breeding behaviour and success of the tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file ›...

73
Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge -tailed eagle (Aquila audax fleayi ) Tierney O’Sullivan BSc (UTAS) 2014 A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements of the Degree of Bachelor of Science with Honours School of Biological Sciences University of Tasmania

Upload: others

Post on 30-May-2020

10 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

Breeding behaviour and success of the

Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle

(Aquila audax fleayi)

Tierney O’Sullivan BSc (UTAS)

2014

A thesis submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements of the Degree of

Bachelor of Science with Honours

School of Biological Sciences

University of Tasmania

Page 2: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

ii

Declaration

I hereby declare that this thesis contains no material which has previously been accepted

for the award of any other degree or diploma and contains no copy or paraphrase of

material previously published or written by any other person, except where due

reference is made in the text of this thesis.

Tierney O'Sullivan

26th

May 2014

Statement on authority to access

This thesis may be made available for loan and limited copying in accordance with the

Copyright Act 1968.

Page 3: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

iii

Abstract

Vertebrate predators, and raptors in particular, are of high conservation concern.

However, despite their high ecological importance, there is a paucity of knowledge of

many raptor species, including the threatened Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle, Aquila

audax fleayi. The Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle is sensitive to disturbance during the

breeding season, which has been qualitatively linked to breeding failure. Breeding

success is a product of breeding timing and behaviour on the nest, as well as biotic and

abiotic environmental factors. This study aimed to establish the critical abiotic and

biotic factors that influence the timing and success of breeding pairs throughout

Tasmania. On an individual level, it aimed to determine which behaviours wedge-tailed

eagles exhibit on their nest, how they respond to disturbance, and whether their

responses indicate habituation or sensitisation over time. Mixed effects modelling

techniques were employed to examine the impact of several candidate environmental

factors, including climate, prey availability, road density and nest density, on breeding

success and timing. Breeding success data was obtained from aerial surveys conducted

by the Forest Practices Authority (2007-present). Breeding success was negatively

associated with spring rainfall and nest density, while no significant association was

found with other variables (prey, temperature, and road density). Colder winters and

increased spring rainfall corresponded to later breeding times, while prey availability

did not have a significant impact. Behavioural responses to pedestrian disturbance

observed at 12 nests during this study supported the hypothesis that wedge-tailed eagles

are extremely sensitive to disturbance, with flushing off the nest a common response to

the observers’ approach. The behavioural responses to the approach worsened over time,

providing evidence for a tendency to sensitise to repeated disturbance. This sensitivity

was confirmed by the high levels of nest failure within the study nests (2 successful, 10

failed). Overall, this study identifies a number of interacting variables which can limit

wedge-tailed eagle reproduction. On a population level, rainfall, cold temperatures, and

nest density are associated with not only lower breeding success in Tasmanian wedge-

tailed eagles, but also delays in the timing of the breeding season. On an individual

scale, human disturbance alters breeding behaviour, and responses to disturbance (i.e.

flushing) can leave chicks or eggs exposed to the elements or predation, thus increasing

the likelihood of nest failure.

Page 4: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

iv

Acknowledgements

There are numerous people and bodies that supported this project, and without them it

would not have been possible.

Firstly I would like to thank my supervisors, Elissa Cameron, Amy Koch, and Clare

Hawkins. It was a pleasure to work with each of you, and I could not have asked for a

more inspiring group of women to guide me this year.

Thank you Jason Wiersma. Without your expertise and help in the field, I would

probably still be looking for eagle nests. Your passion for raptors is undeniable, and I

appreciate the time you took out of your schedule to make sure that this project went

ahead. Also, speaking of raptor experts, thanks to Bill Brown from the Department of

Primary Industry, Parks, Water and Environment for his advice.

Thanks to Scott Carver, Leon Barmuta, and Yuliya Karpievitch for taking time out of

their busy schedules to give statistical advice.

Anthony. You were hands down the volunteer of the century. From putting up with my

emotional breakdown, to agreeing to wake up at 1 am nightly so that we could hike to

nests, your dedication to this project was unwavering. I certainly would have been hard-

pressed to do it without your help.

Thanks to everyone who assisted with my fieldwork, including Nora, Dave, Sydney,

Simon, and Amy. It wasn’t exactly glamorous work, but you all helped me get the job

done.

I would not be here if it was not for the Fulbright grant, so I’d like to heartily thank the

Australian-American Fulbright commission for selecting me and this project and for

their support this year. It has been an incredible experience and I am lucky to be part of

such a wonderful program.

This project would not have been possible without financial support from industry

partners including Norske Skog, Forestry Tasmania, and SFM Forest Products. In

particular, Sandra Hetherington, Marie Yee, Daniel Hodge, Gareth Tempest, and

Suzette Weeding for their interest in the project and their help with the nest selection

process. Additionally, I would like to thanks the landowners who allowed us access to

their property. A large proportion of the study nests we visited were on private land, so

their cooperation and willingness to help us find the nests was critical.

I would also like to thank the Forest Practices Authority and its wonderful staff for

support of the project, providing a long-term data set for me to analyse, and for the

student grant that allowed me to purchase the hide equipment. Additionally, the

Threatened Species Section and other staff at DPIPWE, particularly Bill Wilson and

Allison Woolley, for recognising the merit of this project and supporting me throughout.

Page 5: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

v

Additionally, thanks to Greg Hocking, Mike Driesson, and Eric Schwarz from DPIPWE

for providing the spotlighting data for my analyses.

Coming from the U.S., I was not sure what to expect from Honours, and it turned out to

be much more challenging that I had envisioned. But thanks to my fellow Honours

students, Chris Kozakiewicz, Alyson Stobo-Wilson, Jenny Lewis, Lily Leahy, and

Lauren Richards I made it through and came out with some great friends.

Thanks to the graduate students for their help and advice along the way including

Shannon Troy, Amy Edwards, Bronwyn Fancourt, and Nick Beeton.

And last but not least, my partner Simon and all my housemates for supporting me this

year and giving me something to look forward to after uni. Also, thanks to my family

and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout the year and

being supportive of me even though I’m so far away. Particularly my parents with their

numerous care packages, which made me feel right at home.

This work was carried out under the University of Tasmania Animal Ethics Permit

number A13381.

Page 6: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

vi

Contents

Declaration ....................................................................................................................... ii

Abstract............................................................................................................................ iii

Acknowledgements ......................................................................................................... iv

Contents ........................................................................................................................... vi

1. Introduction ................................................................................................................1

Study species..................................................................................................................7

Study aims......................................................................................................................9

2. Methods .......................................................................................................................11

Study area ....................................................................................................................11

Data collection .............................................................................................................11

Breeding timing ........................................................................................................11

Breeding success ......................................................................................................12

Environmental variables ...........................................................................................14

Behavioural observations .........................................................................................16

Data analysis ................................................................................................................21

Breeding timing and success ....................................................................................21

Behavioural study .....................................................................................................23

3. Results ......................................................................................................................24

Breeding timing ...........................................................................................................24

Breeding success ..........................................................................................................26

Behavioural observations .............................................................................................30

4. Discussion....................................................................................................................37

Breeding timing ...........................................................................................................37

Breeding success ..........................................................................................................39

Breeding behaviour and response to disturbance ........................................................43

Areas for further research ............................................................................................46

Synthesis ......................................................................................................................47

5. References ...................................................................................................................49

6. Appendix 1………………………………………………………………………………………. 65

Page 7: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

1

1. Introduction

Vertebrate predators are of special concern for biodiversity conservation (Gittleman et

al. 2001; Sergio et al. 2006; Letnic & Koch 2010). They play a regulatory role in

maintaining ecosystem structure and function, as evidenced by widespread and dramatic

shifts following the declines of predators in marine, terrestrial, and freshwater

ecosystems worldwide (Schmitz, Hamback & Beckerman 2000; Smith, Peterson &

Houston 2003; Finke & Denno 2004; Estes et al. 2011). Furthermore, predators have

intrinsic properties that make them more vulnerable than other species, and they are

threatened at a disproportionate rate, with recent mass extinctions characterised by apex

predators (Ray et al. 2005; Estes et al. 2011). Predators face threats that affect most

animals, including habitat loss and disease (Butler 2004; McCallum 2008; Forero-

Medina et al. 2009), but also additional threats, including vulnerability due to naturally

low population densities stemming from a need for expansive foraging territories

(Gittleman & Harvey 1982; Grant, Chapman & Richardson 1992). As a result of their

wide-ranging nature and real or perceived conflict with humans and domestic animals,

predators can also be a target of human persecution (Treves & Karanth 2003; Graham,

Beckerman & Thirgood 2005; Ogada & Keesing 2010; Ahmed et al. 2012).

Raptors, as predatory birds, face similar challenges to other predators, including

intrinsically low population densities (Schoener 1969), persecution (Green et al. 2004;

Thiollay 2006) and disease (Schettler et al. 2003; Ellis et al. 2007), as well as special

challenges such as bioaccumulation of toxins (Dykstra et al. 1998; Pain et al. 2005;

Perez-Lopez et al. 2008), and collision with anthropogenic structures (Sergio et al. 2004;

Tapia, Dominguez & Rodriguez 2009; Dahl et al. 2012; Hull et al. 2013). Raptors are

threatened at a higher rate than non-predatory bird species: 17.3% of Falconiformes and

Strigiformes species are listed as threatened, compared to 12.8% of all other bird

species (IUCN 2013). To aid their conservation, numerous management efforts have

been put in place to help protect raptor species worldwide (McCollough, Todd & Owen

1994; Bell & Mooney 1999; Lehman, Kennedy & Savidge 2007). However, these

efforts are costly, and their effectiveness is dependent on the scientific research

available, so ongoing research and monitoring is essential for improving the efficacy of

these campaigns.

Page 8: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

2

Despite the ecological importance of raptors as apex predators (Sergio et al. 2006), and

the high proportion of species that are threatened, there is a paucity of basic knowledge

of many raptor species (Bamford et al. 2007; Whitfield, Ruddock & Bullman 2008).

Many raptor species are intrinsically difficult to study due to low population densities,

high agility, and avoidance of humans, so much of our knowledge of raptors is based on

expert opinion (Whitfield, Ruddock & Bullman 2008). Of the research conducted on

raptors, breeding success has been a primary focus, as nests are easier to study than

highly agile birds (Newton 1979). Raptors have intrinsically low reproductive rates due

to a number of life history traits (e.g. small clutch sizes, protracted nest dependence of

offspring, and late sexual maturity; Amadon 1964), and low reproductive rates

contribute to population decline in species of conservation concern (Dykstra et al. 1998;

González et al. 2006b).

Reproductive rates typically vary between years (Steenhof & Newton 2007), and

depending on the measure of breeding success, observed breeding performance can be

influenced by territory occupancy (Sergio & Newton 2003), probability of birds nesting

within each territory, the nesting success of individual breeding pairs, and fledgling

survival (McIntyre & Schmidt 2012). Different studies often utilize different measures,

so comparing breeding performance between species is not necessarily accurate,

particularly in species with supernumerary nests (Steenhof & Newton 2007).

The timing of the breeding season and breeding behaviour are both adapted to increase

the likelihood of breeding success while minimising costs to adults, particularly in long-

lived raptor species (Hanssen et al. 2005; Shawkey et al. 2009; D'Alba, Oborn &

Shawkey 2010). Thus, alterations to the timing or behavioural patterns of breeding can

impact breeding success (Bart & Tornes 1989; Samelius & Alisauskas 2001; Duncan

Rastogi, Zanette & Clinchy 2006; Verhulst & Nilsson 2008). The timing of a bird’s

breeding season, typically defined as the laying date, or start date of incubation

(Steenhof & Newton 2007), has been shown to affect nest success, in that early breeders

tend to produce more numerous and higher quality young (Gargett 1977; Krüger 2004;

Steenhof & Newton 2007; Verhulst & Nilsson 2008; Koch, Martin & Aitken 2012).

There are strong impacts of breeding behaviour on timing and success, as higher nest

attendance is correlated with higher breeding success (Samelius & Alisauskas 2001) and

shorter incubation periods (Wiebe, Wiehn & Korpimäki 1998). Though breeding timing

Page 9: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

3

and behaviour are highly adapted, they can exhibit plasticity, and can vary based on

changes in intrinsic and extrinsic, biotic and abiotic factors.

Breeding success is linked to a number of biotic and abiotic factors that influence

breeding on both an individual and population level. Factors that commonly influence

breeding in raptors include bird age (Penteriani, Balbontin & Ferrer 2003), weather

(Gargett, Gargett & Damania 1995; Steenhof, Kochert & McDonald 1997; Steidl, Kozie

& Anthony 1997; Krüger 2004), prey availability (Dykstra et al. 1998; Martin et al.

2009; Whitfield et al. 2009; McIntyre & Schmidt 2012), individual characteristics of the

breeding pair (e.g. plumage morph) (Krüger 2004), nest characteristics (Radford &

Plessis 2003; Jiménez-Franco, Martínez & Calvo 2014), and human influence

(Holthuijzen et al. 1990; Kaisanlahti-Jokimäki et al. 2008; Lambertucci & Speziale

2009). The degree to which these variables influence breeding success, timing, and

behaviour is often species and habitat specific (Kostrzewa & Kostrzewa 1990; Olsen &

Olsen 1992).

Weather, particularly temperature and rainfall extremes, can affect breeding success,

timing and behaviour. Species that breed at high latitudes tend to rely on photic cues

and thus have highly synchronous breeding seasons (Garamszegi et al. 2008), while

birds at lower latitudes tend to have longer and more variable breeding seasons with

increasing reliance on weather cues (Zann et al. 1995; MacDougall-Shackleton & Hahn

2007). Temperature is a cue for many birds to begin breeding (Visser, Holleman & Caro

2009), with warm temperatures leading to an early onset of incubation (Ardia, Cooper

& Dhont 2006). Colder temperatures during the winter and spring relate to later

breeding times and a lower proportion of egg-laying pairs of golden eagles, Aquila

chrysaetos, in North America (Steenhof, Kochert & McDonald 1997). The onset and

success of the breeding season can be dependent on rainfall (Zann et al. 1995; Lloyd

1999), particularly in arid environments, where higher rainfall corresponds to higher

prey abundance (Ridpath & Brooker 1985; Gargett, Gargett & Damania 1995).

However, in areas with higher average rainfall, inclement weather conditions prior to

and during incubation and brood rearing stages can reduce hunting efficiency and be

detrimental to offspring survival (Gargett 1977; Hustler & Howells 1986; Kostrzewa &

Kostrzewa 1990; Radford & Plessis 2003; Bionda & Brambilla 2012). Weather can also

Page 10: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

4

influence breeding behaviour, with higher nest attentiveness in adverse conditions, and

reduced risk of flushing when disturbed (Beer 1961; Steidl & Anthony 2000).

Prey availability also plays a role in determining the onset of incubation and breeding

success of many raptors, particularly in species with specialised diets (Hanski, Hansson

& Henttonen 1991; Gargett, Gargett & Damania 1995; iebe, orpim ki iehn

1998). Reproduction comes at a high energy cost to breeding birds, and requires a

substantial allocation of nutrients, which must be either internally drawn from body

reserves or obtained externally from food resources at the onset of and during breeding

(Jones & Ward 1976; Krapu 1981; Wiebe, Wiehn & Korpimäki 1998). Thus, it is

expected that higher prey populations correspond to earlier breeding attempts

(Kostrzewa & Kostrzewa 1990; Steenhof, Kochert & McDonald 1997) and more

consistent breeding timing between individuals (Ridpath & Brooker 1985). Since

generalist raptor species can supplement their diet from a number of prey species

(Harder 2000; Ontiveros & Pleguezneles 2000), the impact of prey availability on

breeding is greater in areas where prey is a limiting resource for raptors (González et al.

2006b; McIntyre & Schmidt 2012). If prey is a limiting resource, its availability can

impact breeding success in two ways: 1) low prey abundance prior to the breeding

season can result in fewer pairs laying eggs, likely due to a minimum body condition

required prior to incubation (Jones & Ward 1976; Steenhof, Kochert & McDonald

1997); and 2) low prey availability during the breeding season could reduce the

likelihood of breeding success, with adults requiring more time for foraging to maintain

their own condition and provide for nestlings (Steenhof, Kochert & McDonald 1997;

Salamolard et al. 2000; González et al. 2006b). Prey availability may also influence

breeding behaviour, as some raptors exhibit lower nest attentiveness (Wiebe, Wiehn &

Korpimäki 1998; Dewey & Kennedy 2001), or are less tolerant to human disturbance

during periods of food stress (Van der Zande & Verstrael 1985; White & Thurow 1985).

