1 language and age: studying linguistic change in real and apparent time lin120: sociolinguistics...

Post on 20-Dec-2015

227 Views

Category:

Documents

5 Downloads

Preview:

Click to see full reader

TRANSCRIPT

1

Language and age:

Studying linguistic change in real and apparent time

LIN120: Sociolinguistics

Instructor: Marjorie Pak

February 27, 2008

2

We know that language changes over time…

Great Vowel Shift in Middle English:/mu:s/ /maws/

Do-support:I know not I don’t know

These changes have all been observed through diachronic studies of historical texts.

3

But can we also see language change happening in real time, through synchronic methods of study?

How do we know when we’re looking at a change in progress?

4

In the ideal world, linguists would have access to recorded speech…

spanning several generations

representing speakers of various ages within each sample

maintaining consistency over other factors (style, sex, ethnicity, formality…)

5

We’re not living in an ideal world…

Linguistics is a young science

Recording and archiving technology is also young

6

But we can make a lot of progress by looking at the role of speakers’ age in synchronic studies of linguistic variation.

We start with cases where…

Young people sound different from old people.

The older you are, the more/less likely you are to do X.

7

Example: Bailey et al. 1991 looked at 14 features of Texas speech, including:

i I before /l/ (field, real)

e ε before /l/ (sale, jail)

y Ø after alveolar (Tuesday, student)

8

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

70

1894-1929 1930-1944 1945-1959 1960-1971

Respondents' date of birth

% o

f re

sp

on

de

nts

us

ing

inn

ov

ati

ve

fo

rm

field

sale

Tuesday

9

All three slopes are monotonic (they don’t change direction)

The older the speakers, the less likely they were to say /fIld/, /sεl/, /tuzdi/ instead of /fild/, /sel/, /tyuzdi/

When we see a pattern like this, we can at least entertain the hypothesis that we’re looking at a linguistic change in progress.

10

Apparent-time method (Labov 1963, 1966; following Gauchat 1905)

We can view synchronic age patterns as a window on what has happened in a community over the last few generations

Basic assumption: Adults speak the language they learned as children.

11

Critical period: humans have a period when they’re ‘primed’ to acquire language

once we’re past this age (early teens?) it’s impossible to acquire native-like competence in a new language

The way you talk won’t change once you hit a certain age – even if the language changes

So we can listen to the speech of a 55-year-old and get a sense of what the community norms were when s/he was a child (50 years ago)

12

Sound change advances from generation togeneration by incrementation (Labov 2007)

Child hears speakers of various ages and notices that the younger the speaker, the more advanced the change

Child positions him/herself at the end of the trajectory and advances the change a little more

13

Sound change advances from generation togeneration by incrementation (Labov 2007)

Short-a tensing:/æ/ is raised, fronted and diphthongized before nasals (other contexts depend on dialect)

mæǝn mɛǝn meǝn mIǝn miǝn

(grandma) (mom) (aunt) (sister) (me)

14

The apparent-time method has allowed linguists to make considerable advances in understanding language change in progress

But this method isn’t perfect! Why not?

There’s always another possible interpretation of a monotonic age slope: age grading

15

English t/d deletion (Guy 1980, Guy/Boyd 1990)

Alveolar stop Ø after a consonant at the end of a syllable

best friend /bεs frεn/

Conditioning factors:

Preceding/following segment

Style, speech rate

Morphological structure

16

t/d deletion is less likely to apply if the final t/d is a past-tense suffix

probability of deletion

Un-suffixed word (mist, past) .65

‘Semiweak’ past-tense word (lost) .55

Regular past-tense word (missed, passed) .31

17

‘Semiweak’ verbs have a vowel change as well as a suffix in the past tense…

keep, tell, sell, feel…

With semiweak verbs in particular, age plays an important role in t/d deletion

The older the speakers, the less likely they were to delete t/d in semiweak verbs

Probability of t/d deletion in semiweak verbs

Age >.75 .60-.75 <.60

0-18 7 1 0

19-44 0 9 3

45+ 0 4 10

18

Observed speech in a ~4-year-old girl:

and then I started /startId/ crying… and then I screamed /skrimd/… and then daddy yelled /yεld/ at me.. but I kept /kεp/ on crying…

19

So is t/d deletion in the semiweak verb class a change in progress?

