a cross-sectional comparison of delay discounting in ... · discount rate is quantified by a...

53
A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN SMOKERS AND NON-SMOKERS WITH SCHIZOPHRENIA AND RESPECTIVE CONTROLS By Taryn Gabrielle Moss A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts Graduate Department of Psychology University of Toronto © Copyright by Taryn Gabrielle Moss 2009

Upload: others

Post on 20-Jun-2020

1 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

Page 1: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN SMOKERS AND NON-SMOKERS WITH SCHIZOPHRENIA AND RESPECTIVE CONTROLS

By

Taryn Gabrielle Moss

A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts

Graduate Department of Psychology University of Toronto

© Copyright by Taryn Gabrielle Moss 2009

Page 2: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

ii A Cross-Sectional Comparison of Delay Discounting in Smokers and Non-Smokers with

Schizophrenia and Respective Control Comparisons

Master of Arts

Taryn Gabrielle Moss

Department of Psychology

University of Toronto

2009

Abstract

Background: Schizophrenia is associated with deficits in decision-making. Aim: To determine

the effects of smoking status on delay discounting in schizophrenia in comparison to non-

psychiatric controls. Method: Cross-sectional comparison of delay discounting across smoking

and psychiatric status. Hypotheses: Individuals with schizophrenia were hypothesized to have

higher rates of delay discounting than controls; Non-smokers with schizophrenia would have

higher rates of delay discounting than smokers. Control smokers would discount future rewards

more than non-smokers. Results: No significant differences in delay discounting were observed

between psychiatric groups. Smokers with schizophrenia exhibited more delay discounting than

non-smokers. Within the psychiatric group, former smokers discount rates were similar to

current smokers. Conclusion: Delay discounting deficits in schizophrenia and modulation by

cigarette smoking were not supported; our pattern of results in schizophrenia does suggest that

deficits in delay discounting in these patients appears to be a trait rather than a state-dependent

phenomenon.

Page 3: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

iii Acknowledgements

I would like to express my gratitude to my supervisor, Dr. Tony P. George, for his

guidance and support, and for providing me with the opportunity to be involved in this research,

as well as various other opportunities throughout my degree. I am grateful to my dedicated

committee members, Dr. Peter Herman and Dr. David Nussbaum for their time and invaluable

feedback on this project and thesis document. Lastly, this thesis would not have been possible

without the hard work and dedication of the following BACDRL lab members: Rusty

Souleymanov, Andrea Woznica, Rachel Rabin, Eleanor Liu, Dominique Morisano, and Enza

Mancuso.

Page 4: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

iv TABLE OF CONTENTS

Page

List of Tables ...................................................................................................................... vii

List of Appendices .............................................................................................................. viii

Chapter 1: Introduction ....................................................................................................... 1

1.1 Statement of Problem………………………………………………………... 1

Chapter 2: Literature Review............................................................................................... 3

2.1 Delay Discounting…………………………………………………………… 3

2.2 Delay Discounting and Schizophrenia………………………………………. 6

2.3 Delay Discounting and Addiction……………………………………………. 8

2.4 Schizophrenia and Cigarette Smoking………………………………………... 10

2.5 Current Study…………………………………………………………………. 11

2.6 Hypotheses ....................................................................................................... 11

Chapter 3: Method .............................................................................................................. 13

3.1 Participants ...................................................................................................... 13

3.2 Measures .......................................................................................................... 14

3.2.1 Estimates of pre-morbid intelligence and clinical stability…………. 14

3.2.2 Smoking Dependence………………………………………………. 15

3.2.3 Decision-Making Measures………………………………………… 15

3.2.4 Executive Function…………………………………………………. 16

3.3 Procedure .............................................................................................. 17

Chapter 4: Results ............................................................................................................... 18

4.1 Demographic Characteristics of Study Sample……………………………… 18

4.2 Cognitive Testing……………………………………………………………. 19

Chapter 5: Discussion ......................................................................................................... 25

5.1 Executive Function ...................................................................................... 25

Page 5: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

v

5.2 Delay Discounting…………............................................................................ 26

5.3 Limitations & Future Directions .................................................................... 28

5.4 General Conclusions ........................................................................................ 29

References ........................................................................................................................... 31

Page 6: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

vi

LIST OF TABLES

Page

Table 1: Clinical and demographic means and standard deviations of the sample………… 19

Table 2: Means and standard deviations of cognitive tasks................................................. 22

Table 3: Differences in delay discounting across the three reward magnitudes.................. 23

Page 7: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

vii LIST OF FIGURES

Page

Figure 1: Hyperbolic discounting curve................................................................................. 5

Figure 2: Relationship between delay discounting, computer tasks, and the brain............. 7

Figure 3: Delay discounting compared across all groups………………………………… 22

Figure 4: Delay discounting compared across smoking history………………………….. 23

Page 8: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

1 CHAPTER 1

INTRODUCTION

1. 1 Statement of Problem

We are continually faced with the need to make a multitude of decisions. These decisions

range in magnitude from buying or renting a home, or simply which foods to consume. The

temporal distance of an outcome is associated with the likelihood that it will be preferred. It has

been demonstrated that the more a reward is delayed, the less subjective worth it holds (Heerey,

Robinson, McMahon, & Gold, 2007; Holt, Green, & Myerson, 2003). The term that has been

coined to describe this phenomenon is Delay Discounting; a reduction in the subjective worth of

a commodity as a function of the amount of and delay to that commodity, or reward (Johnson &

Bickel, 2003). Schizophrenia is a psychiatric disorder, which is associated with difficulty in the

realm of decision-making (Heerey, et al., 2007; Kester, et al., 2006); a core feature of the illness.

Once such example of this impairment is evidenced through their extremely high rates of

cigarette smoking (58-88%) when compared to the Canadian general population (~18%) (Le Foll

& George, 2007).

1.2 Purpose of the Study

The purpose of the current study was to examine delay discounting across four groups,

namely individuals with schizophrenia with and without co-morbid addiction, and their

respective non-schizophrenic controls. This would allow us to replicate previous findings that

those with schizophrenia (Heerey, et al., 2007; Kester, et al., 2006) and cigarette smokers

(Bickel, Odum, & Madden, 1999; Krishnan-Sarin, et al., 2006) delay discount more steeply than

comparison controls. The primary aim of this study was to determine the effects of smoking

status on delay discounting in patients with schizophrenia in comparisons to non-psychiatric

controls. The secondary aim was to determine the relationship between delay discounting and

performance on the Iowa Gambling Task (IGT; Bechara, Damasio, Damasio, & Anderson, 1994)

Page 9: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

2 and Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (WCST; Heaton, Chelune, Talley, & al., 1993) in individuals

with schizophrenia and controls as a function of smoking status. An exploratory element of this

study investigated whether impulsivity, as measured through delay discounting, is a state or trait.

This would be achieved by analyzing whether former smokers are more similar to never or

current smokers.

Page 10: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

3 CHAPTER TWO

LITERATURE REVIEW

2.1 Delay Discounting

Cost, in the form of money, time, or effort, is an essential factor of behavioural

economics. Delay discounting focuses on the time component, specifically, it examines how

temporally distanced events are discounted (Bickel, Marsch, & Carroll, 2000; Bickel, et al.,

2007). The discount rate establishes the steepness of the reduction in the current worth with

increase in delay (Petry, Kirby, & Kranzler, 2002). An example of delay discounting includes

the extent to which an individual would prefer $500 now (impulsivity), as opposed to waiting six

months for $1000 (self-control), with both the length of the delay and magnitude of the reward

influencing our choice.

The extent that delayed rewards are discounted has been investigated in animals

employing adjusting-delay (Mazur, 1988) and adjusting-amount procedures (Richards, Mitchell,

de Wit, & Seiden, 1997). In these trials participants are initially exposed to the forced-choice

trials, which involve both small immediate rewards and large delayed rewards. Then the free-

choice trials begin where participants are permitted to make a choice between two reward

options. If the smaller and more immediate reward is constantly preferred, the larger delayed

reward is made more appealing by lessoning the delay or increasing the magnitude, before the

series is repeated. The choice-dependent adjusting procedure is continued until participants reach

the point of indifference between the small immediate and large delayed rewards, at which time

the magnitude of delay discounting can be calculated (Lagorio & Madden, 2005).

An example of this procedure involves a thirsty rat provided with the choice between

100ml of water presented after a brief delay or a smaller amount offered immediately. If that rat

picks the delayed reward, the amount of water made available on the next trial is increased by

10%; however, if the smaller immediate reward is chosen, the water reward is decreased by 10%

Page 11: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

4 at the next trial. This procedure is repeated with the amount of water being adjusted until the rat

chooses both reward options equally, and the indifference point is reached. The amount of

immediate water consumed at the indifference point is used as the measure of the value of the

standard, and a series of standards can produce this discounting curve (Richards, et al., 1997).

The extent of discounting can be calculated in humans by using a similar choice

procedure in which the reward is also methodically adjusted until the preference for both rewards

is equivalent. A discounting curve can be created if this process is repeated at a variety of

different time points (e.g., 1 week, 1 month, 1 year; Bickel, et al., 2007; Madden, Petry, Badger,

& Bickel, 1997). Behavioural economists have studied delay discounting by measuring the shape

and location of these indifference curves (Kagel, Battalio, & Green, 1995; Madden, et al., 1997).

