aging implicit sequence learning and white matter integrity

100
AGING, IMPLICIT SEQUENCE LEARNING, AND WHITE MATTER INTEGRITY A Dissertation submitted to the Faculty of the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences of Georgetown University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Psychology By Ilana Jacqueline Bennett, M.A. Washington, DC August 31, 2009

Upload: others

Post on 07-Apr-2022

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

AGING, IMPLICIT SEQUENCE LEARNING, AND WHITE MATTER INTEGRITY

A Dissertation submitted to the Faculty of the

Graduate School of Arts and Sciences of Georgetown University

in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of

Doctor of Philosophy in Psychology

By

Ilana Jacqueline Bennett, M.A.

Washington, DC August 31, 2009

ii

AGING, IMPLICIT SEQUENCE LEARNING, AND WHITE MATTER INTEGRITY

Ilana Jacqueline Bennett, M.A.

Thesis Advisor: Darlene V. Howard, Ph.D.

ABSTRACT

Cognitive neuroscience of aging research investigates neural mechanisms

associated with cognitive stability and decline in older versus younger adults. This

field has been dominated by neuroimaging techniques that emphasize brain function

over structure, gray over white matter, and isolated brain regions over neural networks.

Furthermore, the scope of the field has been limited by a focus on explicit cognitive

processes (e.g., working memory, executive functions). To broaden our current

understanding, this dissertation examined relationships among healthy aging, implicit

sequence learning, and white matter integrity. In the first study, diffusion tensor

imaging (DTI), which measures diffusion of molecular water, was used to characterize

age-related differences in multiple measures of white matter integrity. Results revealed

age-related declines in fractional anisotropy (FA), a measure of diffusion coherence,

with the magnitude of these differences being largest in frontal white matter.

Additional measures of diffusion parallel (axial diffusivity, AD) and perpendicular

(radial diffusivity, RD) to the primary diffusion direction revealed region-specific

patterns of age group differences that may reflect differential aging of microstructural

(e.g., degree of myelination) and macrostructural (e.g., coherence of fiber orientation)

properties of white matter. In the second study, white matter integrity correlates of

iii

implicit sequence learning were examined in younger and healthy older adults. Implicit

sequence learning was assessed with the alternating serial reaction time task (ASRT).

Significant learning was seen in both age groups (i.e., faster and more accurate

responses to frequent, pattern-related events compared to intervening, random events),

with an age-related decline in the late learning stage. In line with functional imaging

studies that identified subcortical (e.g., caudate, hippocampus) and cortical (e.g.,

dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, DLPFC) correlates of implicit sequence learning,

integrity of caudate-DLPFC and hippocampus-DLPFC tracts was related to sequence

learning in the ASRT, and these relationships did not differ with age group.

Additionally, age-related differences in caudate-DLPFC tract integrity mediated age-

related differences in sequence learning. Taken together, results of these studies

support the theory of white matter disconnection, which proposes that age-related

declines in white matter integrity may explain age-related cognitive declines, as

communication between distributed cortical regions involved in the task is disrupted.

iv

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This dissertation would not have been possible without the efforts of many

individuals. First and foremost, I would like to thank the healthy older adults who

participate in our research. These successful agers inspire my work and motivate me to

uncover the neurobiological underpinnings of their cognitive advantage.

Of my many mentors, I am deeply indebted to my academic parents, Drs.

Darlene and Jim Howard. Together, they have provided guidance and support for both

my professional and personal development during the past five years. I am also

thankful to the imaging experts on my committee, Drs. Dave Madden and Chandan

Vaidya, who enabled me to embark on this diffusion tensor imaging project and were

central in helping me obtain an NIH grant. I would like to give additional thanks to Dr.

Madden for his inherent mentoring demeanor that makes every email communication

another invaluable lesson for me. Finally, I would like to acknowledge my early

mentors, Drs. Arnie Starr, Elizabeth Parker, and especially Edward Golob who taught

me to write like a scientist, which made this dissertation a slightly less arduous task.

My friends and family, with their endless love and support, are responsible for

making this journey so enjoyable. Fellow graduate students Alexis Lauricella, Amy

Lowenstein, Jennifer Romano, Jessica Simon, Kelly Barnes, Melanie Stollstorff, and

Samantha Harvell were always on hand for a laugh or a cry. I am grateful to call this

incredible group of budding researchers my friends. I would like to give special thanks

to my lab mate, Jessica Simon, whose inquisitive nature and dedication to her work

v

constantly inspire me to be a better researcher; and to my class mate, Amy Lowenstein,

who persevered through every milestone by my side and always had a mature

perspective that I strived to see. Words cannot express how thankful I am to my

parents, Ivan Bennett and Kathy and Dennis Stajic, for I would not be who I am today

without their influence. Finally, I am grateful for the unwavering support of my sisters,

Amy Bennett and Sasha Stajic, and for my long-time friend, Jamie Brown, who

believes I am a better scientist than I could even aspire to be.

vi

TABLE OF CONTENTS

CHAPTER I: INTRODUCTION............................................................................................. 1

CHAPTER II: AGE-RELATED DIFFERENCES IN MULTIPLE MEASURES OF WHITE MATTER:

A DIFFUSION TENSOR IMAGING STUDY IN HEALTHY AGING........................................... 7

Materials and Methods.................................................................................................. 11

Results…....................................................................................................................... 16

Discussion..................................................................................................................... 25

CHAPTER III: WHITE MATTER INTEGRITY CORRELATES OF IMPLICIT SEQUENCE

LEARNING IN HEALTHY AGING...................................................................................... 38

Method…...................................................................................................................... 42

Results and Discussion................................................................................................. 51

General Discussion....................................................................................................... 61

CHAPTER IV: GENERAL DISCUSSION............................................................................. 66

Future Directions.......................................................................................................... 66

Limitations.................................................................................................................... 69

APPENDIX...................................................................................................................... 72

REFERENCES.................................................................................................................. 75

vii

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure 1. Statistical maps showing white matter clusters with significant age group

differences in fractional anisotropy (FA)...................................................................... 18

Figure 2. Scatterplot showing age group differences in FA as a function of the cluster’s

anterior-posterior location............................................................................................. 25

Figure 3. Population maps showing the three tracts of interest separately for younger

and older adults............................................................................................................. 50

Figure 4. Line graphs showing overall learning performance for mean of median

reaction time and proportion correct in both groups, and bar graphs showing

differences scores across the three stages of learning................................................... 52

Figure 5. Scatterplots showing significant relationships between white matter tract

integrity (FA) and sequence learning............................................................................ 56

viii

LIST OF TABLES

Table 1. Demographic and neuropsychological test results.......................................... 13

Table 2. Descriptions of white matter clusters.............................................................. 19

Table 3. Age group differences in white matter integrity............................................. 20

Table 4. Statistics for age group differences in diffusivity measures........................... 21

Table 5. Descriptions of tractography seed and target regions..................................... 49

1

CHAPTER I: INTRODUCTION

The aging population will increase to ~71 million people over the age of 65

years by 2030, accounting for almost 20% of the total population in the United States

(Goulding, Rogers, & Smith, 2003). Because of this abundance of older adults, there

will be an increased need to promote successful aging and minimize cognitive decline.

Researchers in the cognitive neuroscience of aging are contributing to this goal by

studying how the brain changes with aging and whether these neural changes affect

cognitive functioning. A number of recent books and special issue journal articles have

increased awareness of both cognitive neuroscience of aging (Cabeza, Nyberg, & Park,

2005; Grady, 2008) and the broader field of lifespan cognitive neuroscience (Bialystok

& Craik, 2006; Craik, 2006; Crone & Huizinga, 2006; Muller & Mayes, 2001), which

marks an important step toward advancing our understanding of the neural substrates

underlying cognition and their relation to healthy and pathological development and

aging.

Despite these advances, the scope of cognitive neuroscience of aging research

has been limited in at least two important ways. First, our current understanding has

been restricted by imaging techniques that focus on brain function over brain structure,

gray matter more than white matter, and localized brain regions versus brain systems.

That is, research in this field has been dominated by studies using functional imaging

techniques, including positron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic

resonance imaging (fMRI), that assess age-related differences in regional gray matter

2

activity during performance of various tasks (Cabeza, 2001; Langley & Madden,

2000). Studies using other techniques, such as structural magnetic resonance imaging

(sMRI), also examine age group differences in isolated brain regions, relating the

volume of these gray and white matter structures to cognitive performance (e.g., Raz,

Gunning-Dixon, Head, Dupuis, & Acker, 1998). Second, cognitive neuroscience of

aging research has primarily examined a small set of cognitive processes including

working memory, episodic memory, visual perception, and attention (Cabeza, 2001;

Grady, 2008). As a result of this limited focus, certain cognitive functions, such as

implicit forms of learning and memory, have received little attention in reviews of the

cognitive neuroscience of aging (e.g., Grady, 2008; Hedden & Gabrieli, 2004).

The first of these limitations can be avoided by employing a relatively new

structural imaging technique, diffusion tensor imaging (DTI)(Basser, Mattiello, &

LeBihan, 1994; Pierpaoli & Basser, 1996). DTI measures the diffusion, or movement,

of water, which can be used as a proxy for the integrity of the underlying white matter

microstructure. White matter integrity in discrete brain regions can be assessed via

voxel-based comparisons such as tract-based spatial statistics (TBSS)(Smith et al.,

2006; Smith et al., 2007). However, integrity from white matter tracts that connect

distributed brain regions can be examined using fiber tracking, or tractography

methods (Beaulieu, 2002; Jones, 2008); thus, taking neural networks into account. In

healthy aging, the integrity of these tracts is affected by a variety of age-related white

3

matter changes, including atrophy, neuron loss, and demyelination (Gunning-Dixon,

Brickman, Cheng, & Alexopoulos, 2009; Raz, 2005).

The second limitation is readily overcome by employing implicit learning

paradigms. In general, implicit learning describes sensitivity to regularities in the

environment (e.g., repeated sequences of events or spatial patterns) that occurs without

intent to learn or awareness of what has been learned (Seger, 1994). Implicit

acquisition of subtle sequential regularities plays a role in learning a variety of every

day activities (e.g., playing sports, typing) and high-level cognitive skills (e.g.,

language, social intuition). For example, language acquisition initially involves

implicitly learning to extract phonemes from speech streams through repeated

exposure to these word chunks (Kuhl, 2004). The ability to extract predictable

relationships from the environment is also important for adapting to change (e.g., new

people, places, and technologies)(Reber, 1993). In healthy aging, this allows older

adults to respond effectively to their environment in the face of declines in explicit

forms of learning (Craik & Jennings, 1992).

Capitalizing on the limitations of previous cognitive neuroscience of aging

research, the primary aim of this dissertation was to examine relationships among

healthy aging, implicit sequence learning, and white matter integrity. More

specifically, two studies will use DTI to first characterize age-related differences in

multiple measures of white matter integrity (Chapter II), and then assess white matter

integrity correlates of implicit sequence learning in younger and healthy older adults in

4

light of age-related integrity differences (Chapter III). The studies presented here were

conceptualized within the framework of the theory of white matter disconnection (e.g.,

Andrews-Hanna et al., 2007; Charlton et al., 2008; O'Sullivan et al., 2001). According

to this theory, age-related declines in cognitive functioning are due to disrupted

communication between distributed cortical regions involved in a task as a result of

age-related damage to the underlying white matter.

Age group differences in white matter integrity will be examined in discrete

clusters using TBSS analyses (Chapter II) and in white matter tracts using tractography

(Chapter III). In line with previous findings, it is expected that older adults will have

reduced fractional anisotropy (FA), a measure of the uniformity of diffusion

orientation, compared to younger adults, with the magnitude of these age group

differences being largest in anterior white matter (Gunning-Dixon et al., 2009;

Sullivan, Adalsteinsson, & Pfefferbaum, 2006). Additional measures of axial (AD) and

radial (RD) diffusivity, which measure diffusion parallel and perpendicular to the

primary diffusion direction respectively, will also be examined. Research suggests that

these measures are selectively sensitive to changes in specific neural substrates, with

AD reflecting axonal damage or loss and RD reflecting differences in the degree of

myelination (Budde et al., 2007; Nair et al., 2005; Song et al., 2003; Song et al., 2002;

Song et al., 2005). Based on this evidence, previous DTI aging studies have found that

decreased FA in older adults is often accompanied by an age-related increase in RD,

suggesting that compromised white matter integrity in healthy aging may be attributed

5

to loss of myelin in older versus younger adults (Bhagat & Beaulieu, 2004; Madden et

al., 2009; Zhang et al., 2008). However, more recent work suggests that these

interpretations may be misleading, especially in regions with multiple white matter

tracts, and thus should be accepted with caution (Wheeler-Kingshott & Cercignani,

2009).

Implicit sequence learning will be assessed in the second study with the

alternating serial reaction time (ASRT)(Howard & Howard, 1997), a modified version

of the original serial reaction time task (SRT)(Nissen & Bullemer, 1987), in which

participants view a series of stimuli and respond to the location of each event.

Unbeknownst to them, stimuli that follow a repeating sequence of locations alternate

with stimuli that occur in randomly determined locations. Implicit learning is seen as

faster reaction times and/or higher accuracy to pattern-related events compared to

random events, without explicit knowledge of the repeating regularity. Age group

differences in learning are expected based on previous research showing that older

adults learn significantly less than younger adults when subtle regularities are used,

such as those in the ASRT where the alternating structure forces the smallest repeating

units of the pattern to span three trials (Bennett, Howard, & Howard, 2007; Curran,

1997; Howard et al., 2004).

This dissertation will be the first to examine white matter integrity correlates of

implicit sequence learning using DTI. Previous functional imaging and patient research

indicates that implicit sequence learning is mediated by fronto-striatal (Curran, 1995)

6

and medial temporal lobe (Rose, Haider, Weiller, & Buchel, 2002; Schendan, Searl,

Melrose, & Stern, 2003) networks. Therefore, it is expected that the integrity of

separate tracts connecting the caudate and hippocampus to dorsolateral prefrontal

cortex (DLPFC) will be related to sequence learning in the ASRT. These integrity-

sequence learning relationships are predicted to be present in younger and healthy

older adults, consistent with two functional imaging studies that found both age groups

activated similar gray matter regions during implicit sequence learning (Daselaar,

Rombouts, Veltman, Raaijmakers, & Jonker, 2003; Aizenstein et al., 2006). Finally, in

a direct test of the white matter disconnection theory, analyses will assess whether

decreased integrity in older versus younger adults in white matter tracts involved in

implicit sequence learning (i.e., caudate-DLPFC and hippocampus-DLPFC) mediate

age-related deficits in sequence learning.

Taken together, results of the two studies presented in this dissertation will

advance the field of cognitive neuroscience of aging by further characterizing patterns

of healthy age-related decline in white matter integrity, assessing relationships between

white matter integrity and implicit sequence learning in younger and older adults, and

determining whether age group differences in white matter integrity underlie age-

related deficits in implicit sequence learning. These data will be the first to

demonstrate that age-related differences in implicit learning can be explained by white

matter disconnection theories as complex coordination within the underlying neural

networks is compromised by age-related declines in white matter integrity.

