the health belief model - uniwersytet medyczny w

Post on 16-Oct-2021

1 Views

Category:

Documents

0 Downloads

Preview:

Click to see full reader

TRANSCRIPT

THE HEALTH BELIEF

MODEL

Factors Influencing Patient Compliance

Reading for the lecture:

Health Psychology by Jane Ogden

Chapter 2: HEALTH BELIEFS

Theories 3

Health Belief Model

Irwin Rosenstock. Historical Origins of the Health Belief Model. Health Education Monographs. Vol. 2 No. 4, 1974.

M.H. Becker. The Health Belief Model and Personal Health Behavior. Health Education Monographs. Vol. 2 No. 4, 1974.

Theory of Reasoned Action/Theory of Planned Behaviour

The Transtheoretical Model (stages of change model)

What we will be considering

Healthy Living = 3 Areas

Theories of health belief

Methods of health promotion

Features of adherence to medical regimes

First we focus on theories of health belief

Behavioural factors affecting

longevity (how long we live)

Belloc & Breslow (1972) found a correlational

relationship between mortality rates and

behaviour in a study of 7,000 people

The 7 health behaviours identified were also

found to show a correlation with mortality in a

prospective study carried out at 5.5 and 10

years follow-up

Behaviours related to health

Sleeping 7-8 hours a day

Having breakfast every day

Not smoking

Rarely eating between meals

Being near or at prescribed weight

Having moderate or no use of alcohol

Taking regular exercise

How can we predict health-related

behaviours?

Kristiansen (1985)- correlational study

Relationship between 7 health behaviours and

a person’s beliefs

Found 7 health behaviours correlated with:

A high value on health

A belief in world peace (!)

A low value on leading an exciting life

Health Behaviours

What we do to remain healthy

Our concern is with why individuals adopt

certain health behaviours.

Theories of health belief

Social – how others influence our behaviour

Main approach

Cognitive – assumes that we think through our

actions logically, before rationally choosing a

behaviour to adopt

Do we always think logically?

There are still times where we will choose to

adopt an illogical behaviour

What factors affect our decision making process

when we make choices about our health?

How many of you:

1. Binge drink

2. Smoke

3. Take or do not take prescription or non prescription drugs

4. Exercise less than once a week

5. Eat a lot of junk food

6. Go or do not go for health checks

7. Do not always use a condom

8. Floss your teeth

Why do some people…?

do things that

are bad for

their health

such as

smoking

cigarettes?

12

Why?

Choose one of things you said „yes” to on

slide 10 and think about why you do that

behaviour.

Start each of your explanations with the words:

I believe I am lucky because….. (and so my

risk of becoming ill is smaller)

I believe ….. I will give up ………

(smoking/drinking/whatever/ before I get ill)

What are Health Beliefs?

What I believe about smoking

I believe smoking causes

lung cancer

I believe only some people are susceptible

to cancer

I believe I look cool when

I smoke

What are Health Beliefs?

I believe a belief in God can help me

overcome illness

I believe I have no genetic predisposition

to cancer in my family so I believe I am at less risk

than other people

I believe I have plenty of time later to

worry about my health – Ill health will not happen

to me – yet!

I do not believe doctors always know what

they are talking about I do not believe I can always

trust them

Psychology and Health Beliefs

Psychologists have been interested in finding out what beliefs are behind: 1. Reasons for going or not going to the doctor 2. Decision making behind unhealthy or healthy behaviours (It is ok to

eat cake all day because I deserve it) 3. How seriously we view different illnesses (I am scared of all cancer

because it kills you) 4. Beliefs based on religion or culture 5. A range of different health beliefs 6. https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=aVp4eNIEXv0 And today we might add: 1. Beliefs concerning coronavirus 2. https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=xqK4NfOOF9A

If we know what people

believe we have more of

a chance of

understanding and

predicting their

behaviours and then

possibly changing those

behaviours!

3 lifestyle theories To explain our adoption of health behaviours

Health belief model – predicts uptake of health behaviours based on several factors

Locus of control – where people believe their health is controlled by themselves or others

Self–efficacy – how effective people believe they will be in changing their behaviour directly influences their tendency to change

The Health Belief Model

With the beginning of

widespread media

advertising in the 1950s

psychologists started to

investigate why so

many people ignored

preventative health

campaigns such as free

tuberculosis screening!