Human activities can significantly influence raptor breeding success and behaviour, and

in a small number of studies have been linked to the timing of the breeding season

(Millsap et al. 2004). Some raptor species respond positively to human-altered

environments, and have enhanced breeding performance in suburban and urban habitats

(Chace & Walsh 2006; Stout, Temple & Papp 2006). However, the majority of raptor

species are more sensitive to human disturbance and exhibit lower breeding success in

Page 11: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

5

areas with greater human presence (Krüger 2004). Human disturbance occurs in a

number of ways, including direct disturbance that reduces habitat physically (e.g.

logging), or indirect disturbances which are non-consumptive activities (e.g. recreation,

traffic) that temporarily exclude animals from optimal habitats or alter behaviours

required for survival and reproduction (Steidl & Anthony 2000). Even scientific

methods of checking nests for activity can have a negative influence on breeding

success (Grier 1969; Westmoreland & Best 1985; Gotmark 1992; Carney & Sydeman

1999).

Human disturbance can affect the breeding behaviour of raptors in both subtle and overt

ways, with corresponding impacts on their reproduction (Steidl & Anthony 2000). A

number of studies have examined breeding behaviour of raptors (Levenson 1979;

Bollen 1991; Bryan et al. 2005), and the response of raptors to disturbance outside of

the breeding season (Stalmaster & Newman 1978; Holmes et al. 1993; Stalmaster &

Kaiser 1997). However, the majority of studies examining breeding success do not take

behaviour into account, leaving a gap in knowledge of how behaviour can influence nest

success (Steenhof & Newton 2007). Substantial disturbance can cause adults to abandon

nests, or flush off nests temporarily (Mooney & Holdsworth 1991; Whitfield et al.

2003). Flushing is the most common ‘disturbed’ behaviour reported (Grubb & King

1991; Richardson & Miller 1997) and can have detrimental effects on chick health,

ranging from periodic cooling resulting in prolonged incubation periods (Lyon &

Montgomerie 1985; Nilsson & Henrik 1988), reduced immunity and body condition of

nestlings (Ardia, Perez & Clotfelter 2010), to nest failure due to embryonic death or

nest predation (Martin et al. 2007). Prolonged incubations can result in later hatch dates

and thus disadvantage breeding adults as well as chicks. As incubation is associated

with high energetic costs for breeding adults, prolonged incubation could result in lower

future fecundity as it has been linked to reduced immune function and weight loss in

long-lived bird species (Hanssen et al. 2005). Prolonged incubation also has negative

impacts on chicks by reducing time available for acquiring flying and foraging skills

before winter when daylight hours are shorter and conditions are less favourable, thus

reducing offspring fitness (Martin 1987). Minor disturbances can induce more subtle

behavioural or physiological changes that may lower adult fitness, but these subtle

Page 12: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

6

changes are much harder to detect and thus have less of a presence in the literature

(White & Thurow 1985; González et al. 2006a).

Nest switching, or the use of alternative nests in subsequent breeding seasons, is a

behaviour that has long been associated as a response to disturbance, but recent findings

suggest that it may have other foundations. Nest reuse is lower in territories with more

alternative nests (Saga & Selås 2012). Switching between alternative nests in a territory

can increase breeding success by reducing the prevalence of ectoparasites on nests

(Ontiveros, Caro & Pleguezuelos 2008b; Ontiveros, Caro & Pleguezuelos 2008a).

Switching may also be necessary if a nest is usurped by competitors (Ontiveros, Caro &

Pleguezuelos 2008b). Nest switching and the subsequent high number of alternative

nests has also been hypothesised to increase with nest failure (Brown 1976; Newton

1979), particularly following disturbance events (De Santo et al. 2002; Saga & Selås

2012). However, the link between alternative nests and disturbance has remained

largely untested and recent studies of eagle nests have not supported the hypothesis

(Ontiveros, Caro & Pleguezuelos 2008b; Kochert & Steenhof 2012).

Sensitivity to disturbance is variable in eagles, possibly indicating habituation to

repeated disturbance in tolerant breeding pairs, particularly if pairs nest successfully in

areas with consistent human activity (Knight 1995; Richardson & Miller 1997).

However, there are few studies that experimentally test whether eagles habituate or

sensitise to disturbance (Steidl & Anthony 2000). Steidl and Anthony (2000) found

evidence for habituation of bald eagles, Haliaeetus leucocephalus, to disturbances

lasting for prolonged periods of time, whereupon their behaviour tended to resemble the

behaviour found at undisturbed nests. However, the number of vocalisations, which was

significantly higher in disturbed nests than controls, did not decrease over the length of

the disturbance, suggesting that birds were still disturbed even though most of their

behaviours approached those of undisturbed nests.

Classification of individuals or species as tolerant to disturbance based solely on single

observations of behavioural responses to disturbances is insufficient. Perceived

tolerance or habituation to human activities can be misleading, as disturbed animals can

respond in non-behavioural ways: physiologically (e.g. higher levels of stress hormones,

or glucocorticoids) (Culik, Adelung & Woakes 1990), or facing long term declines in

fitness (Millsap et al. 2004). A recent study conducted by Strasser and Heath (2013)

Page 13: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

7

examining the effects of anthropogenic disturbance on a ‘tolerant’ raptor species, the

European kestrel (Falco tinnunculus), found that proximity to human development was

common, but was associated with higher levels of glucocorticoids and nest

abandonment. Bald eagles have also been described as tolerant to human disturbance,

with nests in suburban areas often producing chicks (Nesbitt 2001; Millsap et al. 2004).

However, recent findings show that the chicks from suburban nests have significantly

reduced survival within a year of fledging compared to chicks from more rural areas

(Millsap et al. 2004). Thus, the apparent tolerance of anthropogenic activities by some

species or individuals may not necessarily be the case and these animals may be in an

ecological trap, which is typified by animals selecting low quality and often human-

modified habitat (Gill, Norris & Sutherland 2001; Battin 2004). Behavioural studies

coupled with longitudinal studies on fitness or reproductive output can provide evidence

for the true costs of human disturbance to fitness or reproductive ability.

Study species

The Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle, Aquila audax fleayi, is one of Tasmania’s few

remaining apex predators. Of the island state’s four large marsupial carnivores, one (the

thylacine, Thylacinus cynocephalus) is extinct and two are listed as threatened (the

spotted-tailed quoll, Dasyurus maculatus, and the Tasmanian devil, Sarcophilus harrisii)

(State Government of Tasmania 1995; McCallum & Jones 2006; Byholm & Kekkonen

2008). The other avian apex predator, the white-bellied sea eagle (Haliaeetus

leucogaster) is also listed as threatened in Tasmania (State Government of Tasmania

1995) and has a more confined distribution than A. a. fleayi, as it tends to nest close to

coastal areas and inland lakes (Thurstans 2009).

The Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle has been classified as a separate subspecies of the

wedge-tailed eagle present on the mainland of Australia, Aquila audax audax. Mainland

adults have darker plumages (Bell & Mooney 1999); while Tasmanian adults have a

larger body size (Nankervis 2010), are obligate tree nesters with smaller clutch sizes

(Brown & Mooney 1997), and exhibit higher sensitivity during the breeding season

(Mooney & Holdsworth 1991). However, recent genetic research has suggested that the

divergence between the mainland population and the Tasmanian population is relatively

Page 14: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

8

recent and that phenotypic differences may be due to strong influences of founding

individuals (Burridge et al. 2013).

The Tasmanian subspecies is listed as threatened on both a state and federal level (State

Government of Tasmania 1995 ; Commonwealth of Australia 1999). Principal threats

include loss of nesting habitat, disturbance of nesting birds, collision with man-made

structures, and persecution; though persecution by humans is becoming less prevalent

(Starker Leopold & Wolfe 1970; Gaffney & Mooney 1992). The high sensitivity to

disturbance at nest sites has been documented for decades, and human activities near

nests have been linked to nest failures and nest abandonments (Mooney & Holdsworth

1991; Gaffney & Mooney 1992; Mooney & Taylor 1996), but the majority of reports

have been qualitative. Breeding pairs exhibit significant variation in their tolerance to

humans, with examples of tolerant pairs breeding successfully in suburban areas

(Harder 2000). The mechanism for the increased tolerance (e.g. via genetic factors or

habituation) is unknown.

The Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle has relatively specific breeding habitat requirements,

with nests primarily found in emergent trees in sheltered patches of old-growth native

forest (Brown & Mooney 1997). Foraging requirements are much less specific.

Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagles are generalists and opportunistic predators and can

forage capably over most types of terrestrial habitat (Gaffney & Mooney 1992). On the

mainland of Australia A. a. audax diets vary regionally, with the predominant prey

species of rabbits and hares, birds, kangaroos, or reptiles, depending on the habitat in

question (Starker Leopold & Wolfe 1970; Brooker & Ridpath 1980; Ridpath & Brooker

1985; Harder 2000; Collins & Croft 2007). The diet of the Tasmanian wedge-tailed

eagle is similarly varied and is comprised of both live prey and carrion. Known prey

species include rabbits, hares, cats, wallabies, possums, echidnas, wombats, birds, sheep,

goats, and reptiles (Gaffney & Mooney 1992).

The ecological importance of the wedge-tailed eagle in Tasmania, coupled with its

endangered status, has resulted in measures to aid in the conservation of this subspecies.

Environmental regulators impose significant constraints on human and industry

activities around eagle nests (FPA 2012). Current management prescriptions carried out

in areas covered by the Forest Practices System include pre-harvest surveys for nests,

establishment of reserves around known nests (minimum 10 ha), and restrictions on

Page 15: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

9

activities within 500 m or 1 km line-of-sight of active nests during the breeding season

(FPA 2012). Similar conditions are generally required by other state government

regulators for activities including quarrying, dam works, mining, and other

developments (C. Hawkins, pers. comm.). As there are over 1,400 known Tasmanian

wedge-tailed eagle nests (DPIPWE 2013), and the breeding season restrictions occur

from July to January or February, the management implications of eagle nest avoidance

are costly for a dominant sector of Tasmanian industry.

Understanding the factors driving the observed variation in breeding success and timing

of the Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle could help guide future management. Ongoing

nest surveys indicate that the timing of the breeding season and overall nest success

varies between years and individual nests, and that earlier breeding seasons may

correspond to higher breeding performance (Koch, Wiersma & Munks 2012). However,

reasons for the annual and inter-individual variation in nest success and breeding timing

of the Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle are unknown.

The limited knowledge of the Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle, combined with its

endangered status and costly management provisions, call for a greater understanding of

its ecology, particularly during the breeding season when it is most sensitive. Much of

the knowledge of the breeding behaviour and impacts of disturbance on the Tasmanian

wedge-tailed eagle is based on expert opinion and limited quantitative data, or

extrapolated knowledge from the mainland subspecies. Though expert opinion can be a

valuable and realistic resource, its use should be temporary until more empirical data are

gathered (Whitfield, Ruddock & Bullman 2008). Additionally, since Tasmania differs

from the mainland in a number of characteristics including vegetation, climate, and

species composition, and the Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle is phenotypically different

from the mainland subspecies in many ways (Burridge et al. 2013), extrapolating

knowledge about the mainland subspecies to the lesser-studied Tasmanian subspecies is

not necessarily accurate.

Study aims

The timing of the breeding season, breeding success, and breeding behaviour are all

interrelated factors influencing both adult and chick fitness in the short-term, and

population dynamics over the long-term, so understanding these factors is likely to be

Page 16: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

10

essential to the conservation of any raptor species. In this study, I examined the

breeding success of the Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle on two scales. On a population

scale, I examined possible explanations for the variation in the timing of the breeding

season and breeding success of a large sample of Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle nests

over multiple breeding seasons. On an individual scale, I investigated breeding

behaviour and breeding success through intensive behavioural surveys on a subset of

Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle nests. I incorporated these two approaches to address the

following aims:

1. Timing of the breeding season: Determine if weather and prey influence the timing of

the breeding season in the Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle.

2. Breeding success: Estimate the level of nest failure state-wide, and test the impact of

weather, prey, and disturbance variables on the likelihood of nest success in the

Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle.

3. Breeding behaviour: Determine the breeding behaviour of individual breeding pairs,

their behavioural responses to disturbance, and test whether their responses to repeated

disturbance indicate habituation or sensitisation over time.

Due to the variability in the climate of Tasmania, particularly with high rainfall in the

winter and spring, I hypothesised that colder, wetter weather prior to the breeding

season would correspond to delays in the onset of incubation and lower breeding

success. Since the Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle is a generalist predator, I expected

only a weak relationship with prey abundance, but that greater prey abundance would

correspond to earlier and more successful breeding seasons. I also hypothesised that

human activity would correspond to lower nest success by altering adult behaviour and

increasing the likelihood of nest failure. However, due to the incidence of ‘tolerant’

breeding pairs I expected that breeding birds habituate and behavioural responses to

repeated disturbance would lessen over time.

Page 17: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

11

2. Methods

Study area

Nests included in the study are distributed across both public and private lands

throughout the island of Tasmania, Australia, which is located 240 km south of the

mainland of Australia (Figure 1). With an area of 68,401 km2, Tasmania is centred at

approximately 42° S, 147° E. Tasmania’s temperate maritime climate is characterised

by mild summers, and cool wet winters. Due to variation in elevation (0 – 1,617 m),

geology, and climate throughout the state, Tasmania’s vegetation is defined by a

complex mosaic of communities ranging from button grass moorland to wet sclerophyll

forests (Williams 2012).

Data collection

Breeding timing

The timing of the breeding season, (i.e. the start date of incubation) was estimated using

data on chicks of known age, and extrapolated using information on the timing of the

different stages of breeding (Koch, Wiersma & Munks 2012). Nestlings were aged from

two sources: direct observations during aerial surveys conducted by the FPA (see below

for details), and photographs submitted by forest planners. All nests with chicks that

could be aged with confidence were included in the analysis, irrespective of the data

source. The age of nestlings can be estimated by size, colour, and feather development,

which are techniques utilised for many raptor species, including the wedge-tailed eagle

(Sergio & Bogliani 1999; Olsen 2005; Penteriani et al. 2005; Debus et al. 2007; Bionda

& Brambilla 2012). Hatching dates were obtained by backdating from feather

development of nestlings (Steenhof & Newton 2007), while the start date of incubation

was obtained by subtracting 45 days, the average incubation period, from the hatching

date (Debus et al. 2007; Koch, Wiersma & Munks 2012). Given changes in the

availability of data, the number of chicks included in the breeding timing for each year

was variable (Table 1) (Koch, Wiersma & Munks 2012).

Page 18: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

12

Breeding success

To assess the breeding success of the Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle, nests were selected

from the Raptor Nest Database in a variety of land use types, including state forest and

private land across Tasmania (DPIPWE 2013). The nests were monitored from a fixed

wing aircraft during November and early December each year from 2007-2013 by the

Tasmanian Forest Practices Authority (FPA) (Koch, Wiersma & Munks 2012). The

nests surveyed varied slightly between years (Table 1). Nests that could be adequately

surveyed (i.e. a good view into the nest was achieved) were classified into one of two

categories: nests containing a nestling were considered successful, while nests not

containing a nestling were considered unsuccessful. Unsuccessful nests include both

those where breeding was attempted and failed and nests that were not attempted at all.

An additional classification was added during the 2012 breeding season, where nests

that were unsuccessful were also defined as having “recent material” or “no recent

material” to estimate nest usage in addition to nest success.

In an effort to include only independent nests, of those that were surveyed in the same

year and were less than 3 km apart, only a single nest was included in the analysis (nests

with more years of data were selected). Nests within 3 km of one another are likely to

be within the same breeding territory as the estimated wedge-tailed eagle territory size

is 30 km2 in optimal habitat (Bell & Mooney 1999). However, some territories may be

larger, so it is possible that some nests included in the analysis belonged to the same

breeding territories. More details regarding methods of nest selection and survey used

during the first 5 years of monitoring can be found in Koch, Wiersma and Munks

(2012).

Page 19: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

13

Figure 1. The location of wedge-tailed eagle nests (n=292) included in the current study. Nests

were surveyed via aircraft between 1 and 7 times throughout the survey period (2007-2013).

Table 1. The number of wedge-tailed eagle nests included in the modelling of the breeding

success and timing from each year of surveys.

Year Number of nests included in

breeding success analysis

Number of nests included in

breeding timing analysis

2007 86 31

2008 68 12

2009 139 26

2010 126 22

2011 145 20

2012 141 50

2013 139 36

Page 20: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

14

Environmental variables

I developed a preliminary set of 10 candidate environmental variables considered likely

to influence the breeding success of the Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle, based on

findings for similar species as outlined in the Introduction. I included variables

describing climate, disturbance, prey availability, and number of nearby nests. A subset

of the predictor variables expected to influence nest success, specifically rainfall,

temperature, and prey availability, is also likely to influence the timing of the breeding

season (Steenhof, Kochert & McDonald 1997).

Climatic variables were obtained for each of the nest coordinates from the Australian

Water Availability Project database (Raupach et al. 2009). The variables considered

were monthly minimum recorded temperatures (ºC), and total monthly rainfall (mm) for

winter (June-August) and spring (September-November) of each year, since these

variables and seasons have been shown to impact the breeding success of other raptors

(Steenhof, Kochert & McDonald 1997; Bionda & Brambilla 2012).