Guy & Boyd: No! t/d deletion is a stable variable, but there’s an age grading effect:

Age grading: Each generation of speakers modifies its linguistic behavior at a particular stage in life – sometimes well into adulthood. But the language itself does not change across generations.

20

t/d deletion is a case of late acquisition:

Kids don’t realize that there’s a past-tense -ed on semiweak verbs at all – they think the past tense of keep is just /kεp/

Much later in life – perhaps well past the critical period – speakers learn that semiweak verbs do have a -ed suffix.

Speakers then apply t/d deletion at the same (low) rates as with the regular past-tense -ed.

21

Support: t/d deletion doesn’t have the characteristic features of change in progress

Changes in progress are usually…

led by working/middle classes and women

differentiated ethnically and geographically

socially evaluated, with ‘prestige’ forms increasing in more formal speech

None of these are true of semiweak-verb t/d deletion in particular

22

Another way to tease apart generational change-in-progress and age-grading interpretations: Real-time studies!

Go back and re-study a community after some time has passed.

Trend study (resample a community with comparable speakers)

Panel study (locate and re-interview the same speakers)

23

Real-time trend study 1: Fowler’s (1986) restudy of Labov’s (1963) ‘department store’ study

Fowler replicated the NYC department store study in exact detail (23 years later)

All results were reproduced, with rates of /r/-pronunciation 10-20% higher

24

0

5

10

15

20

25

30

35

40

45

50

Saks Macy's Klein/ May's

% a

ll /

r/

Labov 1963

Fowler 1986

25

Real-time trend study #2:Bailey et al. (1990) compared their results with an older survey (Linguistic Atlas of the Gulf States (LAGS), data collected 15-20 years earlier)

For all the innovative features studied in Bailey et al., the percentage of respondents who used the feature was higher than the percentage of LAGS respondents who did

26

0

10

20

30

40

50

60

field sale school

% o

f re

sp

on

de

nts

us

ing

inn

ov

ati

ve

fo

rm

LAGS (1969-1974) Bailey et al (1989)

27

Panel studies: track down the same speakers and re-study them after an interval of time

Cukor-Avila (2000) interviewed 4 speakers of African-American Vernacular English in a small Texas town in 1988, 1992, and 1998

Older speakers (b. 1913, 1961) change very little in the 10-year interval

Younger speakers (b. 1979, 1982) show increase in AAVE features – even in late adolescence – as they spend more time in a nearby city

28

Other panel studies:

Development of uvular /r/ in Montreal (Blondeau et al. 2003)

Yiddish folksinger Sara Gorby (Prince 1987)

Trudgill’s study of /r/ pronunciation in British pop singers (Beatles, etc.)

29

Panel study results have largely vindicated the apparent-time method

People generally remain stable – and when they do change, it’s in the direction of the community change

30

The problem of how to interpret age trends isn’t limited to linguistics…

31

Summary: If a synchronic observation of a community shows a steady increase/ decrease in the frequency of a variable with age, there are at least two interpretations:

Generational change in progress (apparent-time interpretation)

Age grading

Real-time studies (trend or panel) can help disentangle these two interpretations, as can understanding fundamental principles of language change.

32

The apparent-time construct has proven to be an excellent surrogate for real-time evidence.

As with all linguistic research…the more data, the better.

But considerable progress can be made with limited data, as long as the underlying issues are understood.

33

References

Bailey, Guy. 2002. Real and apparent time. In Chambers et al. (eds.), Handbook of language variation and change (pp. 312-332). Blackwell.

Guy, Gregory R. and Boyd, Sally. 1990. The development of a morphological class. Language Variation and Change 2, 1-18.

Labov, William. 2007. Transmission and diffusion. Language 83, 344-387.

Meyerhoff, Miriam. 2006. Introducing sociolinguistics [ch.7]. Routledge.

Sankoff, Gillian. (to appear). Cross-sectional and longitudinal studies in sociolinguistics. In Ammon U, Dittmar N, Mattheier K & Trudgill P (eds.) Handbook of sociolinguistics, vol. II. Berlin: De Gruyter.

top related