males, in contrast to the hierarchical relationships in Lagothrix and Ateles,

    suggest that the ability of males to dominate females is an importantdeterminant of whether within group competition among males is of the

    contest or the scramble type. Even when estrus is asynchronous, as is the

    case in all three atelins, female codominance in Brachyteles may reduce the

    advantages of contest competition among males, increase the oppor-tunities for female mate choice, and lead to egalitarian relationships that

  • 8/11/2019 Brotherhoods Atelines

    14/17

    164

    Fig. 3. Factors contributing to male affiliative behavior in atelins.

    facilitate, but are not necessary for, male cooperation in between group

    competition.

    Indeed, the atelins provide suggestive evidence that kinship, affiliative

    associations, and cooperation among males in between and within groupcompetition do not always co-vary (Figure 3). Kinship need not lead to

    strong affiliative associations or cooperation among male group members

    when between group competition is low (VANHOOFF & VAN SCHAIK, this

    volume), as appears to be the case in Lagothrix. Even when cooperation

    among male kin in defending groups of females in intergroup encounters

    occurs, as in Ateles and Brachyteles, contest competition among the same

    males over access to individual females occurs in Ateles but not in Bra-

    chyteles due to differences in the type of within group competition. More-over, male cooperative coalitions have been observed in other primateseven when there is little evidence of affiliative associations outside of

    agonistic contexts (e.g. PACKER, 1979; SMUTS & WATANABE, 1990). Con-

  • 8/11/2019 Brotherhoods Atelines

    15/17

    165

    versely, strong affiliative associations may occur between males which

    nonetheless fail to support one another in agonistic interactions (HILL,

    this volume).Further data on the correlates of within group competition are needed

    from species in which dispersal and philopatry are not sex-biased, and in

    which dominance relationships between males and females are more

    equitable. Similarly, long-term demographic and behavioral data are

    needed from a greater number of species in order to better understand the

    interactive effects of sex ratios and male dominance on the dynamics of

    male competition and social relationships between and within primate

    groups.

    References

    BOINSKI,S. (1987). Mating patterns in squirrel monkeys (Saimirioerstedi)._ Behav. Ecol.Sociobiol. 21, p. 13-21.

    COWLISHAW,G. & DUNBAR,R.I.M. (1991). Dominance rank and mating success in maleprimates. - An. Behav. 41, p. 1045-1056.

    DEFLER,T.R. (1987). Ranging and the use of space in a group of woolly monkeys (Lagothrix

    lagotricha)in the NW Amazon of Colombia. - Internat. J. Primatol. 8, p. 420.FEDIGAN,L.M. & BAXTER,M.J. (1984). Sex differences and social organization in free-

    ranging spider monkeys (Atelesgeoffroyi).- Primates 25, p. 279-294.FURUICHI,T. & IHOBE,H. (this volume). Variation in male relationships in bonobos and

    chimpanzees.GOUZOULES,S. & GOUZOULES,H. (1987). Kinship. - In: Primate societies. (B.B. SMUTS,

    D.L. CHENEY,R.M. SEYFARTH,R.W. WRANGHAM& T.T. STRUHSAKER,eds). Univer-sity of Chicago Press, Chicago, p. 299-305.

    HAMILTON,W.D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. I and II. -J. theor.Biol 12, p. 1-52.

    HILL,D.A. (this volume). Affiliative relations between adult males of the genus Macaca.

    VANHOOFF,J.A.R.A.M. & VANSCHAIK,C.P. (1992). Cooperation in competition: theecology of primate bonds. - In: Coalitions and alliances in humans and otheranimals. (H.A. HARCOURT& F.B.M. DEWAAL,eds). Oxford University Press, Oxford,p. 357-389._ _ &_ _ (this volume). Male bonds: affiliative relationships among nonhuman primatemales.

    LEMOSDES, R.M. (1988). Situao de uma populao de mono-carvoeiro, Brachytelesarachnoides,em fragmento de mata Atlntica (M.G.), e implicaotilde;espara sua conser-vao. - MA thesis, Universidade de Brasilia.

    _ _ & STRIER,K.B. (1992). A preliminary comparison of forest structure and use by twoisolated groups of woolly spider monkeys, Brachytelesarachnoides.- Biotropica 24,

    p.455-459.MENDES,F.D.C. (1990).Affilio e hierarquia no muriqui: o grupo Mato de Caratinga. -MA thesis, Universidade de So Paulo.

    MILTON,K. (1984). Habitat, diet, and activity patterns of free-ranging woolly spidermonkeys (BrachytelesarachnoidesE. Geoffroy 1806). - Internat. J. Primatol. 5, p.491-514.

  • 8/11/2019 Brotherhoods Atelines

    16/17

    166

    _ _ (1985). Mating patterns of woolly spider monkeys, Brachytelesarachnoides:implicationsfor female choice. - Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 17, p. 53-59.