7

CHAPTER II: AGE-RELATED DIFFERENCES IN MULTIPLE MEASURES OF WHITE

MATTER INTEGRITY: A DIFFUSION TENSOR IMAGING STUDY

This Chapter has been published as: Bennett, Madden, Vaidya, Howard, and

J.H. Howard, Jr. (2009). Age-related differences in multiple measures of white matter

integrity: A diffusion tensor imaging study of healthy aging. Human Brain Mapping.

Introduction

Brain aging research has been dominated by examinations of age-related

differences in the structure and function of gray matter (Cabeza et al., 2005), with the

other half of the brain—white matter—having been largely ignored. The lack of

attention to white matter aging in the past likely resulted from limitations in imaging

technology, because the relatively recent advent of diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) has

led to widespread in interest in age-related changes in white matter.

DTI is a magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) technique that measures the

diffusion of molecular water (Basser et al., 1994; Pierpaoli & Basser, 1996). Water

diffuses 3-7 times more rapidly along the length of axons aligned in white matter tracts

compared to movement perpendicular to the axons (Le Bihan, 2003; Pierpaoli, Jezzard,

Basser, Barnett, & Di Chiro, 1996) because the latter is restricted by axonal cell

membranes, myelin sheaths, and neurofilaments (Beaulieu, 2002). Various properties

of water diffusion can be calculated from DTI-based eigenvalue measures (λ1, λ2, and

λ3; which indicate the rate of diffusion along the three principal axes of the diffusion

ellipsoid), providing information about the integrity of these white matter structures.

8

Mean diffusivity (MD) and fractional anisotropy (FA) are the two most

commonly used measures of white matter integrity. MD is the average amount of water

diffusion, calculated as the average diffusivity across all three eigenvalues, with higher

values denoting increased rate of diffusion. On the other hand, FA refers to the

coherence of the orientation of water diffusion, independent of rate. It is calculated as

the fraction of total diffusion that can be attributed to anisotropic diffusion, which is

derived from the normalized variances of the three eigenvalues (Basser & Pierpaoli,

1996), with higher values corresponding to a more consistent diffusion orientation.

Taken together, a breakdown in white matter integrity would be seen as higher MD

and/or lower FA.

Accordingly, it is not surprising that the most pervasive findings from DTI

aging research are age-related increases in MD and decreases in FA (Abe et al., 2002;

Grieve, Williams, Paul, Clark, & Gordon, 2007; Head et al., 2004; Hsu et al., 2008;

Hugenschmidt et al., 2008; Pfefferbaum, Adalsteinsson, & Sullivan, 2005;

Pfefferbaum et al., 2000; Salat et al., 2005; Sullivan & Pfefferbaum, 2006; Yoon,

Shim, Lee, Shon, & Yang, 2008). These age differences in integrity are seen

throughout the brain, with most studies reporting the magnitude of the difference to be

larger in anterior white matter compared to posterior regions (Abe et al., 2002;

Ardekani, Kumar, Bartzokis, & Sinha, 2007; Bucur et al., 2008; Grieve et al., 2007;

Gunning-Dixon et al., 2009; Head et al., 2004; Hedden & Gabrieli, 2005; Madden et

al., 2009; Pfefferbaum et al., 2005; Pfefferbaum & Sullivan, 2003; Salat et al., 2004;

9

Salat et al., 2005; Sullivan, Rohlfing, & Pfefferbaum, 2008; Zahr, Rohlfing,

Pfefferbaum, & Sullivan, 2009). This effect, referred to as the anterior-posterior

gradient, has also been seen in the time course of age-related declines in white matter

integrity, with age differences in FA occurring earlier in frontal white matter (Yoon et

al., 2008).

What remains relatively unknown is what these age differences in white matter

integrity mean in terms of underlying neural substrates. With the aim of shedding light

on this issue, DTI aging studies have begun to examine the more frequently used

integrity measures (FA, MD) in relation to axial (AD) and radial (RD) diffusivity,

which correspond to diffusion in the primary (λ1) and perpendicular ((λ2 +λ3)/2)

directions, respectively. Though DTI cannot resolve neural changes within a voxel, this

shift was motivated by animal research indicating that these measures may be

selectively sensitive to specific neural changes, with AD reflecting axonal differences

(e.g., axonal damage or loss)(Budde et al., 2007; Song et al., 2003) and RD reflecting

differences in the degree of myelination (Budde et al., 2007; Nair et al., 2005; Song et

al., 2003; Song et al., 2002; Song et al., 2005).

Previous DTI aging studies that included AD and RD measures have revealed

two prominent patterns of age differences in diffusivity. In the first pattern (Radial

Increase Only), age-related decreases in FA are primarily associated with a significant

increase in RD, but not AD (Bhagat & Beaulieu, 2004; Davis et al., 2009; Madden et

al., 2009; Zhang et al., 2008). In the second pattern (Radial/Axial Increase), age-related

10

decreases in FA are associated with significant increases in both RD and AD (Sullivan

et al., 2006; Sullivan et al., 2008; Zahr et al., 2009). Both patterns are characterized by

an increase in RD with aging. Given the animal literature cited above showing that

increased RD is associated with decreased myelin, and evidence that healthy aging is

accompanied by myelin damage and loss (Peters, 2002), it is not surprising that these

patterns are often interpreted to reflect age-related demyelination. However, because

the focus has been on age differences in RD, potentially important age differences in

AD, and the corresponding neural underpinnings, have been largely ignored.

Shifting attention to age differences in AD may provide insight into a number

of neural substrates underlying age differences in white matter integrity. Diffusion

anisotropy is known to be influenced by both microstructural, cellular variables (e.g.,

axonal packing density, degree of myelination, axonal diameter, and inflammation) and

macrostructural, voxel-level variables (e.g., axonal organization such as aligned,

crossing, and “kissing” fibers)(Giorgio et al., 2008; Mori & Zhang, 2006; Pierpaoli et

al., 1996). Thus, if AD does reflect axonal differences as suggested by the animal

literature cited above, then inferences can be made about the contribution of axon-

based microstructural variables to white matter aging. In addition, we may identify

macrostructural level changes with aging by examining the regional variation of age

differences in AD, because the organization of underlying white matter tracts would

need to be considered.

11

Therefore, the present study assessed region-specific patterns of age differences

in multiple measures of white matter integrity. In order to furthering our understanding

of the neural substrates underlying the widely-reported age differences in FA, younger

and healthy older adults were compared on diffusivity measures (AD and RD; MD was

not included because it is not independent of these measures) in white matter regions

that showed significant age differences in FA, with particularly attention to age

differences in AD. Regional variation of these patterns may reflect differential aging of

the underlying white matter, comparable to studies showing different patterns of aging

across gray matter regions (e.g., Head, Snyder, Girton, Morris, & Buckner, 2005; Raz

et al., 2005). Furthermore, because white matter integrity is compromised (i.e.,

decreased FA) in a variety of populations, including individuals diagnosed with

schizophrenia, multiple sclerosis, and Alzheimer’s disease (Sullivan & Pfefferbaum,

2003; Sundgren et al., 2004), our results may help identify region-specific patterns of

diffusivity that are unique to healthy aging.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Fourteen Georgetown University undergraduate students (18-20 years old) and

14 older adults (63-72 years old) who responded to advertisements in the Washington

Post Health Section were recruited. Demographic and neuropsychological

characterizations for each group are presented in Table 1. All participants gave

informed consent, and received either payment or course credit for their participation.

12

The Georgetown University Institutional Review Board approved the experimental

procedures.

Prior to participation, individuals were screened for conditions that would

prevent them from being able to enter the MRI scanner. These conditions included

being pregnant, having ferrous metal implants, having difficulty lying in the supine

position for 30 minutes, and being claustrophobic. In addition, individuals were

excluded prior to scanning if they reported having a health condition or neurological

disease or disorder that is known to influence cognitive functioning and/or contribute

to white matter pathology (e.g., stroke, dementia, diabetes, and uncontrolled depression

or hypertension).

General Procedure

Participants completed three days of testing. On the first day, they completed

screening procedures (informed consent, biographical and health screen questionnaires,

and MRI safety form) and the MRI scanning protocol. On the second and third days of

testing, participants completed the comprehensive neuropsychological test battery (see

Table 1) and a computer-based learning task (data not presented here).

MRI scanning protocol. Participants were scanned using the 3.0 Tesla MRI

system (Siemens Magnetom Trio, Erlangen, Germany) at Georgetown University’s

Center for Functional and Molecular Imaging. An imaging technician positioned

participants in the scanner, laying them in the supine position with a circularly

polarized head coil. A mirror mounted on the head coil allowed them to watch cable

13

14

television programming during scanning. Fitted padding was used to minimize head

movements.

A high resolution T1-weighted structural scan (MPRAGE) was acquired first

with the following parameters: scan time = 7:23 minutes, TR = 2300 ms, TE = 2.94

ms, TI = 900 ms, 9o flip angle, 1 slab, 160 sagittal slices with a 1.0 mm slice thickness,

and FOV = 256 x 256 mm with a 256 x 256 matrix resulting in an effective resolution

of 1.0 mm3 isotropic voxels. A Neurologist reviewed these images, and no participant

exhibited a clinically significant structural abnormality that was atypical for their age

(e.g., lesions, excessive atrophy).

Two 35-direction diffusion weighted echo planar imaging sequences were then

acquired using gradient values of b = 0 and b = 1000 s/mm2 applied in 35 orthogonal

directions. Each acquisition had the following parameters: scan time = 4:39 minutes,

TR = 7700 ms, TE = 100 ms, 55 axial interleaved slices with a 2.5 mm slices thickness

with no gap, and FOV = 240 x 240 mm with a 64 x 64 matrix resulting in an effective

resolution of 2.5 mm3 isotropic voxels. The entire scanning session took approximately

30 minutes.

Data analysis

Diffusion data processing. Diffusion-weighted data were separately processed

for each participant using the University of Oxford’s Center for Functional Magnetic

Resonance Imaging of the Brain (FMRIB) Software Library (FSL) release 4.0

(http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). First, the two diffusion acquisitions were concatenated

15

in time to increase the signal-to-noise ratio. The first volume within this merged data

file that did not have gradient applied (i.e., the first b = 0 image) was used to generate a

binary brain mask with the Brain Extraction Tool. These data were then corrected for

head movement and eddy current distortions using Eddycorrect, which aligns all the

volumes. Finally, DTIfit was used to independently fit diffusion tensors to each voxel,

with the brain mask limiting the fitting of tensors to brain space. The output of DTIfit

yielded voxelwise maps of FA, AD (λ1), and RD (average of λ2 and λ3) for each

participant.

Between-group t-tests. A between-group skeleton-wise t-test analysis was

performed for the FA measure using Tract-Based Spatial Statistics (Smith et al., 2006).

In this analysis, individual FA maps were non-linearly aligned to the FMRIB58_FA

template, affine transformed into MNI152 1 mm3 standard space, and then averaged

across all participants to form a mean FA image. The mean FA image was used to

generate a white matter skeleton that identifies the center of white matter tracts shared

by all participants. This mean FA skeleton was thresholded at 2000 (corresponding to

FA > 0.2) to exclude voxels containing gray matter or cerebrospinal fluid. Pre-aligned

FA images from each participant were registered to the mean FA skeleton by searching

for maximum FA values perpendicular to the skeleton. This second registration

corrected for any misalignment from the initial nonlinear registration and affine

transformation. The resulting skeletonized FA data were then subjected to a skeleton-

wise (i.e., voxel-wise comparison limited to voxels within the mean skeleton)

16

comparison of FA between younger and older adults. This between-group t-test was

conducted using the Randomise tool, which tests the t value at each voxel against a

null distribution generated from 5000 random permutations of group membership. The

output contained statistical maps corrected for multiple comparisons at the cluster level

(cluster-forming threshold t > 3.0, p < .05).

Significant clusters from the FA analysis were separately masked and labeled

with reference to the JHU ICBM-DTI-81 White Matter Labels, part of the FSL atlas

tools. To obtain mean FA values for each participant from the significant clusters, each

mask was projected back into the native space of each individual’s FA map using the

FSL Deproject tool. Back projected masks were then binarized and multiplied by the

individual’s FA map, leaving just the FA values for voxels within that cluster, which

were then averaged. The same back projection and averaging was conducted for each

of the non-FA maps (λ1, λ2, and λ3) to obtain mean values for each participant for the

significant clusters from the FA analysis. Values for the λ2 and λ3 maps were averaged

to get a single measure of mean RD for each individual.

Finally, between-group t-test analyses were conducted separately for AD and

RD at each of the 28 significant clusters from the FA analysis. Results for each

diffusivity measure were corrected for multiple comparisons using Bonferroni

corrections for 28 comparisons.

Results

Neuropsychological Data

17

Neuropsychological test results, seen in Table 1, revealed the typical pattern of

age effects, with all participants performing within the age-expected range. That is,

older adults performed significantly worse than younger adults on measures of

processing speed, cued recall, and free recall, but not vocabulary or reading ability. All

participants had Mini-Mental State Examination (Folstein, Folstein, & McHugh, 1975)

scores that were ≥ 28.

Age Group Differences in FA

Results of the skeleton-wise between group t-test for FA are presented in

Figure 1. Significantly lower FA was seen in older adults compared to younger adults

in 28 white matter clusters (see descriptions in Table 2), which included two midline,

11 bilateral, and four unilateral regions. Three of the unilateral clusters were in the left

hemisphere, suggesting a slight hemispheric asymmetry, especially in frontal regions

(left anterior/inferior corona radiata, left frontal cluster). Consistent with the notion that

the integrity of white matter connections degrade with age, there were no regions with

significantly higher FA in older versus younger adults.

Age Group Differences in Diffusivity Measures

Between group t-test results for the non-FA, diffusivity measures are

summarized in Table 3, and the corresponding statistics are presented in Table 4. These

data revealed that age-related decreases in FA are characterized by three distinct

patterns of age differences in diffusivity measures. In the first pattern, Radial Increase

Only, clusters with significant age-related decreases in FA also had significant age-

18

19

related increases in RD, but no age group effects in AD. For the second pattern,

Radial/Axial Increase, clusters in which FA decreased with age also had significantly

higher RD and AD values for older adults relative to younger adults. In the third

20

pattern, Radial Increase/Axial Decrease, clusters with significant age-related decreases

in FA also had significantly higher RD and lower AD in older versus younger adults.