Tuberculosis screening

At the beginning of the twentieth century

tuberculosis (TB) was a major cause of death.

Later when screening and treatment became

readily available, people did not go for screening

They did not go even when the screening came

to them in the form of mobile units which came to

where they lived and all they had to do was to

step out of their homes and walk a few metres to

be X-rayed for the disease

The Health Belief Model Hochbaum (1958) first

developed a model about health beliefs when he found that:

1. Those who believed TB was very dangerous

2. Those who believed they were susceptible to developing TB

3. Those who believed x-ray screening was useful in early detection

4.Those who perceived few barriers to attending the x-ray

Were most likely to attend for screening!

Health Belief Model (HBM) 23

Developed in 1950s by social psychologists (Godfrey Hochbaum, Irwin Rosenstock, Stephen Kegels) working in the U.S. Public Health Services in response to the failure of a free tuberculosis health screening programme.

Focus is on individual’s decision to avoid a negative health consequence and considers the following factors identified by Hochbaum (previous slide) as deciding whether a person would attend for screening:

• Severity

• Susceptibility

• Costs/Benefits

• Barriers

Health Belief Model Adopts the Cognitive approach

Why people did or did not go for tuberculosis screening

The four factors: severity, susceptibility, costs/benefits, barriers -

indicated that the main consideration was how serious they perceived TB to be to their health.

Serious threat to health = screened and treated

Beck and Rosenstock developed the HBM from this study on tuberculosis screening.

The Health Belief Model (HBM)

The HBM has been since adapted to explore

various long-term and short-term health

behaviours, including:

Cervical cancer screening

Behaviours associated with high risk for CHD

Managing diabetes

Sexual risk behaviours

Transmission of HIV/AIDS

and many others

Core assumptions of HBM

A person will take a health-related action (i.e.

take regular exercise) if the person:

Feels that a negative health condition can be avoided

(e.g CHD)

Has a positive expectation that by taking the

recommended action, s/he will avoid the negative

health condition (e.g. exercise will prevent CHD)

Believes that s/he can successfully take the

recommended health action (e.g. is able to exercise

regularly without unreasonable effort

Factors affecting adoption of health

behaviours

There are 2 aspects to any perceived threat to health Perceived seriousness (will it actually kill me)

Perceived susceptibility (am I likely to get it)

Cost benefit analysis The cost and benefits of adopting a behaviour are weighed up and the individual decides whether the costs outweigh the benefits;

Barriers How difficult is it for me to engage in these behaviours? This may include distance to health facility or feeling embarrassed about having a medical examination

The four constructs of the HBM

The 4 constructs of the HBM were defined as

the person’s „readiness to take action”.

Perceived severity

Perceived susceptibility

Perceived benefits and

Perceived barriers

Perceived Severity

The consequence is

perceived as being

severe as opposed

to mild.

29

Perceived Susceptibility

How likely a person

thinks there is a risk of a

bad outcome

(e.g., getting ill) if he/she

persists in a behaviour

(i.e. doesn’t change).

30

Perceived Costs and Benefits

The individual’s

estimation of the

benefits of treatment

weighed against

cost, risks and

inconvenience.

Perceived barriers to behaviour

There are not

significant

psychological,

financial or other

barriers to engaging

in the behaviour.

33

Jane is not likely to continue

smoking because… 34

She thinks that she might get lung cancer if she continues to smoke (susceptibility).

She believes that dying from lung cancer is terrible (severity).

Jane does not find smoking very pleasurable (cost/benefits).

Her friends are supportive of her giving up (absence of barrier)

John is likely to continue smoking

because 35

He agrees with the tobacco industry--smoking

doesn’t cause lung cancer (susceptibility).

He believes that dying from lung cancer is not

any worse than any other way of dying

(severity).

John feels that smoking relaxes him

(cost/benefits).

His friends offer him cigarettes (barrier to

quitting)

However…

There are other factors that influence our

health beliefs… These can be...

