(article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (harvey, watkins, mansell,...

35
Running head: ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION Acknowledgement This article may not exactly replicate the final version published in the journal. It is not the copy of record. Publisher copyright and source : Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, vol(issue), startpage- endpage Archived version Author manuscript: post-print (post-refereeing) Published version http://dx.doi.org/10. Journal homepage (Copyright) http://jep.textrum.com/ Issn 2043-8087 Author contact at KU Leuven [email protected] (article begins on next page) Abstract Thinking about Negative Events in Dysphoric Students Leads to Negative Generalization Jens Van Lier, Bram Vervliet, Koen Vanbrabant, Bert Lenaert, & Filip Raes

Upload: others

Post on 10-Jul-2020

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

Running head: ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION

Acknowledgement

This article may not exactly replicate the final version published in the journal. It is not the copy of record. Publisher copyright and source : Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, vol(issue), startpage-endpage

Archived version Author manuscript: post-print (post-refereeing)

Published version http://dx.doi.org/10.

Journal homepage (Copyright)

http://jep.textrum.com/ Issn 2043-8087

Author contact at KU Leuven

[email protected]

(article begins on next page)

Abstract Thinking about Negative Events in Dysphoric Students

Leads to Negative Generalization

Jens Van Lier, Bram Vervliet, Koen Vanbrabant, Bert Lenaert, & Filip Raes

Page 2: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 2

KU Leuven

This research was supported by the Center for Excellence on Generalization Research

(GRIP*TT; KU Leuven grant PF/10/005). Jens Van Lier is a research assistant of the

Research Foundation- Flanders (FWO)

Acknowledgements: The authors would like to thank H. De Meyer and A. Kestens for

their help with the data collection.

Page 3: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 3

ABSTRACT

The severity of many psychological disorders is associated with an increasing

amount of different stimuli or situations that elicit a maladaptive response. This is

known as the process of (over)-generalization and is often characteristic of

individuals with emotional disorders. Recently, abstract repetitive thought has been

proposed to be a transdiagnostic marker in several disorders (e.g., worry in anxiety;

rumination in depression). The present study examined the impact of an abstract

thinking style (compared to a more concrete thinking style) as a mechanism that

contributes to generalization. Students (N=83) were trained in either an abstract or

concrete thinking mode and then completed a learning phase and finally a

generalization test phase. High dysphoric students showed more negative

generalization in the abstract condition compared to the concrete condition. For low

dysphoric participants, the two thinking styles did not result in a difference in

generalization. Implications for the transdiagnostic value of an abstract processing

style in depression and anxiety are discussed.

Page 4: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 4

INTRODUCTION

Consider the following two examples. First, a man is diagnosed with depression. He

recently had a conflict with one of his best friends over a promise that was not kept.

This event has touched him deeply, leading him not to trust any of his friends. His

lack of trust has not only spread to his friends but even to his family and his wife.

This has led him to avoid contact with other people as much as possible. Secondly,

the neighbor’s dog bit Mark one day. His normal fear of the neighbor’s dog rapidly

spreads to a fear of many different dogs. Now he does not only fear the neighbor’s

dog or dogs of that same breed but even small friendly looking dogs. His fear of dogs

escalates to the extent that even a picture of a dog elicits fear. He becomes afraid of

leaving his house because of the chance of encountering a dog. He worries constantly

and his fear of dogs becomes a serious impairment in his life.

These two examples illustrate that it is maybe not so much the intensity of fear or

bad feelings that makes those feelings detrimental to an individual’s functioning.

Rather, many psychological problems are characterized by some form of over-

generalization (e.g., Hermans, Baeyens, & Vervliet, 2013). In the context of fear

conditioning, generalization-conditioning paradigms have been developed to study

generalization in humans (e.g. Lissek et al., 2008; Vervliet, Kindt, Vansteenwegen, &

Hermans, 2010). In the depression literature, using self-report measures of

overgeneralization (Carver & Ganellen, 1983), Carver (1998) found that negative

overgeneralization to the self predicted subsequent depressive symptoms. In a recent

study Fulford, Rosen, Johnson, and Carver (2012) also found a relationship between

negative overgeneralization to the self and symptoms of anxiety. Other studies have

found that depressed participants differ from non-depressed participants on negative

Page 5: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 5

generalizations across situations (e.g., Klar, Gabai, & Baron, 1997; van den Heuvel,

Derksen, Eling, & van der Staak, 2012). In this study we examine the role of another

important transdiagnostic mechanism, namely the abstract processing of events, in

generalization.

The way people process events (e.g., rumination) has been singled out as one of

the most important factors leading to a pathological outcome (Kinderman,

Schwannauer, Pontin, & Tai, 2013). The foundational and pioneering work of Susan

Nolen-Hoeksema has been of immeasurable value in this context. More recently, she

was also one of the major contributors in the area of transdiagnostic rumination

research (e.g., McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Aldao, Nolen-Hoeksema, &

Schweizer, 2010; Nolen-Hoeksma & Watkins, 2011).

Indeed, repetitive thinking (like rumination, but also worry) has been found to

characterize individuals with many different psychological problems and is thus

considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004;

McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao et al., 2010).

However, not every single form of repetitive thinking has been found to be

detrimental (Watkins, 2008). Again, in this context, Susan Nolen-Hoeksema led

pioneering work, e.g. her work on the distinction between a maladaptive rumination

type (brooding) and a relatively less maladaptive and in some cases even protective

form of rumination (reflection; Treynor, Gonzalez, & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2003). One

(other) element of repetitive thinking that has been consistently found to be

maladaptive is when this repetitive thinking is abstract (vs. concrete) in nature

(Watkins, 2008).

Watkins (2008) adopted the representations of abstract and concrete construal (or

processing mode) of repetitive thought that originated in the cognitive literature (e.g.,

Page 6: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 6

Trope & Liberman, 2003). “Abstract construals are general, superordinate, and

decontextualized mental representations that convey the essential gist and meaning of

events and actions, such as inferences of global traits that are invariant across

different situations (e.g., “laziness”) or representations of “why” an action is

performed and of its ends and consequences. In contrast, concrete construals are more

low-level mental representations that include subordinate, contextual, and incidental

details of events and actions, such as inferences of situation-specific states, such as

“tiredness,” or representations of the specific “how” details of an action and of the

means to an end” (Watkins, 2008, p. 187).

