a psychosocial perspective on pediatric functional ... psychosocial perspective on pediatric...

205
UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (http://dare.uva.nl) UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository) A psychosocial perspective on pediatric functional abdominal pain: risk factors and treatment van der Veek, S.M.C. Link to publication Citation for published version (APA): van der Veek, S. M. C. (2012). A psychosocial perspective on pediatric functional abdominal pain: risk factors and treatment. General rights It is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s), other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons). Disclaimer/Complaints regulations If you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, stating your reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Ask the Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam, The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible. Download date: 18 Apr 2020

Upload: others

Post on 17-Apr-2020

11 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

UvA-DARE is a service provided by the library of the University of Amsterdam (http://dare.uva.nl)

UvA-DARE (Digital Academic Repository)

A psychosocial perspective on pediatric functional abdominal pain: risk factors and treatment

van der Veek, S.M.C.

Link to publication

Citation for published version (APA):van der Veek, S. M. C. (2012). A psychosocial perspective on pediatric functional abdominal pain: risk factorsand treatment.

General rightsIt is not permitted to download or to forward/distribute the text or part of it without the consent of the author(s) and/or copyright holder(s),other than for strictly personal, individual use, unless the work is under an open content license (like Creative Commons).

Disclaimer/Complaints regulationsIf you believe that digital publication of certain material infringes any of your rights or (privacy) interests, please let the Library know, statingyour reasons. In case of a legitimate complaint, the Library will make the material inaccessible and/or remove it from the website. Please Askthe Library: https://uba.uva.nl/en/contact, or a letter to: Library of the University of Amsterdam, Secretariat, Singel 425, 1012 WP Amsterdam,The Netherlands. You will be contacted as soon as possible.

Download date: 18 Apr 2020

Page 2: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

A Psychosocial Persp

ective on Pediatric Functional Ab

dominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatm

ent Shelley M. C

. van der Veek

A Psychosocial Perspective onPediatric

Functional Abdominal Pain:

Shelley M. C. van der Veek

Risk Factors and Treatment

22864 Veek_Omslag 18-07-12 16:04 Pagina 1

Page 3: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,
Page 4: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

A Psychosocial Perspective on

Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain:

Risk Factors and Treatment

Shelley M. C. van der Veek

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 1 18-07-12 17:19

Page 5: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

Shelley Maria Cornelia van der VeekA Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and TreatmentPhD thesis, Academic Medical Center, University of Amsterdam

The research described in this thesis was performed at the Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry of the Academic Medical Center, Amsterdam, and at De Bascule, academic center for child- and adolescent psychiatry. The contribution of the department of pediatrics and the department of pediatric gastroenterology of the Emma Children’s Hospital AMC is gratefully acknowledged.

This thesis was financially supported by the Dutch Digestive Foundation (Maag Lever Darm Stichting), grant number SWO 05-09. Financial support for printing of this thesis was kindly provided by De Bascule, academic center for child- and adolescent psychiatry, the Dutch Digestive Foundation, and the Nederlandse Vereniging voor Gastroenterologie.

Lay-out: Ferdinand van Nispen, Citroenvlinder-dtp.nl, Bilthoven, The Netherlands Cover: Shelley van der VeekPrinted by: GVO drukkers & vormgevers B.V. | Ponsen & Looijen, Ede, The Netherlands

ISBN 978-90-6464-577-8© 2012, S.M.C. van der Veek. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced in any form or by any means without prior permission from the author.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 2 18-07-12 17:19

Page 6: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

A Psychosocial Perspective on

Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain:

Risk Factors and Treatment

ACADEMISCH PROEFSCHRIFT

ter verkrijging van de graad van doctoraan de Universiteit van Amsterdamop gezag van de Rector Magnificus

prof. dr. D.C. van den Boomten overstaan van een door het college voor promoties ingestelde

commissie, in het openbaar te verdedigen in de Agnietenkapelop vrijdag 21 september 2012, te 14.00 uur

door

Shelley Maria Cornelia van der Veek

geboren te Lisse

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 3 18-07-12 17:19

Page 7: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

Promotiecommissie

Promotores: Prof. dr. F. Boer Prof. dr. E. de Haan

Co-promotores: Dr. H.H.F. Derkx Prof. dr. M.A. Benninga Dr. R.J.L. Lindauer

Overige leden: Prof. dr. H.S.A. Heymans Prof. dr. H.C.P.M. van Weert Prof. dr. P. Spinhoven Prof. dr. C.M.J.G. Maes Dr. Y.R. van Rood Dr. A.M. Vlieger

Faculteit der Geneeskunde

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 4 18-07-12 17:19

Page 8: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

ContentsPage

Chapter 1. General introduction. Functional abdominal pain: An integrated view from medical and psychological literature 7

Part I. Psychosocial risk factors for pediatric functional abdominal pain

Chapter 2 Abdominal pain in Dutch schoolchildren: Relationships with co-morbid complaints in children and their parents

27

Chapter 3 Do parents maintain or exacerbate pediatric functional abdominal pain? A systematic review and meta-analysis

43

Chapter 4 Emotion awareness and coping in children with functional abdominal pain: A controlled study

61

Chapter 5 The relative contribution of parental and children’s cognitive and behavioral reactions to pediatric functional abdominal pain

79

Chapter 6 Attentional bias to activity of different parts of the body in children with functional abdominal pain 97

Part II. Cognitive behavior therapy for pediatric functional abdominal pain

Chapter 7 Cognitive behavior therapy versus medical care for the treatment of pediatric functional abdominal pain: A randomized controlled trial

119

Chapter 8 Exploring moderating and mediating factors of cognitive behavior therapy for the treatment of pediatric functional abdominal pain

139

Chapter 9 General discussion and summary 159

Chapter 10 Nederlandse samenvatting 173

Dankwoord 195

References 181

Curriculum Vitae 195

Publications 200

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 5 18-07-12 17:19

Page 9: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 6 18-07-12 17:19

Page 10: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

Ch

apte

r

1 General introduction.

Functional abdominal pain: An integrated view

from medical and psychological literature

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 7 18-07-12 17:19

Page 11: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

8 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Tuesday, September 8th, 2009, 11.00 am, de Bascule, Amsterdam.

Downstairs in the waiting room, Elin and her parents are waiting for their

first visit to the psychologist. Elin is holding her hands to her stomach,

hoping that no one will hear the sound of her bowels growling. She’ll have

to return to school this afternoon, and she has gym-class. Surely, the

pain will get worse again when she has to exert herself during softball, or

worse, soccer. I hope the psychologist will give me an excuse note, Elin

thinks, so I won’t have to go. But do psychologists even write excuse

notes? She still feels it’s strange that she has to go to a psychologist.

The pain is real, it’s not like she’s making it up. And the fact that the

doctor can’t find a cause for her pain, doesn’t mean it isn’t there! As these

thoughts cross her mind, a sharp stab of pain courses through her abdomen.

This is the third one this morning. Elin tries not to react to the searing

pain, but her face twitches before she can stop it. Her mother sees her

face, and asks her if she’s all right. Elin bends over and hopes that the

pain will pass soon. What do I care that I have to go to a psychologist,

Elin thinks, as long as it helps.

I can’t take this any longer.

More than fifty years ago, Apley and Naish described recurrent abdominal pain (AP) as three or more episodes of AP, severe enough to affect daily activities, occurring over a period of at least 3 months (Apley & Naish, 1958). In the pre-endoscopy era, organic pathology could be identified in less than 10% of children, which led Apley to conclude that recurrent AP had a psychological origin. Due to advances in diagnostic procedures, organic abnormalities are now found more often. Precise numbers are hard to obtain but vary around 30%, depending on the setting in which the investigation was performed and the inclusion criteria used (Alfvén, 2003; Croffie, Fitzgerald, & Chong, 2000; El-Matary, Spray, & Sandhu, 2004; Gijsbers, Kneepkens, Schweizer, Benninga, & Buller, 2011; Kokkonen, Haapalahti, Tikkanen, Karttunen, & Savilahti, 2004; Mulvaney, Lambert, Garber, & Walker, 2006). However, these organic abnormalities do not always provide a cause for the complaints; e.g., the parasite Blastocystis hominis is a frequently found parasite in human feces, but its role in intestinal diseases is controversial (Stark, Van Hal, Marriott, Ellis, & Harkness, 2007). In fact, if ‘red flags’ such as unexplained fever or persistent vomiting (see table 2) are absent, an organic cause for the AP is seldom found (Boyle, 1997; Di Lorenzo et al., 2005; Hyams et al., 1995; Hyams et al., 2000). When no organic cause can be found to explain the pain, as in the case of Elin, the pain is considered ‘functional’. The etiology of functional abdominal pain (FAP) is still largely unknown. Different

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 8 18-07-12 17:19

Page 12: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

1

General introduction | 9

biological, psychological and social factors are hypothesized to be related to the complaints. The present dissertation takes on a psychosocial perspective on pediatric FAP, with the aim to investigate whether psychosocial factors may be either risk factors for the complaints, or factors that maintain or exacerbate FAP, and to study whether a psychological intervention based on these factors is effective. To give a complete picture of FAP, this chapter will provide an overview of its epidemiological and clinical characteristics, the biological, psychological and social factors that may contribute to its etiology, and the treatment options that exist for FAP. The chapter concludes with an outline of the present thesis.

Characteristics of FAP

EpidemiologyFAP is one of the most common gastrointestinal disorders in children, affecting between .3 to 19% of children and adolescents in Western countries (Chitkara, Rawat, & Talley, 2005; King et al., 2011) and accounting for 2% to 4% of pediatric office visits (Starfield et al., 1980). In the Netherlands, abdominal pain is ranked in the top five of reasons to visit a general practitioner (van Suijlekom-Smit et al., 1995; Wijga, Scholtens, Van Oeffelen, & Beckers, 2010). Additionally, it is one of the most prevalent chronic pain types for Dutch children (Perquin et al., 2000; Wijga et al., 2010). FAP shows a relationship with age, with highest prevalences for children aged 4-6 and in early adolescence (Chitkara et al., 2005; Wijga et al., 2010). There is also a strong correlation with gender, as from 8 years on, girls report FAP more often than boys with a ratio of about 1.5 to 1 (Chitkara et al., 2005).

Classification As mentioned above, recurrent AP in childhood was first defined by Apley and Naish in 1958. Although their definition originated as a selection criterion for research purposes, the term recurrent AP was more and more used in the literature as though it was a diagnostic entity (von Baeyer & Walker, 1999). Because of the large heterogeneity in complaints seen in functional recurrent AP, it was necessary to design better symptom-based diagnostic criteria for the different functional gastrointestinal disorders (FGIDs), as existed in adults. These diagnostic criteria, known as the Pediatric Rome III Criteria, provide a tool for making a positive diagnosis in the majority of children by simply taking a history and completing a physical examination revealing no evidence of an inflammatory, anatomic, metabolic, or neoplastic process that explains the child’s symptoms. It introduces a detailed classification of functional abdominal disorders, in part based on the existing classification of functional gastrointestinal disorders in adults. In the Rome III criteria FAP is divided in 5 groups: functional dyspepsia, irritable bowel syndrome, abdominal migraine, functional abdominal pain, and functional abdominal pain syndrome, see also table 1 (Rasquin et al., 2006).

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 9 18-07-12 17:19

Page 13: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

10 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

With the new Rome III criteria, the Rome Committee updated and revised the Rome II criteria first introduced in 1999 (Rasquin-Weber et al., 1999) according to the most relevant criticism. Although some objections to the Rome criteria were solved in this new version (Baber, Anderson, Puzanovova, & Walker, 2008), others were not. For example, extra-abdominal symptoms like headache, fatigue and sleep disturbance are not included in the Rome definition, despite the fact that they are observed frequently in the FGIDs and may be essential for the severity and experienced burden of the disorder (Alpers, 2008). Moreover, patients often fulfill criteria for more than one specific diagnosis, making it unclear whether some of the diagnoses represent distinct disorders or artificial categories (Helgeland et al., 2009). Finally, the Rome criteria cannot yet be used to inform the general practitioner or pediatrician which treatment to use for what type of diagnoses, thus providing little evidence for clinical relevance of the criteria (Berger, Gieteling, & Benninga, 2007). As such, it is at present difficult to determine the utility of the criteria in making clinically useful distinctions between groups of patients.

Diagnostic toolsWhen confronted with a patient for whom with routine tests no abnormalities are found, physicians might be tempted to do as many tests as possible to find an organic cause. However, excessive health-care consumerism has been associated with worse outcome (Lindley, Glaser, & Milla, 2005), urging practitioners to be careful in how many diagnostic tests he/she sets to work in the exclusion of organic disease. A review by the North American Society for Pediatric Gastroenterology, Hepathology and Nutrition (NASPGHAN) concluded that inquiring after the presence of the ‘red flag’ symptoms mentioned in table 2 is useful, as they are associated with a higher prevalence of organic disease and may justify the performance of diagnostic tests (Di Lorenzo et al., 2005). However, no studies have found convincing evidence for the usefulness of performing common laboratory tests like urine and stool analysis when red flag symptoms are absent, nor for the diagnostic yield of other techniques like ultrasonographic examination, endoscopy and biopsy (Di Lorenzo et al., 2005). A recent Dutch study aimed to investigate in how many children with RAP an organic abnormality could be found if they were subjected to an extensive standardized protocol for diagnostic work-up, consisting of tests for gastrointestinal infections, celiac disease, food allergy and carbohydrate intolerance (Gijsbers et al., 2011). This study showed that the clinical usefulness for many tests was low; for example, endoscopically confirmed Helicobacter pylori infection was demonstrated in 22 patients, but after successful eradication of the Hp infection in all patients, only five (22.7%) became pain free. Also, while one or more signs for food allergies were present in 123 children, only five of them (4%) were actually diagnosed with a food allergy following a positive double-blinded placebo-controlled food challenge (Gijsbers et al., 2011). A study in the United States reached the same conclusion, stressing that “In children with pain-predominant FGIDs,

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 10 18-07-12 17:19

Page 14: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

1

General introduction | 11

investigations are common, costs are substantial, and yield is minimal” (Dhroove, Chogle, & Saps, 2010, pp. 579). Diagnostic yield of esophagogastroduodenoscopy seems a bit larger with a diagnosis found in 38% of cases (Thakkar et al., 2009). In some cases, doctors may be tempted to employ an endoscopy as a way to reassure the patient and thereby reducing symptoms. However, a recent study found no evidence for such a reassuring effect, as children who received an endoscopy with a negative result reported just as much AP as children who did not receive an endoscopy 12 to 18 months later (Bonilla, Deli, & Saps, 2011). Thus, using the NASPGHAN recommendations to check for ‘red flags’ and being conservative in the number of additional tests employed, seems like the best strategy for excluding organic causes for recurrent AP.

Table 1. Rome III Criteria (derived from Rasquin et al., 2006)

Childhood Functional Abdominal Pain Must include all of the following:1. Episodic or continuous abdominal pain;2. Insufficient criteria for other FGIDs3. No evidence of an inflammatory, anatomic, metabolic, or neoplastic process that explains the

subject’s symptoms.Criteria should be fulfilled at least once per week for at least 2 months before diagnosis.

Childhood Functional Abdominal Pain SyndromeMust include childhood functional abdominal pain at least 25% of the time and 1 or more of the following:1. Some loss of daily functioning2. Additional somatic symptoms such as headache, limb pain, or difficulty sleeping.Criteria should be fulfilled at least one per week for at least 2 months before diagnosis.

Irritable Bowel Syndrome (IBS)Must include all of the following:1. Abdominal discomfort (an uncomfortable sensation that is not described as pain) or pain associated

with two or more of the following at least 25% of the time:a) Improved with defecation; b) Onset associated with a change in frequency of stool;c) Onset associated with a change in form (appearance) of stool.2. No evidence of an inflammatory, anatomic, metabolic, or neoplastic process that explains the

subject’s symptoms.Criteria should be fulfilled at least once per week for at least 2 months before diagnosis.

Functional dyspepsiaMust include all of the following:1. Persistent or recurrent pain or discomfort centered in the upper abdomen (above the umbilicus)2. Not relieved by defecation or associated with the onset of a change in stool frequency or stool form

(i.e., not IBS)3. No evidence of an inflammatory, anatomic, metabolic, or neoplastic process that explains the

subject’s symptoms.Criteria should be fulfilled at least once per week for at least 2 months before diagnosis.

Abdominal migraine1. Paroxysmal episodes of intense, acute periumbilical pain that lasts for one hour or more 2. Intervening periods of usual health lasting weeks to months3. The pain interferes with normal activities4. The pain is associated with 2 or more of the following: anorexia, nausea, vomiting, headache,

photophobia, pallor.5. No evidence of an inflammatory, anatomic, metabolic, or neoplastic process considered that explains

the subject’s symptoms.Criteria should be fulfilled at least 2 or more times in the preceding 12 months.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 11 18-07-12 17:19

Page 15: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

12 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Table 2. Red flags that may signal possible organic causes of recurrent abdominal pain (Di Lorenzo et al., 2005)

involuntary weight loss gastrointestinal bleedingdeceleration of linear growth velocity unexplained feverchronic severe diarrhea persistent vomitingpersistent right upper or right lower quadrant pain laboratory evidence of anemia or inflammationfamily history of inflammatory bowel disease abnormal physical findings

Health and social limitationsThe consequences of FAP for daily life are substantial. FAP has been associated with health limitations, excess use of health care services, a heightened risk to undergo expensive multiple investigations and occasionally unnecessary surgical intervention (Campo et al., 2004; Konijnenberg et al., 2005; Levy et al., 2004; Lindley et al., 2005; Stickler & Murphy, 1979). It is also clear that FAP is often accompanied by other somatic symptoms such as fatigue, dizziness, headache and other physical pains (Claar & Walker, 2006; Ramchandani, Fazel, Stein, Wiles, & Hotopf, 2007; Riedl et al., 2008). Children with FAP report similar reductions in quality of life as children with AP with a proven organic cause like inflammatory bowel disease or gastroesophageal reflux disease (Youssef, Murphy, Langseder, & Rosh, 2006). Finally, children with FAP that visit a doctor for their complaints have less social contacts, are less physically active and have high rates of school absenteeism (Claar & Walker, 2006; Mulvaney et al., 2006; Perquin et al., 2003; Ramchandani et al., 2007; Walker, Garber, Van Slyke, & Greene, 1995; Youssef, Atienza, Langseder, & Strauss, 2008).

Psychiatric co-morbidityChildren with FAP have been found to experience increased levels of anxiety and depression compared to healthy children, both in cross-sectional (Campo et al., 2004; Dorn et al., 2003; Garber, Zeman, & Walker, 1990; Ramchandani, Hotopf, Sandhu, & Stein, 2005; Walker & Greene, 1989) and longitudinal studies (Campo et al., 2001; Hotopf, Carr, Mayou, Wadsworth, & Wessely, 1998). This psychiatric co-morbidity seems specific for internalizing psychopathology, since there is no higher morbidity of externalizing problems like conduct disorders or ADHD in children with FAP (Galli et al., 2007). A frequently cited study underlining these results is the one by Campo et al, who found an anxiety disorder in 79% of children with FAP, and depression in 43% (Campo et al., 2004). Although other studies found somewhat lower co-morbidity rates (Campo, 2012; Liakopoulou-Kairis et al., 2002), it is clear that psychiatric internalizing co-morbidity is common in children with FAP. Question is of course how this relationship between internalizing disorders and FAP might best be explained. First of all, FAP might cause anxiety as well as anxiety might cause FAP. This can be observed closely in time, when for example concerns about the condition cause anxiety, or when anxious feelings cause abdominal complaints. However, longitudinal relationships are also found, as having recurrent AP in childhood increases the risk of psychiatric disorders

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 12 18-07-12 17:19

Page 16: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

1

General introduction | 13

later in life (Campo et al., 2001; Hotopf et al., 1998). Anxiety and FAP may also have a common biological ground. For example, within the central nervous system, gut control is mostly located in the limbic system, which also plays a central role in emotionality (Jones, Dilley, Drossman, & Crowell, 2006). Additionally, children with FAP and children with anxiety both show stronger autonomic reactivity than healthy controls (Dorn et al., 2003; Hyman & Cocjin, 2005) and both groups show a potential hypersensitivity of the central nervous system (Bakker, Boer, Benninga, Koelman, & Tijssen, 2009; Bakker, Tijssen, van der Meer, Koelman, & Boer, 2009). Finally, FAP and internalizing disorders might both be caused by similar psychological or social factors, such as stress, social learning, negative cognitions, biases in information processing, etcetera. These will be discussed in more detail later in this chapter.

PrognosisThe sparse epidemiological studies on long-term prognosis of childhood FAP are not optimistic. Most studies investigated the prognosis of any type of recurrent AP, making no distinction between FAP and AP with an organic cause. A number of small long-term follow-up studies were conducted in clinical samples in the 1970’s and ‘80’s, which found that recurrent AP persisted over at least 10 years in about 25%-50% of the cases and changed to other painful symptoms (mostly headaches) in 25%-30% (Apley & Hale, 1973; Christensen & Mortensen, 1975; Magni, Pierri, & Donzelli, 1987). More recent and larger studies also found relationships between childhood AP and adult FGIDs (Gulewitsch, Enck, Hautzinger, & Schlarb, 2011; Howell, Poulton, & Talley, 2005). Walker and colleagues found that 35% of a cohort of children with FAP fulfilled Rome III criteria for an FGID up to 15 years later (Dengler-Crish, Horst, & Walker, 2011; Walker, Dengler-Crish, Rippel, & Bruehl, 2010), and about 25% experienced chronic pain of another kind (Walker et al., 2010). In younger children, recurrent AP also seems likely to persist (Ramchandani et al., 2005). Other studies find a relationship between childhood recurrent AP and psychiatric disorders in adulthood (Campo et al., 2001; Hotopf et al., 1998). Factors associated with persistence of complaints vary from occurrence before the age of six and longer history of complaints (Christensen & Mortensen, 1975; Magni et al., 1987), experiencing more non-gastrointestinal somatic symptoms (Dengler-Crish et al., 2011), to having a more ‘neurotic’ mother, or having parents with recurrent pains (Gieteling, Bierma-Zeinstra, Van Leeuwen, Passchier, & Berger, 2011; Hotopf et al., 1998). Although it is suggested by some studies that female gender is related to worse outcome (Apley & Hale, 1973), a recent review found no association at all between female gender and duration of AP (Gieteling et al., 2011). More anxious and depressive symptoms, lower perceived self-worth and more negative life-events have also been found to be related to worse outcome (Mulvaney et al., 2006).

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 13 18-07-12 17:19

Page 17: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

14 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Etiology

Pathophysiology The pathophysiology of pediatric functional gastrointestinal disorders is not well understood. The most important hypotheses concern abnormal gut motility, infection, and changes in the brain-gut axis. The pathophysiology of FAP was not studied in the present thesis, but will be briefly discussed in the overview provided below.

Abnormal gut motilityAn important hypothesis throughout the literature is that FAP might be associated with abnormal gut motility. Ample literature exists to describe different phenomena in specific subgroups of patients. For example, in children with functional dyspepsia, it was found that 40% showed slow small bowel transit, and this, as well as fast gastric emptying into the small intestine, was related to bloating (Chitkara et al., 2003). A study in children (age 7-14) with FAP showed a more rapid emptying of the lower, reservoir portion of the stomach. These children reported more AP and bloating in response to a standardized meal compared to healthy controls (Olafsdottir, Gilja, Aslaksen, Berstad, & Fluge, 2000). Although these abnormalities have been described, the association between contractile patterns and symptoms is often small, or does not exist in all patients (Di Lorenzo, 2005). It is now generally assumed that abnormal gut motility only occurs in response to a physiological stimulus (food or gastrointestinal hormones), instead of being present all the time. Thus, the actual etiologic role of dysmotility for FAP remains questionable. Moreover, pharmacological interventions targeted at the motility problems have come up with disappointing results (Di Lorenzo, 2005).

Gastrointestinal infectionAnother biological mechanism that might relate to FAP and specifically to IBS, is gastrointestinal infection. Several studies in adults have shown that 3.7-36% of the patients that experience bacterial gastroenteritis, develop postinfectious IBS (Spiller & Garsed, 2009). Patients that have more severe and longer lasting gastroenteritis have a larger risk to develop postinfectious IBS than patients for which the infection was resolved more quickly (Spiller, 2011). A study in children showed similar results, with 36% of children reporting AP at least 6 months after an episode of Salmonella, Campylobacter or Shigella infection. In 88% of these children, AP improved with bowel movements; these children thus fulfilled criteria for IBS (Saps et al., 2008). However, over one third also mentioned that they experienced AP before the infection, which makes it difficult to conclude that the bacterial infection actually caused the complaints. Also, the relationship seems most profound for IBS, and less for other FGIDs (Saps et al., 2008). The mechanisms through which gastrointestinal infection increases the risk to develop IBS are still unclear, but may include changes in gut microbiota and changes in mucosal histology (Spiller, 2011).

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 14 18-07-12 17:19

Page 18: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

1

General introduction | 15

Changes in brain-gut axisA definition for the brain-gut axis might be ‘the bidirectional “communication system”

between the gut and the brain’ (Van Oudenhove, Coen, & Aziz, 2007). It can be divided into three parts: the enteric nervous system (ENS), the autonomic nervous system and the central nervous system (CNS). The enteric nervous system (ENS) and the central nervous system (CNS) have a common embryological origin, and resemble each other in many ways. The ENS is even sometimes called ‘the mini-brain’ or ‘brain-in-the-gut’ because of its shared features with the CNS, like the common neurotransmitter serotonin (Van Oudenhove, Demyttenaere, Tack, & Aziz, 2004). The gut and the brain show significant neuronal and endocrine interaction and communicate bidirectionally, mostly through the autonomic nervous system and the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal-axis (HPA-axis) (Jones et al., 2006; Lydiard & Falsetti, 1999; Van Oudenhove et al., 2007). A number of brain regions have been found to be important in visceral sensation and pain. For example, when the gut or stomach is artificially stimulated (for example, by slowly filling a balloon in the intestines with air, using a barostat), the secondary somatosensory cortex, the orbital and medial prefrontal cortices and the anterior cingulated cortex (ACC) are differently activated in patients with FGIDs than in healthy controls. These structures mostly make up the affective-motivational pain systems of the brain, which encode pain affect and suffering (Van Oudenhove et al., 2004). The differential activation of these brain regions suggests that the affective dimension of the pain experience may be altered in patients with FGIDs. The growing evidence that links FAP to visceral hyperalgesia or visceral hypersensitivity, a decreased threshold of pain in response to changes in intraluminal pressure, supports this hypothesis (Di Lorenzo et al., 2001; Halac, Noble, & Faure, 2010; Van Ginkel, Voskuijl, Benninga, Taminiau, & Boeckxstaens, 2001). However, both increased and decreased activity of these brain structures is reported, as well as no changes in activity, making it difficult to interpret the clinical significance of these findings (Van Oudenhove, 2008). Also, although these differences are found at the CNS level, this does not necessarily mean that central pain perception is dysfunctional; they could also be caused by abnormal input from the ENS (Van Oudenhove et al., 2004). For example, mucosal inflammatory processes may cause sensitization of afferent nerves in the gut and have been associated with the onset of visceral hypersensitivity (Talley & Spiller, 2002). Thus, visceral hypersensitivity may be caused centrally and/or peripherally. However, the concept of visceral hypersensitivity is still poorly understood, which is underlined by the study of Naliboff and colleagues (2001). They found that adult IBS patients show different brain activity in response to rectal stimulation compared to control subjects. However, all subjects, both IBS patients and controls, showed similar brain activity in response to delivered and just anticipated rectal stimulation. Thus, it seems that for brain activation, it does not matter whether rectal stimulation is just anticipated or actually

delivered (Naliboff et al., 2001). Evidence on mirror-neurons highlights the neurological

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 15 18-07-12 17:19

Page 19: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

16 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

foundations of such a mechanism, as the brain reacts to seeing, hearing and imagining actions (and emotions and pain) by activating the same brain regions as it would when actually performing the action (Gallese, Eagle, & Migone, 2005). The fact that it does not seem to matter for the activity registered in the brain whether the rectum is actually stimulated or not, poses some questions about the visceral hypersensitivity-hypothesis. Indeed, psychological factors like attentional biases to pain or conditioning might also contribute to visceral hypersensitivity. It seems logical that patients suffering from IBS might be more prone to pay close attention to visceral sensation, or may be vigilant of the pain being induced in barostat-experiments, resulting in a faster reporting of pain (Whitehead & Palsson, 1998). Summarizing, it is not yet clear from which part of the brain-gut axis the pain originates. In some patients, alterations in the peripheral enteric nervous system may cause the pain, while in others the pain might be due to a disturbed processing of signals from the enteric nervous system by the central nervous system (Öhman & Simrén, 2007; Van Oudenhove et al., 2004). Also, most of the research on the brain-gut axis is performed in adults, and more research is needed in children to investigate if the same processes are at work as in adults.

Psychosocial mechanismsPsychosocial factors that are traditionally linked to FAP are the influence of parents through modeling and social learning, stress, and alexithymia. However, recent years have seen an upsurge in research on other factors as well, such as cognitions, coping and information processing. In this section, the psychosocial factors most often implicated to play a role in pediatric FAP will be discussed.

Family characteristics: nature or nurture?Functional complaints aggregate in families. Indeed, several studies have found that children of mothers with IBS report more physical symptoms, which are not limited to AP. Of course, such an observation tempts us to go into the old ‘nature or nurture’-debate. Mechanisms through which nurture may play its part are modeling and social or operant

learning. Modeling refers to children’s tendency to adopt behavior modeled by (one of ) the parents or another important person. Social learning or operant conditioning of AP refers to the idea that parents may facilitate maladaptive sickness behavior by acknowledging positive consequences to this behavior (also called solicitous behavior; Walker & Zeman, 1992). For example, allowing the child to stay home from school, stay in bed and watch television may cause the child to learn that having a stomach ache also leads to positive things (i.e. ‘secondary gain’). On the other hand, the fact that children and parents display similar physical complaints could of course also be explained by a genetic susceptibility to these complaints, pleading for ‘nature’ effects. In an attempt to separate the effects of nature and nurture, Levy and colleagues performed a twin study, which investigated the concordance for IBS in 6060

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 16 18-07-12 17:19

Page 20: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

1

General introduction | 17

twin pairs and their parents. It was found that concordance for IBS was significantly greater in monozygotic (17.2%) than in dizygotic twins (8.4%), thus providing evidence for a genetic component. However, dizygotic twins who had IBS were much more likely to have a mother (15.2%) than a twin with IBS (6.7%), pleading for nurture. Additionally, having a mother or father with IBS was more strongly related to IBS status than having a twin with IBS (Levy et al., 2001). Indeed, a recent review on IBS in adults concluded that there seems to be a genetic susceptibility to IBS, though it is most likely rather modest (Öhman & Simrén, 2007). Thus, heredity seems to contribute to the etiology of IBS, but learning processes might have an equal or greater influence. Apart from the potential influence of parents displaying pain symptoms, having parents suffering from psychiatric problems might also influence the occurrence or intensity of FAP. For example, a prospective study found that maternal anxiety at age two was significantly related to recurrent AP (without making a distinction between FAP or organic AP), school absence and anxiety symptoms of the child at age 7 (Ramchandani et al., 2007). Moreover, maternal anxiety about abdominal symptoms was found to be related to medical health care seeking for FAP (Venepalli, van Tilburg, & Whitehead, 2006). Parents’ attitudes towards the abdominal complaints might also be important: if parents accepted a biopsychosocial model for the explanation of the child’s symptoms, this was associated with recovery from FAP (Crushell et al., 2003). Walker, Garber, & Greene (1993) propose a family model of illness behavior, where FAP occurs in a climate of greater expression of somatic and emotional distress, and is associated with increased rates of anxiety and depression in both parents and children (see also: Bosch & Slot, 1998).

Stress and coping Stressful circumstances (daily hassles or negative life events) might cause or exacerbate the abdominal complaints. An important observation which contributed to this hypothesis is that in adult women suffering from gastrointestinal complaints, a large percentage (44%) reported a history of childhood or adult sexual abuse. Moreover, patients with functional disorders were more likely to have experienced these events than patients with organic disorders (Drossman, 2011; Drossman et al., 1990). There are some studies that support this hypothesis in children as well. In a retrospective study, Alfvén found that in 48% of children with recurrent AP (including children with organic AP), exposure to chronic negative stress seemed likely related to the origin of the complaints (Alfvén, 2003). The effects of negative life events seem to persist prospectively, as experiencing more negative life events proved to be related to maintenance of FAP after 3 months (Walker & Greene, 1991a) and to more somatic symptoms after one year (Walker, Garber, & Greene, 1994). To report on a more specific type of stress, being victim of bullying led to significantly increased odds of developing AP prospectively (Fekkes, Pijpers, Fredricks, Vogels, & Verloove-Vanhorick, 2006). Even on a biological level, there is evidence that stress relates to FAP. Rats that were separated from their mother for three hours on a

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 17 18-07-12 17:19

Page 21: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

18 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

daily basis in the first two weeks of their life, showed increased visceral sensitivity, reduced somatic analgesia and increased colonic motility in response to an acute psychological stressor, mimicking important features of IBS (Coutinho et al., 2002). Also, studies in adults showed that a history of sexual or physical abuse was associated with hypersensitivity to distention of the stomach (Geeraerts et al., 2009), a decreased sensitivity to distention of the rectum (Raphael, 2005), and altered brain activation in response to rectal distention (Ringel et al., 2008). Although stressful circumstances seem to be related to abdominal symptoms, the literature is inconclusive on the question whether children with FAP experience more stress than their healthy peers. Some studies find that children with FAP report more daily stressors (Walker, Garber, Smith, Van Slyke, & Claar, 2001) and more life events (Liakopoulou-Kairis et al., 2002; Robinson, Alvarez, & Dodge, 1990), but others report no differences (Walker, Garber, & Greene, 1993; Walker, Smith, Garber, & Claar, 2007). Indeed, being confronted with stressors is, to an extent, part of everyday life, and will not always have a negative impact on a child’s life. Question is why it leads to continued complaints in one person, while the stress is successfully resolved in another. An important factor that influences this relationship between stress and well-being, is the way people cope with stress (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984). People can use several distinct coping strategies to cope with stress, for example seeking social support or thinking about the problem to try and figure out what to do. Depending on how effective the coping strategy is, one might develop physical or emotional complaints in response to the stressor, or adapt well. It has been fairly well investigated how children with FAP cope with their pain. Studies typically find that using passive coping strategies like self-isolation, catastrophizing and disengagement is related to more AP cross-sectionally as well as over time and to more depressive symptoms (Compas et al., 2006; Crombez et al., 2003; Kaminsky, Robertson, & Dewey, 2006; Levy et al., 2004; Thomsen et al., 2002; Walker, Baber, Garber, & Smith, 2008; Walker, Smith, Garber, & Claar, 2005). Using accommodative strategies like positive thinking and acceptance of symptoms usually relates to less severe somatic and emotional symptoms and better outcomes over time (Compas et al., 2006; Walker et al., 2005, 2008). However, to our knowledge just one study has focused on how children with FAP cope with general stress and problems in life and if this relates to their abdominal complaints (Walker et al., 2007). Considering the link between general stress and AP presented above, it seems logical to assume such a connection. Indeed, this one study found that children with FAP used accommodative coping less often, a strategy that is usually found adaptive. Obviously, it seems important to investigate these general coping skills in children with FAP more extensively.

AlexithymiaAlexithymia is usually defined as a) difficulties in distinguishing between different emotions and insufficient realization that some physical sensations may be the manifestation of

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 18 18-07-12 17:19

Page 22: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

1

General introduction | 19

emotions; b) difficulties in verbally expressing emotions; c) limited imagination and fantasy life; d) thought directed at external reality and hardly or not at inner experience (Kooiman, Spinhoven, & Trijsburg, 2002, pp. 1084). The term ‘alexithymia’ was first introduced by Sifneos in 1973, to describe his observation that patients with functional somatic complaints often have difficulties in describing their feelings. Thus, alexithymia has traditionally been linked to somatization and medically unexplained symptoms (Kooiman et al., 2002).

Studies investigating the relevance of alexithymia in our population of interest have not yet been performed. It has been shown that adult patients suffering from FAP are more alexithymic than psychiatric patients or patients suffering from organic AP (Porcelli et al., 2004; Porcelli, Taylor, Bagby, & De Carne, 1999). Also, being more alexithymic was shown to be related to worse treatment outcomes in patients with FAP (Porcelli, De Carne, & Todarello, 2004). However, other studies found no or only indirect relationships between alexithymia and the occurrence and prognosis of medically unexplained symptoms in both adults and children (De Gucht, Fischler, & Heiser, 2004; De Gucht & Heiser, 2003; Friedberg & Quick, 2007; Jellesma, Rieffe, Terwogt, & Kneepkens, 2006; Jones, Schettler, Olden, & Crowell, 2004; Kooiman, Bolk, Rooijmans, & Trijsburg, 2004; Van de Putte, Engelbert, Kuis, Kimpen, & Uiterwaal, 2007). Therefore, more research in children with FAP is needed to determine its relevance for the etiology of FAP.

Negative thoughts or ‘illness beliefs’Beliefs children hold about their AP can influence the persistence of the pain. Children suffering from AP can think about their pain in different ways. Examples of thoughts might be “This pain means I have a serious illness”, “My pain never stops”, or “My pain always goes away quickly”. Obviously, the first two thoughts are less adaptive than the latter, and are actually thoughts that are typical in disorders like panic disorder and hypochondria. In the literature on psychopathology, it is generally assumed that these kinds of thoughts have a maintaining or even exacerbating influence on the complaints (Clark & Fairburn, 1997). Surprisingly enough, it has scarcely been investigated whether children with FAP experience such thoughts. Only recently an instrument was developed to measure pain appraisal in children suffering from FAP. It was found that when children reported more negative thoughts, this was related to more worrying by the mother and more limitations in family activities (Lipani & Walker, 2006). However, it is not yet clear whether these thoughts are related to more AP. In other populations, negative cognitions have been found to be of influence. In adolescents with type 1 diabetes, thinking more negatively about the variability, unpredictability and consequences of the symptoms was positively related to negative adjustment (Olsen, Berg, & Wiebe, 2008). Worrying more about one’s health was found to be related to more subjective health complaints in healthy students (Eminson, Benjamin, Shortall, Woods, & Faragher, 1996; Verkuil, Brosschot, & Thayer, 2007). The question remains what direction these relationships have, as it is also likely that children who have more severe somatic complaints, worry more about them. For FAP, it

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 19 18-07-12 17:19

Page 23: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

20 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

seems prudent to further investigate the role of cognitions; if negative thoughts or other dysfunctional cognitions can be demonstrated in children with FAP in relation to their AP, these cognitions can be targeted in psychological therapies.

Control or coping self-efficacyHaving a sense of control over AP or feeling confident about your own abilities to cope with AP may be another important psychological factor in the etiology of FAP. Data from other populations suggests that having a greater sense of self-efficacy relates to better diabetes self-management adherence (Iannotti et al., 2006) and less negative adjustment (Olsen et al., 2008). Prospectively, having a more internal locus of control at age 10 proved related to a reduced risk of obesity, overweight, poor self-rated health, and psychological distress at age 30, stressing the importance of having a sense of control over one’s own life for future outcomes (Gale, Batty, & Deary, 2008). Self-efficacy and control have in the last few years also been investigated in children with FAP. Experiencing less self-efficacy or control was found to be related to other somatic symptoms, more disabilities due to the AP, using less effective coping strategies (Walker et al., 2005) and more depressive symptoms (Kaminsky et al., 2006; Walker et al., 2005). Though these results are obviously important, again little is known about the relationship between a sense of self-efficacy/control and how much AP a child experiences.

Information processingThe last factor which might play an important role in the origin or continuation of AP was derived from anxiety literature, and concerns information processing. Several studies on anxiety disorders in children investigated whether these children selectively direct their attention to threatening stimuli in the environment. It seems that children who report high levels of anxiety show an attentional bias towards those threats, while children who score low on anxiety show an attentional bias away from threat cues (Vasey & MacLeod, 2001), although the evidence for the latter phenomenon appeared to be weak in a meta-analysis (Bar-Haim, Lamy, Pergamin, Bakermans-Kranenburg, & Van IJzendoorn, 2007). Indeed, it has been suggested by the theory on ‘pain vigilance’ and the ‘symptom perception hypothesis’ that patients suffering from chronic pain might also experience such an attentional bias. Pain vigilance refers to the tendency to focus attention on the own body, scanning for signs of pain. Similarly, the Symptom Perception Hypothesis by Pennebaker suggests that people with many somatic complaints pay more attention to vague or ambiguous internal sensations (Pennebaker, 1982), and falsely interpret these physical sensations as a sign of physical dysfunction or illness (Terwogt, Rieffe, Miers, Jellesma, & Tolland, 2006). Recently, two studies tried to explicitly investigate the presence of such an attentional bias by using a computer task. However, these studies had contradictory results: one study showed that children with FAP avoided pain-words (Boyer et al., 2006), whereas the other reported that children directed their attention towards pain-words (Beck et al., 2011). More studies using other computer tasks to measure attention biases seem warranted.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 20 18-07-12 17:19

Page 24: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

1

General introduction | 21

Treatment

Considering the impact of FAP on the child’s life and the unfavorable prognosis of FAP, there is a dire need to treat these children effectively. However, in light of so many possible etiologic factors, the question is what a treatment should specifically focus on.

The interventions that have been investigated for their efficacy in treating pediatric FAP are – not surprisingly – diverse, ranging from pharmacological interventions like peppermint oil capsules and antidepressants, to psychosocial interventions like hypnotherapy and cognitive behavior therapy. Four systematic Cochrane Reviews have been published within the last three years, discussing the evidence for pharmacological therapies and antidepressants in particular, dietary interventions, and psychosocial interventions for the treatment of FAP (Huertas-Ceballos, Logan, Bennet, & Macarthur, 2009a; Huertas-Ceballos, Logan, Bennet, & Macarthur, 2009b; Huertas-Ceballos, Logan, Bennet, & Macarthur, 2009c; Kaminski, Kamper, Thaler, Chapman, & Gartlehner, 2011). The general conclusions of these reviews are not optimistic, as for most of these interventions, very few RCTs have been performed to investigate their effectiveness, and the RCTs that have been performed usually show considerably methodological drawbacks. Below, the evidence for each type of intervention is discussed.

AntidepressantsThe first Cochrane review concluded that for the effectiveness of antidepressants, limited evidence is available. Only two RCTs investigating the effectiveness of antidepressants fulfilled inclusion criteria for the Cochrane review (Bahar, Collins, Steinmetz, & Ament, 2008; Saps et al., 2009b). Both investigated the effects of amitriptyline, but no convincing evidence was found that amitriptyline is more effective than placebo (Kaminski et al., 2011). The evidence for the effectiveness of antidepressants to treat IBS in adults is also not convincing (Ladabaum et al., 2010). A more recent study not included in the Cochrane review (Teitelbaum & Arora, 2011) concluded that over 75% of children with FGIDs respond to low-dose tricyclic antidepressants. However, this study was a retrospective chart review study, and defined treatment response as any remark by the patient during follow-up visits that the medication helped to decrease symptomatology to any degree; therefore, the validity of this study is questionable at best. Considering the potential adverse effects of antidepressants, more and better studies should be conducted before practitioners can start prescribing antidepressants to children with FAP (Kaminski et al., 2011).

Other pharmacological interventionsSeveral other types of medications have been investigated for their effectiveness in treating pediatric FAP. However, only three RCTs were of sufficient quality to be included in the second Cochrane review, and these investigated three different medications:

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 21 18-07-12 17:19

Page 25: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

22 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

famotidine (reduces the production of gastric acid, used for heartburn), pizotifen (a serotonin antagonist mostly used as a preventative to reduce the frequency of recurrent migraine headaches), and peppermint oil enteric coated capsules (has a calming effect on the stomach and intestinal tract). When the pharmacological intervention was directed at a specific functional gastrointestinal disorder, efficacy was highest (famotidine for dyspepsia, pizotifen for abdominal migraine and peppermint oil for IBS) (Weydert, Ball, & Davis, 2003). However, with only one study per medication type, and low sample sizes in each study, the evidence for the effectiveness was weak, and it was concluded that there is little reason for the use of these medications in daily practice (Huertas-Ceballos et al., 2009b).

Dietary interventionsThe Cochrane review on dietary interventions showed that more RCTs were performed on this type of intervention (N=7), but these lacked quality and overall it was concluded that there was no convincing evidence that fiber supplements, lactose free diets or Lactobacillus supplementation are effective treatments for recurrent AP (Huertas-Ceballos et al., 2009a). A large RCT published after the Cochrane review showed that Lactobacillus rhamnosus GG was more effective than placebo for young children (mean age of 6) with IBS, but not for children with FAP (Francavilla et al., 2010).

Psychosocial interventionsThe fourth Cochrane review focused on the effects of psychosocial interventions, and on cognitive behavioral interventions in particular. Six RCTs were identified that were of sufficient quality to be included. The results of these trials were promising. However, the six studies suffered from methodological weaknesses like small sample sizes and large drop-out rates; thus, further, better-quality research was advised (Huertas-Ceballos et al., 2009c). Other psychosocial interventions not included in the review like hypnotherapy and guided imagery have also been tested for their effectiveness to treat pediatric FAP in three small RCTs. Hypnotherapy including cognitive and behavioral elements was shown to be highly effective, with 85% of children in remission at 1 year follow-up (Vlieger, Menko-Frankenhuis, Wolfkamp, Tromp, & Benninga, 2007), and 68% still in remission at 5 years follow-up (Vlieger, Rutten, Govers, Frankenhuis, & Benninga, 2012). Guided imagery was also found effective for 63% of children in a small pilot study (van Tilburg et al., 2009), and another trial showed guided imagery to be superior to simple breathing exercises (Weydert et al., 2006).

SummaryIn summary, different treatments are available for pediatric FAP but the evidence for most treatments is weak at best. Cognitive behavior therapy has been investigated most often and its effect has been found to be promising, but the RCTs investigating the effectiveness of this therapy also lack methodological rigor. Moreover, apart from the fact that for most

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 22 18-07-12 17:19

Page 26: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

1

General introduction | 23

interventions short-term effects are not well established, it is at present unknown if specific treatments are of benefit for children with FAP in the long run. The only longer-term follow-up performed was the five-year follow-up of the hypnotherapy trial. More research is needed to investigate whether intervening at a young age might protect children from experiencing continued somatic complaints or developing psychiatric disorders in adulthood.

Aims and outline of the present thesis

The present thesis consists of two parts, corresponding to two primary aims. The first aim was to explore which psychosocial factors may be risk, maintaining or exacerbating factors of pediatric FAP. This was investigated by means of three questionnaire studies, one meta-analysis and one experimental study, described in Part I: Psychosocial risk factors

for pediatric functional abdominal pain. In this part of the thesis, chapter 2 reports on a large survey among Dutch school children, investigating the relationship between AP and co-morbid psychological and physical symptoms of children and parents. In chapter 3, the existing literature on parental modeling and social learning is systematically reviewed and meta-analyzed, to gain insight in whether parents maintain or exacerbate the child’s complaints. The theories on alexithymia and coping presented in the current chapter are empirically investigated in chapter 4, which reports the results of a questionnaire study in over 100 children with FAP and over 600 school children. In this study, it is tested whether alexithymia and coping with everyday problems are related to FAP, and whether coping mediates the relationship between alexithymia and FAP. Next, evidence for the relevance of newer theories of cognitions, coping and coping self-efficacy, and whether children’s thoughts and coping behavior or parental thoughts and coping behavior are more strongly related to FAP, is presented in chapter 5. A final psychosocial risk factor is investigated in chapter 6, which reports on an experimental study that tests whether an attentional bias for bodily activity is present in children with FAP. The aim of the second part of this thesis, Cognitive behavior therapy for pediatric

functional abdominal pain, was to investigate whether cognitive behavior therapy (CBT) is more effective than medical care in the treatment of children with FAP, and through what psychological and social mechanisms CBT and medical care reach their effects. Chapter 7 presents the results of a large RCT, comparing the effectiveness of six sessions of CBT to six visits to a pediatrician in treating children with FAP. Which psychological and social mechanisms moderate and mediate the effectiveness of these treatments is presented in chapter 8. This thesis concludes with a general discussion in chapter 9, in which the main findings throughout the different chapters are summarized and integrated and future directions for research and practice are considered.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 23 18-07-12 17:19

Page 27: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 24 18-07-12 17:19

Page 28: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

IPsychosocial risk factors

for pediatric functional abdominal pain

Part

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 25 18-07-12 17:19

Page 29: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 26 18-07-12 17:19

Page 30: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

Ch

apte

r

2Abdominal pain in Dutch

schoolchildren: Relationships with physical and psychological

co-morbid complaints in children and their parents.

The contents of this chapter are published in:Van der Veek, S. M. C., Derkx, H. H. F., de Haan, E., Benninga, M. A., & Boer, F. (2010). Abdominal pain in Dutch schoolchildren: Relationships with physical and psychological co-morbid complaints in children and their

parents. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 51, 481-487.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 27 18-07-12 17:19

Page 31: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

28 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Abstract

Objectives: Children with functional abdominal pain (FAP) frequently report co-morbid complaints such as anxiety and activity limitations. Their parents often experience heightened levels of anxiety, depression and somatization. Aim of the present study was to investigate whether these co-morbid complaints in children and their parents are specific for FAP, or can also be found in a community sample. Method: 665 schoolchildren (age 7-18) filled out questionnaires concerning abdominal pain, activity limitations, somatic complaints, quality of life, and symptoms of anxiety and depression. A total of 391 of their parents filled out questionnaires concerning parental anxiety, depression and somatization. Pearson correlations and multiple regression analyses were performed. Results: 56.5% of the children reported abdominal pain at least once in a two-week period. Univariate relationships with abdominal pain were found for activity limitations (r = .392), somatic complaints (r = .408), 3 out of 5 domains of quality of life (r ranged from -.120 to -.209), and symptoms of anxiety and depression (r respectively .329 and .361). Multivariate analyses showed only significant relationships for female gender (β = .230), younger age (β = -.077), activity limitations (β = .247), somatic complaints (β = .170), and depressive symptoms (β = .093). Discussion: Activity limitations, somatic complaints and depressive symptoms are related to abdominal pain in the general population, whereas a reduced quality of life, anxiety and parental internalizing problems seem specific co-morbid complaints for FAP. Future research of parental internalizing problems, quality of life, and anxiety as risk factors for FAP is warranted.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 28 18-07-12 17:19

Page 32: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

2

Physical and psychological co-morbid complaints in children and parents | 29

Introduction

Abdominal pain is a common complaint in children and adolescents. In the Netherlands, the 3-month prevalence of abdominal pain (AP) in the general population has been estimated at 22% (Perquin et al., 2000), but higher rates have been reported in the United States (e.g., 90% experienced pain at least once in 4-6 months; Saps et al., 2009a) and other European countries (e.g.,. Sweden: monthly pain in over 60% of children (Östberg, Alfvén, & Hjern, 2006); Germany: monthly prevalence of 36% (Hirsch, John, Schaller, & Türp, 2006)). About 1.6% of all visits of Dutch children to the general practitioner are due to AP (Bruijnzeels, van Suijlekom-Smit, Van der Velden, & van der Wouden, 1993). When the pain returns on a regular basis, health care consumption increases. This is the case in the .3 to 19% of children in Western countries that suffer from recurrent AP (Chitkara et al., 2005). In the majority of these children no organic cause can be found which can explain the pain. Consequently, the pain is considered ‘functional’ (Rasquin et al., 2006). The impact of functional abdominal pain (FAP) on a child’s life is usually significant, which is partly due to several consistently reported co-morbid complaints. Children with FAP experience comparable reductions in quality of life as children with inflammatory bowel disease and gastroesophageal reflux disease (Youssef et al., 2006). They often experience a multitude of other somatic complaints such as headaches or fatigue, and are limited in their activities because of their abdominal pain (e.g., less social contacts, high rates of school absenteeism) (Claar & Walker, 2006; Claar, Walker, & Smith, 1999; Mulvaney et al., 2006; Perquin et al., 2003; Ramchandani et al., 2007; Walker et al., 1995; Youssef et al., 2008). Furthermore, co-morbidity rates of anxiety and depression are high (Campo et al., 2004; Dufton, Dunn, & Compas, 2009; Liakopoulou-Kairis et al., 2002). Apart from co-morbid complaints reported by the children, it has also consistently been found that parents of children with FAP experience more symptoms of anxiety, depression, and somatization (Ramchandani et al., 2005; Ramchandani, Stein, Hotopf, & Wiles, 2006; Walker, Garber, & Greene, 1991; Walker et al., 1995; Walker & Greene, 1989). It is as yet unclear whether this pattern of co-morbid complaints is specific for children with FAP, or can also be found in relatively healthy children suffering from more occasional AP. Previous studies did not find evidence for a specific pattern: they found the same relationships between AP and symptoms of anxiety, depression, a lowered quality of life, and other somatic complaints in children from the general population (Alfvén, 1993; Hyams, Burke, Davis, Rzepski, & Andrulonis, 1996; Saps et al., 2009a). However, these studies did not investigate co-existing activity limitations or parental psychopathology, nor were these variables simultaneously studied. Studying the co-morbidity of different complaints simultaneously (i.e., in a multivariate way) is crucial for investigating the unique

co-morbidity of each complaint with AP. For example, anxiety and depression usually show a high correlation. If we would only study a one-to-one (univariate) relationship between

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 29 18-07-12 17:19

Page 33: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

30 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

anxiety and AP without correcting for the correlation between anxiety and depression, the co-morbidity of anxiety with AP might be overrated. Therefore, the current study will focus on the univariate and multivariate relationships between AP on the one hand, and activity limitations, other somatic complaints, quality of life, symptoms of anxiety and depression, and parental symptoms of anxiety, depression and somatization on the other, in a non-selected sample of school children, aged 7-18. We hypothesize that AP will be related to more limitations in activities, more somatic complaints, a diminished quality of life and more symptoms of anxiety and depression in children. Additionally, we expect that the severity of AP in children will be related to symptoms of anxiety, depression and somatization in parents.