Cultural

Related to age

Related to gender

Or information from the media…

Health Motivation

The individual’s

general interest in

health matters,

which may correlate

with personality,

social class, ethnic

group, religion etc

etc.

Demographic variables

Can influence the final decisions e.g.

income, age, sex, occupation, education, family size

External and internal cues may remind us

about the behaviour

TV adverts Period of ill health

External Cues

Trigger factors such

as alarming

symptoms, advice

from family or

friends, messages

from the media,

disruption of work or

play.

The Health Belief Model

was then developed by Becker 1970.

Becker added other factors to the HBM:

Factors which affect our beliefs / decisions are:

1) Demographic variables like age, sex, education, occupation, family size, income etc.

Then our decision making is based on cost benefit analysis between:

2) Perceived seriousness of the decision (will it actually kill me?)

3) Perceived susceptibility (am I likely to get it?)

4) Cues from external factors like advertising

5) How motivated we are

Health belief model A person will adopt

a healthy behaviour

If they

perceive a

threat to their

health if they

don’t

If the

benefits

Outweigh

the costs

If they are

reminded by

internal

And/or

external ‘cues’

Demographic

variables

Perceived

Seriousness

Will increase

The threat

Perceived

Susceptibility

Will increase

The threat Beck and Rosenstock (1978)

The Health Belief Model

KEY STUDY Becker (78) compliance with a medical regimen for asthma

Research support for HBM

Becker (1978) found a positive correlation

between health beliefs reported in interviews

and compliance of administration of asthma

medication by mothers to their children. A blood

test was also used to confirm the mothers were

being accurate in their self reporting.

Champion (1990) found that motivation

correlated with whether women self examined

their breasts.

The main ideas:

HBM sees people as rational and suggests that the likelihood that they will engage in healthy behaviour depends on factors such as:

Evaluation of threat (perceived vunerability). The model suggests that people only act if they perceive their behaviour to be dangerous.

Cost-benefit analysis. Will the benefits be higher than perceived barriers (the costs)? Is it worth it? If so – action will take place according to the model.

Health Beliefs About Illness or

Symptoms

Difficult to elicit.

Patients frightened of looking foolish or

ignorant.

Patients may be reluctant to “waste a doctor’s

time” with personal views and attitudes.

But this does NOT mean that patients don’t

have health beliefs!

Why is eliciting health beliefs important?

Half of medical advice is not taken up.

£100 million of prescribed medication is

discarded every year, at least.

Compliance with screening or preventative

programmes is essential if they are to be

effective.

Health Beliefs About Illness or

Symptoms

Avoid direct questions e.g. “What do you think

will happen if you don’t get your blood

pressure checked?”

Indirect questions helpful:

“What is going through your mind?”

“What does your wife / husband think?”

Etc etc.

Checklist

Elicit patient’s health

beliefs.

Reinforce positive

attitudes to health.

i.e. praise for giving

up smoking for a

period in the past,

don’t dwell on the fact

they re-started.

Checklist

Counter myths and

negative attitudes.

Inform patient about

causes and

prognosis.

Checklist

Plan an appropriate

course of action to

suit his/her needs

and lifestyle.

Don’t ask them to

give up cigarettes,

alcohol and sexual

promiscuity all at

once !

We also need to consider…

Why we would adopt a health behaviour

2nd theory

Rotter’s (1966) locus of control theory

From the Latin meaning ‘place’

Rotter ‘Locus of Control’ 1966

This theory looked at the beliefs or perceptions of an

individual about the underlying causes of the main

events in their lives.

Think of something important to you – finding a new

boyfriend, finishing with a partner, taking an exam. What

things made it good and what things made it bad? How

much of your life is affected by fate or chance?

Rotter: Locus of Control

Some people have a powerful belief in fate and

the power of external forces to shape their lives.

Other people have a strong belief that what

happens in their lives is all down to themselves

and their own actions and choices.

Rotter’s Locus of Control Theory

Reductionist = reduces the explanation down

to a person’s locus of control

Deterministic theory which is quite pessimistic

Suggests that where a person thinks the

control of their health lies will influence

whether they adopt a certain health behaviour

KEY STUDY Rotter (1966) investigated internal versus external Locus of Control

Evidence to support Rotter

James et al (1965) found that male smokers who did not relapse had a higher level of internal locus of control than those who did not quit smoking.