The impact of adopting an abstract vs. concrete processing mode has been

examined in several studies with different outcomes. In studies that manipulate these

processing modes, participants in the concrete condition are typically asked to focus

on contextual details and thus on the specific “how” details of an event (e.g., “Think

about what you could hear, smell, touch, etc. in that situation; Moberly & Watkins,

2006), whereas participants in the abstract condition are asked to focus on the causes,

meanings and implications and thus on the “why” aspects of an event (e.g., “Think

about the causes, consequences and implications for that situation”; Moberly &

Watkins, 2006). Experimental studies have shown unconstructive consequences of an

abstract processing mode, such as recalling more overgeneral autobiographical

memories (Raes, Watkins, Williams, & Hermans, 2008), endorsing more global self-

judgments (Rimes & Watkins, 2005), poorer problem solving (Watkins & Moulds,

2005), and poorer emotional recovery from prior failure (Watkins, 2004).

Although manipulations of abstract processing have been used in different

studies, none have looked at the impact of this processing mode on generalization.

However, it has been posited that the mechanism by which abstract processing has its

Page 7: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 7

unconstructive consequence may be via its impact on the degree of generalization in

response to emotional events (Watkins, 2008); but, again, this has not yet been put to

an experimental test. Therefore, we now directly tested this hypothesis by inducing

participants to adopt either an abstract or concrete processing mode and afterwards

assessed their generalization by using a generalization-conditioning paradigm

commonly used in the context of fear generalization (e.g., Lissek et al., 2008; Vervliet

et al., 2010). A generalization-conditioning paradigm usually consists of two phases,

i.e., an acquisition and generalization phase. In the acquisition phase a stimulus (CS+)

is paired with an aversive outcome (US). Next, in the generalization phase the

response to stimuli similar to the CS+ (e.g., morphs of the CS+), but never paired

with the US, is tested.

In our conditioning procedure we sought to mimic the kind of generalization

across situations seen in depression (e.g., Klar et al., 1997). After some negative

encounters, depressed individuals will make abstraction of the specifics of the

individual situations and (over)-generalize to situations that look (most) similar to the

original situation. By using a conditioning paradigm with negative and neutral events

and including an induction procedure of abstract and concrete thought that has

previously proved to be successful (Moberly & Watkins, 2006) we aim to bridge a

gap between the anxiety literature, in which the use of such procedures is very

common, and the depression literature.

We expected to observe more generalization in participants who received an

abstract processing mode induction compared to those who received a concrete

processing mode. However, Watkins (2008) notes that the negative consequences of

the inductions that trigger abstract thought are often moderated by current mood.

Consequently, studies have found that these abstract thought manipulations often only

Page 8: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 8

have differential effects in vulnerable groups (i.e., individuals experiencing dysphoric

mood; e.g., Watkins & Teasdale, 2004; Watkins & Moulds, 2005). This means that

the negative effects of the abstract thought induction are mostly (or only) present for

people who are already in a dysphoric mood or people high in the tendency to engage

in repetitive negative thinking. Higher levels of depressive symptoms and repetitive

negative thinking are predicted to moderate (i.e., strengthen) the relationship between

induced abstract (vs. concrete) processing style and generalization.

A somewhat related issue is that in general, depressed individuals show more

generalization for negative events than do non-depressed individuals. However, for

neutral events, Klar and colleagues (1997) found no difference in the degree of

generalization between depressed and non-depressed participants. Consistent with

these findings, we further hypothesized that high dysphoric participants and

individuals high in repetitive negative thinking that are induced in an abstract thinking

mode (vs. a concrete thinking mode) would show more generalization for stimuli with

a closer resemblance to the original negative stimulus (i.e., negative generalization).

METHOD

Participants

We opted to study adolescent participants because this an important age period

concerning the onset of a first major depressive episode (e.g., Zisook et al., 2007).

Participants were recruited from a secondary school (Sint-Jozefscollege, Aalst) in

Flanders, Belgium. All participated without compensation. A total of 83 students (54

boys) participated in the study. Their mean age was 16.9 years (SD = .68, age range:

16 – 19; age info missing for two participants). The experiment in this study was

conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the local

Page 9: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 9

ethics committee of the University of Leuven. Written informed consent was obtained

from all participants1.

Materials

Training materials for abstract and concrete processing mode (Moberly &

Watkins, 2006). All participants are presented with the same six scenarios as training

materials (three negative: “falling out”, “having a bad day-messing up”, “being new”;

three positive: “dinner party”, “a successful job interview”, “learning to surf”). In

both conditions, participants are asked to spend one minute concentrating on each

scenario. Following each scenario, they are instructed to write down answers to three

questions about the scenario to further reinforce the induced processing mode.

Moberly and Watkins (2006) used this procedure to successfully induce the intended

processing modes: abstract thought mode and concrete thought mode.

In the abstract condition participants are presented with a brief description of the

situation. For example, for the “being new” scenario, the text reads: “You arrive at the

university and you don’t know anyone. You wander around campus and the halls. By

the end of the first day, everyone else seemed to have formed groups and are chatting

and laughing, but you seem to end up alone.” Participants are asked to think about this

situation in words and most importantly to think about the causes, consequences and

implications of each situation. The abstract thought induction is further reinforced by

asking three questions focusing on abstract aspects of each situation (e.g., “What do

you think about yourself at the end of the day?”, “Why did you find it hard to talk to

others?”, “What do you think will happen tomorrow?”).

In the concrete thought condition participants are presented with one or more

black and white photographs illustrating the situation. The photographs encourage a

1 In Belgium, where the experiment was conducted, age of consent is 16.

Page 10: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 10

more concrete, visual imagery-based approach to the situation. For example, for the

“being new” scenario the photographs show a girl sitting alone on a staircase

watching outside with people around her but the people do not notice her. Participants

are asked to use the photographs as a starting point to form a detailed image of the

situation, as if they are playing a movie of that situation in your head. Specifically,

they are asked to “imagine as vividly as possible what you can see, hear, feel, touch

and experience in that situation”. The concrete thought induction is further reinforced

by asking three questions focusing on concrete aspects of each situation (e.g., “What

are the other people doing?”, “What is happening outside the window?”).