The level of human disturbance at each nest site was estimated by calculating two

measures of roads near each nest, as the presence of roads is generally correlated with

increased use of areas by humans (Trombulak & Frissell 2000). Road presence was

determined using the LIST 2009 Transport Layer as maintained by the Department of

Infrastructure, Energy, and Resources using ArcGIS v 10.1 (ESRI 2013). All road

classes and types were considered, from highways to unsealed private roads, but

walking tracks were excluded. The first measure used the total length of roads (m)

within 1 km of each nest, while the second is the distance (m) between each nest and the

nearest road.

Prey availability was estimated from the Department of Primary Industries, Parks,

ater and Environment’s spotlighting data. From 1985 to the present, native and non-

native mammal observations across Tasmania were recorded through spotlighting once-

yearly from a vehicle along 10 km line transects during November-March (Southwell &

Fletcher 1993). I obtained a mean of the prey counts at each transect for the total

number of yearly surveys conducted, including all native and non-native species. For

each year of interest, the relative prey abundance was calculated by taking a ratio of the

given year’s total prey count and dividing it by the overall mean for that transect to give

Page 21: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

15

an estimate of the yearly variation from the mean. In order to reduce the random

variation that can occur from transect to transect, the prey abundance for each nest was

estimated as a mean of the nearest 6 transects to each nest.

The final predictor I examined was the number of known nests within 3 km of each nest,

as an estimate of the number of alternative nests in each breeding territory. This

information was collected using data from the raptor nest database (DPIPWE 2013).

Each of the continuous variables was examined for normality. In an effort to improve

the normality of the variables with non-normal distributions, data were either log-

transformed or square root transformed. No highly correlated variables (Pearson’s

correlation coefficient, r > 0.7) were included in the models to avoid instability in

parameter estimation (Quinn & Keough 2002). To determine which of the correlated

variables to include in the analysis, I used R (R Core Team 2013) and the lme4 package

(Bates et al. 2014) to compare generalized linear mixed effects models (GLMMs) of

single variables. Models containing each correlated variable were compared, and those

with lower AIC values were used for later models. As a result of this preliminary

correlation analysis, four predictor variables were removed from candidacy, leaving six

variables as candidates for further modelling of breeding success, and three for

modelling breeding timing (Table 2).

Page 22: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

16

Table 2. Environmental variables included in wedge-tailed eagle breeding success and breeding

timing modelling, with transformations done and expected impacts. All variables listed were

included in modelling breeding success; those marked with * were also used to model breeding

timing.

Predictor

variable

Predictor variable

description

Transformation Expected

response on

success

Expected

response on

timing

roads 1k Total distance of roads

(m) within 1 km of nest

Square root Positive

near nests Number of nests within

3 km of nest

None Negative

winter rain Total amount of winter

rainfall (mm)

Square root Negative

spring rain* Total amount of spring

rainfall (mm)

Square root Negative Delayed

winter

min.*

Winter mean minimum

temperature (ºC)

None Positive Delayed

prey* Relative prey

availability

Square root Positive Earlier

Behavioural observations

Methods to observe eagle behaviour on the nest involved practices not normally

permitted for standard eagle nest activity surveys. All efforts were made to design

methods that would minimise additional disturbance, as detailed below, but it was

recognised that the work might present a risk to eagle breeding success. Methods were

reviewed and approved by both the Threatened Species and Marine Section and Animal

Ethics Committee of DPIPWE on the basis of the anticipated conservation benefits of

the findings, and the recognition that this research would involve a one-off procedure

rather than a standard, widely used approach.

Over 40 potential nest sites from the Raptor Nest Database were identified for this study

(DPIPWE 2013). Aerial survey data and forest planner knowledge were used to select

potential nest sites based on the presence of good viewing points into the nest, and nests

Page 23: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

17

that have been regularly used by breeding birds in the past. Each of the potential nests

was visited in August or the first week of September to assess if it was likely to be used

in the 2013 breeding season. If a good vantage point was found and there was evidence

that the nest might be used for breeding (e.g. had recent nesting material added, Gaffney

& Mooney 1992), a small tent was erected 100-200 m from the nest. Efforts were made

to ensure the nest assessment and tent erection created minimal disturbance. If eagles

were present at or around nest sites, a tent was only erected if it could be done in less

than 20 minutes in order to reduce the amount of time spent in the vicinity of the nest

site. This provision was only required at a single nest, at which eagles had already

commenced incubation.

Behavioural observations from the hides were done during the incubation and early

nestling stages of the breeding season, when adults were consistently attending nests.

The activity status of each nest (i.e. whether or not the nest is in use during a particular

breeding season) was also recorded for each of the study sites. During the behavioural

observations nests were recorded as active if there was an incubating bird at the nest and

the presence of eggs or chicks was determined. If there were no eagles present and no

signs of recent use on the nest, nests were recorded as inactive. The distance between

each study nest and the nearest road was recorded as an estimate of the level of human

disturbance for each site. Of the 16 nests selected for the study, 12 were active and 4

were not active during the 2013-2014 breeding season (Figure 2).

Page 24: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

18

Figure 2. Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle nests included in the behavioural study and their

activity status (black = active, n=12; red = inactive, n=4) during the 2013-2014 breeding season.

The nearest vehicle access point was a minimum of 300 m from each nest and out of

line-of-sight. For the behavioural surveys I walked to the hide accompanied by a co-

researcher. Once we arrived at the hide, I entered and began to record behavioural data,

while the co-researcher returned to the vehicle. Having an accompanying person to

leave the hide after I had entered it was recommended by wedge-tailed eagle experts as

a technique established to minimise disturbance to sensitive bird species. The goal of

this ‘walk-in’ technique is for the birds to view the second person leaving the vicinity of

the nest and subsequently return to an ‘undisturbed’ state (Beer 1961).

The study began when the incubation phase was expected to be underway, and finished

when the chicks produced were approximately 4 weeks old (the start of October until

the first week of December). Nests were not visited during the period likely to be the

first week of incubation to minimise the level of disturbance at this more sensitive time

period (Richardson & Miller 1997).

I employed a combination of sampling methods to record behavioural observations.

Each survey period lasted approximately 2.5 hours. To get an estimate of the time

eagles engaged in prolonged behaviours, or states, the “scan sampling” method was

Page 25: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

19

employed. For this method the eagle’s activities were recorded at 5 minute intervals in

order to determine the percentage of time spent in certain activities. Examples of

behavioural states include sitting on the nest, or preening. Because the scan sampling

method tends to underrepresent shorter-lasting behaviours, or behavioural events, I also

employed the “all-occurrence” method to record each time that behavioural events

occurred, along with a corresponding time. From the all-occurrence method, I obtained

a frequency of the behavioural events. Examples of behavioural events include

vocalisations, or arrivals and departures from the nest. The combination of the two

methods enabled me to record not only a frequency of behaviours that occurred, but also

the time spent engaged in each behaviour. In the event of a flushing response, the time

to return (defined as the length of time an adult was away from the nest after flushing)

was calculated. Behaviours were divided into two categories: relaxed and disturbed,

based on a priori recommendations from wedge-tailed eagle experts and reviewing

literature that documented common raptor responses to disturbance (Table 3).

Page 26: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

20

Table 3. Terminology used in describing breeding behaviours of Tasmanian wedge-tailed

eagles, along with their categorisation into ‘relaxed’ or ‘disturbed’ categories, if applicable.

Term Definition Relaxed/Disturbed (source)

Sitting on the nest Sitting in nest bowl, either

incubating or brooding

n/a

Standing on the

nest

Adults attending to nest, but

standing off to side

n/a

Flushing Flying off the nest, leaving nest

unattended

Disturbed (Steidl & Anthony

2000; Whitfield, Ruddock &

Bullman 2008; Grubb et al.

2010)

Nest maintenance Arranging nest material, or

removing old prey items from nest

Relaxed (Steidl et al. 1993;

Steidl & Anthony 2000)

Rousing Fluffing out and shaking feathers

(common when wet)

Relaxed (B. Brown, and J.

Wiersma, pers. comm)

Shuffling Readjusting body positioning while

seated on nest; can be turning eggs

but not usually visible

Relaxed (B. Brown, and J.

Wiersma, pers. comm)

Preening Self-grooming; dressing feathers

with the beak

Relaxed (Steidl et al. 1993;

Steidl & Anthony 2000)

Feeding Consuming prey or feeding chicks

on the nest

Relaxed (Steidl et al. 1993;

Steidl & Anthony 2000)

Ducking down Flattening down into the nest Disturbed (Ellis 1981; Grubb

et al. 2010)

Head bobbing Rapidly raising and lowering head Disturbed (Manning & Kaler

2011)

Sitting upright Sudden change in body position to

an alert upright stance

Disturbed (Ellis 1981;

González et al. 2006a;

Whitfield, Ruddock &

Bullman 2008)

Vocalisation Calling; typically during territory

defence or communication within a

breeding pair

Disturbed (Ellis 1981;

Gaffney & Mooney 1992;

Grubb & Bowerman 1997;

Steidl & Anthony 2000)

Nest changeover When one eagle returns to the nest

and switches with attending eagle,

which then departs

Relaxed (Grubb et al. 2010)

Page 27: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

21

Due the chance that eagles would flush from the nest due to our approach, nests were

only visited on days with favourable weather conditions, i.e. non-rainy days with

temperatures between 8°C and 32°C, as inclement weather can increase the risk of nest

failure (Pagel, Whittington & Allen 2010). Precautions also included a commitment to

leave the study nests if our approach caused the eagles to flush from the nest for a

prolonged period of time, leaving eggs or chicks exposed. On warm (>20ºC) non-windy

days, birds could be absent from the nest for up to 50 minutes before I had to leave the

observation point. If birds flushed for longer than the 50 minute time period at multiple

nests (>3), then we would change our approach and access the hides before dawn to

further minimise disturbance to the birds.

Breeding results were also recorded by aerial survey as described above. Active nests

that successfully raised a chick until the end of the observation period (end of

November) were recorded as successful. Active nests with either eggs that did not

hatch or chicks that did not survive until the end of the observation period were

recorded as breeding failures.

Data analysis

Breeding timing and success

To model the timing and success of the breeding season, I used mixed effects modelling

techniques, using the lme4 package (Bates et al. 2014) in the program R (R Core Team

2013). For the timing model, I used linear mixed effects modelling (LMM) with a

Gaussian response variable (the start date of incubation). For the breeding success

model, I used generalised linear mixed effects modelling (GLMM) with a binomial

response variable (successful=1, unsuccessful=0). The fixed effects were the

environmental variables outlined in (Table 2) and NestID and Year were the random

effects.

For each set of analyses I first fit a global model, which included all relevant

environmental variables as fixed effects (Table 2). I then used the R package MuMIn

(Barton 2013) to generate a complete set of candidate models, which included all

possible combinations of the fixed effects from the global model without changing the

random effects. Though the “all subsets” approach has been warned against (Burnham

Page 28: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

22

& Anderson 2002), it is appropriate in this case, because all environmental variables

were selected with a priori reason to expect that they are influential to the timing and

success of the breeding season.

The set of best-fitting candidate models were selected based on the Akaike information

criterion (AIC) values, where smaller AIC values indicate better, more parsimonious

models (Quinn & Keough 2002). The ratio of observations to the number of covariates

(breeding timing: 197/3; breeding success: 844/6) were sufficiently large for the AIC to

be an appropriate selection criterion compared to alternatives (e.g. AICc) (Burnham &

Anderson 2002). In this case, LMM and GLMM resulted in a large number of closely

related models, and designation of a single best model via traditional model selection

techniques, including stepwise regression, was unsatisfactory because this technique

ignores the uncertainty that arises when two or more models have similar support

(Johnson & Omland 2004; Bolker et al. 2009). According to Burnham and Anderson

(2002), selecting a single best model is not ideal unless the best model has an Akaike

weight (wi) greater than 0.9. In order to account for the uncertainty in identification of

an ‘optimal’ model, I took a model averaging approach using the MuMIn package

(Barton 2013). Coefficients and standard errors were averaged over the entire set of

candidate models, producing more robust parameter estimations and predictions of the

timing of the breeding season than simply choosing the model with the lowest AIC

(Johnson & Omland 2004; Anderson 2008).

In addition to determining the best model, or set of models using model averaging, I

also examined the relative variable importance using the MuMIn package in R (Barton

2013). Akaike weights (wi) were determined for each candidate model, which represent

the likelihood that a given model is the best model for the observed data out of the set of

candidate models (Burnham & Anderson 2002). I obtained the relative variable

importance by summing wi for all of the candidate models in which the variable of

interest appeared.

I evaluated LMMs qualitatively by visual inspection of residual plots for deviations

from homoscedasticity or normality, an approach ineffective for GLMMs due to a

binomial response variable. To evaluate model fit, conditional pseudo-R2 values of top-

ranked GLMMs and LMMs were calculated using the MuMIn package in R (Barton

2013).

Page 29: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

23

Behavioural study

Given the small amount of data collected, quantitative analyses were not appropriate, so

results are presented qualitatively. Observations from each nest were pooled to give an

overall description of the breeding behaviour of the eagle at that nest, with distinctions

made for whether the observations occurred during the incubation period or early

nestling period of the breeding season. The frequency per hour for each behavioural

event is calculated for each breeding period (note: this does not indicate the amount of

time spent, only the number of times this behaviour was initiated). The activity budget,

which gives a percentage of time spent engaged in each behavioural state, is also

presented for each breeding period.

Time spent engaged in relaxed vs. disturbed behaviours were totalled for each nest for

each visit. Frequencies of relaxed vs. disturbed behaviours were also totalled for each

visit. The time to return from flushing from each nest was compared between visits to

investigate whether the behavioural response to the approach increased or decreased

with repeated disturbance. Potential relationships between 1) the behavioural responses

to disturbance at each nest and the result of the breeding attempt for each active nest;

and 2) the distance to the nearest road versus the result of the breeding attempt for each

active nest were visually examined for any trends.

Page 30: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

24

3. Results

Breeding timing

The full set of eight competing models included all combinations of the three

environmental variables (winter min., prey, and spring rain) (Table 4). The strongest

model, M8, included all three environmental variables. M8 has an Akaike weight (w8) of

0.656, which is considerably higher than the next best model M6 (w6 = 0.182). The

ΔAIC between M8 and M6 is 2.51, indicating that M6 and all subsequent models have

considerably less empirical support from the data than the best model (Burnham &

Anderson 2002). Visual inspection of residual plots for the best model did not result in

any obvious deviations from normality or homoscedasticity, indicating that the model

did not violate any assumptions of the LMM. The conditional pseudo-R2 value

calculated for M8 was 0.229, indicating that prey, spring rain, and winter minimum,

along with the random effects, explained over 22% of the variation in the timing of the

breeding season.

Table 4. The global model set for predicting the timing of the breeding season of the Tasmanian

wedge-tailed eagle. Coefficient estimates, AIC values, and Akaike weights are given. Random

effects, Nest ID and Year, are included in all models. Square root transformations were

performed on prey and spring rain.

Each of the three predictor variables had a variable importance weight >0.75 (Figure

3b). Prey abundance and average winter minimum temperature carried higher variable

importance weights than spring rain due to their inclusion in both the top two models,

which together carried over 80% of the Akaike weight.

Model Intercept prey spring rain winter

min.

df logLik ΔAIC Weight

8 65.57 3.70 0.80 -1.20 7 -745.0 0 0.656

6 76.93 4.34 -1.07 6 -747.3 2.56 0.182

7 67.96 0.86 -1.13 6 -748.1 4.17 0.081

4 65.60 3.18 0.64 6 -748.1 5.88 0.035

2 74.96 3.72 5 -750.4 6.81 0.022

5 80.67 -0.965 5 -750.8 7.65 0.014

3 67.74 0.69 5 -751.6 9.26 0.006

1 78.38 4 -753.5 10.87 0.003

Page 31: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

25

Since there was still some uncertainty in selecting the best model from the candidate

models, (w8<0.9) (Burnham & Anderson 2002), I averaged the breeding timing LMMs

to gain a more robust estimate of the effects of each predictor variable (Table 5, Figure

3). As predicted, the start date of incubation was delayed by higher total spring rainfall

(Figure 4a) and was earlier with warmer winters (Figure 4b). Contrary to expectations,

higher relative prey availability was linked to a later start date of incubation. However,

this relationship was highly variable between models, resulting in a high standard error,

and the 95% confidence interval overlapped with zero, which indicates a non-significant

relationship (Figure 3a).

Table 5. Model averaged coefficient estimates and standard errors of the environmental

variables used in the linear mixed model of Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle breeding timing. In

bold type are the estimates significantly different from zero (p < 0.05).

Figure 3. (a) Regression coefficients for each predictor variable for breeding timing, averaged

across all competing model combinations, weighted by their AIC score, together with 95%

confidence intervals. (b) Relative variable importance across all eight models predicting timing

of the breeding season. Values of 1.0 indicate greatest relative importance.

Model averaged coefficients

prey

spring.rain

winter.min

-2 0 2 4 6 8

spring rain

prey

winter min.