Studies investigating temporal discounting of delayed rewards have demonstrated that

subjective value does not decrease in a linear fashion with delay duration. Rather, a hyperbolic

association between subjective value and delay has consistently been observed in research

involving humans (Madden, et al., 1997). Hyperbolic discounting is a reduction in the value of

the delayed reward relative to the delay (Ainslie & Haslam, 1992; Bickel, et al., 2007). This

discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987):

In this equation, the Vd represents the discounted value of an outcome, V the

undiscounted value or amount; k is a constant relative to the degree of temporal discounting and

d the delay to the reward. The free parameter, k, offers a measure of the preference for the

smaller reward to the larger delayed reward. The rational theory of addiction (Becker & Murphy,

1988) assumes that delayed rewards are discounted exponentially. This implies that for every

unit of time that represents the delay to the reward, the value of that reward decreases, or is

discounted by a fixed proportion (Bickel & Marsch, 2001; Kirby, Petry, & Bickel, 1999). In

Page 12: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

5 actual fact, hyperbolic discounting has consistently been observed in which for each unit of time,

the reward’s current value decreases by a progressively smaller proportion (see Figure 1) (Bickel

& Marsch, 2001; Kirby, et al., 1999). This equation characterizes these findings as a hyperbolic

relationship between the subjective value of a reward and time until it occurs (Bickel, et al.,

2007).

Fig 1. An example of a hyperbolic curve created in delay discounting experiments. The y-axis represents the indifference point or the value of the smaller reinforcer at which both reinforcers were chosen equally.

Researchers have questioned the validity of using hypothetical rewards to measure delay

discounting (Bickel & Marsch, 2001; Critchfield & Kollins, 2001; Navarick, 2004). Navarick

(2004) has suggested that distinctive discounting processes are used for hypothetical rewards and

for real rewards that may be immediately consumed, as demonstrated by significant differences

in discounting rate across the two options. Kirby (1997) examined the degree of discounting in

real versus hypothetical rewards in a survey of the literature. Kirby found that real rewards were

often discounted less than hypothetical rewards, but emphasized that the size of the reward was

quite influential. Studies that used real rewards tended to use smaller rewards, which may

explain the difference noted by Navarick (Kirby, 1997; Lagorio & Madden, 2005). Real versus

hypothetical rewards were also examined by Johnson and Bickel (2002) who were careful to

Page 13: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

6 avoid reward type and magnitude confounds. This was achieved by adjusting the amounts

procedure within reward type (real or hypothetical), which were manipulated within-participants.

No consistent effects of reward type were found, suggesting that hypothetical rewards may often

serve as a valid proxy for real rewards in delay discounting research, consistent with majority of

research which has failed to find a significant difference between reward type (Baker, Johnson,

& Bickel, 2003; Madden, Begotka, Raiff, & Kastern, 2003). Hypothetical rewards, as a measure

of delay discounting, is acceptable as demonstrated by previous research failing to find

differences in comparison to real rewards. As a result of these findings, hypothetical rewards

were examined in the current study.

2.2 Delay Discounting and Schizophrenia

Psychiatric populations have been linked to decision-making impairments (Dunn,

Dalgleish, & Lawrence, 2006; Jollant, et al., 2007). Specifically, these impairments have been

found in those with obsessive-compulsive disorder (Cavedini, et al., 2002; Lawrence, et al.,

2006); anorexia nervosa (Cavedini, et al., 2004); specific mood disorders (Murphy, et al., 2001;

Must, et al., 2006); schizophrenia (Heerey, et al., 2007; Kester, et al., 2006); as well as

borderline (Bazanis, et al., 2002), antisocial (D. Mitchell, Colledge, Leonard, & Blair, 2002), and

impulsive-aggressive (Best, Williams, & Coccaro, 2002) personality disorders. The current study

focused on delay discounting in individuals diagnosed with schizophrenia.

The orbital frontal cortex (OFC) (see Figure 2) is a region of the association cortex of the

human brain involved in cognitive processes such as decision-making (Rolls, 2000). In healthy

adults, damage to the OFC has been linked to decision-making deficits (Salmond, Menon,

Chatfield, Pickard, & Sahakian, 2005; Tranel, Bechara, & Denburg, 2002). Structural

abnormalities in the OFC have also been observed in the brains of individuals with schizophrenia

(Goldstein, et al., 1999; Meador-Woodruff & Watson, 1997), which may explain their

disadvantageous decision-making patterns. Compromised white-matter (WM) integrity in

Page 14: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

7 inferior frontal WM has also been associated with social dysfunction and impulsivity in people

with schizophrenia (Wolkin, et al., 2003). The Iowa Gambling Task (IGT) (Bechara, et al., 1994)

is sensitive to OFC function as it employs decision-making processes including events that have

emotionally significant consequences, such as gains and losses (Hongwanishkul, Happaney, Lee,

& Zelazo, 2005; Kester, et al., 2006). As a consequence of these structural abnormalities,

individuals with schizophrenia do not perform well on tasks specific to OFC function (Moberg,

et al., 1999), such as the IGT (Shurman, Horan, & Nuechterlein, 2005).

Fig 2. Relationship between delay discounting, computer tasks, and the brain.

Memory has been suggested to play a significant role in delay discounting as well.

Hinson and colleagues (2003) revealed that when the memory load was increased during delay-

discounting tasks, delayed rewards were discounted more steeply. Also, functional imaging

results have shown more activity in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) than ventromedial

regions when a delayed reward is chosen and parts of the limbic system associated with the

midbrain dopamine system, including paralimbic cortex, are activated when an immediate

reward is chosen (McClure, Laibson, Loewenstein, & Cohen, 2004). These findings suggest that

Delay Discounting

Original figure extracted from http://www.nida.nih.gov/

WCST

IGT

WCST – Wisconsin Card Sorting Task IGT – Iowa Gambling Task OFC – Orbital Frontal Cortex SCC – Subcallosal Cortex PFC – Pre Frontal Cortex ACG – Anterior Cingulate Gyrus NAc – Nucleus Accumbens VP – Ventral Pallidum Amyg – Amygdala Hipp - Hippocampus

Page 15: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

8 individual differences in the extent of delay discounting may be accounted for by the relative

contributions of separate neural and cognitive systems in decision making (Heerey, et al., 2007).

Individuals with schizophrenia may discount future rewards more than matched healthy controls

as a result of their compromised memory (Lee & Park, 2005), DLPFC function impairments

(Cannon, et al., 2005), and abnormal dopamine function (Kapur & Remington, 2001; Knable &

Weinberger, 1997).

Heerey and colleagues (2007) investigated delay discounting in schizophrenia by

comparing stable outpatients with schizophrenia (n = 42) to healthy controls (n = 29). Delay

discounting was assessed using the computerized version of the Kirby Delay Discounting Task

(KDDT; Kirby et al., 1999). The hypothesis that individuals with schizophrenia would discount

more than healthy controls was confirmed. Also, those psychiatric participants with better

memory function and higher levels of negative symptoms were found to discount future rewards

less severely. These findings suggest that individuals with schizophrenia may be predisposed to

choose immediate rewards while discounting delayed rewards.

2.3 Delay Discounting and Addiction

The discounting model of impulsiveness (Ainslie, 1975) suggests that delay-discounting

rates are positively correlated with impulsiveness, which validates using delay discounting as a

measure of impulsivity, a common feature among substance abusers (Kirby, et al., 1999).

Present-focused behaviour would be represented in this previously mentioned discounting

equation by a larger k value (Bickel, et al., 2007). As a result, a smaller k value would represent

less impulsivity and more future-directed planning. Impulsivity, acting momentarily, is a factor

that largely influences the risk for addiction. It involves a reduced capability to inhibit unsuitable

behaviours, the propensity to act without planning ahead and without considering the

behavioural consequences of one’s actions (Kreek, Nielsen, Butelman, & LaForge, 2005;

Krishnan-Sarin, et al., 2007). These impulsive choices may prevail in chronic substance abusers,

Page 16: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

9 as they often choose immediate and brief drug intoxication over tangible and social rewards.

Also, those who abuse substances often experience delayed aversive consequences, including

high prevalence of illnesses, loss of employment, and more frequent interpersonal problems with

friends and family (Madden, et al., 1997). Intravenous drug users may decide to share

hypodermic needless rather than spending the time to disinfect or obtain new needles (Bickel &

Marsch, 2001; Normand, Vlahov, & Moses, 1995).

Drugs with addictive properties have been suggested to compel maladaptive decision-

making through pharmacological interactions with neurophysiological mechanisms developed

for regular learning systems (Berke, 2003; Everitt, Dickinson, & Robbins, 2001; Redish, Jensen,

& Johnson, 2008). It is the interaction between the normal learning systems and the reward

distribution of specific behaviours that lead to these addictive patterns (Dowling, Smith, &

Thomas, 2005; Redish, et al., 2008; Redish, Jensen, Johnson, & Kurth-Nelson, 2007). Substance

abusing populations have been found to discount future rewards more than controls. This has

been demonstrated by increased temporal discounting rates observed in opioid-dependents

(Kirby, et al., 1999; Madden, et al., 1997), cocaine-dependents (Coffey, Gudleski, Saladin, &

Brady, 2003), alcoholics (Petry, 2001a), problem and pathological gamblers (Alessi & Petry,

2003; Petry, 2001b), and cigarette smokers (Baker, et al., 2003; Bickel, et al., 1999; S. Mitchell,

1999). These findings suggest that this behavioural process may be a prerequisite for addiction

(Bickel, et al., 2007). The present study investigated delay discounting among cigarette smokers

with and without schizophrenia.