Materials and Methods

Sample and procedureThe design of this study was cross-sectional. Data were gathered through self-report questionnaires that were filled out by children aged 7-18 and their parents. Psychology students from Leiden University gathered most of the data as part of their bachelor’s thesis; therefore, the available time to recruit schools and participants was limited. Ethical approval for administering the questionnaires was obtained from the Psychological Ethics Committee of Leiden University. Children and adolescents were contacted through their primary and secondary schools. Schools were contacted throughout the Dutch provinces Noord-Holland, Zuid-Holland and Utrecht. As an incentive, primary schools were offered a gift token of 15 euros for each participating class. Parents were informed about the study either by a message in the weekly school paper or by a letter directly addressed to their homes. In this letter, the objectives of the study were explained. Parents were informed that their children would receive a little reward for their efforts (a sweet). The questionnaires were part of a larger study, in which more instruments were used. As it took the children in primary school (aged 7-12) approximately 1,5 to 2 hours to fill out the questionnaires, they were tested in two sessions, with no more than one week between the sessions. The children in secondary/high school (aged 12-18) mostly managed to fill out the questionnaires during 1 to 1,5 class hours. The questionnaires were filled out in the classroom, either electronically or by paper and pencil. A psychology student was present at all times to motivate the children and answer any questions. Instruments

Abdominal painAbdominal pain was measured by the Abdominal Pain Index (API; Walker, Smith, Garber,

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 30 18-07-12 17:19

Page 34: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

2

Physical and psychological co-morbid complaints in children and parents | 31

& VanSlyke, 1997). The API consists of five questions, and taps on the frequency, duration and intensity of the abdominal pain the child experienced in the past two weeks. The first question considers the frequency of AP and is rated on a 6-point scale ranging from (0) not at all to (5) every day. The second question concerns the typical daily frequency of the AP, and is also rated on a 6-point scale, ranging from (0) none to (5) constant during the day. The typical duration of pain episodes is rated on a 9-point scale ranging from (0) none to (8) all day. The typical and maximum intensity are measured by the last two items, and are rated on an 11-point scale ranging from (0) no pain to (10) the most pain possible. The API has been shown to be a reliable instrument in previous studies, with a Cronbach’s alpha ranging from .80 to .93 (Walker et al., 1997). A total score for the API is usually computed by standardizing all items, and summing them. However, an important disadvantage of this approach is that it is impossible to compare API scores across studies, as the scores are standardized on the sample means of each new study. Therefore, we used another approach, which still makes sure that every item has an equal weight on the total score. We recoded each item to reflect a scale ranging from 0 to 10, and summed all items, yielding a total score ranging from 0-50. A correlation of .999 between the two methods of calculation justified our new computational approach.

Activity limitationsThe Functional Disability Inventory (FDI; Claar & Walker, 2006; Walker & Greene, 1991b) assesses activity limitations in children and adolescents with a variety of pediatric conditions, and has specifically been validated in children with recurrent abdominal pain. The instrument consists of 15 items which concern limitations in activities during the past two weeks. Examples of items are “walking the length of a football field”, “being at school all day”, or “doing something with a friend”. Answers are scored on a five point scale, ranging from (0) no trouble at all to (4) impossible. The scores on the items are summed to compute a total score, with higher scores indicating more disabilities. Children that had never experienced AP did not have to fill out the FDI, and received a score of ‘0’. The FDI has been proven to be a reliable instrument, with Cronbach’s alpha’s ranging from .86 to .91.

Somatic complaintsThe Dutch version of the Children’s Somatization Inventory (CSI; Garber, Walker, & Zeman, 1991; Ghys & Meesters, 1993; Meesters, Muris, Ghys, Reumerman, & Rooijmans, 2003; Walker et al., 1991; Walker & Greene, 1989) was used to assess somatic complaints. The CSI contains 35 items that have to be rated on a 5-point scale, ranging from (0) not at all to (4) a whole lot, reflecting the extent to which the symptoms were experienced in the past two weeks. A total score can be computed by summing the scores across all items, with higher scores indicating a higher intensity of somatic complaints. As a number of items on the CSI are items concerning AP, these items were not used in the total score for this study, to avoid artificially elevated correlations. The following items were omitted: nausea/upset stomach; constipation; diarrhea; pain in stomach; vomiting; bloated stomach; food

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 31 18-07-12 17:19

Page 35: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

32 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

intolerance. The CSI has been shown to be a reliable and valid instrument for assessing somatization symptoms in children and adolescents (Meesters et al., 2003).

Quality of LifeThe KIDSCREEN-27 (The KIDSCREEN Group Europe, 2006) was used to assess different domains of generic quality of life. The KIDSCREEN-27 is based on a comprehensive process of development across different countries, and consists of 27 items, to be scored on a 5-point scale. Several items are reverse-scored. The instrument consists of five scales:- Physical well-being, consisting of five items- Psychological well-being, consisting of seven items - Parent relations & autonomy, consisting of seven items- Social support & peers, consisting of four items- School environment, consisting of four itemsScores for each scale are calculated using Rasch analysis and then transformed into T-values with a mean of 50 and a standard deviation of 10. Higher scores reflect a better quality of life. The KIDSCREEN-27 has been shown to be a reliable and valid instrument (The KIDSCREEN Group Europe, 2006).

Anxious and depressive symptomsAnxious and depressive symptoms were measured by the Revised Anxiety and Depression Scale – short version (RCADS-25; Muris, Meesters, & Schouten, 2002). The shortened version of the RCADS was developed by exploratory and confirmatory factor analysis on the original 47 items of the RCADS, and has been shown to be a valid and reliable instrument to measure generalized anxiety disorder, separation anxiety disorder, social phobia, panic disorder, and major depressive disorder (Muris et al., 2002). Each of the five scales consists of five items that have to be scored on a 4-point scale, ranging from (0) never to (3) always. Cronbach’s alphas of the five scales ranged from .65 to .83 in previous research (Muris et al., 2002). For the present study, the four scales of the different anxiety disorders were summed to compute one score for anxious symptoms.

Parental depression, anxiety and somatizationDepressive, anxious and somatization symptoms of the parents were measured by three subscales of the Symptom Check List (SCL-90; Arrindell & Ettema, 2003; Derogatis, 1977). The depression subscale consists of 16 items, the anxiety subscale of 10 and the somatization subscale of 12 items. The items are measured using a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from (1) not at all to (5) very much. Individual scale scores were obtained by summing the items belonging to the subscale; higher scores reflect more depressive, anxious and somatization symptoms. Test-retest reliability has been found to be good for this instrument and strong convergent validity was found with other conceptually related scales (Arrindell & Ettema, 2003).

Physical illnesses related to abdominal painTo control for any substantial physical illnesses that might explain the AP, parents were

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 32 18-07-12 17:19

Page 36: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

2

Physical and psychological co-morbid complaints in children and parents | 33

asked in an open-ended question whether their child had a physical illness (with the further specification that this should not just be a common cold or the flu) which affected their child at the moment.

Statistical analysesAll data were analyzed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) version 16.0. Before the main analyses were conducted, we checked four aspects of our data in preliminary analyses. First, the representativeness of the sample was checked by comparing the mean scores of our sample to the means of other community samples studied in the Netherlands and other countries. Effect sizes d (difference between the two means, devided by the pooled standard deviation) were calculated to investigate how large each difference was. Second, as age and gender have been shown to be related to AP (Chitkara et al., 2005; Kaczynski, Claar, & Logan, 2009; Perquin et al., 2000), it was checked whether these variables had any significant relationships with AP by calculating Pearson correlations. Third, it was checked whether the children that had a substantial physical illness that might explain the AP, influenced the outcome of the analyses. Finally, the statistical assumptions relevant for Multiple Regression Analysis (MRA), that is absence of multicollinearity (very high correlations among the variables entered in the MRA that cause statistical problems) and absence of multivariate outliers (outliers on a combination of all the variables entered in the MRA) were checked by calculating Pearson correlations and Mehalanobis distances, respectively. For the main analyses, univariate correlations between AP and the different co-morbid complaints were calculated by means of Pearson correlations. Second, a hierarchical MRA, method enter, was performed to investigate the multivariate relationships.

Results

ParticipantsThirteen of the eighteen schools contacted, agreed to participate (72.2%). Of the 2,396 children invited to participate, 665 children filled out the questionnaires (27.8%). Parents of 391 children (58.8%) also filled out a questionnaire, either electronically or by paper and pencil. Demographic characteristics of the sample are shown in table 1.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 33 18-07-12 17:19

Page 37: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

34 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Table 1. Sociodemographic characteristics of the sample.

Sociodemographic characteristics

Child (N = 665)Mean (SD; range) or %

Parent (N = 391)Mean (SD; range) or %

Gender Male 45.7%Female 54.3%

Male: 26.1%Female: 73.9%

Age 13.53 (2.61; 7-18) 45.01 (4.90; 31-64)Nationality Child and both parents Dutch: 83.0%

One or two parents born in other country, child born in the Netherlands: 10.7%Child born in other country, parents born in the Netherlands: 2.4%Child and one or two parents born in other country: 2.9%

Dutch: 96.7%Other: 3.3%

Family situation Lives with both parents: 83.2%Lives with one parent: 16.8%

Married or living together: 90.8%Divorced: 6.1%Widow(er): .8%Unmarried (never been married): 2.0%

Educational level Primary school (age 7-12): 24.1%Secondary school/High school (age 12-18): 75.6%

Bachelor: 27.9%Master: 17.4%

School absence (past 6 months)

None: 33.1%Once or twice: 43.2%A few times, but less than once a month: 15.3%About once a month: 5.1%More often than once a month: 2.9%

None: 33.7%Once or twice: 48.1%A few times, but less than once a month: 12.9%About once a month: 4.4%More often than once a month: 1.1%

Health care use (general practitioner or specialist in past 6 months)

1.49 (2.30; 0-20) .93 (1.46; 0-11)

Physical illness a - Yes, and may be related to abdominal pain: 2.0%Yes, and most likely unrelated to abdominal pain: 11.0% b

No: 86.9%a Question was: “Does your child have a physical illness that affects him/her at this moment? Note: we do not mean a common cold or flu.” b Most reported types of illness were eczema, allergies, asthma.

Preliminary analyses

Representativeness of the sampleIt was first checked whether the mean scores of our sample were comparable to the means of other community samples studied in the Netherlands and other countries. For the API, no comparative data were available. Therefore, we decided to look at the first item of the API, reported frequency of AP in the last two weeks. In our sample, 289 children reported no AP in the last two weeks (43.5%), 261 children had experienced pain once or twice (39.2%), and 101 children experienced pain 3 or 4 days or more in the past two weeks (16.8%). These rates are higher than previously found in the Netherlands by Perquin and colleagues, who found that 22% of children in the general population experienced AP at least once in three months (Perquin et al., 2000). However, they are comparable to

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 34 18-07-12 17:19

Page 38: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

2

Physical and psychological co-morbid complaints in children and parents | 35

rates reported in the United States, like the 90% of children that experienced AP at least once in a 4-6 months period (Saps et al., 2009a). For all other measures, except for the CSI, normative data are presented in table 2. As depicted in table 2, most of the effect sizes are small, and two approach a medium size (range .06-.40). None of the effect sizes were large. As no other studies calculated the CSI scores without items about AP in healthy children, no normative data was available.

Age and genderAbdominal pain correlated significantly with age (r = -.174; p <.001) and gender (boy coded as 0 and girl coded as 1; r = .347; p <.001). Therefore, we decided to include gender and age as control variables in a first step of the hierarchical MRA. Moreover, as the correlation with gender was substantial and gender was found to be related to 11 out of the 15 other investigated variables as well, it was decided to check for any interaction effects of gender by including the interaction terms between gender and all of the co-morbid complaints in a third step of the MRA. As the correlation between age and AP was much smaller, it was decided not to check for any interaction effects of age.

Physical illnessAs shown in table 1, parents were asked whether their child had a physical illness (with the further specification that this should not just be a common cold or the flu) which affected their child at the moment. Eight parents responded that their child had a physical illness that might be related to the AP (e.g., celiac disease, food allergies, trouble urinating). The analyses were performed with and without these eight cases, and it was found that they did not influence the results of the analyses. Therefore, it was decided to perform the analyses including these eight cases.

Statistical assumptionsFinally, some statistical assumptions were checked. First it was investigated whether the co-morbid complaints showed any signs of multicollinearity. Although the complaints were mostly significantly related, there was no evidence of statistical problems due to multicollinearity as the correlations were not higher than .9 (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). Also, the collinearity diagnostics printed for the MRA did not show any problems. Finally, the data were scanned for cases that were multivariate outliers, which resulted in deletion of one case due to a conspicuous score on the CSI (a maximum score on all items).

Main analysesTo investigate the univariate relationships between AP and the co-morbid complaints, Pearson correlations were calculated (table 3). Significant positive correlations were found for activity limitations, other somatic complaints, anxious and depressive symptoms, indicating that an increase in AP coincides with an increase in these complaints. Negative significant correlations were found for physical and psychological quality of life and quality of life related to parent relations and autonomy, indicating that more AP was related to a

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 35 18-07-12 17:19

Page 39: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

36 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

diminished quality of life in these domains. No relationships were found between AP and parental anxiety, depression and somatization, nor did AP have significant correlations with the social or school domains of quality of life.

Table 2. Cronbach’s alpha, mean scores (standard deviations) and range of all measures. Comparison of mean with normative data.

α Mean (SD) Range Mean (SD) of norm group

Effect size d of difference with norm group c

Children a

Abdominal pain .91 11.01 (11.07) 0-43 - -Activity limitations .92 10.90 (11.69) 0-54 8.18 (7.82) .25Other somatic complaints - 14.29 (13.09) 0-112 - -Physical QoL .82 51.45 (10.84) 20.70-73.20 52.88 (10.02) .14Psychological QoL .77 49.04 (9.13) 24.84-73.53 52.79 (9.46) .40QoL: - Parent relations & autonomy .82 51.50 (10.58) 1.75-74.39 53.95 (9.51) .25QoL: social support & peers .83 51.77 (9.62) 11.24-66.34 52.36 (9.04) .06QoL: school environment .81 50.47 (9.82) 16.28-71.00 53.06 (9.71) .27Anxiety .88 7.92 (7.02) 0-55 10.04 (6.28) .31Depression .68 2.37 (2.12) 0-15 2.8 (2.0) .21

Parents b

Depression .87 19.39 (4.64) 16-44 12.76 (4.41) .31Anxiety .72 11.46 (2.16) 10-23 21.58 (7.56) .30Somatization .68 14.86 (3.06) 12-30 16.68 (5.34) .36

Note: QoL = Quality of Life. NS = not significant. a N varied from 637 to 661. b N was 386. c Normative data were derived from: FDI (Vervoort, Goubert, Eccleston, Bijttebier, & Crombez, 2005); Kidscreen (The KIDSCREEN Group Europe, 2006); RCADS: Anxiety (Muris, Meesters, de Kanter, & Timmerman, 2005), Depression (Muris et al., 2002); SCL-90 (Arrindell & Ettema, 2003). Effect size conventions: d = .20 - small; d = .50 - medium; d = .80 - large (Cohen, 1977).

Table 3. Pearson correlations between abdominal pain and co-morbid complaints.

Abdominal pain

r pChildren a

Activity limitations .392 <.001Other somatic complaints .408 <.001Physical QoL -.209 <.001Psychological QoL -.218 <.001QoL: - Parent relations & autonomy -.120 .003QoL: social support & peers -.025 .527QoL: school environment -.020 .614Anxiety .329 <.001Depression .361 <.001

Parents b

Depression -.045 .377Anxiety -.041 .426Somatization -.013 .801

a N varied from 633 to 655. b N was 385

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 36 18-07-12 17:19

Page 40: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

2

Physical and psychological co-morbid complaints in children and parents | 37

To investigate the multivariate relationships, a hierarchical MRA was performed (table 4). Parental depression, anxiety and somatization were left out of the analysis, because they were not significantly correlated with AP. The first step, including the variables age and gender, was found to add a significant amount of explained variance of 14.7%. In the second step, all child variables were entered. This step added an additional significant amount of explained variance of 15.3%. In the third step of the MRA, the interaction terms between gender and the variables of step 2 were included. This step did not add a significant amount of explained variance to the model, and thus, the model including step 1 and 2 was accepted as the final model. This model explained 30.0% of the variance of abdominal pain. Female sex, younger age, experiencing more limitations in activities, more somatic complaints and more depressive symptoms were all positively related to experiencing more AP.

Table 4. Hierarchical regression analysis, method enter

Abdominal pain

β p R2 change

Step 1 .147*Age -.077 .047Gender a .230 <.001

Step 2 .153*Activity limitations .247 <.001Other somatic complaints .170 .001Physical QoL -.038 .355Psychological QoL .005 .910QoL: - Parent relations & autonomy -.038 .395QoL: social support & peers .004 .916QoL: school environment .009 .843Anxiety -.012 .789Depression .093 .049

Step 3 (interaction gender and variables step 2) .018R2 model 2 .300Df model 2 (11, 620)F model 2 24.155*

a Boy is coded as 0, girl is coded as 1.*p<.001

Discussion

The aim of the present study was to investigate whether a number of co-morbid complaints often reported by children with FAP and their parents are specific for FAP, or can also be found in relatively healthy children suffering from more occasional AP. This study suggests that AP in children from the general population correlates with female gender, younger age, more limitations in activities, other somatic complaints like headache or fatigue, and more symptoms of depression. Relationships were also found for anxiety, physical and

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 37 18-07-12 17:19

Page 41: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

38 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

psychological quality of life and quality of life concerning parent relations and autonomy. However, when we corrected for the correlations between the co-morbid complaints, anxiety and these three quality of life domains proved to be redundant and did not have a unique relationship with the level of AP. Also, internalizing problems experienced by the parent were not related (neither univariately nor multivariately) to AP in this general sample. The associations for activity limitations, somatic complaints and depressive symptoms seem to be the same for boys and girls, as no interaction effects were found. These findings are partly in line with our hypotheses. One notable difference is the fact that none of the parental complaints were related to the AP reported by the child. This is inconsistent with results from clinical populations. These studies usually find that parents of children suffering from FAP report more anxiety, depression, and somatization symptoms (Ramchandani et al., 2002, 2005; Walker et al., 1991, 1995; Walker & Greene, 1989); thus the co-existence of parental complaints seems to be specific for FAP. This may suggest that having parents suffering from anxiety, depression and/or somatization might be a risk factor for developing FAP. However, it seems just as likely that having a child with FAP causes distress in the parents, which might be reflected in increased symptoms of anxiety, depression and somatization. Then, the parental complaints would not be a risk factor for FAP, but just a sign of the impact of FAP on the family. Note that these explanations are not mutually exclusive: the distress of having an ill child may trigger parental psychopathology, and these psychological problems may lead to parental behavior that facilitates FAP (social learning). Then, parental complaints may have an indirect influence on developing FAP, mediated by social learning processes. Indeed, an experimental study by Walker and colleagues showed that parental behavior can influence the pain perceived by children significantly (Walker et al., 2006). Unfortunately, much of the literature on parental modeling and social learning effects is not experimental or prospective but cross-sectional, which makes it impossible to sort out what is cause and what is effect. Future research could usefully focus further on the directionality of the relationship between social learning processes and FAP. Another important discrepancy with existing literature is that when other somatic complaints, limitations in activities and depressive symptoms are taken into account, quality of life and anxiety are no longer significantly related to AP in this sample from the general population. Thus, the significant small to moderate correlations that we found for quality of life and anxiety are probably due to their shared variance with the other co-morbid complaints, as these variables did not uniquely contribute to the explained variance of AP. As for quality of life, this may not be a very surprising finding, since it seems likely that generic quality of life only diminishes if a child has an illness or condition he/she is confronted with daily, which was not the case for most of the children in the current sample. However, the results for anxiety are striking. With high co-morbidity rates of up to 79% in clinical samples (Campo et al., 2004), one would expect to find a strong and unique

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 38 18-07-12 17:19

Page 42: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

2

Physical and psychological co-morbid complaints in children and parents | 39

relationship between anxiety and AP in a general sample also, even when other variables are controlled for. This discrepancy might be explained in several ways. First, it could be that having FAP (in stead of only minor AP) can cause the child to become anxious, because of worrying about it’s own health or the social isolation often seen in these children. However, the anxiety disorders reported in, for example, the article by Campo and colleagues have a broad range and do not solely concern types of anxiety that can be directly caused by the AP, like generalized anxiety disorder or specific phobia (constant worrying about or being afraid of the AP) (Campo et al., 2004). Therefore, it seems more plausible that anxiety might be an actual risk factor for developing FAP, or that anxiety and FAP are both caused by similar mechanisms, either psychological or biological, but do not cause each other. However, it can also be questioned whether the co-morbidity between FAP and anxiety is really this high. For example, it might be the case that having a co-morbid anxiety disorder is a reason to visit a health care professional sooner, a phenomenon that has been described in adults (Van der Veek, 2009). Then, the high co-morbidity of anxiety disorders is just an artifact of gathering data from hospitals, and is not really this high in all FAP patients, including those not seeking professional health care. Indeed, it should be noted that only the study performed by Campo and colleagues found such a high co-morbidity rate for anxiety. Other studies find relationships between these two variables as well, but these are usually smaller. A meta-analysis might prove useful in sorting out the precise relationship between anxiety and FAP. This study also has some limitations that should be addressed. Firstly, the response rate of our survey was rather small. Although this is not uncommon for studies of children in the general population that depend on the consent of parents and are limited in the time available to include participants, it may have caused attrition bias. However, this concern is mitigated by the results of our comparisons with normative data, as the rate of AP was comparable to a number of American studies (Ghandour, Overpeck, Huang, Kogan, & Scheidt, 2004; Saps et al., 2009a). Also, the effect sizes of the differences in scores of our other measures with normative data are mostly small. A second limitation of our study is that all measures used were self-report questionnaires, which may have caused some bias. Finally, it should be noted that for a considerable number of children, no parental data were available, and thus we had no information on any physical illnesses they might have. Also, it is not certain that all organic causes of AP were identified by the open-ended question for parents. However, as only eight (2%) of the participating parents reported a physical illness that might relate to the AP and as these eight cases did not influence the results, we do not think that this limitation constitutes a major problem. It should be noted that the parents that did respond to the questionnaire were mostly female and of Dutch nationality. It might be interesting to investigate the specific influence of paternal complaints and participants of other nationalities on the AP of the child. Despite these limitations, the strength of the current study is that it is the first to

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 39 18-07-12 17:19

Page 43: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

40 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

investigate a multitude of co-morbid complaints simultaneously in a large sample of children from the general population. We have shown that a) parental anxiety, depression and somatization, and b) anxiety and a diminished quality of life experienced by the children themselves, are not related to AP in this sample from the general population. Thus, these might be specific co-morbidity patterns for children with FAP. Whether these complaints are risk factors of FAP or are simply ‘collateral damage’ due to having a disabling condition, is an important question for future research.

AcknowledgementsWe would like to thank all psychology students from Leiden University who helped with the data collection.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 40 18-07-12 17:19

Page 44: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 41 18-07-12 17:19

Page 45: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 42 18-07-12 17:19

Page 46: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

Ch

apte

r

3Do parents maintain or

exacerbate pediatric functional abdominal pain?

A systematic review and meta-analysis

The contents of this article are published in:

Van der Veek, S. M. C., Derkx, H. H. F., de Haan, E., Benninga, M. A., Plak, R. D., & Boer, F. (2012). Do parents

maintain or exacerbate pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of

Health Psychology, 17, 258-272 .

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 43 18-07-12 17:19

Page 47: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

44 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Abstract

Objectives: Parents may maintain or exacerbate functional abdominal pain (FAP) in children through modeling of physical symptoms and solicitous responses to the child’s complaints. This systematic review and meta-analysis aimed to examine these relationships. Method: A systematic review and meta-analysis was performed by searching the databases MEDLINE, PsycINFO, EMBASE and Web of Science. Results: Parents of children with FAP reported more physical symptoms than parents of healthy children (effect size d = .36). As all studies were cross-sectional or retrospective, the causal direction was unclear. For parental responses to child complaints, not enough studies were available to perform a meta-analysis. Discussion: We conclude that the present literature is unfit to establish whether parents maintain or exacerbate pediatric FAP through the processes investigated.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 44 18-07-12 17:19

Page 48: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

3

Parental influences on FAP | 45

Introduction

Recurrent abdominal pain (AP) affects about 10% of school-aged children in Western countries and is therefore a large health problem (Chitkara et al., 2005). In most cases no organic abnormalities can be found to explain the pain, and thus the pain is considered ´functional´ (FAP) (Rasquin et al., 2006). Although no physical diseases are found in children with FAP, their daily functioning is usually significantly impaired (Campo et al., 2004; Claar & Walker, 2006). To develop effective treatments, it is crucial to have a thorough understanding of the factors that cause and maintain FAP. Unfortunately, however, the etiology of FAP is still unclear. Throughout the literature the etiology of FAP is most often viewed within a biopsychosocial model. In the biopsychosocial model, biological factors like visceral hypersensitivity and psychological factors like coping style play a major role (Di Lorenzo et al., 2005; Walker et al., 2008). A crucial social factor is the influence of parents on FAP. Although parents might also contribute to FAP biologically by passing on a genetic vulnerability – or, to use another term, through ‘nature’ – it has been suggested that their behavior (nurture) has an equal, if not larger influential role (Levy et al., 2001). The social learning theory proposed by Bandura (Bandura, 1977) explicates how this ‘nurture’ influence might work. Human behavior is for a large part learned by observing behavior modeled by persons in the environment. Whether modeled behavior is adopted by the observer depends on several factors, such as the number of times the behavior is modeled, or consequences of the behavior. Parents play a crucial role in two of these factors. Firstly, parents are the primary models for children. With regard to FAP, this implies that parents who experience many physical symptoms themselves, may model a focus on physical symptoms to their children. Secondly, parents shape the behavior of their children with the feedback they provide. Children will more likely adopt modeled behavior if it results in positive outcomes than if it has unrewarding or punishing effects (Bandura, 1977). Thus according to social learning theory, if parents react to FAP by, for example, allowing the child to stay home from school and watch television, this parental behavioral response might maintain or exacerbate FAP. Pioneering work by Whitehead and colleagues in the 1980’s (Whitehead, Busch, Heller, & Costa, 1986; Whitehead, Winget, Fedoravicius, Wooley, & Blackwell, 1982) has led to many publications on both the occurrence of physical symptoms in parents of children with FAP and the way parents react behaviorally when their child complains of AP. The results of these studies are mixed, and although some expert reviews have been published on this topic (Levy, 2011; Levy, Langer, & Whitehead, 2007; Palermo & Eccleston, 2009), up till now no systematic or quantitative reviews have been performed. As such it is unclear whether these social learning processes show robust relationships with FAP over multiple studies. Another important issue that remains unclear after almost thirty years of research,

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 45 18-07-12 17:19

Page 49: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

46 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

is how these factors specifically relate to FAP. Do they contribute to the etiology of FAP or are they just epiphenomena, disappearing when the pain is resolved? Although it is a common belief among practitioners and scientists that social learning processes play a maintaining, exacerbating or even causal role in the etiology of FAP, it has never been reviewed whether this is actually the case. Thus, the present study systematically reviewed and meta-analyzed the literature on pediatric FAP to investigate both the magnitude and direction of the relationship between social learning processes and FAP. There are a number of factors that could influence this relationship which should be considered when conducting a meta-analysis. For the present study we focused on both methodological parameters (methodological quality of the investigated studies, type of instrument used to measure social learning) and the theoretical parameters of parent gender, definition of FAP, and – specific for physical symptoms – type and moment of parental symptoms. The influence of parent gender was selected as a possible moderator, as previous studies showed that the relationship between parental and child complaints might be different for mothers and fathers (Walker & Greene, 1989). Second, definition of FAP was selected because the term FAP has been used rather loosely to describe definitions ranging from ‘abdominal pain without a physical cause’ to definitions specifying duration and severity of the complaints. Third, as modeling effects might be specific to or stronger for the exact physical symptom that is portrayed by the parent (Buonavolonta et al., 2010; Levy, Whitehead, Von Korff, & Feld, 2000; Whitehead et al., 1994), type of physical symptom was included as a moderator. Moreover, as it can be expected that more current displays of physical symptoms will have larger modeling effects than physical symptoms displayed in the past (Bandura, 1977), the moment of parental symptoms was included as a final moderator. In summary, the purpose of this systematic review and meta-analysis was to investigate: 1) the magnitude of the relationship between parental physical symptoms/behavioral reactions and FAP in children and adolescents aged 4-18; 2) whether existing data can assess the causal direction of this relationship; and 3) whether the results of the studies are influenced by methodological moderators (methodological quality and type of instrument used to measure parental physical symptoms/behavioral reactions), and by the theoretical moderators of parent gender, definition of FAP, and – specific for physical symptoms – type and moment of parental symptoms.

Method

Search strategy The MEDLINE, PsycINFO, EMBASE and SCI-EXPANDED, SSCI and A&HCI (Web of Science) databases were searched up to 4 January 2011. In MEDLINE, PsychINFO and EMBASE,

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 46 18-07-12 17:19

Page 50: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

3

Parental influences on FAP | 47

the thesaurus was used in combination with distinct key words. In Web of Science, no thesaurus is available, so the search was conducted by making use of key words only. Examples of key words used are (asterisks were used to truncate search terms): social learning, modeling, solicitous*, observational learning, abdominal pain*, irritable bowel syndrome*, functional dyspepsia, adolescent*, youth*, children. The specific search strings can be requested from the first author (SV). The reference lists of the relevant retrieved articles were searched for additional studies. Also, relevant reviews retrieved by the search were checked for additional studies.

Study selectionThe ‘PICO’ system (Population/Problem, Intervention, Comparison, Outcome; Richardson, Wilson, Nishikawa, & Hayward, 1995) was used to define the study selection criteria. An article was eligible if it met all of the following criteria: (P) An empirical study presenting original data from children and/or adolescents (aged 4-18) or adults (in the case of a retrospective study); (I) concerning the effects of parental physical symptoms (occurring at the same time as the child’s pain or preceding the child’s pain) or parental behavioral reactions to the child’s pain; (O) on FAP (thus: either case-control studies comparing children with FAP and healthy children, or studies without control group in which a correlation between the measure of social learning and a measure of severity of FAP is calculated). Exclusion criteria were: Case studies, no information reported on whether a medical cause was excluded or not, social learning behavior of others than parents or primary caregivers (e.g., siblings, grandparents). Note that the description of patients does not restrict the selected studies to research on clinical samples presenting to a hospital only, as a sole focus on children seeking medical consultation for FAP might bias the results. Similarly, the ‘C’ of the PICO system was not specified, as we also wanted to include studies that do not make use of a control group. No language restrictions were set beforehand.

Methodological quality assessmentTwo reviewers (SV and RP) independently scored the quality of the studies. As no ‘golden standard’ for assessing the methodological quality of studies with varying research designs was available, we based our choice on a review by Sanderson and colleagues (Sanderson, Tatt, & Higgins, 2007) and chose the checklist published by Fowkes and Fulton (Fowkes & Fulton, 1991) as a starting point. Five topics were reviewed, with several sub criteria per topic: (1) representativeness of the sample (reliable source and sampling method is adequate /

clear and relevant in- and exclusion criteria and demographic characteristics / adequate responsrate);

(2) representativeness of the control group (if used; control group consists of well children / control group has comparable demographic characteristics as clinical group);

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 47 18-07-12 17:19

Page 51: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

48 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

(3) quality of the instruments measuring social learning and quality of the instruments used to diagnose FAP or the procedure to exclude an identifiable organic cause of FAP (for both: a validated instrument is used / the procedure is reproducible / reliability of instrument is reported and sufficient);

(4) completeness of the data (drop outs and missing data do not influence the results); and (5) whether or not the raw data necessary to calculate an effect size was available. Answer options were ‘yes’ (1 point), ‘no’ (0 points), and ‘don’t know’ (0 points). A total score for methodological quality was calculated by averaging the sub criteria per topic and then adding the scores for each topic. This resulted in a possible range of 0 (very low quality) to 5 (very high quality). Disagreement between the reviewers was resolved through consensus.

Data extractionSV extracted the data from the retrieved articles. Data concerning the sample, design, instruments used to measure social learning, the way in which an organic cause for the AP was excluded, and the outcomes were gathered on a standardized form. Data from studies that were suitable for meta-analysis were verified by RP.

Data analysisThe available studies were first screened for eligibility for meta-analysis. To be eligible, a sufficient number of effect sizes (ES) had to be available (at least 10) from separate studies with a comparable research design. If a meta-analysis could be performed, an overall ES was calculated across the studies, which was tested for significance, using the macro for SPSS written by David Wilson (Wilson, 2005). A homogeneity analysis was performed to check if the ES across studies were homogeneous. The influence of the methodological and theoretical moderators was investigated by performing a meta-regression (Lipsey & Wilson, 2001), making use of David Wilson’s (Wilson, 2005) macro for meta-analysis analog to ANOVA for the categorical moderators, and his macro for modified weighted regression analysis for the continuous moderator of methodological quality.

Results

Search resultsIn total, the search strategy resulted in 731 citations (MEDLINE 234; PsychINFO 114; EMBASE 179; Web of Science 204). After removing doubles, the titles and abstracts were scanned for relevance. 94 Articles were retrieved in full; of these, 34 fulfilled the inclusion criteria. Searching for references in reviews retrieved in the original search resulted in an additional 6 studies that fulfilled the inclusion criteria. Upon further examination, 21 articles were excluded because they did not investigate whether there was an organic cause for the

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 48 18-07-12 17:19

Page 52: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

3

Parental influences on FAP | 49

AP of the child (Apley & Naish, 1958; Bode, Brenner, Adler, & Rothenbacher, 2003; Cardol et al., 2006; Christensen & Mortensen, 1975; Craig, Cox, & Klein, 2002; Crane & Martin, 2004; Groholt, Stigum, Nordhagen, & Kohler, 2003; Helgeland, Sandvik, Mathiesen, & Kristensen, 2010; Hommel et al., 2010; Jamison & Walker, 1992; Langer et al., 2007; Levy et al., 2004; Levy et al., 2000; Lipani & Walker, 2006; Mortimer, Kay, Jaron, & Good, 1992; Ramchandani et al., 2006; Saunders, Von Korff, LeResche, & Mancl, 2007; Stanford, Chambers, Biesanz, & Chen, 2008; Walker, Levy, & Whitehead, 2006; Whitehead et al., 1994; Whitehead et al., 1982). Three articles were excluded from the analyses for parental physical symptoms because they did not solely focus on parents (Kramer & Petermann, 2005; Routh & Ernst, 1984; Walker et al., 1993). Finally, 10 studies reporting data on parental physical symptoms, 6 studies on parental behavioral reactions, and 1 on both, were included, making a total of 11 studies for parental physical symptoms and 7 studies for parental behavioral reactions. These studies are indicated in the reference list by means of an asterisk.

Parental physical symptoms Study characteristics

The characteristics of the studies included for parental physical symptoms are shown in table 1 and 2. Ten out of the 11 studies had a cross-sectional design and 9 were performed in clinical samples from secondary or tertiary hospital clinics. One study had a retrospective design and asked adults about the physical symptoms of their parents when they were children. Nine out of the 11 studies used self-report questionnaires to measure parental physical symptoms. In total, the studies investigated parental physical symptoms in 481 clinical children and 3846 control children. As for three studies the number of participating parents was unknown, it is not clear how many parents participated in total in these studies.

Calculation of effect sizesAs a sufficient number of studies was available, the results for this search were meta-analyzed. To do this, one standardized mean difference ES d was calculated for each study. If more than one ES was available for a study because it measured the same kind of symptoms in both mothers and fathers, an average of the different ES was calculated (Garber et al., 1990; Robinson et al., 1990; Walker & Greene, 1989). If both an ES for parental gastrointestinal (GI) symptoms and for other physical symptoms was available, then the ES concerning GI symptoms was selected (Campo et al., 2007; Kaufman et al., 1997; Robinson et al., 1990; Venepalli et al., 2006). For the study by Garber and colleagues (Garber et al., 1990), a measure of a history of somatization, a history of psychogenic pain and current somatization was available. However, only the current somatization measure provided raw data to calculate an ES. We contacted the authors to gain the required information, but unfortunately, this request stayed unanswered. Therefore, only the ES for current somatization was used for this study.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 49 18-07-12 17:19

Page 53: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

50 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Tab

le 1

. Stu

dy c

hara

cter

istic

s of

all

incl

uded

stu

dies

.

Stud

yTy

pe s

ocia

l le

arni

ng

N c

hild

N p

aren

tTy

pe s

ourc

eD

esig

nO

rigin

sam

ple

Excl

usio

n m

edic

al

caus

e%

Mal

e ch

ildG

ende

r pa

rent

Type

FA

P

Cam

po e

t al.,

2007

PS

AP:

59

C: 7

6A

P: 5

9C

: 76

AC

ross

2nd

Cha

rtTo

tal:

401

1

Czy

zew

ski e

t al.,

2007

PSA

P: 3

5C

: 38

Tota

l: 12

0A

Cro

ss2nd

and

3rd

C

hart

AP:

74.

3C

: 55.

31

1

Gar

ber e

t al.,

1990

PSA

P: 1

3C

: 16

-A

Cro

ss2nd

Ph

ys e

xam

-2

2

Hot

opf e

t al.,

1998

PS

AP:

71

C: 3

488

-A

Cro

ssM

RC

Cha

rtTo

tal:

± 5

02

3

Kauf

man

et a

l., 19

97

PS

AP:

24

C: 1

9A

P: 2

4C

: 19

AC

ross

3rd

Phys

exa

mA

P: 3

8C

: 37

11

Liak

opou

lou-

Kairi

s et

al.,

2002

PS

AP:

38

C: 6

0A

P: 3

8C

: 60

AC

ross

2nd a

nd 3

rd

Cha

rtA

P: 3

7.7

C: 5

6.7

11

Low

man

et a

l., 19

87PS

AP:

125

C: 7

9N

AA

Retr

o*

Phys

exa

mA

P: 1

2C

: 10

22

McG

rath

et a

l., 19

83

PS

AP:

30

C: 3

0A

P: 5

3C

: 51

AC

ross

3rd

Phys

exa

mTo

tal:

43.3

21

Robi

nson

et a

l., 19

90

PS

AP:

40

C: 4

0A

P: 8

0C

: -A

Cro

ss2nd

and

sch

ools

Phys

exa

mA

P: 4

7.5

C: 4

7.5

21

Wal

ker &

Gre

ene,

198

9PS

AP:

41

C: 4

1A

P: 4

1C

: 41

AC

ross

2nd a

nd 3

rd

Phys

exa

mA

P: 5

3.7

C: 5

3.7

22

Vene

palli

et a

l., 20

06

PS +

BR

AP:

81

C: 3

6A

P: 8

1C

: 36

AC

ross

Scho

ols

Pare

nt re

port

Tota

l: 50

.41

1

Lang

er e

t al.,

2009

BRA

P: 1

32A

P: 1

32A

Cro

ss1st

, 2nd

, 3rd

and

co

mm

unity

Phys

exa

mA

P: 3

2.6

21

Levy

et a

l., 20

10BR

AP:

200

AP:

200

ARC

T1st

, 2nd

, 3rd

and

co

mm

unity

Phys

exa

mA

P: 2

7.5

21

Pelz

er, 1

997

BRA

P: 2

5C

: 36

NA

Dis

sRe

tro

1st, 2

nd a

nd 3

rd

GE

AP:

24

C: 4

1.7

23

Wal

ker e

t al.,

1993

BRA

P: 8

8C

: 56

-A

Cro

ss2nd

Ph

ys e

xam

Tota

l: 44

.61

2

Wal

ker e

t al.,

2006

BRA

P: 1

10C

: 120

AP:

110

C: 1

20A

Exp

3rd

Phys

exa

mA

P: 4

0C

: 50

21

Wen

dlan

d et

al.,

2010

BRA

P: 1

00A

P: 1

00A

Cro

ss3rd

Cha

rtA

P: 3

51

2

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 50 18-07-12 17:19

Page 54: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

3

Parental influences on FAP | 51

Note to table 1. PS=parental physical symptoms. BR=behavioral reactions. C=control group. - = not reported. NA=Not applicable. A=article. Diss=dissertation. Cross = cross-sectional. RCT=randomized controlled trial. Retro = retrospective. Exp = experimental.1st = through GP practices. 2nd = through secondary care clinics. 3rd = through tertiary gastroenterology clinics. MRC=MRC national survey of health and development from 1946. *Controls were recruited through people working in medical community not seeing physicians, IBS through secondary care clinics and student health services. Chart=by chart review. Phys exam=through physical examination. GE=patients were diagnosed by gastroenterologist, not specified if gastroenterologist used chart review or physical examination. Gender parent: 1=only mothers; 2=both mothers and fathers. Type FAP: 1=FAP for at least 3 months; 2=FAP for at least 1 month; 3=FAP without specification of duration.

The raw data reported in these studies were either means or frequencies. To calculate one standardized mean ES d, the differences in frequencies had to be transformed to be comparable to the differences in means. For this purpose, the d

bis transformation was

chosen (Sánchez-Meca, Marín-Martínez, & Chacón-Moscoso, 2003). For three studies, no sufficient raw data were available to calculate part of (Garber et al., 1990; Walker & Greene, 1989) the ES, or the entire ES (Lowman, Drossman, Cramer, & McKee, 1987). In all of these cases, the studies reported that the effect was not significant. Therefore, an ES of 0 was imputed. Although this may underestimate the actual value of the overall population ES by creating a downward bias, this strategy might also compensate for the well-known effect that non significant results are more likely to remain unpublished (the file-drawer problem), which can create an upward-bias. The ES per study are shown in table 2. For interpretation purposes, the general conventions of Cohen can be used: d = .20 - small; d = .50 - medium; d = .80 - large (Cohen, 1977). No outlying effect sizes were identified.

Methodological qualityThe results of the methodological quality assessment are shown in table 2. The two reviewers SV and RP agreed on 88.81% of the items, resulting in a good interobserver reliability of κ = .750. The quality assessment showed a number of important shortcomings of the studies:a) only 2 out of the 11 studies reported some information on response rate (Hotopf et al.,

1998; Robinson et al., 1990); b) no studies reported the reliability of the instruments used to measure social learning

and to diagnose FAP in the sample at hand;c) only 1 out of the 11 studies used a validated instrument to diagnose FAP (Lowman et

al., 1987), and only 5 out of the 11 studies provided enough information to be able to reproduce their diagnosis of FAP (Czyzewski, Eakin, Lane, Jarrett, & Shulman, 2007; Garber et al., 1990; Kaufman et al., 1997; McGrath, Goodman, Firestone, Shipman, & Peters, 1983; Walker & Greene, 1989).

d) only 5 of the 11 studies used control groups for which we were fairly certain they consisted of healthy children (Hotopf et al., 1998; Lowman et al., 1987; Robinson et al., 1990; Venepalli et al., 2006; Walker & Greene, 1989).

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 51 18-07-12 17:19

Page 55: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

52 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

A strong point of the studies was the absence of considerable missing data in 7 of the studies. On average, the methodological quality of the studies was 2.86 (range 1.83-3.73). None received the maximum possible score of 5.

Table 2. Scores of moderator variables, methodological quality and ES per study on parental physical symptoms.

Study Type of parental symptom

Moment of parental symptom

Type of instrument MQ (range 0-5) ES

Campo et al., 2007

GI symptoms Both current and history

Validated 2.83 .90

Czyzewski et al., 2007

Somatisation Current Validated 3.17 .93

Garber et al., 1990 Somatisation Current Validated 2.33 .34Hotopf et al., 1998

Other physical complaints

Unclear Not validated 3.67 .64

Kaufman et al., 1997

GI symptoms History Not validated 3.00 .45

Liakopoulou-Kairis et al., 2002

Somatisation Current Validated 2.67 -.20

Lowman et al., 1987 GI symptoms History Not validated 2.00 .00McGrath et al., 1983

Other physical complaints

Current Not validated 1.83 -.15

Robinson et al., 1990

GI symptoms Unclear Both validated and not validated

3.73 .18

Venepalli et al., 2006

GI symptoms Current Both validated and not validated

3.44 .51

Walker & Greene, 1989 Somatisation Current Validated 2.83 .42

Note: MQ = methodological quality; GI = gastrointestinal

Meta-analysis and meta-regressionTo investigate whether the overall ES of the different studies was significant and homogeneous across studies, a meta-analysis was performed (table 3). A significant small-to-medium overall ES d of .36 (95% CI: .13-.60; random effects model; p =.002) was found, indicating that parents of children with FAP report more physical symptoms than parents of healthy children. The homogeneity analysis showed the set of ES to be heterogeneous (Q

within = 43.825; p<.001). We therefore performed a set of meta-regressions in order to

investigate whether our predefined moderators could explain this variance. In all of these analyses a mixed effects model was assumed, as this model has been shown to have a smaller risk on type I errors (Higgins & Thompson, 2004). None of the investigated categorical moderators could explain the variance in ES across studies (see table 3). For methodological quality, we did find a significant effect, indicating that studies receiving a higher score on methodological quality also reported higher ES (mixed effects model; Q

model (1) = 4.262, p =.039; Q

residual (9) = 10.598, p =.304; β =.54, p =.039; R2 =.29).

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 52 18-07-12 17:19

Page 56: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

3

Parental influences on FAP | 53

Table 3. Results meta-analyses for parental physical complaints, random and mixed effects models.

Moderator variables N K d 95% CI Q between Q withinOverall 4606 11 .36*** .13 - .60 43.825***Parent gender 1.172

Mother 511 5 .53** .21 - .85 6.720Both 4095 6 .24‡ -.03 - .51 3.820

Type of physical symptom .512GI symptoms 657 5 .39* .06 - .72 3.645Somatization 357 4 .37‡ -.01 - .75 4.402Other physical symptoms 3592 2 .29 -.20 - .78 2.425

Moment of parental symptoms 2.054Current 578 6 .31* .02 - .59 7.625History 247 2 .16 -.33 - .65 .761Unclear/both 3781 3 .57** .21 - .93 2.550

Definition of FAP .834FAP at least 1-3 months 1047 10 .33** .10 - .55 10.763Unspecified 3559 1 .64* .01 - 1.27 .000

Type of instrument used to measure parental symptoms

1.028

Validated 490 5 .49** .17 - .81 6.570Not validated/both 4116 6 .27 -.01 - .55 4.077

Note. d = standardized mean effect size d. Q between = variance in ES between groups defined by moderator. Q within = variance in ES within a group defined by moderator. ‡ p <.10; *p <.05; **p <.01; ***p <.001

Parental behavioral reactions to FAPAs not enough studies with a comparable design were available to perform a meta-analysis, data on parental behavioral reactions to FAP were systematically reviewed. Seven studies were found with a total N for children of 736 for the clinical group and 248 for the control group (table 1). The designs of these studies are quite diverse: four are cross-sectional (Langer, Romano, Levy, Walker, & Whitehead, 2009; Venepalli et al., 2006; Walker et al., 1993; Wendland, Jackson, & Stokes, 2010), one retrospective (Pelzer, 1997), one experimental (Walker et al., 2006) and one describes a randomized controlled trial (RCT) in which the effectiveness of a cognitive behavioral intervention based on social learning theory is tested (Levy et al., 2010). Table 4 shows the definitions of parental behavioral reactions and the results of the seven studies. As can be seen in this table, the way behavioral reactions were defined ranged from punishment for problems related to bowel movements (Pelzer, 1997) to solicitous behavior (Venepalli et al., 2006; Walker et al., 1993; Wendland et al., 2010). The experimental study, the RCT and one of the cross-sectional studies reported some positive relationships between more rewarding parental reactions to FAP and more AP (Levy et al., 2010; Walker et al., 2006; Walker et al., 1993). Two cross-sectional studies found no significant relationship between either protective or solicitous parental behavior and pain severity (Langer et al., 2009; Wendland et al., 2010). Venepalli and colleagues (Venepalli et al., 2006) found that mothers of children with FAP score higher on maternal minimizing/distracting behavior than mothers of healthy children. Finally, no relationship was found for punishing behavior (Pelzer, 1997).

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 53 18-07-12 17:19

Page 57: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

54 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

For the research question on whether existing data can assess the causal direction of the relationship between parental behavioral reactions and FAP, two out of the seven retrieved studies had a methodological design that had the potential to answer such a question: the RCT by Levy and colleagues (Levy et al., 2010) and the experimental study by Walker and colleagues (Walker et al., 2006). These studies were thus further scrutinized to investigate whether a causal relationship could be established. Starting with the former, the researchers investigated the effectiveness of a CBT protocol based on social learning principles. When looking at the contents of the protocol more closely, however, we found it also incorporated more general strategies like relaxation training and coping skills training. The researchers did not investigate which part of the effect was due to the social learning principles used in the protocol. Although it was established that solicitous parental behavior improved after treatment, the authors did not investigate whether this improvement could explain any of the treatment effects. Thus, this study could not provide evidence that social learning has a causal relationship with FAP. The latter study by Walker and colleagues investigated the effects of parental attention or distraction in an experimental study (Walker et al., 2006). In this experiment, children first had to drink water until they felt completely full, which caused gastrointestinal symptoms. Parents were trained either to give attention to their child’s complaints or to distract their children. Whether this affected how much complaints a child experienced, was measured by observing the parent-child interaction for five minutes and scoring how often the child verbally complained about symptoms. In addition, children filled out a self-report questionnaire about gastrointestinal complaints when the parent-child interaction had ended. The design of this experiment is thus truly equipped to investigate whether there is a causal relationship between parental behavior and child complaints. The investigators found that if parents distracted their children from their AP, these children made less utterances of pain than if parents were instructed to pay attention to their child’s AP. Thus, it seems that distracting your child when it experiences AP is a positive reaction. However, this effect was not found when the level of AP experienced by the child was measured by a self-report questionnaire instead of by observing utterances of pain.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 54 18-07-12 17:19

Page 58: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

3

Parental influences on FAP | 55

Table 4. Results of studies on parental behavioral reactions to pediatric FAP.

Study Definition of behavioral reaction ResultsLanger et al., 2009

Parental protectiveness (90.2% mothers) in response to abdominal pain symptoms of the child. (validated instrument)

No significant relationship between severity of pain and protectiveness when child age, gender and parental catastrophizing were controlled for.

Levy et al., 2010

Effect of a CBT treatment based on social learning principles on GI symptoms and pain.

FACES pain scale: no difference between treatment and control group according to child report, significant difference according to parent report in favor of CBT.GI symptom severity: CBT treatment is superior to attention-control group according to child and parent report directly after treatment, but not at 3 and 6 months follow-up.

Pelzer, 1997

Whether or not adults remembered being punished as a child for problems related to bowel movements (not validated instrument)

No significant difference between the group with IBS patients and a control group of medical patients without IBS.

Venepalli et al., 2006

Maternal solicitous, monitoring/distracting and minimizing/distracting behavior in response to GI complaints of the child (validated instrument)

No significant difference between RAP and controls on solicitous and monitoring/distracting behavior. Mothers of RAP patients score significantly higher on minimizing/distracting behavior.

Walker et al., 1993

Maternal solicitous behavior in response to GI complaints of the child(validated instrument)

According to child report, RAP group scores higher than control group. According to mother report, no significant difference.

Walker et al., 2006

Children were subjected to Water Load Symptom Provocation Task to induce AP, parents were asked to react to symptoms of child by 1. distracting child;2. paying attention to child’s complaints. This was compared to giving parents no instruction.

Children in the attention condition expressed significantly more symptom complaints than children in the distraction condition. There was no effect of condition on the level of GI symptoms reported before and after the experiment on a self-report questionnaire.

Wendland et al., 2010

Child report of maternal solicitous behavior in response to GI complaints of the child(validated instrument)

No significant relationship between child report of level of pain and child report of maternal solicitous behavior.

Discussion

The present structured review and meta-analysis shows that parents of children with FAP report more physical symptoms than parents of healthy children. Quantifying the relationship between parental symptoms and FAP over multiple studies for the first time, our meta-analysis showed a significant effect which was small-to-medium in size (ES d = .36). However, as all studies were either cross-sectional or retrospective, the direction of this effect could not be assessed. For parental behavioral reactions it wasn’t possible to calculate the magnitude of the relationship with FAP, as not enough studies were available to perform a meta-analysis. Only one study assessed the causal direction of the relationship between this type of social learning and FAP. As the results of one study are not sufficient to draw a general conclusion, it is clear that the present literature cannot

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 55 18-07-12 17:19

Page 59: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

56 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

ascertain whether the common belief that parents may cause, maintain or exacerbate pediatric FAP is accurate. Importantly, this is in contrast with the conclusion of a recent expert review, which concluded that ‘parental response to child pain behaviors appears to

be a key factor in the development and maintenance of FAP’ (Levy, 2011, pp. 1). However, this review did not critically evaluate whether the designs of the studies included in the review could answer such a question about etiology. In fact, the expert based its conclusion solely on own – mostly observational – work, and on the same single experimental study we found in our structured review. Although the theory that parents may influence the etiology of FAP may still hold true, our systematic review clearly shows that the present literature is not equipped to support it. Our last research question focused on whether methodological or theoretical moderators could explain differences across the studies. Although we did find that the effect sizes for parental physical symptoms differed significantly across studies, the only moderator that could explain some of this variance was methodological quality – studies that scored higher on methodological quality reported a larger effect size. The results for type of parental symptoms were especially surprising. Apparently, it does not matter whether parents suffer from gastrointestinal symptoms, somatization or other physical symptoms - the effect sizes for these different types of symptoms did not significantly differ from one another. This raises the question whether parents model specific types of illness behavior or illness behavior in general, which is for some reason expressed by these children primarily through abdominal complaints instead of other physical symptoms. However, it can be questioned whether the operationalization of modeling as ‘parental self-reports of physical symptoms’, which is used so often in the literature, is appropriate. Modeling can only occur if parents actually display physical symptoms to their child, which parents may or may not do even if they do report that they have physical symptoms. Nine out of the eleven studies investigated here measured parental physical symptoms by self-report, and we found no studies which asked parents to rate how often they displayed physical symptoms to their child, or children to rate how often their parents experienced physical symptoms, which both would be more sensitive measures of modeling. Also, stress might offer an alternative explanation to modeling for the found relationship between parental and child symptoms - either the children experience more stress because of having an ill parent and develop FAP as a consequence, or the parents experience more stress because of having an ill child, and experience somatic symptoms as a result (Palermo & Eccleston, 2009). Parents and their children might also experience common life stressors which lead to somatic symptoms in both the parent and the child.