0.70 0.80 0.90 1.00

Relative variable importance

Estimate Std. Error z value Pr(>|z|)

(Intercept) 68.260 6.847 9.969 < 2e-16

spring rain 0.798 0.328 2.429 0.015

winter min. -1.153 0.427 2.698 0.007

prey 3.998 2.220 1.818 0.069

(a) (b)

Model averaged coefficients

prey

spring.rain

winter.min

-2 0 2 4 6 8

spring rain

prey

winter min.

0.70 0.80 0.90 1.00

Relative variable importance

Page 32: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

26

Figure 4. The predicted start date of incubation for the Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle in

relation to (a) spring rainfall and (b) mean winter minimum temperature. Raw data are indicated

by circles, and the red line is the linear model, with averaged intercept and coefficients for each

variable. Only statistically significant variables are illustrated.

Breeding success

Of the full set of 64 competing models, the top-ranking seven models had ΔAIC<2

(Table 6). The top ranked model (M22) included three variables: near nests, spring rain,

and winter min., all of which were inversely related to breeding success. The

conditional pseudo-R2 value calculated from the top model was 0.325, which indicates

that M22 explained over 32% of the variation in the data. However, M22 had an Akaike

weight of just 0.115. This low model weight, coupled with the high number of models

within two AIC values of the top model indicated considerable model uncertainty.

Subsequently, a model averaging approach was used, which averaged intercepts and

coefficients across the entire candidate set of models. For a list of all 64 candidate

models see Appendix 1.

spring rainfall (mm)

Sta

rt d

ate

of

incubation

10 15 20

Aug 1

1S

ep 1

1O

ct

11

Mean winter minimum

0 2 4 6

Aug 1

1S

ep 1

1O

ct

11

(a) (b)

Page 33: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

27

27

Table 6. The top ranked (ΔAIC<2) models predicting the nest success of the Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle, along with coefficients, AIC values, and

Akaike weights. Random effects, ‘Nest ID’ and ‘Year’, are included in all models. Square root transformations were performed on roads 1k, winter rain,

spring rain, and prey.

Model Intercept near nests prey spring rain roads 1k winter min. winter rain df ΔAIC weight

22 0.591 -0.221 -0.115 -0.096 6 0 0.115

6 0.282 -0.207 -0.113 5 0.23 0.102

8 0.900 -0.201 -0.695 -0.111 6 0.78 0.078

24 1.090 -0.215 -0.604 -0.112 -0.088 7 0.91 0.073

54 0.513 -0.229 -0.141 -0.107 0.0293 7 1.49 0.054

30 0.671 -0.215 -0.115 -0.0017 -0.098 7 1.85 0.045

56 1.107 -0.225 -0.745 -0.148 -0.103 0.0422 8 1.93 0.044

Page 34: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

28

28

Model-averaged coefficients

near nests

spring rain

winter min.

prey

winter rain

roads 1k

-1.5 -1.0 -0.5 0.0 0.5

roads 1k

winter rain

prey

winter min.

near nests

spring rain

0.0 0.2 0.4 0.6 0.8 1.0

Relative variable importance

Figure 5. (a) Regression coefficients for each environmental variable, averaged across all competing model combinations predicting breeding success,

weighted by their AIC score, together with 95% confidence intervals [mean coefficients ±(unconditional standard error × 1.96)]. (b) Relative variable

importance across all 64 models predicting breeding success. Values of 1.0 indicate greatest relative importance of variables.

(a)

(b)

Page 35: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

29

Of the six environmental variables included in the analysis, the only two that had a

significant effect association with nest success when averaged across all 64 weighted

models are near nests and spring rain (Table 7, Figure 5a). Mean values for all other

variables (winter minimum temperature, relative prey availability, total winter rainfall,

and the total distance of roads within 1km of each nest) had confidence intervals that

overlapped with 0, and thus there was no significant association with breeding success

(Table 7, Figure 5a).

Table 7. Model averaged coefficient estimates, and standard errors, of environmental variables

for the generalised linear mixed model of Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle breeding success.

Estimates significantly different from zero (p <0.05) are in bold type.

The results of the variable importance analysis confirm the results from model

averaging (Figure 5). To determine the relative importance of each environmental

variable in predicting the breeding success of the Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle, I tested

each of the 6 variables included in the model building. Variables with the highest

relative importance (Figure 5b) were the total amount of spring rainfall and the number

of near nests (within 3 km), with variable importance weights of 0.92 and 0.82

respectively out of a possible maximum of 1. The average winter minimum temperature

was the third most important variable with a weight of 0.51. The rest of the variables,

including the relative prey abundance, total distance of roads within 1km of each nest,

and total amount of winter rainfall, had lower variable importance weights (Figure 5b).

As predicted, the total amount of spring rainfall and number of known nests within 3

km were both inverselt related to breeding success (Figure 6). Contrary to expectations,

there was an inverse trend between average winter minimum temperature and breeding

success, although this relationship was not significant (Table 7).

Estimate Std. Error z value Pr(>|z|)

(Intercept) 0.529 0.927 0.571 0.568

near nests -0.213 0.093 2.283 0.022

spring rain -0.121 0.048 2.553 0.011

winter min. -0.094 0.064 1.473 0.141

prey -0.704 0.571 1.233 0.218

winter rain 0.020 0.045 0.431 0.667

roads 1k -0.002 0.005 0.404 0.686

Page 36: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

30

Figure 6. The predicted likelihood that a chick was successfully raised at a given nest in

relation to the amount of (a) spring rainfall and (b) number of nearby nests (within a 3km

radius). Raw data are represented by empty circles in (a) and bars in (b): proportions of

unsuccessful (dark) and successful (light) nests, with totals for each category in parentheses.

The red lines are the inverse-logit functions fit by model averaged intercepts and coefficients.

Only statistically significant variables are illustrated.

Behavioural observations

In October and November, behavioural surveys were conducted at 12 active nests.

Eagles were observed on the nest for a total of 19 hours during the incubation stage of

the breeding season, and for seven hours during the early nestling stage.

The most frequent behavioural event during incubation was vocalising (5.91/hour),

followed by ducking the head down (3.74/hr) and sitting upright (2.48/hr) (Figure 7).

The most frequent behavioural event during the early nestling stage was also vocalising

(0.86/hr), followed by feeding (0.73/hr) (Figure 7).

For behavioural states I recorded activity time budgets. As expected, sitting on the nest

was the most common behaviour both during incubation and the early nestling stages,

10 15 20 25 30

0.0

0.4

0.8

spring rainfall (mm)

pro

ba

bility o

f n

est su

cce

ss

number of nests within 3 km

pro

ba

bility o

f n

est su

cce

ss

0.0

0.4

0.8

0

(n=255)

1

(n=259)

2

(n=165)

3

(n=104)

4

(n=43)

5

(n=16)

7

(n=2)

10 15 20 25 30

0.0

0.4

0.8

spring rainfall (mm)

pro

ba

bility o

f n

est su

cce

ss

number of nests within 3 km

pro

ba

bility o

f n

est su

cce

ss

0.0

0.4

0.8

0

(n=255)

1

(n=259)

2

(n=165)

3

(n=104)

4

(n=43)

5

(n=16)

7

(n=2)

(a)

(b)

Page 37: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

31

taking up 90% of the time spent on the nest during incubation and 67% during the early

nestling stage (Figure 8). Standing on the nest was the second most prevalent behaviour

(6% of the activity budget) during incubation, while feeding was the second most

common behaviour (15%) during the early nestling stage (Figure 8).

Figure 7. The frequency per hour of each behaviour event observed for both the incubation

(black) and early nestling periods (grey) of the Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle. For behaviour

definitions, refer to Table 3.

Figure 8. Proportion of time spent in various behavioural states on the nest by Tasmanian

wedge-tailed eagles during the (a) incubation and (b) nestling stages of the breeding season. For

behaviour definitions, refer to Table 3.

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Fre

qu

en

cy/h

ou

r o

bse

rve

d

incubation

nestling

(a) (b) (b)

Page 38: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

32

During behavioural surveys, flushing or leaving the nest was a common behavioural

response to the observers approaching the hides, occurring at 9 of 11 nests that were

active during the first visit (Figure 9). The 12th

active nest was not included because

although an incubating eagle was observed during the hide setup, breeding had failed by

the time of the first behavioural survey visit (Nest 12, Figure 9). The duration of time

that each eagle was away from the nest after flushing was highly variable between nests

as well as between visits for single nests (Figure 9). For example, during the first visit to

each nest two eagles did not flush in response to the approach, while one eagle flushed

and did not return before the 50 minute threshold (the actual amount of time it was off

the nest is unknown, but was at least 54 minutes).

In response to observers approaching the hides at three nests for the second visit, all

eagles left the nest for longer than the 50 minute threshold, irrespective of the time to

return during the initial visits. Due to the consistent pattern of extended periods off the

nest occurring (presumably) as a result of the survey techniques, we had to abandon the

survey method. As the survey methods had to be changed due to the impact on the birds,

only these three nests were visited twice before we changed techniques and approached

the hides before dawn to minimise disturbance.

Figure 9. The time to return after flushing from the nest by Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagles for

each survey visit. Nests marked with * are nests where the eagles did not flush during the

approach, while † indicates that a nest failed prior to the first behavioural visit. The dashed line

at 50 minutes corresponds to the threshold at which I had to leave the nests, so each bar that

crossed this threshold represents the time observed off the nest, and the total time to return is

unknown.

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12

TIm

e t

o r

etu

rn (

min

)

Nest

Visit 1 Flush time

Visit 2 Flush time

* * †

Page 39: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

33

In terms of breeding success, most study nests failed (n=10). The next most common

outcome was no breeding attempt (n=4), with nest success being the least common

outcome (n=2). Thus, of the nests where a breeding attempt was made, 83% failed and

17% were successful (Figure 11).

The limited behavioural data obtained (one visit at most nests) meant the behavioural

analyses had to be qualitative. The prevalence of ‘relaxed’ vs. ‘disturbed’ behaviours

were recorded as frequencies for behavioural events and as a percentage of time for the

behavioural states, and varied considerably between nests (Figure 10). There was also

variation in the proportion of time eagles spent off the nest at different nest sites (Figure

10). Of the two successful nests, one flushed without returning during the first nest visit

(Nest 7, Figure 9), and so is excluded from this analysis due to a lack of behavioural

data. However, there was no evidence of a link between nest success and a lower

prevalence of ‘disturbed’ behaviours (Figure 9). In fact, the eagle observed at one

successful nest exhibited the lowest frequency of relaxed behaviours and close to the

lowest percentage of time spent engaged in relaxed behaviours of all the study nests

during the first visit (Figure 10). This eagle also exhibited higher than average levels of

disturbed behaviours, in both frequency and percentage of time. The eagles at this

successful nest did however spend the lowest amount of time off the nest during the

survey period, at just 4% of the 2.5 hours. However, the eagles at the only other

successful nest spent the highest amount of time off the nest (100% of the survey time).

This high variability between individuals limited the inferences that could be made from

comparing behavioural responses and breeding outcomes.

Page 40: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

34

Figure 10. (a) Frequencies and (b) percentage of time eagles spent engaged in relaxed

behaviours, disturbed behaviours, and time spent off nest as observed during the first visit to

each active wedge-tailed eagle nest. Nests are distinguished by their breeding result: black

circles represent breeding failures, while red triangles represent the successful nest where

behavioural data was collected.

log

(fre

qu

en

cy o

f b

eh

avio

urs

)

Relaxed Disturbed

23

4

log

(% o

f tim

e s

pe

nt e

ng

ag

ed

in

be

ha

vio

urs

)

Off nest Relaxed Disturbed

-2-1

01

23

(a) (b)

Page 41: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

35

Figure 11. The proportion of estimated breeding failures among Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle

nests aerially surveyed during the past two years, and nests included in behavioural surveys in

2013. Black columns represent the percentage of wedge-tailed eagle nests where breeding

appeared to have been attempted and failed, based on evidence of recent nest use without the

presence of a chick. The grey column is a definitive percentage of behavioural survey nests that

attempted incubation and failed.

State-wide nest breeding failure rates for the two breeding seasons with data on

evidence of recent nest material, were 55% in 2012 and 50% in 2013. A significantly

higher proportion of attempted breeding failures was observed within the behavioural

study than within the aerial study from the 2013 breeding season (χ2 = 4.644, p = 0.031)

with 83% of the 12 active behavioural study nests resulting nest failure (Figure 11).

It is possible that an even stronger difference in the rate of nest failure existed between

the two survey methods in 2013 than is suggested in Figure 9. The successful nests from

the aerial surveys include all nests where chicks were observed. Since the aerial surveys

only occurred at the end of the breeding season, the nests classified as attempted and

failed were nests recorded with evidence of recent use with no chicks present. It is likely

that this estimate includes some nests that were maintained by the breeding pairs but in

which no breeding attempt was made. However, correcting this potential bias would

result in a lower number of “attempted failed” nests, which would result in a more

pronounced difference between the study nests and the aerials survey nests.

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

80

90

Aerial survey nests2012 (n=107)

Aerial survey nests2013 (n=76)

Behavioural survey nests2013 (n=12)

Pe

rce

nta

ge o

f n

est

s su

very

ed

est

imat

ed

as

ne

st f

ailu

res

Page 42: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

36

Figure 12. The distance from each Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle nest included in the

behavioural study to the nearest road, for each breeding outcome.

To identify any differences between the nests included in the behavioural study that

could explain the breeding results, I examined the distance to the nearest road from each

nest as a possible explanatory variable and proxy for long-term disturbance levels

(Figure 12). One of the two successful nests was much further from roads than all other

sites. However, with the limited sample size (n=16), the inferences that can be made

from the results are very weak.

Dis

tan

ce

to

ne

are

st ro

ad

(m

)

Success

(n=2)

Failure

(n=10)

Not attempted

(n=4)

05

00

10

00

15

00

20

00

25

00

Page 43: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

37

4. Discussion

To breed successfully, eagles require suitable biotic and abiotic conditions that allow

them to remain on the nest for a sufficient duration to incubate and rear offspring. I

found that abiotic conditions, primarily spring rain and winter temperatures, affected

breeding eagles as predicted. However, the primary biotic variable influencing breeding

eagles was not prey availability, but instead was disturbance at the nest site. While

wedge-tailed eagles were sensitive to disturbance, the small sample of birds studied

could not link the behaviour of the birds on the nest to the likelihood of nest failure,

perhaps because disturbance can be expressed in different ways. Interactions between

biotic and abiotic variables may contribute to the variability of nest success seen over

the years in Tasmania.

Breeding timing

The timing of the breeding season in bird species can influence subsequent breeding

success. For the Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle, breeding timing was related to spring

rainfall and winter temperatures, but not prey availability. Successful breeding attempts

tended to be later in the season if spring rainfall was high, which could be due to a later

onset of incubation, or high failure rate of early breeding attempts. Tasmanian wedge-

tailed eagles may start incubation later when spring rainfall is elevated, with a similar

association in non-raptor bird species (Ost et al. 2008), but not in the closely related

golden eagle (Steenhof, Kochert & McDonald 1997). Typically, rainfall is strongly

linked to the onset of incubation in arid environments, where birds respond positively to

rainfall and may even require rainfall before incubation can commence (Zann et al.

1995), which would not apply to the Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle. Since breeding

timing data in the current study were determined from surveys of chicks at the end of

each breeding season, the observed delay in the timing of the breeding season could be

biased. High intensity rainfall during early spring could disadvantage earlier breeders by

promoting opportunities for nest failure due to egg and chick exposure over the

incubation and early nestling stages (Radford & Plessis 2003). Thus, the observed age

of chicks on successful nests, and the subsequent extrapolation of the average start date

of incubation could be ‘delayed’ because the earlier incubating breeding pairs were

unsuccessful and not detected in the data. Additionally, as raptors can successfully re-

attempt to breed within a single season if the initial failure is early enough (Martinez &

Page 44: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

38

Blanco 2002; Steenhof & Peterson 2009), nest failures caused by heavy rainfall early in

the season could be re-attempted, and if successful, delay the observed timing of the

breeding season. With the data available, it is not possible to distinguish between a real

or perceived delay in breeding with high spring rain. However, as high rainfall is also

correlated with lower nest success, the second explanation seems more likely to account

for the observed delay.

Colder winter temperatures corresponded to delayed breeding, which could be due to

later incubation start dates or prolonged incubation from slower embryonic

development. Delayed incubation start dates after cold winters have been found for the

closely related golden eagle (Steenhof, Kochert & McDonald 1997), as well as a

number of other raptor species (Kostrzewa & Kostrzewa 1990; Marti 1994; Fairhurst &

Bechard 2005; Steenhof & Peterson 2009). It is possible that the egg laying date in the

Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle is based at least partially on temperature cues, which

have been experimentally linked to incubation onset in many bird species (Visser,

Holleman & Caro 2009). A second potential reason for the observed delay in the timing

of the breeding season with colder winter temperatures, which were highly correlated

with colder spring temperatures, is that colder incubation temperatures can extend the

incubation period by slowing embryonic development (Deeming & Fergusion 1991;

Martin et al. 2007). The plasticity of incubation periods can be dramatic. In a classic

example of this plasticity, an incubation period normally lasting 50 days was reduced by

22 days as a result of higher nest attentiveness, and associated warmer incubation

temperatures (Ward 1940). Thus, the incubation period can vary markedly for species

with relatively long incubation requirements, like the Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle.