21

Age Group Differences in RD across Diffusivity Patterns

22

Significant age-related increases in RD were seen in all clusters that had

significant age differences in FA. To determine whether the magnitude of the age

difference in RD differed across the three diffusivity patterns, an Age Group (younger,

older) x Pattern (Radial Increase Only; Radial/Axial Increase; Radial Increase/Axial

Decrease) ANOVA was conducted for the RD measure. As expected, results revealed a

significant main effect of Age Group, F (1,26) = 65.7, p < .001, indicating that RD was

higher in older (8.52 ± 2.48 x 10-4 mm2/sec) compared to younger (5.39 ± 1.07 x 10-4

mm2/sec) adults. There was also a significant main effect of Pattern, F (2,52) = 224.1,

p < .001, with higher RD in the Radial/Axial Increase pattern (8.52 ± 2.30 x 10-4

mm2/sec) compared to the Radial Increase Only (3.23 ± 0.65 x 10-4 mm2/sec) and

Radial Increase/Axial Decrease (5.16 ± 0.54 x 10-4 mm2/sec) patterns. Importantly, the

Age Group x Pattern interaction, F (2,52) = 43.3, p < .001, revealed that the age-related

difference in RD (older minus younger adults) was significantly larger for the

Radial/Axial Increase pattern (3.78 x 10-4 mm2/sec) compared to the Radial Increase

Only (0.90 x 10-4 mm2/sec) and Radial Increase/Axial Decrease (0.80 x 10-4 mm2/sec)

patterns. A follow-up 2 Age Group x 2 Pattern ANOVA confirmed that the magnitude

of the age difference in RD was comparable in the Radial Increase Only and Radial

Increase/Axial Decrease patterns (p > .20).

Age Group Differences in AD across Diffusivity Patterns

As can be seen in Table 3, the three diffusivity patterns differ with respect to

age differences in AD. That is, clusters either had no significant age differences in AD

23

(Radial Increase Only), significantly higher AD in older versus younger adults

(Radial/Axial Increase), or significantly lower AD in older versus younger adults

(Radial Increase/Axial Decrease). To determine whether the magnitude of the age

difference in AD was significantly different across the three diffusivity patterns, an

Age Group x Pattern ANOVA was conducted for the AD measure. Results revealed

significant main effects of Age Group, F (1,26) = 14.3, p < .001 and Pattern, F (2,52) =

211.1, p < .001, such that AD was higher in older (1.40 ± 0.28 x 10-3 mm2/sec) versus

younger (1.34 ± 0.10 x 10-3 mm2/sec) adults, and higher in the Radial/Axial Increase

pattern (1.61 ± 0.20 x 10-3 mm2/sec) compared to the Radial Increase Only (1.24 ± 0.05

x 10-3 mm2/sec) and Radial Increase/Axial Decrease (1.26 ± 0.05 x 10-3 mm2/sec)

patterns. There was also a significant Age Group x Pattern interaction, F (2,52) = 58.3,

p < .001, which indicated that the age-related difference in AD (older minus younger

adults) was significantly larger for the Radial/Axial Increase pattern (3.15 x 10-3

mm2/sec) compared to the Radial Increase Only (-0.05 x 10-3 mm2/sec) and Radial

Increase/Axial Decrease (-0.08 x 10-3 mm2/sec) patterns. Importantly, a follow-up 2

Age Group x 2 Pattern ANOVA revealed a significant interaction, F (1,26) = 6.8, p <

.02, indicating that the magnitude of the age difference in AD was significantly

different for the Radial Increase Only and Radial Increase/Axial Decrease patterns.

Anterior-Posterior Gradient

There was evidence of an anterior-posterior gradient in the magnitude of the

age-related difference in FA (see Figure 2). A correlation between the percent age

24

group difference in FA (calculated as FA in older adults minus FA in younger adults,

divided by the average FA value) and the location of each cluster (measured as the

average y coordinate) revealed a significant positive relationship, r = .40, p < .04, such

that age-related decreases in FA were larger in more anterior clusters. When analyzed

separately for each hemisphere, the relationship remained positive (left hemisphere, r =

.43; right hemisphere, r = .39), but no longer attained significance (p’s > .13). The

strength of the correlation was magnified when only superior clusters with z

coordinates above or traversing zero were assessed, r = .60, p < .01, whereas it did not

approach significance for inferior clusters, p > .85. This latter result is comparable to

previous research in which significant anterior-posterior gradients of age differences in

FA are reported for superior white matter (e.g., Davis et al., 2009; Pfefferbaum et al.,

2005).

Before concluding that anterior white matter is more susceptible to aging than

posterior regions, it is important to recognize that the age-related decline is regionally

complex. For example, variability of the magnitude of age differences was greater in

posterior clusters, which included regions with both the largest (left inferior sagittal

stratum, right cerebellum) and smallest (bilateral retrolenticular part of the internal

capsule and posterior pericallosal white matter) age group differences in FA. Note that

the fornix was removed from this analysis and from Figure 2 because the percentage of

age-related FA decrease was more than four standard deviations above the average

difference of the remaining white matter clusters.

25

Discussion

Summary of findings

26

The present study examined age group differences in white matter

microstructure by comparing younger and healthy older adults on multiple measures of

white matter integrity. The main finding was that age-related decreases in FA were

associated with three region-specific patterns of age differences in diffusivity measures

(AD and RD).

Radial Increase Only. In most clusters, decreased FA in older adults was

associated with an age-related increase in RD but no age difference in AD. This has

been the predominant pattern reported in several previous DTI aging studies (Bhagat &

Beaulieu, 2004; Davis et al., 2009; Fjell et al., 2008; Madden et al., 2009; Zhang et al.,

2008). The present findings replicated earlier studies that also showed this pattern in

frontal (Zhang et al., 2008), posterior pericallosal (Vernooij et al., 2008; Zhang et al.,

2008), superior longitudinal fasciculus (Madden et al., 2009; Sullivan et al., 2008), and

sagittal stratum (Burzynska et al., 2009) white matter.

Radial/Axial Increase. In other clusters, decreased FA in older adults was

associated with an age-related increase in both RD and AD, which has also been the

primary pattern reported in several other DTI aging studies (Sullivan et al., 2006;

Sullivan et al., 2008; Zahr et al., 2009). The current findings replicated some earlier

reports of this pattern in the genu of the corpus callosum (Burzynska et al., 2009;

Sullivan et al., 2006; Sullivan et al., 2008; Zahr et al., 2009), external capsule

(Burzynska et al., 2009; Sullivan et al., 2008), and fornix (Burzynska et al., 2009;

Sullivan et al., 2008; Vernooij et al., 2008; Zahr et al., 2009). A few studies found that

27

the genu of the corpus callosum (Davis et al., 2009; Madden et al., 2009; Vernooij et

al., 2008) or external capsule (Bhagat & Beaulieu, 2004) fit into the Radial Increase

Only pattern, which may be linked to the possibility that the Radial Increase Only and

Radial/Axial Increase patterns reflect different degrees of severity of the same

underlying neural changes that affect RD.

Radial Increase/Axial Decrease. Remaining clusters with decreased FA in

older adults had an age-related increase in RD and decrease in AD. This pattern was

only recently indentified in another DTI aging study (Burzynska et al., 2009), with two

earlier studies having each observed this pattern in a single white matter region. The

present finding of the Radial Increase/Axial Decrease pattern in frontal white matter

(anterior pericallosal, anterior/superior corona radiata) overlaps with earlier

observations in the superior frontal gyrus (Bhagat & Beaulieu, 2004), superior corona

radiata (Burzynska et al., 2009), and frontal forceps (Burzynska et al., 2009; Sullivan

et al., 2008). This pattern was also previously seen in the retrolenticular part of the

internal capsule (Burzynska et al., 2009). In contrast to the other patterns, possible

neural substrates underlying the Radial Increase/Axial Decrease pattern in healthy

aging have only recently been proposed (e.g., lesion-induced axonal loss and gliosis;

Burzynska et al., 2009).

Age-related increases in RD

In the present study, age-related increases in RD were characteristic of all

clusters in each of the three patterns, suggesting that the neural changes underlying RD

28

increases with healthy aging affect white matter microstructure throughout the brain.

However, the magnitude of the age difference in RD was significantly larger in the

Radial/Axial Increase pattern, indicating that the severity of these neural changes may

be exacerbated in these clusters.

Animal literature has shown RD increases in mice that have been genetically

modified to be myelin deficient (Nair et al., 2005; Song et al., 2002) or treated with

cuprizone to induce myelin loss (Song et al., 2005). Combined with evidence that

myelin damage and loss is prevalent in healthy aging (Peters, 2002), these findings

suggest that age-related demyelination contributes to RD increases in older compared

to younger adults. However, myelin is not the only factor contributing to restricted

diffusion measured by FA because anisotropic diffusion can occur in the absence of

myelin (Beaulieu, 2002; Berman et al., 2005; Le Bihan, 2003; Pierpaoli et al., 1996).

Thus, it may be an oversimplification to conclude that age-related increases in RD are

due solely to demyelination in healthy aging.

Alternatively, some researchers have proposed that the primary determinant of

anisotropy is the packing density of axons within a voxel (Beaulieu, 2002; Shenkin et

al., 2003; c.f., Pierpaoli et al., 1996). Axonal packing density encompasses a variety of

microstructural level variables (e.g., degree of myelination, axonal diameter,

inflammation, etc.) that can influence the amount of extracellular water between axons,

and thus the amount of diffusion in the non-primary direction. One benefit of this

explanation is that it applies to multiple white matter changes known to occur with

29

aging (Gunning-Dixon et al., 2009; Peters, 2002), but that cannot be separately

assessed with the current resolution of most DTI sequences (Sullivan, et al. 2008).

Region-specific differences in the degree of age-related change in axonal

packing density may explain why the age difference in RD was significantly larger in

the Radial/Axial Increase pattern. For example, age differences in microstructural level

variables contributing to decreased axonal packing density may be mild enough in

Radial Increase Only and Radial Increase/Axial Decrease clusters to only increase RD.

However, more severe decreases in axonal packing density from, for example, a

greater loss of myelin or axons in aging, would lead to a global increase in

extracellular water, resulting in larger RD increases and subsequent AD increases in

Radial/Axial Increase clusters. This hypothesis is supported by evidence that the genu

of the corpus callosum, a Radial/Axial Increase cluster, contains a many small

diameter unmyelinated and thinly myelinated axons (Aboitiz, Scheibel, Fisher, &

Zaidel, 1992), the latter of which are highly susceptible to degeneration and

demyelination in aging (Marner, Nyengaard, Tang, & Pakkenberg, 2003; Tang,

Nyengaard, Pakkenberg, & Gundersen, 1997).

Age-related decreases in AD

Previous DTI aging articles that have examined AD and RD tend to focus on

age-related increases in the latter, which, as mentioned above, are often interpreted to

reflect age-related demyelination (e.g., Davis et al., 2009). As a result, the DTI aging

30

literature has largely ignored age differences in AD and its potential underlying neural

substrates.

In the present study, the three observed patterns differed with respect to age

effects for the AD measure, with either no significant age differences in AD (Radial

Increase Only), significantly higher AD in older adults (Radial/Axial Increase), or

significantly higher AD in younger adults (Radial Increase/Axial Decrease). As can be

seen from the statistics presented in Table 4, 80% of the Radial Increase Only clusters

have non-significant age-related decreases in AD. Importantly, however, follow-up

comparisons revealed that the magnitude of the age-related decrease in AD was

significantly larger in the Radial Increase/Axial Decrease pattern compared to the

Radial Increase Only pattern, indicating that these patterns are quantitatively different.

One interpretation of age-related decreases in AD can be inferred from previous

research on the effect of ischemic stroke on DTI-based measures of white matter

integrity in humans (Pierpaoli et al., 2001; Thomalla et al., 2004) and mice (Song et al.,

2003). These studies suggest that axonal degeneration and subsequent gliosis that

follow an ischemic incident or lesion formation lead to a disruption of diffusion

coherence, which decreases diffusion parallel to the primary diffusion direction.

Burzynska, et al. (2009) recently interpreted a Radial Increase/Axial Decrease pattern

in this manner. However, it may not completely explain the present results given that

examination of high resolution structural scans by a Neurologist did not reveal any

clinically significant structural abnormalities. Furthermore, this pattern of diffusivity

31

change is commonly seen following insults in regions with consistent fiber orientation

(Pierpaoli et al., 2001), whereas Radial Increase/Axial Decrease clusters in the present

study often contained projection tracts in regions with many crossing fibers, especially

inferior-to-superior tracts that pass through long range anterior-to-posterior tracts (e.g.,

thalamic radiations in the RLIC, anterior/superior and superior corona radiata, and

anterior pericallosal clusters; and corticospinal tract in superior corona radiata and

cerebral peduncle clusters).

We propose an alternative interpretation: age-related AD decreases may result

from disrupted macrostructural organization. Radial Increase/Axial Decrease clusters

with multiple white matter tracts likely have less coherent diffusion in any given

orientation, and thus decreased AD. On its own, this effect should not differ across age

groups (Head et al., 2004). However, macrostructural organization of these regions

may be disrupted in older adults because of existing age differences in microstructural

variables that affect axonal packing density (e.g., demyelination and axonal

shrinkage)(Stadlbauer, Salomonowitz, Strunk, Hammen, & Ganslandt, 2008; Zhou,

Goto, Otsuka, Moriyama, & Nakamura, 1997), which may magnify the AD decrease in

these regions with aging. In other words, AD in a cluster with crossing fibers would

reflect the average primary diffusion direction of two highly aligned tracts in younger

adults, but the average of two loosely aligned tracts (due to age-related decreased

packing density) in older adults, leading to lower AD in the latter group. Future

research will be necessary to determine whether the Radial Increase/Axial Decrease

32

pattern results from age-related axonal degeneration versus age differences in

macrostructural organization.

Anterior-posterior gradient

Results of the present study support an anterior-posterior gradient, with the

magnitude of the age difference in FA being significantly larger in anterior white

matter clusters. The present findings are in line with histological studies showing that

anterior white matter is more susceptible to age-related alterations on variables

affecting measures of integrity (e.g., myelin loss or damage)(e.g., Peters, 2002), and

studies showing that other changes in the aging brain (e.g., gray matter shrinkage,

neurotransmitter functioning) predominantly affect frontal regions (Raz, 2000; Salat et

al., 2004; West, 1996). Nonetheless, it is important to note that the anterior-posterior

gradient of age differences may be misleading because age differences in white matter

integrity often occur throughout the brain, as seen in the present data (see Figure 1) and

in previous studies (Head et al., 2004; Madden et al., 2004; Salat et al., 2005), with

substantial variability in the magnitude of age differences across regions (see Figure 2).

Limitations

The primary limitation of this study is that DTI, as implemented here and in

most previous studies, cannot assess individual neural substrates that contribute to

anisotropy and diffusivity smaller than the 1-3 mm voxel size, such as degree of

myelination, fiber orientation, and axonal loss. Thus, future studies using either higher

resolution DTI or other techniques that can assess white matter microstructure at the

33

cellular level (e.g., magnetic resonance spectroscopy) will be necessary to validate

interpretations of patterns of age differences in diffusivity, such as those proposed

here.

Nonetheless, certain DTI-based studies may be useful in furthering our

understanding of these patterns. For example, future studies that can incorporate

measures of intervoxel coherence (e.g., lattice index) may be able to assess whether

clusters with decreased FA and AD are in regions with crossing fibers (i.e., in regions

with low lattice index), which may provide additional support for the theory that the

Radial Increase/Axial Decrease pattern reflects age differences in macrostructural

organization in these regions. Similarly, future studies could examine the classification

of various white matter regions longitudinally to provide additional support for the

notion that the patterns reflect varying degrees of severity of underlying neural

changes. For example, such studies may show that at younger ages where white matter

aging is minimal, most regions are classified as the Radial Increase Only pattern,

whereas with increasing age and severity of underlying microstructural changes, more

regions may be classified by the Radial/Axial Increase pattern. The presence of non-

significant trends for age-related increases and decreases in AD in most regions

classified by the Radial Increase Only pattern (see Table 4) indicate that clusters have

the potential to be reclassified over time depending on the progression of underlying

microstructural and/or macrostructral level white matter changes.