Instructions about the training of abstract and concrete thought were read out

loud by the experimenter. In addition, participants could read the instructions on the

first page of the booklet. As recommended by Moberly and Watkins (2006), the

experimenter controlled the time (using a stop-watch) that could be used to think

about the scenario (one minute) and the time spent on writing the answers to the three

questions (one minute and 30 seconds) to ensure that each participant spent an equal

amount of time on every scenario.

Generalization paradigm. Stimuli. Two pictures of inside a train served as

conditioned stimuli, the CS+ and CS- (see ‘blue’ and ‘red’ train in Figure 1). The

generalization stimuli (GS) were seven morphs between the two CSs. We used

specialized software (Norrkross MorphX) to form these GSs in seven gradual steps.

Each of the seven resulting pictures served as a generalization stimulus. Figure 1

shows the CS+ (CS-) and the resulting GSs. The GSs resemble the CS+ (CS-) but in

seven decreasing (increasing) steps of perceptual similarity. For half of the

participants, the ‘blue’ train served as the CS+, while in the other half the ‘red’ train

Page 11: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 11

served as the CS+. All stimuli were presented in the middle of the computer screen on

a black background.

Scenarios. Following the presentation of the CS+ and CS- in the acquisition

phase, participants heard a positive or neutral scenario of an event that took place in

that train. Twenty scenarios (10 aversive/failure, 10 neutral) were first rated for

valence in a pilot study. The scenarios were always matched in structure. For

example, one of the negative/failure scenarios was as follows: “You are sitting in this

train, and the conductor asks for your ticket. The conductor suddenly starts yelling at

you while others are looking at you. You don’t speak his language and you only

notice that he is really mad and that you have to pay an extra fee.” A neutral scenario,

for example, reads: “You are sitting in this train, and the conductor asks for your

ticket. The conductor asks something else, but you don’t speak his language. You

excuse yourself for not speaking the language. He smiles friendly and goes along.

You get back to watching outside.”

The five most negatively rated failure scenarios and their neutral matches were

chosen for the actual experiment. These failure scenarios were created to be

depression relevant, such that they invoked situations that involved failure or other

people laughing or being angry2. Scenarios were recorded with recording software by

the first author. The recordings of all scenarios were relatively matched (i.e., range:

16–19 seconds). The aversive/failure scenarios served as the US. Hence, the CS+

(e.g., the ‘blue’ train) was always paired with an aversive/failure scenario; that is,

participants learnt that every time they supposedly sit in this ‘blue’ train something

negative happens. The CS- (e.g., the ‘red’ train) was never followed by an aversive

2 We acknowledge that such scenarios are possibly not only relevant for depression but also for

individuals with social anxiety symptoms. However, we would argue that ‘people laughing at you’ and

‘feeling inferior’ are situations that depressed people might also relate to.

Page 12: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 12

scenario but always by a neutral scenario; that is, participants learnt that every time

they supposedly sit in this ‘red’ train something neutral happens.

US-expectancy. Participants were requested to indicate to what extent they expect

an aversive/failure event to occur in that train on an 11-point scale ranging from 0

(definitely NO negative event will follow) to 10 (there will definitely follow a negative

event). This scale was presented below the picture on the computer screen.

Participants had to move a red dot (using the left and right arrows) across the 11-point

expectancy scale to select their desired expectancy score. They confirmed their

answer by hitting ‘Enter’. There was no time limit for this response.

Valence. Participants were requested to indicate to what extent the train felt as

negative or positive to them. They were asked to not think too much about it and to

reflect their opinion. The 11-point valence scale ranged from negative (0) to positive

(10).

Manipulation check. Participants were asked to rate this question: “After hearing

each scenario you had to think in a way that you trained at the start of the experiment.

How well were you able to do this? Indicate on the scale below. Try to judge yourself

in an honest way.” The five-point scale ranged from not well to very well.

Procedure of generalization paradigm. Affect 4.0 software (Spruyt, Clarysse,

Vansteenwegen, Baeyens, & Hermans, 2010) was used to control the stimulus

sequence, the presentation of the stimuli and the inter trial intervals. Participants

started off with a pre-acquisition phase in which each CS was presented three times in

a random order. They were instructed to rate their US expectancy; however, at this

point in the experiment they had not yet experienced the US. This phase was

primarily used to familiarize participants with the pictures.

Page 13: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 13

After this phase, participants received instructions about the acquisition phase

and were asked to put their headphones on to listen to the spoken scenarios

individually. One CS (CS-) was presented five times and was always followed by a

neutral scenario that the participants heard after they gave their US-expectancy rating

(by pressing ‘Enter’). The other CS (CS+), also presented five times, was always

followed by an aversive/failure event. The trials were presented in a random order. In

total the participants had to give 10 US-expectancy ratings. After every trial they were

instructed to think back about the train scenario in the particular mode that was

previously trained. Thus, in the abstract condition group they were again instructed to

think about the causes, implications and consequences of the situation. The CS

remained on the screen for 30 seconds while the participant was thinking.

Once participants completed the acquisition phase they could remove the

headphones. In the generalization test phase there were two blocks of 13 trials. The

CSs were each presented three times (never followed by a US) and the seven GSs

(never followed by a US) were presented one time in each block. US-expectancy was

rated in the same way as in the previous phases. In the last phase valence was

measured for the CSs and the GSs. The CSs were each presented three times and the

seven GSs were each presented once. After completing the valence measure,

participants answered the manipulation check question. This was the last screen on

the computer. Upon completion, participants were instructed to return to the booklet

for the remaining questionnaires.

Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ; Ehring, Zetsche, Weidacker, Wahl,

Schönfeld & Ehlers, 2011). The PTQ is a 15-item questionnaire measuring repetitive

negative thought that is independent of a specific content. The items assess the

repetitiveness (e.g., The same thoughts keep going through my mind again and again),

Page 14: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 14

intrusiveness (e.g., Thoughts just pop into my mind), difficulties to disengage (e.g., I

can’t stop dwelling on them) and unproductiveness of recurrent negative thinking

(e.g., I keep asking myself questions without finding an answer) as well as the degree

to which rumination captures mental capacity (e.g., My thoughts prevent me from

focusing on other things). The instructions read: “In this questionnaire, you will be

asked to describe how you typically think about negative experiences or problems.