Directions for future studiesOur study highlights a number of voids in the literature that need attention. The first obviously concerns the lack of studies with appropriate designs to sort out cause and

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 56 18-07-12 17:19

Page 60: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

3

Parental influences on FAP | 57

effect. Second, for more than half (52.5%) of the studies that met our inclusion criteria, it could not be established that the AP of the children was in fact functional. These studies mostly focused on recurrent AP in general, without excluding organic causes for AP. Although in most cases of recurrent AP an organic cause cannot be found, studies should still explicitly exclude children for which an organic cause can be found, or they might risk a considerable sampling bias. Third, only three studies on parental physical symptoms reported separate scores for mothers and fathers, and two out of these three did not report sufficient raw data to calculate a separate effect size for fathers (Garber et al., 1990; Robinson et al., 1990; Walker & Greene, 1989). Therefore, it was impossible to establish the contribution of each parent to the total effect. Studies might do well to broaden their focus to incorporate fathers or secondary caregivers as well, especially as recent experimental research in adults has shown that people might be more likely to adopt behavior modeled by a same-sex model (Lorber, Mazzoni, & Kirsch, 2007; Mazzoni, Foan, Hyland, & Kirsch, 2010). Finally, we could find very few studies that focused on the second element of social learning we investigated, parental reactions to a child’s AP. Additionally, these studies had largely differing designs, which made it impossible to perform a meta-analysis on their combined results. Three out of the seven studies found a positive relationship between maladaptive parental reactions and FAP. Obviously, these results leave ample room for discussion on the relevance of this type of social learning for FAP. Indeed, studies in other (pediatric) populations have also questioned the relevance of solicitous parental reactions (Jellesma, Rieffe, Terwogt, & Westenberg, 2008; Palermo & Eccleston, 2009). The study by Walker and colleagues is especially relevant in this regard, as it is the only study with an appropriate design to sort out cause and effect (Walker et al., 2006). This study found that if parents distract their children from their AP, children make less utterances of pain than if parents are instructed to pay attention to their child’s AP. Thus, it seems that distracting your child when it experiences AP is a positive reaction. However, this effect was not found when the level of AP experienced by the child was measured by a self-report questionnaire instead of by observing utterances of pain. Thus, the question is whether these children actually experienced less AP when their parents distracted them, or that they merely learned that they should not complain about the pain and should stop mentioning it, as the authors also point out themselves (Walker et al., 2006). Clearly, more (experimental) research is needed to sort out what the relevance of this type of social learning process is for FAP. There are three issues this review did not focus on, which should be incorporated in future research. First, we have assumed that parents will model maladaptive illness behavior, while in fact parents may also react in adaptive ways to their own symptoms. If so, they might model adaptive coping behavior, and consequently have a beneficial influence on the symptoms of their children. Second, it should be investigated whether parental

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 57 18-07-12 17:19

Page 61: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

58 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

physical symptoms and behavioral reactions interact with each other. Bandura describes social learning as a process in which both of these factors have a separate influence, but also interact: the odds that modeled behavior is actually adopted by the child will increase when motivational processes like a parent ‘rewarding’ the child’s behavior are added to the equation (Bandura, 1977). Then, perhaps the effects of social learning are strongest in parents that both experience physical symptoms themselves, and reward their children when having AP. Finally, future studies would do well to investigate the way parents think about the complaints of their child. The current study focused on behavioral reactions which could be observed directly by the child, but this behavior is influenced by the thoughts, beliefs and worries parents have (Bonner & Finney, 1996; van Tilburg et al., 2006). Finally, it should be acknowledged that performing a systematic review and meta-analysis on a subject for which little empirical papers are available, has it’s limitations. For example, there was no room to explore alternative ways in which parents might influence the complaints of their child, as can be done in qualitative or expert reviews. We feel that this drawback is compensated, however, by the benefits of meta-analysis in this case, as it has clearly brought to light the lack of substantial evidence for causality that expert reviews have failed to underscore. Moreover, the meta-analytic approach adds new insights to the literature as it is the first meta-analysis performed, whereas other types of reviews on the relevance of social learning have appeared in abundance. We conclude that the present literature is not equipped to either confirm or refute the common belief that parents may cause, maintain or exacerbate pediatric FAP. Parents of children with FAP do experience somewhat more somatic complaints than parents of healthy children, but cause and effect cannot be established. Although no compelling evidence on causality is available at this point, the theory that parents influence FAP might still hold true, as evidence underlining the opposite is also lacking. Performing more longitudinal and experimental studies is essential if we want to determine what role social learning plays in the etiology of FAP. Only then can we judge whether treatments of pediatric FAP should attempt to focus on the influence of parents.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 58 18-07-12 17:19

Page 62: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 59 18-07-12 17:19

Page 63: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 60 18-07-12 17:19

Page 64: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

Ch

apte

r

4Emotion awareness and coping

in children with functional abdominal pain:

A controlled study

The contents of this chapter were published in:Van der Veek, S. M. C., Derkx, H. H. F., de Haan, E., Benninga, M. A., & Boer, F. (2012). Emotion awareness and

coping in children with Functional Abdominal Pain: A controlled study. Social Science and Medicine, 74, 112-119.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 61 18-07-12 17:19

Page 65: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

62 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Abstract

Objectives: Literature on somatization suggests that patients suffering from medically unexplained symptoms are less aware of their emotions and use maladaptive coping strategies when coping with everyday problems. In addition, coping is hypothesized to mediate between emotion awareness and medically unexplained symptoms. Scientific evidence for the relevance of this hypothesis for children with functional abdominal pain (FAP) is, however, lacking. Therefore, the purpose of the present study was to investigate this hypothesis in Dutch children with functional abdominal pain (FAP), aged 7-18 years. Method: Between April 2007 and April 2010, a total of 114 referred children with FAP, 235 schoolchildren without abdominal pain and 407 schoolchildren with some abdominal pain (AP) of diverse etiology filled out questionnaires concerning their pain, emotion awareness and coping. MANOVA was used to investigate group differences in emotional awareness and coping. Structural equation modeling was used to investigate the mediational role of coping. Results: The results showed that children with FAP scored significantly lower on most aspects of emotion awareness than children without AP, although these differences were small. Contrary to expectations, children with FAP were more aware of a link between emotions and bodily sensations than children without AP. As for coping, we found that children with FAP used avoidant coping more often than children without AP. Overall, children with FAP mostly did not differ in their emotional awareness and coping compared to children with some AP. Problem-focused coping had a small mediating effect for two aspects of emotion awareness. Discussion: We conclude that children with FAP show only small differences in emotion awareness and coping compared to children without AP, and are practically no different from children with some AP. Contrary to common belief, it can be questioned whether emotion awareness and general coping are useful targets for psychological treatments of FAP to focus on.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 62 18-07-12 17:19

Page 66: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

4

Emotion awareness, coping and FAP | 63

Introduction

Functional abdominal pain (FAP) is a common disorder in children and adolescents, and has a large impact on daily functioning (Chitkara et al., 2005; Di Lorenzo et al., 2005). According to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV), FAP can be classified under the diagnostic subgroup of somatoform disorders. In the absence of a somatic cause, researchers and practitioners have been searching for other etiologic factors to explain these complaints. One common thought is that patients with medically unexplained symptoms have problems in the conscious experience of emotions (Lumley, Stettner, & Wehmer, 1996; Sifneos, 1973). Why this would result in medically unexplained symptoms links back to one of the first definitions of emotional experience. As already acknowledged in the 19th century by James, an essential part of an emotional experience is sensing changes in the body – in fact, James suggested that an emotion is nothing more than that (Lambie & Marcel, 2002). However, as suggested by Schachter and Singer in the 1960’s, it is just as important for the emotional experience that these bodily changes are attributed to the right emotional cause (Lambie & Marcel, 2002); otherwise, how would a person be able to distinguish a heart rate raised by fear from a heart rate raised by running? If persons are unable to make such distinctions, as is suggested in persons with medically unexplained symptoms, they may be vulnerable to incorrectly attribute physical sensations to a physical disease and thus seek medical care for symptoms for which no physical disease can be found (Kooiman et al., 2004).

The phenomenon of having problems in the conscious experience of emotions was first described by Sifneos, who called it ‘alexithymia’ (Sifneos, 1973). Core components of alexithymia are difficulties with a) identifying emotions; b) verbally expressing emotions; and c) differentiating emotions from physiological sensations (Lumley, Gustavson, Partridge, & Labouvie-Vief, 2005). In later literature, the related concept of emotional awareness appeared, which includes the core components of alexithymia as well as attending to, identifying, and correctly labeling emotions in others (Lumley et al., 2005). Alexithymia and a low level of emotional awareness have been linked to numerous somatoform and psychiatric disorders, including FAP (e.g., Porcelli et al., 2004). However, only one study investigated emotional awareness in children with FAP, and although this study found some evidence that children with FAP have more difficulty identifying and verbally sharing their emotions, their sample was small and limited to late childhood/early adolescence (Jellesma et al., 2006). Therefore, the first aim of the current study was to investigate the emotional awareness of children with FAP in a larger and broader sample.

Another important issue that remains unclear, is through what processes alexithymia/low emotion awareness is related to physical complaints like FAP. Next to the direct pathway mentioned above, mediational pathways have been suggested making use of behavioral, physiological, cognitive and social concepts, although most of these are

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 63 18-07-12 17:19

Page 67: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

64 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

poorly tested (Lumley et al., 1996). Rieffe and colleagues (Rieffe et al., 2007) investigated emotional awareness in children, and suggested that the cognitive/behavioral concept of coping might constitute such a pathway, which is in accordance with the hypothesis that not being able to experience an emotion also hampers coping with it (Lambie & Marcel, 2002). Specifically, Rieffe and colleagues hypothesized that “a precise and elaborate

awareness […] enhances the likelihood of finding a more adaptive approach to the situation” (2007, pp. 96). Thus, poor emotion awareness might result in using maladaptive coping strategies, which in turn may lead to experiencing more somatic complaints like FAP.

Although this theory is plausible, empirical support is scarce. The second part of the hypothesis relating general coping strategies to somatic complaints has been investigated in only one single study for pediatric FAP. This study showed that children with FAP less often used accommodative coping strategies – a group of strategies that is usually found adaptive (Walker et al., 2007). The first part of Rieffe’s hypothesis, the relationship between emotional awareness and coping, has also received little attention in the literature. The few studies from other populations (e.g., young adult psychiatric patients, healthy adults, adult patients with myofascial pain) that did focus on this relationship found positive correlations between alexithymia/low emotion awareness and maladaptive coping strategies, and negative relations with adaptive coping strategies (Berrocal, Pennato, & Bernini, 2009; Lumley, Smith, & Longo, 2002; Nyklicek, Poot, & Van Opstal, 2010; Parker, Taylor, & Bagby, 1998). The fact that so few studies have focused on the relationship between emotion awareness and coping might best be attributed to the origins of these concepts, as alexithymia/emotional awareness stems from psychodynamic thinking and coping stems from stress-coping theory, which explicitly distances itself from traditional psycho-analytic theories (Lazarus & Folkman, 1984) and leans more toward cognitive-behavioral thinking. These differential backgrounds also form a problem: as both concepts concern some form of dealing with emotions, their definitions sometimes highly overlap. For example, alexithymia is most commonly viewed as a personality trait or – when an individual shows many alexithymic characteristics – a disorder, but alexithymia is sometimes also viewed as a coping strategy; in an effort to block out unwanted negative emotions, people might subconsciously block all emotional experiencing (Parker et al., 1998).

Whether or not emotional awareness/alexithymia and coping both belong to the same process, it seems likely that they are related to each other, although their exact interrelationship is not clear. To avoid artificially elevated correlations in investigating this relationship, it is essential to use definitions of emotion awareness and coping with minimal overlap. Therefore, the present study used the ‘personality trait’ definition of emotion awareness, and the definition of coping as a conscious, volitional process.

In summary, the aim of the present study was to answer the following research questions: (1) Do children with FAP have less emotion awareness than a control group of schoolchildren without AP, or a control group of schoolchildren with some AP? (2) Do

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 64 18-07-12 17:19

Page 68: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

4

Emotion awareness, coping and FAP | 65

children with FAP cope differently with everyday problems than these two control groups? (3) Does the way children cope with everyday problems mediate the relationship between emotion awareness and AP? We made a distinction between schoolchildren without AP and schoolchildren with some AP as previous research suggests that schoolchildren with and without somatic complaints differ significantly in their emotional functioning (Jellesma et al., 2006). As girls have been found to report abdominal pain more often (Di Lorenzo et al., 2005), gender was controlled in the analyses. Moreover, age is significantly related to abdominal pain (younger children reporting more pain; Di Lorenzo et al., 2005), as well as to emotion awareness and coping (e.g., Compas, Connor-Smith, Saltzman, Harding Thomson, & Wadsworth, 2001; Saarni & Harris, 1989). Therefore, age was used as another control variable in the analyses. We expected to find that children with FAP would have less emotion awareness, and use maladaptive coping strategies more often. Also, we hypothesized that coping would mediate the effect of emotion awareness on AP. Thus, we expected that if children reported less emotion awareness, this would be related to using less adaptive coping strategies, and that this in turn would be related to more AP.

Method

Sample and procedureThe design of this study was cross-sectional. Data were gathered through self-report questionnaires that were filled out by Dutch children aged 7-18 years.

Children with FAPThe clinical group was recruited between April 2007 and April 2010 from the general pediatric and pediatric gastroenterology outpatient clinics at a university hospital in the Netherlands. Children participated in a randomized controlled trial (RCT) investigating the efficacy of Cognitive Behavior Therapy for children with FAP. Ethical approval for this RCT was obtained from the Medical Ethical Committee of the university hospital. The data presented here are derived from the questionnaires they filled out before entering treatment. Children were eligible if they were between 7 and 18 years of age and fulfilled the following criteria, in accordance with the Rome III criteria (Rasquin et al., 2006): AP was the main complaint, AP lasted at least 8 weeks in past 12 months and was present in past 2 weeks, no red flags or alarm signals present (e.g., involuntary weight loss, vomiting, significant diarrhea, blood in stool), no abnormalities in general and abdominal physical examination, no abnormalities in blood, stool, urine or on ultrasound, no physical disease that could explain the symptoms (e.g. Crohn’s disease), no major surgery that could explain the symptoms. Of the 197 children screened for eligibility, 114 (57.9%) were eligible to participate in the RCT and agreed to fill out our questionnaires. All fulfilled the diagnostic

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 65 18-07-12 17:19

Page 69: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

66 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

criteria of one of the subtypes of FAP as described by the Rome III committee (Rasquin et al., 2006).

Control groupsBoth control groups were recruited in April - October 2008 through primary and secondary schools throughout the Dutch provinces Noord-Holland, Zuid-Holland and Utrecht. About two-thirds of the data were gathered by psychology students from Leiden University as part of their bachelor’s degree thesis. The Psychological Ethics Committee of Leiden University obliged the students to gather active consent from parents. No such obligation existed for the other data collected by the first author, who only gave account to the medical ethical committee of the university hospital. Consequently, part of the data were gathered making use of active parental consent, and part was gathered using passive consent. Children recruited through active or passive consent did not score differently on any of the variables of interest, except for differentiating emotions (F(1,611) = 10.642; p =.001), with higher scores for children for which active consent was obtained. As this was the only variable for which a difference was found, and this difference was small in size (ω2 = .014), we can safely assume that the differential procedure did not cause any problems. As an incentive, primary schools were offered a gift token of 15 euros for each participating class. Thirteen of the eighteen schools contacted agreed to participate (72.2%). Of the 2,396 children invited to participate, 665 children filled out the questionnaires (27.8%).

The questionnaires were part of a larger study in which more instruments were used. As it took the children in primary school (aged 7-12; N=253) 1.5 to 2 hours to fill out the questionnaires, they were tested in two sessions, with no more than one week between the sessions. The children in secondary/high school (aged 12-18; N=467) mostly managed to fill out the questionnaires during 1 to 1.5 class hours. The questionnaires were filled out in the classroom, electronically or by paper and pencil. A psychology student was present at all times to motivate the children and answer any questions.

Depending on their score on the Abdominal Pain Index (API; see instruments), the children were assigned to either the control group without AP (no AP in the past two weeks) or the control group with some AP (AP in the past two weeks, irrespective of the amount). This latter control group thus consisted of non-selected schoolchildren who reported any amount of AP of diverse etiology over a two week period. Two hundred and fifty-five (38.3%) children reported on the API questionnaire not to have experienced AP in the past two weeks, and were thus assigned to the healthy control group. A total of 407 children (61.2%) reported at least some AP in the past two weeks. On average, these children reported two to three days of pain in the past two weeks, which lasted about one hour. The usual intensity of their pain was 3.89 on a scale of 0-10.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 66 18-07-12 17:19

Page 70: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

4

Emotion awareness, coping and FAP | 67

Instruments

Abdominal painAbdominal pain was measured by the Abdominal Pain Index (API; Walker et al., 1997). The API consists of five questions, and assesses the frequency, duration and intensity of the AP the child experienced in the past two weeks. The first two questions make use of a 6-point scale, the third of a 9-point scale and the last two questions of an 11-point scale. A total score for the API was computed by recoding each item to reflect a scale ranging from 0 to 10, yielding a total score ranging from 0-50 (Van der Veek, Derkx, De Haan, Benninga, & Boer, 2010). The API has been shown to be a reliable instrument in previous studies (Walker et al., 1997). Cronbach’s alpha for the API in the present study was .93.

Emotion awarenessEmotion awareness was measured by the self-report Emotion Awareness Questionnaire, revised version (EAQ; Rieffe, Oosterveld, Miers, Terwogt, & Ly, 2008), which has specifically been designed for children and adolescents and was partly based on the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20; Bagby, Parker, & Taylor, 1994). The EAQ measures six aspects of emotion awareness: (1) Differentiating Emotions (e.g.,“Sometimes, I feel upset and I have no idea why”), (2) Verbal Sharing of Emotions (e.g., “I find it difficult to explain to a friend how I feel”), (3) Not Hiding Emotions (e.g., “When I am upset, I try not to show it”), (4) Bodily Awareness of Emotions (e.g., “When I am scared or nervous, I feel something in my tummy”), (5) Attending to Others’ Emotions (e.g., “It is important to know how my friends are feeling”), and (6) Analyses of Emotions (e.g., “When I am angry or upset, I try to understand why”). Respondents are asked to rate the degree to which each item is true about them on a three-point scale. A higher score on each subscale reflects a higher ability at the measured concept; e.g., higher scores on the bodily awareness scale means that children are more aware of the bodily sensations accompanying emotions. All scales have been shown to have acceptable psychometric properties (Rieffe et al., 2008). Cronbach’s alphas of the present study ranged from .71 to .77.

CopingCoping strategies were measured by a Dutch translation of the Children’s Coping Strategies Checklist - Revision 1 (CCSC-R1; Ayers & Sandler, 2000; De Boo & Wicherts, 2009). The CCSC-R1 consists of 54 items, all starting with the phrase “If I have a problem…”. The 54 items can be divided in five higher-order scales that measure problem focused coping (e.g., “I do something to make things better”), positive cognitive reframing (e.g., “I remind myself that I am better off than a lot of other kids”), distraction strategies (e.g., “I listen to music”), avoidance strategies (e.g., “I stay away from the problem”) , and support seeking strategies (e.g., “I tell people how I feel about the problem”). Answers are given on a four-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (never) to 4 (always). The CCSC-R1 has been found to have sound psychometric properties. Cronbach’s alphas of the present study were .72 to .89.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 67 18-07-12 17:19

Page 71: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

68 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Statistical analysesTo answer the first two research questions, Multivariate Analyses of Variance (MANOVA) were performed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS) version 16.0. Age and gender were entered as control variables. Effect sizes ω2 were calculated to interpret the size of significant effects, according to the following conventions: ω2 = .01: small effect; ω2 = .06: medium effect; ω2 = .15: large effect (Murphy & Myors, 2004).

To investigate whether the level of abdominal pain as experienced by children was related to their emotional awareness and whether this relationship was mediated by coping, we tested the fit of two different mediation models on the entire sample of both clinical and control children, making use of the AMOS 6.0 program. For mediation to occur, it is a prerequisite that the mediator (coping) is related to the outcome (AP), and that the independent variable (emotion awareness) is related to the mediator (coping) (MacKinnon, Lockwood, Hoffman, West, & Sheets, 2002). To be sure that no variables or relationships were tested in our mediation model that did not conform to these prerequisites, we first calculated correlations between all aspects of emotion awareness, all coping strategies, and AP. It is a common view that another prerequisite of mediation is that the independent and dependent variable show a significant correlation prior to performing the mediation analysis. However, MacKinnon and colleagues (MacKinnon et al., 2002) dispute this prerequisite. Therefore, we included all aspects of emotion awareness in our model, irrespective of whether they showed a correlation with AP beforehand.

The first mediation model tested included both indirect and direct effects of emotion awareness on level of AP, providing a partial mediation model. In the second nested model, the direct paths from emotion awareness to level of AP were constrained to zero, providing a perfect mediation model. The fit of both models were assessed by (1) Chi-square, which should be non-significant and small relative to the degrees of freedom; (2) the comparative fit index (CFI), with values of .95 or higher indicating good fit (Brown, 2006); (3) the normed fit index (NFI), demonstrating good model fit from .90 or above (Bentler & Bonett, 1980); and (4) the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), indicating good model fit if this stays close to .06 or below (Brown, 2006). To test whether the partial or perfect mediation model fitted the data best, we calculated the change in Chi-square from the first to the second model and tested this for significance (Brown, 2006). Finally, to directly test if the mediation effects of coping were significant, we used the distribution of the product approach developed by MacKinnon and colleagues (MacKinnon et al., 2002). In essence, MacKinnon and colleagues state that the mediation effect can be directly measured by multiplying the regression weight between the independent variable and the mediator (a), by the regression weight between the mediator and the dependent variable (b). Whether this product significantly differs from zero can be tested by computing a confidence interval around it. As the product is not normally distributed, MacKinnon and colleagues (MacKinnon, Fritz, Williams, & Lockwood,

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 68 18-07-12 17:19

Page 72: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

4

Emotion awareness, coping and FAP | 69

2007) developed a program PRODCLIN that calculates asymmetric confidence intervals. We used this program to test whether the mediating effects of coping were significant.

Results

ParticipantsDistributions of gender, age and level of AP per group are shown in table 1. The three groups differed significantly in both gender (χ2 (2) = 86.33; p <.001), and age, F(2,773) = 41.765; p <.001, all Bonferroni corrected pairwise comparisons significant at p <.001. For level of AP, the range of the clinical group and the control group with some AP overlapped. However, 92.6% of the children in this control group scored below the mean of the clinical group and the three groups significantly differed in their mean level of AP, F(2,770) = 840.471; p <.001, all Bonferroni corrected pairwise comparisons significant at p <.001.

Table 1. Gender, age and level of abdominal pain (AP) across diagnostic groups showing mean (SD; range) or percentage

% Female Mean age (SD) Level of AP a

Schoolchildren without AP (N=255)Total group 32.9% 14.36 (2.20) 0 (0; 0-0)Age 8-12 25.5% 10.72 (1.26) 0 (0; 0-0)Age 13-18 34.6% 15.18 (1.39) 0 (0; 0-0)

Schoolchildren with some AP (N=407) Total group 68.1% 13.00 (2.70) 17.88 (8.71; 1.25 – 43)Age 7-12 66.0% 10.14 (1.37) 17.11 (8.71; 1.25 - 40.50)Age 13-18 69.4% 14.83 (1.48) 18.38 (8.68; 2-43)

Referred children with FAP (N=114) Total group 68.4% 11.90 (2.76) 33.23 (10.52; 0-48)Age 7-12 69.2% 9.78 (1.37) 31.12 (11.65; 0-48)Age 13-17 67.3% 14.71 (1.11) 36.01 (8.15; 20.5-48)

a As measured by the Abdominal Pain Index.

Data inspectionPrior to the main analyses, the data were screened for outliers and errors. In 17 cases, children filled out all EAQ or CCSC-R1 items with the same answer. As it is unlikely that every question on these instruments yields the same answer, these data were deemed unreliable and were excluded from further analyses. Additionally, one case was excluded because of an extreme score on four out of the five coping strategies.

Differences in emotion awareness between diagnostic subgroupsA MANOVA was performed on the six aspects of emotion awareness, with diagnostic group, gender and age (split into a dichotomous variable; 7-12=0; 13-18=1) as independent variables. Assumptions for performing a MANOVA were met, however, four univariate and

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 69 18-07-12 17:19

Page 73: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

70 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

one multivariate outlier were identified on the emotion awareness aspects as calculated within each diagnostic subgroup. As MANOVA is sensitive to the influence of outliers (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001), these cases were deleted from further analyses for emotion awareness. Significant main effects were found for diagnostic group (Wilk’s Lambda F(12,1408) = 3.99, p <.001), gender (Wilk’s Lambda F(6,704) = 13.11, p <.001), age (Wilk’s Lambda F(6,704) = 5.38, p <.001), and the interaction between gender and age (Wilk’s Lambda F(6,704) = 5.92, p <.001). The interaction effects between diagnostic group and gender, and diagnostic group and age were not significant (respectively: Wilk’s Lambda F(12,1408) = 1.45; p =.136; Wilk’s Lambda F(12,1408) = .48; p =.928). Corrected for the main and interaction effects of age and gender, univariate tests showed significant differences between the diagnostic groups on differentiating between emotions, F(2,709) = 5.93, p

=.003, ω2=.013, verbal sharing of emotions, F(2,709) = 6.99, p =.001, ω2=.016, not hiding emotions, F(2,709) = 4.46, p =.012, ω2=.010, and bodily awareness of emotions, F(2,709) = 12.83, p <.001, ω2=.031. No significant differences were found for attending to others’ emotions and analyses of emotions. Following the conventions for the effect size omega squared, these effects are small for three aspects, and for bodily awareness of emotions, small to moderate (Murphy & Myors, 2004). The results of the post hoc analyses with Bonferroni correction for these four aspects of emotion awareness are depicted in table 2. Children with FAP scored significantly lower on differentiating emotions, verbal sharing of emotions and not hiding emotions than children without AP, and higher than this control group on bodily awareness. The only aspect of emotion awareness on which children with FAP differed from children with some AP, was verbal sharing of emotions.

Differences in coping between diagnostic subgroupsAnother MANOVA was performed on the five coping strategies, with diagnostic group, gender and age as the independent variables. Assumptions for performing a MANOVA were met, and five univariate and four multivariate outliers were identified and deleted from further analyses for coping. Significant multivariate main effects were found for diagnostic group (Wilk’s lambda F(10,1410) = 3.26, p <.001), gender (Wilk’s lambda F(5,705) = 3.34, p =.005), and age (Wilk’s lambda F(5,705) = 7.28, p <.001). Additionally, a significant interaction effect between gender and age (Wilk’s lambda F(5,705) = 3.29, p =.006), and a significant three-way interaction between diagnostic group, age and gender were found (Wilk’s lambda F(10,1410) = 2.05, p =.025). No interaction effects were found between diagnostic group and age (Wilk’s lambda F(10,1412) = .92, p =.515) or between diagnostic group and gender (Wilk’s lambda F(10,1410)=1.22, p =.273). Subsequent univariate analyses showed significant effects for diagnostic group on the following three coping strategies: problem focused coping, F(2,709) = 3.70, p =.025, ω2=.007; distraction, F(2,709) = 4.64, p =.010, ω2=.010; and avoidance, F(2,709) = 5.82, p =.003, ω2=.013. Following the conventions for the effect size omega squared, these effects are all small. The three-way

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 70 18-07-12 17:19

Page 74: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

4

Emotion awareness, coping and FAP | 71

interaction was only significant for the positive cognitive reframing subscale, F(2,709) = 4.645, p =.010, ω2=.010, with a small effect size. The results of the post hoc analyses with Bonferroni correction are depicted in table 2. For problem focused coping, none of the posthoc t-tests showed significant differences between the groups. For distraction, we found that children with some AP used distraction more often than children with FAP or children without AP, but these latter groups did not differ from one another. Finally, children without AP used avoidance less often then children with FAP or children with some AP, but children with FAP did not differ significantly from children with some AP in how often they used avoidance as a coping strategy.

Table 2. Means (SD) and p-values comparing groups on emotion awareness and coping

Diagnostic group Significance of post hoc testsa

A: Control without AP

B: Control with some AP

C: FAP A-B A-C B-C

Emotion awarenessDifferentiating 18.27 (2.30) 17.01 (2.57) 16.92 (3.14) <.001 <.001 NSVerbal sharing 6.68 (1.48) 6.17 (1.75) 5.70 (1.83) .001 <.001 .029Not hiding 10.64 (2.28) 10.16 (2.49) 9.63 (2.59) .058 .001 NSBodily awareness 9.07 (2.06) 10.40 (2.17) 10.42 (2.56) <.001 <.001 NS

CopingProblem focused 2.18 (.54) 2.24 (.49) 2.27 (.50) NS NS NSDistraction 1.84 (.43) 1.94 (.46) 1.76 (.39) .015 NS .001Avoidance 1.91 (.44) 2.07 (.43) 2.10 (.45) <.001 <.001 NS

Note: a. post hoc pair wise comparisons with Bonferroni correction. Group A n= 236 for emotion awareness and 235 for coping Group B n= 375 for emotion awareness and 377 for copingGroup C n= 110 for emotion awareness and 109 for coping

Mediation analysisTo answer our final research question, we investigated whether coping mediated the relationship between emotional awareness and AP in our entire sample. To sort out which coping strategies to use and which relations to test in the models, we calculated correlations between all aspects of emotion awareness, all coping strategies, and AP. As can be seen in table 3, only problem focused and avoidant coping showed a significant relation to AP; therefore, these were the only coping strategies used in our model. Also, as differentiating emotions and not hiding emotions showed no correlation with problem focused coping, these effects were not tested in the mediation model. Avoidance proved to be correlated with all aspects of emotion awareness and thus, all these relationships were included in the mediation model. For a graphic representation of the variables and relationships included in the mediation analysis, see figure 1.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 71 18-07-12 17:19

Page 75: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

72 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Tab

le 3

. Cor

rela

tions

bet

wee

n al

l var

iabl

es a

nd C

ronb

ach’

s al

phas

(N=

720)

.

Pear

son’

s r

α1.

2.3.

4.5.

6.7.

8.9.

10.

11.

12.

13.

14.

1. A

bdom

inal

pai

n.9

3-

2. G

ende

r a-

.310

-3.

Age

--.2

00-.0

56-

4. D

iffer

entia

ting

emot

ions

.77

-.219

-.246

.131

-5.

Ver

bal s

harin

g of

em

otio

ns.7

1-.1

68.0

57.1

28.3

66-

6. N

ot h

idin

g em

otio

ns.7

6-.1

44-.0

77-.0

31.3

28.5

03-

7. B

odily

aw

aren

ess

of e

mot

ions

.68

.212

.231

-.031

-.326

-.193

-.229

-8.

Att

endi

ng to

oth

ers’

emot

ions

.75

.138

.380

.240

-.053

.200

.038

.286

-9.

Ana

lyse

s of

em

otio

ns.7

5.0

81.1

83.0

14-.1

97.0

67-.0

56.3

51.3

88-

10. P

robl

em fo

cuse

d co

ping

.88

.093

.049

.128

-.067

.073

-.067

.194

.318

.566

-11

. Pos

itive

cog

nitiv

e re

fram

ing

.87

.057

.075

.017

-.124

.004

-.123

.165

.222

.482

.642

-12

. Dis

trac

tion

.72

.055

.151

-.007

-.184

.018

-.042

.157

.192

.242

.300

.637

-13

. Avo

idan

ce.7

6.2

03.1

33-.1

24-.3

49-.1

71-.2

44.3

71.1

52.3

27.3

81.4

81.3

48-

14. S

uppo

rt s

eeki

ng.8

9.0

68.1

82.0

44-.0

58.2

83.2

78.1

41.3

35.4

38.5

22.3

76.2

55.2

33-

Mea

ns-

14.0

2.5

713

.28

17.3

66.

2610

.21

9.97

12.0

69.

832.

242.

081.

892.

021.

97SD

-13

.37

.50

2.67

2.72

1.71

2.49

2.30

2.28

2.34

.53

.54

.46

.45

.58

a Va

riabl

e is

cod

ed a

s fo

llow

s: bo

y =

0; g

irl =

1.

Corr

elat

ions

larg

er th

an .0

72 o

r sm

alle

r tha

n -.0

72 a

re s

igni

fican

t at p

< .0

5

Tab

le 4

. Tes

ts o

f ind

irect

effe

cts

of m

edia

tion

mod

el

95%

Con

fiden

ce In

terv

al

Indi

rect

effe

ctPr

oduc

t of c

oeffi

cien

ts (u

nsta

ndar

dize

d)Lo

wer

Upp

erSh

are

Prob

lem

focu

sed

AP

-.005

-.059

.046

Bod

awar

ePr

oble

m fo

cuse

dA

P-.0

05-.0

49.0

35O

th e

mo

Prob

lem

focu

sed

AP

.073

.009

.162

Ana

em

oPr

oble

m fo

cuse

dA

P.3

02.0

42.5

70D

iffA

void

ance

AP

-.056

-.142

.022

Shar

eA

void

ance

AP

-.008

-.051

.024

3N

ot h

ide

Avo

idan

ceA

P-.0

13-.0

91.0

11Bo

d aw

are

Avo

idan

ceA

P.0

64-.0

25.1

64O

th e

mo

Avo

idan

ceA

P.0

19-.0

09.0

65A

na e

mo

Avo

idan

ceA

P.0

66-.0

26.1

68

Not

e: s

hare

= v

erba

l sha

ring

of e

mot

ions

; bod

aw

are

= b

odily

aw

aren

ess

of e

mot

ions

; oth

em

o =

att

endi

ng t

o ot

hers

’ em

otio

ns; a

na e

mo

= a

naly

ses

of e

mot

ions

; diff

=

diffe

rent

iatin

g em

otio

ns; n

ot h

ide

= n

ot h

idin

g em

otio

ns; p

robl

em fo

cuse

d =

pro

blem

focu

sed

copi

ng

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 72 18-07-12 17:19

Page 76: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

4

Emotion awareness, coping and FAP | 73

Subsequently, we used Structural Equation Modeling with maximum likelihood estimation to compare the fit of two path models, making use of the AMOS 6.0 program. The first model represented a path model in which two kinds of effects for emotion awareness on level of AP were investigated (also called a partial mediation model): a) the direct effects of emotion awareness on level of AP; and b) the indirect/mediation effects through coping (see figure 1). The second model included the same variables and relationships as the first model; however, in this model the direct effects of emotion awareness on level of AP were constrained to zero. Gender and age (continuous variable) were included in both models and were allowed to influence every variable within the models. The different aspects of emotion awareness were allowed to co vary, as they are all related to the same construct. For the same reason, the error terms of both coping strategies were also allowed to co vary.

The first model reflecting partial mediation showed a good fit: χ2(3) = 5.923, p =.115; CFI = .998; NFI = .997; RMSEA = .037. The second nested model reflecting perfect mediation had worse fit parameters than the first model, χ2(9) = 33.528, p <.001; CFI = .985; NFI = .981; RMSEA = .062; and this deterioration was significant, ∆χ2(6) = 27.605, p <.001. Thus, a model with partial mediation fitted the data best. The results for the partial mediation model are depicted in figure 1. The numbers over the single headed arrows represent standardized regression weights, and can be interpreted in the same way as betas in regression analyses. As shown in this figure, the following variables were related to reporting more AP: being more aware of bodily sensations accompanying emotions, attending to others’ emotions more, analyzing emotions less, using problem focused coping more often, being female and being of younger age. The effect sizes for emotion awareness and coping were small, and small to moderate for gender and age.

Finally, each mediation effect was directly tested for significance. As can be seen in table 4, only two out of the 10 investigated pathways were significant: the effect of both attending to others’ emotions and analysis of emotions was partially mediated by problem focused coping. If children more often attended to others’ emotions and analyzed their own emotions more, this was related to using problem focused coping more often, and using problem focused coping more often was, in its turn, related to reporting more AP. Note that analyzing emotions thus has contradictory direct and indirect effects on level of abdominal pain. None of the effects for emotion awareness were mediated by avoidant coping.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 73 18-07-12 17:19

Page 77: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

74 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Figure 1. Path model testing whether coping mediates the relationship between emotion awareness and abdominal pain, corrected for the effects of gender and age; N=720.

Not hiding

Bodily awareness

Differentiating

Verbal sharing

Others' emotions

Analyses emotions

Age

Gender

Problem focused coping

Avoidant coping

Abdominal pain

E

E

E

-.20***

-.02

-.11**

.06

.20***

-.04

-.11

-.01

-.01 .13***

.54***

-.09** .09**

.23*** -.20***

-.06

.06

-.08 -.04

.08*

.09*

-.10*

.10*

.25

.18

.35

.30***

.20*** -.06

Note: all aspects of emotion awareness, gender and age were allowed to co vary, but to increase interpretability of this figure, these paths were not drawn here. The numbers over the single headed arrows represent standardized regression weights, and the number next to the double headed arrow represents a correlation. The numbers printed in Italics indicate explained variances. *p<.05; **p<.01; ***p<.001

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 74 18-07-12 17:19

Page 78: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

4

Emotion awareness, coping and FAP | 75

Discussion

The results of our study show that, after correcting for the effects of age and gender, children with FAP were slightly less aware of their emotions than children without AP, and used avoidant coping – a maladaptive strategy – somewhat more often. The differences between these two groups were, however, small. Moreover, children with FAP mostly did not differ from the other comparison group used in this study – non-selected schoolchildren with some AP – in both their level of emotion awareness and use of coping strategies. It thus seems that children with FAP are not so different from the general population in their awareness of their own emotions or in what coping strategies they use in response to everyday problems. As for our final research question which considered through what exact processes emotion awareness is related to FAP, it seems that coping is not a likely candidate, as we only found a small mediation effect for one out of the five coping strategies.

Evaluating the results for emotion awareness in more detail, we find that, although most differences between the children with FAP and healthy children were small, they were in the expected direction. However, this was not true for bodily awareness: children with FAP were more aware of a link between emotions and bodily sensations than children without AP, while the theory on alexithymia suggests that children with FAP are less aware of such a link. The mediation analysis confirmed this reverse relationship, as bodily awareness was found to be related to more AP. Thus, concerning this specific aspect of alexithymia/emotion awareness, the original theories are not proven here. Other researchers using the Emotion Awareness Questionnaire have found similar results (Jellesma et al., 2006; Rieffe et al., 2007, 2008) and posed two explanations for this finding. First, they suggest coping as an explanatory mechanism: as children with many somatic complaints are apparently better at linking bodily sensations to emotions, they might be focusing more on the bodily sensation-part of the emotion, which might prevent them from analyzing the situation that caused the emotion carefully. Consequently, these children might have more difficulty with choosing an appropriate coping strategy to deal with their emotions, which in turn may lead to more emotional and somatic complaints. However, this hypothesis was not supported in our mediation model. Second, Rieffe and colleagues (Rieffe et al., 2007) reason that children with many somatic complaints usually have more intense (negative) emotions as well, which are accompanied by more salient bodily signals. Therefore, children with many somatic complaints would be more likely to directly connect emotions and bodily sensations. Moreover, children acquire knowledge about emotions not only by self-observation, but also by learning from the environment. In the case of children with (F)AP, it is likely that parents or doctors will have asked children whether their AP meant that they were nervous, anxious, sad, etc., increasing the odds that children learn to make such connections (Meerum Terwogt & Olthof, 1989). This

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 75 18-07-12 17:19

Page 79: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

76 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

implies that being aware of bodily sensations that accompany emotions is actually an epiphenomenon or even a consequence of experiencing many somatic complaints. However, this theory has yet to be tested. An intriguing question that emerges from Rieffe’s and our work is how important the construct of bodily awareness actually is for the entire alexithymia/emotional awareness concept. For the population of patients suffering from medically unexplained symptoms, lack of bodily awareness seems a key aspect of alexithymia theory. It would be most interesting to investigate if other samples, including adults, lack an awareness of bodily manifestations of emotions. To be able to do this, however, new instruments should be developed first, as neither of the two most often used questionnaires to measure alexithymia (the Toronto Alexithymia Scale – 20; Bagby et al., 1994; and the Bermond Vorst Alexithymia Questionnaire ;Vorst & Bermond, 2001) include a separate measure of bodily awareness of emotions. Also, it should be noted that the bodily awareness scale used in our study focused on questions like “When I am scared

or nervous, I feel something in my tummy” (Rieffe et al., 2007), which does not mean that children will be equally apt to acknowledge items stating the reverse, like “When I have a

stomach ache, I am nervous about something”. Obviously, more research on the relationship between bodily awareness of emotions and medically unexplained symptoms is needed.

Looking at the analyses for coping and our mediation analysis in more detail, we found that children with FAP reported to use avoidance – a usually maladaptive coping strategy – more often than healthy children, which was in line with our expectations. Problem-focused coping was found to mediate the relationship between two aspects of emotion awareness and level of AP. As problem-focused coping is usually considered an adaptive strategy, we did not expect to find that it was related to reporting more AP. However, it should be noted that both the effect of problem-focused coping and the direct effects of emotion awareness on level of AP were very small. It is likely that these effects only reached significance because of our large sample size. In fact, figure 1 shows that level of AP is most strongly predicted by gender and age, whereas all of the variables of interest had small effects on level of AP, with standardized regression weights of .10 or under. Because of these small effects, it seems clear that both emotion awareness and general coping are unlikely to have a large influence on the etiology or course of pediatric FAP.

It should be noted, however, that especially in children the strength of these associations might be reduced because the correspondence between knowledge or awareness on the one hand and behavior on the other are not one to one. Previous studies found that children do not always know what kind of behavior they are displaying or its goal. Of course, this might also be true for adults and is one of the major disadvantages of using self-report measures. Still, it should be considered that these effects are even more prominent in children, as their understanding of emotions is still developing.

The current study shows a number of limitations that need to be addressed. First, our study solely relied on self-report questionnaires, which especially for emotion awareness –

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 76 18-07-12 17:19

Page 80: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

4

Emotion awareness, coping and FAP | 77

although most often applied in the literature – may be problematic. However, our results are in line with two studies using other, experimental designs to measure alexithymia in children (Jellesma, Rieffe, Terwogt, & Westenberg, 2009; Rieffe, Terwogt, & Bosch, 2004). Second, the response rate of our control groups was rather small. As shown in a previous study making use of this control group, however, the possible bias resulting from this was likely small as our control group scored similar as norm groups used in other studies on level of AP, other physical symptoms and anxiety/depression (Van der Veek et al., 2010). Third, the cause of the AP in the control group of children with some AP could not be verified, which might have led to a bias. Future studies comparing children with FAP to more specific groups of children with AP of known etiology are warranted. Also, for mediation to be properly established, the effects should succeed over time, which we were not able to investigate in our current cross-sectional study. Although the hypothesized directions of the effects investigated here are based on both theoretical and empirical work, future longitudinal studies are necessary. Fourth, the design of our study did not allow the investigation of developmental processes. Previous research has found that, although children experience all emotions by the end of the preschool period, their emotional understanding still increases with age until adolescence (e.g., Harter, 1986; Saarni & Harris, 1989). Although the present study found no evidence for large age effects, future studies are necessary to investigate these processes more fully from a developmental point of view. Finally, the present study defined coping as a conscious, volitional process. However, it is acknowledged by many theorists that the broad spectrum of emotion regulation consists of more than a mere conscious regulation, and also involves involuntary, unconscious processes (Compas et al., 2001). Investigating these processes in children with FAP might shed more light on the emotional functioning of these children.

Despite these limitations, our study is the first to investigate emotion awareness, coping and their interrelationship in a large sample of referred children with FAP and schoolchildren, thereby integrating some psychodynamic and cognitive-behavioral thinking. Overall, we conclude that the effects of emotion awareness and coping on FAP are small, which raises the question whether psychological treatments should focus on changing emotion awareness and coping with everyday problems in children with FAP. Judging from the results of the present study, this does not seem a very effective intervention strategy.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 77 18-07-12 17:19

Page 81: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 78 18-07-12 17:19

Page 82: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

Ch

apte

r

5The relative contribution of

parental and children’s cognitive and behavioral reactions

to pediatric functional abdominal pain

S. M. C. van der Veek, E. de Haan, H. H. F. Derkx, M. A. Benninga, R. J. L. Lindauer, & F. Boer, submitted for publication

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 79 18-07-12 17:19

Page 83: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

80 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Abstract

Objectives: To investigate the contribution of children’s versus parental cognitions and behaviors to a) having pediatric functional abdominal pain (FAP) and b) the physical and psychological well-being of children with FAP.Method: 117 children with FAP, 155 schoolchildren and one of their parents filled out questionnaires concerning cognitions about the child’s abdominal pain (AP), coping with AP, and the child’s level of AP, functional disability, anxiety, depression and quality of life.Results: Both children’s cognitions and behaviors and parental cognitions showed significant relationships with having FAP or not, although parental cognitions had a larger influence. In contrast, children’s cognitions and behaviors were more strongly associated with children’s well-being than parent variables. Discussion: Psychological treatment of FAP may benefit more from changing parental cognitions than concrete displays of parental solicitous behavior. Promoting positive thoughts about control and stimulating adaptive coping behavior in children seems more beneficial than decreasing children’s catastrophic thoughts.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 80 18-07-12 17:19

Page 84: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

5

Parental vs. children’s cognitions and behaviors | 81

Introduction

Many theories of psychological as well as physical dysfunctioning attribute important roles to mistaken cognitions and unhelpful coping behaviors, which is reflected in the vast popularity of cognitive behavior therapy (CBT). Founded in Pavlov’s, Skinner’s, and Eysenck’s learning theory and in Beck and Ellis’s cognitive theory, CBT has a solid and broad theoretical foundation (Rachman, 1997) and can be applied to most types of psychopathology. During the past few decades CBT is also increasingly used to treat children with chronic medically unexplained or ‘functional’ abdominal pain (FAP). FAP is a very common condition among children and adolescents, affecting about 10% of children in Western countries (Chitkara et al., 2005) and has a large impact on daily functioning (Youssef et al., 2006). Several CBT protocols have been developed to treat patients with FAP. The five protocols that have been tested for their effectiveness have in common that they all teach children to cope with their complaints, teach parents to react adaptively to their children’s complaints, and teach both of them to stop negative thinking (Duarte et al., 2006; Humphreys & Gevirtz, 2000; Levy et al., 2010; Robins, Smith, Glutting, & Bishop, 2005; Sanders et al., 1989). All of the studies investigating the effectiveness of these protocols reported a significant benefit for children receiving the CBT intervention.

However, it has never been investigated which of the factors CBT for children with FAP focuses on, show the largest relationship with abdominal pain (AP). Thus, it is unclear whether the way a child thinks about and copes with their AP, or the way parents think about and react to the AP of their child, is more strongly related to FAP. Also, it is unknown whether cognitions or behaviors are most relevant in explaining variation in symptoms. If we would know which of these factors are most strongly associated with FAP, more focused and perhaps more (cost-)effective interventions could be developed. The cognitions CBT for children with FAP focuses on, concern both negative beliefs about the AP such as “My AP means I have a serious illness”, and positive thoughts about coping abilities like “When I have AP, I can find ways to feel better”. However, only few studies investigated whether these cognitions are actually present in children with FAP or whether they relate to the complaints. There is some evidence suggesting an association between negative beliefs of children with FAP and limitations in family functioning (Lipani & Walker, 2006) and health care consultation (Levy, Langer, Walker, Feld, & Whitehead, 2006). Specific catastrophizing thoughts were found to be related to disability, depression and anxiety (Langer et al., 2009). Positive thoughts received even less attention, but generally, feeling more confident about one’s abilities to cope with FAP seems beneficial (Anderson, Acra, Bruehl, & Walker, 2008; Walker et al., 2008). Only recently, studies started to focus on parental cognitions and found that parents are likely to worry about the impact of the complaints on their children and have a desire for diagnosis and care (Smart & Cottrell, 2005; Van Tilburg, Chitkara, Palsson, Levy, & Whitehead, 2009). How these parental

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 81 18-07-12 17:19

Page 85: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

82 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

cognitions relate to the level of AP or other measures of functioning of children with FAP has, however, not yet been investigated.

In addition to these cognitions, CBT for FAP tries to alter the way the child and their parents cope with and react to the AP. Results from observational research suggest that active coping by the child is related to less pain, whereas passive coping is related to more pain (Walker et al., 1997, 2008). For parental behavior, however, compelling evidence is lacking. In CBT, parents are stimulated to cease solicitous behavior like keeping the child home from school. Although this intuitively seems good practice, a recent structured review showed that solid evidence for the effects of solicitous behavior on FAP is lacking (Van der Veek et al., 2012). In summary, the present study aimed to answer the following research question: what are the relative contributions of the way a child thinks about and copes with its complaints, and the way parents think about and react to the complaints of their child to a) having FAP or not, and b) the physical and emotional functioning of a child with FAP. Additionally, we investigated the relative contributions of cognitions and behaviors to these outcomes. As no studies before have directly compared these factors, we have no specific hypotheses beforehand as to which factors will prove to be most strongly related to FAP.

Method

Sample and ProcedureThe design of the current study was cross-sectional. Data were gathered through self-report questionnaires that were filled out by children aged 7-18.

Children with FAP

The clinical group was recruited between April 2007 and April 2010 from the general pediatric and pediatric gastroenterology outpatient clinics at a university hospital in the Netherlands. Children participated in a randomized controlled trial (RCT) investigating the efficacy of CBT versus medical care for children with FAP. Ethical approval for this RCT was obtained from the Medical Ethical Committee of the university hospital. The data presented here are derived from the questionnaires the children and (one of ) their parents filled out before entering treatment. Children were eligible if they were between 7 and 18 years and fulfilled Rome III criteria for FAP (Rasquin et al., 2006). To be included, the AP had to be the child’s main complaint, should have lasted at least 8 weeks in the past 12 months and be present in the past two weeks without the presence of red flags or alarm signals (e.g., involuntary weight loss, vomiting, blood in stool). Children were excluded if a) abnormalities were found in blood, stool, urine or on ultrasound; b) an explanatory physical disease was present; or c) they had had a major surgery that could explain the symptoms.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 82 18-07-12 17:19

Page 86: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

5

Parental vs. children’s cognitions and behaviors | 83

Of the 197 children screened for eligibility, 117 (59.4%) filled out our questionnaires (see figure 1). Consent was obtained from both parents and children. For 59.8% of the children, mothers filled out the questionnaires, for 4.3% fathers did, and for 33.3% both parents filled out the questionnaires together. For three children, no parental data were available.

Control group

The control group consisted of school children aged 7 to 18 and was recruited between April - October 2008 through primary and secondary schools throughout the Netherlands. Details on how the groups were recruited and on the procedure used to administer the questionnaires are described in an earlier publication of our group (Van der Veek et al., 2010). Before administering children the instruments measuring their functional disability, negative and control beliefs, coping behavior and parental solicitous behavior, children were asked whether they could remember ever having had AP. If they stated they never had AP, or that they did occasionally have AP but could not remember what it had been like to experience AP, they did not have to fill out these specific questionnaires. Similarly, parents were asked whether they could remember their child ever had AP, and did not have to fill out the questionnaire on parental negative and control beliefs if they could not.

Figure 1 displays the selection of children for our control group. 274 children (41.2%) were excluded because their parents did not participate. The remaining 391 children did not differ from these children on any of the variables used for this study, except for age (mean age of included children: 13; mean age of excluded children: 14; t(663) = 4.60; p <.001). Another 236 children were excluded because they themselves or their parents could not remember what it was like (for their child) to experience AP. Compared to the other children with parent data, the remaining 155 children had higher levels of AP (t(289.87) = -5.57; p <.001). They also scored higher on functional disability (t(295.85) = -7.21; p <.001), depression (t(379) = -3.46; p =.001), anxiety (t(293.62) = -2.77; p =.007), and lower on quality of life (t(377) = 3.30; p =.001). Only the data of these 155 children were used in further analyses. For 80.6% of these children, mothers filled out the parent questionnaires, and for 19.4% fathers did.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 83 18-07-12 17:19

Page 87: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

84 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Figure 1. Schematic flow of participants screened for this study

Children with FAP Control group

201 children screened for eligibility

2,396 children invited to participate

665 children filled out questionnaires

391 children with parent data

Excluded: 274 children without parent data

155 children included

Excluded (N=236) : - 26 children never experienced AP/could not remember what it was like when they had AP - for 151 children, parents could not remember what it was like when their child had AP - for 59 children, both parents and children could not remember the child having AP

117 children included

Excluded for RCT (N=97) : - 22 no consent

- 34 no more AP after one visit to doctor

- 16 physical cause

- 12 psychological problems more prominent

- 13

other

Of these 97, 13 were included in present study:

- 5 gave no consent for RCT but did consent to fill out questionnaires

- 3 filled out questionnaires before AP disappeared - 3 children had more prominent psychological problems, but also fulfilled Ro me III criteria and filled out questionnaires

- 2 children were too young behaviorally

for

the RCT but did manage to fill out the questionnaires with help

InstrumentsCronbach’s alpha reliability scores of each instrument are presented in table 2.

Outcome measuresAbdominal pain. Children filled out the Abdominal Pain Index (API) to measure level

of AP (Walker et al., 1997). The API consists of five questions, and taps on the frequency, duration and intensity of the AP the child experienced in the past two weeks. Two questions make use of a 6-point scale, one of a 9-point scale and another two of an 11-point scale. A total score for the API was computed by recoding each item to reflect a scale ranging from 0 to 10, and summing all items (Van der Veek et al., 2010). The API has been shown to be

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 84 18-07-12 17:19

Page 88: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

5

Parental vs. children’s cognitions and behaviors | 85

a reliable instrument, with a Cronbach’s alpha ranging from .80 to .93 (Walker et al., 1997). Functional disability. To measure limitations in activities due to AP, children filled out

the Functional Disability Inventory (FDI; Claar & Walker, 2006; Walker & Greene, 1991b). The FDI consists of 15 items which are answered on a five point scale, ranging from (0) no trouble at all to (4) impossible. The scores on the items are summed to a total score, with higher scores indicating more disabilities. The FDI has been validated in children with recurrent AP with Cronbach’s alpha’s ranging from .86 to .91 (Claar & Walker, 2006).

Anxious and depressive symptoms. Children filled out the Revised Anxiety and Depression Scale – short version (RCADS-25; Muris et al., 2002) to measure symptoms of anxiety and depression. The shortened version of the RCADS has been shown to be a valid and reliable instrument to measure symptoms of generalized anxiety disorder, separation anxiety disorder, social phobia, panic disorder, and major depressive disorder (Muris et al., 2002). Each of the five scales consists of five items that have to be scored on a 4-point scale, ranging from (0) never to (3) always. Cronbach’s alphas of the five scales ranged from .65 to .83 in previous research (Muris et al., 2002). For the present study, the four scales of the different anxiety disorders were summed to compute one score for anxious symptoms. A separate score was calculated for depressive symptoms.

Quality of Life. Children filled out the physical well-being subscale of the KIDSCREEN-27 (The KIDSCREEN Group Europe, 2006) to assess physical quality of life. The KIDSCREEN-27 was developed across different countries, and consists of 27 items, to be scored on a 5-point scale. Several items are reverse-scored. Scores for each scale are calculated using Rasch analysis and then transformed into T-values. Higher scores reflect a better quality of life. The KIDSCREEN-27 has been shown to be a reliable and valid instrument, with a Cronbach’s alpha for the physical well-being subscale of .80 and good test-retest reliability (The KIDSCREEN Group Europe, 2006).