Madden and colleagues (1997) were the first to explore delay discounting in substance-

abusing populations. Their study involved both opioid-dependent and control participants. All

involved had a choice between hypothetical monetary rewards, either instantly available or after

a delay. Delayed rewards were in the amount of $1000, whereas the immediate reward was

adjusted until choices reflected indifference. This step was repeated at 7 different delay periods,

Page 17: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

10 ranging from 1 week to 25 years. Those in the opioid-dependent group were given another series

of choices between immediate and delayed heroin. Opioid-dependent individuals discounted

delayed monetary rewards significantly more than controls, and discounted delayed heroin

significantly more than delayed money. The subjective values of the delayed rewards were

discounted more at shorter delays, which left a modest amount of room to further discount at

larger delays. In a follow-up a study (Kirby et al., 1999), these differences were observed when

real rewards were available to participants, rather than hypothetical rewards.

The question has been raised of whether delay discounting is a state or trait dependent

phenomenon. There are disparaging findings within the literature regarding discounting status

with respect to those with current and former addiction, as well as those who never suffered from

an addiction. Previous research on non-psychiatric participants has demonstrated that never and

former smokers discounted similarly, with current smokers discounting the most (Bickel, et al.,

1999). An investigation among alcoholics revealed they had the highest discounting rates,

followed by abstinent alcoholics, and then controls (Petry, 2001a). An unpublished thesis found

similar results in psychiatric cocaine users, where current users discounted significantly more

than formers users, followed by non-users (Marcello, 2007). Further investigation into whether

delay discounting is a state or trait phenomena would facilitate deeper understanding of its

stability overtime and post recovery from an addiction.

2.4 Schizophrenia and Cigarette Smoking

People with schizophrenia have smoking rates three times those of the general population

(George, et al., 2008; Kalman, Morissette, & George, 2005; Weinberger, Reutenauer, et al.,

2007). Research suggests that cigarette smoking can ameliorate clinical and neurocognitive

deficits associated with this illness (Dolan, et al., 2004; George, et al., 2008; George, Vessicchio,

Termine, Bregartner, et al., 2002; Smith, Singh, Infante, Khandat, & Kloos, 2002). This is

achieved though nicotine which may decrease or normalize attentional and haloperidol-induced

Page 18: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

11 working memory deficits (Dolan, et al., 2004; Freedman, et al., 1997; Levin, Wilson, Rose, &

McEvoy, 1996). Visuospatial Working Memory (VSWM) is partially mediated by DA function

in the PFC, which is known to be dysregulated in schizophrenia. Smoking abstinence has been

found to worsen VSWM deficits in smokers with schizophrenia, but not in control smokers

(George, Vessicchio, Termine, Bregartner, et al., 2002). This may provide a partial explanation

for the higher prevalence of tobacco smoking among these patients compared to the general

population.

2.5 Current Study

The current study was a cross-sectional design that defined comparison groups on the

basis of current smoking and psychiatric status. These four conditions include the following

groups: smokers with schizophrenia (SS), non-smokers with schizophrenia (SNS), healthy

control smokers (CS) and healthy control non-smokers (CNS). To our knowledge, no cross-

sectional studies examining delay discounting among these comparison groups have previously

been conducted.

2.6 Hypotheses

To summarize, our primary goal of this study was to determine the effects of smoking

status on delay discounting in patients with schizophrenia in comparisons to non-psychiatric

controls. As a consequence of brain abnormalities among individuals with schizophrenia, and

consistent with previous research (Heerey, et al., 2007; Kester, et al., 2006), it was hypothesized

that:

1. Individuals with schizophrenia would discount rewards to a greater extent than

control comparison groups.

2. Among the non-schizoprenic group, those with a smoking addiction were

hypothesized to discount future rewards more than those without an addiction, which

is also inline with previous findings (Bickel, et al., 1999; Madden, et al., 2003).

Page 19: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

12 3. The opposite prediction was made for the group with schizophrenia, as those with a

co-morbid smoking addiction were hypothesized to delay discount less than non-

smokers, as a result of the neurocognitive benefits of nicotine in this population

(Dolan, et al., 2004; George, et al., 2008; George, et al., 2002; Smith, Singh, Infante,

Khandat, & Kloos, 2002).

The secondary aim of this study was to determine the relationship between delay

discounting and performance on tasks of executive function, IGT and the WCST, in individuals

with schizophrenia and controls as a function of smoking status. We hypothesized that:

3. Delay discounting would be positively correlated with performance on the IGT and

WCST in those with schizophrenia, and negatively correlated with performance in

controls.

2.7 Significance

This research is of significant value as it is the first cross-sectional comparison of delay

discounting in dually diagnosed smokers and non-smokers with schizophrenia and their

respective non-schizophrenic controls. This research facilitates a deeper understanding of the

modulating role that nicotine plays in decision making for individuals with schizophrenia who

are already vulnerable to decision-making impairments. This study generates clinical suggestions

for understanding why psychiatric patients struggle with smoking cessation and how

interventions can focus on developing enhanced behavioural inhibitory processes, target

impulsive decision making in therapy, and use medications which decrease impulsive behaviours

(Krishnan-Sarin, et al., 2007).

Page 20: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

13 CHAPTER THREE

METHODS

3.1 Participants

Ninety-one individuals between the ages of 18 and 55 were recruited over a 6-month

period. Six outliers were not included in the analyses, as their responding on the delay-

discounting task was inconsistent (see Appendix A). Excluding these outliers, our final sample

(N =85) included n = 47 psychiatric participants (SS & SNS) and n = 38 non-psychiatric controls

(CNS & CS). Recruitment procedures of non-psychiatric participants, involving newspaper

advertisements and Internet postings, were aimed at community members within the Greater

Toronto Area. Stable outpatients at the Centre for Addiction and Mental Health (CAMH) were

recruited through mean of flyers, word-of-mouth, and referrals. Psychiatric participants met

diagnostic criteria for either schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder based on the Fourth

Edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR) (American

Psychiatric Association, 2000) and were capable of providing consent. These subjects were all

psychiatrically stable at the time of interview with a score < 70 on the Positive and Negative

Syndrome Scale for Schizophrenia (PANSS; (Kay, Fiszbein, & Opler, 1987), and if on

medication, been on a stable dose of medication for at least one month prior to the study

assessments. Control participants did not meet criteria for any current Axis I disorders assessed

by the Structural Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR (SCID; First, Spitzer, Robert, Gibbon, &

Williams, 1996).

Medtox urine toxicology screens were used to screen for illicit drugs (Cannabinoids,

Opiates, Amphetamine, Phencyclidine, Barbiturates, Benzodiazepines). Participants in both

groups were excluded if they have abused or were dependent on alcohol or illicit drugs in the

past six months prior to study enrollment.

Smoking status was assessed via self-report (cigarettes smoked per day) and

Page 21: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

14 biochemically verified with expired breath carbon monoxide levels ≥10 parts per million (ppm).

Level of nicotine dependence was measured with the Fagerstrom Test of Nicotine Dependence

(FNTD) (Heatherton, Kozlowski, Frecker, & Fagerstrom, 1991). Eligible smokers reported

smoking ≥10 cigarettes per day on average, with expired CO levels ≥10 ppm, and FTND scores

≥ 4. Non-smokers were included with CO levels < 10 ppm and were either abstinent from

cigarettes for ≥ 6 months (former smokers) or never smoked in their lifetime, and were not

currently using any nicotine replacement products.

3.2 Measures

3.2.1 Estimates of pre-morbid intelligence and clinical stability

All participants were administered the National Adult Reading Test (Nelson & Willison,

1991) in order to obtain an estimate of premorbid intelligence. Also, the Positive and Negative

Syndrome Scale for Schizophrenia (Kay, et al., 1987) interview was administered to the

psychiatric participants during the screening interview in order to confirm clinical stability.

Participants were deemed clinically stable if their score on the PANSS was less than 70.

National Adult Reading Test (Nelson & Willison, 1991). The NART is a list of 50 words

presented in order of escalating reading difficulty. The words are unusual with respect to the

common rules of pronunciation, which reduces the likelihood of the subject reading by phonemic

decoding rather than by word recognition. The NART is one of the most commonly used tests of

word-reading ability. This measure is extensively used in schizophrenia research and is presented

as both a method of estimating premorbid IQ and a quick way of assessing current IQ (Russell, et

al., 2000).

Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale for Schizophrenia (Kay, et al., 1987). The PANSS

is a widely used medical scale that measures symptoms severity of patients with schizophrenia.

This 30-item rating instrument evaluates the presence or absence and severity of positive,

negative and general psychopathology of schizophrenia. All items are rated on a 7-point scale

Page 22: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

15 (1=absent; 7=extreme). The PANSS has good inter-rater reliability (Kay, Opler, & Lindenmayer,

1988), adequate construct validity (Kay, et al., 1987; Kay, et al., 1988), and high internal

reliability (Kay, et al., 1987). Internal reliability of the PANSS for the positive, negative, and

general psychopathology scales have Cronbach’s alpha of .80, .82, and .82, respectively.

3.2.2. Smoking Dependence

Fagerstrom Test of Nicotine Dependence (Heatherton, et al., 1991) The FTND is a brief, 6-item

scale that yields a score between 0 and 10, assessing smokers’ level of nicotine dependence.