Please read the following statements and rate the extent to which they apply to you

when you think about negative experiences or problems.” Participants are asked to

rate the items on a five-point scale, from 0 (never) to 4 (almost always). The PTQ has

shown good psychometric properties (Ehring et al., 2011). In this study the Dutch

version (PTQ-NL) was used (Ehring, Raes, Weidacker & Emmelkamp, 2012;

Cronbach’s α = .93). The internal consistency of the PTQ-NL in this sample was good

(α = .89). A recent study further showed that the PTQ-NL has prospective predictive

validity for depressed mood (Raes, 2012).

Depression Anxiety Stress Scales (DASS 21). The DASS 21 is a 21-item self-

report questionnaire that measures negative emotional states of depression, anxiety

and stress (Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995). The depression subscale has good internal

consistency (α = .81, Lovibond & Lovibond, 1995). The 7-item depression subscale

of the Dutch version (de Beurs, Van Dyck, Marquenie, Lange, & Blonk, 2001) was

used in this study. The instructions read: “Please read each statement and circle a

number (0, 1, 2 or 3) which indicates how much the statement applied to you over the

past week.” Internal consistency for the depression subscale in this sample was good

(α = .78). Each item is scored on a four-point scale, from 0 (did not apply to me at all)

to 3 (applied to me very much, or most of the time).

Procedure

Page 15: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 15

The study was conducted in a group setting in a computer room of the school.

Classes were randomly allocated to either the concrete or abstract processing mode

condition. Participants gave written informed consent and received an individual

booklet with all materials, presented in the same order. Instructions about the training

of abstract and concrete thought were read out loud by the experimenter. In addition,

participants could read the instructions on the first page of the booklet. After the

training phase, participants were instructed to start the second phase (i.e., the

generalization paradigm) on the computer, and to complete it at their own pace. When

participants were finished with the computer task they were prompted to go back to

the booklet for the remaining questionnaires.

Data analysis.

For acquisition, US-expectancy data were analyzed with a repeated measures

ANOVA with CS type (2 levels: CS+, CS-) and Trial (1-5) as within-subject variables

and Condition (2 levels: abstract and concrete processing mode) as a between-subject

variable. For generalization, US-expectancy and valence data were analyzed with a

repeated measures ANOVA with Condition (2 levels: abstract and concrete

processing mode) as a between-subjects factor and Stimulus (9 levels: CS+, GS1,

GS2, GS3, GS4, GS5, GS6, GS7, CS-) as a within-subject factor. DASS-Depression

was included as a covariate. These analyses were followed by trend analyses. We

considered it important to conduct the latter in order to test the shape of the

generalization gradient. Quadratic or cubic trend analyses of the whole set of 9 stimuli

would show that there might be a difference only at one end of the stimulus set, with

the a priori hypothesis that the gradient would only show a difference between

Conditions for vulnerable participants closest to the CS+ (i.e., showing negative

Page 16: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 16

generalization). When a quadratic or cubic trend appeared, we opted to look at neutral

(GS5, GS6, GS7, CS-) and negative (CS+, GS1, GS2, GS3) generalization separately.

RESULTS

Participant Characteristics

Means, standard deviations, scoring ranges and correlations for all variables included

are presented in Table 1. There were no differences between the abstract and concrete

processing mode except that participants in the concrete condition scored higher on

the PTQ, t(75) = 2.46, p < .05.

Pre-acquisition

At this stage we should expect no difference in US-expectancy between the two

train-stimuli because they have not yet been paired with an aversive/failure event and

this phase was only used to familiarize participants with the procedure and the

stimuli. However, collapsed across the three trials, it seemed that the ‘red’ train at

baseline had a marginally higher expectancy of a negative event than the ‘blue’ train,

t(81) = 1.31, p = .07. There were no differences between the conditions of abstract

and concrete processing mode.

Acquisition

A repeated measures ANOVA with CS type (2 levels: CS+, CS-) and Trial (1-5)

as within-subject variables and Condition (2 levels: abstract and concrete processing

mode) as a between-subject variable was conducted. The analysis showed a main

effect of Trial, F(4,320) = 12.53, p < .001, ηp2 = .14, a main effect of CS type, F(1,80)

= 51.18, p < .001, ηp2= .39, and a significant CS type × Trial interaction, F(4,320) =

17.12, p < .001, ηp2 = .18. Contrast analyses showed no difference between CS+ and

CS- at trial 1, t(81) < 1, but as expected, at trial 5 the US-expectancy was higher for

the CS+ than for the CS-, t(81) = 7.35, p < .001. There was no main effect of

Page 17: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 17

Condition, F(1,80) < 1, and no interaction effects with Condition. Hence, the

acquisition did not differ for the concrete and the abstract condition (Figure 2). When

we include DASS-Depression as a continuous between-subject variable in the model,

the same main and interaction effects were found. However, there was a main effect

of DASS-Depression, F(1,76) = 6.69, p < .05, ηp2 = .08, and a marginally significant

DASS-Depression × Condition interaction, F(1,76) = 3.18, p = .08, ηp2 = .04. To

clarify this interaction, additional analyses showed no relation between overall

expectancy scores and DASS-Depression for individuals in the abstract condition (r =

.11). However, for individuals in the concrete condition, there were higher overall

expectancy scores for individuals high on DASS-Depression (r = .41, p < .05).

Generalization Test3

Results presented here are for the first block only. To include participants that

actually learned the difference between the CS+ and CS- (in the generalization test

phase), we selected participants with a mean score of CS+ that was higher than the

CS-, i.e. (Mean CS+) – (Mean CS-) > 0. Fifteen participants did not acquire this

criterion (11 in the abstract condition and 4 in the concrete condition)4.

All analyses were corrected using the Greenhouse-Geisser method when

sphericity was violated. A repeated measures ANOVA with Stimulus (9 levels: CS+,

GS1, GS2, GS3, GS4, GS5, GS6, GS7, CS-) as a within-subject factor showed a main

effect for stimulus, F(4.90, 328.50) = 104.96, p < .001, ηp2 = .61. Polynomial contrast

analysis showed that there was a linear trend, F(1,67) = 305.52, p < .001, ηp2 = .82,

reflecting a clear generalization curve for the sample as a whole (Figure 2).