34

Another potential limitation of the present study stems from the use of Tract-

Based Spatial Statistics (TBSS). This technique was chosen because it involves a high

quality across-subject registration procedure that relies on a “skeleton” created from

the center of white matter tracts common to all participants (Smith et al., 2006; Smith

et al., 2007), which minimizes alignment issues commonly seen when comparing

groups, such as healthy older versus younger adults, with gross-level brain differences

(e.g., atrophy, ventricular enlargement, sulcal expansion). This approach restricts

analyses to major white matter tracts, thus reducing the area of white matter that could

be identified as having significant age differences in FA. However, it has the benefit of

reducing false identification of group differences because it excludes white matter

regions subject to partial voluming effects due to, for example, gray matter atrophy

(Hugenschmidt et al., 2008) or cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) contamination (Bhagat &

Beaulieu, 2004; Hirsch, Bock, Essig, & Schad, 1999; Papadakis et al., 2002) that can

decrease anisotropy and increase diffusivity measures. Thus, the skeletonizing

technique used here minimized the likelihood that partial voluming effects can explain

the present results, even in Radial/Axial Increase pattern clusters that are surrounded

by gray matter and CSF.

The present analyses restricted age group comparisons of RD and AD to

regions with significant age differences in FA because the primary aim was to make

inferences about the possible neural substrates underlying this commonly observed age

difference in white matter integrity. However, certain patterns of age differences

35

cannot be detected using this approach (e.g., regions with increased RD and/or AD, but

not FA), and potential variations in the pattern of effects across voxels within each

cluster was not assessed. Importantly, the region specificity of patterns identified here

showed substantial overlap with findings from earlier DTI aging studies that used

unrestricted analyses (i.e., voxel-wise comparisons for each diffusivity

measure)(Burzynska et al., 2009; Vernooij et al., 2008; Zhang et al., 2008). Similarly,

when skeleton-wise comparisons were conducted separately for each diffusivity

measure (AD, RD) using the present data (not shown), every Radial/Axial Increase

region continued to show the same pattern, as did the majority of Radial Increase Only

(9/15) and Radial Increase/Axial Decrease (4/9) regions. Together, these results

suggest that limiting analyses to regions with significant age differences in FA had

minimal impact on the generality of the present results.

Conclusions and implications

The present study used the well-established approach of examining AD and RD

in conjunction with FA to assess patterns of age differences in diffusivity and to make

inferences about the underlying neural substrates. Of the nine previous DTI aging

studies that have also assessed age differences in these diffusivity measures, only one

recent report has taken steps to categorize these patterns and discuss the relevance of

age-related AD decreases (Burzynska et al., 2009), complementing the present study.

However, this study is one of the first to propose that a macrostructural level variable

may underlie age differences in white matter integrity.

36

Taken together, results revealed three patterns of age differences in diffusivity

in white matter clusters that had significant age-related decreases in FA. In line with

evidence from animal research, we propose that these three patterns reflect various

combinations of at least two qualitatively different changes in underlying white matter

that differentially affect RD and AD. First, age-related increases in RD, seen in all

white matter clusters in the present study, may reflect an age-related decrease in axonal

packing density from microstructural level variables including demyelination, axonal

loss or damage, and inflammation. These underlying neural changes may be mild in

certain brain regions (Radial Increase Only pattern), whereas other regions that are

more susceptible to aging, such as the genu of the corpus callosum, may have moderate

to severe changes leading to increased RD and AD (Radial/Axial Increase pattern).

Second, age-related decreases in AD, seen in the Radial Increase/Axial Decrease

pattern, may reflect age-related axonal degeneration. However, given that this pattern

primarily occurred in white matter regions with crossing fibers, the age-related

decrease in the coherence of diffusion may instead be due to age-related differences in

macrostructural organization. Results also revealed that age differences in FA were

larger in anterior white matter clusters, consistent with the anterior-posterior gradient

of aging, though differences were seen throughout the brain.

This study takes an important step toward understanding the neural substrates

underlying differences in white matter integrity in healthy aging, though future

research replicating these results and interpretations is necessary. Because many

37

populations are characterized by decreased FA (e.g., schizophrenia, multiple sclerosis,

mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer’s disease), regional specificity of the

various patterns of diffusivity may help dissociate changes in white matter integrity

that are unique to healthy aging. Previous research suggests that age differences in FA

are largest and occur earliest in frontal brain regions (i.e., the anterior-posterior

gradient), but it may be possible to identify specific patterns of diffusivity occurring in

specific locations that are characteristic of healthy brain aging, such as increases in all

diffusivity measures occurring earliest in the genu of the corpus callosum.

38

CHAPTER III: WHITE MATTER INTEGRITY CORRELATES OF IMPLICIT

SEQUENCE LEARNING IN HEALTHY AGING

Implicit learning refers to sensitivity to regularities in the environment that

occurs without awareness (Seger, 1994). This ubiquitous process plays a role in skills

ranging from language acquisition (Kuhl, 2004) to social intuition (Lieberman, 2000),

which involve extracting predictable words from speech streams and non-verbal cues

from social interactions, respectively. Implicit learning is essential for adapting to new

people, places, and technologies; particularly in aging as explicit forms of learning

decline (Craik & Jennings, 1992).

The present study focuses on implicit probabilistic sequence learning, a form of

implicit learning that involves extracting regularities from a series of events. This type

of learning can be assessed with the alternating serial reaction time (ASRT) task

(Howard & Howard, 1997), in which participants view four open circles on the screen

and respond to the location of the circle that fills in black. Unbeknownst to

participants, every second stimulus follows a repeating pattern and intervening stimuli

are randomly determined. Sequence-specific learning is seen as faster reaction time

and/or higher accuracy to the predictable, pattern-related events that occur with greater

probability than random events.

Behaviorally, implicit sequence learning is sometimes spared in aging. For

example, older adults learn as well as younger adults in studies using sequences with

simple deterministic structure, where single items (0th order structure) or pairs of items

39

(1st order structure) occur more frequently than others (Curran, 1997; Frensch &

Miner, 1994; Howard & Howard, 1989). However, age-related deficits often appear

when more complex sequences are used, such as those in which the lowest level of

regularity to be learned spans three (2nd order structure) or four (3rd order structure)

consecutive trials (Bennett et al., 2007; Curran, 1997; Howard et al., 2004; Howard &

Howard, 1997). When present, the magnitudes of these age-related differences are

greater in later stages of learning as younger adults continue to learn after older adults

reach a plateau.

Neurobiologically, implicit sequence learning has repeatedly been shown to

involve fronto-striatal networks (Curran, 1995; Doyon et al., 2009; Seger, 2006). This

includes projections between the striatum (caudate, putamen) and frontal regions such

as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), and primary, supplementary (SMA), and

premotor areas. There is also evidence that medial temporal lobe (MTL) regions such

as the hippocampus and parahippocampal gyrus (entorhinal, parahippocampal, and

perirhinal cortices) are involved in implicit learning of complex sequences (Rose et al.,

2002; Schendan et al., 2003). Whereas the putamen and frontal motor regions are

thought to be involved in motor demands of the task (e.g., response preparation and

execution), both the caudate and MTL have been implicated in the formation of

associations necessary for learning (e.g., stimulus-stimulus and stimulus-response

associations)(Curran, 1995). Research suggests that these two structures may be

recruited at different stages of implicit sequence learning (Schendan et al., 2003), with

40

the MTL being essential for rapid association formation early in learning and the

caudate being involved in forming associations into later stages of learning (Poldrack

& Packard, 2003).

Previous research has examined neural correlates of implicit sequence learning

in healthy aging. This literature consists of two functional imaging studies that each

observed task-related activity in expected regions for both younger and older adults

(e.g., striatum, frontal regions, hippocampus). However, one study found that the

magnitude of these effects did not differ with age group (Daselaar et al., 2003),

whereas the second study reported an age-related decrease in activity in the prefrontal

cortex and putamen (Aizenstein et al., 2006). Thus, the effect of aging on these brain-

behavior relationships remains unresolved. In addition to these mixed findings, age

group differences in activation patterns for early versus late learning were not assessed.

Together, these earlier examinations of age group differences in the neural

correlates of implicit sequence learning were restricted to isolated cortical and

subcortical gray matter regions (except see Aizenstein et al., 2002). One way to avoid

this limitation is with diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) tractography, which assesses the

integrity of white matter tracts that connect gray matter regions within distributed

neural networks. DTI measures the diffusion, or movement, of molecular water, which

is summarized for each voxel as an ellipsoid where the eigenvalues λ1, λ2, and λ3

indicate the rate of diffusion along its three principal axes (Basser et al., 1994;

Pierpaoli & Basser, 1996). Tractography analyses can then reconstruct white matter

41

tracts, which are assumed to follow the trajectory of the primary diffusion directions

(λ1) of adjacent voxels (see Jones, 2008 for an overview of tractography methods), and

measures of white matter integrity can be calculated from these tracts using the DTI-

based eigenvalues.

Fractional anisotropy (FA) is a commonly used measure of integrity that refers

to the coherence of the orientation of water diffusion, independent of rate. It is

calculated as the fraction of total diffusion that can be attributed to anisotropic

diffusion, which is derived from the normalized variances of the three eigenvalues

(Basser & Pierpaoli, 1996). A single white matter tract with high integrity would have

more consistent diffusion in one orientation, and thus a higher FA value. In healthy

aging, significant decreases in FA are often reported, with the age group differences

being largest in frontal tracts (e.g., Davis et al., 2009; Sullivan et al., 2008). When

accompanied by an age-related decrease in cognitive performance, studies often find

that age group differences in FA from underlying white matter tracts mediate age

group differences in cognition (Madden et al., 2009; Perry et al., 2009; Zahr et al.,

2009). However, previous DTI tractography studies have not examined whether

relationships between tract-based measures of FA and cognitive performance differ

significantly across age groups (i.e., whether these relationships are moderated by age

group).

To address gaps in earlier research, the present study will pursue two aims

regarding relationships between ASRT performance and DTI tractography-based

42

measures of integrity in younger and healthy older adults. The primary aim will be to

examine white matter integrity correlates of implicit sequence learning using

correlations between FA from three bilateral subcortical-cortical tracts and

performance on the ASRT. Given the functions of the gray matter regions these tracts

connect (i.e., association formation versus response preparation and execution), it is

expected that sequence learning will relate to FA in the caudate-DLPFC and

hippocampus-DLPFC tracts, but not the putamen-SMA tract. Step-wise regression

analyses will also be used to determine whether these FA-sequence learning

relationships are moderated by age group. Based on the mixed findings from earlier

functional imaging studies, it is unknown whether the magnitude of these relationships

will vary with age. The secondary aim will separate learning into three stages to assess

age group differences in the time course of learning, and whether white matter integrity

correlates of sequence learning vary with practice. It is expected that larger age group

differences in sequence learning will be seen in the late versus early learning stage, and

that hippocampus-DLPFC tract FA will relate to early learning whereas caudate-

DLPFC tract FA will relate to learning at all stages.

Method

Participants

Fourteen Georgetown University undergraduate students (18.9 ± 0.7 years old,

9 female) and 14 older adults (67.6 ± 3.1 years old, 10 female) who responded to

advertisements in the Washington Post Health Section were recruited.

43

Neuropsychological characterizations for each group are presented in Table 1. All

participants gave informed consent, and received either payment or course credit for

their participation. The Georgetown University Institutional Review Board approved

the experimental procedures.

Prior to participation, individuals were screened for conditions that would

affect their ability to complete the computer-based task (e.g., uncorrected vision,

arthritis, and back problems that would make it difficult to see items presented on the

computer screen or comfortably push the response buttons), prevent them from being

able to enter the MRI scanner (e.g., being pregnant, having ferrous metal implants,

having difficulty lying in the supine position for 30 minutes, and being

claustrophobic), or influence their cognitive functioning and/or contribute to white

matter pathology (e.g., stroke, dementia, diabetes, and uncontrolled depression or

hypertension).

General Procedure

Participants completed three days of testing. On the first day, they completed

screening procedures (informed consent, biographical and health screen questionnaires,

and MRI safety form) and the MRI scanning protocol. On the second and third days of

testing, participants completed the comprehensive neuropsychological test battery (see

Table 1) and a computer-based implicit sequence learning task, the ASRT.

MRI scanning protocol. Participants were scanned using the 3.0 Tesla MRI

system (Siemens Magnetom Trio, Erlangen, Germany) at Georgetown University’s

44

Center for Functional and Molecular Imaging. An imaging technician positioned

participants in the scanner, laying them in the supine position with a circularly

polarized head coil. A mirror mounted on the head coil allowed them to watch cable

television programming during scanning. Fitted padding was used to minimize head

movements.

A high resolution T1-weighted structural scan (MPRAGE) was acquired first

with the following parameters: scan time = 7:23 minutes, TR = 2300 ms, TE = 2.94

ms, TI = 900 ms, 9o flip angle, 1 slab, 160 sagittal slices with a 1.0 mm slice thickness,

and FOV = 256 x 256 mm with a 256 x 256 matrix resulting in an effective resolution

of 1.0 mm3 isotropic voxels. A neurologist reviewed these images, and no participant

exhibited a clinically significant structural abnormality that was atypical for their age

(e.g., lesions, excessive atrophy).

Two 35-direction diffusion weighted echo planar imaging sequences were then

acquired using gradient values of b = 0 and b = 1000 s/mm2 applied in 35 orthogonal

directions. Each acquisition had the following parameters: scan time = 4:39 minutes,

TR = 7700 ms, TE = 100 ms, 55 axial interleaved slices with a 2.5 mm slices thickness

with no gap, and FOV = 240 x 240 mm with a 64 x 64 matrix resulting in an effective

resolution of 2.5 mm3 isotropic voxels. The entire scanning session, which included

three functional runs with a different task (data not reported here), took approximately

45 minutes.

45

Alternating serial reaction time task (ASRT). Participants viewed four black

outlined open circles presented in a row on a 17 in. PC computer monitor. On each

trial, one circle filled in black and participants were instructed to press one of four

buttons that corresponded to the target locations using their dominant hand. If the first

response was incorrect, the target remained on the screen until the correct response was

made. A 120 ms interval separated a correct response on one trial and presentation of

the target on the following trial. Participants completed 45 88-trial blocks, with each

block containing 8 practice trials followed by 10 repetitions of the 8-element sequence.

Short breaks were offered after each block.

Unbeknownst to participants, this ASRT task contained 2nd order sequential

structure, meaning that the location of the target on every other trial was determined by

a repeating sequence, and intervening trials were randomly determined. The six

possible repeating sequences can be presented as 1r2r3r4r, 1r2r4r3r, 1r3r2r4r, 1r3r4r2r,

1r4r2r3r, and 1r4r3r2r; where the number refers to the location of the target from left to

right, and ‘r’ refers to a random trial where the target could occur at any of the four

locations. Each participant was randomly assigned one of these repeating sequences.