Independent variablesNegative and control beliefs about abdominal pain. Children in both groups and their

parents filled out the Pain Beliefs Questionnaire (PBQ) to measure both negative and control beliefs about the child’s AP (PBQ; Van Slyke, 2001; Walker et al., 2005). The PBQ is a 32-item questionnaire, with 20 items assessing beliefs about condition frequency, duration and seriousness, and episode specific intensity and duration. The other 12 items make up the problem focused and emotion focused coping potential scales (PFCP and EFCP), which measure whether children believe they can cope with their pain. Children can respond to each item of the PBQ on a 5-point Likert scale; higher scores reflect that the child has such thoughts more often. As no Dutch version was available, the PBQ was translated by our research group, and back-translated by a native speaker. Any discrepancies in the back-translation were discussed with the head of the research group that developed the PBQ, dr. L.S. Walker. A confirmatory factor analysis in the control group showed that the 20 items

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 85 18-07-12 17:19

Page 89: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

86 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

assessing thoughts about the condition could be construed under one higher order scale, but the five lower order scales could also be preserved (modelfit unweighted least squares model χ2(165) = 536.97; p <.001; GFI = .97; AGFI = .97). For the present study, it was decided to use the condition seriousness scale only, as this scale incorporates the type of thoughts explicitly addressed in cognitive therapy (e.g., “my stomach aches mean I have a serious illness”). The PFCP and EFCP scales were calculated by averaging the 6 items of each scale.

Pain coping strategies. To measure the way children coped with their AP, children in both the clinical and control group filled out the Pain Response Inventory (PRI; Walker et al., 1997). The PRI is a 60 item questionnaire measuring several types of coping strategies. Answers can be given on a 5-point Likert scale, with higher scores reflecting a greater usage of these coping strategies. The PRI was also translated into Dutch by the present research group, then back-translated by a native speaker, and any discrepancies were discussed with dr. L.S. Walker. The PRI consists of 3 higher order scales, measuring active coping (e.g., “try to do something to make it go away”), passive coping (e.g., “not even try to do anything about it because it will not help”) and accommodative coping (e.g., “try to learn to live with it”). A confirmatory factor analysis showed that the factor structure as described by Walker and colleagues fitted the data derived from our school children well (modelfit unweighted least squares model χ2(131) = 10,405.67; p <.001; GFI = .92; AGFI = .91).

Parental solicitous behavior. Children filled out the Illness Behavior Encouragement Scale (IBES; Walker & Zeman, 1992) to measure parental solicitous behavior. The IBES consists of 12 items which are answered on a 5-point scale. Higher scores reflect more parental solicitous behavior. The IBES has been shown to be a reliable instrument, with Cronbach’s alphas of .75 to .85 (Walker & Zeman, 1992).

Statistical AnalysesTo answer the first research question, two logistic regression analyses were performed. As it has been found that younger children and girls report FAP more often (Di Lorenzo et al., 2005), age and gender were entered as control variables. Moreover, as we wanted to know what the effects of cognitions and behaviors were on predicting FAP over and above the level of AP experienced by children, level of AP was entered as another control variable. In the first logistic regression analysis, child cognitions and behaviors were entered as a second block and parental cognitions and behaviors as a third block. In the second logistic regression analysis, this sequence was reversed, entering the parent variables in the second block and the child variables in the third. In doing so, it was possible to investigate both the full and partial effects of the child and parental factors. To investigate the relative importance of each child and parent variable, standardized odds ratios and standardized regression coefficients were computed. Standardized odds ratios were computed by standardizing the independent variables before they entered the analysis (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). Menard’s

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 86 18-07-12 17:19

Page 90: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

5

Parental vs. children’s cognitions and behaviors | 87

method was used to calculate a fully standardized regression coefficient (Menard, 2004).To answer the second research question, a series of multiple regression analyses was

performed, two for each outcome measure. Age and gender were again entered as control variables, and child and parent variables were subsequently entered as separate blocks, according to the same strategy as applied to the logistic regression analyses.

Results

Preliminary Analyses

Missing values and data inspection Three healthy participants were excluded from further analyses because they filled out every item of the PRI and/or IBES with the same answer, which was a very unlikely answering pattern. For 21 children in the clinical group (17.9%), no parental PBQ data was available because this questionnaire was added to the instrument battery at a later time. The reason for the missings in these cases was thus observed, and as such it can be described as “missing at random” (MAR) (Rubin, 1976). For the other variables used in this study, the percent of missing values ranged between 0.6% and 3.9% for the control group, and between 2.6% and 10.3% for the clinical group. Inspection of the missing value patterns provided no reasons to assume that the reason for the missingness of these values was “not at random”. Therefore, we assumed all missing values to be MAR. It has been shown that in the case of MAR it is better to impute data than do a complete-case analysis (Schafer & Graham, 2002). Therefore, we imputed the PBQ and other missing data making use of the multiple imputations subroutine available in SPSS 18.0. All variables used in this study plus all subscales of the PBQ were used to impute the missing data five times.

Physical illness of control groupParents of children in the control group were asked whether their child had a physical illness (with the further specification that this should not just be a common cold or the flu) which affected their child at the moment. Six parents mentioned a physical illness that might be related to the AP (e.g., celiac disease, urinary tract infection). Performing the analyses with and without these cases showed that they did not influence the results. Therefore, we performed the analyses including these six cases.

Descriptive statistics and group differences Table 1 shows descriptive statistics of each group for every variable used in the analyses. As depicted in table 1, the clinical and control group differed largely in their level of AP (mean difference = 17.89; p <.001). This was also true for the other outcome measures, except for symptoms of anxiety. Children in the clinical group experienced more depressive symptoms and a lower quality of life than children in the control group.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 87 18-07-12 17:19

Page 91: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

88 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Interestingly, the control group reported significantly more functional disability due to AP than the clinical group. As shown in table 1, most independent child and parent variables significantly differed between the clinical and control group, except for children’s passive and accommodative coping, and parental solicitous behavior.

Table 1. Cronbach’s alpha and descriptive statistics of all variables used in the analyses.

Clinical group Control group p test difference clinical - control b

α a Original data mean (SD)

Imputed data mean (SD)

Original data mean (SD)

Imputed data mean (SD)

Age - 11.86 (2.83) NA 12.61 (2.43) NA .024Gender (% female) - 69.2 NA 65.2 NA NS c

Abdominal pain .92 33.23 (10.52) 33.09 (10.51) 15.15 (11.34) NA < .001 d

Functional disability .92 11.27 (10.14) 11.36 (10.06) 16.41 (11.70) 16.39 (11.68) < .001 d

Depressive symptoms .68 3.82 (2.51) 3.87 (2.51) 2.84 (2.17) 2.89 (2.17) .001Anxious symptoms .86 9.63 (8.09) 9.75 (8.07) 9.19 (7.15) 9.35 (7.20) NS d

Physical QoL .83 45.41 (10.80) 45.32 (10.89) 49.81 (10.69) 49.84 (10.72) .001Child variables

Cond seriousness .56 1.40 (.71) 1.40 (.71) 0.96 (.71) NA < .001PFCP .79 1.45 (.90) 1.45 (.89) 2.06 (.73) NA < .001EFCP .69 2.39 (.81) 2.38 (.81) 2.74 (.75) 2.74 (.75) < .001Active coping .86 2.78 (.60) 2.78 (.59) 3.06 (.55) 3.06 (.55) < .001Passive coping .85 1.92 (.65) 1.93 (.64) 1.83 (.50) 1.83 (.50) NSAccom coping .88 2.88 (.71) 2.88 (.71) 2.94 (.74) 2.94 (.73) NS

Parent variables Cond seriousness .56 1.40 (.74) 1.41 (.72) 0.98 (.69) 0.99 (.69) < .001PFCP .81 1.47 (.83) 1.48 (.80) 2.25 (.73) 2.25 (.74) < .001EFCP .69 2.63 (.82) 2.59 (.79) 2.93 (.69) 2.93 (.68) .001Solicitous behavior .92 14.91 (7.50) 15.05 (7.60) 16.43 (7.83) 16.41 (7.84) NS

Note. Cond seriousness = condition seriousness. Accom coping = accommodative coping. NA = not applicable; mean and SD are the same as for original data as there was no missing data. NS = not significant. N for imputed data of clinical group was 117; N for imputed data of control group was 152. a Cronbach’s alpha was calculated with original datab Standard test performed was t-testc Tested with Chi-square testd Due to skewed distributions, tested with Mann-Whitney U-test

Main Analyses

Logistic regression To investigate which variables predicted best whether children belonged to the clinical or control group, two logistic regression analyses were performed. Statistical assumptions of logistic regression like ratio of cases to variables and absence of multicollinearity were checked (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001), and showed no problems. The control variables age, gender and level of AP formed a significantly reliable model (χ2(3) ranging between 134.51 and 140.67 for the five imputed datasets, all p <.001). This step explained 53% of the variance according to Nagelkerke’s R2. Adding the child variables as a second step resulted in a significantly better model, with χ2(6) of this step ranging between 24.15 and 25.94, all

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 88 18-07-12 17:19

Page 92: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

5

Parental vs. children’s cognitions and behaviors | 89

p <.001, and a total R2 of about 61%. The same holds true for adding the parent variables in the second step instead, with χ2(4) ranging between 25.36 and 30.92, all p <.001, and a total R2 of about 62%. However, when in a third step all variables were included, this resulted in the best model, with χ2(4) ranging between 20.96 and 24.39, all p <.001 when the parent variables were added to the child variables, and χ2(6) ranging between 17.29 and 21.97, all p <.01 when the child variables were added to the parent variables. R2 for this model was 66.9% and it correctly classified on average 82.9% of the clinical cases, and 87.1% of the control cases. As can be seen in table 2, level of AP was the strongest predictor of having FAP. Parental thoughts about the problem focused coping potential (PFCP) of their child was the next largest contributor, followed by, respectively, active coping behavior of the child, parental thoughts about the seriousness of the condition, and child age. For parental solicitous behavior, a trend was found. Experiencing more AP, younger age, using active coping less often, and having parents that think more negatively about condition seriousness and the PFCP of their children were all related to a higher risk of having FAP.

Table 2. Hierarchical logistic regression analyses, method enter.

95% Confidence interval for Odds Ratio

Standardized B Standardized Odds Ratio

Upper Lower

Control variablesAge -.497* .608 .401 .924Gender a -.160 .852 .560 1.295Level of abdominal pain 1.995*** 7.349 3.879 13.921

Child variablesCondition seriousness .363 1.437 .864 2.389PFCP -.230 .795 .482 1.310EFCP -.279 .756 .463 1.234Active coping -.621** .538 .338 .855Passive coping -.309 .734 .463 1.164Accommodative coping -.123 .884 .564 1.387

Parent variablesCondition seriousness .554* 1.740 1.074 2.818PFCP -.731** .481 .292 .792EFCP -.002 .998 .615 1.619Solicitous behavior -.439† .645 .409 1.016

Note. PFCP = problem focused coping potential. EFCP = emotion focused coping potential. N for clinical group was 117, N for control group was 152. a Variable coded as boy = 0, girl = 1. †p <.010;*p <.05; **p <.01; ***p < .001

Multiple regression analyses To investigate which child and parent variables most strongly contributed to the physiological and psychological well-being of children with FAP, a series of multiple regression analyses (MRAs) were performed on level of AP, functional disability, symptoms of anxiety and depression, and physical quality of life. Only the clinical group was used for these analyses. Assumptions of homoscedasticity, absence of multi-collinearity and

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 89 18-07-12 18:45

Page 93: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

90 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

absence of outliers were checked (Tabachnick & Fidell, 2001). No multi-collinearity was present; however, scatter plots of the residuals showed heteroscedasticity for the MRAs with AP, functional disability and anxiety as outcome measures. Boxplots showed that these outcome measures had a skewed distribution. A reflected square root transformation for AP and a logarithmic transformation for functional disability and anxiety resolved both the skewed distributions and the heteroscedasticity. The MRAs for AP and functional disability also showed a multivariate outlier, which was deleted for the analyses. In a first series of MRAs, gender and age were entered as control variables in the first block, child variables in the second block and parent variables in the third block. Following the same strategy as for the logistic regression analyses, the sequence of the second and third block was then reversed in a second series of MRAs. For AP, functional disability, depressive symptoms and anxious symptoms, the results of the MRAs were no different when entering the child variables or the parent variables first. For quality of life, however, there was a difference. If child variables were entered first, the block entering parent variables failed to reach significance. However, if parent variables were entered first, the reverse held true. Therefore, it was decided for quality of life to interpret the model entering both child and parent variables. This was also done for AP. For functional disability, depression and anxiety, however, the block entering parent variables failed to reach significance. Therefore, for these outcome measures, the model entering only control and child variables was interpreted.

Table 3. Hierarchical multiple regression analyses, method enter.

Abdominal pain c

Functional disability

Depressive symptoms

Anxious symptoms

Quality of life

β ∆R2 β ∆R2 β ∆R2 β ∆R2 β ∆R2

Step 1: Control variables .028 .152* .082* .131* .108*Age -.040 .167* -.056 -.070 -.241*Gender a -.046 .211* .188* .243* -.034

Step 2: Child variables .280* .172* .288* .169* .138*Cond seriousness -.167 .058 -.005 -.117 -.185PFCP .205* -.205* .000 .112 -.003EFCP .123 .023 -.097 -.268* -.019Active coping .048 .167† .163 .031 .032Passive coping -.033 .119 .413* .301* -.191†Accom coping -.312* -.261* .004 .031 .123

Step 3: Parent variables .085* .015 .030 .051 .050*Cond seriousness -.143 - - - -.096PFCP .252* - - - .229*EFCP -.044 - - - -.037Solicitous behavior .161† - - - .033

R2 total model b .394* .324* .370* .301* .296*

Note. Cond seriousness = condition seriousness. Accom coping = accommodative coping. N was 117 for all models.a Variable coded as boy = 0, girl = 1b If step 3 was not significant, R2 of total model only reflects R2 of step 1 and 2.c As this variable was reflected for transformation purposes, higher scores on this variable now mean that the child experiences less abdominal pain.†p <.10; *p <.05

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 90 18-07-12 18:45

Page 94: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

5

Parental vs. children’s cognitions and behaviors | 91

Table 3 shows the results of the final MRAs. To calculate mean R2 across the five imputed datasets, Harel’s method was used (Harel, 2009). It should be noted that as the measure for AP was reflected for transformation purposes, a higher score on the API in the MRA means that

children experienced less abdominal pain. Thus, if children reported to think more positively about their PFCP, this was related to experiencing less AP, whereas using accommodative coping was related to more AP. Also, if parents thought more positively about the PFCP of their child, this was related to less pain. For functional disability, when children experienced less positive thoughts about their own PFCP and used accommodative coping less often, this was related to more disability. Depressive and anxious symptoms were related to female gender and using passive coping more often. In addition, if children had more confidence in their own emotion focused coping potential, this was related to less anxiety. For quality of life, the total block of child variables added a significant percentage of variance to the model, but no single predictor reached significance. There was a trend for passive coping being related to a lower sense of quality of life. If parents were more confident about the PFCP of their children, this was related to an increased quality of life.

Discussion

The present study sought to determine whether the way a child thinks about and copes with FAP, or the way parents think about and react to the complaints of their child more strongly relates to both a) having FAP or not, and b) the physical and emotional functioning of a child with FAP. Additionally, we investigated the contribution of cognitions versus behaviors to these outcomes. We will first discuss the relevance of parental versus child factors, and then focus on the relative importance of cognitions and behaviors.

Parent vs. ChildThe present study shows that both parental and children’s cognitions and behaviors are related to FAP. However, whether either parental or children’s cognitions and behaviors have a stronger relationship with FAP depends on the question we focus on. If we want to determine which child has FAP or not, both child and parent variables were relevant, but parental cognitions had the largest influence. If we want to explain variance in how children with FAP are functioning physically and emotionally, children’s thoughts and behaviors seem most important. Assuming that both questions provide information on a similar topic, it seems curious that they lead to such different answers. An explanation for this discrepancy might be that the first question may actually tap into a different process. This question focuses on which child has FAP or not, or – put differently – which child was

diagnosed with FAP. Of course, the odds of being diagnosed with FAP was best predicted by the level of AP a child experienced. However, after correcting for this level of AP, we

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 91 18-07-12 17:19

Page 95: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

92 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

found that parental negative beliefs uniquely increased the odds of being diagnosed with FAP. Note that a diagnosis of FAP can only be made if a child and his/her parents visit a doctor for the complaints. It is not hard to imagine that negative parental thoughts may cause parents to visit a doctor sooner and thus the child will be diagnosed with FAP. In fact, although the children in the control group experienced less AP than children in the clinical group, some may have fulfilled the diagnostic criteria for FAP if they had visited a doctor, as 6% of the children in the control group scored above the mean level of AP of the clinical group. Thus, the first question may mostly tap into medical care seeking instead of having FAP per se.

Cognitions vs. Behavior Another question this study focused on, concerned the relative contribution of cognitions versus behaviors to FAP. The present study shows that in general, both were relevant, but the specific type of cognition or behavior made a difference. For example, although children with FAP reported negative thoughts about their condition more often than the control group, these thoughts did not significantly contribute to the differentiation of these two groups and were unrelated to the physical or emotional functioning of children with FAP. In contrast, the other types of children’s thoughts – children’s positive beliefs in their own problem and emotion focused coping potential – were found to be related to less pain, less disability and less anxious symptoms, which was in accordance with other studies (Anderson et al., 2008; Walker et al., 2008). Children’s coping behavior, however, had an even more important role than children’s positive cognitions, as it contributed to both the differentiation between clinical and control children, and the physical and emotional functioning of children with FAP. It should be noted here that whereas the results for active and passive coping were in line with previous research, contradictory results were found for accommodative coping, which was related to both more AP and less disability. This finding might be explained by reversing the causal direction of the relationship between accommodative coping and AP; then, if children with FAP experience more AP, they may tend to use accommodative coping more often. Using this strategy more often, in it’s turn, may lead to less disability even though the pain might be worse. Of course, in the present study no conclusions on directionality can be drawn. Future research is needed to further explore the role of accommodative coping. Concerning parental cognitions and behaviors, we found that parental cognitions have a stronger relationship with the complaints of the child than parental behavior. Solicitous behavior failed to reach significance in both analyses. It should be noted here that parents of children with FAP actually reported on average less solicitous behavior than parents of control children. Obviously, solicitous behavior was not very common in our sample, which might explain why it does not seem to have a large general influence on the functioning of every child in our sample. For cognitions, we did find an overall effect. As stated above,

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 92 18-07-12 17:19

Page 96: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

5

Parental vs. children’s cognitions and behaviors | 93

if parents thought more negatively about the seriousness of the condition of their child, the odds of their child having FAP significantly increased. Also, if parents thought more positively about the problem-focused coping potential of their child, this was related to less AP, a better quality of life and decreased odds of being diagnosed with FAP.

Additional FindingsIt should also be noted that our study showed some curious unexpected results. First, we did not find a difference in anxious symptoms between the clinical and control group, whereas other studies in other countries usually find that anxiety disorders are very common in children with FAP (Campo et al., 2004). Second, we found that the control group reported more functional disability due to AP than the clinical group, whereas our clinical group showed comparable levels of disability as reported in other studies (Claar & Walker, 2006). A general explanation for these findings might be that some selection bias occurred when preselecting children that experienced at least some amount of AP for the control group of this study. However, the control group used in this study did differ in the expected direction from children with FAP on other outcome measures like depressive symptoms. Moreover, especially for anxiety the results remain surprising, as anxiety usually is the most prevalent co-morbid condition among children with FAP. Other studies investigating the prevalence of anxiety in children with FAP in the Netherlands making use of other instruments than self-report seem warranted. Also, the results for functional disability are – even when considering a selection bias – very counter-intuitive and might be best explained by the phenomenon of response shift. Response shift theory suggests that when faced with a threatening life event like illness, people readjust life goals, standards and values, providing an explanation for the fact that patients often still label their quality of life as similar to healthy people (Sprangers & Schwartz, 1999). Thus, children that experience AP only once in a while may find AP to be far more bothersome than children with FAP who have to live with it every day.

Implications for TreatmentThe present study shows that CBT protocols justly focus on teaching children to cope with their complaints, as both coping and control beliefs about coping potential were shown to be related to the physical and emotional functioning of the child. Whether it is useful to stop negative thinking patterns of the child, however, can be questioned. Therefore, a useful avenue for future research might be to investigate whether this element of CBT can be skipped without decreasing effectiveness. This study also shows that CBT protocols should continue to engage parents in the treatment of FAP. Although it is questionable whether parents have a large direct influence on the way children feel, it seems essential to discuss with parents how they think about the condition of their child to diminish negative thinking patterns. Also, it seems relevant to involve parents in the coping skills

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 93 18-07-12 17:19

Page 97: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

94 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

training for children, as this might improve parental beliefs about whether their child can cope with the pain. As solicitous behavior of the parent seemed unrelated to FAP, it can be questioned whether treatments should focus on this type of parental behavior in all children.

LimitationsThis study has a number of limitations that need to be addressed. First, the control group we selected for this study experienced more AP and psychological complaints than the other children in the control group with parent data, and thus, as mentioned above, a selection bias might have occurred. The design of the study made this inevitable: as we reasoned it would be useless to ask children who never experienced AP how they thought about and coped with AP, we preselected children who experienced AP regularly enough to be able to remember a current episode. We feel this pre-selection does not invalidate the study because it was necessary. However, it should be considered that the control group we used was not representative for a general population of school children. A second limitation of our study is that all data were gathered by self-report, which may have caused some bias. Third, as the design of this study was cross-sectional, inferences about cause and effect cannot be made. Future longitudinal and experimental studies are warranted to sort out cause and effect. Finally, it should be noted that the parents included in our study were mostly mothers, and the distribution of mothers and fathers was somewhat different in both groups. As a recent study in schoolchildren showed differential effects in the reactions of mothers versus fathers to pain (Vervoort, Huguet, Verhoeven, & Goubert, 2011), it might be interesting to investigate the specific influence of each parent. Also, we have solely focused on solicitous behavior in the present study, whereas other types of behavior might be more readily related to FAP.

ConclusionWe conclude that both parents and children may influence the AP a child experiences. Concerning the influence of parents, the way parents think about the AP of their child seems more important than concrete displays of solicitous behavior. For children, promoting positive thoughts about control over the AP and stimulating adaptive coping behavior might be more important than decreasing catastrophic thoughts. Future studies are encouraged to investigate which specific elements of CBT bring about what type of change in children and parents, to further our understanding on how best to treat children suffering from FAP.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 94 18-07-12 17:19

Page 98: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 95 18-07-12 17:19

Page 99: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 96 18-07-12 17:19

Page 100: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

Ch

apte

r

6Attentional bias to activity

of different parts of the body in children with functional

abdominal pain

S. M. C. van der Veek, H. H. F. Derkx, M. A. Benninga, R. D. Plak, F. Boer, R. J. L. Lindauer, & E. de Haan, submitted for publication

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 97 18-07-12 17:19

Page 101: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

98 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Abstract

Objectives: The present study aimed to investigate whether children with functional abdominal pain (FAP) show an attentional bias for information concerning the activity of their body. Additionally, in accordance with Pennebaker’s symptom perception hypothesis, we investigated whether being presented with information about bodily activity influenced perception of bodily sensations.Method: Thirty children with FAP and thirty healthy children filled out questionnaires and performed a dot-probe detection task, in which children were shown sham pictures about their bodily activity.Results: Results showed no attentional bias for gut-activity in either children with FAP or control children, although children with FAP were slower than healthy children on all supraliminal trials in which gut-activity was displayed. Children with FAP and healthy children showed an attentional bias away from supraliminal pictures about heart-activity. Thirty to forty percent of children with FAP experienced an increase in abdominal pain due to the experiment, which holds implications for interventions. Discussion: We conclude that children with FAP did not show an attentional bias for information about gut-activity, whereas similarly to healthy children they directed their attention away from information about heart-activity. Overall, children with FAP only differed from healthy children concerning their responses to gut-activity, not concerning their responses to other bodily activity.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 98 18-07-12 17:19

Page 102: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

6

Attentional bias to bodily activity | 99

Introduction

Functional abdominal pain (FAP) is very common in childhood, and usually affects daily life significantly (Chitkara et al., 2005; Youssef et al., 2006). Although the exact etiology of FAP remains elusive, some studies suggest that an attentional bias for pain may maintain or prolong the complaints (Eccleston & Crombez, 1999; Pincus & Morley, 2001; Whitehead & Palsson, 1998). Thus far, only two studies have investigated attention processes in children with FAP (Beck et al., 2011; Boyer et al., 2006). Both studies used the well-known dot-probe paradigm to investigate children’s attention allocation to pain-words (Beck et al., 2011; Boyer et al., 2006). The results were contradictory, as one study showed that children with FAP avoided pain-words (Boyer et al., 2006), whereas the other reported that children directed their attention towards pain-words (Beck et al., 2011). Other studies into attention biases for pain making use of pain-words have also yielded mixed results (Asmundson, Wright, & Hadjistavropoulos, 2005; Pincus & Morley, 2001; Roelofs, Peters, Fassaert, & Vlaeyen, 2005; Roelofs, Peters, & Vlaeyen, 2002; Roelofs, Peters, Zeegers, & Vlaeyen, 2002). An explanation for these mixed results might be that studies are using the wrong type of stimuli to measure attentional biases relevant for chronic pain. Attentional biases for pain-words may be caused merely by the negative valence or familiarity of the words (Richter, Ech, Straube, Miltner, & Weiss, 2010; Roelofs et al., 2002; Schoth & Liossi, 2010). Moreover, as it is normal and adaptive for pain to draw attention, an attentional bias for other stimuli that might get associated with pain over time – e.g., harmless bodily sensations – might be a stronger risk factor for (the continuation of ) chronic pain (Eccleston & Crombez, 1999; Lautenbacher, 2010). This idea was also suggested by the symptom perception hypothesis, a theory stemming from the broader literature on medically unexplained symptoms. According to this theory, patients with medically unexplained symptoms are a) more attentive to bodily sensations, scanning their body for signs of threatening activity, and b) more likely to interpret benign, non-noxious bodily sensations as painful or pathological (Pennebaker, 1982; Watson & Pennebaker, 1989). Thus, patients with chronic functional pain might have an attention bias for bodily activity, which may maintain their complaints.

In the present study we investigated whether children with FAP show an attentional bias for information concerning the activity of their body. To do this, a dot-probe paradigm was used similar to the one used by Jellesma, Faddegon and Van der Veek (2011). As bodily activity cannot be measured in a low-cost, non-invasive way, these researchers used sham

pictures of what children believed to display their current heart rate as stimuli in their dot-probe task. Similarly, in the present study, we used sham pictures about the activity of the children’s gut, their heart and the circulation in one of their legs. We included pictures about the heart because the attentional bias might not be specific to a certain body part but may include all threatening bodily information (Pennebaker, 1982). Additionally, we investigated whether the attentional bias could actually be attributed to threatening bodily

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 99 18-07-12 17:19

Page 103: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

100 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

information, by including non-threatening control stimuli about blood-flow in one of the legs. To investigate both automatic and controlled attention allocation, we presented all stimuli at preconscious as well as conscious presentation rates (Vasey & MacLeod, 2001; Wolters et al., 2011). As anxiety and depression, other somatic symptoms like headache, and self-reported vigilance for pain have been shown to correlate significantly with attention biases for pain-words (Beck et al., 2011; Boyer et al., 2006), we investigated the effects of these variables as well. Finally, as focusing attention on bodily activity might increase the perception of bodily sensations (Nouwen, Cloutier, Kappas, Warbrick, & Sheffield, 2006; Pennebaker, 1982; Watson & Pennebaker, 1989), we investigated the influence of the dot-probe task on perceived bodily sensations.

In accordance with the literature described above, we hypothesized that children with FAP would show a conscious and preconscious attentional bias for information about the activity of their gut, and, to a lesser extent, for information about their heart rate. We expected that symptoms of anxiety and depression, other somatic symptoms, and self-reported pain vigilance would show significant relationships with these attentional biases. Finally, we expected children with FAP but not healthy children to report increased bodily sensations following the task.

Materials and Methods

Sample

Children with FAPThe clinical group (N=30; 19 girls, 11 boys) was consecutively recruited between January 2010 and June 2011 from a) the general pediatric and pediatric gastroenterology outpatient clinics at a university hospital in the Netherlands, and b) from an outpatient clinic of an academic center for child and adolescent psychiatry, specialized in the psychological treatment of children with FAP. Children ranged in age from 8 to 17 years, with a mean of 13.1 years and a standard deviation of 2.73. The first 17 included children also participated in a randomized controlled trial investigating the efficacy of cognitive behavior therapy versus medical care for children with FAP. Children were eligible if they were between 8 and 18 years of age and fulfilled the following criteria, in accordance with the Rome III criteria (Rasquin et al., 2006): abdominal pain is main complaint, pain lasted at least 8 weeks in past 12 months, no red flags or alarm signals present (e.g., involuntary weight loss, vomiting, significant diarrhea, blood in stool), no physical disease that can explain the symptoms (e.g. Crohn’s disease), no major surgery that can explain the symptoms, no psychosis or autism spectrum disorders. All children approached to participate in the current study, agreed.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 100 18-07-12 17:19

Page 104: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

6

Attentional bias to bodily activity | 101

Control group Control children were recruited between May 2010 and June 2011 from a database of children that participated in a previous study of our research group (Van der Veek et al., 2010), and who’s parents had indicated to be interested in participating in future studies. Children that matched the age and gender of the clinical children were screened for inclusion by telephone. If children had experienced abdominal pain regularly (more than four times per year) in the past two years or had any other substantial current physical or psychological problems, they were excluded. If no match could be found in the database mentioned above, which was the case for 6 children, colleagues and acquaintances were asked whether their children were willing to participate. Of the 42 children approached, 30 (71.4%) agreed to participate. Nineteen girls and 11 boys were included (age ranged from 8 to 16 years, mean age=13.2, SD=2.58).

ProcedureThe design of this study was experimental. Ethical approval was obtained from the Medical Ethical Committee of the university hospital where children were recruited. Children were asked to participate by one of the researchers. Children with FAP were given the choice to either be tested at home or at the outpatient clinic for child and adolescent psychiatry that participated in this study. Control children were tested at home. When children were tested at home, the experimenter made sure that children could perform the task quietly without distraction. If parents agreed, children received a gift token of €10 for their participation.

Both children and their parents received written information concerning the goals and the procedure of the experiment beforehand, and were required to give written consent prior to the start of the experiment. As for the experiment it was necessary that children believed that the pictures actually displayed a snapshot of the current activity of their body, while in fact, this was not the case, we couldn’t disclose the full purpose and procedure of the experiment before it started. Directly after the experiment, parents and children were debriefed about the full facts of the study. At the start of the experiment, children filled out the questionnaires concerning abdominal pain, symptoms of anxiety and depression, physical symptoms and self-reported vigilance to pain. Children were also asked to rate how much abdominal pain they experienced just before the dot-probe task started. Then, they performed the dot-probe task, followed by a validation check for subliminal presentation. Next, children were asked to rate their level of abdominal pain at that moment, and to fill out the questionnaires concerning the influence of the task on bodily activity/sensations. When all questionnaires were completed, children and their parents were debriefed about the full facts of the study.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 101 18-07-12 17:19

Page 105: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

102 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Measures

Dot-probe detection task Children’s attentional orienting to bodily activity was measured using a dot-probe detection task. During a dot-probe detection task, two stimuli are presented to participants on either side of a computer screen, one threatening and one neutral. When these stimuli disappear, a dot appears in either the location of the threatening or the neutral stimulus, and participants are instructed to indicate as fast as they can at what location the dot appeared, by pressing the right key on the keyboard (MacLeod, Mathews, & Tata, 1986). If patients have an attentional bias for the threatening stimulus, they will be faster on trials in which the dot replaces the threatening stimulus (congruent trials) than on trials in which the dot replaces the neutral one (incongruent trials), because their focus will be on the location of the threatening stimulus and thus they will be able to detect the dot more quickly.

Programming and presentation time. The dot-probe detection task was programmed making use of E-prime 2.0 and was presented to the children on a Dell Inspiron 9300 laptop with a 17-inch screen. All pictures and other stimuli were presented on a white background. Following Beck et al. (2011) and Boyer et al. (2006), a black addition mark (+) was presented at the start of each trial for 1000 ms. Pictures were then displayed on the left and right hand side of the screen, separated by 4.6 centimeters (1.8 inch). The center of the pictures were 18.35 centimeters (7.2 inch) apart, displayed at ¼ and ¾ of the width of the screen. Following the pictures, the dot-probe – a black period (.) – appeared at one of the picture locations.

In accordance with Beck et al. (2011) and Boyer et al. (2006), the pictures were displayed for 1250 ms to measure conscious or supraliminal attentional biases. Also in accordance with these studies, we aimed to measure preconscious or subliminal attention biases by displaying pictures for 20 ms. However, because of the refresh rate of the laptop on which the task was programmed, it was only possible to display pictures for 17 ms. Therefore, subliminal pictures were presented for 17 ms, followed by a mask for 1233 ms.

Stimuli. The stimuli used in this task were pictures (size: width 13.75 cm (5.4 inch) x height 9.4 cm (3.7 inch)) of what was to be believed to depict the current activity of the children’s gut, their current heart rate, the current circulation in one of their legs, and the current activity of the laptop on which they performed the task. To depict each type of activity, four pictures were used which either schematically displayed the silhouette of a person and the relevant body part, or a laptop (see figure 1). The pictures were deemed suitable for children aged 8-18 years by a team of experienced child therapists (2nd, 3rd and 7th author) and pediatric gastroenterologists (3rd and 4th author). The level of activity was indicated by a meter resembling a speedometer of a car. This meter was allowed to randomly vary between 11 different positions. We chose this many positions to increase the credibility of the experiment. The extremes of the meters on the left and on the right hand side of the picture were marked red, indicating a ‘danger zone’. This red

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 102 18-07-12 17:19

Page 106: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

6

Attentional bias to bodily activity | 103

color gradually changed to orange, yellow and green, green indicating a ‘safe zone’. The 11 different positions were all in the green zone, both for ethical considerations (we did not want to scare the children) and practical reasons; this way, it was more likely that the different positions of the meters would not elicit different attentional reactions and could thus be pooled for analyses. The mask for the subliminal trials was constructed by pasting all of the four pictures on top of each other and adding a blurry filter.

To make sure children knew what each picture represented, we explained the pictures in great detail to the children, showing each of the four types of pictures to them and emphasizing that the laptop continuously updated the information about their bodily activity to make new pictures. Then, children were asked to indicate what each picture represented, to be sure they knew what kind of activity each picture displayed. All children found it very easy to rate what each picture represented and none of them made any errors.

Trials. As this study included four types of pictures, it was not possible to present every combination of pictures to the children, as this would make the task too long. Therefore, we made the following selection of combinations:- Gut – laptop- Heart – laptop- Gut – leg- Laptop – laptop- Leg – legBoth the laptop and leg pictures were considered to be neutral stimuli, the pictures about the gut and heart threatening. Thus, in the first three types of trials a threatening stimulus was combined with a neutral stimulus. Making use of these trials, we were able to investigate whether attentional biases were present. The laptop-laptop trials were included to establish the baseline RTs of children, and the RTs on these trials were compared to the RTs on the leg-leg trials to investigate whether ‘leg-activity’ was actually perceived as a neutral stimulus by the children. The three types of neutral – threat trials were presented with the neutral picture on either the left or the right side of the screen. Thus, in total, 8 types of picture-combinations were available. The dot could replace either the left or the right picture, making a total of 16 possible trials. For the neutral – threat trials, trials in which the dot replaced the threatening stimulus were considered ‘congruent’; if the dot replaced the neutral stimulus, the trial was considered ‘incongruent’. Each trial was presented 10 times to the child, 5 times supraliminal and 5 times subliminal, making a total of 160 trials. These were presented to the children in two blocks of 80 trials. The order in which the trials were presented in each block was set randomly beforehand, and was the same for every child. Each type of picture was presented equally often in both blocks. Also, the number of supraliminal and subliminal trials were divided equally between blocks. The position of the meter differed randomly per picture, and this was allowed to be different for every child.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 103 18-07-12 17:19

Page 107: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

104 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Figure 1. Stimuli used in the dot-probe detection task. Position of meter randomly varied between the extremes depicted in picture a and b.

a. Stimulus depicting gut activity b. Stimulus depicting heart rate

c. Stimulus depicting circulation in leg d. Stimulus depicting activity of laptop

e. Mask used for subliminal presentations

Procedure for the dot-probe detection task. At the start of the task, the experimenter explained the children that for the computer task they were going to do, it was necessary to measure the activity of their gut (bowel movements), the activity of their heart (heart rate) and how fast the blood flowed through one of their legs. They were shown the ECG stickers that were going to be used to make these measurements, and how these were connected to the laptop through alligator clips, wires and a device that connected the wires through a USB portal to the laptop. Then, three stickers were put on the children’s abdomen, one was put on their chest and a final sticker was put on one of their calves.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 104 18-07-12 17:19

Page 108: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

6

Attentional bias to bodily activity | 105

After the ECG stickers were connected to the laptop, children were asked to sit in front of the laptop, approximately 60 cm from the screen. The experimenter made sure the screen of the laptop was in an optimal angle for each child. Then, they received instructions on how to perform the task; they were instructed to look at the fixation mark and respond as quickly as possible to the probe, making as few errors as possible. They were instructed to keep their index fingers on the ‘s’ and ‘k’ keys of the keyboard and to press the ‘s’ when the probe was on the left side of the screen, and the ‘k’ if the probe was on the right side. Subsequently, children were allowed to practice with 8 trials, to make sure they understood how the dot-probe task worked. After this training session, two blocks of 80 trials were presented to the children. They were allowed to take a break after the first block. Finally, children were asked to perform the validation check for subliminal presentation (see below). This entire sequence took about 15 minutes.

Validation check for subliminal presentation. To check whether the subliminal pictures were actually processed outside of conscious awareness, we asked children in three subliminal trials following the dot-probe task to indicate whether they saw what picture was displayed on the left and on the right hand side of the screen. They were allowed to choose either of the four possible pictures.

QuestionnairesAbdominal pain. Level of abdominal pain was measured by the Abdominal Pain Index

(API) (Walker et al., 1997). The API consists of five questions, and assesses the frequency, duration and intensity of the abdominal pain the child experienced in the past two weeks. The first two questions make use of a 6-point scale, the third of a 9-point scale and the last two questions of an 11-point scale. A total score for the API was computed by recoding each item so that it reflects a scale ranging from 0 to 10, and summing all items, yielding a total score ranging from 0-50 (Van der Veek et al., 2010). The API has been shown to be a reliable instrument in previous studies, with a Cronbach’s alpha ranging from .80 to .93 (Walker et al., 1997). Cronbach’s alpha in the present study was .94. Anxious and depressive symptoms. Children filled out the Revised Anxiety and Depression Scale – short version (RCADS-25; Muris et al., 2002) to measure symptoms of anxiety and depression. The shortened version of the RCADS has been shown to be a valid and reliable instrument to measure symptoms of generalized anxiety disorder, separation anxiety disorder, social phobia, panic disorder, and major depressive disorder (Muris et al., 2002). Each of the five scales consists of five items that have to be scored on a 4-point scale, ranging from (0) never to (3) always. Cronbach’s alphas of the five scales ranged from .65 to .83 in previous research (Muris et al., 2002). For the present study, the four scales of the different anxiety disorders were summed to compute one score for anxious symptoms. A separate score was calculated for depressive symptoms. Cronbach’s alpha in the present study was .93 for anxious and .77 for depressive symptoms.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 105 18-07-12 17:19

Page 109: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

106 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Somatic complaints. The Dutch version of the Children’s Somatization Inventory (CSI; Garber et al., 1991; Ghys & Meesters, 1993; Meesters et al., 2003; Walker et al., 1991; Walker & Greene, 1989) was used to assess somatic complaints. The CSI contains 35 items that have to be rated on a 5-point scale, ranging from (0) not at all to (4) a whole lot, reflecting the extent to which the symptoms were experienced in the past two weeks. A total score can be computed by summing the scores across all items, with higher scores indicating a higher intensity of somatic complaints. A number of items on the CSI are items concerning abdominal pain, and these items were not used in the total score for this study. The following items were omitted: nausea/upset stomach; constipation; diarrhea; pain in stomach; vomiting; bloated stomach; food intolerance. The CSI has been shown to be a reliable and valid instrument for assessing somatization symptoms in children and adolescents (Meesters et al., 2003).

Pain vigilance. To measure self-reported vigilance to pain, the Pain Vigilance and Awareness Questionnaire was used (PVAQ; Roelofs, Peters, McCracken, & Vlaeyen, 2003; Roelofs, Peters, Muris, & Vlaeyen, 2002). The PVAQ consists of 16 items which can be answered on a 6-point Likert scale, ranging from (0) never to (5) always. It has been validated in a Dutch sample of adult healthy participants and fibromyalgia patients (Roelofs et al., 2003; Roelofs et al., 2002). For the present study, the items were adapted slightly to accommodate the reading level of the children participating in our study. Total scores were calculated by summing all items. Cronbach’s alpha in the present study was .88.

Influence of the task on bodily activity/sensations. To investigate whether performing the dot-probe task influenced the perception of bodily symptoms, children were asked in a series of 14 questions designed for this study whether they perceived any changes in bodily activity or sensations due to the dot-probe task they performed. Children were asked to respond on a five point Likert scale ranging from “completely untrue” (1) to “completely true” (5) to the following questions: 1) Because of this computer task, my abdominal pain got worse; 2) When I saw the “abdominal meter”, I got more abdominal pain; 3) When the abdominal meter was low, I thought the abdominal pain would decrease; 4) When the abdominal meter was high, I got more abdominal pain; 5) When the abdominal meter was low, I got less abdominal pain. These 5 questions were repeated for the influence of pictures of the heart and the leg (for this last category, the last question was omitted as it was not applicable to sensations in the leg). Separate total scores were calculated for each type of picture; higher scores meant that children perceived more changes in bodily sensations due to the task. Cronbach’s alpha in the present study was .79 for the abdominal pain-scale, .82 for the heart-scale, and .64 for the leg-scale.

To investigate the influence of the task on level of abdominal pain, we used an additional measure; a difference score between how much pain children experienced before and directly after the experiment. Children were asked to rate their abdominal

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 106 18-07-12 17:19

Page 110: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

6

Attentional bias to bodily activity | 107

pain on these two occasions on the same 11-point scale as used by the last two items of the API, ranging from “no pain” (0) to “the most pain possible” (10). Positive scores on this measure meant that the abdominal pain increased; negative scores meant that the abdominal pain decreased.

Statistical analysesPrior to performing the main analyses, we checked whether the stimuli depicting activity of circulation in the legs could actually be perceived as a neutral stimulus, by comparing RTs on leg-leg trials with RTs on laptop-laptop trials. Also, we investigated whether the eleven different meter positions affected RTs. Next, to investigate whether children with FAP showed an attentional bias to information about the activity of their gut compared to healthy children, we first performed a 2 (congruency: congruent vs. incongruent) x 2 (presentation time: supraliminal vs. subliminal) repeated measures ANOVA (RM-ANOVA) on the RTs of gut-laptop trials, with diagnostic group (FAP vs. healthy) and age group (8-12 years vs 13-18 years) as between-subject factors. We corrected for age group as previous research from anxiety literature suggests that attentional biases might be different for younger and older children (Bar-Haim et al., 2007; Wolters et al., 2011). This analysis was repeated for the other threat-neutral trials. Second, we calculated attentional bias scores by subtracting RTs of the congruent trials (dot replacing threat-picture) from RTs of the incongruent trials (dot replacing neutral picture). A positive attentional bias score thus reflects that children direct their attention toward the threatening stimulus, whereas a negative attentional bias score reflects that children direct their attention away from the threatening stimulus. The bias scores were compared with zero to investigate whether any bias was present. Additionally, the scores and were compared between both diagnostic groups to establish whether groups differed in their attentional bias.

It should be noted that although both analytical strategies described above seem to have the same endpoint, there is an important difference that justifies the use of both techniques. The advantage of using attentional bias scores is that these scores cancel out confounding factors influencing RTs like age: younger children usually are slower than older children, but they will be slower on both congruent and incongruent trials, and thus this overall age effect will be cancelled out in the bias scores. A disadvantage of this approach is that it might also cancel out overall effects that we are interested in, like the main effect of diagnostic group on RTs. It is conceivable that the experiment in general or some trials of the experiment might be more stressful for children with FAP than for healthy children, resulting in larger RTs overall for the clinical group (e.g., Jellesma et al., 2011). Therefore, both strategies were used.

To investigate whether attentional bias was related to symptoms of anxiety and depression, other somatic symptoms, and self-reported pain vigilance, we calculated correlations. Finally, to investigate whether the dot-probe task influenced children’s

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 107 18-07-12 17:19

Page 111: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

108 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

perception of bodily sensations, we calculated what percentage of children felt changes in bodily sensations in each group and compared these percentages as well as the raw scores on the questionnaires between children with FAP and healthy control children.

Results

Preliminary analyses

Response time preparation and outliersIn accordance with previous studies (Boyer et al., 2006; Wolters et al., 2011), outlying RTs smaller than 100 ms and larger than 4000 ms were deleted, as well as RTs that were at least 3 standard deviations from the individual mean. Additionally, RTs corresponding to erroneous responses (child pressed opposite key) were deleted. Next, before calculating mean RTs per condition, it was investigated whether the eleven different meter positions yielded differences in RTs. Scatter plots and correlations showed no relationships between meter position and RTs per trial type. Thus, mean RTs per condition were calculated, collapsing meter positions. The distributions of these mean RTs were skewed, which was resolved by a log-transformation. Attentional bias scores were calculated by subtracting RTs of congruent trials from RTs of incongruent trials, making use of untransformed RTs. Inspection of the distributions of the bias scores showed significant skewness and kurtosis for every bias score except for the bias score of supraliminal gut-laptop trials. Therefore, parametric tests were used for the supraliminal gut-laptop trials whereas for the other trials, non-parametric tests were used.

For two participants, the masking procedure in the subliminal trials was delayed due to computer failures, resulting in a longer exposure time than 17 ms. These two participants were excluded from analyses on the subliminal trials. Additionally, two children in the control group were outliers on most of the conditions, showing much larger RTs than their peers. These cases were excluded for analyses making use of the log-transformed RT data. For the analyses on attentional bias, only one of these two outliers proved an outlier on most of the bias scores and was therefore excluded from these analyses. Untransformed mean RTs and mean and median attentional bias scores per group are displayed in table 1 and 2.

Credibility experimentWhen children were debriefed about the true nature of the study, all but four of them were genuinely surprised to hear that the ECG stickers did not actually measure their bodily activity. The other four children told the experimenter that they already thought the pictures were not real, although they weren’t sure of that at the time they were making the dot-probe task. A MANOVA showed no differences in RTs on the different trials between children that

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 108 18-07-12 17:19

Page 112: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

6

Attentional bias to bodily activity | 109

had believed the task and children who had not (Wilks’ Lambda F(10,44) = .79; p =.327). Also, no differences were found between both groups on the questionnaires about changes in bodily sensations due to the dot-probe task (Wilks’ Lambda F(4,55) = .98; p =.910). Therefore, the data of the children who did not fully believe the task were used for analyses.

Table 1. Mean reaction times per diagnostic group on each trial type

Congruent trials Incongruent trials

FAP Control FAP ControlSupraliminal a

gut-laptop 448.08 (92.48) 404.74 (70.21) 450.49 (91.18) 410.52 (71.04)heart-laptop 449.06 (89.97) 413.71 (80.02) 433.50 (69.43) 406.99 (71.20)gut-leg 451.77 (84.9) 419.39 (95.33) 446.20 (85.10) 418.98 (85.81)laptop-laptop 442.23 (84.18) 401.88 (78.98) - -leg-leg 467.82 (94.71) 431.51 (79.70) - -

Subliminal b

gut-laptop 408.72 (65.53) 395.98 (77.85) 412.77 (83.37) 389.64 (71.65)heart-laptop 415.70 (76.54) 401.39 (83.07) 412.79 (67.56) 408.67 (72.15)gut-leg 411.31 (72.38) 392.64 (72.97) 411.31 (72.38) 396.43 (79.29)laptop-laptop 424.58 (76.55) 401.41 (85.79) - -leg-leg 427.97 (73.19) 382.58 (74.57) - -

a N for clinical group is 30, N for control group is 28.b N for clinical group is 29, N for control group is 27.

Can leg-pictures be considered neutral stimuli?To investigate whether pictures of the circulation in the legs of the children were conceived as neutral by the children, we compared RTs on leg-leg trials to RTs on laptop-laptop trials in a 2 (trial type: leg-leg vs. laptop-laptop) x 2 (presentation time: supraliminal vs. subliminal) RM-ANOVA with diagnostic group and age group as between subjects factors. This analysis showed significant main effects for trial type (F(1,52) = 6.88; p =.011), presentation time (F (1,52) = 24.69; p <.001), and age group (F(1,52) = 11.08; p =.002), and a significant interaction effect between trial type and presentation time (F(1,52) = 19.80; p <.001), indicating that all children, both FAP and control, had higher RTs following leg-leg trials than laptop-laptop trials. This effect was more pronounced in supraliminal than in subliminal trials. Thus, leg-pictures did not evoke similar RTs as the neutral laptop-pictures. In fact, judging from table 1 we can conclude that, compared to all of the other types of trials, both children with FAP and control children were slowest on supraliminal leg-leg trials. These results were not expected beforehand and we could not think of a sound theoretical explanation for them. In fact, we believe that the most straightforward explanation for this finding has to do with some basic feature of the leg-pictures as opposed to the other pictures: as can be seen in figure 1, the leg picture is somewhat more ‘zoomed out’ than the other pictures. This might explain the slow response of children as they might have been distracted by these somewhat different features, or the pictures may have captured their attention less because of the ‘zoomed out’ quality. As the leg-pictures obviously did not

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 109 18-07-12 17:19

Page 113: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

110 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

evoke the reaction pattern we expected and as this was most probably caused by a design-error, we decided to leave any trials making use of the leg-pictures out of further analyses.

Validation check for subliminal presentationBoth children with FAP and control children did not score significantly above chance level (25% accuracy rate) on 83.3% of the subliminal trials during the validation check. Of course, an optimal rate would be that the children would not score above chance on 100% of the trials. However, this is mitigated by the fact that prior to the validation check, children were specifically instructed that they were going to be asked which picture was presented. This might have enhanced their percentage of correct answers. No such instructions were given prior to performing the actual dot-probe task; children weren’t even told that during masked trials a picture concerning their bodily activity preceded the mask. As even with an explicit instruction children were unable to detect the actual picture on nearly all trials, we feel it is safe to conclude that the subliminal pictures during the dot-probe task were processed outside of conscious awareness.

Main analyses

Attentional bias for activity of the gutThe RM-ANOVAs revealed no significant main effect for congruency (F(1,52) = .001; p =.974), nor a significant interaction effect between congruency and diagnostic group (F(1,52) = .257; p =.614), indicating that both children with FAP and healthy control children did not show an attentional bias for activity of the gut. The only significant effects found were a main effect for age group (younger children were slower; F(1,52) = 15.48; p <.001) and for presentation time (F(1,52) = 17.03; p <.001).

Because of this latter effect, we performed two subsequent separate RM-ANOVAs for supraliminal and subliminal trials. Both analyses also did not show significant main effects for congruency (F(1,54) = .10, p =.754 for supraliminal trials; F(1,52) = .01, p =.926 for subliminal trials) or interaction effects between congruency and diagnostic group (F(1,52) = .01, p =.910 for supraliminal trials; F(1,52) = .87, p =.355 for subliminal trials). Thus, these results indicate there was no attentional bias for gut activity present in either group. The analyses on the attentional bias scores (see table 2) confirmed this absence of an attentional bias, as both groups did not score differently from 0 for both supraliminal and subliminal presentation rates (bias scores of both groups collapsed because there was no difference between groups: supraliminal t(58) = .952, p =.345; subliminal Wilcoxon signed rank test=711.50, N=57, p =.480). Spearman correlation analyses as depicted in table 3 showed no significant correlations between attentional bias scores and symptoms of anxiety and depression, other somatic complaints, and self-reported pain vigilance in either group.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 110 18-07-12 17:19

Page 114: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

6

Attentional bias to bodily activity | 111

Between group analyses on the separate RM-ANOVAs for the supraliminal and subliminal trials additionally showed that younger children were slower on both types of trials (F(1,54) = 13.94, p <.001 for supraliminal trials; F(1,52) = 16.05; p <.001 for subliminal trials). For the supraliminal trials, but not for the subliminal trials, a main effect for diagnostic group was found (F (1,54) = 4.116; p =.047), with larger RTs for children with FAP than for healthy children. This indicates that when children with FAP were presented either congruent or incongruent supraliminal trials depicting pictures of gut and laptop activity, they were slower to respond to the dot than healthy children. This was not the case for trials that were presented outside of conscious awareness.

Attentional bias for activity of the heartAnother RM-ANOVA was performed for heart-laptop trials, investigating the effect of congruency, presentation time, diagnostic group and age group. Again, no significant main effect for congruency was found (F(1,52) = .25; p =.619), nor a significant interaction effect between congruency and diagnostic group (F(1,52) = 1.85; p =.180). However, a significant main effect for presentation time (F(1,52) = 7.72; p =.008) and significant interaction effects between presentation time and diagnostic group (F(1,52) = 5.09; p =.028) and presentation time and congruency (F(1,52) = 5.55; p =.022) justified separate analyses for supraliminal and subliminal trials. The RM-ANOVA for supraliminal trials revealed a trend for congruency (F(1,54) = 3.79; p =.057) and a trend for the three-way interaction between congruency, diagnostic group and age group (F(1,54) = 3.73; p =.059). Another main effect was found for age group, with younger children responding slower (F(1,54) = 13.40; p =.001). The analyses on the supraliminal attentional bias scores (table 2) revealed that – when bias scores of both groups were collapsed as there was no difference between groups – there was a significant effect (median=-7.50, Wilcoxon signed rank test=623.50, N=59, p

=.048), indicating that all children directed their attention away from the picture about heart activity on supraliminal trials. Spearman correlations showed a positive correlation between this attentional bias and level of abdominal pain for healthy children (see table 3).

The RM-ANOVA for subliminal trials showed no significant effects except for the same main effect for age group (F(1,52) = 13.21; p =.001). Similarly, the analysis on attentional bias score showed no attentional bias for subliminal pictures about the heart (bias scores of both groups collapsed because there was no difference between groups: Wilcoxon signed rank test=960.00, N=57, p =.289). Spearman correlations revealed that the attentional bias score for subliminal heart pictures was negatively related to other somatic symptoms in the clinical group, and negatively related to symptoms of anxiety and depression in the healthy group.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 111 18-07-12 17:19

Page 115: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

112 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Table 2. Means (SDs) and medians of attentional bias scores

Total group Children with FAP Control children

Mean (SD) Median Mean (SD) Median Mean (SD) MedianSupraliminal N=59 N=30 N=29

bias gut 5.46 (44.05) 3.1 2.41 (49.63) 1.12 8.61 (38.06) 7.20bias heart -11.25 (40.00)* -7.5 -15.56 (46.82) -7.54 -6.80 (31.68) -7.50

Subliminal N=57 N=29 N=28bias gut -.718 (48.50) -3.3 4.05 (55.08) -15.6 -5.66 (41.02) 1.23bias heart 2.67 (39.12) 6.9 -2.92 (45.15) -.8 8.46 (31.51) 13.36

*p <.05 for comparison with value 0. Note. The means for children with FAP and control children did not differ for the supraliminal gut-laptop trials, nor did the medians between both groups differ for the other trials.