3 Because of the baseline difference in PTQ scores between conditions, we also controlled for PTQ in

all the analysis. Importantly, the results remained the same. For simplicity, we only report the results

here without controlling for PTQ. 4 The excluded participants did not differ from the included participants for demographics, PTQ,

DASS-Depression and manipulation check.

Page 18: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 18

We conducted a repeated measures ANOVA with Condition (2 levels: abstract

and concrete processing mode) as a between-subjects factor and Stimulus (9 levels:

CS+, GS1, GS2, GS3, GS4, GS5, GS6, GS7, CS-) as a within-subject factor and

included DASS-Depression as a covariate. This ANOVA revealed a main effect of

Stimulus, F(4.90, 308.52) = 47.41, p < .001, ηp2 = .43. Within-subject contrasts

showed a significant linear trend, F(1,63) = 136.77, p < .001, ηp2 = .69, and a

significant cubic trend, F(1,63) = 11.18, p < .01, ηp2 = .15. There were no other

significant effects, but polynomial contrasts revealed a significant cubic trend of the

DASS-Depression × Stimulus interaction, F(1,63) = 12.45, p < .01, ηp2 = .17 , and a

significant cubic trend of the DASS-Depression × Stimulus × Condition interaction,

F(1,63) = 4.42, p < .05, ηp2 = .07. We hypothesized that the negative effects of the

abstract thought induction would be most intensified for people already in a dysphoric

mood and would be most apparent for negative generalization (i.e., more at the CS+

side of the gradient). Both of these cubic trends show that the differences in gradients

are not linear and that this difference is only found at one end of the gradient and is

dependent upon the mood of the participants. Therefore, we further analyzed negative

and neutral generalization separately by looking at the CS+ and GS 1 through 3 for

negative generalization, and examined GS5 through 7 and CS- for neutral

generalization.

Negative Generalization

We conducted a repeated measures ANOVA with Condition (2 levels: abstract

and concrete processing mode) as a between-subjects factor and Negative Stimulus

(CS+, GS1, GS2, GS3) as a within-subject factor and included DASS-Depression as a

covariate. This ANOVA showed a main effect for Negative Stimulus, F(2.41, 151.97)

= 22.09, p < .001, ηp2 = .26, with a significant linear trend, F(1, 63) = 42.16, p < .001,

Page 19: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 19

ηp2 = .40, and a marginally significant cubic trend, F(1, 63) = 3.81, p = .06, ηp

2 = .06.

There was a marginally significant effect for the Condition × DASS-Depression ×

Negative Stimulus interaction, F(2.41, 151.97) = 2.47, p = .08, ηp2 = .04, but

importantly polynomial contrasts revealed a significant linear trend of the interaction,

F(1, 63) = 4.84, p < .05, ηp2 = .07. As hypothesized, this analysis suggests that the

difference in generalization between the abstract and concrete processing mode

condition, especially for participants who scored higher on DASS-Depression,

increased with more dissimilarity from the CS+ (i.e., the original negative stimulus).

This is reflected in a less steep generalization gradient for more dysphoric participants

in the abstract condition for stimuli CS+ through GS3 (Figure 3).

Neutral Generalization

We conducted a repeated measures ANOVA with Condition (2 levels: abstract

and concrete processing mode) as between-subjects factors and Neutral Stimulus

(GS5, GS6, GS7, CS-) as a within-subject factor and included DASS-Depression as a

covariate. This ANOVA showed a main effect for Neutral Stimulus, F(2.40, 151.15)

= 25.83, p < .001, ηp2 = .29, with a significant linear trend, F(1, 63) = 55.29, p < .001,

ηp2 = .47 . There were no other main or interaction effects. As hypothesized, these

findings suggest that there is no difference in neutral generalization between

conditions.

PTQ.

When we included PTQ instead of DASS-Depression as a covariate, the results

were in the same direction. However, there were no significant results. Note that the

results might have been influenced by the baseline difference in PTQ between the

abstract and concrete condition.

Valence

Page 20: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 20

A repeated measures ANOVA with Stimulus (CS+, GS1, GS2, GS3, GS4, GS5,

GS6, GS7, CS-) as a within-subject factor showed a main effect for Stimulus,

F(8,536) = 28.26, p < .001, ηp2 = .30. Polynomial contrast analysis showed a linear

trend, F(1,67) = 51.28, p < .001, ηp2 = .43. Hence, there was a clear generalization

curve for the sample as a whole. However, there were no significant interaction

effects between Condition (between-subject) and DASS-Depression or PTQ

(covariates).

DISCUSSION

The present study was conducted in order to examine the impact of abstract

thought on generalization in a depression relevant conditioning procedure. We

hypothesized that participants who received an abstract thinking mode induction

would show more generalization than participants who received a concrete thinking

mode induction. As previous studies have found that induction manipulations may

only have differential effects in vulnerable/dysphoric groups (Watkins & Teasdale,

2004; Watkins & Moulds, 2005; Watkins, 2008), we expected that depression score

might influence the effect of abstract processing style on generalization (i.e., we

predicted more generalization for dysphoric individuals). While most of the negative

repetitive thinking literature is based on adult studies, our results are based on an

adolescent sample, which is an important age period concerning the onset of a first

major depressive episode (e.g., Zisook et al., 2007). Our results showed that

participants with more depressive symptoms in the abstract induction showed more

generalization compared to participants in the concrete induction group. Interestingly,

this increased generalization was only found for the negative stimuli (the

generalization train-stimuli that were closest to the original train-stimulus that was

Page 21: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 21

always paired with a negative/failure event). This effect was not found for participants

with lower levels of depressive symptoms.

Klar and colleagues (1997) found that depressed individuals tend to generalize

more across negative situations than non-depressed individuals, whereas non-

depressed individuals tend to generalize more across positive situations. No

differences between depressed and non-depressed were found for neutral situations. In

our study, when dysphoric participants were instructed to think abstractly about

negative stimuli this led to negative generalization. Accordingly, participants who

were instructed to adopt a concrete processing style showed no increased

generalization for the negative stimuli. We found no differences in generalization to

the neutral stimulus according to processing mode or dysphoria.

