martorellcornell.may12,2010revhandout.ppt [read-only]...t-test = 24.50 p < .005 (2-tail, df = 2)...

9
5/28/2010 1 Long-Term Effects of an Early Life Intervention in Guatemala Reynaldo Martorell PhD Reynaldo Martorell, PhD Hubert Dept. of Global Health Emory University May 12, 2010 Millennium Development Goal 2: Achieve Universal Primary Education Target Ensure that, by 2015, children everywhere, boys and girls alike, will be able to complete a full course of primary education Why do we care about schooling? Importance for child health and nutrition Gender equity and empowerment empowerment Long term health benefits to individuals Earnings Latin America & Caribbean 12.0% The average rate of return to income of another year of schooling Sub-Saharan Africa 11.7% Asia 9.9% World average: 10.0% Psacharopoulos G, Patrinos HA. Returns to investment in education: A further update. Educ Econ 2004; 12(2): 111–134. Gestation and the first 2 years of life represent a window of vulnerability for human development High nutritional requirements Rapid growth and Rapid growth and development Greater susceptibility to infections Fully dependent on others for care Height for age by region, children < 5 years Worldwide timing of growth faltering: revisiting implications for interventions. Victora CG, de Onis M, Hallal PC, Blössner M, Shrimpton R. Pediatrics. 2010 Mar;125(3):e473-80. Epub 2010 Feb 15.

Upload: others

Post on 14-Feb-2021

0 views

Category:

Documents


0 download

TRANSCRIPT

  • 5/28/2010

    1

    Long-Term Effects of an Early Life Intervention in Guatemala

    Reynaldo Martorell PhDReynaldo Martorell, PhDHubert Dept. of Global Health

    Emory University

    May 12, 2010

    Millennium Development Goal 2: Achieve Universal Primary Education

    TargetEnsure that, by 2015, children everywhere, boys and girls alike, will be able to complete a full course of primary education

    Why do we care about schooling?

    • Importance for child health and nutrition

    • Gender equity and empowermentempowerment

    • Long term health benefits to individuals

    • Earnings

    Latin America & Caribbean

    12.0%

    The average rate of return to income of another year of schooling

    Sub-Saharan Africa 11.7%

    Asia 9.9%

    World average: 10.0%

    Psacharopoulos G, Patrinos HA. Returns to investment in education: A further update. Educ Econ 2004; 12(2): 111–134.

    Gestation and the first 2 years of life represent a window of vulnerability for human development

    • High nutritional requirements

    • Rapid growth andRapid growth and development

    • Greater susceptibility to infections

    • Fully dependent on others for care

    Height for age by region, children < 5 years

    Worldwide timing of growth faltering: revisiting implications for interventions.Victora CG, de Onis M, Hallal PC, Blössner M, Shrimpton R.Pediatrics. 2010 Mar;125(3):e473-80. Epub 2010 Feb 15.

  • 5/28/2010

    2

    Human brain development

    Original: Thompson, 2001; taken from Grantham-McGregor, 2007

    INCAPInstituto de Nutrición de Centro América y Panamá

    INCAP longitudinal study, 1969-77

    • Community-randomized supplementation trial (2 large and 2 small villages).

    • Two villages (1 large, 1 small)Two villages (1 large, 1 small) received Atole, a nutritious supplement made from Incaparina, milk and sugar, and two (1 large, 1 small) received Fresco, a less nutritive drink

    Martorell R, Habicht J-P, Rivera JA. History and design of the INCAP longitudinal study (1969-77) and its follow-up (1988-89). Journal of Nutrition 125(Suppl. 4S):1027S-1041S, 1995.

    Children included in the Longitudinal Study ( 1969-77)

    • All children < 7 y in 1969 were followed until they turned 7y or to the study end

    • All newborns were followed until they turned 7 y or to the study end

    • Mothers included during pregnancy and lactation

    Impact on total nutrient intakes and on growth

    • Supplement and home dietary intakes measured• Total diets of young children < 3y from Atole villagesTotal diets of young children 3y from Atole villages

    were greater by 9 g of protein, 100 kcal/day and in micronutrients when compared to diets of children from Fresco villages.

    • Length was increased by 3 cm in Atole vs. Fresco but only in the first three years of life.