Since temperature is a common cue for birds to begin incubation, it is likely that colder

winters correspond to later start dates. However, the perceived delay may also be

influenced by longer incubation periods during cold springs, and further information on

the plasticity of the incubation period for eagles is required.

Contrary to predictions, relative prey availability did not significantly explain any

variation in the timing of the breeding season, in contrast to studies on other eagle

species (Hansen 1987; Steenhof, Kochert & McDonald 1997). One possible explanation

is that prey abundance is often more influential for specialist predators which rely on a

single prey species (e.g. rabbits or voles) (Korpimäki & Norrdahl 1991; Ontiveros &

Page 45: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

39

Pleguezneles 2000; González et al. 2006b). In contrast, Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagles

are generalist and opportunistic predators (Gaffney & Mooney 1992). Therefore wedge-

tailed eagles may be less vulnerable to changes in the abundance of particular prey

species as they can supplement their diet with other species. However, it is also possible

that the spotlighting surveys did not detect variation in prey abundances relevant to

Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagles. Although the spotlighting surveys are the best available

estimate of prey abundance on a state-wide level, they were designed specifically to

monitor abundances of two native macropod species (Bennett’s wallaby, Macropus

rufogriseus; Tasmanian pademelon, Thylogale billardierii) and one arboreal marsupial

(brushtail possum, Trichosurus vulpecula) (Southwell & Fletcher 1993). Thus, counts

from the spotlighting data may provide less accurate abundance estimates for rabbits,

hares and cats, which comprise a large portion of Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle prey

(Gaffney & Mooney 1992). The surveys are also only conducted once a year at each

transect during the summer months (November-April), a time which is not as likely to

be as important to Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle reproduction as winter and spring

(Tjernberg 1983; Korpimäki & Norrdahl 1991). Therefore finer scale prey surveys are

required to confirm whether prey availability is related to the timing of the breeding

season for wedge-tailed eagles.

Breeding success

The estimated rate of breeding failure of attempted wedge-tailed eagle nests falls into

the range of those observed for other eagle species. The breeding failure estimates from

2012 and 2013 were 55% and 50%, respectively. Breeding failure rates in other eagle

species vary significantly between years and species: the golden eagle ranges from 20%

to 68% (Kochert et al. 1999); and the bald eagle ranges from 52% to 93% (Hansen 1987;

Steidl, Kozie & Anthony 1997). As survey methods and definitions of nest success and

failure are not consistent between studies, direct comparisons are not possible. However,

the studies done provide broader context to demonstrate common levels of nest failure

in closely related species. The estimate of state-wide breeding failure in Tasmania was

determined by calculating the ratio of nests that contained recent nesting material

without chicks, versus nests with chicks present, as seen during aerial surveys

conducted in November and early December. My estimate of state-wide breeding

failure is not definitive, as recent material may not actually represent a breeding attempt,

Page 46: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

40

meaning the percentage of nest failure obtained is likely to be lower in reality. The

inclusion of false positives in the ‘nest failure’ category would result in overinflation of

the breeding failure estimate, so true nest failure (whereby a breeding attempt is made

but fails) is likely to be lower than reported here. However, it is possible that the

observed rate of nest failure could be an underestimation, as aerial surveys are

conducted when chicks are still nest dependent (Koch, Wiersma & Munks 2012) and

some chicks may not fledge successfully. Therefore, further research is required, but it

appears Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagles have similar failure rates to other eagles.

Wedge-tailed eagle nests were more likely to be successful when spring rainfall was

lower, a link found in a number of raptor species (Kostrzewa & Kostrzewa 1990;

Krüger 2004; Bionda & Brambilla 2012), but which contradicts to studies on the

mainland subspecies Aquila audax audax (Ridpath & Brooker 1985; Robertson 1987).

Nest success for the Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle was not related to the total amount

of winter rain. The degree to which rainfall can influence breeding success is variable

and depends on the species and habitat in question (Olsen & Olsen 1992). Wedge-tailed

eagles in arid regions may require a minimum amount of rainfall before they will

initiate breeding, while in more temperate areas there may be a maximum amount of

rain that is beneficial. The lower breeding success associated with spring rainfall in the

Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle may result from less efficient hunting in heavy rainfall

(Kostrzewa & Kostrzewa 1990; Olsen & Olsen 1992), and/or a greater chance of egg

cooling or nestling death. However, it is likely that rainfall does not significantly reduce

hunting efficiency because if higher rainfall corresponded strongly to lower hunting

efficiency during the winter one would expect fewer breeding attempts, but no

significant relationship between winter rain and nest success was found in the current

study. Thus, though rain is essential for wedge-tailed eagles in arid habitats, elevated

spring rainfall may be detrimental in Tasmania by lowering offspring survival.

Cold winter temperatures are less influential on the breeding success of the Tasmanian

wedge-tailed eagle than other closely related raptors, potentially due to Tasmania’s

temperate winters. It appeared that colder winters were linked to higher breeding

success, but the relationship was not significant. Previous studies on the closely related

golden eagle found that fewer breeding pairs laid eggs and fewer breeding attempts

were successful after severe winters (Steenhof, Kochert & McDonald 1997; McIntyre &

Page 47: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

41

Schmidt 2012). However, the studies on golden eagle breeding occurred in areas with

much harsher winters than occur in Tasmania. For example, in an Alaskan population of

golden eagles, the winter temperature variable was defined as number of days that the

temperature did not exceed -17.8ºC (McIntyre & Schmidt 2012), whereas the average

winter minimum temperature at nest sites in Tasmania from the survey years was 2.7 ºC

(based on AWAP temperature data; Raupach et al. (2009). Thus there may be a critical

minimum temperature at which nest success begins to decline, but since Tasmania has

relatively mild winters, this variable has comparatively little impact on the breeding

success of the Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle.

Prey availability influences breeding success in a number of raptor species (Martin 1987;

Byholm & Kekkonen 2008; McIntyre & Schmidt 2012), including A. a. audax on

mainland Australia (Ridpath & Brooker 1985; Robertson 1987), although prey

abundances tend to be less influential in species with high diet plasticity (Ontiveros &

Pleguezneles 2000). Relative prey availability, estimated using data from spotlighting

transects from the summer prior to the breeding season, was not a predictor of breeding

success in the Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle. As discussed in relation to the timing of

the breeding season, this lack of relationship may be due to inadequacies in the data

used for the current study or due to the broad foraging requirements of wedge-tailed

eagles (Gaffney & Mooney 1992).

Nest success decreased with greater numbers of known wedge-tailed eagle nests within

a 3 km radius. This relationship is likely due to the fact that a higher number of

alternative nests within a given territory (estimated 3 km radius; Bell & Mooney 1999)

reduces the chances that each nest in the territory will be used in a given year. However,

there is a long standing hypothesis that eagles will abandon nests where they are

disturbed, which results in nest-switching or nest building (Brown 1976; Newton 1979;

Mooney & Holdsworth 1991). Thus, the number of nests within a given territory may

increase with the level of disturbance the eagles have been exposed to, as has been

found for goshawks (Saga & Selås 2012), though support for this hypothesis in eagles is

limited (Ontiveros, Caro & Pleguezuelos 2008b; Kochert & Steenhof 2012). The ten

study nests that failed during the 2013 breeding season should be surveyed in the future

to determine nest reuse after a definitive failure, and provide evidence to test the nest

switching hypothesis.

Page 48: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

42

Nest success was not related to the total length of roads within 1 km of each nest (highly

correlated with the distance to nearest road) contrary to findings from other raptor

species (González, Bustamante & Hiraldo 1992; Martinez, Pagan & Calvo 2006;

Strasser & Heath 2013). However, there was no available data for road usage, only for

existence of roads, and determining road usage would be a better proxy for estimating

disturbance (González, Bustamante & Hiraldo 1992; Krüger 2004). Under current

management prescriptions, use of forestry roads within 500 m or 1 km line-of-sight of

the nest is restricted during the breeding season (FPA 2012), meaning the presence of

roads may be a poor proxy for road use. The lack of evidence supporting a relationship

between road density and breeding success for Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagles may

suggest that this management prescription is effective. However, road restrictions do not

apply to public roads, and are not enforced on agricultural roads. A clearer picture might

be gained if the extent to which nests are affected by these restrictions during the

breeding season could be gauged.

An alternative explanation for why road density did not have an impact on breeding

success is that roads may be correlated with optimal habitat or prey availability, in the

form of road-kill (Bautista et al. 2004). Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagles tend to have

higher breeding success at nests with more agricultural land (and accompanying roads)

nearby, likely due to more optimal foraging habitat within the territory (Koch, Wiersma

& Munks 2012). This preference for open foraging habitat is common in large raptor

species, including the golden eagle (Watson 1997; Whitfield et al. 2001). Wedge-tailed

eagles are known to feed on carrion (Gaffney & Mooney 1992), so higher road density

in a territory may increase food availability. The prevalence of carrion is plentiful in

Tasmania, particularly with the decline of the Tasmanian devil, one of the state’s

principal scavengers (Rounsevell, Taylor & Hocking 1991; Jones et al. 2007).

Depending on the amount of carrion in the diet of the wedge-tailed eagle, the benefit of

road-kill within a given territory could offset any detrimental effects of disturbance.

Finally, vehicular traffic may not be as disturbing to wedge-tailed eagles as pedestrian

activities, such as hiking. This distinction is evident in the Spanish imperial eagle,

Aquila adalberti (González et al. 2006a), the bald eagle (Grubb & King 1991; Steidl &

Anthony 2000), and the common buzzard, Buteo buteo (Krüger 2004). The adverse

behavioural response to humans may be because birds perceive humans as potential

Page 49: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

43

predators and respond accordingly, even though this predation risk is not generally

realised (Beale & Monaghan 2004). The association with humans as predators could be

a lingering effect of human-related persecution. Although persecution is declining in

many areas (Starker Leopold & Wolfe; Mooney & Holdsworth 1991; Foster & Wallis

2010), historic killing of tolerant individuals may have selected for lower tolerant birds

(Kenney & Knight 1992; Foster & Wallis 2010). To summarise, breeding success was

linked with rainfall and nest density, but much of the variability in the data remains

unexplained. Thus, further study is required to investigate other potential factors and to

confirm these findings, particularly regarding the impact of roads and prey.

Breeding behaviour and response to disturbance

Breeding behaviour should have been selected to increase breeding success and adult

fitness. However, in long-lived raptors, adult survival is prioritised over offspring

survival, as adults are likely to breed again (Blas, Sergio & Hiraldo 2009; Sergio et al.

2011). Thus the breeding behaviour of raptors is determined by compromising between

trade-offs supporting offspring and adult fitness, which can change according external

factors, notably human disturbance and stress (Breuner 2010).

Incubation requires higher nest attendance than hatched nestlings, particularly as chicks

develop (Williams 1996). In the current study the time that eagles were observed sitting

on the nest decreased between the incubation and the nestling stage, as expected. The

observed decline in nest attendance corresponds to changes in time allocation in Aquila

audax audax (Debus et al. 2007) and other raptor species (Levenson 1979). The

behavioural observations from the incubation and early nestling periods in the current

study indicated a high frequency of vocalisations, sitting upright, preening, and ducking

head down. With the exception of preening, all of these behaviours are categorised as

‘disturbed’ behaviours. The behaviours categorised as ‘disturbed’ or ‘relaxed’ were

based on recommendations from wedge-tailed eagle experts and on behaviours

commonly related to experimental disturbances in related species (Ellis 1981; Steidl et

al. 1993; Grubb & Bowerman 1997; Steidl & Anthony 2000; González et al. 2006a;

Grubb et al. 2010; Manning & Kaler 2011). The prevalence of disturbed behaviours in

the current study was confounded by the disturbance caused by the researchers approach

to the hides prior to the behavioural monitoring. It is likely that the ‘disturbed’

behaviours predominant in our observations, particularly vocalisations, occur less

Page 50: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

44

frequently during undisturbed incubation while ‘relaxed’ behaviours, such as nest

maintenance and feeding would increase (Steidl & Anthony 2000).

There was no evidence of a link between the result of a breeding attempt and the

prevalence of ‘disturbed’ or ‘relaxed’ behaviour on nests or behavioural responses to

disturbance. However, I suggest that some of the behaviours may have been incorrectly

categorised as either ‘relaxed’ or ‘disturbed,’ particularly if any were actually

displacement activities. Displacement behaviours occur outside of their normal context,

and instead occur uncharacteristically in situations of conflict (Martin 1987). They often

consist of comfort behaviours (e.g. preening) in response to stress, instead of fleeing

(Duncan & Wood-Gush 1972; Martin 1987). Displacement preening, i.e. preening

during a stressful situation, tends to occur at a higher frequency and in shorter bursts,

compared to typical preening (Duncan & Wood-Gush 1972). I hypothesise that under

non-disturbed situations, wedge-tailed eagles will exhibit lower frequencies of preening

bouts, but the overall preening episodes will account for a greater proportion of time

than what was observed post-disturbance in this study. To eliminate observer bias,

assessing this hypothesis would have to be done remotely (e.g. using video surveillance).

The technique used to approach the hides was designed to minimise disturbance, but

still disturbed the Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle. The ‘walk-in’ technique involved the

surveyor approaching the hide another researcher who departed after the surveyor

entered the hide. Although the ‘walk-in’ technique was adopted to minimise the impact

on breeding birds (Beer 1961), it is possible that rather than minimising the impact of

the observers approaching the hide, the technique resulted in higher disturbance.

Because many eagles flushed off the nest, the presence of the accompanying person and

vehicle within the eagles’ territory could have prevented some of the eagles from

returning to the nest even though they were not in line-of-sight of the nest. Constructing

the hides further away, which has been done successfully in other studies on sensitive

raptors (González et al. 2006a; Bird 2007), was not possible at the study nests due to

either obstructed views by vegetation or topographical limitations. We attempted to

approach the hides prior to sunrise after it was clear that the survey methods were

having an adverse effect on the eagles, but the majority of the nests had failed by this

point.

Page 51: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

45

The data suggest that the approach of the surveyor to the nest had considerable impact

on the birds’ breeding success and behaviour. The rate of nest failure from the

behavioural study nests was significantly higher than the state-wide nest failure rate in

the same year, indicating that disturbance from the study methods increased the

likelihood of nest failure. There is likely some inflation of the estimate of breeding

failure state-wide due to the presence of ‘maintained’ nests, but this would decrease the

significant difference between the behavioural study nests and the overall nests for 2013.

Thus, the failure rate of the behavioural study nests was conclusively higher than

expected. Therefore, disturbance from the behavioural study methods influenced the

breeding outcome.

This study confirmed that eagles are very sensitive to disturbance during breeding, but

there is some difference between nests. While eagles mostly flushed from the nest as we

approached the hides, the time for eagles to return to each nest after flushing was highly

variable. Two eagles did not flush during our approach, while others may have flushed

for more than an hour during the first visit. This reduction in nest attentiveness can have

detrimental effects on chick health, resulting in prolonged incubation periods (Lyon &

Montgomerie 1985; Nilsson & Henrik 1988), reduced offspring condition (Ardia, Perez

& Clotfelter 2010), or nest failure due to embryonic death or nest predation. Prolonged

incubations can result in later hatch dates and thus disadvantage breeding adults as well

as chicks by increasing breeding costs (Hanssen et al. 2005; Steenhof & Newton 2007).

Thus it is likely that the behavioural responses to the seemingly minor (Mooney &

Holdsworth 1991) disturbance of humans walking 100-200 m from the nest adversely

affected nest success at the study sites.

While it is commonly suggested that raptors can habituate to repeated disturbance

(González et al. 2006a; Bejder et al. 2009; Strasser & Heath 2013), behavioural

responses of wedge-tailed eagles from this study support sensitisation rather than

habituation. The increased time to return to the nests after repeated disturbance supports

the hypothesis of Steidl and Anthony (2000), which postulates that frequent short-

lasting disturbances (e.g. surveyors approaching the hides) within the same breeding

territory could have cumulative effects on adult behaviour and result in energetic

consequences to offspring.

Page 52: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

46

Assessing the impacts of human activity on raptor behaviour is useful to gauge more

subtle impacts than simply monitoring breeding outcomes. Though behavioural surveys

are difficult and time-consuming, they give a more detailed picture of how changes in

biologically meaningful behaviours at an individual level can result in lower fitness of

adults and offspring. Examples of detrimental behavioural changes include lower

frequencies of prey deliveries to nests, changed responses to environmental variables

(Steidl & Anthony 2000), and higher chances of egg cooling resulting in longer

incubation or embryonic failure (Martin et al. 2007). If disturbances persist and the

population consists of a high proportion of sensitive eagles, then the accumulation of

the effects of disturbance could have detrimental long-term consequences to populations,

particularly for long-lived species such as eagles.