Table 3. Spearman correlations between attentional bias scores and questionnaires

Level of abdominal pain

Other somatic complaints

Anxious symptoms

Depressive symptoms

Pain vigilance

Children with FAPSupraliminal biases

Gut -.198 -.141 .049 -.008 .157Heart .161 .158 -.149 -.148 -.216

Subliminal biasesGut .120 .232 .287 .131 .016Heart -.236 -.411* -.084 -.174 -.235

Control groupSupraliminal biases

Gut .009 -.290 -.062 -.027 -.002Heart .387* -.224 -.205 -.198 -.139

Subliminal biasesGut .161 -.092 -.165 -.171 .167Heart .221 -.223 -.465* -.495** -.114

Note. *p <.05; **p <.01. N for children with FAP varied between 29 and 30; N for control group varied from 27-29.

Effect of dot-probe task on bodily sensationsOn the API difference score measuring differences in abdominal pain from pre- to post-task, 30% of the children with FAP reported an increase in AP due to the dot-probe task and 70% reported no change. In the healthy group, 10% reported a decrease in AP, 86.67% reported no change, and 3.33% reported an increase. These percentages significantly differed between groups (χ2(2) = 9.932; p =.007). The results on our 5-item scale for felt changes in AP due to the task mirrored these results, as 40% of the children with FAP reported to have felt at least a slight change in AP, whereas this was only the case for 16.67% of the control group; a significant difference (χ2(1) = 4.022; p =.045). For changes in heart-sensations due to the task, we found that 56.6% of children in the clinical group and 66.7% in the control group felt at least slight changes; these percentages did not differ significantly (χ2 (1) = .635; p =.426). For sensations in the leg, percentages were lower (36.7% for children with FAP, 23.3% for healthy children), and also not significantly different (χ2 (1) = 1.270; p =.260). The results of the nonparametric analyses making use of the absolute scores on the scales (see table 4) mirrored the results of these categorical analyses.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 112 18-07-12 17:19

Page 116: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

6

Attentional bias to bodily activity | 113

Table 4. Means (SDs) and medians of questionnaires about influence task on bodily sensations

Children with FAP Control children

Mean (SD) Median Mean (SD) Median

Change in abdominal pain due to task (API-item; range -10 to 10)

.45 (.89) a 0.00 -.20 (.96) b 0.00

Influence task on abdominal pain (range 1-5) 1.26 (.52) a 1.00 1.10 (.27) b 1.00Influence task on heart (range 1-5) 1.49 (.70) a 1.20 1.61 (.78) a 1.20Influence task on leg (range 1-5) 1.27 (.47) a 1.00 1.16 (.37) a 1.00

Note. Different subscripts indicate that distributions for these variables were significantly different (p <.05) for children with FAP and control children on Mann-Whitney U test.

Discussion

The present experimental study found no evidence for an attentional bias for activity of the gut in either children with FAP or control children. Symptoms of anxiety, depression, other somatic symptoms and self-reported pain vigilance showed no relationships with children’s attentional bias for the activity of their gut. However, children with FAP did have larger reaction times and were thus slower than healthy children on trials in which pictures of gut activity were displayed. One explanation for this slower response rate might be that the pain in itself may interfere with task performance because the chronic presence of pain chronically draws on attentional resources (Eccleston & Crombez, 1999; Pincus & Morley, 2001; Villemure & Bushnell, 2002). However, this implies that children with FAP would be slower on all trials, which was not the case: children were only slower on trials during which they could process the stimuli consciously, not on preconscious subliminal trials. More importantly, this general slowing effect was not present when children were shown information about their heart rate; on those trials, children with FAP had similar reaction times as healthy children. The fact that children with FAP react similarly as healthy children to stimuli concerning the heart but not to pictures about the gut seems to imply that their attentional processing of other bodily sensations is not altered, whereas their processing of information about gut activity might be, although not in the sense of a ‘traditional’ attention bias. However, as this study is the first to investigate an attentional bias for bodily activity in children with FAP, more studies are needed before any definitive conclusions can be drawn.

Concerning information about heart activity, our study found that all children showed an attentional bias for this type of bodily activity. Both children with FAP and healthy children directed their attention away from information about heart activity when this information was processed consciously. As this attentional bias was found in both groups, it seems unlikely that it has any etiological value for pediatric FAP. Indeed, Shields and Murphy showed in a recent experimental study that both low and high health anxious participants showed an attentional bias toward ill-health words, suggesting that interest

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 113 18-07-12 17:19

Page 117: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

114 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

in health is present in everyone (Shields & Murphy, 2011). It is intriguing that this specific stimulus about heart activity elicited an attentional bias whereas information about gut activity did not. A probable explanation for this finding is that children might perceive information about heart rate as more threatening than information about the activity of their gut.

Our last research question considered whether focusing attention on bodily activity might increase the perception of bodily sensations. Our study showed that children with FAP and healthy children did not differ in how many changes in bodily sensations they felt concerning the heart and the leg. However, for level of abdominal pain we did find a difference between groups: only children with FAP reported an increase in abdominal pain from pre- to post task, not healthy children. Although it seems unsurprising that healthy children did not experience an increase in abdominal pain following the task since they experienced little to no pain to begin with, it is remarkable that a short procedure showing sham pictures of bodily information can increase abdominal pain in 30 to 40% of children with FAP. Obviously, this should stimulate researchers in the field of interventions to try to reach the opposite effect. When we assume that the pain increases because of the mere effect of paying attention to bodily sensations, an obvious course of action for interventions is to try to divert patients’ attention away from their bodily sensations; in essence, to distract them. Indeed, it has been shown that explicitly focusing attention on pain can increase the pain experience (Nouwen et al., 2006; Van Laarhoven, Kraaimaat, Wilder-Smith, & Evers, 2010; Villemure & Bushnell, 2002) and thus, distraction could have merit. However, to what extent distraction is effective to reduce pain highly varies across studies, depending on characteristics of the pain, the distraction task and the individual (Eccleston, 1995; Snijders, Ramsey, Koerselman, & Van Gijn, 2010; Verhoeven et al., 2010; Wiech, Ploner, & Tracey, 2008). In fact, when following the line of thought of the symptom perception hypothesis it seems logical that distraction alone will not be sufficient, as the relationship between focusing attention on bodily sensations and experiencing somatic symptoms or pain may not be a direct one, but may be mediated by the way patients interpret their bodily sensations. Others have also suggested that patients with chronic pain may have an interpretation bias, interpreting ambiguous stimuli catastrophically according to predominant accessible schemas (Pincus & Morley, 2001). Catastrophic thinking about pain is suggested to affect the pain experience (Vervoort, Eccleston, Goubert, Buysse, & Crombez, 2010; Vervoort et al., 2005) and even the effectiveness of distraction (Verhoeven et al., 2010). For the present study, we may conclude that it remains unclear whether the increase of abdominal pain in children with FAP is caused by a mere focusing of attention on the body during the task, or whether there are mediating negative or catastrophic interpretations at work. Future studies are encouraged to try to discern this, as it holds practical implications for interventions; if the effect is indeed mediated by explicit or more automatic, implicit negative interpretations, then interventions should try to change such

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 114 18-07-12 17:19

Page 118: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

6

Attentional bias to bodily activity | 115

interpretations in stead of focusing solely on distraction. Within pain-literature no studies have tried to decrease interpretation biases, but results from anxiety literature seem promising (Salemink & van den Hout, 2011). Finally, it should be noted that the observed increase in AP may not be caused by being confronted with information about bodily activity, but by the mere stress of participating in an experiment. Although this effect was also present in the healthy control group, its effects may have been stronger for children with FAP. Future studies are encouraged to apply a more direct test of the hypothesis that focusing attention on the body leads to an increase in symptom perception, by for example experimentally manipulating attention to versus away from bodily stimuli and test effects on symptom perception.

There are some limitations to this study that need to be addressed. First, this study is, next to the study by Jellesma and colleagues (Jellesma et al., 2011), the only one making use of this type of paradigm using sham pictures to investigate attentional biases for bodily activity in children, and more studies are needed to validate the procedure. Second, the results for non-threatening bodily information could unfortunately not be interpreted. Future studies are encouraged to design other experimental studies making use of such a control condition to investigate this effect. Finally, it should be noted that not every child believed our procedure. Although this did not affect reaction times, other researchers should try to make their experiments as convincing as possible.

Despite these limitations, the present study is the first to investigate attentional biases for bodily activity in children with FAP. Although no traditional attentional biases were found toward or away from information about gut activity, we may conclude that children with FAP only differed from healthy children in a) their reaction times to pictures about the gut, and b) the influence of the experiment on perceived abdominal pain. These results imply that if any biases are present in these children, they might be specific for the abdomen. We encourage future researchers to replicate the present study to be able to use the results in treatments for children with FAP.

AcknowledgementsWe would like to thank Daniël Hoekman for designing the stimuli used in the experiment and Pauline Willems for her help with data collection.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 115 18-07-12 17:19

Page 119: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 116 18-07-12 17:19

Page 120: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

IICognitive behavior therapy

for pediatric functional abdominal pain

Part

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 117 18-07-12 17:19

Page 121: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 118 18-07-12 17:19

Page 122: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

Ch

apte

r

7

Cognitive behavior therapy versus medical care for the treatment

of pediatric functional abdominal pain:

A randomized controlled trial

S. M. C. van der Veek, H. H. F. Derkx, M. A. Benninga, F. Boer, & E. de Haan, submitted for publication

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 119 18-07-12 17:19

Page 123: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

120 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Abstract

Objectives: Cognitive behavior therapy (CBT) is a promising treatment for pediatric functional abdominal pain (FAP), but solid evidence for its effectiveness is lacking. This randomized controlled trial aimed to investigate the effectiveness of a six session protocolized CBT, compared to the effects of six visits to a pediatrician (medical care; MC). Method: One hundred and four children aged 7-18 were randomized to CBT or MC. CBT was mostly delivered by trained master students in psychology, MC was delivered by pediatricians or pediatric gastroenterologists. Assessments were performed pre-treatment, post-treatment, and at 6 and 12 months follow-up. Primary outcomes were level of abdominal pain as reported on questionnaires and diaries. Secondary outcomes were other gastrointestinal complaints, functional disability, other somatic complaints, anxiety, depression, and quality of life. Results: Both CBT and MC led to a significant decrease in abdominal pain (p <.001), but the treatments did not differ in their effectiveness (p >.05 for all end points). According to the questionnaire-derived data, one year after treatment 60% of children that received CBT had significantly improved or recovered, versus 56.4% of children receiving MC, which did not significantly differ (p =.473). These percentages were 65.8% versus 62.8% according to the diary-derived data, which also did not significantly differ (p =.139). For secondary outcomes, it was found that directly after treatment and at six months follow-up, children that received CBT experienced fewer symptoms of anxiety and depression than children that received MC (p <.05). Discussion: CBT was equally effective as MC in reducing abdominal pain in children with FAP. More research into the specific working mechanisms of CBT for pediatric FAP is needed.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 120 18-07-12 17:19

Page 124: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

7

Cognitive behavior therapy for FAP | 121

Introduction

Abdominal pain (AP) is a common complaint in children and adolescents. Most children experience occasional episodes of AP (Saps et al., 2009a; Van der Veek et al., 2010), which often resolve spontaneously. However, in about 10% of children in Western countries, the pain does not disappear and becomes chronic (Chitkara et al., 2005), affecting daily life significantly (Claar & Walker, 2006; Youssef et al., 2006). When children suffer from chronic AP, an explanatory organic disease is rarely found. In these cases, the pain is considered ‘functional’. If left untreated, there is a considerable chance that functional abdominal pain (FAP) will persist into adulthood or change into other somatic complaints or psychopathology (Gieteling, Bierma-Zeinstra, Passchier, & Berger, 2008; Hotopf et al., 1998).

As several psychological and social factors are hypothesized to contribute to the occurrence and maintenance of FAP (Drossman, 1998; Lackner, Morley, Dowzer, Mesmer, & Hamilton, 2004), psychological treatments for FAP have been designed and tested in adults, with positive results (Lackner et al., 2004). Recent years have seen an upsurge of research in children with FAP as well, investigating the effectiveness of several psychological therapies, among which cognitive behavior therapy (CBT) (Huertas-Ceballos et al., 2009c; Sprenger, Gerhards, & Goldbeck, 2011). Although CBT is a promising psychological treatment for children with FAP (Huertas-Ceballos et al., 2009c), only 6 randomized controlled trials (RCTs) have been performed to study the effectiveness of CBT (Duarte et al., 2006; Gross & Warschburger, 2012; Levy et al., 2010; Robins et al., 2005; Sanders et al., 1989; Sanders, Shepherd, Cleghorn, & Woolford, 1994). Moreover, most of these trials have considerable methodological draw-backs like small sample sizes or high drop-out rates (Huertas-Ceballos et al., 2009c; Palermo, Eccleston, Lewandowski, de C Williams, & Morley, 2010; Sprenger et al., 2011). An exception is the well-designed and large RCT performed by Levy and colleagues (Levy et al., 2010), who investigated a CBT with a special emphasis on the potential contribution of parents to the maintenance of pediatric FAP. However, the results of this RCT do not provide unequivocal evidence for the benefit of CBT. The CBT-intervention was superior to an educational support control condition directly after treatment and at 3 and 6-month follow-up, but only according to parent report of the child’s current pain, not according to child reported pain and disability. Considering the low quality of the other trials mentioned above, and the mixed results of Levy et al.’s study, it is hard to draw conclusions on the actual effectiveness of CBT for FAP. Therefore, we performed a large RCT, investigating the effectiveness of a 6-session CBT protocol compared to 6 visits to a pediatrician or pediatric gastroenterologist (medical care; MC). We expected CBT to be superior to MC on all outcome measures.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 121 18-07-12 17:19

Page 125: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

122 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Methods

Design and procedureThe study was a prospective RCT. The study protocol was approved by the medical ethical committee of the Academic Medical Center Amsterdam on May 17, 2006. The trial was registered at the Netherlands National Trial Register, trial number NTR1613. Children were screened for inclusion by pediatricians of the general pediatric and pediatric gastroenterology outpatient clinics at the Emma Children’s Hospital AMC in the Netherlands. During a first consult, pediatricians took a medical history of the children, performed a physical examination, and provided verbal and written information about the present RCT. Then, laboratory tests were performed and children were given a two-week diary to assess their level of AP. If children fulfilled the inclusion criteria, children were invited by the pediatrician to participate in this trial. If they agreed, they received two intake sessions (=T1) at De Bascule, academic center for child- and adolescent psychiatry, with the primary investigator (first author) and one of the master psychology students that delivered the CBT. During the first intake session, children and their parents were interviewed about the complaints of the child and the social, cognitive and physical consequences it had on the child’s and family’s daily life. During the second session, the semi-structured Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule (ADIS-C) was separately administered to the child and to (one of ) the parents, to assess whether any psychiatric diagnoses were present in the child. At the end of both sessions, children and parents filled out the baseline questionnaires. If necessary, children were aided in filling out the questionnaires by the primary investigator or masters student; parents simultaneously filled out their questionnaires in a different room. At the end of the second session, the primary investigator randomized the children using a computerized randomization program, stratifying by age (two age groups: 8-12 and 13-17) and gender. Children and their parents were notified of the result of the randomization immediately. Two to three weeks after treatment (T2), 6 months and 12 months after treatment (T3, T4) children and their parents were contacted for a follow-up assessment. The assessments consisted of filling out a diary for one week, filling out questionnaires and undergoing a face-to-face or phone interview during which the functioning of the child and the effects of the treatment were evaluated. Questionnaires were sent and returned by mail. If any anxiety or depressive disorders were present before entering treatment, the specific part of the ADIS-C pertaining to that diagnosis was administered at the follow-up assessments to ascertain whether any change had occurred considering those psychiatric complaints.

In- and exclusion criteriaChildren were eligible if they were between 7 and 18 years of age and fulfilled the following criteria, in accordance with the Rome III criteria for pediatric abdominal pain-related

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 122 18-07-12 17:19

Page 126: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

7

Cognitive behavior therapy for FAP | 123

functional gastrointestinal disorders (Rasquin et al., 2006): 1) AP is main complaint; 2) AP lasted at least 8 weeks in past 12 months; 3) no evidence of an inflammatory, anatomic, metabolic, or neoplastic process that explains the subject’s symptoms. Additional criteria were: 4) absence of a psychiatric disorder which required treatment prior to treatment for FAP (e.g., psychotic symptoms, presence of an anxiety disorder that was clearly causing the AP); 5) Dutch speaking; 6) AP had to be present in two weeks prior to inclusion. Although this last inclusion criterion is not in accordance with the Rome III criteria, we added it to ensure that complaints still played an important role at the moment of inclusion. Presence of an explanatory organic disease was investigated by performing laboratory tests on stool, urine and blood, and by performing an ultrasound, in addition to taking a physical examination and mapping the medical history of the child. Calculation of sample sizeIn the study by Sanders et al. (1994) comparing 6 sessions of CBT to 4-6 visits to a pediatrician, the difference in success was 31.8% (Sanders et al., 1994). As this study matched the design of our study, we decided to use the results as the basis for our sample size calculation. To be able to detect a difference of 31.8% with an 80% power and a two-sided 5% significance level, a sample size of 45 participants per group is necessary. Therefore it was decided to aim to include 50 participants per group, given an anticipated drop-out rate of 10%.

InterventionsBoth treatments comprised 6 sessions scheduled approximately one week apart. We deliberately chose a control condition with an equal number of sessions to make both treatments comparable as to the amount of attention that children would get from a health care professional. Most children (N=38) received CBT from master students in psychology (N=3), who received training and supervision by the developer of the intervention, an experienced children’s psychotherapist. In the other cases (N=14), CBT was delivered by a therapist with a master’s degree (N=3). On average, each therapist treated 8.67 patients (range 1-18). MC was delivered by pediatricians or pediatric gastroenterologists (N=8; mean number of patients treated by one doctor: 6.5; range 1-28). During both treatments, children were requested to keep a standardized diary about their AP (Vlieger et al., 2007), to enable the therapist/pediatrician to discuss the progress of the child’s complaints.

Cognitive behavior therapyThe CBT-protocol (Thiadens & De Haan, 2007) was based on previous treatment trials published by Sanders and colleagues (1989, 1994) and Robins and colleagues (2005) and consisted of 6 sessions which were generally scheduled weekly. The sixth session followed the fifth approximately 2 weeks later to allow for additional practice of the skills taught. The protocol was designed in a modular fashion making use of several optional modules that the therapist could select depending on the needs and problems of the child, allowing

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 123 18-07-12 17:19

Page 127: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

124 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

tailoring of the protocol to the individual child. This approach has been found to be more effective than applying a standard protocol to every patient (Weisz et al., 2012). The module that was used for every child consisted of a series of relaxation exercises, teaching children to relax when they experienced AP and in doing so, teaching them a way to cope with their AP. During the first session, children were taught breathing exercises, while during the second session Jacobson’s progressive muscle relaxation (tensing followed by relaxing all body-muscles) was introduced (Jacobson, 1938). During the third session another relaxation exercise was taught, during which children were instructed to relax their whole body and connect that relaxed state to a cue (e.g., saying “relax” in your mind, and then relaxing the whole body at once). During the fourth and fifth session, children were taught guided imagery relaxation. The three optional modules consisted of 1) cognitive therapy directed at changing negative cognitions about the pain, or negative cognitions about other things (e.g. worrying about school) that aggravated the pain; 2) behavior therapy directed at changing maladaptive coping behavior of the child (e.g., avoiding school and social activities because of the pain); and 3) behavior therapy directed at changing maladaptive parental coping behavior (e.g., allowing the child to stay home from school). These modules were only used when children or parents exhibited problems in any of these areas. To establish which additional module(s) the therapist should use, children and their parents were interviewed by the therapist about how they were doing in these three areas during the two intake sessions preceding the treatment, and during the first two therapy sessions.

Medical careDuring the six consults, pediatricians educated children and their parents about the complaints, prescribed medication such as laxatives, loperamide, proton pump inhibitors or pain mediation based on the complaints of the patient, gave dietary advice, or gave advice about continuing school and daily activities. During each session the content of the AP diary was discussed.

Measures

Pain diaryPrior to treatment, children filled out a two-week diary, in which they reported about frequency, duration and intensity of their AP on a daily basis. Intensity of AP was scored using a Facial Affective Scale (McGrath et al., 1996; Vlieger et al., 2007) ranging from 1 (smiling face, no pain) to 9 (crying face, most intense pain possible). The second week of the AP diary was used as baseline, measuring the level of AP prior to treatment. During treatment, children were asked to keep filling out a similar AP diary. The last week of this AP diary was used as a measure of AP directly following treatment, and at the two follow-up assessments, children again filled out AP diaries for a week. Data were entered in SPSS by a

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 124 18-07-12 17:19

Page 128: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

7

Cognitive behavior therapy for FAP | 125

medical and a psychology student who were blinded for the treatment children received. Pain intensity scores (PIS) and pain frequency scores (PFS) were calculated (Vlieger et al., 2007). For the PIS, daily scores were transposed to the following range: 0=no pain, 1=faces 1 through 3, 2=faces 4 through 6, and 3=faces 7 through 9. For the PFS, daily scores were transposed as follows: 0=no pain, 1=1 to 30 minutes of pain, 2=31 to 120 minutes of pain, and 3=more than 120 minutes per day. For both PIS and PFS, the daily scores were added for all 7 days of the diary, resulting in a total score with a range of 0-21.

Semi-structured interview for psychiatric diagnosesTo assess whether any psychiatric diagnoses were present, we used the validated Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for Children (ADIS-C; Silverman & Albano, 2001). The ADIS-C is a semi-structured interview assessing DSM-IV diagnoses for all anxiety disorders and depression, and was separately administered to parents and children. The primary investigator and the master students in psychology who also delivered the CBT were trained in administering the ADIS-C and interviewed all parents and children. If any doubts arose about diagnoses resulting from the ADIS-C, these were discussed with an experienced children’s psychotherapist. When from the ADIS-C a diagnosis resulted that required treatment prior to treatment for FAP, children were excluded from the trial.

QuestionnairesThe following questionnaires were filled out by both children and parents. Children reported about their own complaints, and parents reported about the complaints of their child. Abdominal pain was assessed with the Abdominal Pain Index (API; Walker et al., 1997). The API consists of five questions, which focus on the frequency, duration and intensity of the AP the child experienced in the past two weeks. Two questions make use of a 6-point scale, one of a 9-point scale and another two of an 11-point scale. A total score for the API was computed by recoding each item to reflect a scale ranging from 0 to 10, and summing all items (Van der Veek et al., 2010). The API has been shown to be a reliable instrument, with a Cronbach’s alpha ranging from .80 to .93 (Walker et al., 1997). Gastrointestinal symptoms and other physical symptoms were assessed using the validated Dutch version of the Children’s Somatization Inventory (CSI; Garber et al., 1991; Ghys & Meesters, 1993; Meesters et al., 2003; Walker & Greene, 1989). Six items (nausea/upset stomach; constipation; loose bowel movements or diarrhea; vomiting; feeling bloated or gassy; food making you sick) were summed to measure GI symptoms; 28 items were summed to measure other physical symptoms. The items were rated for severity during the past two weeks using a 5-point scale, ranging from (0) not at all to (4) a whole lot. Functional disability was measured with the Functional Disability Inventory (FDI; Claar & Walker, 2006; Walker & Greene, 1991b). The FDI consists of 15 items which are answered on a five point scale, ranging from (0) no trouble at all to (4) impossible. The scores on the

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 125 18-07-12 17:19

Page 129: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

126 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

items are summed to compute a total score, with higher scores indicating more disability. The FDI has been validated in children with recurrent AP and has good reliability with Cronbach’s alpha’s ranging from .86 to .91 (Claar & Walker, 2006). Anxious and depressive symptoms were measured with the Revised Anxiety and Depression Scale – short version (RCADS-25; Muris et al., 2002). The shortened version of the RCADS is a valid and reliable instrument to measure symptoms of generalized anxiety disorder, separation anxiety disorder, social phobia, panic disorder, and major depressive disorder (Muris et al., 2002). The five scales consist of five items that have to be scored on a 4-point scale, ranging from (0) never to (3) always. The four scales of the different anxiety disorders were summed to compute one score for anxious symptoms. and a separate score was calculated for depressive symptoms. Quality of life was assessed using the KIDSCREEN-27 (The KIDSCREEN Group Europe, 2006). Five different aspects of quality of life were measured: physical well-being, psychological well-being, parent relations and autonomy, social support and peers, and school environment. The KIDSCREEN-27 was developed across different countries, and consists of 27 items, to be scored on a 5-point scale. Several items are reverse-scored. Scores for each scale are calculated using Rasch analysis and then transformed into T-values. Higher scores reflect a better quality of life. The KIDSCREEN-27 is a reliable and valid instrument, with good test-retest reliability (The KIDSCREEN Group Europe, 2006).

Statistical analysesThe intention-to-treat principle was used for all analyses. Prior to analyses, distributions of the outcome measures were checked for normality; if significant skewness and/or kurtosis was present, this was resolved by transforming the outcome measure at all time points. To investigate whether both treatments led to a significant decrease of pain and secondary outcomes over time, and whether there was a significant difference in effectiveness between both treatments, linear mixed-model analyses were performed in SPSS 18.0, which make use of every observation for every participant, irrespective of their missing data. Using this technique, a covariance structure can be specified which accounts for within-subject correlation over time. Analyses were performed with both an unstructured covariance matrix and an autoregressive heterogeneous one. The fits of both models were compared making use of the -2 log likelihood value. If the models reported a comparable -2 log likelihood value, the model with the autoregressive heterogeneous covariance matrix was chosen, as this provides a more restrictive model with less parameters (Twisk, 2003). In addition to these analyses, we calculated which percentage of children had a significant clinical improvement after treatment. To calculate these percentages for the questionnaire-derived data, we used the often recommended reliable change index (Jacobson & Truax, 1991). This index calculates a critical change score that patients

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 126 18-07-12 17:19

Page 130: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

7

Cognitive behavior therapy for FAP | 127

have to surpass before they are considered to be reliably improved. This critical change score is based on the baseline standard deviation and Cronbach’s alpha reliability of the instrument used to calculate success (Jacobson & Truax, 1991). For the present study, children were only considered improved if they decreased at least 9.90 points on the API (range 0-50); if in addition to this drop of 9.90 points, their level of AP after treatment was closer to the mean of a healthy population than to the mean of the clinical population, they were considered recovered (Jacobson and Truax’s criterion c; Jacobson & Truax, 1991; Lambert & Ogles, 2009). If the AP increased 9.90 points following treatment, children were considered significantly deteriorated. As no norm data for parental API scores are available, we used the child-reported API to calculate these percentages. To compare our results with the results of an earlier study performed in our institution (Vlieger et al., 2007), we additionally calculated an index of treatment success based on the pain-diaries. Children were considered recovered if both their PIS and PFS decreased more than 80%; if both PIS and PFS decreased between 30% and 80%, children were considered improved, and if children decreased less than 30% in either their PIS or PFS score, they were considered not improved (Vlieger et al., 2007).

Results

Participant flowBetween April 2007 and April 2010, 201 children were screened for participation in the study. As depicted in figure 1, 94 children were excluded from the study. In 34 of these children, the reason for exclusion was that AP disappeared after the first consult with one of the pediatricians. In 16 children, a physical cause was found for AP (constipation in 56.3%, Helicobacter pylori in 12.5%). Twelve children were excluded because a psychiatric diagnosis was present that required treatment prior to treatment for FAP. In 2 cases the decision to exclude patients for this reason was based on the semi-structured ADIS-C diagnostic interview, whereas in the other 10 cases, this decision was made by the pediatrician screening children for inclusion. A total of 104 children were included and randomized, 52 in each condition. Forty-six children completed CBT, and 48 completed MC. The last follow-up assessment was completed in August 2011. The trial was stopped because the intended number of included children was reached. Data of all 104 included children were used for analyses.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 127 18-07-12 17:19

Page 131: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

128 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Figure 1. Flow diagram of the progress of children through the phases of the trial

Excluded (N=97) - Declined to participate (N=22) - Not meeting inclusion criteria: - No more AP after one consult with pediatrician (N=34) - Physical cause for AP (N=16) - Psychiatric problem requiring treatment prior to treatment of FAP (N=12) - Other reasons (N=13)

Randomized (N=104)

Assessed for eligibility (N=201)

Allocated to CBT (N=52) - Received CBT (N=46) - Did not complete CBT (N=6): - declined treatment (N=2) - did not complete intervention

(N=4). Reasons: - 2 declined further

participation - 2 had found a

satisfactory medical treatment during CBT and saw no use in continuing CBT

Allocated to MC (N=52) - Received MC (N=48) - Did not complete MC (N=4): - declined treatment (N=2) - did not complete intervention

because of no-show (N=1) - received CBT instead of MC

on own request (N=1)

Followed up at: - Directly after treatment N=45 (86.5%) - 6 months after treatment N=43 (82.7%) - 12 months after treatment N=46 (88.5%)

Followed up at: - Directly after treatment N=47 (90.4%) - 6 months after treatment N=44 (84.6%) - 12 months after treatment N=42 (80.8%)

Analyzed: N=52

Analyzed: N=52

Enro

llmen

t A

lloca

tion

A

naly

sis

Fo

llow

-up

Demographic and clinical characteristicsAs illustrated in table 1, at baseline no significant difference was found in any of the demographic and clinical characteristics between the two groups, except for the presence of co-morbid anxiety disorders, which were more prevalent in the MC group.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 128 18-07-12 17:19

Page 132: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

7

Cognitive behavior therapy for FAP | 129

Table 1. Baseline characteristics of sample as a function of treatment condition

CBT (mean (SD) or %)

MC (mean (SD) or %)

p (t, χ2 or Fisher’s exact test)

Age 11.94 (2.61) 11.87 (2.93) .888Gender (% female) 71.2 73.1 .827Nationality (% Dutch) 90.4 78.8 .103Number of months since start of complaints 34.79 (36.77) 33.23 (38.31) .833Number of weeks of AP in past year 42.61 (14.14) 42.71 (14.97) .971Average number of schooldays missed per week .96 (1.38) .70 (1.06) .285Rome III diagnosis .180

FAP/FAPS 65.4 59.6IBS 21.2 34.6Other/combination 13.5 5.8

Functional complaints among family membersFGID in mother/father 24.5 38.0 .147FGID in other family member 18.4 18.0 .962Other functional complaint in family 12.2 16.0 .592

Psychiatric comorbidityAnxiety disorder 19.2 38.5 .030Depressive disorder or dysthymia 5.7 1.9 .618

Note. CBT = Cognitive Behavior Therapy. MC=Medical Care. IBS=Irritable Bowel Syndrome. FGID = Functional Gastrointestinal Disorder.

Differences between treatment effectivenessThe results of the linear mixed models investigating differences between treatment effectiveness, correcting for the effects of age and gender, are displayed in Table 2. Compared to the scores prior to treatment, children improved on all measures at all time points, except for some of the quality of life subscales (Child-reported scales not shown in table 2: parent relations and autonomy: p =.071; school environment: p =.809. Parent-reported scales not shown in table 2: parent relations and autonomy: p =.658; school environment: p =.190). No significant differences were found between the effectiveness of both treatments on any of the time-points concerning child- or parent-reported AP, functional disability, GI complaints, other somatic complaints, and four out of five quality of life domains. Standardized mean differences (effect size d; conventions for interpretation: d = .20 - small; d = .50 - medium; d = .80 - large; Cohen, 1977) for change in AP according to child- and parent-reported API, PIS and PFS varied from .67 to 1.58 for CBT across the different measures and end-points, and from .57 to 1.68 for MC, reflecting medium to large effects on decreasing AP for both interventions. For child-reported anxiety and depression directly after treatment (T2) and at six months follow-up (T3), a significant interaction effect between time and treatment indicated that children’s levels of anxiety and depression decreased more in the group of children receiving CBT, than in the group receiving MC. At 12 months follow-up (T4), the anxiety and depression scores of children receiving MC had dropped to similar levels as those of children who received CBT. For quality of life related to social support and peers, children receiving CBT had improved more at T3 than children receiving MC. This significant effect decreased to a trend at T4.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 129 18-07-12 17:19

Page 133: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

130 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Tab

le 2

. Mea

n sc

ores

of o

utco

me

mea

sure

s at

all

time

poin

ts, t

este

d w

ith li

near

mix

ed m

odel

s.

Prio

r to

txD

irect

ly a

fter

tx6

mon

ths

post

tx12

mon

ths

post

tx

CBT

MC

CBT

MC

p T

p T*

txC

BTM

Cp

Tp

T*tx

CBT

MC

p T

p T*

txC

hild

repo

rted

var

iabl

esA

bdom

inal

pai

n 32

.18

33.5

623

.10

26.5

1<

.001

*.5

1418

.67

24.6

6<

.001

*.1

7519

.03

17.7

2<

.001

*.3

89Pa

in in

tens

ity (d

iary

)15

.43

14.7

66.

828.

80<

.001

*.0

79†

5.61

7.45

<.0

01*

.121

5.73

5.49

<.0

01*

.812

Pain

freq

uenc

y (d

iary

)14

.55

15.5

66.

848.

67<

.001

*.9

595.

328.

58<

.001

*.2

486.

116.

89<

.001

*.8

02Fu

nctio

nal d

isab

ility

10

.06

12.2

97.

177.

79.0

08*

.950

4.70

9.17

<.0

01*

.142

5.80

4.87

<.0

01*

.549

GI c

ompl

aint

s 6.

555.

943.

654.

20<

.001

*.3

433.

443.

80<

.001

*.1

472.

802.

47<

.001

*.3

99O

ther

phy

sica

l com

plai

nts

11.9

913

.74

9.08

10.7

4.0

05*

.935

7.53

10.2

4.0

03*

.652

5.88

7.75

<.0

01*

.779

Anx

iety

10

.61

8.54

6.83

7.76

<.0

01*

.046

*5.

387.

72<

.001

*.0

30*

5.47

5.82

<.0

01*

.417

Dep

ress

ion

3.92

3.72

2.17

2.33

<.0

01*

.027

*1.

883.

06<

.001

*.0

21*

1.85

1.79

<.0

01*

.808

Phys

ical

QoL

45.4

145

.30

48.2

647

.61

.096

†.8

0649

.74

46.9

5.0

34*

.302

50.0

850

.06

.015

*.8

06Ps

ycho

logi

cal Q

oL50

.60

50.0

853

.77

52.1

6.0

46*

.497

54.1

750

.06

.071

†.1

3754

.97

52.7

2.0

19*

.480

QoL

sup

port

and

pee

rs50

.26

51.9

852

.91

51.8

5.0

90†

.191

54.9

451

.53

.002

*.0

10*

54.9

452

.79

.001

*.0

56†

Pare

nt re

port

ed v

aria

bles

Abd

omin

al p

ain

(API

)34

.46

32.3

821

.78

24.0

3<

.001

*.2

0218

.68

20.8

2<

.001

*.2

2917

.99

17.3

9<

.001

*.7

38Fu

nctio

nal d

isab

ility

15

.86

15.1

38.

028.

45<

.001

*.0

92†

5.12

7.29

<.0

01*

.111

6.41

4.43

<.0

01*

.813

GI c

ompl

aint

s 6.

716.

064.

383.

90<

.001

*.8

713.

473.

45<

.001

*.3

352.

752.

34<

.001

*.7

41O

ther

phy

sica

l com

plai

nts

10.4

69.

238.

177.

79.0

10*

.330

5.74

7.72

.001

*.1

575.

545.

14<

.001

*.4

01A

nxie

ty

9.62

9.05

6.42

6.87

.001

*.6

375.

327.

33<

.001

*.2

045.

835.

96<

.001

*.8

67D

epre

ssio

n3.

303.

411.

821.

96<

.001

*.3

541.

561.

82<

.001

*.8

981.

671.

57<

.001

*.7

83Ph

ysic

al Q

oL40

.66

41.2

248

.71

47.8

0<

.001

*.7

6046

.43

46.0

1.0

15*

.895

47.3

847

.41

.002

*.9

08Ps

ycho

logi

cal Q

oL48

.67

49.5

354

.54

54.2

1.0

04*

.673

53.1

849

.94

.059

†.2

7853

.93

53.6

8.0

10*

.660

QoL

sup

port

and

pee

rs54

.27

53.3

855

.96

54.1

0.4

81.8

5454

.08

53.1

8.9

21.9

4153

.96

56.4

2.8

98.2

54

Not

e. G

roup

s di

d no

t diff

er o

n an

y of

the

outc

ome

mea

sure

s pr

ior t

o tr

eatm

ent.

For t

he q

ualit

y of

life

sca

les,

high

er s

core

s re

flect

bet

ter w

ell-b

eing

. For

all

othe

r mea

sure

s, lo

wer

sc

ores

refle

ct b

ette

r wel

l-bei

ng. A

naly

ses

wer

e co

rrec

ted

for t

he e

ffect

s of

age

and

gen

der.

Test

s of

sig

nific

ance

of t

ime

and

time

x tr

eatm

ent i

nter

actio

n us

e th

e sc

ore

prio

r to

trea

tmen

t as

refe

renc

e po

int.

CBT

=Co

gniti

ve b

ehav

ior t

hera

py; M

C=

Med

ical

car

e; T

x=tr

eatm

ent;

T=tim

e; Q

oL=

Qua

lity

of li

fe. *

p <

.05;

†p

<.1

0

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 130 18-07-12 17:19

Page 134: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

7

Cognitive behavior therapy for FAP | 131

Treatment successTable 3 shows the treatment success of both treatments at the three different end points. According to the Jacobson and Truax (JT) method applied to the questionnaire data, 31.8% of children were recovered or improved at T2, whereas 29.8% of the children receiving MC recovered. These percentages increased to 51.2% and 60.0% for CBT at T3 and T4, and to 41.5% and 56.4% for MC. None of these percentages differed significantly between the two treatments (p of all Fisher’s exact tests >.05). The success percentages according to the diary data were much higher, with percentages of improvement or recovery of 66.6%, 65.0% and 65.8% for CBT at the three end points, and 47.5%, 47.4% and 62.8% for MC. These percentages also did not significantly differ (p of all χ2-tests > .05). Additionally, it was possible using the JT method and the questionnaire data to calculate which children significantly deteriorated following treatment. In the group receiving CBT, 2 children deteriorated at T2, none at T3, and 4 at T4, whereas 2 children receiving MC deteriorated at T2 and T3. Most of the children who deteriorated had a low child-reported API score at baseline: child-reported baseline API scores were significantly lower than parent-reported baseline API scores for the children who deteriorated (mean children = 19.88, mean parents = 30.00, t(6) = -2.530; p =.045).

The success percentages were calculated on the basis of valid data only. As for the diary data the number of missing observations was somewhat higher (range 21-28) than for the questionnaire data (range 13-20), we additionally investigated whether children with a missing success score differed at baseline from the other children. Children with a missing success score on the questionnaire data did not differ from the other children concerning their level of AP prior to treatment, the time since the start of their complaints, the number of weeks of AP in the past 12 months, gender, or age (p of all t-tests > .05). For the diary data, a trend was found for children that did not respond at T3 to report higher levels of AP prior to treatment (mean of children responding: 31.80, mean of children not responding: 35.91, t(101) = -1.747, p =.084). Additionally, the children not responding at T4 scored significantly higher on the number of weeks of AP in the year preceding treatment (mean of children responding: 41.33; mean of children not responding: 47.55, t(50.76) = -2.281; p =.027).

Table 3. Percentage of patients recovered and improved directly after treatment and at six and twelve months follow-up

Directly after treatment 6 months follow-up 12 months follow-up

CBT MC CBT MC CBT MCReliable change API a

Recovered 29.5 25.5 44.2 36.6 51.1 53.8Improved 2.3 4.3 7.0 4.9 8.9 2.6Diary (combination of PIS and PFS score) a

Recovered 41.0 25.0 42.5 28.9 58.5 41.9Improved 25.6 22.5 22.5 18.4 7.3 20.9

Note. a N ranged from 78 to 84; b N ranged from 84 to 91. API=abdominal pain index. PIS=pain intensity score. PFS=pain frequency score. p >.05 for all χ2-comparisons between treatment groups at all time points.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 131 18-07-12 17:20

Page 135: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

132 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Adverse eventsNo serious adverse events were reported and for only one child receiving CBT, the first breathing exercise led to hyperventilation, which was successfully reduced in the remainder of the CBT sessions.

Figure 2. Mean and standard deviation of child reported abdominal pain throughout the study.

T

T4T3T2Baseline

Ab

dom

inal

pai

n in

dex

50,00

40,00

30,00

20,00

10,00

0,00

Error Bars: +/- 1. SD

MCCBT

T

T3T2T1Baseline

Pain

freq

uenc

y sc

ore

25,00

20,00

15,00

10,00

5,00

0,00

Error Bars: +/- 1. SD

MCCBT

T

T3T2T1Baseline

Pain

inte

nsity

sco

re

20,00

15,00

10,00

5,00

0,00

Error Bars: +/- 1. SD

MCCBT

Satisfaction with treatment and therapist/doctorChildren receiving CBT rated the therapy with a 7.61 on a scale from 0-10, and rated the therapist with an 8.45. Parents of children receiving CBT rated the therapy with a 7.52, and the therapist with an 8.28. For MC, these ratings were 7.74 and 8.57 respectively as rated by children, and 7.57 and 8.54 as rated by parents. The ratings did not differ between treatments or between therapists/doctors (p’s of all t-tests >.05).

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 132 18-07-12 17:20

Page 136: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

7

Cognitive behavior therapy for FAP | 133

Health care use after completion of treatmentDuring the 6 months between the last treatment session and the first follow-up, respectively 69.4% and 64% of children receiving CBT or MC reported no health care use for FAP, whereas 24.5% and 30% had visited one other health care professional once or more often for their complaints, and 6.1% and 6% respectively, had visited two other health care professionals. Between 6 and 12 months follow-up, 60.4% of children receiving CBT and 75.5% of children receiving MC reported no health care use for FAP, whereas 39.6% vs. 24.5% visited 1 to 4 other health care providers. None of these percentages significantly differed between treatment groups (post-treatment to 6 months follow-up: χ2(2) = .384, p =.825; 6 to 12 months follow-up: χ2(4) = .5.354, p =.253).

Five children switched to CBT after they finished MC at some point during the follow-up period; 9 children that received CBT had one or more MC visits with the pediatrician that included them in the trial after they finished CBT. Excluding the data of these children after they made the switch did not have any influence on the results of the linear mixed models on pain outcomes, nor did it make a difference in the analyses concerning treatment success.

Effect of therapist/doctor and modules used for CBTTo investigate whether treatment effect was related to the specific therapist or doctor that delivered the treatment, linear mixed models were used with child and parent reported API scores and child reported PIS and PFS scores as dependent variables, correcting for the effects of age and gender. No significant interaction effects were found for CBT, indicating that all therapists were equally effective in decreasing AP (all p’s of time x therapist interaction >.05). For MC, the same held true (p’s of time x doctor interaction >.05 for API and PFS; for PIS, p of time x doctor interaction was .033 for main effect, but none of the subsequent univariate comparisons were significant, all p’s >.05). Similarly, we investigated whether applying a specific optional module within the CBT treatment had an influence on treatment effectiveness using linear mixed models. The cognitive module was used for 58.8% of the children receiving CBT, the module focusing on the behavior of the child in 60.8%, and the module focusing on behavior of the parent in 47.1% of the cases. In 11.8% of the cases, children received only the relaxation module, 33.3% of the children received one additional module, 31.4% received two additional modules, and 23.5% received all available modules. How well children responded to treatment showed no relationships with whether they received one of the optional modules (p’s of all time x module interaction effects > .05) or what specific combination of modules they received (p’s of all Bonferroni corrected post hoc t-tests >.05).

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 133 18-07-12 17:20

Page 137: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

134 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Discussion

The present RCT shows that 6 weekly sessions of CBT resulted in a significant reduction of abdominal pain in over 60% of children with FAP, up to one year after treatment. However, 6 weekly visits to a pediatrician or pediatric gastroenterologist was equally effective in reducing AP, with 56% of children being improved or recovered one year after treatment. CBT and MC were also equally effective in decreasing other GI complaints, functional disability, and physical and psychological quality of life. CBT, however, was more effective than MC directly after treatment and at six months follow-up in decreasing symptoms of anxiety and depression. This advantage of CBT over MC disappeared at 12 months follow-up, when children that received either treatment reported equally decreased levels of anxiety and depression. Both interventions did not differ with respect to health care consumption after treatment, or satisfaction with treatment.

The success rates for CBT found in our study are in accordance with earlier studies on CBT for children with FAP (Levy et al., 2010; Robins et al., 2005). In the study by Levy et al. (2010), parents reported a similar decrease in disability, whereas the trial by Robins et al. (2005) showed an equivalent child-reported reduction of disability directly after treatment and at 6-12 months follow-up. However, in contrast to the former trials on CBT, no significant difference was found in effectiveness between CBT and MC as performed by an experienced pediatrician or pediatric gastroenterologist. The most likely explanation for this result lies in the active nature of the present control condition compared to the control conditions as used in these other studies, both in frequency and in content (Levy et al., 2010; Robins et al., 2005). The MC investigated in this trial was previously performed in another RCT at our institution, resulting in similar success rates (Vlieger et al., 2007). Note, however, that the intensive protocol used in this study is not representative of daily practice of a pediatric gastroenterologist or pediatrician, where patients are seen less frequently. Considering the intensified form in which the MC was given, and the fact that MC was delivered by experienced pediatricians and pediatric gastroenterologists, while CBT was successfully delivered by specially trained master students in psychology, CBT has the potential advantage of being less costly and more easily implemented. Future studies are encouraged to focus on the cost effectiveness of CBT.

As the present CBT protocol shared many ingredients with a hypnotherapy protocol that was previously investigated for its effectiveness at our institution (Vlieger et al., 2007), we also compared our results to this protocol. Surprisingly, based on the abdominal pain diaries, higher improvement or remission rates at all time points were reported after hypnotherapy: 85%, 93% and 96% compared to 66.6%, 65.0% and 65.8% after CBT. However, it should be noted that the success percentages for hypnotherapy were extremely high for a treatment trial. Considering the fact that the hypnotherapy-trial was small (N=53) and made use of only one therapist, a larger RCT including more therapists

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 134 18-07-12 17:20

Page 138: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

7

Cognitive behavior therapy for FAP | 135

performing a standardized hypnotherapy protocol is needed to replicate these findings. It should be noted that an important reason why both CBT and MC resulted in similar outcomes, is their sharing of non-specific treatment ingredients. Most importantly, children in both treatments were taken seriously by their doctor/therapist, were treated kindly, and received attention for their complaints. The physicians involved in this study were experienced in pediatric functional gastro-intestinal problems. They were well aware of both the importance of allocation of time to the initial consultation to validate the patients’ symptoms, the importance of reassurance and education, and the importance of responding to the patients’ specific needs and fears. Indeed, children and parents in both treatment conditions reported to be highly satisfied with the treatment they received, and with how the doctor/therapist had interacted with and treated them. Although one may expect that children and their parents would also be treated kindly by the health care professionals they previously encountered, many of them mentioned that previous doctors had suggested the pain was ‘all in their heads’ and that they felt they had not been taken seriously. The substantial effects of supportive interaction with a practitioner were confirmed by a study in adult IBS patients, showing that the effect of a placebo-treatment was much stronger if it was accompanied by supportive interaction with a practitioner (Kaptchuk et al., 2008). The benefits of a supportive relationship with a practitioner may also provide an explanation for our finding that 34 children recovered before they even received treatment, after just one consult with one of the pediatricians. These results suggest that much can be gained by instructing general practitioners and pediatricians how to effectively interact with and treat patients with functional complaints. For a substantial amount of patients, this may make further health care consumption unnecessary. This leaves us with the question what percentage of the effect of CBT is attributable to non-specific treatment effects, and what percentage to specific techniques used in CBT. In other words: what are the processes through which CBT reaches its effect? This question has been scarcely investigated in the literature on CBT in general, let alone in RCTs investigating the efficacy of CBT for pediatric FAP. Moreover, it is also mostly unknown for which type of children CBT is most effective. This is an important question, as with 60% of children successfully treated, this means that 40% was not, showing a considerable variance in how well children respond to treatment. This suggests that CBT might be most effective for specific subgroups of patients. Investigating through which processes CBT accomplishes its effects and for which patients CBT is most effective, may enable us to improve and tailor the intervention (Kraemer, Wilson, Fairburn, & Agras, 2002; Nock, 2007; Weersing & Weisz, 2002). Future researchers are therefore encouraged to further investigate these issues. It should also be noted that this is the first RCT reporting data on health care use following CBT for pediatric FAP. Most children did not visit any other health care professional up to one year following either treatment. Considering the fact that at study entry, the

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 135 18-07-12 17:20

Page 139: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

136 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

children had experienced an average three years of pain and usually had visited several doctors and other health care professionals, this is a promising result.

There are a number of limitations of this study that need to be addressed. First, because of the modular nature of the CBT protocol, not every child received the same treatment. Although treatment effectiveness showed no relations with which module or which combination of modules was applied, future studies are encouraged to investigate the effect of applying additional modules in a larger sample. Second, there was a notable difference in clinical experience between the doctors delivering MC on the one hand, and the therapists delivering CBT on the other. However, it has been found that inexperienced therapists when trained in an intervention and receiving supervision, are equally effective as experienced therapists (Öst, Karlstedt, & Widén, 2012). Finally, it should be noted that the diary-derived data showed larger success percentages than the questionnaire data, which was most likely due to missing data in the diaries. Children that did not fill out a diary at follow-up reported more severe complaints at baseline, suggesting that the success percentages of the diary data may overestimate the actual benefit of both treatments. Therefore, future studies should be careful not to incorporate just one of both outcome measures.

We conclude from the present study that CBT appears effective in the treatment of children with FAP, but is equally effective as an intensified form of medical care delivered by pediatricians or pediatric gastroenterologists experienced in treating children with functional gastrointestinal disorders. Future studies should focus on which treatment is most effective for which subgroup of patients and on the working mechanisms for CBT, in order to improve and tailor the treatment of children with FAP.

AcknowledgementsWe would like to thank Elisabeth Thiadens, Lydia Briët, Annika de Bourgraaf, Rachel Plak and Martine Schoonenberg for delivering the CBT protocol, and Bart Koot, Merit Tabbers, Andrieke Knottnerus, Felix Kreier, Femke van Herrewegen and Chris de Kruiff for delivering part of the MC.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 136 18-07-12 17:20

Page 140: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 137 18-07-12 17:20

Page 141: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 138 18-07-12 17:20

Page 142: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

Ch

apte

r

8Exploring moderating and

mediating factors of cognitive behavior therapy for the

treatment of pediatric functional abdominal pain

S. M.C. van der Veek, H. H. F. Derkx, M. A. Benninga, F. Boer, & E. de Haan, submitted for publication

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 139 18-07-12 17:20

Page 143: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

140 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Abstract

Objectives: Cognitive behavior therapy (CBT) is a promising treatment for pediatric functional abdominal pain (FAP). However, a considerable percentage of children does not respond to CBT. To be able to improve and tailor CBT for pediatric FAP, we explored which factors moderate and mediate treatment effectiveness of CBT. Method: A total of 104 children aged 7-18 were included and randomized between 6 sessions of CBT and 6 sessions of MC. Measures for outcome, moderation and mediation were derived from child and parent self report (diaries and questionnaires). Investigated moderators were age, gender, Rome III diagnosis, time since start of complaints, anxiety, depression, emotion awareness, coping with everyday problems, parental functional gastrointestinal disorder, and parental stress, anxiety, depression and somatization. Investigated mediators were parental and children’s negative cognitions about AP and positive thoughts about coping potential, children’s coping with AP, parental solicitous responses to AP, and vigilance for pain. Assessments were performed pre- and post-treatment, and at 6 and 12 months follow-up. Results: Children that were more likely to show their emotions and to seek social support for everyday problems prior to treatment, benefitted significantly more from CBT (about 80% effectiveness) than children scoring low on these variables (44%). Decreases in children’s negative thoughts about their condition were related to better treatment outcome for both CBT and MC. Increases in children’s positive thoughts about coping potential and decreases in parental solicitous behavior were related to better treatment outcome in CBT only. Discussion: The present study shows that CBT is more effective for children who are more likely to show their emotions and to seek social support prior to treatment. Changes in children’s positive and negative cognitions and changes in parental solicitous behavior may be mechanisms through which CBT exerts its influence.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 140 18-07-12 17:20

Page 144: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

8

Moderators and mediators of CBT for FAP | 141

Introduction

Pediatric functional abdominal pain is a common and disabling condition, affecting about 10% of children in Western countries (Chitkara et al., 2005; Youssef et al., 2006). Cognitive behavior therapy (CBT) has been found to be a promising treatment for children with FAP (Huertas-Ceballos et al., 2009c; Sprenger et al., 2011). However, a considerable percentage of children and adults with FAP do not respond to CBT (20-40%; Lackner et al., 2004; Van der Veek, Derkx, Benninga, Boer, & De Haan, submitted b). Moreover, a recent randomized controlled trial by our research group showed that, although CBT reduced abdominal pain (AP) significantly in over 60% of children, CBT was not more effective than an intensified form of medical care in decreasing levels of AP, co-morbid physical complaints and quality of life (Van der Veek et al., submitted b). As two different treatments resulted in similar outcomes, this raises the question through what processes or mechanisms CBT might work. Additionally, we found considerable variance in how children responded to treatment, suggesting that CBT might be most effective for certain subgroups of patients. Therefore, the aim of the present study was to explore for which children CBT works best (i.e., to find moderators of treatment effectiveness), and through what processes or mechanisms CBT works (i.e., which factors mediate treatment effectiveness) (Kraemer et al., 2002). Learning more about mediators and moderators of CBT may enable us to improve and tailor the intervention (Kraemer et al., 2002; Nock, 2007; Weersing & Weisz, 2002). Which factors moderate the effectiveness of CBT has not yet been investigated for pediatric FAP, and only to a limited extent for adult functional gastrointestinal disorders (FGID). Studies in adults have mostly focused on the moderating influence of demographic and clinical characteristics like gender, symptom severity or distress. These variables seem to exert some influence on outcome, but the size of this influence is only modest, which led researchers to conclude that other factors are likely to be responsible for treatment effectiveness (Blanchard et al., 2006; Lackner et al., 2010). Therefore, the present study focused, in addition to baseline clinical and demographic characteristics, on the moderating influence of the psychological variables emotion awareness and coping with everyday problems. Previous studies suggest that a psychological intervention is less effective if patients are more alexithymic/less aware of their emotions, or are less inclined to talk about or show their emotions to others (Lumley, Neely, & Burger, 2007). Indeed, alexithymia has been shown to be related to a decreased effectiveness of medical and psychological treatment in adult patients with FGIDs (Porcelli et al., 2003). The way children cope with everyday problems is another possible psychological moderator of effectiveness of CBT in pediatric FAP. Specifically, it has been suggested that CBT is more effective for those children using maladaptive strategies to cope with general problems, as CBT may teach them to use more effective coping strategies in other areas of life as well, resulting in a greater treatment response (Oxman, Hull, & Dietrich, 2008). Finally, in addition

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 141 18-07-12 17:20

Page 145: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

142 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

to these child-related variables, the moderating influence of parental stress, parental anxious, depressive and somatization symptoms and presence of an FGID in one of the parents was investigated. Previous studies suggest that, although the influence seems modest, parents may maintain the AP of their children if they experience a lot of somatic or psychological complaints themselves (Levy, 2011; Van der Veek et al., 2012). Therefore, we expected that if parents reported more physical or psychological complaints, this would be related to lower treatment effectiveness. Concerning mediating factors of treatment effects, the study by Sanders and colleagues (1994) was the first study explicitly focusing on such factors for a CBT protocol designed for pediatric FAP. They found that children’s and parent’s ways of coping with AP after treatment, were related to post-treatment levels of AP. However, to establish that a factor mediates the effect of treatment on outcome, that factor should not only be related to treatment outcome, but treatment should also lead to a change in that factor (the mediator) (Baron & Kenny, 1986; MacKinnon et al., 2002), which was not studied by Sanders et al.. Levy and colleagues (Levy et al., 2010) and Gross and Warschburger (Gross & Warschburger, 2012) did investigate whether CBT led to a change in variables targeted by the CBT (i.e., cognitions, parental solicitous behavior, coping with the AP), but did not relate these changes to changes in outcome, again making it impossible to conclude whether the investigated factors were actually mediators of treatment effects. Therefore, we decided to investigate which factors mediate the effectiveness of CBT in treating pediatric FAP by studying a) whether treatment resulted in changes in possible mediating variables directly after treatment and at 6 months follow-up, and whether such changes were larger for the children receiving CBT than for children in the control condition receiving an intensified form of medical care (MC); and b) whether these changes in mediators related to changes in outcome. The mediating variables that were selected for this study focused on the variables that CBT intends to change: a) negative cognitions about AP of children and parents; b) parental and children’s positive cognitions about the coping potential of the child; c) children’s coping with the AP; d) parental solicitous responses to the AP; and e) children’s vigilance towards their AP. Note that to properly establish whether a mediator is an actual mechanism of change of the therapy, it should be shown that a change in mediator precedes a change in outcome in time, necessitating measures of the mediators during treatment (Kraemer et al., 2002; Maric, Wiers, & Prins, 2012; Weersing & Weisz, 2002). Because our CBT only lasted six sessions and we focused on many different possible mediators, we decided against incorporating an assessment during treatment and to first explore which mediators might be candidate mechanisms of change. It was expected that the mediators would change more strongly following CBT than following MC, and that a change in each mediator would be related to a change in treatment outcome.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 142 18-07-12 17:20

Page 146: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

8

Moderators and mediators of CBT for FAP | 143

Methods

Design and procedureThe study was part of a prospective randomized controlled trial. The study protocol was approved by the medical ethical committee of the Academic Medical Center Amsterdam. The trial was registered at the Netherlands National Trial Register, trial number NTR1613. The study protocol is described in more detail elsewhere (Van der Veek et al., submitted b) and can be requested from the first author. Children were included at the general pediatric and pediatric gastroenterology outpatient clinics at the Emma Children’s Hospital AMC in the Netherlands. They were randomized between 6 sessions of CBT and 6 sessions of medical care (MC). CBT took place at De Bascule, academic center for child and adolescent psychiatry, Amsterdam, and MC was provided at the Emma Children’s Hospital AMC. Prior to treatment, two to three weeks after treatment, 6 months and 12 months after treatment, assessments took place. The assessments consisted of filling out a diary for one week, filling out questionnaires and undergoing a face-to-face or phone interview during which the functioning of the child and the effects of the treatment were evaluated.