There is evidence that depressed individuals tend to endorse a more abstract

thinking style, especially to negative information or problems (e.g., Takano & Tanno,

2010; Watkins & Moulds, 2007). Hence, these studies and our findings might further

explain the difference in overgeneralization between depressed and non-depressed

individuals for negative situations. However, it remains unclear as to how abstract or

concrete thinking might influence generalization for positive stimuli/situations.

Watkins (2011) posits that abstract processing might have beneficial effects when

faced with easy (positive) or familiar situations but suggests that depressed

individuals do not use the most adaptive mode of processing when faced with positive

content. Related to this, a study by Marigold, Holmes and Ross (2007) showed that

people low in self-esteem tend to identify positive events (compliments from their

romantic partner) more concretely than participants who are high in self-esteem. This

finding could suggest that depressed individuals might act similarly (Watkins, 2011).

If depressed individuals indeed adopt a concrete processing style when they are faced

Page 22: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 22

with positive stimuli or events, this could explain why non-depressed (compared to

depressed) individuals generalized more across positive situations in Klar and

colleagues’ study (1997). Therefore, further research could focus on positive

generalization by using more explicit positive stimuli in a depression relevant

generalization paradigm.

It has been noted that little is known about cognitive (vulnerability) factors that

influence generalization (see Hermans et al., 2013). Our study builds on the

knowledge about one possible variable (i.e., abstract or concrete processing mode)

that could have an impact upon generalization. In another study, Lenaert et al. (2012)

focused on the effects of autobiographical memory specificity on generalization. They

found that participants high in overgeneral autobiographical memory (OGM) showed

stronger generalization (also with a US-expectancy measure) to the GSs than

participants low in OGM. OGM is known to predict a negative development in

depression (Raes, Hermans, Williams, Beyers, Brunfaut, & Eelen, 2006). Moreover,

studies have found that abstract thought also leads to more overgeneral

autobiographical memory retrieval (Watkins & Teasdale, 2001; Raes, Watkins,

Williams, & Hermans, 2008). Therefore, our results might converge with the findings

of OGM research (Watkins & Teasdale, 2001; Raes et al., 2008; Lenaert et al. 2012)

because we experimentally induced participants in an abstract thinking style (also

leading to more OGM; see Raes et al., 2008), which lead to negative generalization in

dysphoric individuals. Hence, our results provide further evidence in support of the

claim that an abstract thinking style is an important underlying mechanism for related

unconstructive cognitive outcomes.

This study has several limitations. One limitation is that the induction of abstract

and concrete processing style was limited to six scenarios whereas the full induction

Page 23: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 23

consists of 12 scenarios (Moberly & Watkins, 2006). This may have constrained the

size of our effects and may account for the absence of generalization in the later

stages of the experiment, namely for the second block and the valence measure. Also,

our manipulation check was different to that which has been employed in other

research using inductions of abstract and concrete processing mode5. Another

limitation is that we used a community sample which was only mildly dysphoric. In

future research it would be interesting to see whether these effects would generalize

to, or even be stronger for currently depressed or remitted depressed patients. We

might expect that generalization would be even greater in a depressed group when

they are instructed to adopt an abstract processing style.

Another limitation of the current study is that we do not have a control condition.

We cannot make strong claims as to whether the observed effects are caused by the

active increasing effect of abstract processing or the active diminishing effect of

concrete processing on generalization. However, selecting a suitable control condition

would be challenging. A no-instruction condition in which participants were not given

a specific processing instruction (i.e., were asked to do nothing) might simply result

in them engaging in spontaneous rumination, making a ‘do nothing’ group very

ambiguous as a reference group (for a similar point, see Watkins & Moulds, 2005).

Also, because of practical constraints, participants were assigned to the respective

conditions by class. As such, randomization occurred at the level of the classroom.

Hence, this could have resulted in the higher repetitive negative thinking scores for

5 Other studies have collected written answers to the respective abstract and concrete

manipulations. Ratings of these answers on an abstract/concrete scale served as a

manipulation check (e.g., Moberly & Watkins, 2006; Watkins, 2004). However, our

manipulation of abstract or concrete processing mode was embedded in the

conditioning procedure (i.e., in the acquisition task) where it was crucial that the

participant remained fixated on the picture of the train while thinking in an abstract or

concrete way. Therefore, participants were not able to write their thoughts down in

our procedure.

Page 24: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 24

the concrete condition.

We did not find that our measure of repetitive negative thinking in interaction

with an abstract thinking style was related to generalization. This finding could seem

challenging because of the substantial relationship between repetitive negative

thinking and depression. Therefore one might expect the same pattern for dysphoric

participants as well as for participants who score highly on repetitive negative

thinking. However, in his review, Watkins (2008) points out that only measures of

mood might interact with the induction of abstract or concrete processing style. Also,

the PTQ (as a measure of repetitive negative thinking) does not assess abstract

thinking per se but measures: (1) the key features of repetitive negative thinking (i.e.,

repetitive, intrusive, difficult to engage from), (2) its perceived unproductiveness, and

(3) the extent to which it captures mental capacity (Ehring et al., 2012). Therefore, a

more direct measure of trait abstract thinking might be necessary to observe

interactions with the inductions. Still, it remains very plausible that adopting an

abstract processing style has no detrimental effects when an individual is in a neutral

or happy mood. Watkins and Moulds (2005) note that in the absence of a significant

negative mood, people find it harder to sustain a prolonged and meaningful abstract or

concrete thinking style. This makes it difficult to induce either mode of processing.

This study further emphasizes the clinical value of targeting abstract thought and

training patients to adopt a more concrete processing style. Recent studies that

implemented concreteness training in therapy have found positive effects on

dysphoria (Watkins, Baeyens, & Read, 2009) and depression (Watkins et al., 2012).

Also, Stöber and Borkovec (2002) reported that successful cognitive therapy for

generalized anxiety disorder produced significant reductions in abstract thinking. Our

study promotes concreteness training as a possible valuable transdiagnostic

Page 25: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 25

therapeutic tool because of the effect the processing styles have on generalization

which is a common underlying factor in several emotional disorders (Hermans et al.,

2013). Future research could thus also focus on the effects that these cognitive

therapies (concreteness training) have on (over)-generalization.