Rotter’s studies

Rotter carried out numerous studies investigating how his concept of „locus of control” i.e. the perception of individuals that they have control over the outcomes of things that happen to them, including illness.

He found that people with an „internal locus of control” were more able to show behaviours that would enable them to cope with a threat than those who had an „external locus of control”.

Rotter concluded that „locus of control” affects many of our behaviours, not just health.

The concept continues to be widely used, especially in industry.

Bandura

Agreed that concepts such as Locus of Control

affect behaviour

Thought that determining behaviour went

beyond such a simple explanation

Bandura

He introduced another key concept

A person’s belief that they can successfully do

whatever is required to achieve the outcome

Efficacy expectation

Self Efficacy: Bandura 1986

Self efficacy: the belief in oneself to organize and execute a course of action. Your belief in your ability to succeed or fail in a particular situation.

Self-efficacy

A cognitive model based on thought processes

of the individual

How effective a person THINKS they will be at

successfully changing/adopting a health

behaviour directly influences their tendency to

change

Bandura (1977)

Previous experiences give a person a likely

outcome

Have you ever not done something because you

knew how it would turn out?

Felt learning from consequences was

a cognitive process and would result in an

outcome expectancy

Key concept

Also . . .

Cognitive appraisal of a situation might also

affect expectations of personal efficacy.

Could be …. Social, situational and temporal

circumstances

A person’s self-efficacy can alter depending on

the situation

Limited time

thinking

Self Efficacy

People with a strong self efficacy:

View challenging problems as tasks to be mastered.

People with a weak self efficacy:

Avoid challenging tasks

Bandura suggests that LOC and SE

are different

Locus of control

Concerned with the outcome

Self-efficacy

Linked to the cognitive idea of locus of control

Is a person’s conviction that their own behaviour will influence the outcome

Unchangeable

belief

Internal and external

3 key factors which affect efficacy

expectation

1. Vicarious experience– seeing other people

do something successfully

2. Verbal persuasion – someone telling you

that you can do something

3. Emotional arousal – too much anxiety can

reduce a person’s self-efficacy

Practical example

A typical scenario for each of the 3 factors can

be found in

Weight watchers and the 3 factors

Vicarious experience – seeing someone else losing weight (you can lose it)

Verbal persuasion – meeting is about achievable goals and building self confidence. (you can do it).

Emotional arousal – standing on the scales creates anxiety (I can’t do this)

How do we develop self efficacy?

Life experiences, being or successful or

unsuccessful at previous at tasks?

Seeing other people succeed (modelling and

vicarious reinforcement)?

Verbal support and encouragement from others?

Our psychological responses (mood stress and

emotional state can effect self efficacy) for

example if we are naturally nervous?

KEY STUDY Bandura and Adams (1977) analysis of self efficacy theory of behavioural change

Evidence to support Bandura

Bandura and Adams (1977) carried out a controlled quasi experiment with patients with snake phobias undergoing a course of desensitisation. Findings showed that the higher the levels of self efficacy before the desensitisation predicted the success of the technique.

Mittag (1993) low perceived self efficacy regarding re-employment in the unemployed was associated with heavy drinking when high self efficacy was not.

THEORY OF REASONED

ACTION FISHBEIN (1975)

The theory of reasoned action states that

intention is the best predictor of health

behaviour.

Theory of reasoned action

Theory of Reasoned Action 79

Attitudes

Beliefs (outcome expectancies)

Values

Subjective Norms

Beliefs (about what others think you should do)

Motivation to comply

Intentions

Attitudes 80

One’s positive or negative evaluation of

performing a behaviour

Beliefs: about the consequences of performing

the behaviuor (outcome expectancies)

Values: appraisal (importance) of the

consequences

Subjective Norms 81

One’s perception of the social pressures to

perform or not perform a behaviour.

Beliefs: about whether specific individuals or

groups think one should perform the behavior.

Motivation to comply with these people.

Intentions 82

“Barring unforeseen events, a person will

usually act in accordance with his or her

intentions” (Ajzen & Fishbein, 1980).