Areas for further research

To gain a better understanding of the timing of the breeding season, factors that affect

nest success, and the breeding behaviour of the Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle, further

behavioural surveys along with state-wide nest surveys are needed. The best technique

to obtain behavioural data from the nests of such a sensitive raptor species is using

remote video cameras, set up prior to the onset of incubation (Rogers 2005; Delaney et

al. 2011). Though there are considerable upfront costs to this approach, the data

collected from even a small number of cameras over a few breeding seasons would help

provide answers many of the questions raised by this study while minimising

disturbance and limiting the bias that direct observation techniques create (Booms &

Fuller 2003; Delaney et al. 2011).

Using cameras that acquired data from the entire breeding season could help determine

the true start date of incubation and confirm the length of breeding stages. This would

remove the bias present in the current timing data, which bases the timing of the

breeding season solely on successful nests. The onset of incubation can easily be

detected on video, either based on presence of eggs or behavioural changes (Debus et al.

2007). The length of each period of the breeding season used to determine the onset of

incubation for this study was based on findings from a behavioural study at three Aquila

audax audax nests (Debus et al. 2007). However, as there can be intraspecific variation

in the timing of breeding periods, notably prolonged incubation due to egg cooling in

colder parts of a species’ geographic range (Nilsson & Henrik 1988; Olson, Vleck &

Page 53: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

47

Vleck 2006), data collected using video surveillance would help determine if the

incubation period estimate is accurate for Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagles.

The influence of weather on wedge-tailed eagles’ breeding behaviour and responses to

disturbance is another area requiring further research. Breeding behaviour can be

affected by weather, with more inclement weather corresponding to increased nest

attendance and an observed tolerance to human disturbance (Beer 1961; Afton 1980).

This increased nest attendance is likely to prevent exposure of eggs during cold, wet

weather to limit egg cooling and reduce the risk of microbial infection, a risk mitigated

by incubation and increased by moisture (D'Alba, Oborn & Shawkey 2010). Further

studies are necessary to determine if higher nest attendance and observed tolerance to

disturbance in poor weather conditions occurs in Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagles,

because the behavioural studies were only conducted on days with good weather to

minimise the risk of nest failure if birds were flushed by our approach.

An additional area of research is examining the prevalence of nest predation by corvids,

which may pose a greater threat than cooling to exposed eggs or chicks. Nest predation

is a common cause for nest failure in other eagle species (Gil-Sanchez et al. 2004).

Although nest predation of a Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle egg by a forest raven

(Corvis tasmanicus) was only observed at a single study nest, the relatively low number

of observation hours suggest that nest predation could be a prevalent occurrence while

adults are off the nest, particularly since the raven was able to extract the chick from the

egg within eight minutes from the eagle leaving the nest. As human presence can attract

corvids to nests and increase likelihood of nest predation (Strang 1980; Ibanez-Alamo,

Santllorente & Soler 2012), the high sensitivity of wedge-tailed eagles to humans may

put them at high risk of egg predation. Additionally, nest predation by corvids has been

shown to increase in forest fragments close to forest-farmland edges (Andrén 1992), and

many wedge-tailed eagle nests are located forest fragments adjacent to agricultural land

(Koch, Wiersma & Munks 2012).

Synthesis

Biotic and abiotic factors can affect the timing of the breeding season and the likelihood

of breeding success in the Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle on both an individual and

population level. Increased spring rainfall corresponded to delayed breeding seasons

Page 54: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

48

and lower nest success. Colder winter temperatures corresponded to delayed breeding

times, but was not related to the likelihood of breeding success. Despite expectations

developed from studies of other raptors, there was no evidence for a relationship

between prey abundance and the timing of the breeding season or likelihood of nest

success for the Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle. On an individual nest level, high nest

density within a territory, which is hypothesised to increase with levels of disturbance in

the territory, was associated with the lower nest success. Disturbance, particularly in the

form of pedestrian activity in close proximity to nests (100-200 m away) during the

breeding season often resulted in eagles flushing from the nest, leaving eggs and chicks

exposed and increasing the likelihood of nest failure at an individual nest level. Though

pedestrian activity was highly disturbing in close proximity to nests, road density near

nests was not found to influence breeding success. However, the impact of prey and

road density on breeding could be re-examined if more detailed and fine-scale data sets

become available, such as prey abundances within breeding territories during the winter

and spring or examining road usage rather than just the existence of roads. As the

timing of the breeding season has been determined retrospectively by chick age, a more

accurate determination of the onset of incubation and the length of each breeding phase

in Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagles, would enable confirmation of the findings regarding

the timing of the breeding season.

The breeding success of the Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagle is of high conservation

concern, as it is essential for maintaining a stable population. Results from this study

confirmed the long-standing hypothesis that Tasmanian wedge-tailed eagles are

extremely sensitive to disturbance, and their behavioural responses to human activity in

close proximity to the nest increase the likelihood of nest failure. This study highlighted

the influence of rainfall, temperature, and human influence on the breeding timing,

success and behaviour of the subspecies. However, as detailed above, further research is

essential to provide a deeper understanding of the sensitive nature of these eagles and

how human influence can be mitigated.

Page 55: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

49

5. References

Afton, A.D. (1980) Factors affecting incubation rhythms of northern shovelers. The

Condor, 82, 132-137.

Ahmed, R.A., Prusty, K., Jena, J., Dave, C., Das, S.K.R., Sahu, H.K. & Rout, S.D.

(2012) Prevailing human carnivore conflict in Kanha-Achanakmar Corridor,

central India. World Journal of Zoology, 7, 158-164.

Amadon, D. (1964) The evolution of low reproductive rates in birds. Evolution, 18,

105-110.

Anderson, D.R. (2008) Model based inference in the life sciences: a primer on evidence.

Springer, Fort Collins, CO.

Andrén, H. (1992) Corvid density and nest predation in relation to forest fragmentation:

a landscape perspective. Ecology, 73, 794-804.

Ardia, D.R., Cooper, C.B. & Dhont, A.A. (2006) Warm temperatures lead to early onset

of incubation, shorter incubation periods and greater hatching asynchrony in tree

swallows Tachycineta bicolor at the extremes of their range. Journal of Avian

Biology, 37, 137-142.

Ardia, D.R., Perez, J.H. & Clotfelter, E.D. (2010) Experimental cooling during

incubation leads to reduced innate immunity and body condition in nestling tree

swallows. Proc Biol Sci, 277, 1881-1888.

Bamford, A.J., Diekmann, M., Monadjem, A. & Mendelsohn, J. (2007) Ranging

behaviour of Cape Vultures Gyps coprotheres from an endangered population in

Namibia. Bird Conservation International, 17.

Bart, J. & Tornes, A. (1989) Importance of monogamous male birds in determining

reproductive success. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 24, 109-116.

Barton, K. (2013) MuMIn: Multi-model inference. http://CRAN.R-

project.org/package=MuMIn.

Bates, D., Maechler, M., Bolker, B. & Walker, S. (2014) lme4: Linear mixed effects

models using Eigen and S4. http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4.

Battin, J. (2004) When good animals love bad habits: ecological traps and the

conservation of animal populations. Conservation Biology, 18, 1482-1491.

Page 56: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

50

Bautista, L.M., Garcia, J.T., Calmaestra, R.G., Palacin, C., Martin, C., Morales, M.B.,

Bonal, R. & Vinuela, J. (2004) Effect of weekend road traffic on the use of

space by raptors. Conservation Biology, 18, 726-732.

Beale, C.M. & Monaghan, P. (2004) Human disturbance: people as predation-free

predators? Journal of Applied Ecology, 41, 335-343.

Beer, C.G. (1961) Incubation and nest building behaviour of black-headed gulls. I:

Incubation behaviour in the incubation period. Behaviour, 18, 62-106.

Bejder, L., Samuels, A., Whitehead, H., Finn, H. & Allen, S. (2009) Impact assessment

research: use and misuse of habituation, sensitisation and tolerance in describing

wildlife responses to anthropogenic stimuli. Marine Ecology Progress Series,

395, 177-185.

Bell, P. & Mooney, N.J. (1999) The Wedge-tailed Eagle Recovery Plan 1998-2003.

Tasmanian Parks and Wildlife Service, Hobart.

Bionda, R. & Brambilla, M. (2012) Rainfall and landscape features affect productivity

in an alpine population of Eagle Owl Bubo bubo. Journal of Ornithology, 153,

167-171.

Bird, D.M. (2007) Raptor Research and Management Techniques. Hancock House,

Blaine, WA, USA.

Blas, J., Sergio, F. & Hiraldo, F. (2009) Age-related improvement in reproductive

performance in a long-lived raptor: a cross-sectional and longitudinal study.

Ecography, 32, 647-657.

Bolker, B.M., Brooks, M.E., Clark, C.J., Geange, S.W., Poulsen, J.R., Stevens, M.H. &

White, J.S. (2009) Generalized linear mixed models: a practical guide for

ecology and evolution. Trends Ecol Evol, 24, 127-135.

Bollen, C. (1991) Breeding Behaviour and diet of the Australian Kestrel Falco

Cenchroides on the Southern Tablelands of New South Wales. Australian Bird

Watcher, 14, 44-50.

Booms, T.L. & Fuller, M.R. (2003) Time-lapse video system used to study nesting

Gyrfalcons. Journal of Field Ornithology, 74, 416-422.

Breuner, C.W. (2010) Stress and Reproduction in Birds. Hormones and Reprodcution in

Vertebrates (ed. D.O. Norris). Elsevier, Oxford.

Brooker, M.G. & Ridpath, M.G. (1980) The diet of the wedge-tailed ealge, Aquila

audax, in Western Australia. Australian Wildlife Research, 7, 433-452.

Page 57: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

51

Brown, L. (1976) Eagles of the World. David &Charles limited, Canada.

Brown, W.E. & Mooney, N.J. (1997) Modelling of Nesting Habitat of the Wedge-tailed

Eagle (Aquila audax fleayi) in Tasmania. Unpublished Report to the Tasmania

RFA Environment and Heritage Technical Committee, Commonwealth of

Australia and State of Tasmania. Unpublished Report to the Tasmania RFA

Environment and Heritage Technical Committee, Commonwealth of Australia

and State of Tasmania, Hobart.

Bryan, A.L.J., Hopkins, L.B., Eldridge, C.S., Brisbin, I.L. & Jagoe, C.H. (2005)

Behavior and food habits at a bald eagle nest in inland South Carolina.

Southeastern Naturalist, 4, 459-468.

Burnham, K.P. & Anderson, D.R. (2002) Model selection and multimodel inference: a

practical information-theoretic approach. Springer, New York.

Burridge, C.P., Brown, W.E., Wadley, J., Nankervis, D.L., Olivier, L., Gardner, M.G.,

Hull, C., Barbour, R. & Austin, J.J. (2013) Did postglacial sea-level changes

initiate the evolutionary divergence of a Tasmanian endemic raptor from its

mainland relative? Proc Biol Sci, 280, 20132448.

Butler, J. (2004) Free-ranging domestic dogs (Canis familiaris) as predators and prey in

rural Zimbabwe: threats of competition and disease to large wild carnivores.

Biological Conservation, 115, 369-378.

Byholm, P. & Kekkonen, M. (2008) Food regulates reproduction differently in different

habitats: experimental evidence in the goshawk. Ecology, 89, 1696-1702.

Carney, K.M. & Sydeman, W.J. (1999) A review of human disturbance effects on

nesting colonial waterbirds. Waterbirds: The International Journal of Waterbird

Biology, 22, 68-79.

Chace, J.F. & Walsh, J.J. (2006) Urban effects on native avifauna: a review. Landscape

and Urban Planning, 74, 46-69.

Collins, L. & Croft, D.B. (2007) Factors influencing chick survival in the wedge-tailed

eagle Aquila audax. Corella, 2007, 32-40.

Commonwealth of Australia (1999) Environment Protection and Biodiversity

Conservation Act 1999. Commonwealth of Australia, Canberra.

Culik, B., Adelung, D. & Woakes, A.J. (1990) The effect of disturbance on the heart

rate and behaviour of Adélie penguins (Pygoscelis adeliae) during the breeding

Page 58: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

52

season. Antarctic Ecosystems (eds K.R. Kerry & G. Hempel), pp. 177-182.

Springer Berlin Heidelberg.

D'Alba, L., Oborn, A. & Shawkey, M.D. (2010) Experimental evidence that keeping

eggs dry is a mechanism for the antimicrobial effects of avian incubation.

Naturwissenschaften, 97, 1089-1095.

Dahl, E.L., Bevanger, K., Nygard, T., Roskaft, E. & Stokke, B.G. (2012) Reduced

breeding success in white-tailed eagles at Smola windfarm, western Norway, is

caused by mortality and displacement. Biological Conservation, 145, 79-85.

De Santo, T.L., Willson, M.F., Sieving, K.E. & Armesto, J.J. (2002) Nesting biology of

tapaculos (Rinocryptidae) in fragmented south-temperate rainforests of Chile.

BioOne, 104, 482-495.

Debus, S.J.T., Hatfield, T.S., Ley, A.J. & Rose, A.B. (2007) Breeding Biology and Diet

of the Wedge-tailed Eagle Aquila audax in the New England Region of New

South Wales. Australian Field ornithology, 24, 93-120.

Deeming, D.C. & Fergusion, M.W.J. (1991) Egg Incubation: Its Effects on Embryonic

Development in Birds and Reptiles. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge,

UK.

Delaney, D.K., Pater, L.L., Carlile, L.D., Spadgenske, E.W., Beaty, T.A. & Melton, R.H.

(2011) Response of red-cockaded woodpeckers to military training operations.

Wildlife Monographs, 177, 1-38.

Dewey, S.R. & Kennedy, P.L. (2001) Effects of supplemental food on parental-care

strategies and juvenile survival of northern goshawks. The Auk, 118, 352-365.

DPIPWE (2013) Natural Values Atlas. State of Tasmania,

www.naturalvaluesatlas.tas.gov.au.

Duncan, I.J.H. & Wood-Gush, D.G.M. (1972) Thwarting of feeding behaviour in the

domestic fowl. Animal Behaviour, 20, 444-451.

Duncan Rastogi, A., Zanette, L. & Clinchy, M. (2006) Food availability affects diurnal

nest predation and adult antipredator behaviour in song sparrows, Melospiza

melodia. Animal Behaviour, 72, 933-940.

Dykstra, C.R., Meyer, M.W., Warnke, D.K., Karasov, W.H., Andersen, D.E.,

Bowerman, W.W. & Giesy, J.P. (1998) Low reproductive rates of Lake Superior

bald eagles: low food delivery rates or environmental contaminants? Journal of

Great Lakes Research, 24, 32-44.

Page 59: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

53

Ellis, A.E., Mead, D.G., Allison, A.B., Stallknecht, D.E. & Howerth, E.W. (2007)

Pathology and epidemiology of natural West Nile viral infection of raptors in

Georgia. Journal of Wildlife Diseases, 43, 214-223.

Ellis, D.H. (1981) Responses of raptorial birds to low level military jets and sonic

booms. (ed. U.S.A.F.a.U.S.F.a.W. Service). Institute for Raptor Studies, Oracle,

Arizona.

ESRI (2013) ArcMap 10.1. (ed. E.S.R. Institute).

Estes, J.A., Terborgh, J., Brashares, J.S., Power, M.E., Berger, J., Bond, W.J., Carpenter,

S.R., Essington, T.E., Holt, R.D., Jackson, J.B., Marquis, R.J., Oksanen, L.,

Oksanen, T., Paine, R.T., Pikitch, E.K., Ripple, W.J., Sandin, S.A., Scheffer, M.,

Schoener, T.W., Shurin, J.B., Sinclair, A.R., Soule, M.E., Virtanen, R. & Wardle,

D.A. (2011) Trophic downgrading of planet Earth. Science, 333, 301-306.

Fairhurst, G.D. & Bechard, M.J. (2005) Relationships between winter and spring

weather and northern goshawk (Accipiter gentilis) reproduction in northern

Nevada. Journal of Raptor Research, 39, 229-236.

Finke, D.L. & Denno, R.F. (2004) Predator diversity dampens trophic cascades. Nature,

429, 407-410.

Forero-Medina, G., Vieira, M.V., Grelle, C.E.V. & Almeida, P.J. (2009) Body size and

extinction risk in Brazil. Biota Neotropica, 9, 45-49.

Foster, A. & Wallis, R. (2010) Nest-site characteristics of the wedge-tailed eagle in

southern Victoria. Corella, 34, 36-44.

FPA (2012) Fauna Technical Note No. 1. Eagle nest searching, activity checking and

nest management. Forest Practices Authority, Hobart.

Gaffney, R.F. & Mooney, N.J. (1992) The Wedge-tailed Eagle Recovery Plan. Parks

and Wildlife Service, Tasmania.

Garamszegi, L.Z., Hirschenhauser, K., Bokony, V., Eens, M., Hurtrez-Bousses, S.,

Moller, A.P., Oliveira, R.F. & Wingfield, J.C. (2008) Latitudinal distribution,

migration, and testosterone levels in birds. Am Nat, 172, 533-546.

Gargett, V. (1977) A 13-year population study of the black eagles in the Matopos,

Rhodesia, 1964-1976. Ostrich, 48, 17-27.