Participants were instructed to respond to each target as quickly as possible. As

older adults tend to make fewer errors than younger adults, which complicates age

comparisons, differential feedback previously shown to match the groups on overall

accuracy (Bennett et al., 2007; Negash, 2003) was used to maintain performance at

92% accuracy. The feedback display included mean reaction time and accuracy scores

46

for a given block for younger adults, but only mean reaction time scores for older

adults. Depending on their mean accuracy for a given block, a statement also prompted

participants to “focus more on speed” (> 93% for younger and 90% for older adults) or

“focus more on accuracy” (< 91% for younger and 80% for older adults). Feedback

stated “speed and accuracy are about right” when mean accuracy scores were between

91-93% for younger adults and 80-90% for older adults.

Explicit awareness. Two tests of explicit awareness were used. First, a brief

post-experiment interview assessed explicit knowledge of the repeating sequence with

questions ranging from general (“Do you have anything to report about the task or

stimuli?”) to specific (“Did you notice a pattern, and if so, can you describe it to me?”).

Participants then completed a card sort task in which they sorted cards depicting all

possible combinations of three consecutive trials (three rows of four circles, with one

filled in on each row representing the target for that trial) into piles indicating whether

that triplet occurred more often or less often.

ASRT data analysis

Sequence learning is thought to result from individuals learning the relative

frequency of the smallest repeating units of a sequence rather than the alternating

pattern-random structure (Howard et al., 2004; Perruchet, Gallego, & Savy, 1990).

Thus, for ASRT sequences with second-order structure, participants learn that certain

three-element sequence chunks, or runs of three consecutive trials referred to as

triplets, occur more frequently that others. High frequency triplets are those in which

47

the first and third elements are part of the repeating pattern (e.g., 112, 122, 132, 142 for

the sequence 1r2r3r4r), whereas low frequency triplets occur with lower probability

because they do not fall on pattern trials (e.g., 113 and 124). Sequence learning will

therefore be assessed with triplet type effects, where performance on high and low

frequency triplets is compared for reaction time and accuracy measures. As in previous

research, certain low frequency triplets, repetitions (e.g., 111 and 222) and trills (e.g.,

121 and 232), will be omitted because responses to them are influenced by preexisting

response biases, rather than learning per se (Howard et al., 2004).

Diffusion data analysis

Pre-processing. Diffusion-weighted data were separately processed for each

participant using the University of Oxford’s Center for Functional Magnetic

Resonance Imaging of the Brain (FMRIB) Software Library (FSL) release 4.0

(http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). First, the two diffusion acquisitions were concatenated

in time to increase the signal-to-noise ratio. The first volume within this merged data

file that did not have gradient applied (i.e., the first b = 0 image) was used to generate a

binary brain mask with the Brain Extraction Tool. These data were then corrected for

head movement and eddy current distortions using Eddycorrect, which aligns all the

volumes. Finally, DTIfit was used to independently fit diffusion tensors to each voxel,

with the brain mask limiting the fitting of tensors to brain space. The output of DTIfit

yielded voxelwise maps of FA for each participant.

48

Tractography. Probabilistic fiber tracking was conducted separately in each

participant using FMRIB’s Diffusion Toolbox (FDT)(Behrens et al., 2003). First,

diffusion parameters were estimated at each voxel using BEDPOSTX, in which

Markov Chain Monte Carlo sampling generates a probability distribution function of

the principle diffusion direction. ProbtrackX was then used to estimate the distribution

of connections between seed and target regions (see Table 5). To generate this

connectivity distribution, 10,000 streamline samples were initiated from all voxels

within the seed regions, traveling along the probability distribution functions of local

voxels (steplength = 0.5 mm, curvature threshold = 0.2), until they terminated in voxels

within the target regions. The output contains a connectivity value for each voxel that

represents the number of streamline samples that passed through that voxel.

Three bilateral tracts of interest were generated using the seed and target

regions described in Table 5. These included caudate-DLPFC (dorsolateral prefrontal

cortex), hippocampus-DLPFC, and putamen-SMA (supplementary motor area). Seed

and target regions were masked in standard space (MNI152 2 mm3 standard brain),

which has been shown to be a highly reproducible way of generating tracts (Heiervang,

Behrens, Mackay, Robson, & Johansen-Berg, 2006), and a midline exclusion mask (at

x = 0) was used to restrict tracking to the left or right hemisphere. In order to conduct

tractography in diffusion space, Flirt was used to register the masks from standard

space to each individual’s diffusion space using a transformation with 6 degrees of

freedom. Resulting tracts were thresholded at 15% of the maximum connectivity value,

49

leaving only those voxels with a high likelihood of being connected to the seed

regions. To illustrate these tracts (see Figure 3), population maps were created by using

the reverse transformation to convert thresholded tracts from diffusion space into

standard space, and then overlaying tracts from each individual onto the standard

MNI152 2 mm3 brain.

Tract-based measures of integrity. Mean FA values were calculated for the

three bilateral tracts of interest for each participant. Each thresholded tract was

binarized and multiplied by the individual’s FA map, leaving just the FA values for

50

voxels within that tract, which were then averaged. All tracts were visually checked for

quality. Due to significant distortions (e.g., pixilated, non-continuous tracts; branching

to non-target regions) some individual’s tracts were excluded from analyses of the left

caudate-DLPFC (two younger, one older), right caudate-DLPFC (two older), left and

51

right hippocampus-DLPFC (one younger), and right putamen-SMA (one younger, two

older) tracts.

Results and Discussion

Neuropsychological profiles

The neuropsychological test results in Table 1 revealed the typical pattern of

age effects, with all participants performing within the age-expected range. That is,

older adults performed significantly worse than younger adults on measures of

processing speed, cued recall, and free recall, but not vocabulary or reading ability. All

participants had Mini-Mental State Examination scores (Folstein et al., 1975) that were

≥ 28.

ASRT performance

Sequence learning. For each participant, median reaction times for correct trials

and percentage of correct responses were calculated separately for each triplet type for

each block, and then averaged into nine, five-block epochs. Mean of median reaction

times and mean accuracy scores were analyzed separately in Age Group (Young, Old)

x Triplet Type (High Frequency, Low Frequency) x Epoch (1-9) repeated measures

ANOVAs, with Age Group as a between-subject variable, and Triplet Type and Epoch

as within-subject variables. These data are presented in Figure 4 (Overall

Performance).

For reaction time, skill learning was seen as a significant main effect of Epoch,

F (8, 208) = 33.8, p < .001, in which responses became faster with practice. Sequence

52

learning was revealed by significant Triplet Type, F (1, 26) = 121.0, p < .001, and

Triplet Type x Epoch effects, F (8, 208) = 6.0, p < .001, showing that participants

responded faster to High Frequency versus Low Frequency triplets, and this difference

increased across epochs. A significant main effect of Age Group, F (1, 26) = 62.7, p <

.001, and Age Group x Epoch interaction, F (1, 208) = 10.2, p < .001, indicated that

53

responses of older adults were slower and increased more with practice compared to

younger adults. However, in contrast to our predictions, there were no group

differences in sequence learning as seen by non-significant Age Group x Triplet Type

and Age Group x Triplet Type x Epoch interactions, p’s > .45.

Similar results were seen for accuracy. A significant main effect of Epoch, F (8,

208) = 9.4, p < .001, revealed skill learning as a decrease in accuracy with practice,

which is typical in sequence learning tasks as participants make increasingly more

errors on pattern-inconsistent trials as they learn the regularity. Sequence learning was

again observed as significant Triplet Type, F (1, 26) = 48.8, p < .001, and Triplet Type

x Epoch effects, F (8, 208) = 3.5, p < .001, with participants responding more

accurately to High Frequency versus Low Frequency triplets, and this difference

increasing across epochs. There were no significant effects with Age Group, p’s > .12,

indicating that there were no age group differences in sequence learning, and that the

differential feedback was successful in equating both age groups on overall accuracy.

Implicitness. Self-reports from the post-experiment interview did not reveal any

explicit awareness. Five younger adults and five older adults reported noticing a

regularity in the location of target presentations, but no individual was able to

accurately describe the nature of the regularity. Similarly, participants did not

recognize that High Frequency triplets occurred more often than Low Frequency

triplets. Mean recognition ratings were calculated for each participant as the proportion

of times each triplet type was reported as having occurred “frequently” during the

54

recognition task. An Age Group (Young, Old) x Triplet Type (High Frequency, Low

Frequency) mixed design ANOVA showed a significant main effect of Triplet Type, F

(1, 26) = 347.2, p < .001, with Low Frequency triplets (0.67 ± 0.05) being rated as

occurring more often than High Frequency triplets (0.33 ± 0.05) across age groups.

Taken together, these behavioral data revealed implicit sequence learning in

younger and older adults as faster and more accurate responses to high frequency

versus low frequency triplets, with no explicit awareness of this regularity in either age

group. In contrast to our predictions based on earlier work (Curran, 1997; Howard et

al., 2004; Howard & Howard, 1997), this analysis did not reveal significant age group

differences in sequence learning. One advantage of this unexpected finding is that any

age-related differences in white matter integrity correlates of implicit sequence

learning cannot be attributed to group differences in the magnitude of learning.

However, this also results in one limitation of the present study in that certain results,

such as finding that age group does not moderate FA-sequence learning relationships

(see below), may be limited to groups with equivalent learning.

Age group differences in white matter integrity

Separate between-group t-tests compared FA in younger and older adults for

each tract of interest. Significant age group differences were seen in the left, t (23) = -

2.82, p < .01, and right, t (24) = -3.71, p < .01, caudate-DLPFC tracts, with higher FA

in younger adults compared to older adults (left young, 0.31 ± 0.05; left old, 0.25 ±

55

0.06; right young, 0.32 ± 0.04; right old, 0.25 ± 0.04). FA did not differ across groups

in bilateral hippocampus-DLPFC or putamen-SMA tracts, p’s > .13.

The age-related decrease in caudate-DLPFC tract integrity is consistent with

previous DTI tractrogaphy studies that report age group difference in FA in frontal

white matter tracts (e.g., Davis et al., 2009; Sullivan et al., 2008). It also fits with other

known changes in the aging brain that primarily affect frontal and striatal regions. For

example, structural imaging studies show robust age-related shrinkage of prefrontal

and striatal gray matter, functional imaging studies find hyper- and hypo-activation of

frontal regions in aging, and neurochemical studies report significant age-related

disruption of neurotransmitter systems associated with the frontal-striatal network (see

Hedden & Gabrieli, 2005 for review). For the hippocampus-DLPFC tract, the lack of

an age group difference is in line with findings that MTL structures are relatively

unaffected in healthy aging (Head et al., 2005; Hedden & Gabrieli, 2005).

White matter integrity correlates of sequence learning

Correlations were performed between FA from the tracts of interest and triplet

type learning scores for reaction time and accuracy, calculated as the difference in

performance on high versus low frequency triplets (low minus high for reaction time,

high minus low for accuracy) summed across all epochs. To maximize power for

detecting these effects, all participants were included regardless of age group.

Significant effects are presented in Figure 5.

56

As expected, left caudate-DLPFC tract FA was positively correlated with triplet

type learning scores for reaction time, r = +0.49, p < .02, and accuracy, r = +0.44, p <

.03; and right hippocampus-DLPFC tract FA was significantly related to triplet type

learning scores for reaction time r = +0.41, p < .04. There was also a trend between the

accuracy learning score and left putamen-SMA tract FA, p < .09. Remaining effects

did not attain significance, p’s > .23.

57

Age group does not moderate FA-sequence learning relationships. Stepwise

regression analyses assess whether relationships between FA and ASRT sequence

learning were different in younger versus older adults. Separate analyses were

conducted using triplet type learning scores for reaction time and accuracy as outcome

variables. The predictor variable age group was entered into the model first, followed

by FA from the tracts of interest (left caudate-DLPFC and right hippocampus-DLPFC

for reaction time, left caudate-DLPFC for accuracy), and the interactions between age

group and FA.

Results revealed that only left caudate-DLPFC tract FA remained a significant

predictor of sequence learning for reaction time, F (1, 24) = 7.30, r = +0.49, p < .02,

and accuracy, F (1, 24) = 5.47, r = +0.49, p < .03. The interaction term was not a

significant predictor of sequence learning for either outcome measure, indicating that

age group did not moderate the FA-sequence learning relationships. That is, the

magnitude of these relationships was similar in younger and older adults.

Controlling for overall reaction time. The impact of response speed on FA-

sequence learning relationships was assessed using a measure of overall reaction time

in the ASRT (mean reaction time on all trials). Correlation analyses revealed that

overall reaction time was significantly related to sequence learning for the accuracy

measure, r = -.44, p < .02, and FA in the left, r = -.53, p < .01, and right, r = -.42, p <

.04, caudate-DLPFC tract. Relationships for reaction time and remaining tracts were

not significant, p’s > .11.

58

Follow-up multiple regression analyses showed that, when controlling for

overall reaction time, left caudate-DLPFC FA was no longer a significant predictor of

sequence learning for the accuracy measure, p > .30, though the overall model

remained significant, F (2, 24) = 5.14, p < .02, with overall reaction time as a

marginally significant predictor, r = -.42, p < .06. Importantly, FA in the left caudate-

DLPFC and right hippocampus-DLPFC tracts remained marginally significant

predictors of sequence learning for the reaction time measure after controlling for

overall reaction time, p’s < .06, indicating that these learning effects are independent of

overall speed.

Stages of sequence learning and relation to white matter integrity

Age group differences in later learning. Following from earlier behavioral

research indicating that age group differences in sequence learning are largest at later

stages (e.g., Howard et al., 2004), and the observation that younger adults appeared to

show more learning in the last few epochs on the accuracy measure compared to older

adults, the present study assess age group differences in the time course of learning by

separating sequence learning into three stages (early, middle, and late). Between-group

t-tests compared triplet type effects for younger and older adults at each learning stage.

For each participant, the difference in performance on high versus low frequency

triplets was calculated separately for each epoch, and then summed into three, three-

epoch time periods. These data are presented in Figure 4 (Difference Scores).

59

There were no significant age-related differences in sequence learning at any

stage for reaction time, p’s > .18, and the early and middle learning stages for

accuracy, p’s > .45. However, as expected, there was a significant age-related decrease

in late learning for accuracy, t (26) = -2.65, p < .02, indicating that younger adults

(0.17 ± 0.07) learned more than older adults (0.09 ± 0.09) later in practice. This age

group difference cannot be attributed to older adults being more cautious (i.e., making

fewer errors) with practice, because as Figure 4 reveals, younger and older adults have

nearly identical accuracy on the high frequency triples.

White matter integrity correlates of sequence learning stages. Previous

functional imaging research suggested that the hippocampus is engaged early in

learning whereas the caudate is engaged throughout learning (Poldrack & Packard,

2003; Schendan et al., 2003). To assess whether white matter integrity correlates of

sequence learning also vary with stages of learning, correlation analyses were

conducted between FA and triplet type learning effects for reaction time and accuracy

at each learning stage.