Someone likely to smoke 83

ATTITUDE: Bob feels positive about smoking because he expects it will relax him and being relaxed is important to him (beliefs about the consequences and values)

SUBJECTIVE NORM: Other students encourage Bob to smoke (belief) and he wants them to like him (motivation to comply)

INTENTION: Bob intends (expects) to smoke with friends after school (intentions).

THEORY OF PLANNED

BEHAVIOUR

Watch the video

The theory of planned behaviour:

https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=77QiDn8lsUA

Theory of Planned Behaviour 86

Behavioural Control/Locus of Control/Self-

Efficacy

Intention to behaviour link is problematic when

not fully under the individual’s control

Past Behaviour

Always the best predictor of future behaviour

HEALTH PROMOTION

1. KNOWLEDGE APPEALS

Q. Does explaining

healthy eating

actually increase

healthy eating?

Of course it

does!

FOOD AND HEALTH PARTNERSHIP

Using nursery children in the UK, they

designed a “Healthy Eating” programme ;

Used two classes in a multicultural school

within an area of high poverty.

Children’s eating habits before, during and

after intervention were studied.

Interviews and questionnaires with nursery

workers and from parental feedback.

Experimental Intervention

Series of three minute videos, shown at

snack time in nurseries.

Children given the foodstuff featured in

the video as a snack.

Those that ate the food were given a

wall-chart with stickers as a reward.

Child receives a prize when wall chart

complete (Operant conditioning).

RESULTS

Nursery leaders reported day to day

improvements in eating in the experimental

group (but not the control group).

Parents reported children in experimental

group more adventurous in their eating habits

at home.

CONCLUSION:

Q. Does giving people knowledge

actually change their behaviour?

Q. What else needs to be involved?

A fear appeal is......

a persuasive

message which

emphasises the

harmful

physical/social

consequences of

failing to comply with

the recommendations

of the message

2.

The HEALTH BELIEF MODEL and the

THEORY OF PLANNED BEHAVIOUR

both suggest that perceived threat is

necessary for a person to change their

behaviour.

Q. Why do we use fear appeals?

The most obvious way to introduce this

threat is through FEAR APPEAL adverts.

TASK: 1. Consider whether each advert is a mild, moderate or strong fear appeal.

2. Would it alter your behaviour? 3. What emotions does it create in you?

1

2

3

4

5

Q. Do these adverts work?

Janis and Feshbach (1953)

Researched the

use of fear for

promoting oral

hygiene.

METHOD: 4 groups of participants.

3 were given a 15 min lecture on tooth

decay and oral hygiene consisting of:

AIM:

To study the

motivational effects

of fear arousal in

health promotion

PARTICIPANTS

The entire freshman

year of a large

Connecticut high

school, average age

15 years.

GROUP 1

Strong fear appeal

They received pictures and descriptions of diseased mouths, including explanations about the pain of tooth decay and gum

disease and awful consequences like cancer and blindness.

GROUP 2

moderate fear appeal

They received similar pictures and descriptions but they were much less

disturbing and dramatic.

GROUP 3

MINIMAL FEAR APPEAL

A lecture about teeth and cavities but without referring to very serious

consequences and using diagrams and x-rays rather than emotive pictures.

LECTURE FORM STRONG MODERATE MINIMAL CONTROL

INCREASED ANXIETY

APPRAISAL OF INFO

CHANGE IN HEALTH CARE

42 % increase

24 % increase

0% increase

Highest appraisal

Lowest appraisal

27 % increase

8% increase

36% increase

0 % increase

CONCLUSION?

Q. Was fear generated by the campaign?

The strong fear appeal created the most worry in the students and was rated as more interesting

Q. Did fear change the behaviour?

The overall effectiveness of a health promotion campaign is likely to be REDUCED by the use of strong fear appeal. It produced the least change in behaviour.

Research: Quist Paulsen (2003)

Field experiment investigating participants with heart problems.

Aim: to see whether intervention including fear arousal (information, personal advice stressing the risks) would promote smoking cessation and prevent relapse. Normal 30-45 stop smoking by themselves.

Result: 57% of the intervention group (compared to 37% of the control gruop) had stopped smoking.