Gargett, V., Gargett, E. & Damania, D. (1995) The influence of rainfall on Black Eagle

breeding over 31 years in the Matobo Hills, Zimbabwe. Ostrich, 66.

Page 60: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

54

Gil-Sanchez, J.M., Moleon, M., Otero, M. & Bautista, J. (2004) A nine-year study of

successful breeding in a Bonelli's eagle population in southeast Spain: a basis for

conservation. Biological Conservation, 118, 685-694.

Gill, J.A., Norris, K. & Sutherland, W.J. (2001) Why behavioural responses may not

reflect the population consequences of human disturbance. Biological

Conservation, 97, 265-268.

Gittleman, J.L., Funk, S.M., Macdonald, S.M. & Wayne, R.K. (2001) Carnivore

Conservation. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.

Gittleman, J.L. & Harvey, P.H. (1982) Carnivore home-range size, metabolic needs and

ecology. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology, 10, 57-63.

González, L.M., Arroyo, B.E., Margalida, A., Sánchez, R. & Oria, J. (2006a) Effect of

human activities on the behaviour of breeding Spanish imperial eagles (Aquila

adalberti): management implications for the conservation of a threatened species.

Animal Conservation, 9, 85-93.

González, L.M., Bustamante, J. & Hiraldo, F. (1992) Nesting habitat selection by the

Spanish imperial eagle Aquila adalberti. Biological Conservation, 59, 45-50.

González, L.M., Margalida, A., Sánchez, R. & Oria, J. (2006b) Supplementary feeding

as an effective tool for improving breeding success in the Spanish imperial eagle

(Aquila adalberti). Biological Conservation, 129, 477-486.

Gotmark, F. (1992) The effects of investigator disturbance on nesting birds. Current

Ornithology, 9.

Graham, K., Beckerman, A.P. & Thirgood, S. (2005) Human–predator–prey conflicts:

ecological correlates, prey losses and patterns of management. Biological

Conservation, 122, 159-171.

Grant, J.W.A., Chapman, C.A. & Richardson, K.S. (1992) Defended versus undefended

home range size of carnivores, ungulates and primates. Behavioral Ecology and

Sociobiology, 31.

Green, R.E., Newton, I., Shultz, S., Cunningham, A.A., Gilbert, M., Pain, D.J. &

Prakash, V. (2004) Diclofenac poisoning as a cause of vulture population

declines across the Indian subcontient. Journal of Applied Ecology, 41, 793-800.

Grier, J.W. (1969) Bald eagle behavior and productivity responses to climbing to nests.

Journal of Wildlife Management, 33, 961-966.

Page 61: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

55

Grubb, T.G. & Bowerman, W.W. (1997) Variations in breeding bald eagles responses

to jets, light planes and helicopters. Journal of Raptor Research, 31, 213-222.

Grubb, T.G., Delaney, D.K., Bowerman, W.W. & Wierda, M.R. (2010) Golden eagle

indifference to heli-skiing and military helicopters in northern Utah. Journal of

Wildlife Management, 74, 1275-1285.

Grubb, T.G. & King, R.M. (1991) Assessing human disturbance of breeding Bald

Eagles with Classification Tree models. Journal of Wildlife Management, 55,

500-511.

Hansen, A.J. (1987) Regulation of bald eagle reproductive rates in southeast Alaska.

Ecology, 68, 1387-1392.

Hanski, I., Hansson, L. & Henttonen, H. (1991) Specialist predators, generalist

predators, and the microtine rodent cycle. Journal of Animal Ecology, 60, 353-

367.

Hanssen, S.A., Hasselquist, D., Folstad, I. & Erikstad, K.E. (2005) Cost of reproduction

in a long-lived bird: incubation effort reduces immune function and future

reproduction. Proc Biol Sci, 272, 1039-1046.

Harder, M. (2000) Diet and breeding biology of the wedge-tailed eagle Aquila audax at

three nests in north-eastern New South Wales. Corella, 24, 1-5.

Holmes, T.L., Knight, R.L., Stegall, L. & Craig, G.R. (1993) Responses of wintering

grassland raptors to human disturbance. Wildlife Society Bulletin, 21, 467-468.

Holthuijzen, A.M.A., Eastlands, W.G., Ansell, A.R., Kochert, M.N., Williams, R.G. &

Young, L.S. (1990) Effects of blasting on behaviour and productivity of nesting

prairie falcons. Wildlife Society Bulletin, 18, 270-281.

Hull, C.L., Stark, E.M., Peruzzo, S. & Sims, C.C. (2013) Avian collisions at two wind

farms in Tasmania, Australia: taxonomic and ecological characteristics of

colliders versus non-colliders. New Zealand Journal of Zoology, 40, 47-62.

Hustler, K. & Howells, W.W. (1986) A population study of tawny eagles in the Hwange

National Park, Zimbabwe. Ostrich, 57, 101-106.

Ibanez-Alamo, J.D., Santllorente, O. & Soler, M. (2012) The impact of researcher

disturbance on nest predation rates: a meta-analysis. Ibis, 54, 5-14.

IUCN (2013) IUCN Redlist of Threatened Species. www.iucnredlist.org.

Page 62: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

56

Jiménez-Franco, M.V., Martínez, J.E. & Calvo, J.F. (2014) Patterns of nest reuse in

forest raptors and their effects on reproductive output. Journal of Zoology, 292,

64-70.

Johnson, J.B. & Omland, K.S. (2004) Model selection in ecology and evolution. Trends

Ecol Evol, 19, 101-108.

Jones, M.E., Jarman, P.J., Lees, C.M., Hesterman, H., Hamede, R.K., Mooney, N.J.,

Mann, D., Pukk, C.E., Bergfeld, J. & McCallum, H. (2007) Conservation

Management of Tasmanian Devils in the Context of an Emerging, Extinction-

threatening Disease: Devil Facial Tumor Disease. EcoHealth, 4, 326-337.

Jones, P.J. & Ward, P. (1976) The level of reserve protein as the proximate factor

controlling the timing of the breeding and clutch-size in the red-billed quelea

Quelea quelea. Ibis, 118, 547-574.

Kaisanlahti-Jokimäki, M.-L., Jokimäki, J., Huhta, E., Ukkola, M., Helle, P. & Ollila, T.

(2008) Territory occupancy and breeding success of the Golden Eagle (Aquila

chrysaetos) around tourist destinations in northern Finland. Ornis Fennica, 85,

2-12.

Kenney, S.P. & Knight, R.L. (1992) Flight distances of black-billed magpies in

different regimes of human density and persecution. The Condor, 94, 545-547.

Knight, R.L. (1995) Wildlife and Recreationists: Coexistence Through Management

and Research. Island Press, Washington, DC.

Koch, A.J., Martin, K. & Aitken, K.E. (2012) The relationship between introduced

European starlings and the reproductive activities of mountain bluebirds and tree

swallows in British Columbia, Canada. Ibis, 154, 590-600.

Koch, A.J., Wiersma, J. & Munks, S.A. (2012) Wedge-tailed Eagle Nest Monitoring

Project 2007-12. Nest site use, timing of breeding, and a review of the nesting

habitat model. Report to Roaring 40s, Threatened Species Section (DPIPWE)

and the Forest Practices Authority. Forest Practices Authority Scientific Report

(Draft).

Kochert, M.N. & Steenhof, K. (2012) Frequency of nest use by golden eagles in

southwestern Idaho. Journal of Raptor Research, 46, 239-247.

Kochert, M.N., Steenhof, K., Carpenter, L.B. & Marzluff, J.M. (1999) Effects of fire on

Golden Eagle territory occupancy and reproductive success. The Journal of

Wildlife Management, 63, 773-780.

Page 63: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

57

Korpimäki, E. & Norrdahl, K. (1991) Numerical and Functional Responses of Kestrels,

Short-Eared Owls, and Long-Eared Owls to Vole Densities. Ecology, 72, 814-

826.

Kostrzewa, A. & Kostrzewa, R. (1990) The relationship of spring and summer weather

with density and breeding performance of the Buzzard Buteo buteo, Goshawk

Accipiter gentilis and Kestrel Falco tinnunculus. Ibis, 132, 550-559.

Krapu, G.L. (1981) The role of nutrient reserves in mallard reproduction. The Auk, 98,

29-38.

Krüger, O. (2004) The importance of competition, food, habitat, weather and phenotype

for the reproduction of BuzzardButeo buteo:CapsuleVariation in reproduction

between territories was strongly influenced by intra- and interspecific

competition, phenotype, levels of rainfall and human disturbance. Bird Study, 51,

125-132.

Lambertucci, S.A. & Speziale, K.L. (2009) Some Possible Anthropogenic Threats to

Breeding Andean Condors (Vulturgryphus). Journal of Raptor Research, 43,

245-249.

Lehman, R.N., Kennedy, P.L. & Savidge, J.A. (2007) The state of the art in raptor

electrocution research: A global review. Biological Conservation, 136, 159-174.

Letnic, M. & Koch, F. (2010) Are dingoes a trophic regulator in arid Australia? A

comparison of mammal communities on either side of the dingo fence. Austral

Ecology, 35, 167-175.

Levenson, H. (1979) Time and activity budget of ospreys nesting in northern California.

The Condor, 81, 364-369.

Lloyd, P. (1999) Rainfall as a breeding stimulus and clutch size determinant in South

African arid-zone birds. Ibis, 141.

Lyon, B.E. & Montgomerie, R.D. (1985) Incubation feeding in Snow Buntings: female

manipulation or indirect male parental care? Behavioral Ecology and

Sociobiology, 17, 279-284.

MacDougall-Shackleton, S.A. & Hahn, T.P. (2007) Adaptation and evolution of

photoperiod response systems in birds. Journal of Ornithology, 148, 219-224.

Manning, J.A. & Kaler, R.S.A. (2011) Effects of survey methods on burrowing owl

behaviors. The Journal of Wildlife Management, 75, 525-530.

Page 64: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

58

Marti, C.D. (1994) Barn owl reproduction: patterns and variation near the limit of the

species' distribution. The Condor, 96, 468-484.

Martin, J., McIntyre, C.L., Hines, J.E., Nichols, J.D., Schmutz, J.A. & MacCluskie,

M.C. (2009) Dynamic multistate site occupancy models to evaluate hypotheses

relevant to conservation of Golden Eagles in Denali National Park, Alaska.

Biological Conservation, 142, 2726-2731.

Martin, T.E. (1987) Food as a limit on breeding birds: a life-history perspective. Annual

Review of Ecology and Systematics, 18, 453-487.

Martin, T.E., Auer, S.K., Bassar, R.D., Niklison, A.M. & Lloyd, P. (2007) Geographic

variation in avian incubation periods and parental influences on embryonic

temperature. Evolution, 61, 2558-2569.

Martinez, F. & Blanco, G. (2002) Use of alternative nests for clutch replacement in the

Egyptian vulture Neophron percnopterus. Ardeola, 49, 297-299.

Martinez, J.E., Pagan, I. & Calvo, J.F. (2006) Factors influencing territorial occupancy

and reproductive output in the Booted Eagle Hieraaetus pennatus. Ibis, 148, 807-

819.

McCallum, H. (2008) Tasmanian devil facial tumour disease: lessons for conservation

biology. Trends Ecol Evol, 23, 631-637.

McCallum, H. & Jones, M. (2006) To lose both would look like carelessness:

Tasmanian devil facial tumour disease. PLoS Biology, 4, e342.

McCollough, M.A., Todd, C.S. & Owen, R.B.J. (1994) Supplemental feeding program

for wintering bald eagles in Maine. Wildlife Society Bulletin, 22, 147-154.

McIntyre, C.L. & Schmidt, J.H. (2012) Ecological and environmental correlates of

territory occupancy and breeding performance of migratory Golden Eagles

Aquila chrysaetos in interior Alaska. Ibis, 154, 124-135.

Millsap, B., Breen, T.I.M., McConnell, E., Steffer, T., Phillips, L., Douglass, N., Taylor,

S. & Boal (2004) Comparative Fecundity and Survival of Bald Eagles Fledged

from Suburban and Rural Natal Areas in Florida. Journal of Wildlife

Management, 68, 1018-1031.

Mooney, N.M. & Holdsworth, M. (1991) The Effects of Disturbance on Nesting

Wedge-tailed eagles (Aquila audax fleayi) in Tasmania. Tasforests, 3, 15 - 31.

Mooney, N.M. & Taylor, R.J. (1996) Value of nest site protection in ameliorating the

effects of forestry operations on Wedge-tailed Eagles in Tasmania. Raptors in

Page 65: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

59

Human Landscape (eds Bird D. M., Varland D. D. & Negro J. J.), pp. 275 - 282.

Academic Press Inc. Raptor Research Foundation, San Diego, California.

Nankervis, D.L. (2010) Spatial patterns of genetic and morphological variation in the

wedge-tailed eagle (Aquila audax). University of Tasmania.

Nesbitt, S.A. (2001) Bald eagle population monitoring. Annual performance report. (ed.

F.G.a.F.W.F. Commission). Gainesville, Florida, USA.

Newton, I. (1979) Population Ecology of Raptors. T & AD Poyser, London, UK.

Nilsson, J. & Henrik, G.S. (1988) Incubation feeding as a male tactic for early hatching.

Animal Behaviour, 36, 641-647.

Ogada, D.L. & Keesing, F. (2010) Decline of Raptors over a Three-Year Period in

Laikipia, Central Kenya. Journal of Raptor Research, 44, 129-135.

Olsen, P. & Olsen, J. (1992) Does rain hamper hunting by breeding raptors? Emu, 92,

184-187.

Olsen, P.D. (2005) Wedge-Tailed Eagle, First edition edn. CSIRO Publishing.

Olson, C.R., Vleck, C.M. & Vleck, D. (2006) Periodic cooling of bird eggs reduces

embryonic embryonic growth efficiency. Physiological and Biochemical

Zoology, 79, 927-936.

Ontiveros, D., Caro, J. & Pleguezuelos, J.M. (2008a) Green plant material versus

ectoparasites in nests of Bonelli's eagle. Journal of Zoology, 274, 99-104.

Ontiveros, D., Caro, J. & Pleguezuelos, J.M. (2008b) Possible functions of alternative

nests in raptors: the case of Bonelli's Eagle. Journal of Ornithology, 149, 253-

259.

Ontiveros, D. & Pleguezneles, J.J. (2000) Influence of prey density in the distribution

and breeding success of Bonelli's eagles Hieraaetus fasciatus: management

implications. Biological Conservation, 93, 19-25.

Ost, M., Wickman, M., Matulionis, E. & Steele, B. (2008) Habitat-specific clutch size

and cost of incubation in eiders reconsidered. Oecologia, 158, 205-216.

Pagel, J.E., Whittington, D.M. & Allen, G.T. (2010) Interim golden eagle inventory and

monitoring protocols; and other recommendations. (ed. D.o.M.B. Management).

U.S. Fish and Wildlife Service.

Pain, D.J., Meharg, A.A., Ferrer, M., Taggart, M. & Penteriani, V. (2005) Lead

concentrations in bones and feathers of the globally threatened Spanish imperial

eagle. Biological Conservation, 121, 603-610.

Page 66: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

60

Penteriani, V., Balbontin, J. & Ferrer, M. (2003) Simultaneous effects of age and

territory quality on fecundity in Bonelli's Eagle Hieraaetus fasciatus. Ibis, 145

(online).

Penteriani, V., Delgado, M.M., Maggio, C., Aradis, A. & Sergio, F. (2005)

Development of chicks and predispersal behaviour of young in the Eagle Owl

Bubo bubo. Ibis, 2005, 155-168.

Perez-Lopez, M., Hermoso de Mendoza, M., Lopez Beceiro, A. & Soler Rodriguez, F.

(2008) Heavy metal (Cd, Pb, Zn) and metalloid (As) content in raptor species

from Galicia (NW Spain). Ecotoxicol Environ Saf, 70, 154-162.

Quinn, G.P. & Keough, M.J. (2002) Experimental design and data analysis for

biologists. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK.

R Core Team (2013) R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R

Foundation for Statistical Computing, http://www.R-project.org/.

Radford, A.N. & Plessis, M.A. (2003) The importance of rainfall to a cavity-nesting

species. Ibis, 145, 692-694.

Raupach, M.R., Briggs, P.R., Haverd, V., King, E.A., Paget, M. & Trudinger, C.M.

(2009) Australian Water Availibility Project (AWAP): CSIRO Marine and

Atmospheric Research Component: Final Report for Phase 3. (ed. C.f.A.W.a.C.

Research). Melbourne, Australia.

Ray, J., Redford, K.H., Steneck, R. & Berger, J. (2005) Large Carnivores and the

Conservation of Biodiversity. Island Press, Washington, DC.

Richardson, C.T. & Miller, C.K. (1997) Recommendations for protecting raptors from

human disturbance: a review. Wildlife Society Bulletin, 25, 634-638.

Ridpath, M.G. & Brooker, M.G. (1985) The breeding of the wedge-tailed eagle Aquila

audax in relation to its food supply in arid Western Australia. Ibis, 128, 177-194.