Individuals respond to multiple-choice type questions, with each answer corresponding to a score

and higher scores indicating higher levels of dependence. Examples of items include “How

many cigarettes do you smoke per day?” and “Do you smoke if you are so ill that you are in bed

most of the day?” The FTND is one of the most commonly used measures to assess nicotine

dependence, and has acceptable reliability for use among smokers with and without

schizophrenia (Weinberger, Reutenauer, et al., 2007). Weinberger et al. (2007) report the internal

consistency of the FTND to be equal to Cronbach’s alpha of .74 and test-retest reliability equal

to .65 in the psychiatric smokers. These researchers report similar findings in control smokers,

with Cronbach’s alpha of .72 and test-retest reliability of .82.

3.2.3. Decision-making measures

The Kirby Delay Discounting Task (KDDT; Kirby, et al., 1999). The KDDT is a 27-item

questionnaire that assesses discounting of hypothetical monetary amounts across three different

delayed-reward magnitudes: small ($25 - $35), medium ($50 - $60), and large ($75 - $85).

Examples of items include “Would you prefer $100 today, or $101 in 300 days?” and “Would

you prefer $20 today, or $55 in 7 days?” K-values are calculated and are based on the extent to

which the respondents choose smaller immediate rewards rather than larger delayed rewards.

The KDDT has decent internal consistency among discount rates for the three different reward

magnitudes, and adequate test-retest reliability (r = .71) (Kirby, 2009).

Page 23: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

16 3.2.4. Executive Function

Iowa Gambling Task - Computerized (IGT; Bechara, et al., 1994). The IGT is a widely

used measure that is highly sensitive to measuring impaired decision-making in a variety of

neurological and psychiatric conditions, including schizophrenia (Bechara, et al., 1994). All

participants sat facing a computer screen which displayed four decks of cards (A, B, C, and D)

and were told to pick a card from the decks one at a time. They were informed they would

receive a monetary reward or penalty for every card they choose, and that the goal of the game is

to maximize profits on a $2000 loan they will receive before they begin the task. The subjects

were permitted to pick from any deck and to switch decks at any time. Subjects were allowed to

take as much time as they needed to complete this task.

The four decks in the IGT vary in the amount and ratio of reward to penalty that each

provides. Decks A and B initially offer large monetary rewards, but are disadvantageous because

some selections from these decks are accompanied by large monetary penalties. Decks C and D

offer smaller monetary rewards, but also involve smaller penalties. Previous research has

suggested that individuals with schizophrenia are significantly impaired on IGT in comparison to

healthy controls, as they earn significantly lower scores and make more disadvantageous picks

(Beninger, et al., 2003; Kester, et al., 2006), resulting in less hypothetical monetary winnings by

the end of the task.

Wisconsin Card Sorting Task (WCST; Heaton, Chelune, Talley, & al., 1993). The WCST

is an extensively used measure that assesses executive functions, including planning and set-

shifting. Outcome measures include number of categories completed, percent total errors, percent

perseverative errors, percent non-perseverative errors, and number of trials to complete first

category. Performance on this task has been linked with activation of the dorsolateral prefrontal

cortex (Egan, et al., 2001). Reliability of the WCST, interscorer and intrascorer agreements have

Page 24: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

17 been found to be excellent. Since this task assesses decision-making ability, poor performance on

this task should be consistent with high levels of delay discounting.

3.3 Procedure

At the screening visit, the consent forms were reviewed and signed, and eligibility was

determined using the SCID-IV, Medtox™ urine toxicology screen, Expired Breath Carbon

Monoxide (CO) monitoring, and the PANSS for the psychiatric participants. These assessments

were conducted by specially trained Bachelor’s- and Master’s-level psychology students.

Participants whom met eligibility criteria were invited back for a cognitive-testing session. This

cognitive session involved the KDDT, the IGT, and then the WCST. During both visits,

participants were permitted breaks as often as they required. Smokers were asked to notify the

research staff of how many cigarettes they smoked on their break. After the sessions were

completed, participants were compensated for their time at a rate of $10 per hour. Completion

time was approximately three hours for the screening visit and an additional two hours for the

cognitive session, totaling five hours. Those who completed this study received a total of $50.

This protocol was reviewed and approved by the Centre for Addiction and Mental Health

Research Ethics Board (CAMH-REB).

Page 25: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

18 CHAPTER 4

RESULTS

4.1 Demographic Characteristics of Study Sample

As noted, once outliers were excluded (see Appendix A), the study population consisted

of 85 subjects, including 47 psychiatric participants (SS; n = 25 & SNS; n = 22) and 38 non-

psychiatric control comparisons (CS; n = 17 & CNS; n = 21). The study adopted the

conventional 0.05 alpha level as evidence of statistical significance. The demographic and

clinical characteristics of the four groups are presented in Table 1. A one-way analysis of

variance (ANOVA) was utilized in order to compare continuous demographic and clinical

variables and Chi Square analyses for categorical variables across the four groups. There were no

significant differences between the four diagnostic groups (SS, SNS, CS, or CNS) in terms of

race (ns); however, there was for gender, X2 (3)= 1.88, p < .001. There was also a significant

difference in level of education completed, F (3, 81) = 26.7 (p < .001), and full scale IQ F, (3,

80) = 11.7 (p < .001) across groups (CNS > CS > SNS > SS). Furthermore, a significant age

difference across the four groups was observed, F (3, 81) = 3.4 (p < .05). Independent-samples t-

tests revealed that both psychiatric and non-psychiatric smokers had comparable levels of

nicotine dependence as measured on the FTND, and similar numbers of cigarettes smoked per

day (ns). Smoking behaviors defined by CO levels were significantly higher in the psychiatric

smoker group as compared to the non-psychiatric control smokers, t (40) = -2.06, p = .046,

consistent with previous research (Weinberger, Sacco, et al., 2007).

Page 26: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

19 Table 1.

Clinical and demographic means and standard deviations of the sample

Schizophrenia Controls Statistics

Smoker (n = 25)

Non-Smoker (n = 22)

Smoker (n = 17) Non-Smoker

(n = 21)

All groups

Control vs. Psychiatric Individuals

Control

Smokers vs. Control Non-

Smokers

Psychiatric Smokers

vs. Psychiatric

Non- Smokers

Age M (SD) 39.6 (9.2) 34.6 (9.9) 38.3 (8.0) 32.9 (8.1) p = .05 p = .36 p = .05 p = .08

Sex (% male) 88% (22) 64% (14) 59% (10) 40% (6) p < .001* p = .001** p = .06 p = .008**

Race

Caucasian 18 10 15 13

African-American 3 2 1 2

Asian 1 5 0 2

Hispanic 0 0 0 1

Other 3 5 2 3

p = .25

p = .42

p = .39

p = .14

Education 11.8 (1.7) 14.0 (1.9) 14.9 (1.6) 17.0 (2.4) p < .001* p < .001** p = .004** p < .001**

Estimated Full-Scale IQ 104.5 (7.7) 110.2 (7.6) 112.9 (4.9) 115.4 (4.1) p < .001* p < .001** p = .10 p = .02*

# Cigs per day 20.5 (6.0) - 18.4 (6.0) - - p = .20 - -

Baseline Carbon Monoxide Level (ppm) 21.9 (10.9) .73 (1.1) 15.9 (6.2) .38 (0.8)

p < .001*

p = .07

- -

FTND 6.3 (1.5) - 5.7 (1.9) - - p =.22 - -

PANSS Positive 13.5 (3.2) 15.5 (4.0) - ‐ - - - p = .07

PANSS Negative 13.2 (4.0) 11.2 (3.3) - ‐ - - - p = .07

PANSS General 24.5 (6.4) 25.3 (4.3) - - - - - p = .64

PANSS Total 51.7 (10.0) 52.0 (7.9) - - - - - p = .93

Page 27: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

20 Medication

Atypical 92% (23) 91% (20) ‐ ‐ - - -

Typical 8% (2) 9% (2) ‐ ‐ - - -

p = .90

Medication Name

Clozapine

40% (10) 36% (8) ‐ ‐ - - - -

Risperdone

28% (7) 23% 5 ‐ ‐ - - - -

Loxapine

4% (1) 5% (1) ‐ ‐ - - - -

Quetiapine

4% (1) 0% (0) ‐ ‐ - - - -

Olanzapine

20% (5) 18% (4) ‐ ‐ - - - -

Fluphenazine 0% (0) 5% (1) ‐ ‐ - - - -

Note. *p < .05, two-tailed. **p < .01 two tailed.

4.2 Cognitive Testing

The means and standard deviations for the outcomes on the cognitive tasks are provided

in Table 2. An independent-samples t-test was conducted in order to compare IGT performance

in psychiatric patients and control comparisons. On average, psychiatric participants performed

significantly worse on the IGT (net score) than did controls, t (82) = 3.6, p = .001. A similar

pattern was observed in the WCST, with the psychiatric participants displaying poorer

performance on the WCST, with more perseverative errors, t (83) = -4.3, p < .001 and fewer

categories completed, t (83) = 4.0, p = .001.

Bivariate correlations were conducted in order to investigate the relationship between

IGT net scores and delay discounting. When examined separately (controls versus patients), no

Page 28: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

21 significant correlations were observed. However, when psychiatric status was collapsed across

groups, IGT net scores were negatively correlated with overall discounting rates, r (85) = -.22, p

< .05. Additional bivariate correlations were run to examine the relationship between the WCST

and delay discounting. Both perseverative responses and errors were positively associated with

delay discounting in controls, r (38) = .42, and patients, r (47) = .37, ps < .01.