In- and exclusion criteriaChildren were eligible if they were between 7 and 18 years of age and fulfilled the following criteria, in accordance with the Rome III criteria for pediatric abdominal pain-related functional gastrointestinal disorders (Rasquin et al., 2006): 1) AP is main complaint; 2) AP lasted at least 8 weeks in past 12 months; 3) no evidence of an inflammatory, anatomic, metabolic, or neoplastic process that explains the subject’s symptoms. Additional criteria were: 4) absence of a psychiatric disorder which required treatment prior to treatment for FAP (e.g., psychotic symptoms, presence of an anxiety disorder that was clearly causing the AP); 5) Dutch speaking; 6) AP had to be present in two weeks prior to inclusion. Presence of an explanatory organic disease was investigated by performing laboratory tests on stool, urine and blood, and by performing an ultrasound, in addition to taking a physical examination and mapping the medical history of the child.

Sample Over two hundred children were screened for eligibility, 97 of which were excluded for various reasons (for more detail on participant flow throughout the study, see Van der Veek et al., submitted b). A total of 104 children were randomized to either CBT (N=52) or MC (N=52). Children were predominantly female (72.1%) and of Dutch nationality (84.6%). Mean age was 11.9 years (range 7-18, sd=2.76). On average, children had experienced AP for nearly three years when they entered treatment (mean=34.01 months, sd=37.37) and most of them had experienced AP for the largest part of the preceding year (mean=42.66

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 143 18-07-12 17:20

Page 147: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

144 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

weeks, sd=14.49). Because of the AP, the children missed approximately one day of school per week at inclusion (mean= .83 days, sd=1.23). Ninety-two children (88.5%) participated at follow-up directly after treatment, 87 (83.7%) at 6 months follow-up and 88 (84.6%) at 12 months follow-up. InterventionsBoth treatments comprised of 6 sessions, scheduled approximately one week apart. Most children (N=33) received CBT from master students in psychology, who received training and supervision by the developer of the intervention, an experienced children’s psychotherapist (fifth author). In the other cases (N=13), CBT was delivered by a therapist with a master’s degree or higher. MC was delivered by experienced pediatricians or pediatric gastroenterologists. Detailed descriptions of the content of both treatments are provided elsewhere (Van der Veek et al., submitted b). In essence, the CBT-protocol (Thiadens & De Haan, 2007) made use of 4 distinct modules, three of which were only used when the therapist deemed them appropriate for the child, allowing some tailoring of the protocol to the individual child. The module that was used for every child, consisted of a series of relaxation exercises, teaching children to relax when they experienced AP and in doing so, teaching them a way to cope with their AP. The three optional modules consisted of 1) cognitive therapy directed at changing negative cognitions about the pain, or negative cognitions about other things (e.g. worrying about school) that aggravated the pain; 2) behavior therapy directed at changing maladaptive coping behavior of the child (e.g., avoiding school and social activities because of the pain); and 3) behavior therapy directed at changing maladaptive parental coping behavior (e.g., allowing the child to stay home from school). To establish which modules the therapist should use, children and their parents were interviewed by the therapist about how they were doing in these three areas during the two intake sessions preceding the treatment, and during the first two therapy sessions. Applying additional modules or not was found to be unrelated to treatment effectiveness, suggesting that although the treatment is tailored to the individual child, it can be regarded as one protocol and analyzed as such (Van der Veek et al., submitted b). The pediatricians providing the MC educated children and their parents about the complaints, prescribed medication, gave dietary advice, or gave advice about continuing school and daily activities, as they saw fit.

Measures

Pain diaryAt all four assessments, children filled out a diary, in which they had to report about the frequency, duration and intensity of their AP on a daily basis for one week. The intensity of

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 144 18-07-12 17:20

Page 148: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

8

Moderators and mediators of CBT for FAP | 145

their AP was scored making use of a Facial Affective Scale (McGrath et al., 1996; Vlieger et al., 2007), with intensity scores ranging from 1 (smiling face, no pain) to 9 (crying face, most intense pain possible). The diary data were entered in SPSS by a medical and a psychology student who were blinded for randomization. From these diaries, we calculated two scales indicating pain intensity (pain intensity scores; PIS) and pain frequency (pain frequency scores; PFS) (Vlieger et al., 2007). For the PIS, daily scores were transposed to the following range: 0=no pain, 1=faces 1 through 3, 2=faces 4 through 6, and 3=faces 7 through 9. For the PFS, daily scores were transposed as follows: 0=no pain, 1=1 to 30 minutes of pain, 2=31 to 120 minutes of pain, and 3=more than 120 minutes per day. For both PIS and PFS, the daily scores were added for all 7 days of the diary, resulting in a total score with a possible range of 0-21.

Semi-structured interview for psychiatric diagnosesTo assess whether any psychiatric diagnoses were present, the Anxiety Disorders Interview Schedule for Children was used (ADIS-C; Silverman & Albano, 2001). The ADIS-C is a semi-structured interview assessing DSM-IV diagnoses for all anxiety disorders and depression. Additionally, it screens whether any other psychiatric diagnoses like developmental disorders or AD(H)D may be present. The ADIS-C takes between 30 to 90 minutes to administer. It was separately administered to parents and children. The primary investigator and the masters students in psychology who also delivered the CBT were trained in administering the ADIS-C and interviewed all parents and children. If any doubts rose about diagnoses resulting from the ADIS-C, these were discussed with an experienced children’s psychotherapist (fifth author). The ADIS-C has been found to be a valid and reliable instrument to diagnose DSM-IV psychiatric disorders in children and adolescents (Silverman, Saavedra, & Pina, 2001).

QuestionnairesThe following questionnaires were filled out by both children and parents. Children reported about their own complaints, and parents reported about the complaints of their child. Moderating variables were measured prior to treatment, and mediating variables were measured prior to treatment, directly after treatment and at 6 months follow-up.

Abdominal pain. The Abdominal Pain Index (API) was used to measure level of AP (Walker et al., 1997). The API consists of five questions, and taps on the frequency, duration and intensity of the AP the child experienced in the past two weeks. Two questions make use of a 6-point scale, one of a 9-point scale and another two of an 11-point scale. A total score for the API was computed by recoding each item to reflect a scale ranging from 0 to 10, and summing all items (Van der Veek et al., 2010). The API has been shown to be a reliable instrument, with a Cronbach’s alpha ranging from .80 to .93 (Walker et al., 1997).

Anxious and depressive symptoms. The Revised Anxiety and Depression Scale – short version (RCADS-25; Muris et al., 2002) was used to measure symptoms of anxiety and depression. The shortened version of the RCADS has been shown to be a valid and reliable

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 145 18-07-12 17:20

Page 149: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

146 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

instrument to measure symptoms of generalized anxiety disorder, separation anxiety disorder, social phobia, panic disorder, and major depressive disorder (Muris et al., 2002). Each of the five scales consists of five items that have to be scored on a 4-point scale, ranging from (0) never to (3) always. Cronbach’s alphas of the five scales ranged from .65 to .83 in previous research (Muris et al., 2002). The four scales of the different anxiety disorders were summed to compute one score for anxious symptoms. A separate score was calculated for depressive symptoms.

Emotion awareness. Emotion awareness was measured by the self-report Emotion Awareness Questionnaire – revised version (EAQ; Rieffe et al., 2008), which has specifically been designed for children and adolescents and was partly based on the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20; Bagby et al., 1994). The EAQ measures six aspects of emotion awareness: (1) Differentiating Emotions, (2) Verbal Sharing of Emotions, (3) Not Hiding Emotions, (4) Bodily Awareness of Emotions, (5) Attending to Others’ Emotions, and (6) Analyses of Emotions. The respondents are asked to rate the degree to which each item is true about them on a three-point scale (1 = never, 2 = sometimes, 3 = often). A higher score on each subscale reflects a higher ability at the measured concept. All scales have been shown to have acceptable psychometric properties, with Cronbach’s alphas ranging from .64 to .77 (Rieffe et al., 2008).

Coping with everyday problems. Coping strategies were measured by a Dutch translation of the Children’s Coping Strategies Checklist - Revision 1 (CCSC-R1; Ayers & Sandler, 2000; De Boo & Wicherts, 2009). The CCSC-R1 consists of 54 items, all starting with the words “If I have a problem…”. The 54 items can be divided in five higher-order scales, which measure problem focused coping, positive cognitive reframing, distraction strategies, avoidance strategies, and support seeking strategies. Answers are given on a four-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (never) to 4 (always). The CCSC-R1 has been found to have sound psychometric properties, with Cronbach’s alphas ranging from .72 to .88 (De Boo & Wicherts, 2009).

Parental FGID, anxiety, depression, somatization, and stress. Whether parents suffered from any FGIDs was enquired after by the pediatrician during a consult prior to inclusion. Depressive, anxious and somatization symptoms of the parents were measured by three subscales of the Symptom Check List (SCL-90; Derogatis, 1977, Dutch translation and adaptation by Arrindell & Ettema, 2003). The depression subscale consists of 16 items, the anxiety subscale of 10 and the somatization subscale of 12 items. The items are measured using a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from (1) not at all to (5) very much. Individual scale scores were obtained by summing the items belonging to the subscale; higher scores reflect more depressive, anxious and somatization symptoms. Test-retest reliability has been found to be good for this instrument and strong convergent validity was found with other conceptually related scales (Arrindell & Ettema, 2003). Finally, to measure parental stress, the NOSI-K (De Brock, Vermulst, Gerris, & Abadin, 1992) a Dutch version of the

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 146 18-07-12 17:20

Page 150: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

8

Moderators and mediators of CBT for FAP | 147

Parenting Stress Index (PSI; Abidin, 1983) was filled out by parents. This scale asks parents to rate 25 statements about their child, for example, “I notice I am not as capable in taking care of my child as I thought I would be” and “Parenting this child is harder than I thought it would be”. The NOSI-K is a short version of a more comprehensive measure – the NOSI. This shorter version was constructed by factor analysis on the original items. A selection of 25 items was made, which all loaded high on one common factor, ‘parental stress’. Within these 25 items, 10 of the original 13 scales of the NOSI are represented (De Brock et al., 1992). The 25 items are totaled to form one new scale, with high scores reflecting a high level of parental stress. Participants were asked to rate on a six-point Likert scale ranging from ‘totally disagree’ (1) to ‘totally agree’ (6) whether or not they agreed with the 25 statements. The NOSI-K has been found to have good psychometric properties (De Brock et al., 1992).

Negative and positive cognitions about FAP. Children in both groups and their parents filled out the Pain Beliefs Questionnaire (PBQ) to measure both negative and positive beliefs about the child’s AP (Van Slyke, 2001; Walker et al., 2005). The PBQ was translated into Dutch by the present research group (Van der Veek et al., submitted a). The PBQ is a 32-item questionnaire, with 20 items assessing beliefs about condition frequency, duration and seriousness, and episode specific intensity and duration. The other 12 items make up the problem focused and emotion focused coping potential scales (PFCP and EFCP), which measure whether children and parents believe that the child can cope with the pain. Responses are given on a 5-point Likert scale; higher scores reflect that the child or parent has such thoughts more often. The negative beliefs scale was calculated by averaging all 20 items pertaining to that subject. The PFCP and EFCP scales were calculated by averaging the 6 items of each scale.

Coping with FAP. To measure the way children coped with their AP, children filled out the Pain Response Inventory (PRI; Walker et al., 1997). The PRI is a 60 item questionnaire measuring several types of coping strategies. Answers can be given on a 5-point Likert scale, with higher scores reflecting a greater usage of these coping strategies. The PRI was also translated into Dutch by the present research group (Van der Veek et al., submitted a). The PRI consists of 3 higher order scales, measuring active coping (e.g., “try to do something to make it go away”), passive coping (e.g., “not even try to do anything about it because it will not help”) and accommodative coping (e.g., “try to learn to live with it”). Scale scores are calculated by averaging the items pertaining to that scale. The PRI has been shown to be a valid instrument, with Cronbach’s alpha’s ranging from .71 to .78 (Walker et al., 1997).

Parental solicitous behavior. Parents filled out the Illness Behavior Encouragement Scale (IBES; Walker & Zeman, 1992) to measure parental solicitous behavior. The IBES consists of 12 items which are answered on a 5-point scale. Higher scores reflect more parental solicitous behavior. The IBES has been shown to be a reliable instrument, with Cronbach’s alphas of .75 to .85 (Walker & Zeman, 1992).

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 147 18-07-12 17:20

Page 151: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

148 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Vigilance to pain. To measure self-reported vigilance to pain, the Pain Vigilance and Awareness Questionnaire was used (PVAQ; Roelofs et al., 2002, 2003). The PVAQ consists of 16 items which can be answered on a 6-point Likert scale, ranging from (0) never to (5) always. Exemplary items are “I focus on sensations of pain” and “I know immediately when pain starts or increases”. It has been validated in a Dutch sample of adult healthy participants and fibromyalgia patients (Roelofs et al., 2002, 2003). For the present study, the items were adapted slightly to accommodate the reading level of the children participating in our study. Two subscales were calculated, measuring attention to pain and attention to changes in pain. Subscale scores were calculated by summing the items pertaining to each scale.

Statistical analysesTo investigate whether age, gender, Rome III diagnosis, time since start of complaints, anxiety, depression, emotion awareness, coping with everyday problems, parental FGID diagnosis, and parental stress, anxiety, depression and somatization moderated treatment effects, a series of linear mixed models was run in SPSS 18.0 with the moderating variable and all its interactions with time and treatment as independent factors, and child and parent API scores and PIS and PFS scores as dependent variables. Age and gender were entered as covariates. Our objective was to ascertain whether the time x treatment x moderator interaction effect reached significance, as this indicates that for different levels of the moderator, the interventions lead to a significantly different effect over time. To investigate whether the hypothesized mediator variables parental and children’s negative cognitions about the AP and positive thoughts about coping potential, children’s coping with AP, parental solicitous responses to AP, and children’s vigilance towards their AP changed during each treatment, another series of linear mixed models was performed. Time, treatment, and time x treatment were entered as independent variables, and age and gender as covariates. Second, to investigate whether a change in mediator was related to a change in AP, we calculated Pearson correlations between changes on the mediators and changes on self and parent reported API scores, and PIS and PFS diary scores. This was only done for those mediators showing significant changes over time in the mixed models analyses. Two change scores were calculated per mediator, one indicating change from pre- to post-treatment, and one from pre-treatment to 6 months follow-up. For the four outcome measures, these two change scores were also calculated, and a third change score was added to indicate the change from pre-treatment to 12 months follow-up. As such, six possible correlations between change in mediator and change in outcome could be calculated. With twelve mediator variables and four outcome measures, this resulted in 288 correlations. Because of the large number of correlations calculated and the exploratory nature of this part of the study, we decided to only interpret the influence of those mediators who showed significant correlations

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 148 18-07-12 17:20

Page 152: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

8

Moderators and mediators of CBT for FAP | 149

on at least three of the six possible correlations between change in mediator and change in outcome, and these three correlations had to be significant for at least two of the four possible outcomes. Correlations were calculated separately for the two treatments, which resulted in rather small sample sizes, showing some outliers. Therefore, Spearman instead of Pearson correlations were calculated (Field, 2005).

Results

Data inspectionFor 20 children (17.9%), no parental PBQ data were available because this questionnaire was added to the instrument battery at a later time. The reason for the missings in these cases was thus observed, and as such it can be described as “missing at random” (MAR) (Rubin, 1976). It has been shown that in the case of MAR it is better to impute data than do a complete-case analysis (Schafer & Graham, 2002). Therefore, we imputed the PBQ data making use of the multiple imputations subroutine available in SPSS 18.0. All variables used in this study were used to impute the missing data five times.

Moderators of treatment effectNone of the demographic (age, gender) or clinical (time since start of complaints, Rome III diagnosis, anxiety, depression) characteristics had any moderating effects on treatment effectiveness (p’s of all time x treatment x moderator effects >.05 in linear mixed models). Also, none of the parent related variables had a moderating effect on treatment effectiveness (p’s for time x treatment x moderator effects >.05 for presence of FGID in parents and parental stress, anxiety, depression and somatization). The only two variables for which we found a moderating effect on two, respectively one of the four outcome measures, were ‘not hiding emotions’ (aspect of emotion awareness; PIS: F(3, 90.76) = 2.947; p =.037. PFS: F(3, 86.66) = 3.093; p =.031), and ‘seeking social support’ (coping strategy; parent reported API: F(3, 85.52) = 2.970; p =.036). The moderating effects of these variables are displayed in figure 1 and 2. For children who reported to hide their emotions from others less often prior to treatment, CBT was more effective than MC at decreasing PIS and PFS diary scores at 6 months follow-up (PIS: t(81.71) = 2.949; p =.004. PFS: t(85.13) = 2.710; p =.008). For children who reported to seek social support in the face of everyday problems more often prior to treatment, CBT was more effective than MC at decreasing parent reported API scores at 12 months follow-up (t(86.49) = 2.874; p =.005).

To see how large these significant effects were on the treatment success of CBT, we calculated the percentage of treatment success on the respective outcome measures in the group of children scoring below and above the mean scores of not hiding emotions and seeking social support (see Van der Veek et al., submitted b, for more detail on how

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 149 18-07-12 17:20

Page 153: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

150 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

scores on treatment success were calculated). This analysis showed that in the group of children scoring above the mean of not hiding emotions, 79.2% of children who received CBT had improved or recovered at 6 months follow-up according to their diaries, versus 43.75% of children in the group scoring below the mean of not hiding, which was a significant difference (χ2(2) = 9.995; p =.007). For MC, no significant relationship was found between levels of not hiding emotions and treatment effectiveness (50.0% vs. 45.5% success, χ2(2) = 1.261; p =.532). Concerning social support, we found that 81.0% of the children scoring above the mean of seeking social support prior to treatment had reliably improved at 12 months follow-up according to parent reported API scores, whereas 43.5% of the children scoring below the mean of seeking social support had reliably improved. This difference was also significant (χ2(1) = 6.502; p =.011). Again, for MC, there was no relationship between levels of seeking social support and treatment success (64.7% vs. 56.5% success, χ2(1)=.273; p =.601).

Figure 1. Moderating effect of not hiding emotions on pain intensity (PIS) and pain frequency (PFS) scores at 6 months follow-up

Not hiding15,0013,0011,009,007,005,00

Cha

nge

PIS

6 m

onth

s fo

llow

-up

30,00

20,00

10,00

0,00

-10,00

MCCBTMCCBT

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 150 18-07-12 17:20

Page 154: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

8

Moderators and mediators of CBT for FAP | 151

Not hiding15,0012,5010,007,505,00

Cha

nge

PFS

6 m

onth

s fo

llow

-up

25,00

20,00

15,00

10,00

5,00

0,00

-5,00

MCCBTMCCBT

Figure 2. Moderating effect of seeking social support on parental abdominal pain index score at 12 months follow-up.

Seeking social support

3,503,002,502,001,501,00

Cha

nge

par

enta

l API

at 1

2 m

onth

s fo

llow

-up 40,00

20,00

0,00

-20,00

MCCBTMCCBT

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 151 18-07-12 17:20

Page 155: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

152 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Mediators of treatment effectThe linear mixed models showed that for all investigated mediator variables, a significant time effect was found, except for active coping (F(2, 138.273) = .018; p =.982) and for attention to changes in pain (F(2, 108.91) = .084; p =.919). This significant time effect indicated that all children changed on those mediators over time, irrespective of the treatment they received. For two variables, there was a significant difference between treatments. First, a significant time x treatment interaction effect was found for parental thoughts about the problem focused coping potential of their children directly after treatment (pooled t-test on imputed datasets t = -2.278; N=104; p=.027) and a trend was found at 6 months follow-up (pooled t-test on imputed datasets t = -1.769; N=104; p =.086). Post-hoc one sample t-tests showed that both treatments led to a significant increase in positive parental thoughts, but this effect was stronger for CBT (post-treatment: pooled mean difference=1.05, p <.001. 6 months follow-up: pooled mean difference = 1.22, p <.001) than for MC (post-treatment: pooled mean difference=.43, p <.05. Six months follow-up: pooled mean difference=.73, p <.01). The effect is shown in figure 3a. A second significant interaction effect displayed in figure 3b, was for attention to changes in pain (post-treatment: t(124.81) = 2.391; p =.018. Six months follow-up: t(154.67) = 2.501; p =.013). While children receiving MC did not change in how much attention they paid to changes in their AP after treatment (post-treatment: mean difference = 1.79, p =.216. Six months follow-up: mean difference = 1.98, p =.109), this attention decreased in children receiving CBT at 6 months follow-up (mean difference = -2.91, p =.021).

Figure 3. Change in mediating variables over time for both treatments.

a. Parental beliefs about problem focused coping potential of child

0,0

0,5

1,0

1,5

2,0

2,5

3,0

3,5

4,0

Pre-treatment Post-treatment 6 months follow-up

CBT

MC

0

2

4

6

8

10

12

14

16

18

20

Pre-treatment Post-treatment 6 months follow-up

CBT

MC

Note: scores increased significantly for both treatments at post-treatment and 6 months follow-up. Difference between treatments at post-treatment: p <.05; difference between treatments at 6 months follow-up: p <.10.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 152 18-07-12 17:20

Page 156: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

8

Moderators and mediators of CBT for FAP | 153

b. Attention to changes in pain

0,0

0,5

1,0

1,5

2,0

2,5

3,0

3,5

4,0

Pre-treatment Post-treatment 6 months follow-up

CBT

MC

0

2

4

6

8

10

12

14

16

18

20

Pre-treatment Post-treatment 6 months follow-up

CBT

MC

Note: scores were significantly decreased for CBT at 6 months follow-up. Difference between treatments at post-treatment and at 6 months follow-up: p <.05.

Second, to investigate whether changes in mediators were related to changes in outcomes, we calculated correlations between changes on the mediator variables and changes on self and parent reported API scores, and PIS and PFS diary scores. The results are displayed in table 1. The mediators most strongly related to change in CBT were children’s negative cognitions about condition seriousness, children’s positive cognitions about coping potential, and parental solicitous behavior. Improvements in these variables were significantly related to decreases in AP, both at the same time point and prospectively (e.g., decrease in negative cognitions from pre- to post-treatment correlated significantly (r = -.377) with a better outcome on API score from pre-treatment to 6 months follow-up). Changes in children’s negative cognitions were also related to change in AP in children receiving MC.

Discussion

The present exploratory study showed that the psychological factors emotion awareness and coping moderated treatment effectiveness of CBT for pediatric FAP. None of the demographic, clinical or parental characteristics were found to moderate treatment effectiveness. In addition, the results showed that three factors may be candidate working mechanisms of CBT: children’s negative cognitions about their AP, children’s positive cognitions about their potential to cope with their AP, and parental solicitous behavior. Looking at the results for the moderation analyses in more detail, we see that children that were more likely to show their emotions to others and to seek social support prior to treatment, benefitted more from CBT than from MC. This effect was profound: about

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 153 18-07-12 17:20

Page 157: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

154 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

80% of children receiving CBT and scoring high on not hiding emotions or seeking social support were recovered or improved at 6 or 12 months follow-up, while only 44% of the children scoring low on these variables recovered or improved. A possible reason why the current protocol was less effective for children that weren’t likely to seek social support or to show their emotions, is because it may be more difficult for them to go to a therapist and talk about their complaints, making the current protocol less suitable for them. As such, incorporating a screening on these variables prior to treatment may be useful in deciding which children should receive the CBT protocol as it is, and for which children efforts should be undertaken to adapt the CBT protocol, by for example delivering the intervention through the internet or through audiotapes (van Tilburg et al., 2009), or by paying more attention to their difficulties with showing their emotions. However, because of the exploratory nature of the present study, future studies into the influence of these moderators on treatment effectiveness of CBT are needed.

Concerning mediators, decreases in children’s negative thoughts about their condition, increases in children’s positive thoughts about their coping potential, and decreases in parental solicitous behavior were all related to better treatment outcomes. Note, however, that, in contrast to other studies (Levy et al., 2010; Litt, Shafer, Ibanez, Kreutzer, & Tawfik-Yonkers, 2009), changes in the way children coped with their AP, were unrelated to treatment effectiveness. Thus, it seems that for the present population, whether a child believes he/she can cope with the AP is more important than the actual coping mechanism employed, suggesting that feeling you are in control of your AP might be the factor of essence. Also, although parental negative and positive thoughts changed following CBT, and positive thoughts increased significantly more following CBT than following MC, these changes were mostly unrelated to treatment outcome, making it unlikely that changing parental thoughts was a working mechanism for CBT. Instead, changes in parental solicitous behavior were related to treatment outcome, suggesting that the overt behavior displayed by the parent is more important than the covert thoughts parents have about the AP of their child. Surprisingly, decreases in children’s negative cognitions about their condition were related to decreases in AP in children receiving CBT as well as MC. Indeed, a study in adult patients with IBS showed that medical consultations can change negative cognitions about complaints, and these changes were related to better outcome of IBS (Van Dulmen, Fennis, Mokkink, Van der Velden, & Bleijenberg, 1997). Thus, it can be questioned whether the specific techniques used in CBT exclusively effect changes in negative cognitions. Moreover, for all of the mediators it can be questioned what the direction is of the relationships between the mediators and treatment outcome. As shown in table 1, changes in the mediators were related to changes in outcome concurrently and prospectively, but also prospectively in the opposite direction, meaning that a change in outcome from pre- to post-treatment was related to a change in mediator at 6 months follow-up. This makes

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 154 18-07-12 17:20

Page 158: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

8

Moderators and mediators of CBT for FAP | 155

Tab

le 1

. Spe

arm

an c

orre

latio

ns b

etw

een

chan

ge in

med

iato

rs a

nd c

hang

e in

out

com

e.

a. C

BT

Abd

omin

al p

ain

inde

xA

bdom

inal

pai

n in

dex-

pare

ntPa

in in

tens

ity s

core

Pain

freq

uenc

y sc

ore

T1-T

2T1

-T3

T1-T

4T1

-T2

T1-T

3T1

-T4

T1-T

2T1

-T3

T1-T

4T1

-T2

T1-T

3T1

-T4

Chi

ld-r

epor

ted

varia

bles

Neg

ativ

e co

gniti

ons T

1-T2

-.4

63**

-.304

-.224

-.614

***

-.446

**-.4

01*

-.306

-.351

*-.4

10*

-.230

-.411

*-.4

12*

Neg

ativ

e co

gniti

ons T

1-T3

-.5

20**

-.645

***

-.605

***

-.463

**-.6

94**

*-.6

61**

*-.3

74*

-.512

**-.5

32**

-.186

-.585

***

-.515

**PF

CP

T1-T

2.5

12**

.353

*.1

74.3

66*

.280

.270

.016

.372

*.2

51-.0

51.3

20.2

42PF

CP

T1-T

3.4

88**

.445

**.2

90.3

70*

.420

**.4

38**

.041

.434

**.3

14.0

44.2

55.2

79EF

CP

T1-T

2.2

10.1

75.1

69.3

09*

.222

.335

*.1

12.4

18*

.416

*-.0

88.3

63*

.298

EFC

P T1

-T3

.212

.408

**.3

07.3

85*

.397

**.4

19*

.219

.446

**.3

55*

.032

.324

.147

Pare

nt-r

epor

ted

varia

bles

Solic

itous

beh

avio

r T1-

T2.1

30.0

62-.0

39-.0

26-.1

55-.1

89-.1

77-.2

83-.4

58*

-.014

-.098

-.204

Solic

itous

beh

avio

r T1-

T3

-.289

-.317

*-.2

73-.3

50*

-.420

**-.3

70*

-.446

*-.5

73**

*-.5

12**

-.344

-.394

*-.3

47

b. M

C

Abd

omin

al p

ain

inde

xA

bdom

inal

pai

n in

dex-

pare

ntPa

in in

tens

ity s

core

Pain

freq

uenc

y sc

ore

T1-T

2T1

-T3

T1-T

4T1

-T2

T1-T

3T1

-T4

T1-T

2T1

-T3

T1-T

4T1

-T2

T1-T

3T1

-T4

Chi

ld-r

epor

ted

varia

bles

Neg

ativ

e co

gniti

ons T

1-T2

-.4

71**

-.210

-.293

-.358

*-.0

52-.1

44-.2

78-.1

92-.1

54-.1

05-.0

61-.1

73N

egat

ive

cogn

ition

s T1-

T3

-.406

**-.7

36**

*-.3

29*

-.088

-.572

***

-.226

-.171

-.440

*-.4

09*

-.213

-.195

-.187

Not

e. *

p <

.05;

**p

<.0

1; *

**p

<.0

01. T

1 =

pre

-tre

atm

ent,

T2 =

pos

t-tr

eatm

ent,

T3 =

6 m

onth

s fol

low

-up,

T4

= 1

2 m

onth

s fol

low

-up.

PFC

P =

pro

blem

-focu

sed

copi

ng p

oten

tial. E

FCP

=

emot

ion-

focu

sed

copi

ng p

oten

tial. P

ositi

ve sc

ores

on

chan

ge in

out

com

e va

riabl

es in

dica

te d

ecre

ases

in a

bdom

inal

pai

n. P

ositi

ve sc

ores

on

chan

ge in

med

iato

r var

iabl

es in

dica

te

mor

e so

licito

us b

ehav

ior,

mor

e ne

gativ

e co

gniti

ons,

and

mor

e po

sitiv

e th

ough

ts a

bout

cop

ing

pote

ntia

l at f

ollo

w-u

p.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 155 18-07-12 17:20

Page 159: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

156 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

sense intuitively: if the treatment is effective and the AP decreases, negative cognitions about AP will probably also diminish. It is clear that more research is needed to discern the directionality of these processes, before judgments can be made about whether a factor is a mediator or a consequence of CBT. There are a number of limitations to this study that should be addressed. First, not every child received the same treatment because the CBT protocol consisted of optional modules, allowing tailoring of the protocol to the individual child. However, this limitation is mitigated because treatment effectiveness showed no relations with which module or which combination of modules was applied (Van der Veek et al., submitted b). Second, the present study did not measure change in mediators during treatment, precluding an investigation of whether the change in mediator precedes change in outcome and thus whether a mediator is an actual mechanism of change of the therapy (Kraemer et al., 2002; Maric et al., 2012; Weersing & Weisz, 2002). Because our CBT only lasted six sessions and we focused on many different possible mediators, we decided against incorporating an assessment during treatment and to perform an exploratory study on which mediators might be candidate mechanisms of change. To be able to study whether children’s negative and positive cognitions and parental solicitous behavior are actually mechanisms of change, however, future research should incorporate an assessment of mediators during treatment. Also, we did not assess whether the moderating variables emotion awareness and coping with everyday problems changed during treatment, which may have occurred. Finally, a lot of exploratory analyses were done, resulting in an increased chance on type I errors. More research is needed focusing on specific moderators and mediators of treatment effectiveness, to formally establish their influence. Concluding, this study shows that CBT is more effective in decreasing AP in children with FAP who show their emotions to others and seek social support more often prior to treatment. Changing children’s positive and negative cognitions and changing parental solicitous behavior may be mechanisms through which CBT exerts its influence. The results of our study may be used as a basis for future studies, incorporating a more vigorous design to test for moderators and mechanisms of change of CBT for pediatric FAP.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 156 18-07-12 17:20

Page 160: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 157 18-07-12 17:20

Page 161: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 158 18-07-12 17:20

Page 162: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

Ch

apte

r

9General discussion and summary

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 159 18-07-12 17:20

Page 163: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

160 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Tuesday, September 8th 2009, 12.00 am, de Bascule, Amsterdam.

Downstairs, in front of the psychiatry building, Elin and her mother are

waiting for Elin’s father to get the car from the parking garage. Her pain

is too intense right now to walk to the car. While they are waiting, Elin

thinks about all of the things that have been discussed. It was a lot. It’s

a pity that the psychologist wouldn’t write an excuse note for gym. Well,

nothing to it than going to gym class this afternoon, then, Elin thinks,

feeling depressed. Maybe I can be the goalkeeper with soccer, or catcher

with softball. The psychologist was a little strange, wearing jeans and no

white coat, Elin thinks. And she was so young. But also sort of nice. Elin

wonders whether the exercises she should practice, are really going to

help her. She asks her mother, who replies that she doesn’t know, and

that they’ll have to wait and see. Elin takes a deep breath, and remembers

just in time that she has to breath by moving her abdomen now, not by

moving her chest. That feels funny. But also kind of pleasant.

Maybe practicing at home is not such a bad idea.

The present thesis aimed to study the effectiveness of a psychosocial treatment for children like Elin who suffer from functional abdominal pain (FAP). In addition, throughout the first part of this thesis, we investigated whether a number of psychological and social factors play a role in the well-being of children with FAP. Where Elin is just starting her treatment, at the end of this thesis we have arrived at a beginning of understanding how to best treat children with FAP. The present chapter summarizes and integrates the findings of the different chapters and will set out future directions for research and practice.

Summary

Part I: Psychosocial risk factors for pediatric FAPThe first empirical study of this thesis is described in chapter 2, and focuses on the prevalence of abdominal pain and its relationships with other physical and psychological complaints in a community sample of schoolchildren. Additionally, it was investigated whether occurrence of AP in this sample coincided with parental physical and psychological complaints, as is often found in pediatric FAP. A large sample of schoolchildren aged 7-18 (N=665) filled out questionnaires, and a total of 391 of their parents also participated. The results of this study showed that AP was frequent in this school sample, with 56.5% of the children reporting AP at least once in a two-week period. Largest relationships with AP were found for female gender, younger age, activity limitations, other somatic complaints

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 160 18-07-12 17:20

Page 164: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

9

General discussion and summary | 161

and depressive symptoms. The relationship between anxiety and AP was only small in this sample, which contradicts findings from clinical studies in children with FAP (Campo et al., 2004). Also, parental physical and psychological complaints were unrelated to child reports of AP, in contrast to what is usually found in children with FAP (Levy, 2011). The relationship between parental physical complaints and FAP, and the influence of parental behavioral reactions to the child’s complaints, was further investigated in a systematic review and meta-analysis described in chapter 3. The aim of this review was to investigate whether parents maintain or exacerbate the complaints of their children through modeling of physical symptoms and solicitous responses to the child’s complaints. We found that parents of children with FAP reported more physical symptoms than parents of healthy children, but this difference was only small. Additionally, as all studies were cross-sectional or retrospective, the causal direction was unclear. For parental responses to child complaints, not enough studies were available to perform a meta-analysis. Moreover, half of the studies that were available, found no significant relationship with FAP. We concluded that the present literature is unfit to establish whether parents maintain or exacerbate pediatric FAP through the processes investigated. Chapter 4 describes the results of a study investigating the level of emotion awareness of children with FAP and what type of coping strategies these children use when coping with everyday problems, compared to children with no AP and children with occasional AP. In addition, it was investigated whether coping mediates the relationship between emotion awareness and FAP. The results showed that children with FAP scored significantly lower on most aspects of emotion awareness than children without AP, although these differences were small. Contrary to expectations, children with FAP were more aware of a link between emotions and bodily sensations than children without AP. As for coping, we found that children with FAP used avoidant coping more often than children without AP, but coping was not a convincing mediator for the relationship between emotion awareness and FAP. It was concluded that children with FAP show only small differences in emotion awareness and coping compared to children without AP, and practically no difference when compared to children with some AP.

The objective of our fourth study was to investigate whether children’s or parental cognitions and coping behaviors more strongly contribute to a) having pediatric FAP and b) the physical and psychological well-being of children with FAP. As described in chapter 5, children with FAP and their parents were compared to a control group of 155 schoolchildren with occasional AP. Both groups filled out questionnaires concerning negative cognitions about the severity of the child’s condition, positive cognitions about the coping potential of the child, coping with AP, parental solicitous responses to AP, and the child’s level of AP, functional disability, anxiety, depression and quality of life. This study showed that both children’s cognitions and behaviors and parental cognitions were significantly related to FAP, although parental cognitions had a stronger relationship. In

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 161 18-07-12 17:20

Page 165: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

162 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

contrast, children’s cognitions and behaviors were more strongly associated with children’s well-being than parent variables. Concrete displays of parental solicitous behavior were unrelated to either having FAP or the well-being of children with FAP. Children’s coping behavior and positive thoughts were more strongly related to having FAP and to the well-being of children with FAP, than children’s negative thoughts about their condition.

The final study for this part of the thesis described in chapter 6 focused on whether children with functional abdominal pain (FAP) show an attentional bias for information concerning the activity of their body. Additionally, as is suggested by Pennebaker’s symptom perception hypothesis, we investigated whether being presented with information about bodily activity influenced perception of bodily sensations. Thirty children with FAP and thirty healthy children filled out questionnaires and performed a dot-probe detection task, in which children were shown sham pictures about their bodily activity. Results showed no attentional bias for gut-activity in either children with FAP or control children, although children with FAP were slower than healthy children on all supraliminal trials in which gut-activity was displayed. Children with FAP and healthy children showed an attentional bias away from supraliminal pictures about heart-activity. Thirty to forty percent of children with FAP experienced a light increase in abdominal pain after the experiment, which suggests that focusing on bodily activity may increase symptom perception. Overall, children with FAP only differed from healthy children concerning their responses to gut-activity, not concerning their responses to other bodily activity.

Part II: Cognitive behavior therapy for pediatric functional abdominal painCognitive behavior therapy (CBT) is a promising treatment for pediatric functional abdominal pain (FAP), but solid evidence for its effectiveness is lacking. Chapter 7 describes the results of a randomized controlled trial, aiming to investigate the effectiveness of a six session protocolized CBT, compared to the effects of six visits to a pediatrician (medical care; MC). One hundred and four children aged 7-18 were randomized to CBT or MC. CBT was mostly delivered by trained master students in psychology, MC was delivered by pediatricians or pediatric gastroenterologists. Both CBT and MC led to a significant decrease in abdominal pain, but the treatments did not differ in their effectiveness. One year after treatment over 60% of children that received CBT had significantly improved or recovered, versus 56% of children receiving MC, which did not significantly differ. For secondary outcomes, it was found that directly after treatment and at six months follow-up, children that received CBT experienced fewer symptoms of anxiety and depression than children that received MC. As two different treatments led to similar results, more research into the working mechanisms of each treatment is warranted. A first step into this research on working mechanisms is made in our final empirical study presented in chapter 8, in which we explored which factors moderate and mediate treatment effectiveness of CBT. Investigated moderators were age, gender, Rome III

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 162 18-07-12 17:20

Page 166: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

9

General discussion and summary | 163

diagnosis, time since start of complaints, anxiety, depression, emotion awareness, coping with everyday problems, parental functional gastrointestinal disorder, and parental stress, anxiety, depression and somatization. Investigated mediators were parental and children’s negative cognitions about AP and positive thoughts about coping potential, children’s coping with AP, parental solicitous responses to AP, and vigilance for pain. Results showed that children that were more likely to show their emotions and to seek social support for everyday problems prior to treatment, benefitted significantly more from CBT (about 80% effectiveness) than children scoring low on these variables (44%). Decreases in children’s negative thoughts about their condition were related to better treatment outcome for both CBT and MC. Increases in children’s positive thoughts about coping potential and decreases in parental solicitous behavior were related to better treatment outcome in CBT only. Concluding, CBT is more effective for children who are more likely to show their emotions and to seek social support prior to treatment. Changes in children’s positive and negative cognitions and changes in parental solicitous behavior may be mechanisms through which CBT exerts its influence.

Integration of findings

Which psychosocial factors may be risk factors for pediatric FAP? Throughout this thesis, many psychosocial factors have been tested for their relationships with either the presence of FAP, or the psychological and physical well-being of children with FAP. Before describing our conclusions, however, I would like to stress that because the main objective of our research was to test the effectiveness of CBT for children with FAP, any investigations concerning risk factors could only be performed cross-sectionally by measuring these factors during the pre-treatment assessments. For a true investigation of whether a psychosocial factor is a risk factor for FAP, longitudinal or experimental studies are needed. We have compensated for this limitation to an extent by making comparisons with control groups, thus searching for factors that differentiate children with FAP from children with no abdominal pain and children with occasional AP of diverse etiology. However, while reading the discussion below, one should bare in mind that because of the limitations in design, all suggestions about causality are necessarily preliminary. Nevertheless, the cross-sectional studies described in this thesis do provide clues for which factors are likely to be risk factors and which are not. For example, it seems unlikely that alexithymia/emotional awareness is an important risk factor, as it showed only small relationships with FAP, and the same holds true for the way children coped with everyday stressors. Also, we could find only small or no effects for parental solicitous responses and parental modeling of physical complaints in both our meta-analysis and our own empirical studies presented in chapter 2, 3 and 5. This result deserves special notion, as despite

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 163 18-07-12 17:20

Page 167: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

164 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

the feeble evidence available, some experts in the field state that “Parental response to

child pain behaviors appears to be a key factor in the development and maintenance of FAP” (Levy, 2011, pp. 1), and it has been suggested to develop interventions based purely on this factor, only ‘treating’ parents and not including children in the treatment. Clearly, the literature and the present thesis cannot support such an intervention.

Factors that were more strongly related to the presence of FAP and the psychological and physical well-being of children with FAP, were the way children coped with their abdominal pain, children’s and parental negative cognitions about the seriousness of the child’s condition, and their positive cognitions about the coping potential of the child. However, the note about causality mentioned above seems extra relevant for these factors. For example, some of the negative cognitions investigated were catastrophic thoughts like “My stomach aches mean I have a serious illness” and “I’ll always have stomach aches”. Although theory suggests that these negative cognitions will increase the experience of AP, it is equally likely that a reverse relationship holds true, and that having more severe AP will prompt such thoughts. A likely scenario is that they constantly influence each other, resulting in a vicious circle. Also, the fact that we found that healthy children cope more actively with AP then children with FAP do, does not automatically mean that failing to use active coping is a risk factor for FAP, as here too the relationship can be reversed: active coping may be effective to reduce occasional episodes of AP, but may not effectively reduce chronic AP, and as such, children with FAP will cease to use active coping. Although most research on coping and cognitions in children with FAP is cross-sectional, there are a few studies that have employed a longitudinal design and found that coping and cognitions are related to outcome over time (Shirkey, Smith, & Walker, 2011; Walker et al., 2005, 2007). However, these studies are scarce, and more are needed to gain more insight into whether these factors may be risk factors for FAP.

A final risk factor investigated in this thesis that we did not yet mention, is information processing, and more specifically, whether children with FAP have an attentional bias for the activity of their body. We did not find convincing evidence for the presence of an attentional bias in the present thesis; in fact, the three studies including our own that up to now have been performed in children with FAP yielded very inconsistent results, with one study showing an attentional bias toward pain stimuli (Beck et al., 2011), the second showing evidence for an attentional bias away from pain stimuli (Boyer et al., 2006), and our study showing no evidence at all for an attentional bias. Chapter six does, however, show that attentional processes may play an important role in FAP, as we found that in 30 to 40% of children with FAP, AP increased following the dot-probe task during which children were confronted with information about their bodily activity. As this task lasted only about twenty minutes, it is surprising that such a short amount of time spent focusing on the body may increase symptom reporting. Of course, we cannot say for sure that focusing on the body caused this increase in AP, as it could also have been the stress of being in an

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 164 18-07-12 17:20

Page 168: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

9

General discussion and summary | 165

experiment. Nevertheless, the results deserve further investigation, especially because of its potential for developing new intervention strategies like decreasing interpretation and attentional biases using computer paradigms.

Concluding, the present thesis showed that coping with AP, parental and children’s cognitions about AP and information processing may be factors relevant to the continuance, exacerbation and possibly onset of FAP, whereas for emotion awareness, social learning and coping with everyday problems it is unlikely that they play important roles in the etiology of FAP.

Treating children with FAP – the benefits of a supportive relationshipThe second aim of this thesis was to investigate the effectiveness of CBT in treating children with FAP. The most important conclusion that can be drawn from our RCT is that children with FAP can be effectively treated. Sixty percent of the children that were included in the RCT were significantly improved or recovered up to one year after treatment. However, what is also clear, is that it doesn’t seem to matter which of the two treatments was used, or whether treatment was provided by relatively inexperienced, trained master students in psychology, or by more experienced pediatricians and pediatric gastroenterologists. Both treatments resulted in approximately equal success rates: CBT significantly reduced AP in over 60% of children at one year follow-up, and MC reduced AP significantly in about 56% of children. However, one should bear in mind that the intensive protocol for MC used in this study is not representative of daily practice of a pediatric gastroenterologist or pediatrician, where patients are seen much less frequently.

One explanation for the similar effectiveness of CBT and MC may be that both used certain specific and unique therapeutic ingredients (e.g., the relaxation exercises used in CBT and medications prescribed in MC) that were equally effective. Also, both treatments had some similar ingredients like stressing the importance to go to school, although this was applied in a more structured way during CBT. However, we know from psychotherapy research that a considerable part of the treatment effect can be attributed to nonspecific treatment factors like a supportive relationship with a health care professional (Horvath & Symonds, 1991). As described in chapter 7, we feel that this supportive relationship is an important therapeutic ingredient that CBT and the intensive MC protocol had in common, and that this relationship may thus account for part of the similarity in effectiveness. Another result underscoring the relevance of this factor, was that 34 of the children that were screened for inclusion did not need any further treatment, because their AP was resolved after just one or two visits to one of our pediatricians. During those initial visits, children and parents were taken seriously: they received attention for and explanation about the complaints, diagnostic tests were performed when necessary, and they were treated kindly and with respect. Although one would expect that children would be treated as such by other health care professionals as well, one of the more unfortunate

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 165 18-07-12 17:20

Page 169: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

166 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

conclusions we have to draw in this thesis is that this is not the case. Parents and children often said that they felt that previous doctors had not taken them seriously and had suggested that the pain was ‘all between the ears’. The fact that so many children got better after just one consultation with one of the pediatricians, suggests that a lot can be gained from teaching pediatricians and general practitioners how to best respond to patients with functional complaints. A stepped care approach in which the first step is to take time for and listen to the patient and thereby validating their complaints, thus seems fruitful. It should be noted that as the present RCT did not include a control condition in which no supportive therapeutic relationship was provided, we cannot test what percentage of the treatment effect can be specifically attributed to this factor. As it took three years to gather enough children for the present RCT, it would not have been possible to include another treatment arm like real standard medical care in this single center trial. However, we encourage future multicenter trials to try to quantify the effects of providing a supportive relationship in the treatment of pediatric FAP.

Is CBT effective, and how does it reach its effects?In light of the discussion above, what can we say about the effectiveness of our CBT protocol? Should we stop using any specific techniques from now on and just sit down and talk with the patient, building a rapport, or does the specific content of the protocol bear additional value? Previous research provides some clues for the answer to this question. For example, the effects of CBT for pediatric FAP have been reported to be larger than the effects of purely receiving attention for the complaints (Levy et al., 2010). Additionally, interventions that for the largest part exclude the component of a supportive therapeutic relationship like internet interventions and guided relaxation through audiotapes, have been shown to have positive effects on decreasing AP in children with FAP (Hicks, von Baeyer, & McGrath, 2008; Palermo, Wilson, Peters, Lemandowski, & Somhegyi, 2009; van Tilburg et al., 2009). These results thus suggest that the techniques used in CBT also contribute to its effectiveness. However, what do our own data tell us about how CBT reaches its effects?

Chapter 8 provides part of the answer. Here we investigated which psychosocial factors changed due to or following CBT, and if a change in these factors related to a change in outcome. We found that CBT reduced children’s negative cognitions about their condition, increased children’s positive cognitions about their potential to cope with their pain, and decreased parental solicitous behavior, and that changes in these factors were related to changes in outcome. Thus, part of the effectiveness of CBT may indeed be attributed to changes in the factors that CBT focuses on. However, these relationships can also indicate a reverse causal direction, as the described factors may also change as a result of disappearance of the AP. One should note that the study described in chapter

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 166 18-07-12 17:20

Page 170: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

9

General discussion and summary | 167

8 was exploratory and the design was not suitable to formally investigate mechanisms of change, urging future studies to incorporate a more vigorous study design, including measuring change in mediating factors during treatment. Nevertheless, we may conclude that the specific content of CBT does seem to add to the treatment effect.

Another question we can ask ourselves about the effectiveness of CBT is: is it enough? How enthusiastic should we get about the 60% of children that were recovered or improved one year after follow-up? Although this is a promising result, it also means that 40% of children did not recover, which leaves ample room for improvement. Chapter 8 suggests how we may accomplish this to an extent, as this chapter showed that CBT was more effective in a specific subgroup of patients. For children who were more willing to show their emotions and who were more likely to seek social support when confronted with everyday stressors, CBT was significantly more effective at 6-12 months follow-up, with 80% improvement versus 44% in children scoring low on these factors. Thus, screening for such factors prior to treatment may be useful. In addition, this suggests that the treatment protocol may need adaptation for those children scoring low on these factors. This can be done by providing the intervention through the internet or through audiotapes, thereby offering them an intervention in which they do not have to share with others how they are doing. In light of the discussion above about the beneficial effects of a therapeutic relationship, we feel it would be highly interesting to investigate the effectiveness of such a stripped-down intervention for this subgroup of patients. On the other hand, exactly because of the potential benefits of a therapeutic relationship, the face to face therapy could also be adapted or expanded to help children deal with these specific difficulties. Note that we did not investigate whether seeking social support and being reluctant to show your emotions changed during treatment. It is easily imaginable that CBT can have an effect on one’s willingness to share emotions with others and to seek social support in the face of problems. So perhaps, the therapist delivering the CBT should pay more attention to this in the treatment of this subgroup of children. However, again one should keep in mind that the data presented in chapter 8 were exploratory and the relevance of social support seeking and hiding emotions as moderators of treatment effectiveness should be replicated before a screening as mentioned above is implemented in practice.

Finally, the attentive reader may have noticed that whereas not hiding emotions and seeking social support were significant moderators of the effectiveness of CBT for FAP, the first part of this thesis showed that these factors were mostly unrelated to FAP. Although these results seem contradictory, they actually are not, as it is likely that these aspects of coping and emotion awareness are relevant moderators of psychological therapies for any disorder, and not just for pediatric FAP (e.g., Lumley et al., 2007). In other words, not every factor that should be considered in the treatment of FAP, needs to be a risk factor for FAP. However, also note that because they have no large relationship with FAP, focusing on changing these factors in all children with FAP seems inappropriate. Another discrepancy

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 167 18-07-12 17:20

Page 171: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

168 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

exists for parental solicitous behavior, which was found to be unrelated to being diagnosed with FAP and to the psychological and physical well-being of children in chapter 5, but was identified as a potential mediator of treatment effectiveness in chapter 9. An explanation for this finding is less obvious than for the moderators, because if solicitous behavior has no relation with AP, why would decreases in this behavior be related to decreases in AP? More studies discerning cause and effect seem warranted to shed more light on the relevance of changing parental solicitous behavior during CBT for FAP.