Results revealed that, for reaction time, learning in the middle stage was

positively correlated with FA in the left caudate-DLPFC, r = +0.44, p < .01, and right

hippocampus-DLPFC, r = +0.44, p < .03, tracts. For accuracy, the late learning stage

was significantly related to FA in the left caudate-DLPFC tract, r = +0.62, p < .01, and

marginally significant in the right caudate-DLPFC tract, r = +0.38, p < .06. These

findings are generally consistent with predictions that integrity of the hippocampus-

60

DLPFC tract would relate to early learning, integrity of the caudate-DLPFC tract

would relate to early and later stages of learning, and putamen-SMA tract FA would

not be related to sequence learning. Note that correlations with sequence learning in

the early stage may not have been detected because the magnitude of the learning

scores were significantly lower in the early versus middle and late stages for both

reaction time (32.8 versus 49.0 and 65.0) and accuracy (0.05 versus 0.09 and 0.13).

Age group differences in later learning are mediated by age group differences

in FA. The previously reported age-related decrease in the late learning stage for

accuracy, in combination with the age-related decrease in FA in the left and right

caudate-DLPFC tracts, suggest that age group differences in FA may mediate age

group differences in ASRT sequence learning. To test this, hierarchical regression

analyses were conducted separately for these two tracts in accordance with the

procedures laid out by Baron and Kenny (Baron & Kenny, 1986).

When age group was the sole predictor of the late learning score for accuracy,

21.3% of the variance in learning was age-related. When FA in the left caudate-

DLPFC tract was entered before age group, FA remained a significant predictor,

accounting for 41.2% of the variance, but age group no longer accounted for a

significant portion of the variance. To calculate the degree to which FA attenuated the

amount of variance in ASRT sequence learning that can be explained by age, the

amount of variance uniquely associated with age group (after partialling out the effect

of FA in the left caudate-DLPFC tract) was subtracted from the amount of variance

61

associated with age group as the sole predictor, and then divided by the amount of

variance associated with age group as the sole predictor (see Salthouse, 1991). This

procedure revealed that FA attenuated age-related variance in sequence learning by

93.0%.

The same analysis for FA in the right caudate-DLPFC tract revealed that

neither FA nor age group remained significant predictors of the later learning score for

accuracy when they were both entered into the model. Thus, FA in this tract was not a

significant mediator of the age group-sequence learning relationship.

General Discussion

The present study was the first to use DTI tractography to examine tract-based

white matter integrity correlates of implicit sequence learning, and to assess whether

these relationships were moderated by healthy aging. The main result from our primary

aim was that FA in the caudate-DLPFC and hippocampus-DLPFC tracts, but not the

putamen-SMA tract, was related to sequence learning in the ASRT. Importantly, the

relationships between FA and sequence learning were not moderated by age group in

that they did not differ for younger and older adults. For our secondary aim that

involved separating learning into three stages, results revealed significant age-related

decreases in late learning (for accuracy). In addition, FA in the hippocampus-DLPFC

tract was related to earlier learning (middle learning stage), and FA in the caudate-

DLPFC tract was related to earlier and later learning (middle and late learning stages).

62

Further analyses revealed that the age-related decrease in sequence learning was

mediated by the age-related decreases in caudate-DLPFC tract integrity.

Of central importance here was whether sequence learning in the ASRT was

related to white matter integrity from tracts connecting discrete gray matter regions

that were previously identified as being involved in the task. Using a different measure

of white matter integrity, one earlier study had found that one measure of implicit

learning (skill learning) was related to the degree of white matter hyperintensity (i.e.,

white matter lesion) burden in older adults (Aizenstein et al., 2002). The present study

expanded these findings by examining relationships between sequence-specific

learning and white matter integrity in younger and older adults, and using tract-based

measures of integrity that are more sensitive to specific white matter regions

(compared to periventricular and deep white matter).

Results revealed that sequence learning was significantly related to integrity of

the caudate-DLPFC and hippocampus-DLPFC tracts, with greater sequence learning

relating to higher integrity. These findings may be due to the role of the caudate and

hippocampus in forming learning-related associations (Curran, 1995), such as

associations among the locations of stimuli in frequently occurring triplets, or between

a stimulus location and the corresponding motor response. In support of this view,

hippocampus-DLPFC tract integrity was related to early learning, whereas caudate-

DLPFC integrity was related to learning at early and late stages, which is consistent

with evidence that the hippocampus is engaged in forming rapid associations early in

63

learning, whereas the caudate is involved in forming associations throughout learning

(Poldrack & Packard, 2003; Schendan et al., 2003). The marginally significant

relationship between sequence learning and FA in the putamen-SMA tract may reflect

this network being involved in response selection based on sequence-specific implicit

knowledge (Curran, 1995).

Importantly, the white matter integrity correlates of sequence learning did not

differ for younger and older adults. Finding that age group did not moderate these FA-

sequence learning relationships is consistent with one functional imaging study that

found similar gray matter regions activated during implicit sequence learning

performance in younger and older adults (Daselaar et al., 2003), though a comparable

study found different degrees of activation across age groups (Aizenstein et al., 2006).

Together, these data suggest that younger and older adults are calling on the same

neural networks during sequence learning, with this study providing the first evidence

that sequence learning depends on the integrity of fronto-striatal (caudate) and MTL

networks.

One of the most interesting findings from the present study was that age-related

decreases in FA in the left caudate-DLPFC tract mediated age-related decreases in the

ASRT late learning score for accuracy. In fact, integrity of this caudate-DLPFC tract

attenuated the amount of variance in sequence learning that could be attributed to age

group by 93%. This finding is consistent with the notion that age-related declines in

implicit sequence learning may be due to healthy brain aging of the underlying frontal

64

and striatal regions (see Hedden & Gabrieli, 2005), with the present study providing

the first evidence for the role of fronto-striatal white matter integrity mediating age-

related differences in implicit sequence learning. One explanation for this mediating

relationship is that age-related changes in white matter (e.g., axonal loss or degree of

myelination) may alter communication between distributed gray matter structures

within a neural network, for example by disrupting the timing of neural signals, which

in turn affects higher-level cognitive processes that depend on these communications

(e.g., Andrews-Hanna et al., 2007; Nordahl et al., 2006; Sullivan & Pfefferbaum,

2006).

Conclusions and future directions

In summary, the present results complement functional imaging and patient

studies of gray matter substrates involved in implicit sequence learning, finding that

the integrity of white matter tracts connecting these distributed regions (caudate-

DLPFC, hippocampus-DLPFC) is significantly related to sequence learning in the

ASRT. Furthermore, the present results provide evidence that neural substrates of

implicit sequence learning do not vary with healthy aging. Future research combining

DTI and functional imaging will be central to advancing our understanding of the

interaction between white matter integrity and gray matter activity within the neural

networks underlying implicit sequence learning. Such research would also improve

interpretations of the mediating role of white matter integrity, identifying whether age-

related declines in white matter integrity are associated with age-related differences in

65

functional activity, which in turn lead to decreased performance in older versus

younger adults.

66

CHAPTER IV: GENERAL DISCUSSION

Two studies were presented that characterized age-related differences in

multiple measures of white matter integrity (Chapters II and III), identified white

matter integrity correlates of implicit sequence learning in younger and healthy older

adults (Chapter III), and determined that age-related integrity differences mediated

age-related differences in implicit sequence learning (Chapter III). Together, these

findings can be interpreted in light of the white matter disconnection theory (e.g.,

Andrews-Hanna et al., 2007; Charlton et al., 2008; O'Sullivan et al., 2001). That is,

white matter integrity decreases were seen in healthy older adults compared to younger

adults. In turn, this compromised white matter may have disrupted the quality or timing

of communication between distributed cortical regions within the neural networks

involved in the ASRT (e.g., fronto-striatal network). As a result, performance was

affected as seen by significantly less sequence learning in the ASRT in later stages of

learning for older versus younger adults.

Future Directions

In contrast to the vast majority of cognitive neuroscience of aging research that

has focused on age-related differences in functional activity in discrete gray matter

regions during performance of explicit cognitive tasks, the present studies emphasized

age group differences in white matter tracts and their relation to implicit learning

performance. In addition to broadening the scope of cognitive neuroscience of aging

67

research, these data will stimulate future research that addresses key issues in the field,

and research aimed at fostering successful aging.

One prominent issue in cognitive neuroscience of aging research is qualifying

the relationship between brain structure and brain function, especially in the context of

age-related cognitive stability and decline. Functional imaging studies have revealed

both greater and less activity in older adults compared to younger adults across a

variety of tasks (Grady, 2008; Reuter-Lorenz, 2002). These under-activations are

thought to reflect inadequate recruitment of relevant brain regions, which are often

associated with age-related performance declines, whereas over-activations may reflect

compensatory activity (Cabeza, 2002; Reuter-Lorenz & Cappell, 2008) or non-

selective recruitment/inefficient processing (Logan, Sanders, Snyder, Morris, &

Buckner, 2002; Rypma et al., 2006), especially in the absence of age-related

differences in performance. The various functional activity-behavior relationships are

further thought to be mediated by age differences in underlying brain structure. For

example, one review proposed that certain age-related white matter changes (e.g.,

demyelination) may lead to mild disruptions of communication between brain regions

that may be compensated for by increased functional activity and thus improved

performance, whereas other changes (e.g., white matter hyperintensities, WMH) may

lead to more severe disruptions that would decrease activity and performance (Grady,

2008).

68

Future cognitive neuroscience of aging research should aim to integrate brain

function, structure, and cognitive performance to further our understanding of these

complex relationships. In the present study, such an undertaking would have been

invaluable for providing information about relationships between brain structure and

function in implicit learning. Follow-up projects may be able to determine whether the

age-related decrease in the integrity of the caudate-DLPFC tract is associated with an

age-related decrease (under-recruitment) or increase (non-selective recruitment) in

frontal activity. This may determine whether the sequence learning deficit in older

versus younger adults is due to inadequate recruitment of the same networks engaged

by younger adults (e.g., fronto-striatal and MTL networks examined here), or whether

older adults are also calling on additional brain regions (networks not examined here).

Of further interest is whether compensatory structure-function relationships would be

limited to frontal brain regions, which showed the largest age group differences in

integrity, or whether integrity differences that were seen throughout the brain (see

Chapter II) affect global patterns of brain activity.

Research aimed at promoting successful aging may also benefit from the

present studies. As mentioned earlier, implicit learning supports independent living by

enabling people to adapt to change, including new settings, people, and technologies.

Although implicit sequence learning declines are seen in healthy older adults in later

stages of learning, these implicit learning systems remain somewhat intact in healthy

and pathological aging. Thus, implicit learning tasks and their underlying neural

69

networks may be targeted by training programs aimed at maximizing successful aging

and recovery after trauma or stroke, which are frequent in old age (e.g., Camp, Bird, &

Cherry, 2000).

Limitations

Two main limitations of the present data were the relatively small sample size,

and the inclusion of high functioning older adults. Small sample sizes reduce the

ability to detect significant effects. This may explain why certain well-defined results,

such as age-related decreases in high-order sequence learning, were not readily

detected in the second study presented in Chapter III. It may also underlie the non-

significant interaction from the moderation analysis in Chapter III. That it, rather than

the null result reflecting that age group did not moderate the FA-learning relationships,

it may be that we did not have enough power to detect this effect. This limited power

issue may also be due to the high functioning older adults examined here, who showed

substantial overlap in their sequence learning scores with the younger group. These

older individuals may affect the generalizability of the present results because their

performance and underlying white matter may not be characteristic of “normal” or low

functioning older individuals.

Additional limitations stem from the fact that there is currently no “gold

standard” for interpreting diffusion data (Moseley, Bammer, & Illes, 2002) as the

implementation of DTI is still in its infancy, especially in cognitive neuroscience

research. Thus, responsibility falls on the researchers to carefully consider their data in

70

light of a few notable limitations. For instance, measures of white matter integrity are

considerably less accurate in certain brain regions, including areas with crossing fibers,

fiber degeneration, gray matter, and voxels with more than one tissue type (i.e., those

affected by partial volume)(Catani, 2006; Moseley et al., 2002). These inaccuracies

become especially important when making inferences about neural substrates

underlying differences in white matter integrity, as is commonly done with

examinations of axial and radial diffusivities (Wheeler-Kingshott & Cercignani, 2009).

In Chapter II, the location and organization of underlying fiber structure were taken

into account when patterns of age group differences were characterized based on these

diffusivity measures, which should have minimized the impact of this limitation to the

present results. Nonetheless, it will be necessary to confirm the proposed

interpretations with alternative techniques that are capable of identifying, accounting

for, or avoiding these error-prone regions (e.g., high-resolution DTI, multiple-fiber

DTI tractography).

Similarly, tractography analyses are subject to inaccuracies. For instance,

although highly reproducible seed and target masks were used here, the size and

placement of these masks may have resulted in erroneous tract reproductions, possibly

by over- or under-estimating a given tract of interest (Catani, 2006). There is also

uncertainty about the nature (anterograde versus retrograde) and functionality of

reconstructed tracts (Johansen-Berg & Behrens, 2006; Mori & Zhang, 2006; Powell et

al., 2004). These issues are especially relevant when the tracts are not major, well-

71

defined fiber pathways, as was the case for the tracts reconstructed in Chapter III.

Significant correlations between tract integrity and ASRT learning measures suggest

that the tracts examined here were at least partially accurate, and that they are

functionally relevant to implicit sequence learning. However, future research

replicating these results is necessary.

A broader limitation that should be addressed in future DTI aging research is

the impact of white matter hyperintensities (WMH) in relationships between white

matter integrity and cognition in healthy aging. As mentioned in the Discussion section

of Chapter II, WMH were not taken into account in this dataset because there were no

clinically significant structural abnormalities identified on the high resolution structural

scans. Nonetheless, WHM are present in healthy older adults, especially in frontal and

periventricular white matter (Nebes et al., 2006; Raz & Rodrigue, 2006), and their

prevalence increases with cerebrovascular risk factors commonly seen in healthy aging

(e.g., hypertension, silent strokes, and high blood pressure)(Gunning-Dixon & Raz,

2000; Longstrength et al., 1996). These regions, which are characterized by significant

white matter damage (e.g., myelin rarefaction, vessel endothelium breakdown,

microvascular disease)(Gunning-Dixon et al., 2009), may have affected the present

results, and should be acknowledged in future studies.

72

APPENDIX

Supplemental material from Chapter III

Following from the Chapter II findings regarding diffusivity measures of white

matter integrity, additional analyses assessed age-related differences in axial (AD) and

radial (RD) diffusivity in the tracts of interest from Chapter III, and related these

diffusivity measures to ASRT sequence learning scores. As in the earlier chapter, AD

and RD were used to make inferences about neural substrates underlying significant

differences in white matter integrity.

Age group differences in diffusivity measures. Separate between-group t-tests

compared AD and RD in younger and older adults for each tract of interest. For the

caudate-DLPFC tracts, significant age-related decreases in FA were accompanied by

age-related decreases in AD that were marginally significant for the left hemisphere, t

(23) = -2.01, p < .06, and significant for the right hemisphere, t (24) = -3.62, p < .01,

but no age group differences in RD. Based on earlier research (Budde et al., 2007;

Song et al., 2003), one explanation may be that age-related axonal differences (e.g.,

axonal damage or loss) contribute to this decline in white matter integrity. However,

given that the caudate-DLPFC tracts traverse long range anterior-posterior fibers that

pass through the internal and external capsules (e.g., anterior thalamic radiations,

superior longitudinal fasciculus), age group differences in macrostructural organization

may also be a contributing factor.