A one-way ANOVA was employed to examine differences on IGT and WCST

performance across the four groups (SNS, SS, CNS, CS). A significant effect of group on IGT

net total scores was observed, F (3, 81) = 4.5, p = .006. Bonferonni post hoc tests revealed that

SS performed significantly worse than CS (p < .05). No other groups were significantly different.

As well, there was a significant group effect on the WCST perseverative responses, F (3, 81) =

9.1 and errors, F (3, 81) = 8.5, ps = .000, and categories completed F (3, 81) = 5.8, p = .001.

Bonferonni post hoc tests revealed that for perseverative responses and errors, SS had

significantly higher scores than CNS and CS (ps < .001). CNS and CS completed significantly

more categories than SS (ps < .05). SNS were not statistically different from the other groups,

and responded similarly to SS on all WCST categories.

Page 29: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

22

Table 2.

Means and Standard Deviations of Cognitive Tasks

Note. **p < .01 two tailed.

A two-way between-subjects ANOVA was conducted in order to evaluate the effect of

psychiatric (schizophrenia, control) and smoking (smoker, non-smoker) status on delay

discounting (see Figure 3). The covariate, years of education, was significantly related to delay

discounting, F (1, 80) = 4.0, p = .005. There was a significant main effect of psychiatric status on

overall delay discounting score after controlling for the effect of education, F (1, 80) = 3.9, p =

.05, partial η2 = .05, with controls discounting future rewards more than patients. No significant

results were observed for the main effect of smoking status (ns).

Schizophrenia Control

Smoker (n = 25)

Non-Smoker (n = 22)

Smoker (n = 17)

Non-Smoker (n = 21) P Value

Small log k -3.81 (1.4) -4.57 (1.5) -4.01 (1.0) 3.81 (1.1) .18

Medium log k -4.44 (1.7) -4.99 (1.3) -4.39 (1.3) -4.44 (1.2) .46

Large log k -5.20 (1.6) -5.28 (1.1) -5.28 (1.1) -4.92 (1.1) .66

Geometric k 1.96 (1.9) 1.12 (1.0) 1.81 (1.7) 1.89 (1.6) .27

IGT Net -4.40 (25) -1.27 (22.3) 16.24 (25.1) 16.67 (25.1)

.005**

WCST – Categories Completed

3.12 (2.4) 3.63 (2.6) 5.12 (1.9) 5.38 (1.5)

.002**

WCST – Perseverative

Responses 38.88 (27.4) 25.36 (23.0) 12.47 (11.9) 12.76 (9.7)

.000**

WCST – Perseverative

Errors 32.16 (21.2) 22.05 (18.2) 11.76 (10.7) 12.05 (9.1)

.000**

Page 30: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

23

Figure 3. Delay discounting compared across all groups.

An additional between-subjects ANOVA was employed in order to examine the effects of

psychiatric status and smoking history (former, never, current smoker) on delay discounting,

while controlling for years of education, see Table 3 for means and standard deviations. No

significant main effects of psychiatric status or smoking history were observed (ns); however,

there was a trend towards an interaction effect between these variables on delay discounting F (2,

77) = 7.1 (p = .05), partial η2 =.07. Post hoc analyses (Bonferroni) revealed significant

differences between psychiatric current smokers and never smokers (p < .05), all other

differences were non-significant (former > current > never) (see Figure 4). Interestingly, there

were no observable significant differences between groups among the control participants (never

> current > former).

Page 31: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

24

Table 3: Delay Discounting Across Smoking History

Fig 4. Comparison of delay discounting between psychiatric and controls participants across smoking history.

An independent samples t-test was conducted in order to examine differences between

Smoking History

Small log k Medium log k Large log k Geometric k

Never (n = 15) -5.0 (1.5) -5.4 (1.2) -5.47 (1.2) 0.75 (0.6)

Former (n =6) -3.5 (1.1) -3.8 (0.7) -4.77 (0.7) 2.10 (1.1) Psychiatric Group

(n = 45)

Current (n = 24) -3.8 (1.4) -4.44 (1.5) -5.20 (1.6) 2.00 (1.9)

Never (n =18) -3.7 (1.2) -4.41 (1.3) -4.91 (1.2) 2.03 (1.7)

Former (n = 3) -4.3 (0.6) -4.63 (.00) -4.93 (0.5) 1.01 (0.3) Controls (n = 38)

Current (n =17) -4.0 (1.0) -4.39 (1.3) -4.89 (1.0) 1.81 (1.7)

Page 32: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

25 smoking cessation treatment seeking (TS) psychiatric participants versus non-treatment seeking

(NTS) individuals, on delay discounting. Those who were considered treatment seeking were

recruited from an ongoing smoking-cessation clinical trial within the same laboratory. These

individuals were motivated to quit smoking, as demonstrated by their self-enrollment in the

smoking-cessation trial. Interestingly, no significant differences were observed between the TS

(M =1.38, SD = 1.4) and NTS (M =2.59, SD = 2.2), t (18) = -1.45 (ns).

Additional bivariate correlations were run in order to examine relationships between

individual characteristics (FTND, cigarettes smoked per day, age, years of education, and IQ)

and delay discounting. No significant relationships were observed. However, when psychiatric

symptoms were examined, a positive significant correlation was found between negative

symptoms within schizophrenia, and delay discounting of medium reward sizes, r (47) = .42, p <

.01. No other significant correlations were observed with the other reward sizes, or positive

symptoms of schizophrenia.

A mixed factorial ANOVA was employed to examine differences in delay discounting

across the three reward magnitudes (small, medium, large), between the controls and patients. A

main effect of reward size was observed, small (M = -4.05, SD = 1.3), medium (M = -4.58, SD =

1.4), and large (M = -5.09, SD = 1.2), F (2, 166) = 34.3, p < .001, partial η2 =.29, but not of

psychiatric status (ns).

Page 33: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

26 CHAPTER 5

DISCUSSION

5.1 Executive Function

There are conflicting finding examining psychiatric patients performance on the IGT.

Although some studies have found no significant deficits (Cavallaro, et al., 2003; Evans,

Bowman, & Turnbull, 2005; Martino, Bucay, Butman, & Allegri, 2007; Turnbull, Evans,

Kemish, Park, & Bowman, 2006), our findings are consistent with those studies that found that

psychiatric participants exhibit deficits in decision-making, as evidenced by their poor

performance on the IGT (Beninger, et al., 2003; Kester, et al., 2006; Shurman, et al., 2005; Yip,

Sacco, George, & Potenza, 2009). These findings suggest a considerable diagnostic-group effect

on risk/reward decision-making (Yip, et al., 2009).

Within both the psychiatric and control group, there were no significant differences in

decision-making deficits between smokers and non-smokers, although smokers in both groups

demonstrated poorer performance (lower IGT net total scores). While several studies suggest that

nicotine effects of cigarette smoking can ameliorate performance deficits on neuropsychological

tasks in individuals with schizophrenia (Depatie, et al., 2002; George, et al., 2006; George,

Vessicchio, Termine, Sahady, et al., 2002; Sacco, et al., 2006; Sacco, et al., 2005), our

hypothesis that smokers with schizophrenia would perform better on the IGT was not confirmed.

Yip et al. (2009) also found no significant differences between psychiatric smokers and non-

smokers; however they did find that female smokers with schizophrenia outperformed their male

counterparts. Interestingly, the smokers with schizophrenia in the present study displayed (non-

significantly) poorer performance than the non-smokers.

The hypotheses that delay discounting would be positively correlated with performance

on the IGT in those with schizophrenia and negatively correlated with IGT scores in controls

were not confirmed. Rather, IGT performance was negatively correlated with delay discounting

Page 34: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

27 when combining the psychiatric and control participants scores. This suggests that for both

groups, those who demonstrate superior risk/reward decisions are more inclined to consider to

long-term consequences.

The WCST assesses tactical planning, utilizing environmental feedback to shift cognitive

sets, directing behavior toward achieving a goal, and adjusting impulsive responding, all

implicated in the realm of executive functioning and ultimately decision-making. The controls

outperformed the patients, as evidenced through their lower scores on perseverative responses

and errors, and higher numbers of categories completed on the WCST. This further supports the

notion that psychiatric patients suffer from deficits in decision-making, and may explain some of

the poor choices they make (e.g., high rates of cigarette smoking). Also, performance on the

WCST was associated with delay discounting for both the controls and patients, such that as

perseverative errors increased on the WCST, so did the k value on the delay discounting task,

meaning higher rates of delay discounting. Similar to our findings on the IGT, performance on

these tasks of executive functioning is related to the magnitude of delay discounting. Although

we had hypothesized this finding for controls but not for psychiatric participants, this finding is

logical since both of these groups had similar patterns of delay discounting.

5.2 Delay Discounting

Although we have already demonstrated decision-making deficits in those with

schizophrenia (poor performance on measures of executive function), we further analyzed these

deficits by looking at the degree to which small immediate rewards were chosen over larger

delayed rewards. While patients did not respond to questions of delay discounting differently

than the controls, they demonstrated lower levels of delay discounting [after educational level

was covaried out], which is opposite to what was hypothesized and inconsistent with previous

findings (Heerey, et al., 2007). The control non-smokers in the present study responded

similarly to the control smokers, which is surprising since numerous studies have reported that

Page 35: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

28 smokers discount the future more than non-smokers (Baker, et al., 2003; Reynolds, 2004). This

may have contributed to the atypical findings that controls are more impulsive than the patients.