Directions for future research and intervention

At the end of this thesis, it is apparent that a number of voids in the literature remain that deserve attention. Concerning research, one of the most important recommendations we can make, is that more longitudinal and experimental research should be performed. Much of the literature on psychosocial risk factors for pediatric FAP is cross-sectional, as were our own studies. However, if we ever want to discern whether the psychological and social factors investigated in this thesis are actually maintaining or exacerbating factors, experimental and longitudinal research is needed. Experimental study designs are starting to be implemented in the research fields on parental influences (Goubert, Vervoort, Cano, & Crombez, 2009; Walker et al., 2006) and attentional biases (Beck et al., 2011; Boyer et al., 2006; chapter 6), but more studies are needed to validate these preliminary findings. Another intriguing challenge for future research is how best to measure the concepts under investigation. First of all, our RCT showed that questionnaire data or diary data, although both self-reported, showed very different treatment effects directly after treatment, with success rates for CBT of 66.6% according to the diary data, and 30% according to the questionnaire data. This suggests that either the diaries overestimate the actual success of treatment, or the questionnaires underestimate its effect. Considering the problems accompanying diary data, with much missing data and children filling out a diary in retrospect for a whole week instead of daily like is intended, it is likely that the questionnaire data are more reliable, but these also suffer from the usual drawbacks of self-report. Although observation is always mentioned as an alternative to self-report, this is difficult for the subjective experience of pain. In the present study we asked parents to observe and rate the pain of their children, but parents often mentioned they found this difficult to do, especially those parents who had older children that did not talk as much about their pain with their parents. Thus, every single mode of measurement has its drawbacks and using multiple sources of pain rating as used in the present study is essential to get the most complete picture on the effectiveness of a treatment. Similarly, for the two concepts of emotional awareness and attentional biases, it can be questioned whether the measures we used represent the concepts in the best

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 168 18-07-12 17:20

Page 172: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

9

General discussion and summary | 169

way. To start with the latter, many remarks can be made about the validity of the dot-probe task used in the present thesis and, in fact, about the dot-probe task in general when used in pain-related attentional bias research (Dear, Sharpe, Nicholas, & Refshauge, 2011). For example, one may wonder whether the pictures we used were clear enough to be processed at a subliminal level, whether they were threatening enough to elicit an attentional response, whether the curiously slow reaction times in response to leg-pictures indeed can be explained by characteristics of the pictures or by some other unknown effect, etcetera. More research is needed to validate this procedure, before we can conclude that the fact that we did not find an attentional bias, indeed means that no attentional bias is present in children with FAP. How to reliably measure emotional awareness is also an intriguing and difficult question. The present study used a validated self-report instrument, but thinking about the concept, it seems strange to use self-report for a concept measuring awareness. To be able to fill out such a questionnaire, one must be aware of not being aware of his emotions - this is an obvious paradox. The large body of research using self-report to measure alexithymia and emotion awareness seems to suggest that it is possible to measure these concepts by self-report, but it can at least be questioned whether this is correct, and efforts should be undertaken to measure emotion awareness on a more experimental level as was done in the studies by Jellesma et al. (2009) and Rieffe et al. (2004). Moreover, it should be noted that the literature on alexithymia and emotion awareness is confused, with ongoing discussions about its definition and the concepts that are embraced by these umbrella terms (Larsen, Brand, Bermond, & Hijman, 2003; Lumley et al., 2007). This was also seen in the present thesis, where we found that a specific aspect of alexithymia/emotion awareness that seems key to its relationship with somatic complaints – the awareness of bodily sensations accompanying emotions – consistently shows relationships with somatic complaints that contradict alexithymia-theory. Alexithymia theory suggests that persons that are more alexithymic report medically unexplained symptoms, because they incorrectly attribute physical sensations that belong to an emotion to a physical disease and thus seek medical care for symptoms for which no physical disease can be found (Kooiman et al., 2004). However, our data showed that children with FAP had no trouble acknowledging that certain physical sensations accompany emotions. On the contrary, they were more aware of this fact than their healthy peers (chapter 4), which is in line with earlier research findings (Jellesma et al., 2006; Rieffe et al., 2007, 2008). This poses questions about the alexithymia concept as a whole, that should be sorted out in future research. The present thesis also holds implications for future studies on treatment of children with FAP. In our opinion, the most important step forward is to investigate mechanisms of change of CBT to be able to improve and tailor the intervention, focusing on the mediators and moderators that were found relevant in this thesis. Additionally, as the treatment was successfully delivered by inexperienced trained master students in psychology, this

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 169 18-07-12 17:20

Page 173: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

170 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

suggests that the treatment could perhaps also be successfully delivered by trained nurses or other health care professionals. This may reduce the possible stigma of having to go to a psychologist, although very few of the children in our study expressed that they had problems with this. Finally, another promising research avenue is to investigate whether CBT can also be successfully delivered through the internet, with or without contact with and feedback from a therapist. The nature of the present protocol, with only 6 sessions and clearly described goals, makes the protocol easy to convert to an internet format. Providing the intervention through the internet has the potential to make the intervention more accessible and could perhaps prevent children from coming in to secondary and tertiary care if the intervention could be disseminated from general practitioners’ offices. Previous studies on the effectiveness of internet interventions with therapeutic feedback for pediatric pain are promising (Hicks et al., 2008; Palermo et al., 2009).

A final implication for intervention that should be considered, is whether the fact that intensive MC was equally effective as CBT means that we should implement the intensive MC protocol in more hospitals in the Netherlands. The answer to this question seems to be no. At a presentation of the results of our RCT during a conference of the Dutch Society for Gastroenterology, the audience did not react enthusiastically to a suggestion made by the chairman that gastroenterologists should start to employ the intensive MC in their daily practice. This negative reaction may be because of time or financial considerations, as with the current health care system in the Netherlands, hospitals get a set amount of money for every child with FAP, irrespective of how often the pediatricians see these children. However, it is also commonly known that patients with medically unexplained symptoms are not popular among doctors, who often feel pressurized by the patient to perform more diagnostic tests and apply biomedical interventions. They generally feel that patients with medically unexplained symptoms are ‘difficult to manage’ (Reid, Whooley, Crayford, & Hotopf, 2001). In contrast to this view, studies show that patients mostly just seek to be reassured and understood and are not focused on additional diagnostic testing (Olde Hartman, Hassink-Franke, Lucassen, Van Spaendonck, & Van Weel, 2009). Thus, there seems to be a chasm between the way most physicians feel about patients with medically unexplained symptoms and about what they can offer them on the one hand, and what they can in fact, as underlined by this study, offer them on the other. Medical training up till now pays little attention to how doctors can effectively interact with patients with medically unexplained symptoms. Considering the beneficial effects of a good doctor-patient relationship found in this study, efforts should be undertaken to change physicians’ attitudes toward patients with medically unexplained symptoms, starting at medical school. This seems a more important, fruitful and feasible goal than implementing the MC protocol throughout hospitals in the Netherlands.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 170 18-07-12 17:20

Page 174: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

9

General discussion and summary | 171

Conclusion

At the end of this thesis, we have arrived at a beginning of understanding how to best treat children with FAP like Elin presented in the introduction and at the beginning of this discussion. As is often the case, in our attempt to answer our primary research questions, we have raised many new ones. Nevertheless, we have also found some answers. Most importantly, we have shown that children like Elin cán be effectively treated with a short, protocolized CBT. Although further evidence about the working mechanisms of CBT is needed to be able to improve and tailor the intervention, we hope to have made clear that sending children home with the message that they “will have to learn to live with it”, does not do justice to what we can provide them. Future researchers are encouraged to perform more longitudinal and experimental studies on psychosocial risk factors and treatment for children with FAP, to be able to help children like Elin more effectively in the future.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 171 18-07-12 17:20

Page 175: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 172 18-07-12 17:20

Page 176: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

Ch

apte

r 10

Nederlandse samenvatting

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 173 18-07-12 17:20

Page 177: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

174 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Chronische buikpijn is een veelvoorkomende klacht bij kinderen en jongeren. Ongeveer 10% van de kinderen in Westerse landen heeft last van terugkerende buikpijn (Chitkara et al., 2005) en in de meeste gevallen kan er geen lichamelijke oorzaak voor deze pijn gevonden worden. Indien er geen lichamelijke oorzaak wordt gevonden, spreekt men over ‘functionele’ buikpijn (FAP). Het doel van het huidige proefschrift was om de effectiviteit te onderzoeken van een psychosociale behandeling voor kinderen en jongeren met FAP. Daarnaast is in het eerste deel van dit proefschrift onderzoek gedaan naar de relatie tussen het welzijn van kinderen met FAP en een aantal psychologische en sociale factoren die mogelijk een rol spelen in de etiologie van FAP. Dit hoofdstuk vat de belangrijkste bevindingen uit het huidige proefschrift samen.

Deel 1: Psychosociale risicofactoren voor functionele buikpijn bij kinderen

Buikpijn en comorbide klachtenHet eerste wetenschappelijke onderzoek van dit proefschrift wordt beschreven in hoofdstuk 2. Dit hoofdstuk richt zich op de prevalentie van buikpijn in een groep schoolkinderen uit de algemene populatie, en op de relatie tussen buikpijn en andere lichamelijke en psychische klachten bij deze schoolkinderen. Ook wordt in dit hoofdstuk onderzocht of buikpijn bij schoolkinderen samenhangt met lichamelijke en psychische klachten van ouders, zoals vaak wordt gevonden bij kinderen met FAP. Een grote groep kinderen tussen de 7 en 18 jaar vulden hiertoe vragenlijsten in (N=665), en bij 391 van deze kinderen namen ook de ouders deel aan het onderzoek. Uit de resultaten van deze studie bleek dat buikpijn vaak voorkwam; 56.5% van de kinderen vertelden dat ze in de twee weken voorafgaand aan het invullen van de vragenlijst, minstens één keer buikpijn hadden gehad. De mate waarin kinderen buikpijn rapporteerden hing het sterkst samen met geslacht, leeftijd, beperkingen in het functioneren, andere lichamelijke klachten en depressieve klachten. Kinderen die meer buikpijn rapporteerden waren vaker van het vrouwelijk geslacht, waren jonger, en rapporteerden naast de buikpijn ook meer beperkingen in het functioneren, meer andere lichamelijke klachten en meer depressieve klachten. Er werd slechts een zwakke relatie tussen buikpijn en angstklachten gevonden, wat in strijd is met resultaten van studies die onderzoek doen bij kinderen met FAP (Campo et al., 2004). Lichamelijke en psychische klachten van ouders bleken geen relatie te vertonen met de klachten van het kind, en ook dit is in strijd met resultaten uit eerder onderzoek bij kinderen met FAP (Levy, 2011). Verder onderzoek hiernaar lijkt dan ook geboden.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 174 18-07-12 17:20

Page 178: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

10

Nederlandse samenvatting | 175

Invloed van oudersNaar de lichamelijke klachten van ouders van kinderen met FAP en de manier waarop ouders reageren op de buikpijnklachten van het kind, werd verder onderzoek gedaan in een systematische review en meta-analyse beschreven in hoofdstuk 3. Het doel van deze meta-analyse was om te onderzoeken of ouders de buikpijnklachten van hun kind mogelijk in stand houden of verergeren via de psychologische processen modeling en bekrachtigen van ziektegedrag. Modeling verwijst in dit geval naar de theorie dat kinderen de buikpijn ‘aan kunnen leren’ van hun ouders indien ouders zelf ook veel lichamelijke klachten hebben. Het bekrachtigen van ziektegedrag verwijst naar het idee dat ouders de klachten van hun kind mogelijk in stand houden door onhandig ziektegedrag van het kind te belonen (bijvoorbeeld door het kind bij pijn thuis te laten blijven van school en te vertroetelen). Uit de gevonden artikelen bleek dat ouders van kinderen met FAP meer lichamelijke klachten rapporteren dan ouders van gezonde kinderen, maar het verschil tussen deze twee groepen was slechts klein. Bovendien waren alle gevonden studies ofwel cross-sectioneel ofwel retrospectief van design, waardoor er geen oorzaak-gevolg relatie kon worden aangetoond. Over de vraag of ouders onhandig ziektegedrag van het kind bekrachtigen, waren slechts weinig studies van voldoende kwaliteit gepubliceerd. Er kon dan ook geen meta-analyse uitgevoerd worden op deze beperkte gegevens. De helft van de studies die wél naar ouderlijke reacties keken, vond geen verband met de klachten van het kind. Concluderend kan worden gezegd dat er te weinig bewijs is in de bestaande literatuur om vast te kunnen stellen of ouders de klachten van hun kinderen in stand houden of verergeren. Emotioneel bewustzijn en copingIn de literatuur wordt gesuggereerd dat patiënten met medisch onverklaarde klachten problemen hebben met het bewust ervaren van emoties en met het adaptief omgaan met dagelijkse spanningen (coping), en dat deze problemen gerelateerd zouden zijn aan de etiologie van de klachten. Hoofdstuk 4 beschrijft een onderzoek waarin gekeken werd naar hoe bewust kinderen met FAP zich zijn van hun eigen emoties, en hoe zij omgaan met dagelijkse spanningen. Daarbij werd een vergelijking gemaakt tussen het emotioneel bewustzijn en de coping vaardigheden van kinderen met FAP, kinderen zonder buikpijn en kinderen die af en toe buikpijn ervaren. Bovendien werd onderzocht of de manier waarop kinderen omgaan met dagelijkse problemen, de relatie tussen emotioneel bewustzijn en buikpijn medieert. Uit de resultaten bleek dat zoals verwacht, kinderen met FAP zich minder goed bewust waren van hun emoties dan kinderen zonder buikpijn. De verschillen waren echter slechts klein, en kinderen met FAP verschilden vrijwel niet van kinderen die af en toe buikpijn rapporteerden. Bovendien lieten de resultaten zien dat, in tegenstelling tot wat we

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 175 23-07-12 15:53

Page 179: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

176 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

hadden verwacht, kinderen met FAP beter waren in het erkennen van een relatie tussen emoties en bijbehorende lichamelijke sensaties. De resultaten voor coping lieten zien dat kinderen met FAP vaker problemen vermijden dan gezonde kinderen, maar coping was geen overtuigende mediator in de relatie tussen bewustzijn van emoties en FAP. Over het geheel genomen bleken kinderen met FAP wat betreft hun bewustzijn van emoties en coping vaardigheden slechts in beperkte mate te verschillen van kinderen zonder buikpijn, en zij verschilden vrijwel niet van kinderen die af en toe buikpijn ervaren.

Gedachten en gedrag van ouders en kinderenOnze vierde studie, beschreven in hoofdstuk 5, richtte zich op de manier waarop kinderen en hun ouders over de buikpijnklachten denken en met de buikpijn omgaan. Dergelijke gedachten en manieren van coping worden namelijk vaak aangepakt tijdens cognitieve gedragstherapie, maar het is tot op heden nog niet duidelijk welke van deze factoren de sterkste relatie met FAP vertoont en dus wellicht het meest belangrijk is om tijdens de behandeling te veranderen. Daarbij werd specifiek onderzocht of de rol van het gedrag en de gedachten van kinderen of dat van ouders sterker gerelateerd was aan a) het hebben van FAP, en b) het fysieke en psychische welbevinden van kinderen met FAP. De gegevens van kinderen met FAP en hun ouders werden hiertoe vergeleken met de gegevens van schoolkinderen die af en toe buikpijn hadden. Beide groepen vulden vragenlijsten in betreffende negatieve gedachten over de ernst van de klachten van het kind, positieve gedachten over de mogelijkheden van het kind om het hoofd te bieden aan de pijn, coping met de buikpijn en het bekrachtigen van onhandig ziektegedrag door de ouders. De analyses lieten zien dat zowel gedachten en gedrag van het kind, als gedachten van de ouder gerelateerd waren aan het hebben van FAP; de relatie tussen gedachten van de ouder en het hebben van FAP bleek echter het sterkst. Daarentegen waren de gedachten en het gedrag van het kind juist sterker gerelateerd aan het fysiek en psychisch welbevinden van het kind dan de gedachten en het gedrag van de ouders. Onhandige, bekrachtigende reacties van ouders op de buikpijn van het kind waren niet gerelateerd aan het hebben van FAP, noch aan het welbevinden van kinderen met FAP. Positieve gedachten en coping van het kind waren sterker gerelateerd aan het hebben van FAP en het welzijn van de kinderen dan negatieve gedachten van het kind over de klachten. Selectieve aandachtTenslotte werd in het laatste hoofdstuk van dit deel van het proefschrift, hoofdstuk 6, onderzocht of kinderen met FAP selectief meer aandacht besteden aan de activiteit van hun lichaam. In de literatuur wordt namelijk verondersteld dat een dergelijke aandachtsbias ook een rol zou kunnen spelen in de etiologie van FAP. Daarnaast werd, in overeenstemming met de symptom perception hypothesis van Pennebaker (1982), onderzocht of kinderen nadat zij geconfronteerd werden met informatie over hun lichaamsactiviteit, meer

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 176 18-07-12 17:20

Page 180: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

10

Nederlandse samenvatting | 177

lichamelijke sensaties zouden waarnemen. Dertig kinderen met FAP en dertig gezonde kinderen vulden hiertoe vragenlijsten in en maakten een taak op de computer, een dot-

probe detectie taak, waarin zij plaatjes te zien kregen die zogenaamd de activiteit van hun lichaam weergaven. Omdat het voor het onderzoek naar de aanwezigheid van een aandachtsbias niet nodig was om de lichaamsactiviteit daadwerkelijk te meten, werd slechts gedaan alsof de kinderen echt de activiteit van bijvoorbeeld hun hart (hartslag) en buik (bewegingen van de darmen) te zien kregen, terwijl dit niet het geval was. De resultaten lieten geen aandachtsbias zien voor informatie over de activiteit van de buik, maar kinderen met FAP vertoonden wel langzamere reactietijden dan gezonde kinderen wanneer zij informatie te zien kregen over hun buik. Alle kinderen, zowel kinderen met FAP als gezonde kinderen, richtten hun aandacht weg van informatie over hun hartslag. Dertig tot veertig procent van de kinderen met FAP rapporteerde enige toename van buikpijn na afloop van het experiment, wat suggereert dat het richten van de aandacht op lichamelijke sensaties de perceptie van symptomen kan vergroten. Over het geheel genomen verschilden kinderen met FAP alleen van gezonde kinderen in hoe zij reageerden op informatie over de activiteit van hun buik, niet in hoe zij reageerden op informatie over andere lichaamsactiviteit. Conclusie deel 1In het eerste deel van dit proefschrift is onderzocht welke psychologische en sociale factoren samenhangen met het hebben van FAP en het welzijn van kinderen met FAP. Hieruit bleek dat de manier waarop kinderen omgaan met hun buikpijn, de manier waarop kinderen en ouders denken over de pijn, en selectieve aandacht voor lichamelijke sensaties mogelijk factoren zijn die een rol kunnen spelen in de etiologie van FAP. Voor de relevantie van emotioneel bewustzijn, bekrachtigende reacties van ouders en coping met alledaagse problemen werd echter geen overtuigend bewijs gevonden. Daarbij moet opgemerkt worden dat veel voorgaand onderzoek, inclusief het huidige onderzoek, cross-sectioneel of retrospectief van aard is, waardoor er geen uitspraken gedaan kunnen worden over oorzaak-gevolg relaties. Meer longitudinale en experimentele studies zijn nodig om te kunnen bepalen of de onderzochte psychosociale factoren daadwerkelijk de klachten in stand houden, verergeren of zelfs veroorzaken.

Deel 2: Cognitieve gedragstherapie voor de behandelingvan functionele buikpijn bij kinderen

Effectiviteit van cognitieve gedragstherapieHet tweede doel van dit proefschrift was om de effectiviteit van cognitieve gedragstherapie (CGT) voor de behandeling van kinderen met FAP te onderzoeken. CGT

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 177 18-07-12 17:20

Page 181: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

178 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

is een veelbelovende behandeling voor kinderen en jongeren met FAP, maar overtuigend bewijs voor de effectiviteit van deze behandeling ontbreekt. In hoofdstuk 7 worden de resultaten beschreven van een gerandomiseerd onderzoek, waarbij de effectiviteit van een zes sessies durend CGT protocol werd vergeleken met de effectiviteit van zes wekelijkse bezoeken aan een kinderarts (medische zorg; MC). Gedurende drie jaar werden 104 kinderen at random toegewezen aan de CGT of MC behandeling. CGT werd gegeven door master studenten psychologie, die training en supervisie ontvingen van een ervaren psychotherapeute. MC werd voor het grootste deel gegeven door ervaren kinderartsen of kindergastroenterologen, en in een aantal gevallen door arts-assistenten die supervisie ontvingen van een ervaren kinderarts. Uit deze studie bleek dat zowel CGT als MC tot een significante verbetering van de klachten leidde. De effectiviteit van de twee behandelingen verschilde niet van elkaar. Een jaar na afloop van de behandeling rapporteerde ruim 60% van de kinderen die CGT hadden gevolgd, significant minder of geen buikpijn. Dit percentage was 56% voor de kinderen die werden behandeld middels MC. Direct na de behandeling en een half jaar na afloop van de behandeling rapporteerden de kinderen die CGT hadden gekregen significant minder angstige en depressieve klachten dan de kinderen die door de arts waren behandeld. Dit verschil was echter een jaar na de behandeling weer verdwenen; op dat tijdstip rapporteerden alle kinderen minder angst en depressie dan voorafgaand aan de behandeling, onafhankelijk van welke behandeling zij hadden gevolgd. Aangezien twee verschillende behandelingen tot zulke vergelijkbare resultaten leidden, is de vraag wat de werkzame elementen zijn van beide behandelingen. Een deel van deze vergelijkbare effectiviteit kan waarschijnlijk verklaard worden door non-specifieke therapie effecten, zoals het hebben van een goede band met een hulpverlener. Een belangrijke richting voor vervolgstudies is om te onderzoeken welk percentage van het behandeleffect te wijten is aan de specifieke technieken die gebruikt worden tijdens CGT.

Moderatoren en mediatoren van het behandeleffect van CGTEen eerste stap hiertoe werd gemaakt in hoofdstuk 8, waarin exploratief werd onderzocht hoe het effect van CGT verklaard kan worden. Hierbij werd onderscheid gemaakt tussen moderatoren – eigenschappen van het kind of de omgeving die het behandeleffect kunnen beïnvloeden – en mediatoren – factoren die veranderen door de behandeling, en die vervolgens leidden tot een afname van buikpijn. De moderatoren die werden onderzocht, waren leeftijd, geslacht, subdiagnose van FAP, duur van de klachten, angst, depressie, bewustzijn van emoties, coping met dagelijkse problemen, functionele darmklachten van ouders, en ouderlijke stress, angst, depressie en somatisatie. Onderzochte mediatoren waren gedachten van kind en ouder over de buikpijn, coping van kinderen met de buikpijn, bekrachtigende reacties van ouders op de buikpijn van het kind, en gevoeligheid voor pijn.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 178 18-07-12 17:20

Page 182: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

10

Nederlandse samenvatting | 179

De resultaten lieten zien dat kinderen die voorafgaand aan de behandeling gemakkelijker hun emoties toonden aan anderen en vaker sociale steun zochten voor hun problemen, meer baat hadden bij CGT (ongeveer 80% effectiviteit) dan kinderen die laag scoorden op deze variabelen (44% effectiviteit). Wat betreft de mediatoren werd gevonden dat het behandeleffect van zowel CGT als MC samenhing met een afname van negatieve gedachten van het kind over de buikpijn. Daarnaast hing een toename in positieve gedachten van het kind over zijn/haar coping vaardigheden met de buikpijn, samen met een groter behandeleffect van CGT, maar niet van MC. Het behandeleffect van CGT bleek ook samen te hangen met een afname in bekrachtigend gedrag van de ouder. Concluderend kunnen we zeggen dat CGT effectiever is wanneer kinderen gemakkelijker hun emoties tonen aan anderen en meer geneigd zijn om steun te zoeken voor hun problemen. Mogelijke werkingsmechanismen voor CGT zijn het veranderen van positieve en negatieve cognities van het kind, en het verminderen van bekrachtigend gedrag van de ouder.

Conclusie

In onze poging een aantal belangrijke onderzoeksvragen te beantwoorden, zijn er veel nieuwe vragen opgeworpen. Wat echter duidelijk is geworden, is dat kinderen met FAP weldegelijk effectief behandeld kunnen worden met een kortdurende psychologische behandeling. Hoewel er meer onderzoek nodig is naar de werkingsmechanismen van CGT om de behandeling te verbeteren, is gebleken dat het naar huis sturen van deze kinderen met de boodschap dat ze “er maar mee moeten leren leven” geen recht doet aan wat we hen kunnen bieden. Verder longitudinaal en experimenteel onderzoek naar risicofactoren voor FAP en onderzoek naar de werkingsmechanismen van behandelingen is nodig om kinderen met FAP in de toekomst nog beter te kunnen helpen.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 179 18-07-12 17:20

Page 183: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 180 18-07-12 17:20

Page 184: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

References

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 181 18-07-12 17:20

Page 185: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

182 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

References marked with an asterisk are included in the meta-analysis and systematic review described in chapter 3.

Abidin, R. R. (1983). The parenting stress index: Form 6. Charlottesville, VA: University of Virginia, Institute of Clinical Psychology.

Alfvén, G. (1993). The covariation of common psychosomatic symptoms among children from socioeconomically differing residential areas: an epidemiological study. Acta Paediatrica, 82, 484-487.

Alfvén, G. (2003). One hundred cases of recurrent abdominal pain in children: diagnostic procedures and criteria fo a psychosomatic diagnosis. Acta Paediatrica, 92, 43-49.

Alpers, D. H. (2008). Multidimensionality of symptom complexes in irritable bowel syndrome and other functional gastrointestinal disorders. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 64, 567-572.

Anderson, J. L., Acra, S., Bruehl, S., & Walker, L. S. (2008). Relation between clinical symptoms and experimental visceral hypersensitivity in pediatric patients with functional abdominal pain. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 47, 309-315.

Apley, J. & Hale, B. (1973). Children with recurrent abdominal pain: How do they grow up? British Medical Journal, 7, 7-9.

Apley, J. & Naish, N. (1958). Recurrent abdominal pains: a field survey of 1000 school children. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 33, 165-170.

Arrindell, W. A. & Ettema, J. H. M. (2003). Symptom Checklist. Handleiding bij een multidimensionele psychopathologie-indicator. Lisse: Swets & Zeitlinger B.V.

Asmundson, G. J. G., Wright, K. D., & Hadjistavropoulos, H. D. (2005). Hypervigilance and attentional fixedness in chronic musculoskeletal pain: Consistency of findings across modified stroop and dot-probe tasks. The Journal of Pain, 6, 497-506.

Ayers, T. A. & Sandler, I. N. (2000). Manual for the Children’s Coping Strategies Checklist & How I Coped Under Pressure Scale. Retrieved June 2010 at http://prc.asu.edu/docs/CCSC-HICUPS%20%20Manual2.pdf

Baber, K. F., Anderson, J., Puzanovova, M., & Walker, L. S. (2008). Rome II versus Rome III classification of functional gastrointestinal disorders in pediatric chronic abdominal pain. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 47, 299-302.

Bagby, R. M., Parker, J. D. A., & Taylor, G. J. (1994). The twenty-item Toronto alexithymia scale-I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 38, 23-32.

Bahar, R. J., Collins, B. S., Steinmetz, B., & Ament, M. E. (2008). Double-blinded placebo-controlled trial of amitriptyline for the treatment of irritable bowel syndrome in adolescents. The Journal of Pediatrics, 152, 685-689.

Bakker, M. J., Boer, F., Benninga, M. A., Koelman, J. H., & Tijssen, M. A. (2009). Increased auditory startle reflex in children with functional abdominal pain. Journal of Pediatrics, 156, 285-291.

Bakker, M. J., Tijssen, M. A., van der Meer, J. N., Koelman, J. H., & Boer, F. (2009). Increased whole-body startle reflex and autonomic reactivity in children with anxiety disorders. Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 34, 314-322.

Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice-Hall.Bar-Haim, Y., Lamy, D., Pergamin, L., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & Van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2007). Threat-related

attentional bias in anxious and nonanxious individuals: A meta-analytic study. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 1-24.

Baron, R. M. & Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: Conceptual, strategic, and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173-1182.

Beck, J. E., Lipani, T. A., Baber, K. F., Dufton, L., Garber, J., Smith, C. A. et al. (2011). Attentional bias to pain and social threat in pediatric patients with functional abdominal pain and pain-free youth before and after performance evaluation. Pain,152, 1061-1067.

Bentler, P. M. & Bonett, D. G. (1980). Significance tests and goodness of fit in the analysis of covariance structures. Psychological Bulletin, 88, 588-606.

Berger, M. Y., Gieteling, M. J., & Benninga, M. A. (2007). Chronic abdominal pain in children. British Medical Journal, 334, 997-1002.

Berrocal, C., Pennato, T., & Bernini, O. (2009). Relating coping, fear of uncertainty and alexithymia with psychological distress: the mediating role of experiential avoidance. Journal of Cognitive and Behavioral Psychotherapies, 9, 149-159.

Blanchard, E. B., Lackner, J. M., Gusmano, R., Gudleski, G. D., Sanders, K., Keefer, L. et al. (2006). Prediction of treatment outcome among patients with irritable bowel syndrome treated with group cognitive therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 317-337.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 182 18-07-12 17:20

Page 186: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

References | 183

Bode, G., Brenner, H., Adler, G., & Rothenbacher, D. (2003). Recurrent abdominal pain in children: evidence from a population-based study that social and familial factors play a major role but not Helicobacter pylori infection. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 54, 417-421.

Bonilla, S., Deli, W., & Saps, M. (2011). The prognostic value of obtaining a negative endoscopy in children with functional gastrointestinal disorders. Clinical Pediatrics, 50, 396-401.

Bonner, M. J. & Finney, J. W. (1996). A psychosocial model of children’s health status. In Advances in clinical child psychology (pp. 231-282). New York: Plenum Press Div Plenum Publishing Corp.

Bosch, J. D. & Slot, W. (1998). Buikpijn bij kinderen: onderzoek en theoretische opvattingen. In J. D. Bosch, H. A. Bosma, R. J. Van der Gaag, A. J. J. M. Ruijssenaars, & A. Vyt (Eds.), Jaarboek Onwikkelingspsychologie, orthopedagogiek en kinderpsychiatrie 3 1998-1999 (pp. 1-20). Houten Diegem: Bohn Stafleu Van Loghum.

Boyer, M. C., Compas, B. E., Stanger, C., Colletti, R. B., Konik, B. S., Morrow, S. B. et al. (2006). Attentional bias to pain and social threat in children with recurrent abdominal pain. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 31, 209-220.

Boyle, J. T. (1997). Recurrent abdominal pain: an update. Pediatrics in Review, 18, 310-320.Brown, T. A. (2006). Confirmatory factor analysis for applied research. New York: The Guilford Press.Bruijnzeels, M. A., van Suijlekom-Smit, L. W. A., Van der Velden, J., & van der Wouden, J. C. (1993). The child in

general practice. Dutch national survey of morbidity and interventions in general practice. Rotterdam: Universiteitsdrukkerij Erasmus Universiteit.

Buonavolonta, R., Coccorullo, P., Turco, R., Boccia, G., Greco, L., & Staiano, A. (2010). Familial aggregation in children affected by Functional Gastrointestinal Disorders. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 50, 500-505.

Campo, J. V. (2012). Annual research review: Functional somatic symptoms and associated anxiety and depression - developmental psychopathology in pediatric practice. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 575-592.

* Campo, J. V., Bridge, J., Ehmann, M., Altman, S., Lucas, A., Birmaher, B. et al. (2004). Recurrent abdominal pain, anxiety, and depression in primary care. Pediatrics, 113, 817-824.

Campo, J. V., Bridge, J., Lucas, A., Savorelli, S., Walker, L., Di, L. C. et al. (2007). Physical and emotional health of mothers of youth with functional abdominal pain. Archives of Pediatrics and Adolescence Medicine, 161, 131-137.

Campo, J. V., Di Lorenzo, C., Chiappetta, L., Bridge, J., Colborn, D. K., Gartner, J. C. et al. (2001). Adult outcomes of pediatric recurrent abdominal pain: Do they just grow out of it? Pediatrics, 108.

Cardol, M., van den Bosch, W. J. H. M., Spreeuwenberg, P., Groenewegen, P. P., van Dijk, L., & de Bakker, D. H. (2006). All in the family: Headaches and abdominal pain as indicators for consultation patterns in families. Annals of Family Medicine, 4, 506-511.

Chitkara, D. K., Delgado-Aros, S., Bredenoord, A. J., Cremonini, F., El-Youssef, M., Freese, D. et al. (2003). Functional dyspepsia, upper gastrointestinal symptoms, and transit in children. Journal of Pediatrics, 143, 609-613.

Chitkara, D. K., Rawat, D. J., & Talley, N. J. (2005). The epidemiology of childhood recurrent abdominal pain in western countries: A systematic review. American Journal of Gastroenterology, 100, 1868-1875.

Christensen, M. & Mortensen, O. (1975). Long-term prognosis in children with recurrent abdominal pain. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 50, 110-115.

Claar, R. L. & Walker, L. S. (2006). Functional assessment of pediatric pain patients: Psychometric properties of the Functional Disability Inventory. Pain, 121, 77-84.

Claar, R. L., Walker, L. S., & Smith, C. A. (1999). Functional disability in adolescents and young adults with symptoms of irritable bowel syndrome: The role of academic, social, and athletic competence. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 24, 271-280.

Clark, D. M. & Fairburn, C. G. (1997). Science and practice of cognitive behaviour therapy. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Cohen, J. (1977). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Revised edition. Orlando, Florida: Academic Press, Inc.

Compas, B. E., Boyer, M. C., Stanger, C., Colletti, R. B., Thomsen, A. H., Dufton, L. M. et al. (2006). Latent variable analysis of coping, anxiety/depression, and somatic symptoms in adolescents with chronic pain. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 1132-1142.

Compas, B. E., Connor-Smith, J. K., Saltzman, H., Harding Thomson, A., & Wadsworth, M. E. (2001). Coping with stress during childhood and adolescence: Problems, progress, and potential in theory and research. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 87-127.

Coutinho, S. V., Plotsky, P. M., Sablad, M., Miller, J. C., Zhou, H., I Bayati, A. et al. (2002). Neonatal maternal seperation alters stress-induced responses to viscerosomatic nociceptive stimuli in rat. American Journal of Physiology: Gastrointestinal and Liver Physiology, 282, G307-G316.

Craig, T. K. J., Cox, A. D., & Klein, K. (2002). Intergenerational transmission of somatization behaviour: a study of chronic somatizers and their children. Psychological Medicine, 32, 805-816.

Crane, C. & Martin, M. (2004). Social learning, affective state and passive coping in irritable bowel syndrome and inflammatory bowel disease. General Hospital Psychiatry, 26, 50-58.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 183 18-07-12 17:20

Page 187: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

184 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Croffie, J. M., Fitzgerald, J. F., & Chong, S. K. F. (2000). Recurrent abdominal pain in children-A retrospective study of outcome in a group referred to a pediatric gastroenterology practice. Clinical Pediatrics, 39, 267-274.

Crombez, G., Bijttebier, P., Eccleston, C., Mascagni, T., Mertens, G., Goubert, L. et al. (2003). The child version of the pain catastrophizing scale (PCS-C): a preliminary validation. Pain, 104, 639-646.

Crushell, E., Rowland, M., Doherty, M., Gormally, S., Harty, S., Bourke, B. et al. (2003). Importance of parental conceptual model of illness in severe recurrent abdominal pain. Pediatrics, 112, 1368-1372.

* Czyzewski, D. I., Eakin, M. N., Lane, M. M., Jarrett, M., & Shulman, R. J. (2007). Recurrent abdominal pain in primary and tertiary care: Differences and similarities. Childrens Health Care, 36, 137-153.

De Boo, G. M. & Wicherts, J. (2009). Assessing cognitive and behavioral coping strategies in children. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 33, 1-20.

De Brock, A. J. L. L., Vermulst, A. A., Gerris, J. R., & Abadin, R. R. (1992). NOSI Nijmeegse Ouderlijke Stress Index. Meetinstrument voor de vaststelling van stress bij opvoeders. Een uitgebreide versie (NOSI) voor psychodiagnostische doeleinden en een verkorte versie (NOSIK) voor signaleringsdoeleinden [NOSI Nijmeegse Ouderlijke Stress Index. An instrument to measure parental stress. An extensive version (NOSI) for diagnostic purposes and an abbreviated version (NOSIK) for screening purposes]. Lisse, The Netherlands: Swetz & Zeitlinger.

De Gucht, V., Fischler, B., & Heiser, W. (2004). Neuroticism, alexithymia, negative affect, and positive affect as determinants of medically unexplained symptoms. Personality and Individual Differences, 36, 1655-1667.

De Gucht, V. & Heiser, W. (2003). Alexithymia and somatisation. A quantitative review of the literature. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 54, 425-434.

Dear, B. F., Sharpe, L., Nicholas, M. K., & Refshauge, K. (2011). The psychometric properties of the dot-probe paradigm when used in pain-related attentional bias research. The Journal of Pain, 12, 1247-1254.

Dengler-Crish, C. M., Horst, S. N., & Walker, L. S. (2011). Somatic complaints in childhood functional abdominal pain are associated with functional gastrointestinal disorders in adolescence and adulthood. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 52, 162-165.

Derogatis, L. R. (1977). Administration, scoring and procedures manual-1 for the r(evised) version. Baltimore: Clinical psychometric research.

Dhroove, G., Chogle, A., & Saps, M. (2010). A million-dollar work-up for abdominal pain: is it worth it? Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 51, 579-583.

Di Lorenzo, C. (2005). Abdominal pain: Is it in the gut or in the head? Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 41, S44-S46.

Di Lorenzo, C., Colletti, R. B., Lehmann, H. P., Boyle, J. T., Gerson, W. T., Hyams, J. S. et al. (2005). Chronic abdominal pain in children. Pediatrics, 115, 812-815.

Di Lorenzo, C., Youssef, N. N., Sigurdsson, L., Scharff, L., Griffiths, J., & Wald, A. (2001). Visceral hyperalgesia in children with functional abdominal pain. Journal of Pediatrics, 139, 838-843.

Dorn, L. D., Campo, J. C., Thato, S., Dahl, R. E., Lewin, D., Chandra, R. et al. (2003). Psychological comorbidity and stress reactivity in children and adolescents with recurrent abdominal pain and anxiety disorders. Journal of the American Acadamy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 42, 66-75.

Drossman, D. (2011). Abuse, trauma, and GI illness: Is there a link? Gastroenterology, 106, 14-25.Drossman, D. A. (1998). Presidential address: Gastrointestinal illness and the biopsychosocial model. Psychosomatic

Medicine, 60, 258-267.Drossman, D. A., Leserman, J., Nachman, G., Li, Z., Gluck, H., Toomey, T. C. et al. (1990). Sexual and physical abuse

in women with functional or organic gastrointestinal disorders. Annals of Internal Medicine, 113, 828-833.Duarte, M. E., Penna, F. J., Andrade, E. M. G., Cancela, C. S. P., Neto, J. C. A., & Barbosa, T. F. (2006). Treatment of

nonorganic recurrent abdominal pain: Cognitive-behavioral family intervention. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 43, 59-64.

Dufton, L. M., Dunn, M. J., & Compas, B. E. (2009). Anxiety and somatic complaints in children with recurrent abdominal pain and anxiety disorders. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 34, 176-186.

Eccleston, C. (1995). The attentional control of pain: methodological and theoretical concerns. Pain, 63, 3-10.Eccleston, C. & Crombez, G. (1999). Pain demands attention: A cognitive-affective model of the interruptive

function of pain. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 356-366.El-Matary, W., Spray, C., & Sandhu, B. (2004). Irritable bowel syndrome: the commonest cause of recurrent

abdominal pain in children. European Journal of Pediatrics, 163, 584-588.Eminson, M., Benjamin, S., Shortall, A., Woods, T., & Faragher, B. (1996). Physical symptoms and illness attitudes in

adolescents: An epidemiological study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 37, 519-528.Fekkes, M., Pijpers, F. I. M., Fredricks, A. M., Vogels, T., & Verloove-Vanhorick, S. P. (2006). Do bullied children get ill,

or do ill children get bullied? A prospective cohort study on the relationship between bullying and health-related symptoms. Pediatrics, 117, 1568-1574.

Field, A. (2005). Discovering statistics using SPSS. London: Sage Publications Ltd.Fowkes, F. G. & Fulton, P. M. (1991). Critical appraisal of published research: introductory guidelines. British Medical

Journal, 1136-1140.Francavilla, R., Miniello, V., Magista, A. M., De Canio, A., Bucci, N., Gagliardi, F. et al. (2010). A randomized controlled

trial of Lactobacillus GG in children with functional abdominal pain. Pediatrics, 126, e1445-e1452.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 184 18-07-12 17:20

Page 188: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

References | 185

Friedberg, F. & Quick, J. (2007). Alexithymia in chronic fatigue syndrome: Associations with momentary, recall, and retrospective measures of somatic complaints and emotions. Psychosomatic Medicine, 69, 54-60.

Gale, C. R., Batty, G. D., & Deary, I. J. (2008). Locus of control at age 10 years and health outcomes and behaviors at age 30 years: The 1970 british cohort study. Psychosomatic Medicine, 70, 394-403.

Gallese, V., Eagle, M. N., & Migone, P. (2005). Intentional attunement: Mirror neurons and the neural underpinnings of interpersonal relations. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 55, 131-176.

Galli, F., D’Antuono, G., Tarantino, S., Viviano, F., Borrelli, O., Chirumbolo, A. et al. (2007). Headache and recurrent abdominal pain: a controlled study by the means of the Child Behaviour Checklist (CBCL). Cephalalgia, 27, 211-219.

Garber, J., Walker, L. S., & Zeman, J. (1991). Somatization symptoms in a community sample of children and adolescents: Further validation of the Children’s Somatization Inventory. Psychological Assessment, 3, 588-595.

* Garber, J., Zeman, J., & Walker, L. S. (1990). Recurrent abdominal pain in children: psychiatric diagnoses and parental psychopathology. Journal of the American Acadamy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 29, 648-656.

Geeraerts, B., Van Oudenhove, L., Fischler, B., Vandenberghe, J., Caenepeel, P., Janssens, J. et al. (2009). Influence of abuse history on gastric sensorimotor function in functional dyspepsia. Neurogastroenterology and Motility, 21, 33-41.

Ghandour, R. M., Overpeck, M. D., Huang, Z. J., Kogan, M. D., & Scheidt, P. C. (2004). Headache, stomachache, backache, and morning fatigue among adolescent girls in the United States; associations with behavioral, sociodemographic, and environmental factors. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 158, 797-803.

Ghys, A. & Meesters, C. (1993). Dutch translation of the Children’s Somatization Inventory. Maastricht, The Netherlands: Academic Hospital Maastricht, Department of Medical Psychology.

Gieteling, M. J., Bierma-Zeinstra, S. M. A., Passchier, J., & Berger, M. Y. (2008). Prognosis of chronic or recurrent abdominal pain in children. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 47, 316-326.

Gieteling, M. J., Bierma-Zeinstra, S. M. A., Van Leeuwen, Y., Passchier, J., & Berger, M. Y. (2011). Prognostic factors for persistence of chronic abdominal pain in children: A systematic review. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 52, 154-161.

Gijsbers, C. F. M., Kneepkens, C. M. F., Schweizer, J. J., Benninga, M. A., & Buller, H. A. (2011). Recurrent abdominal pain in 200 children: somatic causes and diagnostic criteria. Acta Paediatrica, 100, e208-e214.

Goubert, L., Vervoort, T., Cano, A., & Crombez, G. (2009). Catastrophizing about their children’s pain is related to higher parent-child congruency in pain ratings: an experimental investigation. European Journal of Pain, 13, 196-201.

Groholt, E. K., Stigum, H., Nordhagen, R., & Kohler, L. (2003). Recurrent pain in children, socio-economic factors and accumulation in families. European Journal of Epidemiology, 18, 965-975.

Gross, M. & Warschburger, P. (2012). Evaluation of a cognitive-behavioral pain management program for children with chronic abdominal pain: A randomized controlled study. International Journal of Behavioral Medicine, doi: 10.1007/s12529-012-9228-3.

Gulewitsch, M. D., Enck, P., Hautzinger, M., & Schlarb, A. A. (2011). Irritable bowel syndrome symptoms among German students: prevalence, characteristics, and associations to somatic complaints, sleep, quality of life, and childhood abdominal pain. European Journal of Gastroenterology and Hepatology, 23, 311-316.

Halac, U., Noble, A., & Faure, C. (2010). Rectal sensory threshold for pain is a diagnostic marker of irritable bowel syndrome and functional abdominal pain in children. The Journal of Pediatrics, 156, 60-65.

Harel, O. (2009). The estimation of R2 and adjusted R2 in incomplete data sets using multiple imputation. Journal of Applied Statistics, 36, 1109-1118.

Harter, S. (1986). Cognitive-developmental processes in the integration of concepts about emotions and the self. Social Cognition, 4, 119-151.

Helgeland, H., Flagstad, G., Grotta, J., Vandvik, P. O., Kristensen, H., & Markestad, T. (2009). Diagnosing pediatric functional abdominal pain in children (4-15 years old) according to the Rome III criteria: Results from a Norwegian prospective study. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 49, 309-315.

Helgeland, H., Sandvik, L., Mathiesen, K. S., & Kristensen, H. (2010). Childhood predictors of recurrent abdominal pain in adolescence: A 13-year population-based prospective study. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 68, 359-367.

Hicks, C. L., von Baeyer, C. L., & McGrath, P. J. (2008). Online psychological treatment for pediatric recurrent pain: A randomized evaluation. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 31, 724-736.

Higgins, J. P. T. & Thompson, S. G. (2004). Controlling the risk of spurious findings from meta-regression. Statistics in Medicine, 23, 1663-1682.

Hirsch, C., John, M. T., Schaller, H.-G., & Türp, J. C. (2006). Pain-related impairment and health care utilization in children and adolescents: A comparison of orofacial pain with abdominal pain, back pain and headache. Quintessence International, 37, 381-390.

Hommel, K. A., McGraw, K. L., Ammerman, R. T., Heubi, J. E., Hansen, M., Dunlap, E. et al. (2010). Psychosocial functioning in children and adolescents with gastrointestinal complaints and disorders. Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings, 17, 159-166.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 185 18-07-12 17:20

Page 189: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

186 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Horvath, A. O. & Symonds, B. D. (1991). Relation between working alliance and outcome in psychotherapy: A meta-analysis. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 38, 139-149.

* Hotopf, M., Carr, S., Mayou, R., Wadsworth, M., & Wessely, S. (1998). Why do children have chronic abdominal pain, and what happens to them when they grow up? Population based cohort study. British Medical Journal, 316, 1196-1200.

Howell, S., Poulton, R., & Talley, N. J. (2005). The natural history of childhood abdominal pain and its association with adult irritable bowel syndrome: Birth-cohort study. American Journal of Gastroenterology, 100, 2071-2078.

Huertas-Ceballos, A., Logan, S., Bennet, C., & Macarthur, C. (2009a). Dietary interventions for recurrent abdominal pain (RAP) and irritable bowel syndrome (IBS) in childhood. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, doi:10.1002/14651858.CD003019.pub2.

Huertas-Ceballos, A., Logan, S., Bennet, C., & Macarthur, C. (2009b). Pharmacological interventions for recurrent abdominal pain (RAP) and irritable bowel syndrome (IBS) in childhood. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003017.pub2.

Huertas-Ceballos, A., Logan, S., Bennet, C., & Macarthur, C. (2009c). Psychosocial interventions for recurrent abdominal pain (RAP) and irritable bowel syndrome (IBS) in childhood (Review). Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003014.pub2.

Humphreys, P. A. & Gevirtz, R. N. (2000). Treatment of recurrent abdominal pain: Components analysis of four treatment protocols. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 31, 47-51.

Hyams, J. S., Burke, G., Davis, P. M., Rzepski, B., & Andrulonis, P. A. (1996). Abdominal pain and irritable bowel syndrome in adelescents: A community-based study. The Journal of Pediatrics, 129, 220-226.

Hyams, J. S., Davis, P. M., Sylvester, F. A., Zeiter, D. K., Justinich, C. J., & Lerer, T. (2000). Dyspepsia in children and adolescents: a prospective study. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 30, 413-418.

Hyams, J. S., Treem, W., Justinich, C., Davis, P. M., Shoup, M., & Burke, G. (1995). Characterization of symptoms in children with recurrent abdominal pain: resemblance to irritable bowel syndrome. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 20, 209-214.

Hyman, P. E. & Cocjin, J. (2005). Colon motility during a panic attack. Psychosomatic Medicine, 67, 616-617.Iannotti, R. J., Schneider, S., Nansel, T. R., Haynie, D. L., Plotnick, L. P., Clark, L. M. et al. (2006). Self-efficacy, outcome

expactations, and diabetes self-management in adolescents with type 1 diabetes. Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 27, 98-105.

Jacobson, E. (1938). Progressive relaxation. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Jacobson, N. S. & Truax, P. (1991). Clinical significance: A statistical approach to defining meaningful change in

psychotherapy research. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 12-19.Jamison, R. N. & Walker, L. S. (1992). Illness behavior in children of chronic pain patients. International Journal of

Psychiatry Medicine, 22, 329-342.Jellesma, F. C., Faddegon, K., & Van der Veek, S. M. C. (2011). Children with somatic complaints: Do they show a

somatic attention bias? Netherlands Journal of Psychology, 66, 78-84.Jellesma, F. C., Rieffe, C., Terwogt, M. M., & Kneepkens, C. M. F. (2006). Somatic complaints and health care use in

children: Mood, emotion awareness and sense of coherence. Social Science & Medicine, 63, 2640-2648.Jellesma, F. C., Rieffe, C., Terwogt, M. M., & Westenberg, M. (2008). Do parents reinforce somatic complaints in their

children? Health Psychology, 27, 280-285.Jellesma, F. C., Rieffe, C., Terwogt, M. M., & Westenberg, M. (2009). Do I feel sadness, fear or both? Comparing self-

reported alexithymia and emotional task-performance in children with many or few somatic complaints. Psychology & Health, 24, 881-893.

Jones, M. P., Dilley, J. B., Drossman, D., & Crowell, M. D. (2006). Brain-gut connections in functional GI disorders: anatomic and physiologic relationships. Neurogastroenterology and Motility, 18, 91-103.

Jones, M. P., Schettler, A., Olden, K., & Crowell, M. D. (2004). Alexithymia and somatosensory amplification in functional dyspepsia. Psychosomatics, 45, 508-516.

Kaczynski, K. J., Claar, R. L., & Logan, D. E. (2009). Testing gender as a moderator of associations between psychosocial variables and functional disability in children and adolescents with chronic pain. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 34, 738-748.

Kaminski, A., Kamper, A., Thaler, K., Chapman, A., & Gartlehner, G. (2011). Antidepressants for the treatment of abdominal pain-related functional gastrointestinal disorders in children and adolescents. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews, doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD008013.pub2.

Kaminsky, L., Robertson, M., & Dewey, D. (2006). Psychological correlates of depression in children with recurrent abdominal pain. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 31, 956-966.

Kaptchuk, T. J., Kelley, J. M., Conboy, L. A., Davis, R. B., Kerr, C. E., Jacobson, E. E. et al. (2008). Components of placebo effect: randomised controlled trial in patients with irritable bowel syndrome. British Medical Journal, 336, 999-1003.

* Kaufman, K. L., Cromer, B., Daleiden, E. L., ZaronAqua, A., Aqua, K., Greeley, T. et al. (1997). Recurrent abdominal pain in adolescents: Psychosocial correlates of organic and nonorganic pain. Childrens Health Care, 26, 15-30.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 186 18-07-12 17:20

Page 190: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

References | 187

King, S., Chambers, C. T., Huguet, A., MacNevin, R. C., McGrath, P. J., Parker, L. et al. (2011). The epidemiology of chronic pain in children and adolescents revisited: A systematic review. Pain, 152, 2729-2738.

Kokkonen, J., Haapalahti, M., Tikkanen, S., Karttunen, R., & Savilahti, E. (2004). Gastrointestinal complaints and diagnosis in children: a population-based study. Acta Paediatrica, 93, 880-886.

Konijnenberg, A. Y., Uiterwaal, C. S. P. M., Kimpen, J. L. L., Van der Hoeven, J., Buitelaar, J. K., & De Graeff-Meeder, E. R. (2005). Children with unexplained chronic pain: substantial impairment in everyday life. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 90, 680-686.

Kooiman, C. G., Bolk, J. H., Rooijmans, H. G. M., & Trijsburg, R. W. (2004). Alexithymia does not predict the persistence of medically unexplained physical symptoms. Psychosomatic Medicine, 66, 224-232.

Kooiman, C. G., Spinhoven, P., & Trijsburg, R. W. (2002). The assessment of alexithymia. A critical review of the literature and a psychometric study of the Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 53, 1083-1090.

Kraemer, H. C., Wilson, G. T., Fairburn, C. G., & Agras, W. S. (2002). Mediators and moderators of treatment effects in randomized clinical trials. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 877-883.

Kramer, D. & Petermann, U. (2005). Psychosocial risk factors of idiopathic abdominal pain in children. Pravention und Rehabilitation, 17, 65-80.

Lackner, J. M., Gudleski, G. D., Keefer, L., Krasner, S. S., Powell, C., & Katz, L. A. (2010). Rapid response to cognitive behavior therapy predicts treatment outcome in patients with irritable bowel syndrome. Clinical Gastroenterology and Hepatology, 8, 426-432.

Lackner, J. M., Morley, S., Dowzer, C., Mesmer, C., & Hamilton, S. (2004). Psychological treatment for irritable bowel syndrome: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 72, 1100-1113.

Ladabaum, U., Sharabidze, A., Levin, T. R., Zhao, W. K., Chung, E., Bacchetti, P. et al. (2010). Citalopram provides little or no benefit in nondepressed patients with irritable bowel syndrome. Clinical Gastroenterology and Hepatology, 8, 42-48.

Lambert, M. J. & Ogles, B. M. (2009). Using clinical significance in psychotherapy outcome research: The need for a common procedure and validity data. Psychotherapy Research, 19, 493-501.

Lambie, J. A. & Marcel, A. J. (2002). Consciousness and the varieties of emotion experience: a theoretical framework. Psychological Review, 109, 219-259.

* Langer, S. L., Romano, J. M., Levy, R. L., Walker, L. S., & Whitehead, W. E. (2009). Catastrophizing and parental response to child symptom complaints. Children’s Health Care, 38, 169-184.

Langer, S. L., Walker, L. S., Romano, J. M., Whitehead, W. E., Feld, L., & Levy, R. L. (2007). Predictors of maternal responses to child abdominal pain. Children’s Health Care, 36, 63-81.

Larsen, J. K., Brand, N., Bermond, B., & Hijman, R. (2003). Cognitive and emotional characteristics of alexithymia. A review of neurobiological studies. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 54, 533-541.

Lautenbacher, S. (2010). Commentary to “Do words hurt? Brain activation during the processing of pain words” by Richter et al. Pain, 148, 179.