73

For the putamen-SMA tracts, younger and older adults did not differ on FA.

However, there were significant age-related increase in RD in the left hemisphere tract,

t (26) = 2.44, p < .03, and increases in both AD, t (23) = 2.28, p < .04, and RD, t (23) =

2.45, p < .03, in the right hemisphere tract. As proposed in Chapter II, this pattern

likely reflects age-related decreases in axonal packing density from demyelination,

axonal loss or damage, or inflammation in older versus younger adults, that are mild

enough to only increase RD in the left hemisphere, but more severe in the right

hemisphere, increasing both RD and AD (Bennett, Madden, Vaidya, Howard, &

Howard, 2009). Previous research has found evidence both for (Ardekani et al., 2007)

and against (Takao et al., 2009) asymmetry of age-related differences in white matter

integrity, though the neural underpinnings of these asymmetric effects warrants further

investigation.

Like analyses with FA, there were no significant age group differences in

diffusivity measures for the hippocampus-DLPFC tracts, p’s > .18. The lack of an age

group difference in the hippocampus-DLPFC tract suggests that not all tracts are

compromised in healthy aging.

Diffusivity correlates of sequence learning. Correlations were performed

between AD and RD from the tracts of interest and triplet type learning scores for

reaction time and accuracy across all epochs (overall scores) and for each of the three

stages of learning. The overall learning score for accuracy was significantly related to

AD in the left putamen-SMA tract, r = -0.38, p < .05; with the late learning score for

74

accuracy being related to AD in the left caudate-DLPFC tract, r = +0.40, p < .05, RD in

the right hippocampus-DLPFC, r = -0.44, p < .03, and RD in left putamen-SMA, r = -

0.52, p < .01.

The relationship between the late learning for accuracy and left caudate-

DLPFC tract AD was also seen with FA, suggesting that the correlation for FA may

have reflected individual differences in axonal integrity (e.g., homogeneous

orientation, axonal number or density). The remaining relationships did not overlap

with significant effects for FA, though axonal or myelin integrity may also underlie

these effects. It is worth noting that the pattern of findings seen here is not consistent

with earlier research. That is, half of the diffusivity-sequence learning relationships

were significant for RD, which does not fully support the notion that integrity-

cognition relationships are primarily due to individual differences in the degree of

myelination (Davis et al., 2009; Madden et al., 2009).

Taken together, these analyses revealed various patterns of age-related

differences in diffusivity measures across the three tracts of interest, suggesting that

different underlying neural substrates are responsible to for age-related declines in

white matter integrity. Furthermore, individual differences in a variety of neural

changes (e.g., both axonal and myelin-related) likely underlie individual differences in

cognitive functioning.

75

REFERENCES

Abe, O., Aoki, S., Hayashi, N., Yamada, H., Kunimatsu, A., Mori, H., Yoshikawa, T.,

Okubo, T., & Ohtomo, K. (2002). Normal aging in the central nervous system:

Quantitative MR diffusion-tensor analysis. Neurobiology of Aging, 23, 433-

441.

Aboitiz, F., Scheibel, A. B., Fisher, R. S., & Zaidel, E. (1992). Fiber composition of

the human corpus callosum. Brain Research, 598, 143-153.

Aizenstein, H. J., Butters, M. A., Clark, K. A., Figurski, J. L., Andrew Stenger, V.,

Nebes, R. D., Reynolds, C. F., 3rd, & Carter, C. S. (2006). Prefrontal and

striatal activation in elderly subjects during concurrent implicit and explicit

sequence learning. Neurobiology of Aging, 27, 741-751.

Aizenstein, H. J., Nebes, R. D., Meltzer, C. C., Fukui, M. B., Williams, R. L., Saxton,

J., Houck, P. R., Carter, C. S., Reynolds, C. F., 3rd, & DeKosky, S. T. (2002).

The relation of white matter hyperintensities to implicit learning in healthy

older adults. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 17, 664-669.

Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Snyder, A. Z., Vincent, J. L., Lustig, C., Head, D., Raichle, M.

E., & Buckner, R. L. (2007). Disruption of large-scale brain systems in

advanced aging. Neuron, 56, 924-935.

Ardekani, S., Kumar, A., Bartzokis, G., & Sinha, U. (2007). Exploratory voxel-based

analysis of diffusion indices and hemispheric asymmetry in normal aging.

Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 25, 154-167.

76

Baron, R. M., & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in

social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical

considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182.

Basser, P. J., Mattiello, J., & LeBihan, D. (1994). MR diffusion tensor spectroscopy

and imaging. Biophysical Journal, 66, 259-267.

Basser, P. J., & Pierpaoli, C. (1996). Microstructural and physiological features of

tissues elucidated by quantitative-diffusion-tensor MRI. Journal of Magnetic

Resonance, B 111, 209-219.

Beaulieu, C. (2002). The basis of anisotropic water diffusion in the nervous system - a

technical review. NMR in Biomedicine, 15, 435-455.

Behrens, T. E. J., Woolrich, M. W., Jenkinson, M., Johansen-Berg, H., Hunes, S.,

Clare, S., Matthews, P. M., Brady, J. M., & Smith, S. M. (2003).

Characterization and propagation of uncertainty in diffusion-weighted MR

imaging. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 50, 1077-1088.

Bennett, I. J., Madden, D. J., Vaidya, C. J., Howard, D. V., & Howard, J. H. Jr. (2009).

Age-related differences in multiple measures of white matter integrity: A

diffusion tensor imaging study of healthy aging. Human Brain Mapping.

Bennett, I. L., Howard, J. H. Jr., & Howard, D. V. (2007). Older adults reveal implicit

learning of subtle third-order sequential structure. The Journals of Gerontology.

Series B, Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 62, 98-103.

77

Berman, J. I., Mukherjee, P., Partridge, S. C., Miller, S. P., Ferriero, D. M., Barkovich,

A. J., Vigneron, D. B., & Henry, R. G. (2005). Quantitative diffusion tensor

MRI fiber tractography of sensorimotor white matter development in premature

infants. Neuroimage, 27, 862-871.

Bhagat, Y. A., & Beaulieu, C. (2004). Diffusion anisotropy in subcortical white matter

and cortical gray matter: Changes with aging and the role of CSF-suppression.

Journal of Magnetic Resonance Imaging, 20, 216-227.

Bialystok, E., & Craik, F. I. M. (Eds.). (2006). Lifespan Cognition: Mechanisms of

Change. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Bucur, B., Madden, D. J., Spaniol, J., Provenzale, J. M., Cabeza, R., White, L. E., &

Huettel, S. A. (2008). Age-related slowing of memory retrieval: Contributions

of perceptual speed and cerebral white matter integrity. Neurobiology of Aging,

29, 1070-1079.

Budde, M. D., Kim, J. H., Liang, H. F., Schmidt, R. E., Russell, J. H., Cross, A. H., &

Song, S. K. (2007). Toward accurate diagnosis of white matter pathology using

diffusion tensor imaging. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 57, 688-695.

Burzynska, A. Z., Preuschhof, C., Backman, L., Nyberg, L., Li, S. C., Lindenberger,

U., & Heekeren, H. R. (2009). Age-related differences in white-matter

microstructure: Region-specific patterns of diffusivity. Manuscript submitted

for publication.

78

Cabeza, R. (2001). Cognitive neuroscience of aging: Contributions of functional

neuroimaging. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 42, 277-286.

Cabeza, R. (2002). Hemispheric asymmetry reduction in older adults: The HAROLD

model. Psychology and Aging, 17, 85-100.

Cabeza, R., Nyberg, L., & Park, D. (Eds.). (2005). Cognitive neuroscience of aging:

Linking cognitive and cerebral aging. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Camp, C. J., Bird, M. J., & Cherry, K. E. (2000). Retrieval strategies as a rehabilitation

aid for cognitive loss in pathological aging. In R. D. Hill, L. Backman & A. S.

Neely (Eds.), Cognitive Rehabilitation in Old Age (pp. 224-248). New York:

Oxford University Press.

Catani, M. (2006). Diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging tractography in

cognitive disorders. Current Opinion in Neurology, 19, 599-606.

Charlton, R. A., Landau, S., Schiavone, F., Barrick, T. R., Clark, C. A., Markus, H. S.,

& Morris, R. G. (2008). A structural equation modeling investigation of age-

related variance in executive function and DTI measured white matter damage.

Neurobiology of Aging, 29, 1547-1555.

Craik, F. I. (2006). Brain-behavior relations across the lifespan: A commentary.

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 885-892.

Craik, F. I., & Jennings, J. M. (1992). Human memory. In F. I. M. Craik & T. A.

Salthouse (Eds.), The Handbook of Aging and Cognition (1st ed., pp. 51-110).

Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc.

79

Crone, E., & Huizinga, M. (2006). Preface special issue 'Advances in Developmental

Cognitive Neuroscience'. Neuropsychologia, 44, 2015-2016.

Curran, T. (1995). On the neural mechanisms of sequence learning. PSYCHE, 2.

Curran, T. (1997). Effects of aging on implicit sequence learning: Accounting for

sequence structure and explicit knowledge. Psychological Research, 60, 24-41.

Daselaar, S. M., Rombouts, S. A., Veltman, D. J., Raaijmakers, J. G., & Jonker, C.

(2003). Similar network activated by young and old adults during the

acquisition of a motor sequence. Neurobiology of Aging, 24, 1013-1019.

Davis, S. W., Dennis, N. A., Buchler, N. G., White, L. E., Madden, D. J., & Cabeza, R.

(2009). Assessing the effects of aging on long white matter tracts using

diffusion tensor tractography. Neuroimage, 46, 530-541.

Doyon, J., Bellec, P., Amsel, R., Penhune, V., Monchi, O., Carrier, J., Lehericy, S., &

Benali, H. (2009). Contributions of the basal ganglia and functionally related

brain structures to motor learning. Behavioral Brain Research, 199, 61-75.

Fjell, A. M., Westlye, L. T., Greve, D. N., Fischl, B., Benner, T., van der Kouwe, A. J.,

Salat, D., Bjornerud, A., Due-Tonnessen, P., & Walhovd, K. B. (2008). The

relationship between diffusion tensor imaging and volumetry as measures of

white matter properties. Neuroimage, 42, 1654-1668.

Folstein, M. F., Folstein, S. E., & McHugh, P. R. (1975). "Mini-mental state". A

practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician.

Journal of Psychiatric Research, 12, 189-198.

80

Frensch, P. A., & Miner, C. S. (1994). Effects of presentation rate and individual

differences in short-term memory capacity on an indirect measure of serial

learning. Memory and Cognition, 22, 95-110.

Giorgio, A., Watkins, K. E., Douaud, G., James, A. C., James, S., De Stefano, N.,

Matthews, P. M., Smith, S. M., & Johansen-Berg, H. (2008). Changes in white

matter microstructure during adolescence. Neuroimage, 39, 52-61.

Goulding, M. R., Rogers, M. E., & Smith, S. M. (2003). Trends in aging--United States

and worldwide. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 52, 101-106.

Grady, C. L. (2008). Cognitive neuroscience of aging. Annals of the New York

Academy of Sciences, 1124, 127-144.

Grieve, S. M., Williams, L. M., Paul, R. H., Clark, C. R., & Gordon, E. (2007).

Cognitive aging, executive function, and fractional anisotropy: A diffusion

tenor MR imaging study. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 28, 226-235.

Gunning-Dixon, F. M., Brickman, A. M., Cheng, J. C., & Alexopoulos, G. S. (2009).

Aging of cerebral white matter: A review of MRI findings. International

Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 24, 109-117.

Gunning-Dixon, F. M., & Raz, N. (2000). The cognitive correlates of white matter

abnormalities in normal aging: A quantitative review. Neuropsychology, 14,

224-232.

Head, D., Buckner, R. L., Shimony, J. S., Williams, L. E., Akbudak, E., Conturo, T. E.,

McAvoy, M., Morris, J. C., & Snyder, A. Z. (2004). Differential vulnerability

81

of anterior white matter in nondemented aging with minimal acceleration in

dementia of the Alzheimer type: Evidence from diffusion tensor imaging.

Cerebral Cortex, 14, 410-423.

Head, D., Snyder, A. Z., Girton, L. E., Morris, J. C., & Buckner, R. L. (2005). Frontal-

hippocampal double dissociation between normal aging and Alzheimer's

disease. Cerebral Cortex, 15, 732-739.

Hedden, T., & Gabrieli, J. D. (2005). Healthy and pathological processes in adult

development: New evidence from neuroimaging of the aging brain. Current

Opinion in Neurology, 18, 740-747.

Hedden, T., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2004). Insights into the ageing mind: A view from

cognitive neuroscience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 87-96.

Heiervang, E., Behrens, T. E., Mackay, C. E., Robson, M. D., & Johansen-Berg, H.

(2006). Between session reproducibility and between subject variability of

diffusion MR and tractography measures. Neuroimage, 33, 867-877.

Hirsch, J. G., Bock, M., Essig, M., & Schad, L. R. (1999). Comparison of diffusion

anisotropy measurements in combination with the flair-technique. Magnetic

Resonance in Medicine, 17, 705-716.

Howard, D. V., & Howard, J. H., Jr. (1989). Age differences in learning serial patterns:

Direct versus indirect measures. Psychology and Aging, 4, 357-364.

82

Howard, D. V., Howard, J. H., Jr., Japikse, K., DiYanni, C., Thompson, A., &

Somberg, R. (2004). Implicit sequence learning: Effects of level of structure,

adult age, and extended practice. Psychology and Aging, 19, 79-92.

Howard, J. H., Jr., & Howard, D. V. (1997). Age differences in implicit learning of

higher order dependencies in serial patterns. Psychology and Aging, 12, 634-

656.

Hsu, J. L., Leemans, A., Bai, C. H., Lee, C. H., Tsai, Y. F., Chiu, H. C., & Chen, W. H.

(2008). Gender differences and age-related white matter changes of the human

brain: a diffusion tensor imaging study. Neuroimage, 39, 566-577.

Hugenschmidt, C. E., Peiffer, A. M., Kraft, R. A., Casanova, R., Deibler, A. R.,

Burdette, J. H., Maldjian, J. A., & Laurienti, P. J. (2008). Relating imaging

indices of white matter integrity and volume in healthy older adults. Cerebral

Cortex, 18, 433-442.

Johansen-Berg, H., & Behrens, T. E. (2006). Just pretty pictures? What diffusion

tractography can add in clinical neuroscience. Current Opinion in Neurology,

19, 379-385.

Jones, D. K. (2008). Studying connections in the living human brain with diffusion

MRI. Cortex, 44, 936-952.

Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Early language acquisition: Cracking the speech code. Nature

Reviews Neuroscience, 5, 831-843.

83

Langley, L. K., & Madden, D. J. (2000). Functional neuroimaging of memory:

implications for cognitive aging. Microscopy Research and Technique, 51, 75-

84.