When delay discounting was compared across the groups defined on smoking history

(never, former, and current smoker), a trend towards significance was observed between patients

and controls. Within the psychiatric participants, current and former smokers have comparable

levels of delay discounting, but current smokers significantly discounted future rewards more

when compared with never smokers. This parallels previous research examining controls with

addiction that suggests that current abusers are more impulsive than non-users (Bickel, et al.,

1999; Coffey, et al., 2003; Kirby, et al., 1999; Madden, et al., 1997). The opposite pattern of

delay discounting was demonstrated in the controls, although differences were non-significant.

This finding in controls was surprising, once it again it may be a result of the unusually

impulsive control non-smokers in this sample, or the small sample size of the control former

smokers (n = 3). The present findings suggest that within psychiatric individuals, delay

discounting may be a trait, rather than a state marker, since former and current smokers

responded similarly. Also, when treatment-seeking psychiatric participants were compared to

non-treatment seeking individuals, no significant differences in delay discounting were observed,

which further supports the notion that delay discounting is a trait.

A relationship between negative symptoms of schizophrenia and delay discounting of

medium reward sizes was observed, such that those with more intense negative symptoms had

higher delay-discounting scores, meaning that they are more present-orientated. Heerey and

colleagues (2007) also investigated the relationship between psychiatric symptoms and delay

discounting, and consistent with the present study, found a significant relationship only between

medium reward sizes and negative symptoms. In contrast to the present findings, they found an

inverse relationship, with more intense negative symptoms being associated with less delay

Page 36: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

29 discounting. Although these two studies demonstrated contrasting findings, both confirm a

relationship between symptom severity and delay discounting.

Both controls and patients similarly discount small rewards the most, followed by

medium and large reward sizes. This suggests that within delay discounting, the size of the

reward does play a role. Although someone may choose an immediate small reward over delayed

small or medium rewards, a majority of people will wait if the payoff is large enough. This

would have treatment implications because the focus should be on the smaller immediate

rewards, but when focusing on the future, the emphasis should be on larger rewards, as they are

more influential in the decision-making process.

5.3 Limitations & Future Directions

This study is not without its limitations. First, power was inadequate as a result of our

small sample. Each subgroup (SNS, SS, CS, CNS) had relatively small sample sizes (with ns <

25), which prevented further sub-analyses. Future studies would benefit from increasing the

sample size within each comparison group, and controlling for an equal spread of current,

former, and never smokers in order to further investigate whether impulsivity is a state versus a

trait marker. Another constraint to this study was the measurement of expired breath carbon

monoxide (CO) levels, which occurred only at the screening visit, rather than again on the day of

cognitive testing. Also, smoke breaks were offered but not required for subjects participating in

these assessments. As a result, we were not able to ensure that the smokers were satiated with

nicotine at the time of testing. Furthermore, there were no measures of craving and withdrawal,

which would have informed us about tobacco satiation/deprivation. Although these limitations

were recognized while recruitment was still ongoing, the second half of psychiatric-smoker

sample was satiated, because they were required to smoke immediately before the cognitive-

testing session. Although no significant differences were observed between the first half and

second half of psychiatric smokers, enforcing smoke breaks and measuring CO levels on the day

Page 37: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

30 of testing would ensure that all smokers are satiated, and would significantly improve the

methodology of this study.

The psychiatric participants in this study were all medicated and demonstrated relatively

low to moderate levels of psychosis, as demonstrated by their PANSS scores. As well, their IQ

level and education are quite high for this specific population, which may have contributed to our

non-significant findings. Enforcing a minimal PANSS total score as an inclusion criteria could

ensure more chronic patients, and should be considered for future research.

This present study focused on cigarette dependence rather than other types of substance

dependence. Although cigarette smokers have demonstrated greater impulsivity when compared

with non-smokers (Geist & Herrmann, 1990; Waldeck & Miller, 1997), they do not experience

major life disruptions (e.g., loss of employment) as a result of their nicotine dependence, whereas

someone with a drug or alcohol addiction may (Bickel, et al., 1999). Therefore, our findings

might have been different had we explored other substances. Substance abuse or dependence,

other than cigarettes, was an exclusion criterion for this study. It would be interesting for future

studies to examine differences between psychiatric and controls participants who suffer from

alternate addictions, as well as poly-substance use.

5.4 General Conclusions

Although this study reconfirms that psychiatric patients make disadvantageous decisions

in comparison to controls, this difference may not be reflected in abnormalities in delay

discounting. This is, to our knowledge, the first cross-sectional studies to attempt to elucidate the

relationship between delay discounting in smokers and non-smokers, with and without co-

morbid schizophrenia. Results were unanticipated, inasmuch as smokers with schizophrenia

responded similarly to control smokers of previous studies (Baker, et al., 2003; Bickel, et al.,

1999). This finding is incongruent with our hypothesis that smokers would discount less because

of the neurocognitive benefits of nicotine. However, our results suggest that delay discounting is

Page 38: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

31 a trait rather than a state marker, which might explain this finding, as former and current smokers

may be more impulsive, regardless of current smoking status.

This study may have clinical implications for providing a deeper understanding for why

psychiatric patients have a predisposition to higher rates of smoking compared to the general

population, and more difficulties with quitting smoking. Smokers with schizophrenia focus on

immediate gratification (i.e., smoking), rather than the long-term risks (i.e., health problems,

death). Findings from the present study suggest that smoking-cessation treatments should focus

on the small immediate rewards of cessation (i.e., becoming more socially acceptable, avoiding

the unpleasant smell of smoke), while highlighting the profound long-term benefits of cessation,

such as longer life expectancy and financial savings, for a group of patients with significant

socioeconomic disadvantages, and medical morbidity and mortality due to tobacco-related

disease burden.

Page 39: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

32 References

Ainslie, G. (1975). Specious reward: a behavioral theory of impulsiveness and impulse control.

Psychol Bull, 82(4), 463-496.

Ainslie, G., & Haslam, N. (1992). Hyperbolic discounting. New York: Russell Sage Foundation.

Alessi, S. M., & Petry, N. M. (2003). Pathological gambling severity is associated with

impulsivity in a delay discounting procedure. Behav Processes, 64(3), 345-354.

American Psychiatric Association (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders

(Revised 4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

Baker, F., Johnson, M. W., & Bickel, W. K. (2003). Delay discounting in current and never-

before cigarette smokers: similarities and differences across commodity, sign, and

magnitude. J Abnorm Psychol, 112(3), 382-392.

Bazanis, E., Rogers, R. D., Dowson, J. H., Taylor, P., Meux, C., Staley, C., et al. (2002).

Neurocognitive deficits in decision-making and planning of patients with DSM-III-R

borderline personality disorder. Psychol Med, 32(8), 1395-1405.

Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & Anderson, S. W. (1994). Insensitivity to future

consequences following damage to human prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50(1-3), 7-15.

Becker, G. S., & Murphy, K. M. (1988). A theory of rational addiction. Journal of Political

Economy, 96, 675-700.

Beninger, R. J., Wasserman, J., Zanibbi, K., Charbonneau, D., Mangels, J., & Beninger, B. V.

(2003). Typical and atypical antipsychotic medications differentially affect two

nondeclarative memory tasks in schizophrenic patients: a double dissociation. Schizophr

Res, 61(2-3), 281-292.

Berke, J. D. (2003). Learning and memory mechanisms involved in compulsive drug use and

relapse. Methods Mol Med, 79, 75-101.

Page 40: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

33 Best, M., Williams, J. M., & Coccaro, E. F. (2002). Evidence for a dysfunctional prefrontal

circuit in patients with an impulsive aggressive disorder. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A,

99(12), 8448-8453.

Bickel, W. K., & Marsch, L. A. (2001). Toward a behavioral economic understanding of drug

dependence: delay discounting processes. Addiction, 96(1), 73-86.

Bickel, W. K., Marsch, L. A., & Carroll, M. E. (2000). Deconstructing relative reinforcing

efficacy and situating the measures of pharmacological reinforcement with behavioral

economics: a theoretical proposal. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 153(1), 44-56.

Bickel, W. K., Miller, M. L., Yi, R., Kowal, B. P., Lindquist, D. M., & Pitcock, J. A. (2007).

Behavioral and neuroeconomics of drug addiction: competing neural systems and

temporal discounting processes. Drug Alcohol Depend, 90 Suppl 1, S85-91.

Bickel, W. K., Odum, A. L., & Madden, G. J. (1999). Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: delay

discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 146(4), 447-

454.

Cannon, T. D., Glahn, D. C., Kim, J., Van Erp, T. G., Karlsgodt, K., Cohen, M. S., et al. (2005).

Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activity during maintenance and manipulation of

information in working memory in patients with schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry,

62(10), 1071-1080.

Cavallaro, R., Cavedini, P., Mistretta, P., Bassi, T., Angelone, S. M., Ubbiali, A., et al. (2003).

Basal-corticofrontal circuits in schizophrenia and obsessive-compulsive disorder: a

controlled, double dissociation study. Biol Psychiatry, 54(4), 437-443.

Cavedini, P., Bassi, T., Ubbiali, A., Casolari, A., Giordani, S., Zorzi, C., et al. (2004).

Neuropsychological investigation of decision-making in anorexia nervosa. Psychiatry

Res, 127(3), 259-266.

Page 41: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

34 Cavedini, P., Riboldi, G., D'Annucci, A., Belotti, P., Cisima, M., & Bellodi, L. (2002). Decision-

making heterogeneity in obsessive-compulsive disorder: ventromedial prefrontal cortex

function predicts different treatment outcomes. Neuropsychologia, 40(2), 205-211.