Robertson, G. (1987) Effect of drought on a breeding population of wedge-tailed eagles

Aquila audax. Emu, 87, 220-223.

Rogers, A.S. (2005) Quantifying Northern goshawk diets using remote cameras and

observations from blinds. Journal of Raptor Research, 39, 303-309.

Rounsevell, D.E., Taylor, R.J. & Hocking, G.J. (1991) Distribution records of native

terrestrial mammals in Tasmania. Wildlife Research, 18, 699-717.

Page 67: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

61

Saga, Ø. & Selås, V. (2012) Nest reuse by Goshawks after timber harvesting:

Importance of distance to logging, remaining mature forest area and tree species

composition. Forest Ecology and Management, 270, 66-70.

Salamolard, M., Butet, A., Leroux, A. & Bretagnolle, V. (2000) Responses of avian

predator to variations in prey density at a temperate latitude. Ecology, 81, 2428-

2441.

Samelius, G. & Alisauskas, R.T. (2001) Deterring arctic fox predation: the role of

parental nest attendance by lesser snow geese. Canadian Journal of Zoology, 79,

861-866.

Schettler, E., Fickel, J., Hotzel, H., Sachse, K., Streich, W.J., Wittstatt, U. & Frolich, K.

(2003) Newcastle disease virus and Chlamydia psittaci in free-living raptors

from eastern Germany. Journal of Wildlife Diseases, 39, 57-63.

Schmitz, O.J., Hamback, P.A. & Beckerman, A.P. (2000) Trophic cascades in terrestrial

systems: a review of the effects of carnivore removal on plants. Am Nat, 155,

141-153.

Schoener (1969) Models of optimal size for solitary predators. Am Nat, 103, 227-295

incomplete.

Sergio, F., Blas, J., Lopez, L., Tanferna, A., Diaz-Delgado, R., Donazar, J.A. & Hiraldo,

F. (2011) Coping with uncertainty: breeding adjustments to an unpredictable

environment in an opportunistic raptor. Oecologia, 166, 79-90.

Sergio, F. & Bogliani, G. (1999) Eurasian hobby density, nest area occupancy, diet, and

productivity in relation to intensive agriculture. The Condor, 101, 806-817.

Sergio, F., Marchesi, L., Pedrini, P., Ferrer, M. & Penteriani, V. (2004) Electrocution

alters the distribution and density of a top predato, the eagle owl Bubo bubo.

Journal of Applied ECOLOGY41.

Sergio, F. & Newton, I. (2003) Occupancy as a measure of territory quality. Journal of

Animal Ecology, 72.

Sergio, F., Newton, I.A.N., Marchesi, L. & Pedrini, P. (2006) Ecologically justified

charisma: preservation of top predators delivers biodiversity conservation.

Journal of Applied Ecology, 43, 1049-1055.

Shawkey, M.D., Firestone, M.K., Brodie, E.L. & Beissinger, S.R. (2009) Avian

incubation inhibits growth and diversification of bacterial assemblages on eggs.

PLoS One, 4, e4522.

Page 68: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

62

Smith, D.W., Peterson, R.O. & Houston, D.B. (2003) Yellowstone after wolves.

BioScience, 53, 330-340.

Southwell, C. & Fletcher, M. (1993) Repeatability and standardisation of roadside

spotlight counts of mammals in Tasmania. Australian Mammology, 16, 73-76.

Stalmaster, M.V. & Kaiser, J.L. (1997) Effects of recreational activity on wintering bald

eagles. Wildlife Monographs, 137, 5-46.

Stalmaster, M.V. & Newman, J.R. (1978) Behavioral responses of wintering bald eagles

to human activity. Journal of Wildlife Management, 42, 506-513.

Starker Leopold, A. & Wolfe, T.O. (1970) Food habits of nesting wedge-tailed eagles,

Aquila audax, in south-eastern Australia. CSIRO Wildlife Research, 15, 1-17.

State Government of Tasmania (1995) Threatened Species Protection Act 1995 (No. 83

of 1995), State of Tasmania, Hobart http://www.thelaw.tas.gov.au.

Steenhof, K., Kochert, M.N. & McDonald, T.L. (1997) Interactive effects of prey and

weather on golden eagle reproduction. Journal of Animal Ecology, 55, 350-362.

Steenhof, K. & Newton, I. (2007) Assessing Nestling Success and Productivity. Raptor

Research and Management Techniques (eds D.M. Bird & K.L. Bildstein), pp.

181 - 191. Hancock House.

Steenhof, K. & Peterson, B.E. (2009) American Kestrel Reproduction in Southwestern

Idaho: Annual Variation and Long-Term Trends. Journal of Raptor Research,

43, 283-290.

Steidl, R.J. & Anthony, R.G. (2000) Experimental effects of human activity on breeding

Bald Eagles. Ecological Applications, 10, 258-268.

Steidl, R.J., Kozie, G.J., Dodge, G.J., Pehowski, T. & Hogan, R.E. (1993) Effects of

human activity on breeding behavior of golden eagels in Wrangell-St. Elias

National Park and Preserve: a preliminary assessment. (ed. W.-S.E.N.P.a.P.

National Park Service). Copper Center, Alaska.

Steidl, R.J., Kozie, K.D. & Anthony, R.G. (1997) Reproductive success of bald eagles

in interior Alaska. The Journal of Wildlife Management, 61, 1313-1321.

Stout, W.E., Temple, S.A. & Papp, J.M. (2006) Landscape correlates of reproductive

success for an urban-suburban red-tailed hawk population. The Journal of

Wildlife Management, 70, 989-997.

Strang, C.A. (1980) Incidence of avian predators near people searching for waterfowl

nests. Journal of Wildlife Management, 44, 220-222.

Page 69: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

63

Strasser, E.H. & Heath, J.A. (2013) Reproductive failure of a human-tolerant species,

the American kestrel, is associated with stress and human disturbance. Journal

of Applied Ecology, 2013, doi: 10.1111/1365-2664.12103.

Tapia, L., Dominguez, J. & Rodriguez, L. (2009) Using probability of occurrence to

assess potential interaction between wind farms and a residual population of

golden eagle Aquila chrysaetos in NW Spain. Biodiversity and Conservation, 18,

2033-2041.

Thiollay, J.-M. (2006) The decline of raptors in West Africa: long term assessment and

the role of protected areas. Ibis, 148, 240-254.

Thurstans, S.D. (2009) Modelling the nesting habitat of the White-bellied Sea-Eagle

Haliaeetus leucogaster in Tasmania. Corella, 33, 51-65.

Tjernberg, M. (1983) Prey abundance and reproductive success of the golden eagle

Aquila chrysaetos in Sweden. Holarctic Ecology, 6, 17-23.

Treves, A. & Karanth, K.U. (2003) Human-carnivore conflict and perspectives on

carnivore management worldwide. Conservation Biology, 17, 1491-1499.

Trombulak, S.C. & Frissell, C.A. (2000) Review of ecological effects of roads on

terrestrial and aquatic communities. Conservation Biology, 14, 18-30.

Van der Zande, A.N. & Verstrael, T.J. (1985) Impacts of outdoor recreation upon nest-

choice and breeding success of the kestrel. Ardea, 73, 90-99.

Verhulst, S. & Nilsson, J.A. (2008) The timing of birds' breeding seasons: a review of

experiments that manipulated timing of breeding. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B

Biol Sci, 363, 399-410.

Visser, M.E., Holleman, L.J. & Caro, S.P. (2009) Temperature has a causal effect on

avian timing of reproduction. Proc Biol Sci, 276, 2323-2331.

Ward, J.E. (1940) The passing of the lyre-bird. Bulletin of the New York Zoological

Society, 63, 146-152.

Watson, J. (1997) The Golden eagle. The Bath Press, London.

Westmoreland, D. & Best, L.B. (1985) The effect of disturbance on Mourning Dove

nesting success. The Auk, 102, 774-780.

White, C.M. & Thurow, T.L. (1985) Reproduction of ferruginous hawks exposed to

controlled disturbance. Condor, 87, 14-22.

Page 70: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

64

Whitfield, D.P., McLeod, D.R.A., Fielding, A.H., Broad, R.A., Evans, R.J. & Haworth,

P.F. (2001) The effects of forestry on golden eagles on the island of Mull,

western Scotland. Journal of Applied Ecology, 38, 1208-1220.

Whitfield, D.P., McLeod, D.R.A., Watson, J., Fielding, A.H. & Haworth, P.F. (2003)

The association of grouse moor in Scotland with the illegal use of poisons to

control predators. Biological Conservation, 114, 157-163.

Whitfield, P.D., Reid, R., Haworth, P.F., Madders, M., Marquiss, M., Tingay, R. &

Fielding, A.H. (2009) Diet specificity is not associated with increased

reproductive performance of Golden Eagles Aquila chrysaetos in Western

Scotland. Ibis, 151.

Whitfield, P.D., Ruddock, M. & Bullman, R. (2008) Expert opinion as a tool for

quantifying bird tolerance to human disturbance. Biological Conservation, 141,

2708-2717.

iebe, . ., orpim ki, E. iehn, . (1 ) atching asynchrony in eurasian

kestrels in relation to the abundance and predictability of cyclic prey. Journal of

Animal Ecology, 67, 908-917.

Wiebe, K.L., Wiehn, J. & Korpimäki, E. (1998) The onset of incubation in birds: can

females control hatching patterns? Animal Behaviour, 55, 1043-1052.

Williams, J.B. (1996) Energetics of Avian Incubation. Avian Energetics and Nutritional

Ecology (ed. C. Carey), pp. 375-415. Springer US.

Williams, J.E. (2012) The evolutionary significance of Tasmania's rainforests and

associated vegetation types. A report to the Independent Verification Group.

Tasmania.

Zann, R.A., Morton, S.R., Jones, K.R. & Burley, N.T. (1995) The timing of breeding by

zebra finches in relation to rainfall in central Australia. Emu, 95, 208-222.

Page 71: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

65

65

Appendix 1. Coefficients, AIC values, and Akaike weights for complete set of candidate models of wedge-tailed eagle nest productivity. Random effects

NestID and Year are included in all models.

MODEL INTERCEPT NEAR

NESTS

PREY SPRING

RAIN

ROADS

1K

WINTER

MIN

WINTER

RAIN

DF logLik AIC ΔAIC WEIGHT

22 0.5919 -0.2217 -0.1158 -0.096 6 -411.264 834.5 0 0.115

6 0.2822 -0.2072 -0.1139 5 -412.379 834.8 0.23 0.102

8 0.9005 -0.2012 -0.695 -0.111 6 -411.654 835.3 0.78 0.078

24 1.096 -0.2155 -0.605 -0.1126 -0.088 7 -410.721 835.4 0.91 0.073

54 0.5137 -0.2293 -0.1407 -0.107 0.02931 7 -411.01 836 1.49 0.054

30 0.6714 -0.2157 -0.1152 -0.00174 -0.098 7 -411.191 836.4 1.85 0.045

56 1.107 -0.225 -0.746 -0.1481 -0.103 0.04224 8 -410.229 836.5 1.93 0.044

38 0.2299 -0.2099 -0.125 0.01325 6 -412.323 836.6 2.12 0.04

14 0.3343 -0.2026 -0.1134 -0.00124 6 -412.342 836.7 2.16 0.039

40 0.8861 -0.2061 -0.795 -0.1338 0.02726 7 -411.436 836.9 2.34 0.036

16 0.9367 -0.1976 -0.690 -0.1107 -0.00098 7 -411.631 837.3 2.73 0.029

32 1.154 -0.2104 -0.594 -0.1121 -0.00147 -0.090 8 -410.67 837.3 2.81 0.028

5 0.02991 -0.1176 4 -414.8 837.6 3.07 0.025

62 0.6037 -0.2226 -0.1415 -0.00204 -0.110 0.03093 8 -410.91 837.8 3.29 0.022

7 0.7325 -0.770 -0.1149 5 -413.928 837.9 3.33 0.022

21 0.271 -0.1194 -0.079 5 -414.074 838.1 3.62 0.019

64 1.178 -0.2181 -0.735 -0.1489 -0.00181 -0.105 0.04352 9 -410.152 838.3 3.78 0.017

46 0.2837 -0.2051 -0.1252 -0.00135 0.01403 7 -412.28 838.6 4.03 0.015

23 0.8798 -0.704 -0.1162 -0.070 6 -413.351 838.7 4.17 0.014

48 0.9279 -0.202 -0.790 -0.1338 -0.00114 0.02779 8 -411.405 838.8 4.28 0.013

13 0.1721 -0.1163 -0.00302 5 -414.586 839.2 4.64 0.011

15 0.8426 -0.752 -0.1138 -0.00271 6 -413.758 839.5 4.99 0.009

29 0.4513 -0.1181 -0.00353 -0.083 6 -413.779 839.6 5.03 0.009

Page 72: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

66

66

39 0.7128 -0.845 -0.1318 0.02047 6 -413.807 839.6 5.09 0.009

18 -1.223 -0.2281 -0.090 5 -414.971 839.9 5.41 0.008

53 0.211 -0.1343 -0.085 0.01789 6 -413.98 840 5.43 0.008

2 -1.489 -0.2143 4 -415.98 840 5.43 0.008

34 -0.7388 -0.2036 -0.04362 5 -414.984 840 5.44 0.008

4 -0.7388 -0.2084 -0.783 5 -415.073 840.1 5.62 0.007

55 0.8716 -0.810 -0.1425 -0.081 0.03165 7 -413.076 840.2 5.62 0.007

31 1.018 -0.678 -0.1151 -0.00319 -0.075 7 -413.113 840.2 5.7 0.007

20 -0.5686 -0.2217 -0.707 -0.083 6 -414.231 840.5 5.93 0.006

50 -0.6172 -0.2172 -0.078 -0.03747 6 -414.262 840.5 5.99 0.006

36 -0.2626 -0.2004 -0.631 -0.03627 6 -414.418 840.8 6.31 0.005

45 0.1399 -0.123 -0.0031 0.008071 6 -414.566 841.1 6.6 0.004

47 0.8287 -0.832 -0.1321 -0.00288 0.0222 7 -413.615 841.2 6.7 0.004

61 0.3937 -0.136 -0.00378 -0.091 0.02145 7 -413.646 841.3 6.76 0.004

52 -0.1845 -0.2136 -0.587 -0.073 -0.03073 7 -413.773 841.5 7.02 0.003

63 1.024 -0.790 -0.1437 -0.00352 -0.087 0.03452 8 -412.788 841.6 7.05 0.003

26 -1.115 -0.2208 -0.00214 -0.093 6 -414.864 841.7 7.2 0.003

10 -1.409 -0.2082 -0.00167 5 -415.915 841.8 7.3 0.003

42 -0.6898 -0.1992 -0.00123 -0.04306 6 -414.949 841.9 7.37 0.003

12 -0.6806 -0.2034 -0.776 -0.00138 6 -415.029 842.1 7.53 0.003

28 -0.4876 -0.2156 -0.695 -0.00183 -0.085 7 -414.153 842.3 7.78 0.002

58 -0.5454 -0.2117 -0.00171 -0.080 -0.03653 7 -414.192 842.4 7.86 0.002

33 -0.9512 -0.04894 4 -417.311 842.6 8.09 0.002

44 -0.2223 -0.1966 -0.627 -0.00108 -0.03584 7 -414.391 842.8 8.25 0.002

3 -0.992 -0.847 4 -417.492 843 8.46 0.002

1 -1.813 3 -418.524 843 8.52 0.002

35 -0.4285 -0.680 -0.04126 5 -416.665 843.3 8.8 0.001

60 -0.1254 -0.2086 -0.579 -0.00153 -0.075 -0.03001 8 -413.717 843.4 8.91 0.001

Page 73: Breeding behaviour and success of the Tasmanian wedge ... › __data › assets › pdf_file › 0004 › 113728 … · and friends back home for the Skype chats and phone calls throughout

67

67

17 -1.614 -0.074 4 -417.88 843.8 9.23 0.001

49 -0.8719 -0.058 -0.04448 5 -416.913 843.8 9.3 0.001

19 -0.8688 -0.790 -0.066 5 -416.981 844 9.43 0.001

41 -0.8242 -0.00291 -0.04733 5 -417.113 844.2 9.7 0.001

9 -1.627 -0.00349 4 -418.246 844.5 9.96 0.001

11 -0.8431 -0.827 -0.00315 5 -417.264 844.5 10 0.001

51 -0.3805 -0.65 -0.054 -0.03729 6 -416.324 844.6 10.12 0.001

43 -0.3171 -0.668 -0.00274 -0.03992 6 -416.49 845 10.45 0.001

25 -1.39 -0.00395 -0.078 5 -417.521 845 10.51 0.001

57 -0.7181 -0.00336 -0.0633 -0.04228 6 -416.651 845.3 10.77 0.001

27 -0.6913 -0.763 -0.00359 -0.070 6 -416.685 845.4 10.84 0.001

59 -0.2481 -0.633 -0.00316 -0.059 -0.03544 7 -416.092 846.2 11.66 0