Davis-Kirsch & Pullen (2003)

500,000 children in the U.S. are injured in bicycle

crashes annually,

252 die - 97% of whom were not wearing a helmet.

School-based evaluation study to examine students'

knowledge retention and behavioural changes.

Purpose: to identify associations between student-

reported knowledge of safety-related behaviours, reports

of current safety-related practices and students'

participation in the Safety Central program while in the

4th grade.

Davis-Kirsch & Pullen (2003)

Aim:

To increase fear

arousal of not

wearing bike

helmets

To increase the self-

efficacy of children

attempts to wear

cycle helmets.

Method/procedure

Five schools took part in “Safety Central

initiative” (four schools were given program,

one was control group).

Questionnaires given to 284 students & 11

teachers on their cycling behaviour.

Students were aged 10-12 years old.

Procedure cont. Observers sent to the four schools to watch;

a. Whether helmets were worn.

b. If helmets worn properly

c. Time of day, weather conditions

d. Gender, age, size of group, ethnicity

RESULTS of the Questionnaire?

90% owned a helmet.

82% believed helmets were important for safety.

74% said they wore a helmet the last time they

cycled.

Those on the program reported more willingness

to wear helmets.

55% were able to identify correct fitting points.

Davis-Kirsch & Pullen (2003)

Findings showed a statistically significant association

between participation in the Safety Central program and

retention of knowledge and enactment of safety

messages after a 1- and 2-year period.

Motivators included accessibility to helmets and media

messages about helmets being „cool” (used in „extreme”

sports).

Barriers to helmet use found in older males who tended

to ride alone and used their bicycles 5 days a week.

Most at risk for injury.

Stanford three-city project

AIM:

To promote health behaviours to reduce heart disease.

PROCEDURE

SAMPLE:

Residents from three cities in the USA

PROGRAMME:

CITY 1: Promotion of behaviours to reduce heart disease including a mass media

campaign, school based health education and screening programmes in the work place to

provide early warning

CITY 2: All of the above + one to one counselling

for individuals identified as being at risk

CITY 3: No intervention (control)

Residents interviewed before, during and

after two year project.

Researchers assessed health knowledge and

risk of heart disease.

RESULTS: Farquhar et al, 1985

Initial evaluation showed factors linked with

heart disease INCREASED in control city and

DECREASED in other two.

Further evaluation showed residents in City 1

showed increases in health knowledge BUT

little change.

Residents in City 2 showed dramatic increase

in actual health behaviour.

Results: Three Community Study

Statistically significant reduction was achieved in the risk score for cardiovascular disease

declines in blood pressure, smoking and cholesterol levels.

Risk score decreased approximately 25% for the media-only community and

30% for the community in which media were supplemented by face-to face instruction.

In control community, there was a minimal decrease in risk score (less than 5% for both the total and high-risk participants)

Results: Five City Project

The success of the intial 3 community study led researchers to expand their work to five cities in U.S.

Smoking rates decreased by 14% in proportion of smokers compared to control communities.

The intervention cities also experienced a 15% decrease in risk score based on improvements in blood pressure, physical activity, and cholesterol.

Numerous sub-campaigns including a curriculum for 4th, 7th and 10th grade students were found effective in increasing awareness of nutrition, physical activity and smoking cessation as essential to heart health.

However no effect on exercise

Study strengths

Random sampling from an open population and ability

to assess effectiveness of a community intervention.

While the curriculum, counselling and media

campaigns were successful in the Stanford Community

Trials, the authors were sceptical about the ability to

implement such efforts on a national scale.

Concerns included lack of trained staff, overstretch of

county health departments and the decentralization of

public schools.

Problems with the study

Relatively high cost

Small demonstrable change in health

behaviour at the community level.

Difficulty in comparing the effect of such

campaigns among small numbers of relatively

healthy communities that are already

undergoing large changes in health behaviour.

ADHERENCE TO

MEDICAL ADVICE

What is adherence?