Le Bihan, D. (2003). Looking in the functional architecture of the brain with diffusion

MRI. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 4, 469-480.

Lieberman, M. D. (2000). Intuition: a social cognitive neuroscience approach.

Psychological Bulletin, 126, 109-137.

Logan, J. M., Sanders, A. L., Snyder, A. Z., Morris, J. C., & Buckner, R. L. (2002).

Under-recruitment and nonselective recruitment: Dissociable neural

mechanisms associated with aging. Neuron, 33, 827-840.

Longstrength, W. T., Jr., Manolio, T. A., Arnold, A., Burke, G. L., Bryan, N., Jungreis,

C. A., Enright, P. L., O'Leary, D., & Fried, L. (1996). Clinical correlates of

white matter findings on cranial magnetic resonance imaging of 3301 elderly

people. Stroke, 27, 1274-1282.

Madden, D. J., Spaniol, J., Costello, M. C., Bucur, B., White, L. E., Cabeza, R., Davis,

S. W., Dennis, N. A., Provenzale, J. M., & Huettel, S. A. (2009). Cerebral

white matter integrity mediates adult age differences in cognitive performance.

Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 21, 289-302.

Madden, D. J., Whiting, W. L., Huettel, S. A., White, L. E., MacFall, J. R., &

Provenzale, J. M. (2004). Diffusion tensor imaging of adult age differences in

cerebral white matter: Relation to response time. Neuroimage, 21, 1174-1181.

84

Marner, L., Nyengaard, J. R., Tang, Y., & Pakkenberg, B. (2003). Marked loss of

myelinated nerve fibers in the human brain with age. Journal of Comparative

Neurology, 462, 144-152.

Mori, S., & Zhang, J. (2006). Principles of diffusion tensor imaging and its

applications to basic neuroscience research. Neuron, 51, 527-539.

Moseley, M., Bammer, R., & Illes, J. (2002). Diffusion-tensor imaging of cognitive

performance. Brain and Cognition, 50, 396-413.

Muller, M. M., & Mayes, A. R. (2001). Cognitive neuroscience: Perception, attention,

and memory. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 25, 463-464.

Nair, G., Tanahashi, Y., Low, H. P., Billings-Gagliardi, S., Schwartz, W. J., & Duong,

T. Q. (2005). Myelination and long diffusion times alter diffusion-tensor-

imaging contrast in myelin-deficient shiverer mice. Neuroimage, 28, 165-174.

Nebes, R. D., Meltzer, C. C., Whyte, E. M., Scanlon, J. M., Halligan, E. M., Saxon, J.

A., Houck, P. R., Boada, F. E., & DeKosky, S. T. (2006). The relation of white

matter hyperintensities to cognitive performance in the normal old: Education

matters. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 13, 326-340.

Negash, S. (2003). Adult age differences in implicit learning of short and higher-order

sequential patterns. Washington, DC: Georgetown University.

Nissen, M. J., & Bullemer, P. (1987). Attentional requirements of learning: Evidence

from performance measures. Cognitive Psychology, 19, 1-32.

85

Nordahl, C. W., Ranganath, C., Yonelinas, A. P., Decarli, C., Fletcher, E., & Jagust,

W. J. (2006). White matter changes compromise prefrontal cortex function in

healthy elderly individuals. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 18, 418-429.

O'Sullivan, M., Jones, D. K., Summers, P. E., Morris, R. G., Williams, S. C., &

Markus, H. S. (2001). Evidence for cortical ''disconnection'' as a mechanism of

age-related cognitive decline. Neurology, 57, 632-638.

Papadakis, N. G., Martin, K. M., Mustafa, M. H., Wilkinson, I. D., Griffiths, P. D.,

Huang, C. L., & Woodruff, P. W. (2002). Study of the effect of CSF

suppression on white matter diffusion anisotropy mapping of healthy human

brain. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 48, 394-398.

Perruchet, P., Gallego, J., & Savy, I. (1990). A critical reappraisal of the evidence for

unconscious abstraction of deterministic rules in complex experiment

situations. Cognitive Psychology, 22, 493-516.

Perry, M. E., McDonald, C. R., Hagler, D. J., Jr., Gharapetian, L., Kuperman, J. M.,

Koyama, A. K., Dale, A. M., & McEvoy, L. K. (2009). White matter tracts

associated with set-shifting in healthy aging. Neuropsychologia.

Peters, A. (2002). The effects of normal aging on myelin and nerve fibers: A review.

Journal of Neurocytology, 31, 581-593.

Pfefferbaum, A., Adalsteinsson, E., & Sullivan, E. V. (2005). Frontal circuitry

degradation marks healthy adult aging: Evidence from diffusion tensor

imaging. Neuroimage, 16, 891-899.

86

Pfefferbaum, A., & Sullivan, E. V. (2003). Increased brain white matter diffusivity in

normal adult aging: Relationship to anisotropy and partial voluming. Magnetic

Resonance in Medicine, 49, 953-961.

Pfefferbaum, A., Sullivan, E. V., Hedehus, M., Lim, K. O., Adalsteinsson, E., &

Moseley, M. (2000). Age-related decline in brain white matter anisotropy

measured with spatially corrected echo-planar diffusion tensor imaging.

Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 44, 259-268.

Pierpaoli, C., Barnett, A., Pajevic, S., Chen, R., Penix, L. R., Virta, A., & Basser, P.

(2001). Water diffusion changes in Wallerian degeneration and their

dependence on white matter architecture. Neuroimage, 13, 1174-1185.

Pierpaoli, C., & Basser, P. J. (1996). Toward a quantitative assessment of diffusion

anisotropy. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 36, 893-906.

Pierpaoli, C., Jezzard, P., Basser, P. J., Barnett, A., & Di Chiro, G. (1996). Diffusion

tensor MR imaging of the human brain. Radiology, 201, 637-648.

Poldrack, R. A., & Packard, M. G. (2003). Competition among multiple memory

systems: Converging evidence from animal and human brain studies.

Neuropsychologia, 41, 245-251.

Powell, H. W., Guye, M., Parker, G. J., Symms, M. R., Boulby, P., Koepp, M. J.,

Barker, G. J., & Duncan, J. S. (2004). Noninvasive in vivo demonstration of the

connections of the human parahippocampal gyrus. Neuroimage, 22, 740-747.

87

Raz, N. (2000). Aging of the brain and its impact on cognitive performance:

Integration of structural and functional findings. In F. I. M. Craik & T. A.

Salthouse (Eds.), The Handbook of Aging and Cognition (2nd ed., pp. 1-90).

Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Assoc.

Raz, N. (2005). The aging brain observed in vivo: Differential changes and their

modifiers. In R. Cabeza, L. Nyberg & D. Park (Eds.), Cognitive Neuroscience

of Aging: Linking Cognitive and Cerebral Aging (pp. 19-57). New York, NY:

Oxford University Press.

Raz, N., Gunning-Dixon, F. M., Head, D., Dupuis, J. H., & Acker, J. D. (1998).

Neuroanatomical correlates of cognitive aging: Evidence from structural

magnetic resonance imaging. Neuropsychology, 12, 95-114.

Raz, N., Lindenberger, U., Rodrigue, K. M., Kennedy, K. M., Head, D., Williamson,

A., Dahle, C., Gerstorf, D., & Sacker, J. D. (2005). Regional brain changes in

aging healthy adults: General trends, individual differences, and modifiers.

Cerebral Cortex, 15, 1676-1689.

Raz, N., & Rodrigue, K. M. (2006). Differential aging of the brain: Patterns, cognitive

correlates and modifiers. Neuroscience and Behavioral Reviews, 30, 730-748.

Reber, A. S. (1993). Implicit learning and tacit knowledge: An essay on the cognitive

unconscious. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

Reuter-Lorenz, P. A. (2002). New visions of the aging mind and brain. TRENDS in

Cognitive Sciences, 6, 394-400.

88

Reuter-Lorenz, P. A., & Cappell, K. A. (2008). Neurocognitive aging and the

compensation hypothesis. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17,

177-182.

Rose, M., Haider, H., Weiller, C., & Buchel, C. (2002). The role of medial temporal

lobe structures in implicit learning: An event-related FMRI study. Neuron, 36,

1221-1231.

Rypma, B., Berger, J. S., Prabhakaran, V., Bly, B. M., Kimberg, D. Y., Biswal, B. B.,

& D'Esposito, M. (2006). Neural correlates of cognitive efficiency.

Neuroimage, 33, 969-979.

Salat, D. H., Buckner, R. L., Snyder, A. Z., Greve, D. N., Desikan, R. S., Busa, E.,

Morris, J. C., Dale, A. M., & Fischl, B. (2004). Thinning of the cerebral cortex

in aging. Cerebral Cortex, 14, 721-730.

Salat, D. H., Tuch, D. S., Greve, D. N., van der Kouwe, A. J., Hevelone, N. D., Zaleta,

A. K., Rosen, B. R., Fischl, B., Corkin, S., Rosas, H. D., & Dale, A. M. (2005).

Age-related alterations in white matter microstructure measured by diffusion

tensor imaging. Neurobiology of Aging, 26, 1215-1227.

Salthouse, T. A. (1991). Mediation of adult age differences in cognition by reductions

in working memory and speed of processing. Psychological Science, 2, 179-

183.

89

Schendan, H. E., Searl, M. M., Melrose, R. J., & Stern, C. E. (2003). An fMRI study of

the role of the medial temporal lobe in implicit and explicit sequence learning.

Neuron, 37, 1013-1025.

Seger, C. A. (1994). Implicit learning. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 163-196.

Seger, C. A. (2006). The basal ganglia in human learning. Neuroscientist, 12, 285-290.

Shenkin, S. D., Bastin, M. E., MacGillivray, T. J., Deary, I. J., Starr, J. M., &

Wardlaw, J. M. (2003). Childhood and current cognitive function in healthy 80-

year-olds: A DT-MRI study. Neuroreport, 14, 345-349.

Smith, S. M., Jenkinson, M., Johansen-Berg, H., Rueckert, D., Nichols, T. E., Mackay,

C. E., Watkins, K. E., Ciccarelli, O., Cader, M. Z., Matthews, P. M., &

Behrens, T. E. J. (2006). Tract-based spatial statistics: Voxelwise analysis of

multi-subject diffusion data. Neuroimage, 31, 1487-1505.

Smith, S. M., Johansen-Berg, H., Jenkinson, M., Rueckert, D., Nichols, T. E., Miller,

K. L., Robson, M. D., Jones, D. K., Klein, J. C., Bartsch, A. J., & Behrens, T.

E. (2007). Acquisition and voxelwise analysis of multi-subject diffusion data

with tract-based spatial statistics. Nature Protocols, 2, 499-503.

Song, S. K., Sun, S. W., Ju, W. K., Lin, S. J., Cross, A. H., & Neufeld, A. H. (2003).

Diffusion tensor imaging detects and differentiates axon and myelin

degeneration in mouse optic nerve after retinal ischemia. Neuroimage, 20,

1714-1722.

90

Song, S. K., Sun, S. W., Ramsbottom, M. J., Chang, C., Russell, J., & Cross, A. H.

(2002). Dysmyelination revealed through MRI as increased radial (but

unchanged axial) diffusion of water. Neuroimage, 17, 1429-1436.

Song, S. K., Yoshino, J., Le, T. Q., Lin, S. J., Sun, S. W., Cross, A. H., & Armstrong,

R. C. (2005). Demyelination increases radial diffusivity in corpus callosum of

mouse brain. Neuroimage, 26, 132-140.

Stadlbauer, A., Salomonowitz, E., Strunk, G., Hammen, T., & Ganslandt, O. (2008).

Age-related degradation in the central nervous system: Assessment with

diffusion-tensor imaging and quantitative fiber tracking. Radiology, 247, 179-

188.

Sullivan, E. V., Adalsteinsson, E., & Pfefferbaum, A. (2006). Selective age-related

degradation of anterior callosal fiber bundles quantified in vivo with fiber

tracking. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 1030-1039.

Sullivan, E. V., & Pfefferbaum, A. (2003). Diffusion tensor imaging in normal aging

and neuropsychiatric disorders. European Journal of Radiology, 45, 244-255.

Sullivan, E. V., & Pfefferbaum, A. (2006). Diffusion tensor imaging and aging.

Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 30, 749-761.

Sullivan, E. V., Rohlfing, T., & Pfefferbaum, A. (2008). Quantitative fiber tracking of

lateral and interhemispheric white matter systems in normal aging: Relations to

timed performance. Neurobiology of Aging.

91

Sundgren, P. C., Dong, Q., Gomez-Hassan, D., Mukherji, S. K., Maly, P., & Welsh, R.

(2004). Diffusion tensor imaging of the brain: Review of clinical applications.

Neuroradiology, 46, 339-350.

Takao, H., Abe, O., Yamasue, H., Aoki, S., Kasai, K., Sasaki, H., & Ohtomo, K.

(2009). Aging effects on cerebral asymmetry: A vovel-based morphometry and

diffusion tensor imaging study. Magnetic Resonance Imaging.

Tang, Y., Nyengaard, J. R., Pakkenberg, B., & Gundersen, H. J. (1997). Age-induced

white matter changes in the human brain: A stereological investigation.

Neurobiology of Aging, 18, 609-615.

Thomalla, G., Glauche, V., Koch, M. A., Beaulieu, C., Weiller, C., & Rother, J.

(2004). Diffusion tensor imaging detects early Wallerian degeneration of the

pyramidal tract after ischemic stroke. Neuroimage, 22, 1767-1774.

Vernooij, M. W., de Groot, M., van der Lugt, A., Ikram, M. A., Krestin, G. P.,

Hofman, A., Niessen, W. J., & Breteler, M. M. (2008). White matter atrophy

and lesion formation explain the loss of structural integrity of white matter in

aging. Neuroimage, 43, 470-477.

West, R. L. (1996). An application of prefrontal cortex function theory to cognitive

aging. Psychological Bulletin, 120, 272-292.

Wheeler-Kingshott, C. A. M., & Cercignani, M. (2009). About "axial" and "radial"

diffusivities. Magnetic Resonance in Medicine, 61, 1255-1260.

92

Yoon, B., Shim, Y. S., Lee, K. S., Shon, Y. M., & Yang, D. W. (2008). Region-

specific changes of cerebral white matter during normal aging: A diffusion-

tensor analysis. Archives of Gerontology and Geriatrics, 47, 129-138.

Zahr, N. M., Rohlfing, T., Pfefferbaum, A., & Sullivan, E. V. (2009). Problem solving,

working memory, and motor correlates of association and commissural fiber

bundles in normal aging: A quantitative fiber tracking study. Neuroimage, 44,

1050-1062.

Zhang, Y., Du, A. T., Hayasaka, S., Jahng, G. H., Hlavin, J., Zhan, W., Weiner, M. W.,

& Schuff, N. (2008). Patterns of age-related water diffusion changes in human

brain by concordance and discordance analysis. Neurobiology of Aging.

Zhou, M., Goto, N., Otsuka, N., Moriyama, H., & Nakamura, Y. (1997). Morphometric

analyses of axons in the lateral corticospinal tract with ageing process.

Okajimas Folia Anatomica Japonica, 74, 133-138.