Coffey, S. F., Gudleski, G. D., Saladin, M. E., & Brady, K. T. (2003). Impulsivity and rapid

discounting of delayed hypothetical rewards in cocaine-dependent individuals. Exp Clin

Psychopharmacol, 11(1), 18-25.

Critchfield, T. S., & Kollins, S. H. (2001). Temporal discounting: basic research and the analysis

of socially important behavior. J Appl Behav Anal, 34(1), 101-122.

Depatie, L., O'Driscoll, G. A., Holahan, A. L., Atkinson, V., Thavundayil, J. X., Kin, N. N., et al.

(2002). Nicotine and behavioral markers of risk for schizophrenia: a double-blind,

placebo-controlled, cross-over study. Neuropsychopharmacology, 27(6), 1056-1070.

Dolan, S. L., Sacco, K. A., Termine, A., Seyal, A. A., Dudas, M. M., Vessicchio, J. C., et al.

(2004). Neuropsychological deficits are associated with smoking cessation treatment

failure in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res, 70(2-3), 263-275.

Dowling, N., Smith, D., & Thomas, T. (2005). Electronic gaming machines: are they the 'crack-

cocaine' of gambling? Addiction, 100(1), 33-45.

Dunn, B. D., Dalgleish, T., & Lawrence, A. D. (2006). The somatic marker hypothesis: a critical

evaluation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 30(2), 239-271.

Egan, M. F., Goldberg, T. E., Kolachana, B. S., Callicott, J. H., Mazzanti, C. M., Straub, R. E., et

al. (2001). Effect of COMT Val108/158 Met genotype on frontal lobe function and risk

for schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 98(12), 6917-6922.

Evans, C. E., Bowman, C. H., & Turnbull, O. H. (2005). Subjective awareness on the Iowa

Gambling Task: the key role of emotional experience in schizophrenia. J Clin Exp

Neuropsychol, 27(6), 656-664.

Page 42: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

35 Everitt, B. J., Dickinson, A., & Robbins, T. W. (2001). The neuropsychological basis of

addictive behaviour. Brain Res Brain Res Rev, 36(2-3), 129-138.

Freedman, R., Coon, H., Myles-Worsley, M., Orr-Urtreger, A., Olincy, A., Davis, A., et al.

(1997). Linkage of a neurophysiological deficit in schizophrenia to a chromosome 15

locus. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A, 94(2), 587-592.

Geist, C. R., & Herrmann, S. M. (1990). A comparison of the psychological characteristics of

smokers, ex-smokers, and nonsmokers. J Clin Psychol, 46(1), 102-105.

George, T. P., Termine, A., Sacco, K. A., Allen, T. M., Reutenauer, E., Vessicchio, J. C., et al.

(2006). A preliminary study of the effects of cigarette smoking on prepulse inhibition in

schizophrenia: involvement of nicotinic receptor mechanisms. Schizophr Res, 87(1-3),

307-315.

George, T. P., Vessicchio, J. C., Sacco, K. A., Weinberger, A. H., Dudas, M. M., Allen, T. M., et

al. (2008). A placebo-controlled trial of bupropion combined with nicotine patch for

smoking cessation in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry, 63(11), 1092-1096.

George, T. P., Vessicchio, J. C., Termine, A., Bregartner, T. A., Feingold, A., Rounsaville, B. J.,

et al. (2002). A placebo controlled trial of bupropion for smoking cessation in

schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry, 52(1), 53-61.

George, T. P., Vessicchio, J. C., Termine, A., Sahady, D. M., Head, C. A., Pepper, W. T., et al.

(2002). Effects of smoking abstinence on visuospatial working memory function in

schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology, 26(1), 75-85.

Goldstein, J. M., Goodman, J. M., Seidman, L. J., Kennedy, D. N., Makris, N., Lee, H., et al.

(1999). Cortical abnormalities in schizophrenia identified by structural magnetic

resonance imaging. Arch Gen Psychiatry, 56(6), 537-547.

Page 43: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

36 Gur, R. E., Cowell, P. E., Latshaw, A., Turetsky, B. I., Grossman, R. I., Arnold, S. E., et al.

(2000). Reduced dorsal and orbital prefrontal gray matter volumes in schizophrenia. Arch

Gen Psychiatry, 57(8), 761-768.

Heatherton, T. F., Kozlowski, L. T., Frecker, R. C., & Fagerstrom, K. O. (1991). The Fagerstrom

Test for Nicotine Dependence: a revision of the Fagerstrom Tolerance Questionnaire. Br

J Addict, 86(9), 1119-1127.

Heaton, R. K., Chelune, G. J., Talley, J. L., & al., e. (1993). Wisconsin Card Sorting Test

Manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.

Heerey, E. A., Robinson, B. M., McMahon, R. P., & Gold, J. M. (2007). Delay discounting in

schizophrenia. Cognit Neuropsychiatry, 12(3), 213-221.

Hinson, J. M., Jameson, T. L., & Whitney, P. (2003). Impulsive decision making and working

memory. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn, 29(2), 298-306.

Holt, D. D., Green, L., & Myerson, J. (2003). Is discounting impulsive?. Evidence from temporal

and probability discounting in gambling and non-gambling college students. Behav

Processes, 64(3), 355-367.

Hongwanishkul, D., Happaney, K. R., Lee, W. S., & Zelazo, P. D. (2005). Assessment of hot and

cool executive function in young children: age-related changes and individual

differences. Dev Neuropsychol, 28(2), 617-644.

Johnson, M. W., & Bickel, W. K. (2002). Within-subject comparison of real and hypothetical

money rewards in delay discounting. J Exp Anal Behav, 77(2), 129-146.

Johnson, M. W., & Bickel, W. K. (2003). The behavioral economics of cigarette smoking: The

concurrent presence of a substitute and an independent reinforcer. Behav Pharmacol,

14(2), 137-144.

Page 44: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

37 Jollant, F., Guillaume, S., Jaussent, I., Bellivier, F., Leboyer, M., Castelnau, D., et al. (2007).

Psychiatric diagnoses and personality traits associated with disadvantageous decision-

making. Eur Psychiatry, 22(7), 455-461.

Kagel, J. H., Battalio, R. C., & Green, L. (1995). Economic choice theory: An experimental

analysis of animal behavior. New York: Cambridge Univeristy Press.

Kalman, D., Morissette, S. B., & George, T. P. (2005). Co-morbidity of smoking in patients with

psychiatric and substance use disorders. Am J Addict, 14(2), 106-123.

Kapur, S., & Remington, G. (2001). Dopamine D(2) receptors and their role in atypical

antipsychotic action: still necessary and may even be sufficient. Biol Psychiatry, 50(11),

873-883.

Kay, S. R., Fiszbein, A., & Opler, L. A. (1987). The positive and negative syndrome scale

(PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull, 13(2), 261-276.

Kay, S. R., Opler, L. A., & Lindenmayer, J. P. (1988). Reliability and validity of the positive and

negative syndrome scale for schizophrenics. Psychiatry Res, 23(1), 99-110.

Keefe, R. S., Roitman, S. E., Harvey, P. D., Blum, C. S., DuPre, R. L., Prieto, D. M., et al.

(1995). A pen-and-paper human analogue of a monkey prefrontal cortex activation task:

spatial working memory in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res, 17(1), 25-33.

Kester, H. M., Sevy, S., Yechiam, E., Burdick, K. E., Cervellione, K. L., & Kumra, S. (2006).

Decision-making impairments in adolescents with early-onset schizophrenia. Schizophr

Res, 85(1-3), 113-123.

Kirby, K. N. (1997). Bidding on the future: Evidence against normative discounting of delayed

rewards. J Exp Psychol Gen, 126, 54-70.

Kirby, K. N. (2009). One-year temporal stability of delay-discount rates. Psychon Bull Rev,

16(3), 457-462.

Page 45: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

38 Kirby, K. N., Petry, N. M., & Bickel, W. K. (1999). Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for

delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. J Exp Psychol Gen, 128(1), 78-87.

Knable, M. B., & Weinberger, D. R. (1997). Dopamine, the prefrontal cortex and schizophrenia.

J Psychopharmacol, 11(2), 123-131.

Kreek, M. J., Nielsen, D. A., Butelman, E. R., & LaForge, K. S. (2005). Genetic influences on

impulsivity, risk taking, stress responsivity and vulnerability to drug abuse and addiction.

Nat Neurosci, 8(11), 1450-1457.

Krishnan-Sarin, S., Duhig, A. M., McKee, S. A., McMahon, T. J., Liss, T., McFetridge, A., et al.

(2006). Contingency management for smoking cessation in adolescent smokers. Exp Clin

Psychopharmacol, 14(3), 306-310.

Krishnan-Sarin, S., Reynolds, B., Duhig, A. M., Smith, A., Liss, T., McFetridge, A., et al.

(2007). Behavioral impulsivity predicts treatment outcome in a smoking cessation

program for adolescent smokers. Drug Alcohol Depend, 88(1), 79-82.

Lagorio, C. H., & Madden, G. J. (2005). Delay discounting of real and hypothetical rewards III:

steady-state assessments, forced-choice trials, and all real rewards. Behav Processes,

69(2), 173-187.

Lawrence, N. S., Wooderson, S., Mataix-Cols, D., David, R., Speckens, A., & Phillips, M. L.

(2006). Decision making and set shifting impairments are associated with distinct

symptom dimensions in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Neuropsychology, 20(4), 409-

419.