“the extent to which a person’s behaviour (in

terms of taking medications, following diets, or

executing life style changes) coincides with

medical or health advice”

(Haynes, 1979, pp 2-3)

“a person’s behaviour in relation to a prescribed

medical regimen” (La Greca & Bearman, 2003)

Evolution of terminology

COMPLIANCE

SELF-MANAGEMENT ADHERENCE

CONCORDANCE

Theories of Adherence

The Adherence/Compliance Approach

Applies to patients with an existing problem

Assumptions:

Pt. needs to be treated

Pt. wants to initiate/maintain treatment and has sought medical

care for that purpose

Pt. should be motivated to comply for symptom relief

Limitations: asymptomatic conditions, overlooks

barriers

Theories of Adherence

Health Belief Model

Can be applied to preventative treatments

Views patients as autonomous “decision makers”

Considers the patient’s perceptions of:

Threat of illness

Effectiveness of treatment

Barriers to treatment

Theories of Adherence

Transtheoretical Model (Stages of Change)

Five stages in the adoption of health-related

behaviors:

Precontemplation

Contemplation

Preparation

Action

Maintenance

Match intervention to stage

Stages of Behaviour Change

Precontemplation

Contemplation

Preparation

Action

Maintenance

Relapse

Precontemplation

Contemplation

Preparation Action

Maintenance

Prochaska, Norcros & DiClemente

Relapse

Evaluating Stages of Change

Precontemplation (Denial) “What problem? I’m not thinking about it.”

Contemplation (Ambivalence) “I wonder if I might have a problem? I’m thinking about it but not

ready to decide anything yet.”

Preparation / Determination (Admission) “I have a problem.”

Action (Taking steps / Making changes) “I have a problem and I’m ready to do something about it.”

Maintenance (Continuing what works) “I’m stabilized and doing well. How can I support my ongoing

recovery?”

Relapse / Recycle (Trying again) “I’m stabilized but have relapsed. How can I get back into active

recovery?”

Precontemplation: “Huh? What problem?”

A Precontemplation-

stage person is

described as, “It’s not

that they can’t see the

solution. It’s that they

can’t see the problem.”

This stage of change has

been given the label of

“Denial” in the past.

Precontemplation: “Huh? What problem?”

Treatment for someone in

the Precontemplation

stage would seek to

engage them in the

process of objectively

evaluating whether

they have a problem,

and supporting movement

along to the Contemplative

stage of change.

Precontemplation

Am I Precontemplative?

“What Problem?”

“Who, me?”

“I see no reason to change.”

“I wish people would just leave

me alone!”

Evaluating Stages of Change

Precontemplation (Denial) “What problem? I’m not thinking about it.”

Contemplation (Ambivalence) “I wonder if I might have a problem? I’m thinking about it but not

ready to decide anything yet.”

Preparation / Determination (Admission) “I have a problem.”

Action (Taking steps / Making changes) “I have a problem and I’m ready to do something about it.”

Maintenance (Continuing what works) “I’m stabilized and doing well. How can I support my ongoing

recovery?”

Relapse / Recycle (Trying again) “I’m stabilized but have relapsed. How can I get back into active

recovery?”

Contemplation: “Problem? Yeah . . . Action? Nah.”

Contemplation-stage people may know their destination, and even how to get there, but they are “not ready yet.”

Someone in this stage of change may be ambivalent about doing anything about a problem that they can clearly identify having.

Contemplation: “Problem? Yeah . . . Action? Nah.”

Treatment for someone in

the Contemplation stage

would seek to engage them

in the process of gaining

motivation to address their

problem, and supporting

movement along to the

Preparation stage of

change.

Conte

mpla

tion

Am I Contemplative?

“I might have a

problem.”

“I’m not ready to

make any decisions

yet.”

“Problem? Maybe.”

Considering change,

but ambivalent.

Evaluating Stages of Change

Precontemplation (Denial) “What problem? I’m not thinking about it.”

Contemplation (Ambivalence) “I wonder if I might have a problem? I’m thinking about it but not

ready to decide anything yet.”

Preparation / Determination (Admission) “I have a problem.”

Action (Taking steps / Making changes) “I have a problem and I’m ready to do something about it.”

Maintenance (Continuing what works) “I’m stabilized and doing well. How can I support my ongoing

recovery?”

Relapse / Recycle (Trying again) “I’m stabilized but have relapsed. How can I get back into active

recovery?”

Preparation: “Almost ready to take action . . .”