Lazarus, R. S. & Folkman, S. (1984). Stress, appraisal and coping. New York: Springer Publishing Company, Inc.Levy, R. L. (2011). Exploring the intergenerational transmission of illness behavior: From observations to

experimental intervention. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 41, 174-182.Levy, R. L., Jones, K. R., Whitehead, W. E., Feld, S. I., Talley, N. J., & Corey, L. A. (2001). Irritable bowel syndrome in

twins: heredity and social learning both contribute to etiology. Gastroenterology, 121, 799-804.Levy, R. L., Langer, S. L., Walker, L. S., Feld, L. D., & Whitehead, W. E. (2006). Relationship between the decision to take

a child to the clinic for abdominal pain and maternal psychological distress. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 160, 961-965.

* Levy, R. L., Langer, S. L., Walker, L. S., Romano, J. M., Christie, D. L., Youssef, N. et al. (2010). Cognitive-Behavioral Therapy for children with Functional Abdominal Pain and their parents decreases pain and other symptoms. The American Journal of Gastroenterology, doi:10.1038/ajg.2010.106.

Levy, R. L., Langer, S. L., & Whitehead, W. E. (2007). Social learning contributions to the etiology and treatment of functional abdominal pain and inflammatory bowel disease in children and adults. World Journal of Gastroenterology, 13, 2397-2403.

Levy, R. L., Whitehead, W. E., Von Korff, M. R., & Feld, A. D. (2000). Intergenerational transmission of gastrointestinal illness behavior. American Journal of Gastroenterology, 95, 451-456.

Levy, R. L., Whitehead, W. E., Walker, L. S., Von, K. M., Feld, A. D., Garner, M. et al. (2004). Increased somatic complaints and health-care utilization in children: effects of parent IBS status and parent response to gastrointestinal symptoms. American Journal of Gastroenterology, 99, 2442-2451.

* Liakopoulou-Kairis, M., Alifieraki, T., Protagora, D., Korpa, T., Kondyli, K., Dimosthenous, E. et al. (2002). Recurrent abdominal pain and headache. Psychopathology, life events and family functioning. European Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 11, 115-122.

Lindley, K. J., Glaser, D., & Milla, P. J. (2005). Consumerism in healthcare can be detrimental to child health: Lessons from children with functional abdominal pain. Archives of Disease in Childhood, 90, 335-337.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 187 18-07-12 17:20

Page 191: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

188 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Lipani, T. A. & Walker, L. S. (2006). Children’s appraisal and coping with pain: relation to maternal ratings of worry and restriction in family activities. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 31, 667-673.

Lipsey, M. W. & Wilson, D. B. (2001). Practical meta-analysis. Thousand Oaks, California: Sage Publications, Inc.Litt, M. D., Shafer, D. M., Ibanez, C. R., Kreutzer, D. L., & Tawfik-Yonkers, Z. (2009). Momentary pain and coping

in temporomandibular disorder pain: Exploring mechansims of cognitive behavioral treatment for chronic pain. Pain, 145, 160-168.

Lorber, W., Mazzoni, G., & Kirsch, I. (2007). Illness by suggestion: Expectancy, modeling, and gender in the production of psychosomatic symptoms. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 33, 112-116.

* Lowman, B. C., Drossman, D. A., Cramer, E. M., & McKee, D. C. (1987). Recollection of childhood events in adults with Irritable Bowel Syndrome. Journal of Clinical Gastroenterology, 9, 324-330.

Lumley, M. A., Gustavson, B. J., Partridge, R. T., & Labouvie-Vief, G. (2005). Assessing alexithymia and related emotional ability constructs using multiple methods: Interrelationships among measures. Emotion, 5, 329-342.

Lumley, M. A., Neely, L. C., & Burger, A. J. (2007). The assessment of alexithymia in medical settings: Implications for understanding and treating health problems. Journal of Personality Assessment, 89, 230-246.

Lumley, M. A., Smith, J. A., & Longo, D. J. (2002). The relationship of alexithymia to pain severity and impairment among patients with chronic myofascial pain. Comparisons with self-efficacy, catastrophizing, and depression. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 53, 823-830.

Lumley, M. A., Stettner, L., & Wehmer, F. (1996). How are alexithymia and physical illness linked? A review and critique of pathways. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 41, 505-518.

Lydiard, R. B. & Falsetti, S. A. (1999). Experience with anxiety and depression treatment studies: Implications for designing irritable bowel syndrome clinical trials. The American Journal of Medicine, 107, 65S-73S.

MacKinnon, D. P., Fritz, M. S., Williams, J., & Lockwood, C. M. (2007). Distribution of the product confidence limits for the indirect effect: Program PRODCLIN. Behavior Research Methods, 39, 384-389.

MacKinnon, D. P., Lockwood, C. M., Hoffman, J. M., West, S. G., & Sheets, V. (2002). A comparison of methods to test mediation and other intervening variable effects. Psychological Methods, 7, 83-104.

MacLeod, C., Mathews, A., & Tata, P. (1986). Attentional bias in emotional disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 95, 15-20.

Magni, G., Pierri, M., & Donzelli, F. (1987). Recurrent abdominal pain in children: A long-term follow-up. European Journal of Pediatrics, 146, 72-74.

Maric, M., Wiers, R. W., & Prins, P. J. M. (2012). Ten ways to improve the use of statistical mediation analysis in the practice of child and adolescent treatment research. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, doi: 10.1007/s10567-012-01 14-y.

Mazzoni, G., Foan, L., Hyland, M. E., & Kirsch, I. (2010). The effects of observation and gender on psychogenic symptoms. Health Psychology, 29, 181-185.

McGrath, P. A., Seifert, C. E., Speecley, K. N., Booth, J. C., Stitt, L., & Gibson, M. C. (1996). A new analogue scale for assessing children’s pain: an initial validation study. Pain, 64, 435-443.

* McGrath, P. J., Goodman, J. T., Firestone, P., Shipman, R., & Peters, S. (1983). Recurrent abdominal pain: a psychogenic disorder? Archives Disability Child, 58, 888-890.

Meerum Terwogt, M. & Olthof, T. (1989). Awareness and self-regulation of emotion in young children. In C.Saarni & P. L. Harris (Eds.), Children’s understanding of emotions (pp. 209-237). New York: Cambridge University Press.

Meesters, C., Muris, P., Ghys, A., Reumerman, T., & Rooijmans, M. (2003). The Children’s Somatization Inventory: Further evidence for its reliability and validity in a pediatric and a community sample of dutch children and adolescents. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 28, 413-422.

Menard, S. (2004). Six approaches to calculating standardized logistic regression coefficients. The American Statistician, 58, 218-223.

Mortimer, M. J., Kay, J., Jaron, A., & Good, P. A. (1992). Does a history of maternal migraine or depression predispose children to headache and stomach-ache? Headache, 32, 353-355.

Mulvaney, S., Lambert, E. W., Garber, J., & Walker, L. S. (2006). Trajectories of symptoms and impairment for pediatric patients with functional abdominal pain: A 5-year longitudinal study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 45, 737-744.

Muris, P., Meesters, C., de Kanter, E., & Timmerman, P. E. (2005). Behavioural inhibition and behavioural activation system scales for children: relationships with Eysenck’s personality traits and psychopathological symptoms. Personality and Individual Differences, 38, 831-841.

Muris, P., Meesters, C., & Schouten, E. (2002). A brief questionnaire of DSM-IV-defined anxiety and depression symptoms among children. Clinical Psychology and Psychotherapy, 9, 430-442.

Murphy, K. R. & Myors, B. (2004). Statistical power analysis: A simple and general model for traditional and modern hypothesis tests. Mahwa, NJ: Lawrance Erlbaum Associates.

Naliboff, B. D., Derbyshire, S. W. G., Munakata, J., Berman, S., Mandelkern, M., Chang, L. et al. (2001). Cerebral activation in patients with Irritable Bowel Syndrome and control subjects during rectosigmoid stimulation. Psychosomatic Medicine, 63, 365-375.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 188 18-07-12 17:20

Page 192: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

References | 189

Nock, M. K. (2007). Conceptual and desgn essentials for evaluating mechanisms of change. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 31, 4S-12S.

Nouwen, A., Cloutier, C., Kappas, A., Warbrick, T., & Sheffield, D. (2006). Effects of focusing and distraction on cold pressor-induced pain in chronic back pain patients and control subjects. The Journal of Pain, 7, 62-71.

Nyklicek, I., Poot, J. C., & Van Opstal, J. (2010). Psychological mindedness in relation to personality and coping in a sample of young adult psychiatric patients. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 66, 34-45.

Öhman, L. & Simrén, M. (2007). New insights into the pathogenesis and pathophysiology of irritable bowel syndrome. Digestive and Liver Disease, 39, 201-215.

Olafsdottir, E., Gilja, O. H., Aslaksen, A., Berstad, A., & Fluge, G. (2000). Impaired accomodation of the proximal stomach in children with recurrent abdominal pain. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 30, 157-163.

Olde Hartman, T. C., Hassink-Franke, L. J., Lucassen, P. L., Van Spaendonck, K. P., & Van Weel, C. (2009). Explanation and relations. How do general practitioners deal with patients with persistent medically unexplained symptoms: a focus group study. BMC Family Practice, 10.

Olsen, B., Berg, C. A., & Wiebe, D. J. (2008). Dissimilarity in mother and adolescent illness representations of type 1 diabetes and negative emotional adjustment. Psychology and Health, 23, 113-129.

Öst, L. G., Karlstedt, A., & Widén, S. (2012). The effects of cognitive behavior therapy delivered by students in a psychologist training program: an effectiveness study. Behavior Therapy, 43, 160-173.

Östberg, V., Alfvén, G., & Hjern, A. (2006). Living conditions and psychosomatic complaints in Swedish schoolchildren. Acta Paediatrica, 95, 929-934.

Oxman, T. E., Hull, J. G., & Dietrich, A. J. (2008). Problem-solving treatment and coping styles in primary care for minor depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 76, 933-943.

Palermo, T. M. & Eccleston, C. (2009). Parents of children and adolescents with chronic pain. Pain, 146, 15-17.Palermo, T. M., Eccleston, C., Lewandowski, A. S., de C Williams, A. C., & Morley, S. (2010). Randomized controlled

trials of psychological therapies for management of chronic pain in children and adolescents: An updated meta-analytic review. Pain, 148, 387-397.

Palermo, T. M., Wilson, A. C., Peters, M., Lemandowski, A., & Somhegyi, H. (2009). Randomized controlled trial of an internet-delivered family cognitive-behavioral therapy intervention for children and adolescents with chronic pain. Pain, 146, 205-213.

Parker, J. D. A., Taylor, G. J., & Bagby, R. M. (1998). Alexithymia: Relationship with ego defense and coping styles. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 39, 91-98.

* Pelzer, S. G. (1997). A biophysical model for irritable bowel syndrome. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 57.

Pennebaker, J. W. (1982). The psychology of physical symptoms. New York: Springer.Perquin, C. W., Hazebroek-Kampschreur, A. A. J. M., Hunfeld, J. A. M., Bohnen, A. M., van Suijlekom-Smit, L. W. A.,

Passchier, J. et al. (2000). Pain in children and adolescents: a common experience. Pain, 87, 51-58.Perquin, C. W., Hunfeld, J. A. M., Hazebroek-Kampschreur, A. A. J. M., van Suijlekom-Smit, L. W. A., Passchier, J.,

Koes, B. W. et al. (2003). The natural course of chronic bening pain in childhood and adolescence: a two-year population-based follow-up study. European Journal of Pain, 7, 551-559.

Pincus, T. & Morley, S. (2001). Cognitive processing bias in chronic pain: a review and integration. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 599-617.

Porcelli, P., Affatati, V., Bellomo, A., De Carne, M., Todarello, O., & Taylor, G. J. (2004). Alexithymia and psychopathology in patients with psychiatric and functional gastrointestinal disorders. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 73, 84-91.

Porcelli, P., Bagby, R. M., Taylor, G. J., De Carne, M., Leandro, G., & Todarello, O. (2003). Alexithymia as predictor of treatment outcome in patients with functional gastrointestinal disorders. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 911-918.

Porcelli, P., De Carne, M., & Todarello, O. (2004). Prediction of treatment outcome of patients with functional gastrointestinal disorders by the diagnostic criteria for psychosomatic research. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 73, 166-173.

Porcelli, P., Taylor, G. J., Bagby, R. M., & De Carne, M. (1999). Alexithymia and functional gastrointestinal disorders. A comparison with inflammatory bowel disease. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 68, 263-269.

Rachman, S. (1997). The evolution of cognitive behavior therapy. In D.M.Clark & C. G. Fairburn (Eds.), Science and practice of cognitive behaviour therapy (pp. 3-26). Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Ramchandani, P. G., Fazel, M., Stein, A., Wiles, N. J., & Hotopf, M. (2007). The impact of recurrent abdominal pain: predictors of outcome in a large population cohort. Acta Paediatrica, 96, 697-701.

Ramchandani, P. G., Hotopf, M., Sandhu, B., & Stein, A. (2005). The epidemiology of recurrent abdominal pain from 2 to 6 years of age: Results of a large, population-based study. Pediatrics, 116, 46-50.

Ramchandani, P. G., Stein, A., Hotopf, M., & Wiles, N. J. (2006). Early parental and child predictors of recurrent abdominal pain at school age: Results of a large population-based study. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 45, 729-736.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 189 18-07-12 17:20

Page 193: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

190 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Raphael, K. G. (2005). Childhood abuse and pain in adulthood: more than a modest relationship? Clinical Journal of Pain, 21, 373.

Rasquin, A., Di Lorenzo, C., Forbes, D., Guiraldes, E., Hyams, J. S., Staiano, A. et al. (2006). Childhood functional gastrointestinal disorders: Child/adolescent. Gastroenterology, 130, 1527-1537.

Rasquin-Weber, A., Hyman, P. E., Cucchiara, S., Fleisher, D. R., Hyams, J. S., Milla, P. J. et al. (1999). Childhood functional gastrointestinal disorders. Gut, 45, II60-II68.

Reid, S., Whooley, D., Crayford, T., & Hotopf, M. (2001). Medically unexplained symptoms - GPs’ attitudes towards their cause and management. Family Practice, 18, 519-523.

Richardson, W. S., Wilson, M. S., Nishikawa, J., & Hayward, R. S. (1995). The well-built clinical question: a key to evidence-based decisions. ACP Journal Club, 123, 12-13.

Richter, M., Ech, J., Straube, T., Miltner, W. H., & Weiss, T. (2010). Do words hurt? Brain activation during the processing of pain-related words. Pain, 148, 198-205.

Riedl, A., Schmidtmann, M., Stengel, A., Goebel, M., Wisser, A., Klapp, B. F. et al. (2008). Somatic comorbidities of irritable bowel syndrome: A systematic analysis. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 64, 573-582.

Rieffe, C., Oosterveld, P., Miers, A. C., Terwogt, M. M., & Ly, V. (2008). Emotion awareness and internalising symptoms in children and adolescents: The Emotion Awareness Questionnaire revised. Personality and Individual Differences, 45, 756-761.

Rieffe, C., Terwogt, M. M., & Bosch, J. D. (2004). Emotion understanding in children with frequent somatic complaints. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 1, 31-47.

Rieffe, C., Terwogt, M. M., Petrides, K. V., Cowan, R., Miers, A. C., & Tolland, A. (2007). Psychometric properties of the Emotion Awareness Questionnaire for children. Personality and Individual Differences, 43, 95-105.

Ringel, Y., Drossman, D., Leserman, J. L., Suyenobu, B. Y., Wilber, K., Lin, W. et al. (2008). Effect of abuse history on pain reports and brain responses to aversive visceral stimulation: An fMRI study. Gastroenterology, 134, 396-404.

Robins, P. M., Smith, S. M., Glutting, J. J., & Bishop, C. T. (2005). A randomized controlled trial of a cognitive-behavioral family intervention for pediatric recurrent abdominal pain. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 30, 397-408.

* Robinson, J. O., Alvarez, J. H., & Dodge, J. A. (1990). Life events and family history in children with recurrent abdominal pain. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 34, 171-181.

Roelofs, J., Peters, M. L., Fassaert, T., & Vlaeyen, J. W. S. (2005). The role of fear of movement and injury in selective attentional processing in patients with chronic low back pain: A dot-probe evaluation. The Journal of Pain, 6, 294-300.

Roelofs, J., Peters, M. L., McCracken, L., & Vlaeyen, J. W. S. (2003). The pain vigilance and awareness questionnair (PVAQ): further psychometric evaluation in fibromyalgia and other chronic pain syndromes. Pain, 101, 299-306.

Roelofs, J., Peters, M. L., Muris, P., & Vlaeyen, J. W. S. (2002). Dutch version of the pain vigilance and awareness questionnaire: validity and reliability in a pain-free population. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40, 1081-1090.

Roelofs, J., Peters, M. L., & Vlaeyen, J. W. S. (2002). Selective attention for pain-related information in healthy individuals: the role of pain and fear. European Journal of Pain, 6, 331-339.

Roelofs, J., Peters, M. L., Zeegers, M. P. A., & Vlaeyen, J. W. S. (2002). The modified Stroop paradigm as a measure of selective attention towards pain related stimuli among chronic pain patients: A meta-analysis. European Journal of Pain, 6, 281.

Routh, D. K. & Ernst, A. R. (1984). Somatization disorder in relatives of children and adolescents with Functional Abdominal Pain. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 9, 427-437.

Rubin, D. B. (1976). Inference and missing data. Biometrika, 63, 581-592.Saarni, C. & Harris, P. L. (1989). Children’s understanding of emotion. New York: Cambridge University Press.Salemink, E. & van den Hout, M. (2011). Trained interpretive bias survives mood change. Journal of Behavior

Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 41, 310-315.Sánchez-Meca, J., Marín-Martínez, F., & Chacón-Moscoso, S. (2003). Effect-size indices for dichotomized outcomes

in meta-analysis. Psychological Methods, 8, 448-467.Sanders, M. R., Rebgetz, M., Morrison, M., Bor, W., Gordon, A., Dadds, M. et al. (1989). Cognitive-behavioral treatment

of recurrent nonspecific abdominal pain in children - An analysis of generalization, maintenance, and side-effects. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 57, 294-300.

Sanders, M. R., Shepherd, R. W., Cleghorn, G., & Woolford, H. (1994). The treatment of recurrent abdominal-pain in children - A controlled comparison of cognitive-behavioral family intervention and standard pediatric care. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 306-314.

Sanderson, S., Tatt, I. D., & Higgins, J. P. (2007). Tools for assessing quality and susceptibility to bias in observational studies in epidemiology: a systematic review and annotated bibliography. International Journal of Epidemiology, 2007, 666-676.

Saps, M., Pensabene, L., Di Martino, L., Staiano, A., Wechsler, J., Zheng, X. et al. (2008). Post-infectious functional gastrointestinal disorders in children. Journal of Pediatrics, 152, 812-816.

Saps, M., Seshadri, R., Sztainberg, M., Schaffer, G., Marshall, B., & Di Lorenzo, C. (2009a). A prospective school-based study of abdominal pain and other common somatic complaints in children. The Journal of Pediatrics, 154, 322-326.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 190 18-07-12 17:20

Page 194: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

References | 191

Saps, M., Youssef, N. N., Miranda, A., Nurko, S., Hyman, P., Cocjin, J. et al. (2009b). Multicenter, randomized, placebo-controlled trial of amitriptyline in children with functional gastrointestinal disorders. Gastroenterology, 137, 1261-1269.

Saunders, K., Von Korff, M., LeResche, L., & Mancl, L. (2007). Relationship of common pain conditions in mothers and children. Clinical Journal of Pain, 23, 204-213.

Schafer, J. L. & Graham, J. W. (2002). Missing data: Our view of the state of the art. Psychological Methods, 7, 147-177.Schoth, D. E. & Liossi, C. (2010). Attentional bias toward pictorial representations of pain in individuals with chronic

headache. Clinical Journal of Pain, 26, 244-250.Shields, C. & Murphy, K. (2011). Selective attention and health anxiety: Ill-health stimuli are distracting for

everyone. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 35, 241-252.Shirkey, K. C., Smith, C. A., & Walker, L. S. (2011). Dispositional versus episode-specific assessment of children’s

coping with pain. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 36, 74-83.Sifneos, P. E. (1973). The prevalence of “alexithymic” characteristics in psychosomatic patients. Psychotherapy and

Psychosomatics, 22, 255-262.Silverman, W. K. & Albano, A. M. (2001). ADIS-C. Anxiety disorders interview schedule for DSM-IV. Lisse: Swets &

Zeitlinger.Silverman, W. K., Saavedra, L. M., & Pina, A. A. (2001). Test-retest reliability of anxiety symptoms and diagnoses

with the anxiety disorders interview schedule for DSM-IV: Child and parent versions. Journal of the American Acadamy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 40, 937-944.

Smart, S. & Cottrell, D. (2005). Going to the doctors: The views of mothers of children with recurrent abdominal pain. Child: Care, Health and Development, 31, 265-273.

Snijders, T. J., Ramsey, N. F., Koerselman, F., & Van Gijn, J. (2010). Attentional modulation fails to attenuate the subjective pain experience in chronic, unexplained pain. European Journal of Pain, 14, 282.e1-282.e10.

Spiller, R. & Garsed, K. (2009). Postinfectious irritable bowel syndrome. Gastroenterology, 136, 1979-1988.Spiller, R. C. (2011). Irritable bowel syndrome: gender, infection, lifestyle or what else? Digestive Diseases, 29, 215-

221.Sprangers, M. A. G. & Schwartz, C. E. (1999). Integrating response shift into health-related quality of life research: a

theoretical model. Social Science & Medicine, 48, 1507-1515.Sprenger, L., Gerhards, F., & Goldbeck, L. (2011). Effects of psychological treatment on recurrent abdominal pain

in children - A meta-analysis. Clinical Psychology Review, 31, 1192-1197.Stanford, E. A., Chambers, C. T., Biesanz, J. C., & Chen, E. (2008). The frequency, trajectories and predictors of

adolescent recurrent pain: A population-based approach. Pain, 138, 11-21.Starfield, B., Gross, E., Wood, M., Pantell, R., Allen, C., Gordon, B. et al. (1980). Psychosocial and psychosomatic

diagnoses in primary care of children. Pediatrics, 66, 159-167.Stark, D., Van Hal, S., Marriott, D., Ellis, J., & Harkness, J. (2007). Irritable bowel syndrome: A review on the role of

intestinal protozoa and the importance of their detection and diagnosis. International Journal of Parasitology, 37, 11-20.

Stickler, G. B. & Murphy, D. B. (1979). Recurrent abdominal pain. American Journal of Diseases of Children, 133, 486-489.

Tabachnick, B. G. & Fidell, L. S. (2001). Using multivariate statistics. Needham Heights, Massachusetts: Allyn & Bacon.Talley, N. J. & Spiller, R. (2002). Irritable bowel syndrome: a little understood organic bowel disease? Lancet, 360,

555-564.Teitelbaum, J. E. & Arora, R. (2011). Long-term efficacy of low-dose tricyclic antidepressants for children with

functional gastrointestinal disorders. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 53, 260-264.Terwogt, M. M., Rieffe, C., Miers, A. C., Jellesma, F. C., & Tolland, A. (2006). Emotions and self-esteem as indicators of

somatic complaints in children. Infant and Child Development, 15, 581-592.Thakkar, K., Chen, L., Tatevian, N., Shulman, R. J., McDuffie, A., Tsou, M. et al. (2009). Diagnostic yield of

oesophagogastroduodenoscopy in children with abdominal pain. Alimentary Pharmacology & Therapeutics, 30, 662-669.

The KIDSCREEN Group Europe (2006). The KIDSCREEN questionnaires - Quality of life questionnaires for children and adolescents. Handbook. Lengerich: Pabst Science Publishers.

Thiadens, E. & De Haan, E. (2007). Kortdurende cognitieve gedragstherapie voor kinderen en jongeren met lichamelijk onverklaarde buikpijn. [Brief cognitive behavior therapy for children and adolescents with medically unexplained abdominal pain]. Kinder- & Jeugdpsychotherapie, 34, 31-41.

Thomsen, A. H., Compas, B. E., Colletti, R. B., Stanger, C., Boyer, M. C., & Konik, B. S. (2002). Parent reports of coping and stress responses in children with recurrent abdominal pain. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 27, 215-226.

Twisk, J. W. R. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analysis for epidemiology. A practical guide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Van de Putte, E. M., Engelbert, R. H. H., Kuis, W., Kimpen, J. L. L., & Uiterwaal, C. S. P. M. (2007). Alexithymia in adolescents with chronic fatigue syndrome. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 63, 377-380.

Van der Veek, P. P. J. (2009). Towards an integrated psychoneurophysiological approah of irritable bowel syndrome. Doctoral dissertation, Leiden: Leiden University.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 191 18-07-12 17:20

Page 195: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

192 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Van der Veek, S. M. C., De Haan, E., Derkx, H. H. F., Benninga, M. A., Lindauer, R. J. L., & Boer, F. (submitted a). The relative contribution of parental and children’s cognitive and behavioral reactions to pediatric functional abdominal pain. Journal of Pediatric Psychology.

Van der Veek, S. M. C., Derkx, H. H. F., Benninga, M. A., Boer, F., & De Haan, E. (submitted b). Cognitive behavior therapy for the treatment of pediatric functional abdominal pain: A randomized controlled trial. American Journal of Gastroenterology.

Van der Veek, S. M. C., Derkx, H. H. F., De Haan, E., Benninga, M. A., & Boer, F. (2010). Abdominal pain in Dutch schoolchildren: Relations with physical and psychological comorbid complaints in children and their parents. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 51, 481-487.

Van der Veek, S. M. C., Derkx, H. H. F., De Haan, E., Benninga, M. A., Plak, R. D., & Boer, F. (2012). Do parents maintain or exacerbate pediatric functional abdominal pain? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Health Psychology, 17, 258-272.

Van Dulmen, A. M., Fennis, J. F. M., Mokkink, H. G. A., Van der Velden, H. G. M., & Bleijenberg, G. (1997). Persisting improvement in complaint-related cognitions initiated during medical consultations in functional abdominal complaints. Psychological Medicine, 27, 725-729.

Van Ginkel, R., Voskuijl, W. P., Benninga, M. A., Taminiau, J. A. J. M., & Boeckxstaens, G. E. (2001). Alterations in rectal sensitivity and motility in childhood irritable bowel syndrome. Gastroenterology, 120, 31-38.

Van Laarhoven, A., Kraaimaat, F. W., Wilder-Smith, O., & Evers, A. W. M. (2010). Role of attentional focus on bodily sensations in sensitivity to itch and pain. Acta Dermato-Venereologica, 90, 46-51.

Van Oudenhove, L. (2008). Het brein en de buik: stress, emoties en maagdarmklachten. In B.Van Houdenhove (Ed.), Stress, het lijf, en het brein. Ziekten op de grens tussen psyche en soma (pp. 123-153). Leuven: LannooCampus.

Van Oudenhove, L., Coen, S. J., & Aziz, Q. (2007). Functional brain imaging of gastrointestinal sensation in health and disease. World Journal of Gastroenterology, 13, 3438-3445.

Van Oudenhove, L., Demyttenaere, K., Tack, J., & Aziz, Q. (2004). Central nervous system involvement in functional gastrointestinal disorders. Best Practice & Research Clinical Gastroenterology, 18, 663-680.

Van Slyke, D. A. (2001). Maternal influences on children’s pain behavior. Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 62.

Van Suijlekom-Smit, L. W. A., Bruijnzeels, M. A., van der Wouden, J. C., Van der Velden, J., Visser, H. K. A., & Dokter, H. J. (1995). Voor welke gezondheidsproblemen bij kinderen wordt de huisarts geconsulteerd en hoe vaak? Nederlands Tijdschrift voor Geneeskunde, 139, 1684-1689.

Van Tilburg, M. A., Chitkara, D. K., Palsson, O. S., Turner, M., Blois-Martin, N., Ulshen, M. et al. (2009). Audio-recorded guided imagery treatment reduces functional abdominal pain in children: a pilot study. Pediatrics, 124, e890-e897.

Van Tilburg, M. A., Venepalli, N., Ulshen, M., Freeman, K. L., Levy, R., & Whitehead, W. E. (2006). Parents’ worries about recurrent abdominal pain in children. Gastroenterol.Nurs., 29, 50-55.

Van Tilburg, M. L., Chitkara, D. K., Palsson, O. S., Levy, R. L., & Whitehead, W. E. (2009). Parental worries and beliefs about abdominal pain. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 48, 311-317.

Vasey, M. W. & MacLeod, C. (2001). Information-processing factors in childhood anxiety: A review and developmental perspective. In M.W.Vasey & M. R. Dadds (Eds.), The Developmental Psychopathology of Anxiety (pp. 253-277). Oxford: University Press.

* Venepalli, N. K., van Tilburg, M. A., & Whitehead, W. E. (2006). Recurrent abdominal pain: what determines medical consulting behavior? Dig.Dis.Sci., 51, 192-201.

Verhoeven, K., Crombez, G., Eccleston, C., Van Ryckeghem, D. M. L., Morley, S., & Van Damme, S. (2010). The role of motivation in distracting attention away from pain: An experimental study. Pain, 149, 229-234.

Verkuil, B., Brosschot, J. F., & Thayer, J. F. (2007). A sensitive body or a sensitive mind? Associations among somatic sensitization, cognitive sensitization, health worry, and subjective health complaints. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 63, 673-681.

Vervoort, T., Eccleston, C., Goubert, L., Buysse, A., & Crombez, G. (2010). Children’s catastrophic thinking about their pain predicts pain and disability 6 months later. European Journal of Pain, 14, 90-96.

Vervoort, T., Goubert, L., Eccleston, Ch., Bijttebier, P., & Crombez, G. (2005). Catastrophic thinking about pain is independently associated with pain severity, disability, and somatic complaints in school children and children with chronic pain. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 31, 674-683.

Vervoort, T., Huguet, A., Verhoeven, K., & Goubert, L. (2011). Mothers’ and fathers’ responses to their child’s pain moderate the relationship between the child’s pain catastrophizing and disability. Pain, 152, 786-793.

Villemure, C. & Bushnell, C. (2002). Cognitive modulation of pain: how do attention and emotion influence pain processing? Pain, 95, 195-199.

Vlieger, A. M., Menko-Frankenhuis, C., Wolfkamp, S. C., Tromp, E., & Benninga, M. A. (2007). Hypnotherapy for children with functional abdominal pain or irritable bowel syndrome: a randomised controlled trial. Gastroenterology, 133, 1430-1436.

Vlieger, A. M., Rutten, J. M., Govers, A. M., Frankenhuis, C., & Benninga, M. A. (2012). Long-term follow-up of gut-directed hypnotherapy vs. standard care in children with functional abdominal pain or irritable bowel syndrome. American Journal of Gastroenterology, 107, 627-631.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 192 18-07-12 17:20

Page 196: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

References | 193

von Baeyer, C. L. & Walker, L. S. (1999). Children with recurrent abdominal pain: Issues in the selection and description of research participants. Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 20, 307-313.

Vorst, H. C. M. & Bermond, B. (2001). Validity and reliability of the Bermond-Vorst Alexithymia Questionnaire. Personality and Individual Differences, 30, 413-434.

Walker, L. S., Baber, K. F., Garber, J., & Smith, C. A. (2008). A typology of pain coping strategies in pediatric patients with chronic abdominal pain. Pain, 137, 266-275.

Walker, L. S., Dengler-Crish, C. M., Rippel, S., & Bruehl, S. (2010). Functional abdominal pain in childhood and adolescence increases risk for chronic pain in adulthood. Pain, 150, 568-572.

Walker, L. S., Garber, J., & Greene, J. W. (1991). Somatization symptoms in pediatric abdominal pain patients: Relation to chronicity of abdominal pain and parent somatization. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 19, 379-393.

* Walker, L. S., Garber, J., & Greene, J. W. (1993). Psychosocial correlates of recurrent childhood pain: A comparison of pediatric patients with recurrent abdominal pain, oragnic illness, and psychiatric disorders. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 102, 248-258.

Walker, L. S., Garber, J., & Greene, J. W. (1994). Somatic complaints in pediatric patients: A prospective study of the role of negative life events, child social and academic competence, and parental somatic symptoms. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 62, 1213-1221.

Walker, L. S., Garber, J., Smith, C. A., Van Slyke, D. A., & Claar, R. L. (2001). The relation of daily stressors to somatic and emotional symptoms in children with and without recurrent abdominal pain. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 69, 85-91.

Walker, L. S., Garber, J., Van Slyke, D. A., & Greene, J. W. (1995). Long-term health outcomes in patients with recurrent abdominal pain. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 20, 233-245.

* Walker, L. S. & Greene, J. W. (1989). Children with recurrent abdominal pain and their parents: more somatic complaints, anxiety, and depression than other patient families? Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 14, 231-243.

Walker, L. S. & Greene, J. W. (1991a). Negative life events and symptom resolution in pediatric abdominal pain patients. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 16, 341-360.

Walker, L. S. & Greene, J. W. (1991b). The Functional Disability Inventory - Measuring a neglected dimension of child health-status. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 16, 39-58.

Walker, L. S., Levy, R. L., & Whitehead, W. E. (2006). Validation of a measure of protective parent responses to children’s pain. Clinical Journal of Pain, 22, 712-716.

Walker, L. S., Smith, C. A., Garber, J., & Claar, R. L. (2005). Testing a model of pain appraisal and coping in children with chronic abdominal pain. Health Psychology, 24, 364-374.

Walker, L. S., Smith, C. A., Garber, J., & Claar, R. L. (2007). Appraisal and coping with daily stressors by pediatric patients with chronic abdominal pain. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 32, 206-216.

Walker, L. S., Smith, C. A., Garber, J., & VanSlyke, D. A. (1997). Development and validation of the pain response inventory for children. Psychological Assessment, 9, 392-405.

* Walker, L. S., Williams, S. E., Smith, C. A., Garber, J., Van Slyke, D. A., & Lipani, T. A. (2006). Parent attention versus distraction: impact on symptom complaints by children with and without chronic functional abdominal pain. Pain, 122, 43-52.

Walker, L. S. & Zeman, J. L. (1992). Parental responses to child illness behavior. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 17, 49-71.

Watson, D. & Pennebaker, J. W. (1989). Health complaints, stress, and distress: Exploring the central role of negative affectivity. Psychological Review, 96, 234-254.

Weersing, V. R. & Weisz, J. R. (2002). Mechanisms of action in youth psychotherapy. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 43, 3-29.

Weisz, J. R., Chorpita, B. F., Palinkas, L. A., Schoenwald, S. K., Miranda, J., Bearman, S. K. et al. (2012). Testing standard and modular dsigns for psychotherapy treating depression, anxiety, and conduct problems in youth. A randomized effectiveness trial. Archives of General Psychiatry, 69, 274-282.

* Wendland, M., Jackson, Y., & Stokes, L. D. (2010). Functional disability in paediatric patients with recurrent abdominal pain. Child: Care, Health and Development, 36, 516-523.

Weydert, J. A., Ball, T. M., & Davis, M. F. (2003). Systemetic review of treatments for recurrent abdominal pain. Pediatrics, 111, e1-e11.

Weydert, J. A., Shapiro, D. E., Acra, S. A., Monheim, C. J., Chambers, A. S., & Ball, T. M. (2006). Evaluation of guided imagery as treatment for recurrent abdominal pain in children: a randomized controlled trial. BMC Pediatrics, 6, 29.

Whitehead, W. E., Busch, C. M., Heller, B. R., & Costa, P. T. Jr. (1986). Social learning influences on menstrual symptoms and illness behavior. Health Psychology, 5, 13-23.

Whitehead, W. E., Crowell, M. D., Heller, B. R., Robinson, J. C., Schuster, M. M., & Horn, S. (1994). Modeling and reinforcement of the sick role during childhood predicts adult illness behavior. Psychosomatic Medicine, 56, 541-550.

Whitehead, W. E. & Palsson, O. S. (1998). Is rectal pain sensitivity a biological marker for irritable bowel syndrome: Psychological influences on pain perception. Gastroenterology, 115, 1263-1271.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 193 18-07-12 17:20

Page 197: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

194 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Whitehead, W. E., Winget, C., Fedoravicius, A. S., Wooley, S., & Blackwell, B. (1982). Learned illness behavior in patients with irritable bowel syndrome and peptic ulcer. Digestive Diseases Sciences, 27, 202-208.

Wiech, K., Ploner, M., & Tracey, I. (2008). Neurocognitive aspects of pain perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 306-313.

Wijga, A. H., Scholtens, S., Van Oeffelen, A. A. M., & Beckers, M. (2010). Klachten en kwalen bij kinderen in Nederland: Omvang en gevolgen geïnventariseerd. Bilthoven: Rijksinstituut voor Volksgezondheid en Milieu.

Wilson, D. B. (2005). Meta-analysis macros for SAS, SPSS, and Stata. Retrieved January 2010 at http://mason.gmu.edu/~dwilsonb/ma.html.

Wolters, L. H., De Haan, E., Vervoort, L., Hogendoorn, S. M., Boer, F., & Prins, P. J. M. (2011). The time-course of threat processing in children: a temporal dissociation between selective attention and behavioral interference. Anxiety, Stress, & Coping, 25, 259-273.

Youssef, N. N., Atienza, K., Langseder, A. L., & Strauss, R. S. (2008). Chronic abdominal pain and depressive symptoms: Analysis of the national longitudinal study of adolescent health. Clinical Gastroenterology and Hepatology, 6, 329-332.

Youssef, N. N., Murphy, D. B., Langseder, A. L., & Rosh, J. R. (2006). Quality of life for children with functional abdominal pain: A comparison study of patients’ and parents’ perceptions. Pediatrics, 117, 54-59.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 194 18-07-12 17:20

Page 198: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

Dankwoord

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 195 18-07-12 17:20

Page 199: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

196 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Acht jaar voor Elins bezoek aan de psycholoog in Amsterdam, in een andere stad, oreert Nico Metaal in collegezaal SC01 van de Faculteit der Sociale Wetenschappen over de onmetelijke waarde van het doen van wetenschappelijk onderzoek binnen de psychologie. Ik luister als eerstejaars studente te midden van mijn 600 medestudenten gretig naar zijn verhaal. Dit is mijn eerste echte college! Maar er is wel een probleem: ik ben nu al in de war. Onderzoek bij psychologie? Ik had toch juist afstand gedaan van mijn B-pakket op de middelbare school; out with het exacte and in with de diepgang? Gelukkig verdwijnt de verwarring tijdens dat eerste college al snel wanneer ik besef dat in het doen van wetenschappelijk onderzoek binnen de psychologie, de perfecte middenweg ligt tussen mijn bèta verleden en gamma toekomst. Een soort liefde op het tweede gezicht, en het was mij dan ook na mijn afstuderen direct duidelijk dat ik wilde promoveren. Maar deze liefde voor het doen van onderzoek, hoewel onmisbaar bij het schrijven van mijn proefschrift, was zeker niet voldoende om het ook tot een succesvol einde te brengen. Daar kwam de hulp van heel veel bijzondere mensen bij kijken, die ik hier graag wil bedanken.

Op de eerste plaats vraagt een promotieonderzoek veel begeleiding en samenwerking met collega’ s. Enorm veel dank ben ik dan ook verschuldigd aan mijn dagelijks begeleiders, Bert Derkx en Else de Haan. Lieve Bert en Else, bedankt voor jullie nimmer aflatende intensieve begeleiding. Jullie verschillende en aanvullende visies op het psychisch en lichamelijk functioneren van kinderen en gezinnen hebben dit proefschrift gemaakt tot wat het is. Ongelooflijk hoe veel ik van jullie heb mogen leren in deze voor jullie persoonlijk roerige jaren. Ondanks alle tegenslagen waren jullie altijd bereid mij te blijven begeleiden, iets waar ik enorme bewondering voor heb. Ik ben trots dat ik jullie naast mij heb gehad gedurende deze zes jaar. Ook wil ik Frits Boer, Marc Benninga en Ramón Lindauer bedanken voor hun bijdragen als promotor, respectievelijk co-promotoren. Frits en Marc, ontzettend bedankt voor jullie intensieve begeleiding. Jullie snelle, kritische, maar altijd enthousiaste reacties op mijn stukken hebben mij enorm geholpen, evenals jullie onvoorwaardelijke steun. Ramón, veel dank voor de prettige samenwerking en de mogelijkheid om onderzoek te blijven doen bij de afdeling Kinder- en Jeugdpsychiatrie van het AMC. De hulp van mijn kamergenootjes en mede-promovendi Lidewij Wolters, tevens paranimf, en Sanne Hogendoorn zal ik nooit vergeten. Lieve Lidewij en Sanne, jullie maakten het een feest om iedere dag de reis naar het AMC te maken. Eerst in onze zonovergoten broeikas, en later in onze heerlijke privékamer met openslaande ramen, hebben we onze gang-genoten vaak genoeg lastiggevallen met gegiebel. Samen promoveren is toch een stuk beter dan alleen; het was me een eer, metgezellen! Natuurlijk ook veel dank aan alle andere collega’s van de onderzoeksafdeling. Lieve Susan en Suzan, jullie ondersteuning was onontbeerlijk, maar jullie gezelligheid en het ‘welkome’ gevoel dat jullie ons geven, ga ik pas écht missen. Julia, Marthe, Chaim, Els, Maj, Katja, Katie, Mariëlle, Eva, Caroline, Inger,

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 196 18-07-12 17:20

Page 200: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

Dankwoord | 197

Mirte en alle andere onderzoekers en onderzoeksassistenten die in de loop van de jaren op de afdeling O&O/O&O&O/O4 hebben gewerkt: dank voor de fijne samenwerking op onze kleine maar mooie afdeling, en voor jullie gezelligheid! Daarnaast wil ik alle therapeuten, kinderartsen, verpleegkundigen en studenten die mij hebben geholpen met de uitvoering van het onderzoek graag bedanken. Lieve Elisabeth, Lydia, Annika, Martine en Rachel, ik heb grote bewondering voor jullie geweldige behandelvaardigheden. Die 60% effectiviteit is jullie verdienste! Lieve Rachel, ik hoop in de toekomst nog veel met je samen te werken, zoals we ook bij meerdere projecten in dit boekje hebben gedaan. Dames van de kinderpoli, en in het bijzonder Faiza en Maudy, bedankt dat ik jullie altijd mocht lastig vallen met vervelende vragen over statussen. Anouck, veel dank voor je hulp met het invoeren van die enorme hoeveelheden data. Bart Koot, Merit Tabbers, Andrieke Knottnerus, Felix Kreier, Femke van Herrewegen, Chris de Kruiff, en alle andere kinderartsen en huisartsen uit omgeving Amsterdam en Leiden die aan ons onderzoek hebben gedacht: zonder jullie hadden we deze inclusie nooit kunnen behalen, veel dank voor jullie inzet en behandeling. Ook wil ik graag twee collega’s van de Universiteit Leiden bedanken, Nadia Garnefski en in het bijzonder Vivian Kraaij. Hoewel jullie niet direct mee hebben geholpen aan het onderzoek beschreven in dit proefschrift, hebben jullie mij vanaf dag één geënthousiasmeerd voor het doen van wetenschappelijk onderzoek en het geven van onderwijs. Lieve Vivian, volgens mij wist jij nog eerder dan ikzelf dat ik universitair docent wilde worden. Mijn droom komt uit nu dat zover is, en ook nog zo dicht bij jou in de buurt! Ik ben je meer verschuldigd dan ik kan zeggen, en ik hoop later net zoveel te kunnen betekenen voor studenten als jij voor mij hebt betekend. Laten we in de toekomst nog veel mooie onderzoeken gaan opzetten, samen met Nadia.

Een onderzoek kan natuurlijk niet zonder begeleiding en ondersteuning van collega’s, maar zonder bereidwillige participanten is een onderzoek nergens. Daarom wil ik graag alle kinderen en ouders die zo vol vertrouwen mee hebben gedaan aan het huidige onderzoek ontzettend bedanken. Uw dankbaarheid en enthousiaste medewerking vormden voor mij een grote bron van motivatie om dit proefschrift af te ronden! Eveneens veel dank aan alle scholen in Noord-Holland, Zuid-Holland en Utrecht waar wij data mochten verzamelen, en aan de kinderen, ouders en studenten die aan dat deel van het onderzoek hebben meegewerkt.

De Maag Lever Darm Stichting, de Bascule en het AMC dank ik voor het financieren van dit project en het mogelijk maken van deze promotie. Daarnaast natuurlijk dank aan de leden van de promotiecommissie, prof. dr. H.S.A. Heymans, prof. dr. H.C.P.M. van Weert, prof. dr. P. Spinhoven, prof. dr. C.M.J.G. Maes, dr. Y.R. van Rood, en dr. A.M. Vlieger, voor het lezen en beoordelen van het manuscript.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 197 18-07-12 17:20

Page 201: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

198 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Hoewel liefde voor onderzoek, fijne collega’s en bereidwillige deelnemers onontbeerlijke bouwstenen voor een proefschrift zijn, zijn die elementen alleen helaas niet genoeg; een specie van bloed, zweet en tranen lijken ook nodig te zijn om er een geheel van te maken. Gelukkig zijn er veel mensen die mij ook in dat opzicht hebben ondersteund. Mijn vriend Ben, mijn ouders, mijn broers, zus, hun partners en hun kinderen, mijn schoonfamilie en mijn vrienden en vriendinnen, stuk voor stuk hebben jullie op je eigen manier geholpen bij het tot stand komen van dit proefschrift. Lieve papa en mama, ik ben zo blij met jullie en de manier waarop jullie mij en ons alle vier altijd steunen. Jullie staan altijd onvoorwaardelijk achter mijn keuzes en ik hoop jullie trots te hebben gemaakt met dit proefschrift. Het is zo fijn om een veilige thuishaven te hebben! Lieve Pat, ik vind het supergaaf dat jij mijn paranimf bent. Wat bijzonder dat onze proefschriften, van alle dingen die je in de psychologie en geneeskunde kunt onderzoeken, over vrijwel hetzelfde onderwerp gaan. Heel veel dank voor al je ‘grote-broer’ tips en adviezen de afgelopen jaren (die stelling van Pythagoras komt nog steeds van pas). Lieve zus, bedankt voor je enthousiasme voor mijn onderwerp en onze leuke discussies daarover. Ik kan altijd bij je terecht en dat waardeer ik enorm. Wil je na het lezen van dit boekje toch niet liever buikpijn-verpleegkundige worden? Lieve Arlette, wat mij betreft ben je mijn tweede zusje, en ik kan je niet vertellen hoeveel de manier waarop jij altijd voor mij klaarstaat voor me betekent. Je hebt het laatste jaar enorm met me meegeleefd, en je steun heeft me zeker een heleboel tranen en zweetdruppels bespaard. Lieve Mandy, ik vond het fantastisch om gelijktijdig met jou het promotietraject door te maken. Na al zo lang vriendinnen te zijn, hebben we ook dit kunnen delen. Lieve Marloes, jij bent de enige die weet hoe mijn wetenschappelijke carrière begon met ons biologieproject, en ik hoop voor het vervolg van mijn carrière dat je die kennis voor je houdt. Jullie pikken het altijd feilloos op als ik te hard heb gewerkt en het tijd is voor ontspanning, en staan dan altijd open voor een leuk uitje. Ik hoop dat we er met zijn drietjes nog veel gaan meemaken.

Tot slot zijn er twee bijzondere mensen waaraan ik de grootste dank verschuldigd ben. Lieve Nel, het doet me veel pijn dat je nooit hebt mogen meemaken dat ik aan dit project ben begonnen en het af heb kunnen ronden. Maar wat mij betreft is het geen toeval dat ik, vlak nadat jij niet meer bij ons was, deze mooie kans heb gekregen. Ik hoop dat je altijd over ons blijft waken. En Ben, mijn lieve Ben. Je had de twijfelachtige eer om het grootste deel van het zweet, de tranen en de Shelley-gekte op te vangen, en dat deed je natuurlijk op jouw eigen, altijd vrolijke manier, die nooit faalt! Vooral het laatste jaar heb je me enorm gesteund, en zonder jouw vermogen om me altijd weer op te vrolijken, had ik het nooit gered. Lieve Ben, ik ben eindelijk klaar. Fijn hè. Zullen we wat leuks gaan doen?

Lisse, juli 2012

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 198 18-07-12 17:20

Page 202: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

Curriculum Vitæ | 199

Curriculum Vitæ

Shelley Maria Cornelia van der Veek werd geboren op 2 maart 1983 te Lisse. In 2001 behaalde zij cum laude haar Gymnasium diploma aan het Fioretticollege te Lisse. In datzelfde jaar startte zij haar studie psychologie aan de Universiteit Leiden. Tijdens haar eerste onderzoeksproject onder begeleiding van dr. V. Kraaij in het tweede studiejaar van psychologie, werd haar interesse in onderzoek gewekt. Dit kreeg vervolg toen zij gedurende haar derde studiejaar een Honours Research Project uitvoerde bij dr. V. Kraaij en dr. N. Garnefski naar depressieve klachten, doelfrustratie en coping bij patiënten met HIV, wat tot een publicatie leidde. Na haar scriptie naar de emotionele adaptatie van ouders van kinderen met Downsyndroom, studeerde zij in 2005 cum laude af in Klinische en Gezondheidspsychologie. Van 2005 tot 2006 heeft zij een jaar onderwijs gegeven en onderzoek verricht aan de afdeling Gezondheidspsychologie van de Universiteit Leiden. Van november 2006 tot en met december 2011 is zij part-time werkzaam geweest als promovenda bij de afdeling Kinder- en Jeugdpsychiatrie van het Academisch Medisch Centrum Amsterdam, waar zij onderzoek verrichtte naar de behandeling van kinderen en jongeren met functionele buikpijn, onder begeleiding van prof. dr. F. Boer, prof. dr. E. de Haan, dr. H.H.F. Derkx, prof. dr. M.A. Benninga, en later dr. R.J.L. Lindauer. Dit onderzoek heeft geleid tot het huidige proefschrift. Gedurende diezelfde periode heeft zij diverse onderwijswerkzaamheden verricht voor de afdeling Gezondheidspsychologie aan de Universiteit Leiden. Sinds januari 2012 is zij part-time werkzaam als postdoc onderzoekster bij de afdeling Kinder- en Jeugdpsychiatrie van het AMC. Vanaf februari 2013 zal zij het AMC verlaten, en haar carrière vervolgen als universitair docent bij de afdeling Algemene en Gezinspedagogiek aan de Universiteit Leiden.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 199 18-07-12 17:20

Page 203: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

200 | A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain

Publications

Based on the present thesis:Van der Veek, S. M. C., Derkx, H. H. F., de Haan, E., Benninga, M. A., & Boer, F. (2010). Abdominal pain

in Dutch schoolchildren: Relationships with physical and psychological co-morbid complaints in children and their parents. Journal of Pediatric Gastroenterology and Nutrition, 51, 481-487.

Van der Veek, S. M. C., Derkx, H. H. F., de Haan, E., Benninga, M. A., Plak, R. D., & Boer, F. (2012). Do parents maintain or exacerbate pediatric functional abdominal pain? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Health Psychology, 17, 258-272 .

Van der Veek, S. M. C., Derkx, H. H. F., de Haan, E., Benninga, M. A., & Boer, F. (2012). Emotion awareness and coping in children with functional abdominal pain: A controlled study. Social Science and Medicine, 74, 112-119.

Van der Veek, S. M. C., de Haan, E., Derkx, H. H. F., Benninga, M. A., Lindauer, R. J. L., & Boer, F. (submitted). Cognitive and behavioral reactions to pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: The influence of parents vs. children. Journal of Pediatric Psychology.

Van der Veek, S. M. C., Derkx, H. H. F., Plak, R. D., Benninga, M. A., Boer, F., Lindauer, R. J. L., & De Haan, E. (submitted). Attentional bias to activity of different parts of the body in children with functional abdominal pain: An experimental study. The Journal of Pain.

Van der Veek, S. M. C., Derkx, H. H. F., Benninga, M. A., Boer, F., & De Haan, E. (submitted). Cognitive behavior therapy versus medical care for the treatment of pediatric functional abdominal pain: A randomized controlled trial. American Journal of Gastroenterology.

Van der Veek, S. M. C., Derkx, H. H. F., Benninga, M. A., Boer, F., & De Haan, E. (submitted). Exploring mediating and moderating factors of cognitive behavior therapy for the treatment of pediatric functional abdominal pain. Journal of Clinical and Consulting Psychology.

Other:Jellesma, F. C., Faddegon, K., & Van der Veek, S. M. C. (2011). Children with somatic complaints: Do

they show a somatic attention bias? Netherlands Journal of Psychology, 66, 78-84.Kraaij, V., Garnefski, N., Schroevers, M. J., van der Veek, S. M., Witlox, R., & Maes, S. (2008). Cognitive

coping, goal self-efficacy and personal growth in HIV-infected men who have sex with men. Patient Education and Counseling, 72, 301-304.

Kraaij, V., van der Veek, S. M., Garnefski, N., Schroevers, M. J., Witlox, R., & Maes, S. (2008). Coping, goal adjustment, and psychological well-being in HIV-infected men who have sex with men. AIDS Patient Care STDS, 22, 395-402.

Van der Veek, S. M. C. (2009). Buikpijn bij kinderen: een emotioneel probleem? Nederlands Tijdschrift voor Behavioral Medicine, 21, 20-23.

Van der Veek, S. M. C., Nobel, R. A., & Derkx, H. H. F. (2012). The relationship between emotion awareness and somatic complaints in children and adolescents: Investigating the mediating role of anxiety and depression. Psychology & Health, DOI:10.1080/08870446.2012.685738

Van der Veek, S. M. C., Kraaij, V., & Garnefski, N. (2009). Down or up? Explaining positive and negative emotions in parents of children with Down Syndrome: Goals, cognitive coping and coping resources. Journal of Intellectual and Developmental Disability, 34, 216-229.

Van der Veek, S. M. C., Kraaij, V., & Garnefski, N. (2009). Cognitive coping strategies and stress in parents of children with Down Syndrome: A prospective study. Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 47, 295-306.

Van der Veek, S. M. C., Kraaij, V., & Garnefski, N. (2005). Down om uw kind met Down? Een onderzoek naar het stress-copingmodel in ouders van kinderen met Downsyndroom, samenvatting en conclusies. Down+Update, 72, 1-3.

Van der Veek, S. M. C., Kraaij, V., Van Koppen, W., Garnefski, N. & Joekes, K. (2007). Goal disturbance, cognitive coping and psychological distress in HIV infected persons. Journal of Health Psychology, 12, 225-230.

22864 Veek, Shelley vd.indd 200 23-07-12 15:53

Page 204: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,
Page 205: A Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional ... Psychosocial Perspective on Pediatric Functional Abdominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatment PhD thesis, Academic Medical Center,

A Psychosocial Persp

ective on Pediatric Functional Ab

dominal Pain: Risk Factors and Treatm

ent Shelley M. C

. van der Veek

A Psychosocial Perspective onPediatric

Functional Abdominal Pain:

Shelley M. C. van der Veek

Risk Factors and Treatment

22864 Veek_Omslag 18-07-12 16:04 Pagina 1