Le Foll, B., & George, T. P. (2007). Treatment of tobacco dependence: integrating recent

progress into practice. CMAJ, 177(11), 1373-1380.

Lee, J., & Park, S. (2005). Working memory impairments in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. J

Abnorm Psychol, 114(4), 599-611.

Page 46: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

39 Levin, E. D., Wilson, W., Rose, J. E., & McEvoy, J. (1996). Nicotine-haloperidol interactions

and cognitive performance in schizophrenics. Neuropsychopharmacology, 15(5), 429-

436.

Madden, G. J., Begotka, A. M., Raiff, B. R., & Kastern, L. L. (2003). Delay discounting of real

and hypothetical rewards. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol, 11(2), 139-145.

Madden, G. J., Petry, N. M., Badger, G. J., & Bickel, W. K. (1997). Impulsive and self-control

choices in opioid-dependent patients and non-drug-using control participants: drug and

monetary rewards. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol, 5(3), 256-262.

Marcello, S. (2007). Impulsivity in patients diagnosed with substance use disorder and

schizophrenia. Temple University, Philadelphia.

Martino, D. J., Bucay, D., Butman, J. T., & Allegri, R. F. (2007). Neuropsychological frontal

impairments and negative symptoms in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res, 152(2-3), 121-

128.

Mazur, J. (1987). An adjusting procedure for delayed reinforcement New Jersey: Erlbaum.

Mazur, J. (1988). Estimation of indifference points with an adjusting-delay procedure. J Exp

Anal Behav, 49(1), 37-47.

McClure, S. M., Laibson, D. I., Loewenstein, G., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). Separate neural systems

value immediate and delayed monetary rewards. Science, 306(5695), 503-507.

Meador-Woodruff, J. H., & Watson, S. J. (1997). Postmortem studies in schizophrenic brain. J

Psychiatr Res, 31(2), 157-158.

Mitchell, D., Colledge, E., Leonard, A., & Blair, R. J. (2002). Risky decisions and response

reversal: is there evidence of orbitofrontal cortex dysfunction in psychopathic

individuals? Neuropsychologia, 40(12), 2013-2022.

Mitchell, S. (1999). Measures of impulsivity in cigarette smokers and non-smokers.

Psychopharmacology (Berl), 146(4), 455-464.

Page 47: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

40 Moberg, P. J., Agrin, R., Gur, R. E., Gur, R. C., Turetsky, B. I., & Doty, R. L. (1999). Olfactory

dysfunction in schizophrenia: a qualitative and quantitative review.

Neuropsychopharmacology, 21(3), 325-340.

Morice, R., & Delahunty, A. (1996). Frontal/executive impairments in schizophrenia. Schizophr

Bull, 22(1), 125-137.

Murphy, F. C., Rubinsztein, J. S., Michael, A., Rogers, R. D., Robbins, T. W., Paykel, E. S., et

al. (2001). Decision-making cognition in mania and depression. Psychol Med, 31(4), 679-

693.

Must, A., Szabo, Z., Bodi, N., Szasz, A., Janka, Z., & Keri, S. (2006). Sensitivity to reward and

punishment and the prefrontal cortex in major depression. J Affect Disord, 90(2-3), 209-

215.

Navarick, D. (2004). Discounting of delayed reinforcers: Measurment by questionaires versus

operent choice procedures. Psychol. Rec., 54, 85-94.

Nelson, H. E., & Willison, J. R. (1991). The Revised National Adult Reading Test - Test Manual.

Windsor, UK: NFER-Nelson.

Normand, J., Vlahov, D., & Moses, L. E. (1995). Preventing HIV transmission: The role of

sterile needles and cleach. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.

Park, S., & Holzman, P. S. (1992). Schizophrenics show spatial working memory deficits. Arch

Gen Psychiatry, 49(12), 975-982.

Petry, N. M. (2001a). Delay discounting of money and alcohol in actively using alcoholics,

currently abstinent alcoholics, and controls. Psychopharmacology (Berl), 154(3), 243-

250.

Petry, N. M. (2001b). Pathological gamblers, with and without substance use disorders, discount

delayed rewards at high rates. J Abnorm Psychol, 110(3), 482-487.

Page 48: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

41 Petry, N. M., Kirby, K. N., & Kranzler, H. R. (2002). Effects of gender and family history of

alcohol dependence on a behavioral task of impulsivity in healthy subjects. J Stud

Alcohol, 63(1), 83-90.

Redish, A. D., Jensen, S., & Johnson, A. (2008). A unified framework for addiction:

vulnerabilities in the decision process. Behav Brain Sci, 31(4), 415-437; discussion 437-

487.

Redish, A. D., Jensen, S., Johnson, A., & Kurth-Nelson, Z. (2007). Reconciling reinforcement

learning models with behavioral extinction and renewal: implications for addiction,

relapse, and problem gambling. Psychol Rev, 114(3), 784-805.

Reynolds, B. (2004). Do high rates of cigarette consumption increase delay discounting? A

cross-sectional comparison of adolescent smokers and young-adult smokers and

nonsmokers. Behav Processes, 67(3), 545-549.

Richards, J. B., Mitchell, S. H., de Wit, H., & Seiden, L. S. (1997). Determination of discount

functions in rats with an adjusting-amount procedure. J Exp Anal Behav, 67(3), 353-366.

Rolls, E. T. (2000). The orbitofrontal cortex and reward. Cereb Cortex, 10(3), 284-294.

Russell, A. J., Munro, J., Jones, P. B., Hayward, P., Hemsley, D. R., & Murray, R. M. (2000).

The National Adult Reading Test as a measure of premorbid IQ in schizophrenia. Br J

Clin Psychol, 39 ( Pt 3), 297-305.

Sacco, K. A., Termine, A., Dudas, M. M., Seyal, A. A., Allen, T. M., Vessicchio, J. C., et al.

(2006). Neuropsychological deficits in nonsmokers with schizophrenia: effects of a

nicotinic antagonist. Schizophr Res, 85(1-3), 213-221.

Sacco, K. A., Termine, A., Seyal, A., Dudas, M. M., Vessicchio, J. C., Krishnan-Sarin, S., et al.

(2005). Effects of cigarette smoking on spatial working memory and attentional deficits

in schizophrenia: involvement of nicotinic receptor mechanisms. Arch Gen Psychiatry,

62(6), 649-659.

Page 49: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

42 Salmond, C. H., Menon, D. K., Chatfield, D. A., Pickard, J. D., & Sahakian, B. J. (2005).

Deficits in decision-making in head injury survivors. J Neurotrauma, 22(6), 613-622.

Shurman, B., Horan, W. P., & Nuechterlein, K. H. (2005). Schizophrenia patients demonstrate a

distinctive pattern of decision-making impairment on the Iowa Gambling Task. Schizophr

Res, 72(2-3), 215-224.

Smith, R. C., Singh, A., Infante, M., Khandat, A., & Kloos, A. (2002). Effects of cigarette

smoking and nicotine nasal spray on psychiatric symptoms and cognition in

schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology, 27(3), 479-497.

Tranel, D., Bechara, A., & Denburg, N. L. (2002). Asymmetric functional roles of right and left

ventromedial prefrontal cortices in social conduct, decision-making, and emotional

processing. Cortex, 38(4), 589-612.

Turnbull, O. H., Evans, C. E., Kemish, K., Park, S., & Bowman, C. H. (2006). A novel set-

shifting modification of the iowa gambling task: flexible emotion-based learning in

schizophrenia. Neuropsychology, 20(3), 290-298.

Waldeck, T. L., & Miller, L. S. (1997). Gender and impulsivity differences in licit substance use.

J Subst Abuse, 9, 269-275.

Weinberger, A. H., Reutenauer, E. L., Allen, T. M., Termine, A., Vessicchio, J. C., Sacco, K. A.,

et al. (2007). Reliability of the Fagerstrom Test for Nicotine Dependence, Minnesota

Nicotine Withdrawal Scale, and Tiffany Questionnaire for Smoking Urges in smokers

with and without schizophrenia. Drug Alcohol Depend, 86(2-3), 278-282.

Weinberger, A. H., Sacco, K. A., Creeden, C. L., Vessicchio, J. C., Jatlow, P. I., & George, T. P.

(2007). Effects of acute abstinence, reinstatement, and mecamylamine on biochemical

and behavioral measures of cigarette smoking in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res, 91(1-3),

217-225.

Page 50: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

43 Wolkin, A., Choi, S. J., Szilagyi, S., Sanfilipo, M., Rotrosen, J. P., & Lim, K. O. (2003). Inferior

frontal white matter anisotropy and negative symptoms of schizophrenia: a diffusion

tensor imaging study. Am J Psychiatry, 160(3), 572-574.

Yip, S. W., Sacco, K. A., George, T. P., & Potenza, M. N. (2009). Risk/reward decision-making

in schizophrenia: a preliminary examination of the influence of tobacco smoking and

relationship to Wisconsin Card Sorting Task performance. Schizophr Res, 110(1-3), 156-

164.

Page 51: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

44

APPENDICES

Appendix A

Outliers

SS – 16 removed

Page 52: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

45

SNS – 27, 18, 03 removed

CS – 29 & 32 removed

Page 53: A CROSS-SECTIONAL COMPARISON OF DELAY DISCOUNTING IN ... · discount rate is quantified by a hyperbolic discounting equation (Mazur, 1987): In this equation, the V d represents the

46

* No CNS were excluded from the analyses.