    Difference in length at 3 years of age between children (n = 453) exposed to supplement during their entire lives and those measured at baseline in 1968: village level analysis (Habicht JP, Martorell R, Rivera JA. J Nutr 1995; 125: S1042–50)

    2.6

    3.3

    3

    4

    cm

    t-test = 24.50p < .005 (2-tail, df = 2)

    Large Small Large Small

    0.7

    0.2

    0

    1

    2

    Atole Fresco

    cm

    Villages

  • 5/28/2010

    3

    The preschool battery of the 1969-77 longitudinal study+Preschool battery (cognitive development as well as Piagetian concepts).• 10 tests given annually to all children

    3-7 yrs of age.• 12 tests given annually to all children

    5-7 yrs of age.

    F t l iFactor analysis• Factor 1

    General perceptual – organizational (embedded figures, incomplete figures, odd figures, block design, memory for designs) and verbal (verbal analogies, memory for objects, vocabulary recognition) factor.

    • Factor 2Memory (digit, sentence)

    + Pollitt E, Gorman KS, Engle P, Martorell R and Rivera JA. , 1995

    Comment about effects seen in preschool children in the 1969-77 longitudinal study

    “…there were a few moderately beneficial effects from exposure to

    the Atole supplement”

    + Pollitt E, Gorman KS, Engle P, Martorell R and Rivera JA. , 1995

    • Longitudinal study 1969-77( children < 7 yrs; n = 2392)

    • Follow-up study 1988-89 (youth 11-26 yrs; n = 1574 ;

    Specific studies emphasized in this presentation

    p y (y yincluded migrants to nearby villages and Guatemala City)

    • Nutrition, human capital and economic productivity, 2002-2004 (adults 25-40 years; n = 1560 ; migrants to anywhere in the country)

    Definition of cohorts based on timingof exposure to supplementation

    Cohortnumber Birth cohorts Exposure period

    I ≥ 1 Mar 1974 Gestation, partial birth to 3 y, p y

    II 1 Mar 1969 – 28 Feb 1974Partial during gestation, all birth to 3 y

    III 1 Jan 1966 – 28 Feb 1969 Partial birth to 3 y

    IV ≤ Jan 1966 No exposure duringgestation or birth to 3 y

    The 1988-89 Follow-up Study results

    • Impact largest and more consistent for Cohort II

    • Youth in Atole villages taller , greater fat free mass and greater workmass and greater work capacity ( males).

    • Performance on tests of knowledge, numeracy, reading, comprehension and vocabulary better in Atole villages.

    The 1988-89 Follow-up subjects were 11 to 26 years and this limited our ability to properly assess human

    capital formation and to link it to economic productivity

    • Some had not yet finished growing.

    • S h d t t• Some had not yet finished studying.

    • Some had not chosenan occupation.

    • Some had not yet married or formedunions

  • 5/28/2010

    4

    Human Capital Study2002-04: age ≈ 32 years

    • Original sample : 2393

    • Possible sample: 1856+, ++

    • Achieved : 1560 (84 % )

    + Subjects living in Guatemala in 2002.++ 272 died, mostly in early childhood, 163 left the country ,102 were lost to

    follow up.

    Nutrition, human capital, and economic productivity (2002-04)

    Emory

    INCAP

    Univ. of Penn.

    IFPRI

    Funded by Fogarty International Center, NIH, and NSF

    Average age of cohort was 32 y

    Analytic approach of the Human Capital Study (2002-04)

    • Less emphasis on four birth cohorts ( e.g. Cohort II )g )

    • Focus on testing “windows of exposure” using all subjects to estimate double-differences

    Example: assessing exposure to either Atole or Fresco from 0-36 months

    Intervention period

    Birth

    1962 1969 1974 1977

    Children too old for exposure from

    0–36 m

    Children exposed from

    0–36 m

    Birth Year

    Children too young for

    exposure from 0-36 m

    Double-difference estimate for exposure from 0-36 months of age

    Average outcome for those exposed to atole 0 36 m

    Average outcome for those exposed to fresco 0 36 m

    – –to atole 0–36 m

    completelyto fresco 0–36 m

    completely

    Average outcome for those NOT

    exposed to atole0–36 m completely

    Average outcome for those NOT

    exposed to fresco0–36 m completely

    Human Capital measures

    • Schooling

    • Intelligence (Raven test)(Raven test)

    • Readingability (Inter- AmericanReading test)

  • 5/28/2010

    5

    Exposure to improved nutrition from 0–3 years of age and education (n=1471)+

    • Schooling: Effects found in women only– Improved by 1.2 years (36% of SD)

    • Reading: Effects found in men and womenReading: Effects found in men and women– Improved scores by 28% of SD++

    • Cognition: Effects found in men and women– Improved Raven scores by 24% of SD+++

    + Maluccio J, Hoddinott J, Behrman JR, Martorell R, Quisumbing A, and Stein A. The impact of improving nutrition during early childhood on education among Guatemalan adults . The Economic Journal, 119:734-763. 2009. Use of instrumental variable approach confirms OLS results.