In sum, this study found that for more dysphoric adolescents, manipulating

processing style (abstract vs. concrete) has an impact on negative generalization in a

human conditioning paradigm, such that a concrete thinking mode might decrease

negative generalization. As such, our results are in line with the hypothesis that

abstract thought is an important mechanism affecting the degree of generalization in

response to emotional events (Watkins, 2008). To date, there are only very few

studies that have examined cognitive factors that may influence generalization (see

Hermans et al., 2013). We experimentally tested whether abstract thought could be

such a possible cognitive factor that influences generalization. Our results show that

this generalization might be one way in which abstract thought could have a negative

impact on emotional symptoms or complaints. Hence, this study further emphasizes

the need to study abstract processing as a potentially important (transdiagnostic)

marker, and highlights the need to better understand the effect that this processing

style has on generalization.

Page 26: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 26

REFERENCES

Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S., & Schweizer, S. (2010). Emotion-regulation

strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology

Review, 30, 217–237. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004

Carver, C. S. (1998). Generalization, adverse events, and development of depressive

symptoms. Journal of Personality, 66, 607–19. doi:10.1111/1467-6494.00026

Carver, C. S., & Ganellen, R. J. (1983). Depression and components of self-

punitiveness: High standards, self-criticism, and overgeneralization. Journal of

Abnormal Psychology, 92, 330–7. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.92.3.330

Beurs, E. de, van Dyck, R., Marquenie, L. A., Lange, A., & Blonk, R. W. B. (2001).

De DASS: Een vragenlijst voor het meten van depressie, angst and stress [The

DASS: A questionnaire for the measurement of depression, anxiety and stress].

Gedragstherapie, 34, 35-53.

Ehring, T., Raes, F., Weidacker, K., & Emmelkamp, P. M. G. (2012). Validation of

the perseverative thinking questionnaire – Dutch version (PTQ-NL). European

Journal of Psychological Assessment, 28, 102–108. doi:10.1027/1015-

5759/a000097

Ehring, T., & Watkins, E. R. (2008). Repetitive negative thinking as a transdiagnostic

process. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 1, 192–205.

doi:10.1680/ijct.2008.1.3.192

Ehring, T., Zetsche, U., Weidacker, K., Wahl, K., Schönfeld, S., & Ehlers, A. (2011).

The Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ): Validation of a content-

independent measure of repetitive negative thinking. Journal of Behavior

Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 42, 225–32.

doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.12.003

Page 27: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 27

Fulford, D., Rosen, R. K., Johnson, S. L., & Carver, C. S. (2012). Negative

Generalization and Symptoms of Anxiety Disorders. Journal of Experimental

Psychopathology, 3, 62–68. doi:10.5127/jep.019811

Harvey, A. G.,Watkins, E., Mansell, W., & Shafran, R. (2004). Cognitive behavioural

processes across psychological disorders. Oxford, UK: Oxford University

Press.

Hermans, D., Baeyens, F., & Vervliet, B. (2013). Generalization of acquired

emotional responses. In M. D. Robinson, E. R. Watkins, & E. Harmon-Jones

(Eds.), Handbook of cognition and emotion (pp. 117–134). New York: Guilford

Press.

Klar, Y., Gabai, T., & Baron, Y. (1997). Depression and generalization about the

future: Who overgeneralizes what? Personality and Individual Differences, 22,

575–584. doi:10.1016/S0191- 8869(96)00186-9

Lenaert, B., Claes, S., Raes, F., Boddez, Y., Joos, E., Vervliet, B., Hermans, D.

(2012). Generalization of conditioned responding: Effects of autobiographical

memory specificity. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry,

43, S60–S66. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.12.010

Lissek, S., Biggs, A. L., Rabin, S. J., Cornwell, B. R., Alvarez, R. P., Pine, D. S., &

Grillon, C. (2008). Generalization of conditioned fear-potentiated startle in

humans: Experimental validation and clinical relevance. Behaviour Research

and Therapy, 46, 678–87. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2008.02.005

Lovibond, S. H., & Lovibond, P. F. (1995). Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress

Scales (Second edition). Sydney: Psychology Foundation.

Marigold, D. C., Holmes, J. G., & Ross, M. (2007). More Than Words : Reframing

Compliments From Romantic Partners Fosters Security in Low Self-Esteem

Page 28: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 28

Individuals. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92, 232–248.

doi:10.1037/0022-3514.92.2.232

McLaughlin, K., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2011). Rumination as a transdiagnostic

factor in depression and anxiety. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 49, 186–93.

doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.12.006

Moberly, N. J., & Watkins, E. R. (2006). Processing Mode Influences the

Relationship Between Trait Rumination and Emotional Vulnerability. Behavior

Therapy, 37, 281–291. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2006.02.003

Nolen-hoeksema, S., & Watkins, E. R. (2011). A Heuristic for Developing

Transdiagnostic Models of Psychopathology : Explaining Multifinality and

Divergent Trajectories. Perspectives on Psychological Science.

doi:10.1177/1745691611419672

Raes, F., Hermans, D., Williams, J. M. G., Beyers, W., Brunfaut, E., & Eelen, P.

(2006). Reduced autobiographical memory specificity and rumination in

predicting the course of depression. Journal of abnormal psychology, 115, 699–

704. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.115.4.699

Raes, F., Watkins, E. R., Williams, J. M. G., & Hermans, D. (2008). Non-ruminative

processing reduces overgeneral autobiographical memory retrieval in students.

Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46, 748–56. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2008.03.003

Raes, F. (2012). Repetitive negative thinking predicts depressed mood at 3-year

follow-up in students. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment,

34, 497–501. doi:10.1007/s10862-012-9295-4

Rimes, K. A., &Watkins, E. (2005). The effects of self-focused rumination on global

negative self-judgments in depression. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 43,

1673–1681. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2004.12.002

Page 29: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 29

Spruyt, A., Clarysse, J., Vansteenwegen, D., Baeyens, F., & Hermans, D. (2010).

Affect 4.0: a free software package for implementing psychological and

psychophysiological experiments. Experimental Psychology, 57, 36–45.

doi:10.1027/1618-3169/a000005

Stöber, J., & Borkovec, T. D. (2002). Reduced concreteness of worry in generalized

anxiety disorder: Findings from a therapy study. Cognitive Therapy and

Research, 26, 89–96. doi:10.1023/A:1013845821848

Takano, K., & Tanno, Y. (2010). Concreteness of thinking and self-focus.