    MITCHELL,C. (this volume). Migration alliances and coalitions among adult male SouthAmerican

    squirrel monkeys (Samirisciureus).MOORE,J. (1984). Female transfer in primates. - Internat. J. Primatol. 5, p. 537-589._ _ (1992). Dispersal, nepotism, and primate social behavior.

    - Internat. J. Primatol. 13,p. 361-378.

    NEWTON,P.N. (1988). The variable social organization of hanuman langurs (Presbytisentellus),infanticide, and the monopolization of females.

    _ _Internat. J. Primatol. 9, p.59-77.

    NISHIMURA,A. (1990a). A sociological and behavioral study of woolly monkeys, Lagothrixlagotricha,in the upper Amazon. - Sci. Eng. Rev. Doshisha Univ. 31, p. 87-121.

    _ _ (1990b). Mating behavior of woolly monkeys (Lagothrixlagotricha)at La Macarena,Colombia (II): mating relationships.

    - Field Studies of New World Monkeys, La

    Macarena,Colombia

    3, p.7-12.

    PACKER,C. (1979). Male dominance and reproductive activity in Papio anubis. - An.Behav. 107, p. 37-45.

    PUSEY,A.E. & PACKER,C. (1987). Dispersal and philopatry.- In: Primate societies. (B.B.

    SMUTS,D.L. CHENEY,R.M. SEYFARTH,R.W. WRANGHAM& T.T. STRUHSAKER,eds).University of Chicago Press, Chicago, p. 250-266.

    ROSENBERGER,A.L. & STRIER,K.B. (1989). Adaptive radiation of the ateline primates. -J.Hum. Evol. 18, p. 717-750.

    VANSCHAIK,C.P. (1989). The ecology of social relationships amongst female primates.-

    In : Comparative socioecology, the behavioural ecology of humans and other mam-mals. (V.S. STANTON& R.A. FOLEY,eds). Blackwell Scientific, Oxford, p. 195-218.

    SMUTS,B.B. &

    WATANABE, J.M.(1990).Social relationships and ritualized greetings in adult

    male baboons (Papiocynocephalusanubis)._ Internat. J. Primatol. 11, p. 147-172.SOINI,P. (1987). Ecology of Lagothrixlagotrichaon the Rio Pacaya, northeastern Peru.

    -

    Internat. J. Primatol. 8, p.421.STRIER,K.B. (1986). The behavior and ecology of the woolly spider monkey, or muriqui

    (BrachytelesarachnoidesE. Geoffroy 1806).- PhD thesis, Harvard University.

    _ _ (1987). Reproduo de Brachytelesarachnoides.- In: A primatologia no Brasil- 2. (M.

    THIAGO.DE.MELLO,ed.). Sociedade Brasileira de Primatologia, Brasilia, p. 163-175.

    _ _ (1989). Effectsof patch size on feeding associations in muriquis (Brachytelesarachnoides).- Folia Primatol 52, p. 70-77.

    _ _ (1990). New World primates, new frontiers: insights from the woolly spider monkey,or muriqui (Brachytelesarachnoides).- Internat. J. Primatol. 11, p. 7-19.

    _ _ (1991). Demography and conservation of an endangered primate, Brachytelesarach-noides.- Conserv. Biol. 5, p. 214-218.

    _ _ (1992a). Causes and consequences of nonaggression in the woolly spider monkey, or

    muriqui (Brachytelesarachnoides).- In: Aggression and peacefulness in humans andother primates. (J. SILVERBERG& J.P. GRAY,eds) Oxford University Press, New York,p. 100-116.

    _ _ (1992b). Atelinae adaptations: behavioral strategies and ecological constraints.-

    Amer. J. phys. Anthrop. 88, p. 515-524._ _ (1992c). Faces in the forest: the endangered muriqui monkeys of Brazil. Oxford

    University Press, New York._ _ (in press). Subtle cues of social relations in male muriqui monkeys (Brachytelesarach-

    noides).- In: New world primates: evolution, ecology and behavior (W.G. KINZEY,ed.). Aldine de Gruyter, New York.

    _ _, MENDES,F.D.C., RMOLI,J.&RMOLl,A.O. (1993). Demography and social structurein one group of muriquis (Brachytelesarachnoides).

    - Internat. J. Primatol. 14, p.513-526.

  • 8/11/2019 Brotherhoods Atelines

    17/17

    167

    SYMINGTON,M.M. (1987). Ecological and social correlates of party size in the black spidermonkey, Atelespaniscuschamek.- PhD thesis, Princeton University.

    _ _ (1990). Fission-fusion social organization in Atelesand Pan. _ Internat. J. Primatol.

    11, p. 47-61.VEHRENCAMP,S.L. (1983).A model for the evolution of despotic versus egalitarian societies._ Anim. Behav. 31, p. 667-682.

    WRANGHAM,R.W. (1980). An ecological model of female-bonded primate groups. -Behaviour 75, p. 262-299.

    YOUNG,A.L., RICHARD,A.F. & AIELLO,L.C. (1990). Female dominance and maternalinvestment in strepsirhine primates. - Am. Nat. 135, p. 473-488.