    ++ Raven Progressive Matrices+++ Inter-American Reading Series

    Impact of exposure to atole during early life on income (in US$) earned per hour;

    n = 602 men; age ~32 years+

    P = 0.009P = 0.007

    P = 0.406US$/hr

    Window of exposure (months)+ Hoddinott, Maluccio, Behrman, Flores and Martorell. Effect of a nutrition intervention during early childhood on economic productivity in Guatemalan adults (The Lancet, 2008).

    Long terms effects of a nutrition intervention carried out in Guatemala in 1969-77

    Improved nutrition before, but not after 3 years of

    i d i

    _______________________________Hoddinott, Maluccio, Behrman, Flores and Martorell. Effect of a nutrition intervention during early childhood on economic productivity in Guatemalan adults (The Lancet, 2008).

    age, increased wages in men by 46% and annual incomes by $914

    Child mental development  (childhood)

    Child Heightat 3 y

    BRAIN BRAWNImproved Nutrition(Exposure to Atole) 

    0‐3 years

    Pathways by which improving early nutritionincreases wages in Guatemalan adult men

    Height, strength, cardio vascular endurance 

    (adulthood) 

    Schooling (years) achieved

    Cognitive skills(adulthood)

    More skilledjobs Wages 46%

    Source: Drawn by Marie Ruel from  Behrman JR, Hoddinott J, Maluccio JA, Martorell R. Brains versus brawn: Labor market returns to intellectual and physical human capital in a poor developing country, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, Photocopy, 2009.

    Y Chen and LA Zhou, The long-term health and economic consequences of the 1959–1961 famine in China. J Health

    Econ, 26: 659-681, 2007.

    • 20 million people died in the 1959-62 famine in China (Great Leap Forward)

    • Survivors born in 1959-62 (exposed early in life) wereSurvivors born in 1959 62 (exposed early in life) were compared to cohorts born before or after the famine.

    • Famine exposure in early life was associated with a reduction in:

    – Adult height , 3 cm

    – Annual per capita income, 33% lower.

    Is there a tradeoff from improving nutrition in malnourished infants between

    - improved human capital ( IQ, schooling , income, etc.)- but poor adult health ( increased cardiovascular disease risk, abdominal obesity, etc ?

  • 5/28/2010

    6

    ConclusionInterventions designed

    to address nutrient deficiencies and

    Stein AD, Wang M, Ramirez-Zea M, Flores R, Grajeda R, Melgar P, Ramakrishnan U, Martorell R. Exposure to a nutrition supplementation intervention in early childhood and risk factors for cardiovascular disease in adulthood: evidence from Guatemala. Am J Epidemiol 2006; 164:1160-1170.

    ameliorate stunting that are targeted at pregnant

    women and young children are unlikely to increase cardiovascular disease risk later in life and may instead lower

    the risk.

    Intergenerational Effects ( Behrman,CalderÓn, Preston, Hoddinott, Martorell & Stein; AJCN, 2009)

    • Offspring of women exposed to Atole ( vs. Fresco) were heavier at birth ( 116g) and were taller (1.3 cm) and had greater head circumferences ( 0.6 cm) in childhood. No effects on measures of adiposity . Effects were greater in sons.

    • Father’s exposure to the supplements in early life, on the other hand, did not influence their offspring's characteristics

    GuatemalaDelhi

    Cebu

    PelotasSoweto

    COHORTS meeting in South Africa

    Paper #2 Lancet Series on Maternal and Child Undernutrition(Maternal and child undernutrition: consequences for adult health and human

    capital Victora, Adair, Fall, Hallal, Martorell, Richter and Sachdev, 2008)

    • Review of the literature and original analyses of 5 cohorts (Brazil, Guatemala, India, Philippines and South Africa).

    • Poor fetal growth or stunting in the first two years of• Poor fetal growth or stunting in the first two years of life linked to irreversible damage including: shorter adult height lower attained schooling reduced adult income decreased offspring birthweight

    • Striking consistency of relationships across sites.