Consciousness and Cognition, 19, 419–425. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2009.11.010

Treynor, W., Gonzalez, R., & Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2003). Rumination reconsidered:

A psychometric analysis. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 27, 247–259.

doi:10.1023/A:1023910315561

Trope, Y., & Liberman, N. (2003). Temporal construal. Psychological Review, 110,

403–421. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.110.3.403

van den Heuvel, T. J., Derksen, J. J. L., Eling, P. A. T. M., & van der Staak, C. P. F.

(2012). An investigation of different aspects of overgeneralization in patients

with major depressive disorder and borderline personality disorder. British

Journal of Clinical Psychology, 51, 376–395. doi:10.1111/j.2044-

8260.2012.02034.x

Vervliet, B., Kindt, M., Vansteenwegen, D., & Hermans, D. (2010). Fear

generalization in humans: Impact of prior non-fearful experiences. Behaviour

Research and Therapy, 48, 1078–1084. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2010.07.002

Watkins, E. (2004). Adaptive and maladaptive ruminative self-focus during emotional

processing. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 42, 1037–52.

doi:10.1016/j.brat.2004.01.009

Page 30: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 30

Watkins, E. (2008). Constructive and unconstructive repetitive thought. Psychological

Bulletin, 134, 163–206. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.163

Watkins, E. (2011). Dysregulation in level of goal and action identification across

psychological disorders. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 260–278.

doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2010.05.004

Watkins, E. R., Baeyens, C. B., & Read, R. (2009). Concreteness training reduces

dysphoria: proof-of-principle for repeated cognitive bias modification in

depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 55–64.

doi:10.1037/a0013642

Watkins, E., & Moulds, M. (2005). Distinct modes of ruminative self-focus: impact of

abstract versus concrete rumination on problem solving in depression. Emotion,

5, 319–28. doi:10.1037/1528-3542.5.3.319

Watkins, E., & Moulds, M. L. (2007). Reduced concreteness of rumination in

depression: A pilot study. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 1386–

1395. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2007.04.007

Watkins, E. R., Taylor, R. S., Byng, R., Baeyens, C., Read, R., Pearson, K., &

Watson, L. (2012). Guided self-help concreteness training as an intervention for

major depression in primary care : a Phase II randomized controlled trial.

Psychological Medicine, 1359–1371. doi:10.1017/S0033291711002480

Watkins, E., & Teasdale, J. D. (2001). Rumination and Overgeneral Memory in

Depression : Effects of Self-Focus and Analytic Thinking. Journal of Abnormal

Psychology, 110, 353–357. doi:10.1037//0021-843X.110.2.353

Watkins, E., & Teasdale, J. D. (2004). Adaptive and maladaptive self-focus in

depression. Journal of Affective Disorders, 82, 1–8.

doi:10.1016/j.jad.2003.10.006

Page 31: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 31

Zisook, S., Lesser, I., Stewart, J.W.,Wisniewski, S. R., Balasubramani, G. K., Fava,

M., et al. (2007). Effect of age at onset on the course of major depressive

disorder. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 164,1539–1546.

doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.06101757.

Page 32: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 32

Figure 1. Stimuli used in the study: The CS+, the CS- and the 7 GSs, for half the

participants, the CS+, the CS- and the corresponding GSs were reversed.

CS+ CS- GS1

GS7 GS4 GS4

GS2 GS6

GS3 GS5

GS5 GS3

GS7 GS1

GS6 GS2

CS- CS+

Page 33: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 33

Figure 2. Mean US-expectancy ratings for Trial 1 – 5 in the acquisition phase and

CS+, GS1, GS2, GS3, GS4, GS5, GS6, GS7 and CS- during the generalization test

phase (error bars denote 1 standard error).

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

Trial 1 Trial 2 Trial 3 Trial 4 Trial 5

Exp

ecta

ncy

Rati

ng

Acquisition

CS+/Abs.

CS+/Con.

CS-/Abs.

CS-/Con.

0

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

8

9

10

CS+ GS1 GS2 GS3 GS4 GS5 GS6 GS7 CS-

Exp

ecta

ncy

Rati

ng

Stimulus

Generalization

Page 34: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 34

Figure 3. Mean US-expectancy ratings for CS+, GS1, GS2, GS3, GS4, GS5, GS6,

GS7 and CS- during the generalization test phase for the abstract and concrete

induction in a higher and lower dysphoric group based on a median-split procedure

(error bars denote 1 standard error).

0

2

4

6

8

10

CS+ GS1 GS2 GS3 GS4 GS5 GS6 GS7 CS-

Ex

pe

cta

ncy

Ra

tin

g

Stimulus

Abstract/High Dysphoric

0

2

4

6

8

10

CS+ GS1 GS2 GS3 GS4 GS5 GS6 GS7 CS-

Ex

pe

cta

ncy

Ra

tin

g

Stimulus

Concrete/High Dysphoric

0123456789

10

CS+ GS1 GS2 GS3 GS4 GS5 GS6 GS7 CS-

Ex

pe

cta

ncy

Ra

tin

g

Stimulus

Abstract/Low Dysphoric

0

2

4

6

8

10

CS+ GS1 GS2 GS3 GS4 GS5 GS6 GS7 CS-

Ex

pe

cta

ncy

Ra

tin

g

Stimulus

Concrete/Low Dysphoric

Page 35: (article begins on next page)...considered as a transdiagnostic factor (Harvey, Watkins, Mansell, & Shafran, 2004; McLaughlin & Nolen-Hoeksema, 2011; Ehring & Watkins, 2008; Aldao

ABSTRACT THOUGHT AND NEGATIVE GENERALIZATION 35

Table 1

Correlations, means and standard deviations of study measures 1 2 3 M SD Range

1. DASS (depression) – 6.79 6.31 0-26

2. PTQ .39* – 32.12 9.43 10-57

3. Manipulation-check -.20 .13 – 2.75 1.10 1-5

Abstract M (SD) 7.00

(7.33)

29.59

(9.74)

2.66

(1.12)

Concrete M (SD) 6.56

(5.09)

34.71

(8.46)

2.85

(1.09)

Notes: *p < .001. All p-values are two-tailed.