Preparation-stage individuals are planning to take future action, but are still making the final adjustments before committing.

Someone in this stage of change may be working through the final obstacles that are getting in the way of taking action.

Preparation: “Almost ready to take action . . .”

Treatment for someone in the Preparation stage would seek to engage them in the process of taking action to address their problem, i.e., supporting movement along to the Action stage of change.

Preparation

Am I in Preparation?

“I have a problem, but

don’t want to commit

to doing anything

about it yet”

“I’m developing plans

to change”

“Problem? Yes…

Change? Not yet…”

Evaluating Stages of Change

Precontemplation (Denial) “What problem? I’m not thinking about it.”

Contemplation (Ambivalence) “I wonder if I might have a problem? I’m thinking about it but not

ready to decide anything yet.”

Preparation / Determination (Admission) “I have a problem.”

Action (Taking steps / Making changes) “I have a problem and I’m ready to do something about it.”

Maintenance (Continuing what works) “I’m stabilized and doing well. How can I support my ongoing

recovery?”

Relapse / Recycle (Trying again) “I’m stabilized but have relapsed. How can I get back into active

recovery?”

Action: “Let’s get going”

The Action Stage is described as the one in which individuals most overtly modify their behaviour and surroundings to accomplish their goal.

Someone in this stage of change is taking visible steps and making visible changes in order to work on their recovery.

Action: “Let’s get going”

Treatment for someone

in the Action stage

would seek to assist

them in taking all

indicated steps to be

successful in their

recovery, and to support

movement along to the

Maintenance stage of

change.

Action

Am I at the Action stage?

“I have a problem and

I’m ready to do

something about it.”

“I’m making changes.”

“Problem? Yes…

Change? YES!”

Evaluating Stages of Change

Precontemplation (Denial) “What problem? I’m not thinking about it.”

Contemplation (Ambivalence) “I wonder if I might have a problem? I’m thinking about it but not

ready to decide anything yet.”

Preparation / Determination (Admission) “I have a problem.”

Action (Taking steps / Making changes) “I have a problem and I’m ready to do something about it.”

Maintenance (Continuing what works) “I’m stabilized and doing well. How can I support my ongoing

recovery?”

Relapse / Recycle (Trying again) “I’m stabilized but have relapsed. How can I get back into active

recovery?”

Maintenance: “I’m in a good place, let’s keep it up!”

In the Maintenance Stage, the focus is on consolidating gains and preventing relapse.

Someone is this stage has an

effective set of tools and “program

of recovery” that they commit to

continuing to practice.

Treatment for someone in the

Maintenance stage would seek to

strengthen and increase their “tool

kit” and to support ongoing recovery

success.

Maintenance

Am I at Maintenance?

“I’m stabilized and

doing well. How can I

support my ongoing

recovery?”

“I’ve made the

changes I want; now I

want to maintain my

gains.”

Evaluating Stages of Change

Precontemplation (Denial) “What problem? I’m not thinking about it.”

Contemplation (Ambivalence) “I wonder if I might have a problem? I’m thinking about it but not

ready to decide anything yet.”

Preparation / Determination (Admission) “I have a problem.”

Action (Taking steps / Making changes) “I have a problem and I’m ready to do something about it.”

Maintenance (Continuing what works) “I’m stabilized and doing well. How can I support my ongoing

recovery?”

Relapse / Recycle (Trying again) “I’m stabilized but have relapsed. How can I get back into active

recovery?”

Relapse / Recycle: “How can I get back on track?”

Relapse is often part of the chronic

disease process, and recovering

individuals need to be prepared to

deal with it, including damage-

control strategies.

“Progress not perfection,”

supports gentleness and freedom

from shame, and “Progress not

permission” emphasizes the

importance of personal responsibility to stay active in one’s own recovery, even when slips or relapses occur.

The Recycling aspect of relapse supports the view that recovery-strengthening lessons can be learned from relapse episodes – “The only bad relapse is a WASTED relapse”

Relapse / Recycle

Relapse / Recycle

“I’m stabilized but

have relapsed. How

can I get back into

active recovery?”

“How can I learn from

my relapse to

strengthen my

recovery plan?”

top related