  • 5/28/2010

    7

    Children whose early growth is restricted and gain weight rapidly later in later childhood are more likely to have high blood pressure, diabetes and both cardiovascular and metabolic disease

    Rapid Weight Gain After Being Undernourished Increases Chronic Disease Risk as Adults

    metabolic disease

    No evidence that rapid weight or length gain in first 2 years increases risk of chronic disease later in life

    By supporting early nutrition and growth, incidence of chronic disease could be reduced

    PAPER 2. Lancet series

    Growth and schooling ( Martorell, Horta, Adair et al, J. Nut. 140: 348-354

    • Analyses of the relative importance of birthweight and weight gain between 0-2 y and 2-4 y for three schooling outcomes.

    – Highest grade attainedHighest grade attained– Ever failed a grade– Age at school entry

    Highest grade (y) attained by study site

    9.5+

    13.5

    10.8

    9.0++

    8

    10

    12

    14

    16

    Yrs

    5.0

    0

    2

    4

    6

    8

    Guatemala(4 villages)

    India(New Delhi)

    Brazil (Pelotas)

    South Africa (Soweto)

    Philippines(Cebu)

    + 43% of participants still in school++ Almost all participants in school

    % stunted (< -2 HAZ) at 2 y, by site

    86%

    68%

    60%

    80%

    100%

    12%

    47%

    27%

    0%

    20%

    40%

    60%

    Guatemala(4 villages)

    India(New Delhi)

    Brazil (Pelotas)

    South Africa (Soweto)

    Philippines(Cebu)

    %

    Statistical models

    • Used conditional weight gain variables (0-2 and 2-4 y) that were uncorrelatedwith each other and with birthweight.

    • The basic model adjusted for site j(except for age at school entry) and sex.

    • The fully adjusted model also included household socioeconomic status (when participants were children), maternal years of schooling and whether the subject was still in school (for highest grade attained).

    0.5

    Years of increased schooling associated with a standard deviation shift in birth weight and conditional weight gain (0-24 and 24-48m)

    in 5 cohort studies, after control for confounding

    Adjusted for SES and maternal education

    0.43, p

  • 5/28/2010

    8

    Relationship between stunting at age 2y (1=yes, 0=no) and schooling outcomes in pooled

    analyses from 5 cohorts++

    Coefficient+ 95% C.I. P value

    Highest grade -0.92 -1.08, -0.76 < 0.0001Highest grade attained (y)

    0.92 1.08, 0.76 0.0001

    Ever failed a grade(1=yes, 0=no)

    1.20 1.04, 1.39 0.004

    + Adjusted for SES and maternal education; Martorell et al, 2010 ++ According to the economic literature, the average rate of return to income of another

    year of schooling is 10% (Psacharopoulos & Patrinos, 2004)

    The findings from our intervention study in Guatemala do not provide the full measure of the

    impact of nutrition

    • The intervention had a modest impact on complementary feeding intakes

    • There was no WASH intervention component• There was substantial stunting remaining among children

    receiving Atole ; micronutrient deficiencies, such as in iron, were also probably common among Atole children

    • On the other hand, breastfeeding was common although not practiced exclusively

    We now have experimental evidence that breastfeeding is extremely important for humanextremely important for human

    capital development

    Breast-feeding and cognitive development score: a meta-analysis of differences

    with respect to formula feeding+

    Benefit++ 95% CIUnadjusted 5.3Adjusted 3.2

    4.5, 6.12.4, 4.0

    + Anderson, Johnston & Remley. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 70:525-535, 1999.++ Before and after adjustment for confounding

    Breastfeeding and Cognitive Development (Kramer et al, 2008).

    • Available reports are mostly based on observational data. Although these studies consistently show benefits in cognition, residual confounding remains an issue.

    • A large cluster-randomized trial of breastfeeding (BF) promotion was conducted in Belarus (Kramer et al, 2008).

    E l i BF t 3 43% f th i t l d• Exclusive BF at 3 mo was 43% for the experimental and 6% for the control group (P

  • 5/28/2010

    9

    Programs in health and nutrition aimed at women

    and young children

    Better growth d d l tand development

    Increased Productivity

    Improved Human Capital

    Repositioning nutrition as central todevelopment (World Bank, 2006)

    According to the World Bank (2006), good nutrition is not only a matter of welfare or human rights…it is an economic investment… an engine of economic growth.